Table 3.
Reference | Participants/Site | Results |
---|---|---|
RCTs | ||
Anzman-Frasca et al., 2018 [26] | 58 families with 4–8 year old children, quick-service restaurant | Placemats: ordered more healthy food compared to controls (B = −1.76, 95% CI −3.34, −0.19). No (overall) differences in dietary intake compared to control. |
Cohen et al., 2015 [27] | Students 1–8 grade urban, low-income districts, school cafeteria | Fruit and vegetable selection increased in smart cafe, however smart café intervention alone had no effect on consumption. |
Greene et al., 2017 [28] 9 week cluster |
Ten middle schools (5–8 grade), cafeteria | Overall, fruit selection increased by 36% (p < 0.001), and fruit consumption increased by 23% (p < 0.017) in the fruit intervention group, compared to controls. |
Hollands et al., 2018 [29] stepped wedge |
Nine worksite cafeterias | No significant change in daily energy purchase when data from all six sites were pooled. |
Vasiljevic et al., 2018 [30] | Six worksite cafeterias | No overall effect in energy purchase. One site 6.6% reduction (95% CI −12.9 to –0.3, p = 0.044) in energy purchased, however, the association diminished over time. |
Velema et al., 2018 [31] | Employees | Positive effects on purchases for three of seven products |
Non RCTs | ||
Cole et al., 2018 [32] | US Army active duty soldiers, military installation | Intervention associated with increased diet quality and consumption of healthy food. |
Hubbard et al., 2015 [33] | Students (n 43) 11–22 years with intellectual and developmental disabilities |
Smarter lunchroom increased selection (whole grains) and consumption (whole grains, fruit) of healthy food. |
Kroese et al., 2015 [34] | Travelers, train station snack shops | More healthy (but not fewer unhealthy) products were sold in both nudge conditions. |
Levy et al., 2012 [35] | Employees who were regular cafeteria patrons (n = 4642) | Labeling decreased unhealthy purchases and increased healthy purchases. |
Lowe et al., 2010 [36] | Employees, worksite cafeteria | Total energy intake: no difference. Dietary intake improved over study period. |
Nikolaou et al., 2014 [37] | 120 students, catering | Calorie-labeling associated with a 3.5 kg less weight gain. |
Olstad et al., 2014 [38] | Patrons, recreational swimming pool | In the full sample, sales of healthy items did not differ across periods. In the subsample, the sale of healthy items increased by 30% when signage + taste testing was implemented (p < 0.01). |
Seward et al., 2016 [39] | 6 college cafeterias (Harvard University, Cambridge, Massachusetts) | No significant changes (items served) were revealed when intervention sites were compared with controls. |
Thorndike et al., 2014 [40] | Cafeteria | The traffic-light and choice architecture cafeteria intervention resulted in increased sale of healthier items over 2 years (from 41% to 46%). |
Thorndike et al., 2012 [41] | Hospital cafeteria | A color-coded labeling intervention improved sales of healthy items and was enhanced by a choice architecture intervention. |
Van Kleef et al., 2018 [42] | Participants at a Dutch university |
Regardless of the topping, when the whole wheat bun was the default option, 94% decided to stick with the default. |
Van Kleef et al., 2015 [43] | Customers in self-service restaurant during breakfast |
The sales increased significant during the verbal prompts intervention periods compared to baseline. |
Van Kleef et al., 2014 [44] | Children (n = 1113) primary schools in the Netherlands | Consumption of fun-shaped whole wheat bread rolls almost doubled consumption of whole wheat bread (p = 0.001). |
Van Kleef et al., 2012 [45] | Students | Assortment structure led to higher sales of healthy snacks. |
Vermote et al., 2018 [46] | University students and employees | Total french fries intake decreased by 9.1%, and total plate waste decreased by 66.4%. No differences in satiety or caloric intake (dietary recall) between baseline and intervention week. |
RCT: Randomized Control Trials.