Skip to main content
Wiley Open Access Collection logoLink to Wiley Open Access Collection
. 2019 Feb 6;63(6):498–506. doi: 10.1111/jir.12597

Exploring the cognitive phenotype of Kabuki (Niikawa–Kuroki) syndrome

L C M van Dongen 1,2,3,, P A M Wingbermühle 1,3,6, W M van der Veld 4, C Stumpel 5, T Kleefstra 2,3,, J I M Egger 1,3,6,
PMCID: PMC6850277  PMID: 30724417

Abstract

Background

Kabuki syndrome (KS) is a Mendelian disorder, characterised by short stature, facial dysmorphisms and developmental delay and/or intellectual disability. Clarification of the neurocognitive profile in KS may provide directions for education and treatment interventions for KS. Previous studies on cognitive functioning in KS are scarce and have mainly focused on the general level of intelligence. The few more extensive studies suggested weaknesses in language skills, visuoconstruction, perceptual reasoning and speed of information processing. Other relevant domains such as memory, executive functioning and social cognition have not been studied yet.

Method

This is the first study in which cognitive functioning within multiple domains is systematically explored in 29 participants with KS (age range: 5–48 years) and compared to both norm groups (healthy population) and an appropriate control group of 15 individuals with other genetic syndromes (age range: 6–28 years).

Results

Compared to the norm groups of the cognitive test manuals, as expected, participants with KS show a weaker performance on all cognitive tests. Comparison with the more appropriate genetic control group indicates weaknesses in visuoconstruction and visual memory and no weaknesses in planning, cognitive flexibility or social cognition. Verbal memory seems to be a relative strength.

Conclusions

Individuals with KS suffer from specific weaknesses in visuoconstruction, in addition to their intellectual disability/developmental delay. These impairments in visuoconstruction plausibly result from problems in visual perceptual processing, which highlight the importance of the use of auditory cues instead of visual cues in targeted educational support and psychosocial interventions.

Keywords: case–control study, cognition, contextual neuropsychology, Kabuki syndrome, KMT2D, neurodevelopmental disorder

Introduction

The neurodevelopmental Mendelian disorder Kabuki syndrome (KS; OMIM #147920) was first described in 1981 by two independent research groups (Kuroki et al. 1981; Niikawa et al. 1981). It is caused by heterozygous mutations in the lysine‐specific methyltransferase 2D (KMT2D) gene (OMIM #602113) (Ng et al. 2010) or the lysine‐specific demethylase 6A (KDM6A) gene (OMIM #300867) (Lederer et al. 2012; Miyake et al. 2013). Although the current prevalence is unknown, an earlier study indicated a prevalence of 1 in 32 000 new‐born infants in Japan (Niikawa et al. 1988). KS is characterised by developmental delay and/or intellectual disability (ID), short stature and typical craniofacial features such as long palpebral fissures with an eversion of the lower eyelid, long dense eyelashes and arched eyebrows. People with KS frequently have microcephaly, congenital heart defects, urinary tract anomalies, skeletal abnormality, recurrent upper airway infections, fetal finger pads and abnormality in the dermatoglyphic pattern (Philip et al. 1992; Schrander‐Stumpel et al. 1994; Adam and Hudgins 2005; Bögershausen & Wollnik 2013). Furthermore, the syndrome is associated with hearing and vision impairments. Hearing difficulties are probably a result of the frequently occurring middle ear infections in KS. Although severe visual impairments are rare, 38–61% of the individuals with KS display ocular abnormalities such as strabismus, blue sclera, ptosis, coloboma of the iris and retina and refractive anomalies (Bögershausen & Wollnik 2013).

Multiple studies describe both structural and functional anomalies of the brain in KS. Brain aberrations are generally non‐specific and are reported in approximately 40% of the individuals. These comprise mostly agenesis/hypoplasia of the corpus callosum, but hydrocephalus, cerebellar anomalies, cortical and central hypoplasia, ventricular dilatation, generalised brain atrophy, and white matter abnormalities have been reported as well (Ben‐Omran & Teebi 2005; Li et al. 2011). As for impairments in brain functioning, epileptic seizures have been described in 5–36% of the individuals, mostly characterised as focal, but bilateral convulsive and myoclonic seizures have also been reported (Li et al. 2011; Kurahashi et al. 2017; Lehman et al. 2017). Interictal electroencephalography has most frequently revealed focal paroxysmal epileptiform discharges in regions including fronto‐central, parietal, fronto‐temporal and temporo‐occipital areas. Mixed results were found for the effects of treatment with antiepileptic drugs: the majority of people were not immediately responsive to medication in one study, whereas one other study reported efficient pharmacotherapy in 80% of the individuals (Oksanen et al. 2004; Lodi et al. 2010; Verrotti et al. 2011; Kurahashi et al. 2017).

The level of intellectual functioning in KS ranges from severe ID to normal levels of intelligence, with the majority of the reported individuals displaying a mild to moderate ID (TIQ range: 35–69; Bögershausen & Wollnik 2013, Lehman et al. 2017). Knowledge of the specific strengths and weaknesses in cognitive functioning in individuals with KS, additional to the level of intelligence, is important for the development of interventions in KS. Targeted educational support for instance, may be helpful for people with KS. Understanding the cognitive profile in KS is essential in order to implement this type of support in daily practice. Although most studies of KS include descriptions of developmental delay or ID, there is a lack of studies that focus on cognitive functioning in more detail. The few studies that did include measurements of cognitive functioning only focused on language abilities (Morgan et al. 2015) and visuoconstruction (Caciolo et al. 2018), indeed displaying specific weaknesses in these domains, or included a global cognitive screening based on a battery of standardised intelligence tasks (Mervis et al. 2005; Lehman et al. 2017). Index scores for verbal comprehension and working memory were found to be higher in children with KS in the cognitive screening study than those for perceptual reasoning and processing speed (Lehman et al. 2017). Although this study provides hypotheses regarding weaknesses in perceptual processing, they should first be validated with traditional measures of the domains of cognitive functioning.

None of the aforementioned cited studies included a suitable control group to establish possible strengths and weaknesses in individuals with KS. In these studies, performances of individuals with KS were either compared to performances of other people with KS (e.g. the comparison of a group of individuals with a genetically confirmed diagnosis to a group with a clinical suspicion of KS but without the molecular confirmation) or compared to a norm group of individuals with average levels of intelligence (the majority of individuals in norm groups representing ‘the healthy population’). However, a comparison of individuals with KS using controls with a similar level of intelligence is essential for adequate interpretation of neuropsychological data, as individuals with ID will likely deviate on all domains of cognitive functioning compared to controls with average intelligence levels (e.g. Lezak et al. 2012; Fletcher et al. 2017). Specific cognitive weaknesses or potential strengths related to KS will otherwise remain unnoticed.

Thus, previous studies regarding cognitive functioning in KS did not include an appropriate control group nor included important domains of neurocognitive functioning such as memory and executive functioning. Therefore, the present study examines multiple cognitive domains in a substantial group of participants with KS (n = 28) in order to clarify the neurocognitive profile of KS. Performances on measures of intelligence, adaptive functioning, memory, executive functioning, social cognition and visuoconstruction in participants with KS will be compared to both the norm group represented in the manuals of the cognitive tests and to the performance of an appropriate control group. The latter consists of a mixed group of individuals with other genetic syndromes that are accompanied by ID/developmental delay.

Method

Participants

This study included 28 participants with KS (KS group; 15 male participants; age range: 5–48 years) and 15 control participants with another genetic disorder (GC group; seven male participants; age range 6–28 years). Participants with KS were recruited for the study by clinical geneticists at the Dutch expertise centre for KS (Maastricht University Medical Centre, The Netherlands) and with help from the Kabuki caretakers network group in the Netherlands. Table 1 presents the medical characteristics (ocular problems, hearing problems as well as brain functioning and psychopharmacotherapy) of the KS group. Individual demographical variables and genetic aberrations are displayed in Table S1. Not all participants with KS were able to execute the entire cognitive assessment. These participants are referred to as the ‘KS‐A’ group (n = 10). The other participants with KS completed at least the intelligence battery plus one test of each specific cognitive domain, and they will be referred to as the ‘KS‐B’ group (n = 18). The GC group included a convenience sample of individuals with other genetic disorders who visited the expertise centre for rare genetic neurodevelopmental disorders (Radboud University Medical Centre, The Netherlands). The study was conducted according to the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Central Committee on Research Involving Human Subjects region Arnhem‐Nijmegen (NL43187.091.13) with written informed consent obtained from all participants, or their legal representatives.

Table 1.

Medical characteristics of participants with Kabuki syndrome

Domain n Specification
Ocular problems 17 Minor problems successfully compensated by glasses or disturbed stereoscopic vision/strabism
4 Severe problems: total loss of sight in one eye for all four participants and for three of them additionally a loss of 50% in the other eye1, loss of 85% and tube vision in the other eye1, and nystagmus2
Hearing problems 3 Single sided deafness
7 Minor hearing problems were present without a need for hearing aids
9 Hearing problems that were successfully compensated by hearing aids
7 Hearing problems that were not sufficiently compensated by hearing aids. Hearing was however still sufficient for cognitive assessment
Brain imaging reports Reports present for three participants; these did not show any structural anomalies, besides microcephaly in one participant
Possible neural damage as a result of brain trauma 3 Oxygen shortage during birth 1, cerebrovascular accident 2, oxygen shortage as a result of low blood sugar 2
Brain functioning 1 History of epileptic seizures, currently in remission with the use of Depakine
Psychopharmacotherapy 3 Antipsychotics (aripiprazol, risperidon and Dipiperon)
Sleep medication 5 Melatonin
Hormone treatment 9
1

Participant of the KS‐B group.

2

Participant of the KS‐A group.

Materials and procedure

To facilitate participation, individuals were visited at their homes for the assessment. Testing was performed by a trained and experienced (>4 years) psychologist and took place in two visits of approximately 4 h, in which breaks were provided based on the individual needs of each participant. To measure adaptive functioning, primary caregivers of the participants were interviewed, using the Vineland‐Z (Dutch version of the Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales Survey Form; Sparrow et al. 1984). Adaptive functioning refers to the conceptual, social and practical skills that have been learned by people in order to function in everyday life (American Association on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities 2018). The Vineland‐Z total level of adaptive functioning is expressed in a developmental age. Secondly, a measure of visual and motor abilities as well as visuoconstruction was included (Beery–Buktenica Developmental Test of Visual‐Motor Integration; Beery VMI; Beery & Beery 2004). Intelligence was measured with the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale‐IV‐NL (WAIS‐IV‐NL, Dutch version of the WAIS‐IV; Wechsler 2008; Wechsler 2012) for adults, or the WISC‐III‐NL (Dutch version of the WISC‐III; Wechsler 1991; Wechsler 2005) for children. Cognitive flexibility and planning were assessed by the Intra/Extradimensional shifting task (Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery, CANTAB®) [Cognitive assessment software] 2018), Key search task and Zoo map task (both part of the Behavioural Assessment of the Dysexecutive Syndrome battery; Wilson et al. 1996; Emslie et al. 2003). Furthermore, memory functioning was measured by the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT; Schmand et al. 2012, Osterrieth 1944), Paired Associates Learning test (PAL, CANTAB®) and Pattern Recognition Memory test (PRM, CANTAB®). As for social cognition, performance was assessed by the Dutch Theory of Mind test‐Revised (Steerneman 2009). All tests are commonly used in a variety of international neuropsychological studies. For a more extensive description of the tests, see Table S2.

Statistical analysis

SPSS version 22 for Windows was used for all statistical analyses. First, mean developmental age scores were calculated based on the norm group in the manual of the VABS, to describe adaptive functioning in the total KS sample as well as for both KS groups (A and B) separately. Secondly, in order to explore the more extended cognitive profile, performances of the KS‐B group on the Wechsler scales, intra/extradimensional shifting test, Key search task, Zoo map task, RAVLT, PAL, PRM, Theory of Mind test‐Revised, emotion recognition and Beery were compared to the results of norm groups described in the test manuals (via transformation of raw scores into standardised scores). Finally, cognitive test performances of the KS‐B group were compared to those of the GC group. Although a general linear model repeated measures would have been the preferred choice for this design, this was not feasible due to a lack of power as a result of the relatively small sample size compared to the large number of dependent variables. The use of this statistical test would therefore not be informative in this sample. Instead, Cohen's d was calculated for all differences in cognitive test performances between both groups (Cohen 1992; Lee 2016).

Results

Table 2 displays the means and standard deviations for adaptive functioning in the total KS group and for the KS‐A and KS‐B groups separately. A large difference between chronological and developmental age was present in all three groups. Furthermore, the mean developmental age is substantially higher in the KS‐B group compared to the KS‐A group, in accordance with the fact that the KS‐B group was able to complete more (complex) cognitive tasks. Individual intelligence scores and levels of adaptive functioning of participants with KS are displayed in Table S1.

Table 2.

Chronological age and developmental age for Kabuki syndrome groups

KS total KS‐A KS‐B
N (male participants) 28 (15) 10 (7) 18 (8)
M SD Range M SD Range M SD Range
Chronological age 1 206.4 133.4 63–577 145.9 159.5 63–577 240.1 107.0 72–461
Developmental age 2 61.9 38.7 13–145 29.6 13.5 13–49 79.78 36.5 25–145
1

Age in months.

2

Based on normative means of the Vineland Z, in months.

For the comparison of the KS‐B group with healthy individuals, Table 3 presents the mean differences (IQ and T‐scores) between participants with KS and the available norm groups for all cognitive tests. As expected, the KS‐B group showed a weaker performance on all of these tests. The mean performance on the cognitive domains ranged between 1.2 and 2.9 standard deviations below the healthy population. Weakest performances were present in the domains of intelligence and visuoconstruction.

Table 3.

Cognitive performances in KS‐B group compared to the norm groups of the test manuals

N Devination from the norm group (in SD) Classification
Intelligence WAIS‐IV/WISC‐III 1 18 −2.96 Low
Memory RAVLT immediate recall 1 , 2 18 −1.09 Low average
RAVLT delayed recall 1 , 2 17 −1.61 Below average
Executive functioning Key search 1 16 −1.2 Low average
Zoo map 1 1 15 −1.36 Below average
Zoo map 2 1 15 −1.56 Below average
Social cognition ToM test‐R 1 , 2 16 −1.66 Below average
Visuoconstruction Beery VMI 1 18 −2.97 Low
1

Normative means corrected for age.

2

Maximum age of the normative means was 12 (RAVLT, ToM test‐R) or 16 (Key search, Zoo map) years of age.

Key search, Zoo map 1 and Zoo map 2 are subtests of the Behavioural Assessment of the Dysexecutive Syndrome. Beery VMI, Beery–Buktenica Developmental Test of Visual‐Motor Integration; ToM test‐R, Theory of Mind test‐Revised; RAVLT, Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test; WAIS‐IV, Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale‐Fourth Edition; WISC‐III, Wechsler Intelligence Scale Children‐Third Edition.

Table 4 displays the mean raw scores and effect sizes for the comparison of the results of the KS‐B and GC group. The general level of intelligence for the GC group (M = 66.6, SD = 13.7) was higher than for the KS‐B group (M = 55.6, SD = 10.1; Cohen's d = −.92). Furthermore, visuoconstruction was weaker in the KS‐B group (Beery VMI: M = 13.7, SD = 3.6; Beery Visual: M = 17.2, SD = 4.1; Beery Motor: M = 14.9, SD = 6.3) compared to the GC group (Beery VMI: M = 19.1, SD = 4.9; Cohen's d = −1.29; Beery Visual: M = 21.6, SD = 4.5; Cohen's d = −1.05; Beery Motor: M = 21.1, SD = 5.3; Cohen's d = −1.10). As for memory functioning, mixed results were found. Weaknesses in performances were found on two tests of visual memory in the KS‐B group (PRM: M = 8.1, SD = 2.9; PAL: M = 5.6, SD = 1.9) compared to the GC group (PRM: M = 9.7, SD = 1.9; Cohen's d = −.68; PAL: M = 6.6, SD = 1.6; Cohen's d = −.59). While a slightly higher performance was present in the KS‐B group for a task that taps into verbal memory (RAVLT immediate recall: KS‐B: M = 37.4, SD = 10.1; GC: M = 33.5, SD = 11.0; Cohen's d = .38). As for other domains of cognitive functioning, there were no relevant differences between the KS‐B and GC group in planning, executive functioning or social cognition (−.21 < Cohen's d < .25).

Table 4.

Comparison KS‐B and GC

KS‐B GC Mean difference Effect size2
N M 1 SD 1 N M 1 SD 1
Intelligence TIQ 18 55.6 10.1 22 66.6 13.7 −11.0 −.92
Executive functioning IED 17 6.8 2.4 20 7.0 2.4 −.2 −.09
Key search 17 4.9 4.5 20 3.8 4.4 1.1 .25
Zoo map 1 16 −2.4 6.0 19 −1.3 4.7 −1.1 −.21
Zoo map 2 16 2.8 6.1 19 3.2 5.4 −.4 −.07
Memory RAVLT immediate recall 18 37.4 10.1 21 33.5 11.0 3.9 .38
RAVLT delayed recall 17 6.5 3.4 20 6.2 3.2 .3 .09
PRM 17 8.1 2.9 20 9.7 1.9 −1.6 −.68
PAL 17 5.6 1.9 20 6.6 1.6 −1.0 −.59
Social cognition ToM test‐R 16 25.4 6.0 21 24.8 5.6 .6 .11
Visuoconstruction Beery VMI 18 13.7 3.6 18 19.1 4.9 −5.4 −1.29
Beery Visual 18 17.2 4.1 18 21.6 4.5 −4.4 −1.05
Beery Motor 18 14.9 6.3 18 21.1 5.3 −6.2 −1.10
1

Raw performance scores.

2

Cohen's d effect sizes of .2, .5 and .8 represent, respectively, a small, medium and large effect.

Key search, Zoo map 1 and Zoo map 2 are subtests of the Behavioural Assessment of the Dysexecutive Syndrome; Beery VMI, Beery–Buktenica Developmental Test of Visual‐Motor Integration; IED, intra/extradimensional shift test (CANTAB); PAL, Paired Associates Learning test (CANTAB); PRM, Pattern Recognition Memory test (CANTAB); RAVLT, Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test; ToM test‐R, Theory of Mind test‐Revised; TIQ, total intelligence quotient.

Discussion

This is the first case–control study to explore multiple domains of cognitive functioning in individuals with KS. Performances of intelligence, executive functioning, memory, social cognition and visuoconstruction in participants with KS (KS‐B group) were first compared to the results of norm groups represented in the manuals of the different cognitive tests. As expected, performances of the KS‐B group were lower than those of norm groups consisting of healthy people, for all cognitive tests. Participants within the KS‐B group deviated most strongly on the domains of intelligence and visuoconstruction, in contrast to a relatively small deviation for the domains of executive functioning, memory and social cognition. These results could indicate that these last domains are relatively unaffected in participants with KS, in spite of their intellectual impairment. Performances of the KS‐B group were then compared to those of a more appropriate control group, to further sustain these findings. The latter group consisted of participants with other genetic syndromes associated with ID/developmental delay.

A lower mean level of intelligence was present for the KS‐B group compared to the GC group. Nonetheless, there were no substantial differences in performances between the KS‐B group and PGC group on domains of executive functioning, verbal memory and social cognition. The latter result was also in accordance with the comparison between the KS‐B and the norm group, which might indicate a relative strength of these cognitive domains in individuals with KS. Furthermore, lower performances in the KS‐B group compared to the GC group were present for the domains of visuoconstruction and visual memory. As verbal memory was relatively unaffected in the KS‐B group, the visual memory problems may have been a result of weaknesses in visual processing interfering with imprinting, rather than a result of genuine memory problems.

Multiple factors could have contributed to the weaknesses found in visuoconstruction in the KS‐B group. First of all, it may have been a result of ocular anomalies. As the majority of participants had only minor ocular problems, which probably did not interfere with cognitive test performances, group results may plausibly have been affected by only three of the participants (with more severe ocular problems). Their performances did not deviate substantially more from the group mean than the performances of other participants in the KS‐B group, which suggests only a minor negative influence on the total group performances. Secondly, lower scores on tasks measuring visuoconstruction could have been the result of primary deficits in motor responses, for instance due to hypotonia or developmental delay of fine motor skills, which have often been reported in participants with KS (Bögershausen and Wollnik 2013). A weaker performance was indeed found on a task that taps explicitly into fine motor functioning (Beery Motor). However, these weaknesses did not entirely explain the differences in visuoconstruction between the KS‐B and GC group, which argues for additional cognitive weaknesses. The visuoconstruction problems may have been a reflection of the initial differences in intellectual functioning between the KS‐B and GC groups, although they seem to be too specific to be explained by the differences in intellectual functioning entirely. Thus, it is plausible that our results reflect specific weaknesses in primarily visual perceptive processing (and as a result also in visuoconstruction), which is in line with previous findings of weaker performances of participants with KS on tasks that tap into visual perceptive processing (Lehman et al. 2017; Caciolo et al. 2018). Future studies may focus on the development of specialised interventions for these visual processing problems in KS.

The strengths of the current study lie in the extensiveness of the cognitive assessment and the inclusion of a genetic control group, which enables the observation of specific strengths and weaknesses related to KS. Not all participants were able to execute the entire assessment, which resulted in an exclusion of these individuals for the analyses of cognitive performance. As expected, results on measures of adaptive functioning indicated that developmental age was higher in the KS‐B group compared to the KS‐A group. Because the results on cognitive tasks were solely based on performances of the KS‐B group, findings may therefore reflect an overestimation of the capabilities of the total population of individuals with KS.

This study was executed parallel to another study on cognitive functioning in KS (Harris et al. 2019). Design, data collection and interpretation of the results of that study, however, were performed independently and none of the participants was included in both samples. Interestingly also in this complementary study, weaknesses in similar domains were notified, further emphasising our findings.

In conclusion, this study indicates that individuals with KS suffer from specific weaknesses in visuoconstruction and visual memory, additional to general ID. The visuoconstruction problems that were demonstrated may well reflect a primary problem in visual perceptual processing, which may also be the underlying cause of the observed weaknesses in visual memory. Contrary to the use of visual symbols, often applied in the daily practice of patient care for individuals with ID, the results presented here highlight the importance of the use of auditory cues instead of or additional to visual cues, in targeted educational support and psychosocial interventions for individuals with KS.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Supporting information

Table S1: Characteristics of participants with KS

Table S2: Details and references of cognitive measures

Acknowledgements

The first author is a shared PhD student of Vincent van Gogh Centre of Excellence for Neuropsychiatry and Radboud University Medical Center Department of Human Genetics, and the research is embedded within the collaborative research group ‘Psychopathology and Genetics’ of the aforementioned institutes. The authors would like to thank all patients and their caregivers for both their participation and hospitality during the data collection at their homes. The contribution of Wouter Oomens MSc, Anja Bos‐Roubos MSc, Mirthe Fransz MSc, Liz Driessen MSc, Laura Fornara MSc, Kelly Rutjes MSc and Vincent Janssen MSc in the data collection is gratefully acknowledged.

van Dongen, L. C. M. , Wingbermühle, P. A. M. , van der Veld, W. M. , Stumpel, C. , Kleefstra, T. , and Egger, J. I. M. (2019) Exploring the cognitive phenotype of Kabuki (Niikawa–Kuroki) syndrome. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 63: 498–506. 10.1111/jir.12597.

References

  1. Adam M. P. & Hudgins L. (2005) Kabuki syndrome: a review. Clinical Genetics 67, 209–219. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. American Association on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities (2018). Definition of intellectual disability. [online] Available at: https://aaidd.org/intellectual-disability/definition [Accessed 18 Sep. 2018].
  3. Beery K. E. & Beery N. A. (2004) Beery–Buktenica Developmental Test of Visual‐Motor Integration: Administration, Scoring, and Teaching Manual, 5th edn. NCS Pearson, Minneapolis. [Google Scholar]
  4. Ben‐Omran T. & Teebi A. S. (2005) Structural central nervous system (CNS) anomalies in Kabuki syndrome. American Journal of Medical Genetics. Part A 137, 100–103. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bögershausen N. & Wollnik B. (2013) Unmasking Kabuki syndrome. Clinical Genetics 83, 201–211. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Caciolo C., Alfieri P., Piccini G., Digilio M. C., Lepri F. R., Tartaglia M. et al (2018) Neurobehavioral features in individuals with Kabuki syndrome. Molecular Genetics & Genomic Medicine 6, 322–331. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. CANTAB® [Cognitive assessment software] . (2018) Cambridge cognition. All rights reserved. http://www.cantab.com
  8. Cohen J. (1992) A power primer. Psychological Bulletin 112, 155–159. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Emslie H., Wilson F. C., Burden V., Nimmo‐Smith I. & Wilson B. A. (2003) Behavioural Assessment of the Dysexecutive Syndrome for Children (BADS‐C). Harcourt Assessment, London. [Google Scholar]
  10. Fletcher R. J., Barnhill J. & Cooper A. (2017) DM‐ID‐2: Diagnostic manual, intellectual disability: a textbook of diagnosis of mental disorders in persons with intellectual disability. NADD. An association for persons with developmental disabilities and mental health needs.
  11. Harris J., Mahone E. M. & Bjornsson H. T. (2019) Molecularly confirmed Kabuki (Niikawa‐Kuroki) syndrome patients demonstrate a specific cognitive profile with extensive visuospatial abnormalities. J Intell disabil res, accepted. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  12. Kurahashi N., Miyake N., Mizuno S., Koshimizu E., Kurahashi H., Yamada K. et al (2017) Characteristics of epilepsy in patients with Kabuki syndrome with KMT2D mutations. Brain and Development 39, 672–677. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Kuroki Y., Suzuki Y., Chyo H., Hata A. & Matsui I. (1981) A new malformation syndrome of long palpebral fissures, large ears, depressed nasal tip, and skeletal anomalies associated with postnatal dwarfism and mental retardation. The Journal of Pediatrics 99, 570–573. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Lederer D., Grisart B., Digilio M. C., Benoit V., Crespin M., Ghariani S. C. et al (2012) Deletion of KDM6A, a histone demethylase interacting with MLL2, in three patients with Kabuki syndrome. American Journal of Human Genetics 90, 119–124. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Lee D. K. (2016) Alternatives to P value: confidence interval and effect size. Korean Journal of Anesthesiology 69, 555–562. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Lehman N., Mazery A. C., Visier A., Baumann C., Lachesnais D., Capri Y. et al (2017) Molecular, clinical and neuropsychological study in 31 patients with Kabuki syndrome and KMT2D mutations. Clinical Genetics 92, 298–305. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Lezak M. D., Howieson D. B., Bigler E. D. & Tranel D. (2012) Neuropsychological Assessment, 5th edn. Oxford University Press, New York. [Google Scholar]
  18. Li Y., Bögershausen N., Alanay Y., Kiper P. ö. S., Plume N., Keupp K. et al (2011) A mutation screen in patients with Kabuki syndrome. Human Genetics 130, 715–724. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Lodi M., Chifari R., Parazzini C., Viri M., Beccaria F., Lorenzetti M. E. et al (2010) Seizures and EEG pattern in Kabuki syndrome. Brain and Development 32, 829–834. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Mervis C. B., Becerra A. M., Rowe M. L., Hersh J. H. & Morris C. A. (2005) Intellectual abilities and adaptive behavior of children and adolescents with Kabuki syndrome: a preliminary study. American Journal of Medical Genetics. Part A 132, 248–255. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Miyake N., Mizuno S., Okamoto N., Ohashi H., Shiina M., Ogata K. et al (2013) KDM6A point mutations cause Kabuki syndrome. Human Mutation 34, 108–110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Morgan A. T., Mei C., Da Costa A., Fifer J., Lederer D., Benoit V. et al (2015) Speech and language in a genotyped cohort of individuals with Kabuki syndrome. American Journal of Medical Genetics. Part A 167, 1483–1492. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Ng S. B., Bigham A. W., Buckingham K. J., Hannibal M. C., McMillin M. J., Gildersleeve H. I. et al (2010) Exome sequencing identifies MLL2 mutations as a cause of Kabuki syndrome. Nature Genetics 42, 790–793. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Niikawa N., Kuroki Y., Kajii T., Matsuura N., Ishikiriyama S., Tonoki H. et al (1988) Kabuki make‐up (Niikawa–Kuroki) syndrome: a study of 62 patients. American Journal of Medical Genetics 31, 565–589. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Niikawa N., Matsuura N., Fukushima Y., Ohsawa T. & Kajii T. (1981) Kabuki make‐up syndrome: a syndrome of mental retardation, unusual facies, large and protruding ears, and postnatal growth deficiency. The Journal of Pediatrics 99, 565–569. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Oksanen V. E., Arvio M. A., Peippo M. M., Valanne L. K. & Sainio K. O. (2004) Temporo‐occipital spikes: a typical EEG finding in Kabuki syndrome. Pediatric Neurology 30, 67–70. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Osterrieth P. A. (1944) Le test de copie d'une figure complexe: contribution l'étude de la perception et la mémoire. Archives de Psychologie 30, 286–356. [Google Scholar]
  28. Philip N., Meinecke P., David A., Dean J., Ayme S., Clark R. et al (1992) Kabuki make‐up (Niikawa–Kuroki) syndrome: a study of 16 non‐Japanese cases. Clinical Dysmorphology 1, 63–77. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Schmand B., Houx P. & Koning de I. (2012) Dutch institute for psychologists. [Nederlands Instituut van Psychologen][online] Available at: https://www.psynip.nl/secties/Neuropsychologie/normgegevens (retrieved 18 Sep. 2018).
  30. Schrander‐Stumpel C., Meinecke P., Wilson G., Gilleseen‐Kaesbach G., Tinschert S., König R. et al (1994) The Kabuki (Niikawa–Kuroki) syndrome: further delineation of the phenotype in 29 non‐Japanese patients. European Journal of Pediatrics 153, 438–445. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Sparrow S. S., Balla D. A. & Cicchetti D. V. (1984) Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales. American Guidance Service, Minesota. [Google Scholar]
  32. Bildt A. A. & Kraijer D. W. (2003) Vineland Z: Sociale redzaamheidschaal voor kinderen en jeugdigen met een verstandelijke beperking. PITS, Leiden. [Google Scholar]
  33. Steerneman P. M. C. (2009) ToM test‐R handleiding. Garant, Antwerpen‐Apeldoorn. [Google Scholar]
  34. Verrotti A., Agostinelli S., Cirillo C., D'Egidio C., Mohn A., Boncimino A. et al (2011) Long‐term outcome of epilepsy in Kabuki syndrome. Seizure 20, 650–654. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Wechsler D. (1991) Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, 3rd edn. Psychological Corporation, New York. [Google Scholar]
  36. Wechsler D. (2005) Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, 3rd edn. Pearson Assessment and Information, Amsterdam. [Google Scholar]
  37. Wechsler D. (2008) Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, 4th edn. Pearson Assessment and Information, Amsterdam; [Google Scholar]
  38. Wechsler D. (2012) WAIS‐IV‐NL Nederlandstalige bewerking, afname‐ en scoringshandleiding. Pearson Assessment and Information, Amsterdam. Netherlands. [Google Scholar]
  39. Wilson B. A., Alderman N., Burgess P. W., Emslie H. & Evans J. J. (1996) Behavioural Assessment of the Dysexecutive Syndrome. Thames Valley Test Company, Bury St. Edmunds. [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Table S1: Characteristics of participants with KS

Table S2: Details and references of cognitive measures


Articles from Journal of Intellectual Disability Research are provided here courtesy of Wiley

RESOURCES