Abstract
The superior colliculus (SC) is a layered midbrain structure involved in directing both head and eye movements and coordinating visual attention. Although a retinotopic organization for the mediation of saccadic eye-movements has been shown in monkey SC, in human SC the topography of saccades has not been confirmed. Here, a novel experimental paradigm was performed by five participants (one female) while high-resolution (1.2-mm) functional magnetic resonance imaging was used to measure activity evoked by saccadic eye movements within human SC. Results provide three critical observations about the topography of the SC: (1) saccades along the superior-inferior visual axis are mapped across the medial-lateral anatomy of the SC; (2) the saccadic eye-movement representation is in register with the retinotopic organization of visual stimulation; and (3) activity evoked by saccades occurs deeper within SC than that evoked by visual stimulation. These approaches lay the foundation for studying the organization of human subcortical – and enhanced cortical mapping – of eye-movement mechanisms.
Keywords: Superior colliculus, Saccades, fMRI, Subcortical vision, Topography, Attention
Introduction
The superior colliculus (SC) is a layered midbrain nucleus with multiple functions including processing vision, orienting attention, priming head movements, generating saccadic eye movements and integrating multiple sensory inputs. The SC is organized into seven laminae of distinct cytoarchitecture with alternating cellular and fiber character. There are three superficial layers and four inner layers (Wurtz and Albano, 1980). The superficial layers receive direct retinal input and contain visual neurons with retinotopically organized receptive fields (Cynader and Berman, 1972; Feldon and Kruger, 1970). The inner layers are separated into intermediate and deep layers. The two intermediate layers contain motor output neurons that control eye movements (Robinson, 1972), whereas the two deep layers contain multisensory integration neurons (Meredith and Stein, 1990; Sprague and Meikle, 1965).
The oculomotor function of the SC was first discovered by electrical stimulation (Adamuk, 1872; Donders, 1872), and later induced via strychnine (Apter, 1946) in anesthetized cats. Extensive studies in alert monkeys via electrical stimulation (Robinson, 1972), neuronal recordings (Mohler and Wurtz, 1976), and lesions (Wurtz and Goldberg, 1972) then characterized the non-human primate topography of the SC [see several reviews (Fuchs et al., 1985; Sparks and Hartwich-Young, 1989; Sparks and Jay, 1986; Wurtz and Albano, 1980):]. The intermediate layers of monkey SC contain a retinotopically organized saccadic eye-movement map (Schiller and Stryker, 1972). Saccade magnitude is roughly mapped along the rostral-caudal axis of monkey SC, whereas saccade direction is mapped along the medial-lateral axis of the SC. The mapping is not linear, with over-representation of small-magnitude saccades, and of polar angles close to the horizontal meridian (Sparks et al., 1976). Also, the SC topography shows a small but variable amount of tilt in the left-right direction that is in rough correspondence to the anatomic tilt of the colliculi relative to the neuraxis (Robinson, 1972). Recent studies have further revealed that the upper visual field is over-represented, comprised of smaller and more sensitive receptive fields (Hafed and Chen, 2016).
In humans, studies to infer SC function have historically been restricted to whole-brain functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). Whole-brain or low-resolution (i.e., ≥2-mm voxels) fMRI has documented saccade-related activity in SC (de Weijer et al., 2010; Furlan et al., 2015; Krebs et al., 2010), and reach-related functions performed by the deep layers of SC (Himmelbach et al., 2013). However, the low-resolution measurements did not delineate the detailed topography of SC functions. Further, previous human studies of saccadic mapping in cortex, not SC, have attempted to use fMRI phase-encoding approaches (Connolly et al., 2015; Konen and Kastner, 2008; Schluppeck et al., 2005; Sereno et al., 2001) but with two critical limitations: (1) a very low duty cycle and (2) reverse saccades made immediately after forward saccades. The low duty cycle forces participants to fixate most of the time instead of performing saccades, which reduces the evoked hemodynamic activity. A paradigm with a higher duty cycle that isolates saccades in one direction is likely to be crucial to getting reliable SC topography measurements with fMRI.
More recent human fMRI studies were able to demonstrate the presence of visual stimulation retinotopy (Schneider and Kastner, 2005), and covert attention signals (Schneider and Kastner, 2009) in superficial SC using higher resolution fMRI (1.5 × 1.5 x 2-mm voxels) targeted specifically to midbrain. Our laboratory expanded upon these studies using higher resolution fMRI (1.2-mm cubic voxels) to demonstrate a detailed transverse retinotopic organization of visual attention and stimulation upon SC (Katyal et al., 2012, 2010; Katyal and Ress, 2014).
Here, we improved those methods and developed a novel task to measure the polar angle representation of saccadic eye movements, elucidating their topography within human SC. Using our previous visual stimulation paradigm optimized for human fMRI in SC (Katyal et al., 2010), we show that these eye movement maps are in register with the retinotopy observed in SC superficial layers. Finally, we demonstrate that the measured activity is elicited from the intermediate layers. Altogether, our results again confirm macaque electrophysiology, and create an experimental framework for further experiments in human eye-movement physiology both in brainstem and cortex.
Materials and methods
Participants
We recruited five participants (4 males, all right-handed) to undergo several ~2 h long scanning sessions. One-to-two sessions were acquired for each participant to discern the polar-angle representation of leftward and rightward saccadic eye movements separately, which were expected to evoke activity primarily in the contralateral SC. Each eye-movement session consisted of 12–16 278-s runs. One-to-two scanning sessions were also acquired from each participant for visual stimulation retinotopic mapping. Visual stimulation experiments were intended to evoke activity in both SC since the entire visual field was stimulated in a single session. Retinotopy sessions consisted of 14–16 228-s runs. Participants gave informed consent prior to scanning based on our approved protocol from the Baylor College of Medicine Institutional Review Board.
Experimental design
Stimuli were generated using MATLAB R2015a (Mathworks, Natick, MA) and PyschToolbox-3 (Brainard, 1997). Stimuli were presented on a 32” LCD BOLD Screen (Cambridge Research Systems, Kent, UK) at the back of the scanner bore 1.3 m away from the participants’ eyes. The display was gamma corrected using an i1 Pro 2 spectrophotometer (X-Rite, Grand Rapids, MI), and had a mean luminance of 305 cd/m2.
We designed a paradigm in which participants performed many saccades in one direction while minimizing saccades in the opposing direction (see Movie M1). Participants made saccades either to the left or to the right (activating primarily the contralateral SC) while we cyclically varied the vertical component of the saccade to correspond to the lower, horizontal, and upper visual field (Fig. 1). Participants performed three 6° saccades guided by a green dot target in a static grid of 12 red dots. The static red dots were arranged as 4 dots separated by 6° along each of the three principle axes (horizontal, 45° diagonal, and −45° diagonal). The use of a static grid reduced differential contrast effects from retinal slip. Although each saccade and subsequent pursuit produces motional stimulation, the shape of the static grid minimizes differential effects of this stimulation between angular conditions. The use of green-red color contrast minimized the effects of bottom-up color contrast in target discrimination. Further, human SC has recently been shown to adapt to red-green contrast (Chang et al., 2016), so our static red-green grid reduces cue-evoked visual stimulation during saccadic eye movement measurements.
Supplementary video related to this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.12.080
Participants initially held fixation at an upper corner of the display. The first saccade was initiated when an adjacent red dot turned green to indicate the saccade target. Once the saccade was made, participants had to discriminate between the outlines of two possible shapes (circle or square) presented within the target dot. This required visual attention to be engaged and saccades to be made more reliably. Participants responded via button press, which triggered the green dot to appear at the next target along the current axis. After three saccades, the participants then performed a smooth pursuit (1.2 s) back to the first dot. Saccades and the pursuit were continued along the same axis for 9.6 s, and then participants performed another smooth pursuit to the start of the next axis. The discrimination task had to be performed 0–2 random times (truncated Poisson distribution, λ = 1) during each 1.2-s smooth pursuit, encouraging attention to remain engaged and eye movements to be restrained to the pursuit path. Participants performed 9 cycles in a single run (~4.5 min) and ~15 runs per session. Leftward and rightward saccades were run on separate sessions to measure the contralateral response of each SC independently.
Retinotopic maps were also acquired for all 5 participants using our previous phase-encoded visual stimulation paradigm (Katyal et al., 2010). Participants fixated at the center of the display while a wedge (90° polar angle) of moving dots rotated around the entire polar angle to measure retinotopy for both superior colliculi in a single session. The rotating wedge consisted of 6 virtual sectors, and in one sector, the dots were moving either faster or slower than all other sectors. Participants performed a speed discrimination task every 2 s. The speed difference was adjusted using a staircase design to control task difficulty and engage attention, boosting signal levels for the retinotopy measurement.
Saccadic eye movements
Eye movement were measured with the SR EyeLink 1000 Plus (Scientific Research, Ontario, Canada) both outside of the scanner during training sessions and inside the scanner during fMRI acquisition. Inside the scanner, the infrared light and camera were placed beneath the LCD display and angled at the head-coil mounted mirror allowing us to track the participant’s right eye at ~130cm lens-to-eye distance. Raw (x, y) position coordinates were sampled at 1000 Hz. Saccade reports were generated using the EyeLink Data Viewer (Scientific Research, Ontario, Canada) and further analyzed in MATLAB. Saccades were detected using three minimum thresholds: position (>0.15°), velocity (>30°/s), and acceleration (>9500°/s2). Eye blinks were detected when the pupil diameter was too small (<1 mm), obstructed, or not tracked, and any saccades during blinks were discarded from analysis. Polar plots were created to visualize the saccades, where polar angle represented the direction of a saccade and eccentricity represented the amplitude of a saccade, which was also plotted in a histogram. Smooth pursuits and correction eye movements were further distinguished from saccades by restricting saccade analysis to eye movements towards the cued horizontal visual field (i.e., left or right) and eye movements with amplitudes greater than 1° visual angle.
MRI methods
Imaging was conducted on a Siemens (Erlangen, Germany) 3T Magnetom Trio scanner at the Core for Advanced Magnetic Resonance Imaging (CAMRI) at Baylor College of Medicine. Eight 1.2-mm-thick quasiaxial slices (170-mm field of view) covered the entire SC with the prescription oriented roughly perpendicular to the local neuraxis. Functional data were acquired using a 3-shot spiral (Glover, 1999; Glover and Lai, 1998) dual-echo (both outward) sequence (Li and Glover, 2006) that has been shown to boost functional SNR by >50% compared to single-echo methods (Singh et al., 2017). We used TR = 0.8 s for each shot, yielding a 2.4-s volume-acquisition time for our 3-shots. To obtain best contrast, TE was set to 25 ms for the first echo, and acquisition time for each echo was 34 ms, giving an effective echo time of 59 ms for the second echo. The dual echoes were combined as a signal-weighted average.
A set of T1-weighted structural images was obtained on the same prescription at the end of the session using a three-dimensional (3D) fast low-angle shot (FLASH) sequence (minimum TE and TR, 15° flip angle, 0.9-mm inplane pixel size) (Fig. 2B). These images were used to align the functional data to the segmented structural reference volume. A high-resolution (0.7-mm cubic voxels) T1-weighted structural volume anatomy was obtained for each participant in a separate session, using an MP-RAGE sequence (TR = 2600 ms, minimum TE, TI = 1100 ms, flip angle = 9°, 3 averages, 24-min acquisition time).
Data analysis
Preprocessing
Image analysis was conducted using the mrVista software package (http://web.stanford.edu/group/vista/cgi-bin/wiki/index.php/MrVista) and a host of custom modifications designed to enable high spatial resolution sub-cortical imaging. In particular, we incorporated our sub-cortical depth-mapping procedures (Khan et al., 2011), utilized an inverted form of tissue mesh definition (analogous to gray-matter mesh definition), and a corresponding inverted form of surface-mesh visualization useful for sub-cortical structures. Moreover, a framework was added to deal with the use of multi-echo fMRI data. The first 12 s of each retinotopy run were discarded to remove transient effects. Similarly, the first 19.2s of each eye movement run were discarded to remove transients and to allow participants to become accustomed to the eye-movement task. The following preprocessing was conducted on each of the two echoes for all runs (visual stimulation and eye movement), analogous to our previous pipeline (Katyal et al., 2010): slice-timing correction (zeroed to task onset) and intensity-based motion compensation using a robust algorithm (Nestares and Heeger, 2000). Motion compensation information was obtained only from the first echo because of its higher SNR; these data were then applied to both echoes. The two processed echoes were then combined as a signal-weighted average. Finally, baseline trend removal was performed using a high-pass filtering approach.
Surface analysis
For each participant, we collected a high-resolution (0.7-mm isovoxel) structural volume in separate runs. We used this structural volume to segment their brainstem (including portions of the thalamus) via a combination of automatic (e.g., active contour evolution) and manual approaches in ITK-SNAP (Yushkevich et al., 2006) (Fig. 2A). Two participants’ brains were initially segmented using the newer Bayesian approaches in FreeSurfer 6.0 (Iglesias et al., 2015), followed by manual editing. A surface model was built at the tissue-cerebrospinal fluid interface using a deformable surface algorithm (Xu et al., 2006). Functional data (Fig. 2B) were then spatially aligned and resampled to the high-resolution structural volume, averaged across runs, and visualized on the surface. Distance along the surface, dmesh, was estimated by summing Euclidean distances along the mesh from a reference point on each participant’s inter-collicular axis. A Euclidean nearest-neighbor distance map was also computed from SC tissue voxels to the vertices of the surface to give a measure of the depth (s, mm) of the tissue voxels (Khan et al., 2011; Ress et al., 2007). Time-series data were averaged over a particular range of depth values to improve signal-to-noise ratio (SNR).
Phase mapping
A sinusoid at the stimulus repetition frequency (24 s for visual stimulation data, 28.8 s for eye movement data) was fit to the depth-averaged data (0–1.6 mm for visual stimulation data, 0.6–1.8 mm for eye-movement data) using coherence analysis. These fits provide three parameters: amplitude, phase, and coherence. Amplitude measures the strength of the evoked activation, phase measures the time delay of the response, and coherence is equivalent to the Pearson’s correlation coefficient, quantifying the fraction of variance explained by the sinusoidal fit. The phase, specifically, is used to measure the relationship of the fMRI response to the current state of the stimulus (visual stimulus polar angle or saccade angle). Phase maps, expressed in units of time delay, were projected onto the brainstem surface to visualize the topography of visual stimulation and eye movements.
Region-of-interest (ROI) generation
We generated elliptical ROIs to demarcate the phasic progressions of saccade-evoked activity for quantitative analyses. Given that the task design utilized only a single amplitude saccade (6°) to drive the SC, we expected that phasic progressions representing saccadic activity would result only in narrow bands of activity representing the strongest signal emerging from the intermediate layers, unlike in our previous studies in which visual stimulation and attentional maps activated entire colliculi (Katyal et al., 2010). Further, the intermediate layers are more difficult to measure due to vascular noise sources and the decrease in SNR. As such, we developed an objective approach to delineate the most robust SC activity representing saccadic activity using a grid search initialized only by the putative angle in which the saccadic progression could be seen visually.
First, surface representations of the SC were flattened to form a 2D image (Wade et al., 2002; Wandell et al., 2000) (Fig. 2C). The phasic progression from medial to lateral was visually observed and then delineated with two vertices on the surface to define the start and stop of the putative eye movement maps; these two vertices were then transformed to the flat image. Next, many (~20,000) elliptical ROIs were generated upon the flattened image and each was transformed into the reference volume; note that this transformation creates a somewhat distorted version of the ellipse on the mesh (Fig. 2D). The many ellipses corresponded to gridded variations in five parameters: size (15–60% of one whole SC area); aspect ratio (3.5–7); x, y center coordinates (each varied ±2 pixels from the midline of the delineated phase progression); and the elliptical angle (±15° relative to the angle of the delineated phase progression). An exhaustive search was then conducted to maximize five criteria: (1) number of activated (with a moderate threshold of p < 0.2) voxels, νsig; (2) geometric average p-value, ; (3) phase range (defined by median absolute deviation), ϕrange = median(phi – median(ph)); (4) correlation (variance explained) of the linear fit between the mesh distance dmesh and the phasic progression ; and (5) the reciprocal deviation of the same linear fit and the expected progression across the collicular width, δ = 1/abs(ellipseslope – targetslope). The five optimization criteria were multiplied together to yield an overall metric, , for each ROI. The top four values of opt identified the best ROIs, which were examined visually for quality confirmation. The top fitting ROI was used for subsequent analyses.
Laminar profile analysis
We then examined the amplitude of the complex response as a function of laminar depth within the elliptical SC ROIs, similar to our previous approaches (Katyal et al., 2010, 2012; Katyal and Ress, 2014), using surface-based methods (Khan et al., 2011). Complex amplitude data were first averaged together across all runs for each participant. A phase normalization was performed to correct for the variable hemodynamic time delay across the ROI. Cylindrical volume elements (1.2-mm diameter) are formed normal to the surface mesh; these elements provide transverse averaging of the data that improves functional SNR. Within these elements, we obtained the amplitude-weighted mean phase. Complex amplitudes within each element were then projected upon this phase. After this projection across all elements within the ROI, a boxcar-smoothing kernel (1.2 mm width in bin steps of 0.1 mm) was convolved with the projected amplitude data as a function of depth to obtain the laminar profile. The laminar profiles for both eye movement and visual stimulation experiments were normalized by their peak amplitudes to enable comparisons and averaging across participants.
We used bootstrapping to obtain confidence intervals on the laminar amplitude profiles in each participant and all participants combined, for both visual stimulation and eye movement experiments. For each ROI, we calculated the complex amplitudes for each run. We then resampled across runs with replacement over 5000 iterations, and calculated the laminar profile anew for each resampled average. The 68% confidence intervals, equivalent to the standard-error-of-the-mean for normally distributed data, are shown by shading on laminar profile plots.
Depth values for the peaks of the laminar profiles were calculated to quantify comparisons of depth between the attention and stimulation conditions. The peak depths were also bootstrapped across the ensemble of runs to obtain confidence intervals and p-values for differences between peak depth values for visual stimulation and eye-movement maps.
Retinotopy-eye movement correlation
Within each optimal elliptical ROI for all participants, we measured the registration between phase maps for saccadic eye movements and visual stimulation. The raw eye-movement maps spanned the entire cycle (2π radians), and thus were converted to visual field coordinates in degrees. The phase progressions within the ROI were first scaled onto a range of 90°, then these data were centered around 0° for rightward saccades, and 180° for leftward saccades. Visual stimulation phase data were corrected by estimating the hemodynamic delay as the mean phase observed in significant (p < .05) voxels of each colliculus within the elliptical ROI, and this was then subtracted from the phase data. This procedure centers the data at 0° (right horizontal), so we add 180° for the left visual field (right colliculus).
We again used bootstrapping to obtain confidence intervals on the correlations for each participant and all participants combined. For each session, we calculated a run-by-run ensemble of depth-averaged complex amplitude datasets. We then performed our correlation analysis with the retinotopy data for 5000 averages of the attention-condition runs, each average obtained by resampling the ensemble with replacement. The p-values corresponded to the fraction of the correlations yielding a fit with slope >0.
Results
Eye movements
Before each saccade-mapping session, we trained all participants on 2–3 runs outside of the scanner and quantified the reliability of their eye movements (Fig. 3A). Saccades were detected and visualized on polar plots to show the eccentricity and polar angle of each saccade. Saccades were color coded to represent their cycle timing along the three polar angles of the task. We were also able to obtain reliable eye tracking in the scanner from two participants on both rightward and leftward eye-movement sessions (Fig. 3B). Histograms indicated eye movements were slightly hypermetric to the cued amplitude (6.29 ± 1.95°, shown in gray) in the cued direction (angular error: 0.82°± 10.01°). Long-tailed distributions in the opposite direction (white) were also observed. The task periods designed to evoke smooth pursuits often contained saccades of variable amplitude, but most were less than 3° (median: 0.8°, mean: 3.07°±3.19°). Small correction saccades also contribute to the opposite-direction saccades, as saccades to targets were often hypermetric, followed by a small saccade in the reverse direction. All participants performed reliably in both the discrimination task during the saccades (mean accuracy: 82.8 ± 8.6% across all sessions and participants) and the speed discrimination task during visual stimulation (mean accuracy: 70.3 ± 5.1%); see Fig. S2 for quantification of behavioral task performance.
The quality of the eye-tracking measurements appears much worse inside the scanner because of several issues. First, the eye-tracker inside the scanner must use longer-range optics (~1 meter) than outside the scanner (0.2 meter). This forces use of smaller numerical apertures, reducing light-collection efficiency and increasing noise levels and sensitivity to aberrations. Second, images of the eyes must be obtained through the eye-holes in the head coil, which further complicates the optics, and adds nuisance IR reflections that can confuse the analysis routines. Third, even when eye-tracking is initially well-calibrated inside the scanner, the quality of the eye-tracking often degrades over the course of a run as the participant’s head sinks into the foam padding, as the head vibrates from operation of the gradients, and as the head and body move from run to run. This variability can be reduced by performing re-calibration measurements between runs, but we elected not to perform these recalibrations as the scanning sessions were already long (~2 h). Finally, the long sessions within the confines of the scanner were far more demanding on participant eye-movement performance, so it is not surprising that their variability becomes larger.
Polar angle maps of saccadic eye movements
Saccades along the superior-inferior visual field were mapped along the medial-lateral axis of the SC in all 5 participants (Fig. 4). Our eye-movement task involved making 6° saccades toward one hemifield, with smooth pursuit in the opposite direction. Accordingly, we see organized phase progressions only in one contralateral SC at a time. We generated ROIs by using an objective optimization approach to demarcate the area of phase progression near the crown of the colliculus likely to represent the saccadic eye-movement topography. Within these ROIs, we found significant correlations of saccadic eye-movement phase with distance along the medial-lateral axis of the SC at the group level (slope: 0.451, r = 0.734, p ~ .0002, Fig. 5). Significant correlations (p < .05) were also observed in 7 out of 10 individual SCs (Fig. S3). Strong activity was also evident in many other portions of both contralateral and ipsi-lateral colliculi. In particular, several ipsilateral colliculi exhibit reversed phase progressions (S1L, S2L, S4L, S1R, S2R, S3R, and S4R) that were more rostral than the contralateral progressions. This rostral pole activation could represent activity from the smooth pursuits, but in our experience, the rostral pole is often contaminated with vascular nuisance activations, so these activations will not be further discussed. Also, participant saccades were not perfectly controlled, and reverse eye movements occurred in two ways. First, during the latter portions of the pursuit periods participants occasionally would predictively saccade to the endpoint of the pursuit (see Movie M1). Second, smaller amplitude opposite-direction correction saccades frequently occurred after hypermetric forward saccades. The erratic nature of these poorly controlled eye movements likely contributed to the poorly organized activation on the ipsilateral colliculus.
Weak but significant activation was also observed in early visual cortical areas V1—3 (Fig. S4). However, this activity had a biphasic rather than triphasic character. These results suggest a small amount of activation from unbalanced visual motion between horizontal and off-horizontal eye movement phases (see Discussion).
Visual stimulation (with an attentional task) produced clear, largely medial-lateral phase progressions corresponding to a representation of polar angle in the superficial layers of SC (Fig. 4, center column). The stimulation-evoked phase progressions are qualitatively consistent with the progressions evoked by the saccade tasks in each elliptical ROI within each SC. In most participants, we also observed a small rostral-caudal (superior-inferior) tilt of the saccadic eye-movement phase progressions in the lateral-to-medial phase progression. This tilt also corresponded to the tilt in the saccade data. For the saccade task, no consistent phase progressions were elicited in early visual cortex (Fig. S4), indicating that the phase progressions were not associated with visual stimulation alone.
Correlations between eye-movement and visual stimulation topographies
Within the elliptical ROIs for each participant, we observed the saccadic eye movement phases of the three angles of movement to be in alignment with the visual stimulation retinotopy for both left (slope: 0.733, r = 0.312, p = .0052) and right (slope: 0.928, r = 0.567, p = .0034) SCs (Fig. 6). Quantitative correlations were significant for five out of ten SCs in individual participants (Fig. S5).
Depth profiles
Laminar depth profiles extended deeper into SC for activity evoked by saccadic eye movements compared to visual stimulation (Fig. 7). The peaks are significantly shifted deeper for eye movement maps compared to visual stimulation maps for both left (shift Δ = 0.9 mm, p = .0012) and right (Δ = 0.7 mm, p = .0002) SC. At the individual level, data were significant in four out of 10 SC (Fig. S6).
Discussion
We used high-resolution functional MRI to map the polar angle representation of saccadic eye movements within human SC. We found saccades along the superior-inferior visual axis were mapped roughly medial-to-lateral on the anatomy of the SC. Eliciting these maps required a novel paradigm in which participants were cued to make saccades along one of three specified direction axes, and then a smooth pursuit back to the start of the axis. Doing so isolated forward saccades from reverse saccades, while still allowing participants to make many saccades along a controlled set of directions in an experimental session. In addition, we utilized a fixed and symmetric grid of saccade targets to minimize the effects of motion and contrast evoked by retinal slip.
This experimental design was not immediately obvious to us but evolved over time and was only discovered after many failed paradigms (Fig. S1). We found, for example that it was necessary to avoid cascading forward and reverse saccades. If participants are instructed to make a saccade in the opposite direction of the target, these reverse saccades may be similar to an anti-saccade or an eye-movement away from the cued target. Recent fMRI studies in humans have demonstrated markedly decreased activation in SC for luminance driven anti-saccades, with inhibitory signal likely coming from the intraparietal sulcus (Anderson et al., 2008). Also, performing anti-saccades may involve the release of inhibitory control exerted by frontal regions like the dorsal lateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) onto the colliculus (Condy et al., 2004), which may further confound SC topography measurements. However, ultimately the gaze must be shifted back to the origin. This could be accomplished by a smooth pursuit back to the origin rather than a saccade. Although monkey electrophysiology has shown that rostral SC neurons have activity in the involvement of smooth pursuits (Basso et al., 2000; Krauzlis et al., 1997), such activity appears similar to that evoked by small amplitude saccades, which should be spatially segregated from the 6° saccades guided by our experimental cues. Further, the vascular noise sources already contaminate measurements around the rostral pole with fMRI measurements. Thus, by using many repeated large amplitude saccades in one direction with a minimal number of smooth pursuits for the return to origin, the SC activity was better associated with the saccades.
We found the representation of saccades in human SC to be in good alignment with the overlying retinotopic topography, consistent with the topography of monkey SC (Schiller and Stryker, 1972; Wurtz and Goldberg, 1972; Basso and May 2017). Further, our previous studies showed that superficial attentional maps in human SC are also in alignment with retinotopic maps (Katyal et al., 2010), revealing that attention, retinotopy, and saccadic eye-movement maps all utilize a similar topography.
The depth profiles show that the saccadic eye movement maps lie 0.7–0.9 mm deeper than visual stimulation maps on both SC. This result is consistent with laminar organization inferred from human SC cytoarchitecture (Qu et al., 2006), from human SC response to visual stimulation (Zhang et al., 2015), and from laminar topography across several species including: guinea pig, cat, galago, macaque, and gray squirrel (May 2006). Profiles showed significant depth separations for only 4/10 colliculi individually, but very strong differences were seen on aggregate. This reflects the noisy and blurry character of our hemodynamic correlates of neural function; many sessions were also needed to resolve differences between the laminar profiles of attention and visual stimulation (Katyal et al., 2010).
The confirmation that saccadic eye movement activity is mediated by the intermediate layers of human SC emphasizes the novelty of our earlier discovery that endogenous attention activity is specifically associated with similar intermediate collicular depths (Katyal and Ress, 2014). Specifically, we found that the activation evoked by attention to a very low contrast stimulus evoked activity that peaked 0.5 mm deeper in SC than the boost in activation evoked by attention to a high-contrast stimulus (Fig. 8). Here, we find that oculomotor activation was deeper yet, ~0.8 mm deeper than that evoked by pure visual stimulation combined with attention. This new result generally supports oculomotor theories of visual spatial attention (Corbetta et al., 1998; Katyal and Ress, 2014; Moore et al., 2003; Rizzolatti et al., 1987) with an emphasis on the relationship between oculomotor substrates and covert spatial attention in the near-absence of sensory input. Our results suggest that deeper layers of the SC might mediate both overt (or exogenous) and covert (or endogenous) orientation of attention. In the presence of a strong visual stimulus, such signals may be projected to the superficial layers to augment the response of visual neurons. During weak visual stimulation, these signals become directly evident in the intermediate layers. Previous results in monkeys support similar involvement of intermediate layer neurons responsive to both visual inputs and eye-movements in covert attention (Hafed et al., 2013, 2011; Ignashchenkova et al., 2004; Krauzlis et al., 2013).
Our experimental stimulus still evoked some activity beyond the contralateral medial-lateral phase progression. In 7 out of 10 colliculi, weaker but significant reverse phase progressions appeared on the ipsi-lateral SC. There are at least three possible reasons for this ipsilaterally evoked activity. First, this activity could be driven by small saccades during the smooth pursuits, particularly as the pursuit approaches the target end-point, and the participant makes a predictive saccade ahead of the pursuit. Second, the ipsilateral activity may be evoked by the observed small correction saccades, as the participant often saccades past the target and corrects by performing one-or-more saccades in the opposite direction to reach fixation upon the target. Third, it may be that the prior targets are still remembered during the smooth pursuit; activity in monkey SC of both remembered and visually-guided saccade targets has been reported during such smooth pursuits (Dash et al., 2016).
We utilized a fMRI acquisition sequence that pushed the boundaries of 3T subcortical imaging. Our dual-echo spiral sequence (Singh et al., 2017) gave us the higher CNR needed to resolve the intermediate layers of SC. Each session included nearly 2 h of scanning while the participant performed 4000–5000 saccades – a demanding but feasible physical effort. Our scanning methodology should allow further experimentation of visual-motor and multisensory function of the SC. For example, future work could study the topography of auditory signal representations and multisensory integration in the deeper layers of SC, and test the possibility of ocular segregation in the superficial layers. Our experimental paradigm also offers a means to probe the topography of eye movements within cortical regions, such as the frontal eye fields.
There are some limitations to our approach and results. First, saccadic eye movements were not fully controlled. Participants automatically tended to make undesired saccades because of error-correction or prediction. These tendencies broadened the distribution of saccades, and probably generated weaker activation upon the opposite colliculus. Second, visual contrast was not fully balanced as a function of polar angle, resulting in weak activation in early visual areas (Fig. S4). This weak activation probably reflects the small asymmetry of our fixed cue display. Ideally, this display should have been round, but the display slightly clipped along the top and bottom (Fig. 1). Therefore, off-horizontal eye movements would create less visual motion drive than on-horizontal movements. This choice was forced by the 16:9 aspect ratio of our MRI-compatible LCD display, and the need to maximize display visual angle to permit sufficient duty cycle. The biphasic activation may have slightly affected our results, but we believe that their effects were comparatively weak given the quality of our observed correlations. Third, we did observe some variability in the cued eye movements. However, we were only able to obtain reliable, consistent eye-tracking inside the scanner in two participants. The experimental sessions were long (~2 h) and intentionally conducted under minimal light to avoid visual contamination. This forced frequent eye-tracking recalibrations and constricted pupils that did not yield viable eye-tracking in 3 out of the 5 participants. With reliable eye tracking for all participants in the scanner, we could have correlated saccade accuracy and latency with strength of the coherence analyses to measure the effect of task performance on our observed saccadic eye movement maps. Nonetheless, we trained all participants outside of the scanner with high-quality eye-tracking until they achieved good performance in saccade accuracy and latency. Fourth, we did introduce button-pressing and a discrimination task to the experimental paradigm, which presents some concern because the SC is also has a sensorimotor mapping, as previous human fMRI studies have elicited reach-related responses (Himmelbach et al., 2013; Linzenbold and Himmelbach, 2012). However, the button presses occurred at a high frequency (0.7-s interval) throughout all phases of the task, which is unlikely to present a concern given our comparatively coarse (2.4-s) temporal sample together with our phase-encoding analysis approach. Further, the objects for discrimination (circle or square) were randomized from trial-to-trial, making any differential patterns evoked by this task highly unlikely to emerge in our phase-mapping analysis.
Conclusions
In summary, our techniques allowed us to reliably measure the functional topography of saccadic eye movements on the human SC. This required using high-resolution functional MRI to reliably resolve a laminar nucleus of midbrain, as well as a novel experimental paradigm that allowed participants to make many saccades in one principle direction while minimizing visual stimulation confounds. The topography of the vertical eye movement maps was arranged medial-laterally, in register with the retinotopic representation of visual stimulation, and emerged from deeper layers in SC, similar to the organization observed in monkey.
The new results extend a range of recent measurements in human SC that link together visual stimulation, attention, and eye movements (Fig. 8). Previous work has demonstrated the retinotopic organization of visual stimulation in superficial SC (DeSimone et al., 2015; Katyal et al., 2010; Schneider and Kastner, 2005), as well as detailed registration between stimulation and visual attention (Katyal et al., 2010). More interesting, we learned that attention to a high-contrast stimulus (putative exogenous or overt attention) evokes a very superficial boost in activity, whereas attention to a weak, low-contrast stimulus (putative endogenous or covert attention) evokes deeper activity consistent with SC intermediate layer laminae (Katyal and Ress, 2014). Here, we confirm the depth registration of this activity with the neural substrates of saccadic eye movements, adding additional evidence to the hypothesis that covert attention to weak stimuli have a particularly close connection to the substrates of oculomotor activity.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgements
This work was supported by grants from the NSF (1446377 to D.R.). We would like to thank all members of the CAMRI team at Baylor College of Medicine for housing and facilitating our experiments. Thanks also to Mike Beauchamp and Scott Stephenson for useful feedback on this project.
Footnotes
Conflicts of interest
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
Appendix A. Supplementary data
Supplementary data related to this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.12.080.
References
- Adamuk E, 1872. Uber angeborene und erworbene Association von F. C. Donders. Albrecht Von Graefes Arch. Furklinische Exp 18, 153–164. 10.1007/BF02262657. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Anderson EJ, Husain M, Sumner P, 2008. Human intraparietal sulcus (IPS) and competition between exogenous and endogenous saccade plans. NeuroImage 40, 838–851. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.10.046. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Apter JT, 1946. Eye movements following strychninization of the superior colliculus of cats. J. Neurophysiol. 9, 73–86. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Basso MA, Krauzlis RJ, Wurtz RH, 2000. Activation and inactivation of rostral superior colliculus neurons during smooth-pursuit eye movements in monkeys. J. Neurophysiol 84, 892–908. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Basso MA, May PJ, 2017. Circuits for action and cognition: a view from the superior colliculus. Annu. Rev. Vis. Sci 10.1146/annurev-vision-102016-061234. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Brainard DH, 1997. The psychophysics toolbox. Spatial Vis. 10, 433–436. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chang DHF, Hess RF, Mullen KT, 2016. Color responses and their adaptation in human superior colliculus and lateral geniculate nucleus. NeuroImage. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.04.067. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Condy C, Rivaud-Péchoux S, Ostendorf F, Ploner CJ, Gaymard B, 2004. Neural substrate of antisaccades: role of subcortical structures. Neurology 63, 1571–1578. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Connolly JD, Vuong QC, Thiele A, 2015. Gaze-dependent topography in human posterior parietal cortex. Cereb. Cortex N. Y. N 1991 (25), 1519–1526. 10.1093/cercor/bht344. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Corbetta M, Akbudak E, Conturo TE, Snyder AZ, Ollinger JM, Drury HA, Linenweber MR, Petersen SE, Raichle ME, Van Essen DC, Shulman GL, 1998. A common network of functional areas for attention and eye movements. Neuron 21, 761–773. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cynader M, Berman N, 1972. Receptive-field organization of monkey superior colliculus. J. Neurophysiol 35, 187–201. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dash S, Nazari SA, Yan X, Wang H, Crawford JD, 2016. Superior colliculus responses to attended, unattended, and remembered saccade targets during smooth pursuit eye movements. Front. Syst. Neurosci 10 (34). 10.3389/fnsys.2016.00034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Weijer AD, Mandl RCW, Sommer IEC, Vink M, Kahn RS, Neggers SFW, 2010. Human fronto-tectal and fronto-striatal-tectal pathways activate differently during anti-saccades. Front. Hum. Neurosci 4 (41). 10.3389/fnhum.2010.00041. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- DeSimone K, Viviano JD, Schneider KA, 2015. Population receptive field estimation reveals new retinotopic maps in human subcortex. J. Neurosci. Off. J. Soc. Neurosci 35, 9836–9847. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3840-14.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Donders FC, 1872. Ueber angeborene und erworbene Association. Albrecht Von Graefes Arch. Für Ophthalmol. 18, 153–164. 10.1007/BF02262657. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Feldon P, Kruger L, 1970. Topography of the retinal projection upon the superior colliculus of the cat. Vis. Res 10, 135–143. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fuchs AF, Kaneko CR, Scudder CA, 1985. Brainstem control of saccadic eye movements. Annu. Rev. Neurosci 8, 307–337. 10.1146/annurev.ne.08.030185.001515. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Furlan M, Smith AT, Walker R, 2015. Activity in the human superior colliculus relating to endogenous saccade preparation and execution. J. Neurophysiol 114, 1048–1058. 10.1152/jn.00825.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Glover GH, 1999. Simple analytic spiral K-space algorithm. Magn. Reson. Med 42, 412–415. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Glover GH, Lai S, 1998. Self-navigated spiral fMRI: interleaved versus single-shot. Magn. Reson. Med 39, 361–368. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hafed ZM, Chen C-Y, 2016. Sharper, stronger, faster upper visual field representation in primate superior colliculus. Curr. Biol. CB 26, 1647–1658. 10.1016/j.cub.2016.04.059. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hafed ZM, Lovejoy LP, Krauzlis RJ, 2013. Superior colliculus inactivation alters the relationship between covert visual attention and microsaccades. Eur. J. Neurosci 37, 1169–1181. 10.1111/ejn.12127. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hafed ZM, Lovejoy LP, Krauzlis RJ, 2011. Modulation of microsaccades in monkey during a covert visual attention task. J. Neurosci. Off. J. Soc. Neurosci 31, 15219–15230. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3106-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Himmelbach M, Linzenbold W, Ilg UJ, 2013. Dissociation of reach-related and visual signals in the human superior colliculus. NeuroImage 82, 61–67. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Iglesias JE, Van Leemput K, Bhatt P, Casillas C, Dutt S, Schuff N, Truran-Sacrey D, Boxer A, Fischl B, Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative, 2015. Bayesian segmentation of brainstem structures in MRI. NeuroImage 113, 184–195. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.02.065. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ignashchenkova A, Dicke PW, Haarmeier T, Thier P, 2004. Neuron-specific contribution of the superior colliculus to overt and covert shifts of attention. Nat. Neurosci 7, 56–64. 10.1038/nn1169. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Katyal S, Greene CA, Ress D, 2012. High-resolution functional magnetic resonance imaging methods for human midbrain. J. Vis. Exp. JoVE, e3746 10.3791/3746. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Katyal S, Ress D, 2014. Endogenous attention signals evoked by threshold contrast detection in human superior colliculus. J. Neurosci. Off. J. Soc. Neurosci 34, 892–900. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3026-13.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Katyal S, Zughni S, Greene C, Ress D, 2010. Topography of covert visual attention in human superior colliculus. J. Neurophysiol 104, 3074–3083. 10.1152/jn.00283.2010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Khan R, Zhang Q, Darayan S, Dhandapani S, Katyal S, Greene C, Bajaj C, Ress D, 2011. Surface-based analysis methods for high-resolution functional magnetic resonance imaging. Graph. Models, Computational Modeling in Imaging Sciences 73, 313–322. 10.1016/j.gmod.2010.11.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Konen CS, Kastner S, 2008. Representation of eye movements and stimulus motion in topographically organized areas of human posterior parietal cortex. J. Neurosci. Off. J. Soc. Neurosci 28, 8361–8375. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1930-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krauzlis RJ, Basso MA, Wurtz RH, 1997. Shared motor error for multiple eye movements. Science 276, 1693–1695. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krauzlis RJ, Lovejoy LP, Zeenon A, 2013. Superior colliculus and visual spatial attention. Annu. Rev. Neurosci 36, 165–182. 10.1146/annurev-neuro-062012-170249. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krebs RM, Woldorff MG, Tempelmann C, Bodammer N, Noesselt T, Boehler CN, Scheich H, Hopf J-M, Duzel E, Heinze H-J, Schoenfeld MA, 2010. High-field FMRI reveals brain activation patterns underlying saccade execution in the human superior colliculus. PLoS One 5, e8691 10.1371/journal.pone.000869. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li T-Q, Takahashi A, Wang Y, Mathews V, Glover GH, 2006. Dual-echo spiral in/in acquisition method for reducing magnetic susceptibility artifacts in blood-oxygen-level-dependent functional magnetic resonance imaging. Magn. Reson. Med 55, 325–334. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Linzenbold W, Himmelbach M, 2012. Signals from the deep: reach-related activity in the human superior colliculus. J. Neurosci. Off. J. Soc. Neurosci 32, 13881–13888. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0619-12.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- May PJ, 2006. The mammalian superior colliculus: laminar structure and connections. Prog. Brain Res 151, 321–378. 10.1016/S0079-6123(05)51011-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meredith MA, Stein BE, 1990. The visuotopic component of the multisensory map in the deep laminae of the cat superior colliculus. J. Neurosci 10, 3727–3742. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mohler CW, Wurtz RH, 1976. Organization of monkey superior colliculus: intermediate layer cells discharging before eye movements. J. Neurophysiol 39, 722–744. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moore T, Armstrong KM, Fallah M, 2003. Visuomotor origins of covert spatial attention. Neuron 40, 671–683. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nestares O, Heeger DJ, 2000. Robust multiresolution alignment of MRI brain volumes. Magn. Reson. Med 43, 705–715. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Qu J, Zhou X, Zhu H, Cheng G, Ashwell KWS, Lu F, 2006. Development of the human superior colliculus and the retinocollicular projection. Exp. Eye Res 82, 300–310. 10.1016/j.exer.2005.07.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ress D, Glover GH, Liu J, Wandell B, 2007. Laminar profiles of functional activity in the human brain. NeuroImage 34, 74–84. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.08.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rizzolatti G, Riggio L, Dascola I, Umilta C, 1987. Reorienting attention across the horizontal and vertical meridians: evidence in favor of a premotor theory of attention. Neuropsychologia 25, 31–40. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robinson DA, 1972. Eye movements evoked by collicular stimulation in the alert monkey. Vis. Res 12, 1795–1808. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schiller PH, Stryker M, 1972. Single-unit recording and stimulation in superior colliculus of the alert rhesus monkey. J. Neurophysiol 35, 915–924. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schluppeck D, Glimcher P, Heeger DJ, 2005. Topographic organization for delayed saccades in human posterior parietal cortex. J. Neurophysiol 94, 1372–1384. 10.1152/jn.01290.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schneider KA, Kastner S, 2009. Effects of sustained spatial attention in the human lateral geniculate nucleus and superior colliculus. J. Neurosci. Off. J. Soc. Neurosci 29, 1784–1795. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4452-08.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schneider KA, Kastner S, 2005. Visual responses of the human superior colliculus: a high-resolution functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J. Neurophysiol 94, 2491–2503. 10.1152/jn.00288.2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sereno MI, Pitzalis S, Martinez A, 2001. Mapping of contralateral space in retinotopic coordinates by a parietal cortical area in humans. Science 294, 1350–1354. 10.1126/science.1063695. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Singh V, Pfeuffer J, Zhao T, Ress D, 2017. Evaluation of spiral acquisition variants for functional imaging of human superior colliculus at 3T field strength. Magn. Reson. Med 10.1002/mrm.26845. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sparks DL, Hartwich-Young R, 1989. The deep layers of the superior colliculus. Rev. Oculomot. Res 3, 213–255. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sparks DL, Holland R, Guthrie BL, 1976. Size and distribution of movement fields in the monkey superior colliculus. Brain Res. 113, 21–34. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sparks DL, Jay MF, 1986. The functional organization of the primate superior colliculus: a motor perspective. Prog. Brain Res 64, 235–241. 10.1016/S0079-6123(08)63418-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sprague JM, Meikle TH, 1965. The role of the superior colliculus in visually guided behavior. Exp. Neurol 11, 115–146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wade A, Brewer A, Rieger J, Wandell B, 2002. Functional measurements of human ventral occipital cortex: retinotopy and colour. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B. Biol. Sci 357, 963–973. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wandell BA, Chial S, Backus BT, 2000. Visualization and measurement of the cortical surface. J. Cogn. Neurosci 12, 739–752. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wurtz RH, Albano JE, 1980. Visual-motor function of the primate superior colliculus. Annu. Rev. Neurosci 3, 189–226. 10.1146/annurev.ne.03.030180.001201. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wurtz RH, Goldberg ME, 1972. Activity of superior colliculus in behaving monkey. IV. Effects of lesions on eye movements. J. Neurophysiol 35, 587–596. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Xu G, Pan Q, Bajaj CL, 2006. Discrete surface modelling using partial differential equations. Comput. Aided Geom. Des 23, 125–145. 10.1016/j.cagd.2005.05.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yushkevich PA, Piven J, Hazlett HC, Smith RG, Ho S, Gee JC, Gerig G, 2006. User-guided 3D active contour segmentation of anatomical structures: significantly improved efficiency and reliability. NeuroImage 31, 1116–1128. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhang P, Zhou H, Wen W, He S, 2015. Layer-specific response properties of the human lateral geniculate nucleus and superior colliculus. NeuroImage 111, 159–166. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.02.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.