Short abstract
Objectives
Many studies of cancer worry use items measuring frequency or intensity. Little is known about how each of these relate to cancer screening uptake. This study compared the association between worry frequency vs. intensity and colorectal cancer screening intention/uptake.
Methods
Across four surveys (2014–2016), we collected data from 2878 screening-eligible men and women (aged 60–70) in England. Measures included single-items assessing cancer worry frequency and intensity, and a derived combination of both. We also assessed self-reported past faecal occult blood testing uptake (ever vs. never), intention to participate when next invited (yes vs. no), and demographics. Using logistic regression, we compared a model containing sociodemographic characteristics (Model 1), with four models adding cancer worry frequency (Model 2), intensity (Model 3), both (Model 4), or the combined measure (Model 5).
Results
A model with cancer worry intensity and demographics (Model 3) explained significantly more variance in uptake and intention (R2 = 0.068 and 0.062, respectively) than demographics alone (Model 1: R2 = 0.058 and 0.042; p < 0.001), or a model with demographics and cancer worry frequency (Model 2: R2 = 0.059 and 0.052; p < 0.001). The model was also equally as effective as models including both the frequency and intensity items (Model 4: R2 = 0.070 n.s. and 0.062 n.s.), or using the derived combination of both (Model 5: R2 = 0.063 n.s. and 0.053 n.s.).
Conclusion
A single item measure of cancer worry intensity appeared to be most parsimonious for explaining variance in colorectal cancer screening intention and uptake.
Keywords: Cancer, fear, worry, anxiety, colorectal cancer, screening, faecal occult blood test
Introduction
Colorectal cancer (CRC), the fourth most common cancer worldwide, accounts for 8.5% of all cancer mortality.1 In the UK, nearly 35,000 new cases are diagnosed annually, and CRC accounts for 10% of all cancer deaths.2 Population-based screening using the faecal occult blood test (FOBt) can contribute to reducing the relative risk of CRC mortality by up to 25% in those completing at least one round of screening.3 Since 2006, the Bowel Cancer Screening Programme (BCSP) in England has invited men and women aged 60–69 to undertake free, home-based biennial FOBt screening for colorectal cancer (this programme has since been extended to age 74). Uptake is sub-optimal (56% in 2015–16), with known socio-demographic variation (particularly lower uptake among ethnic minorities and individuals living in poorer areas4–7), and effects of practical and psychological factors.6 One such psychological factor is cancer worry, a negative emotional reaction to the threat of cancer,8 shown to be higher in women, those who are younger, and those from lower socioeconomic or ethnic minority backgrounds.9,10
The effect of cancer worry on CRC screening uptake is uncertain. Some studies have found a positive association,11–13 others a negative association,14,15 some both positive and negative associations when considering different facets of cancer worry,5,16–18 and others finding no effect.19–24 Comparison between studies is hindered by inconsistencies in how cancer worry is operationalized.8,25,26 Although studies should ideally include comprehensive measures of cancer worry, few have the capacity to include detailed measures. Due to space restrictions, most large population-based studies can only measure general worry about cancer, without further specification of the object of the worry. When measuring general cancer worry, some studies operationalize it in terms of the frequency of fearful thoughts about cancer, while others measure the intensity of fearful thoughts about cancer. In our view, cancer worry frequency and intensity are two different, yet correlated, dimensions of cancer worry, on which people may score differently. For example, someone may feel very anxious when they think about cancer (high intensity of cancer worry), and therefore try to avoid thinking about it (low frequency), suggestive of an avoidant coping style for cancer risk. Indeed, a study of underutilizers of breast cancer screening27 found that women who were not planning to have a mammogram tended to be “not at all” worried about breast cancer, yet also tended to agree that they were “so fearful of cancer that you don’t want to know if you have cancer or not”.
The idea of cancer worry as a function of both frequency and intensity of cancer worry is not new. Scales of fear of cancer recurrence often include both,28 and clinical anxiety disorders are also characterized in terms of being excessive (intensity) or persistent (frequency, duration) compared with normative fear or anxiety (leading to clinically important distress or impairment in functioning, unlike cancer worry in the general population as here described29). However, most population-based surveys of cancer worry include only one of these dimensions, so it is difficult to establish whether different associations with behavioural outcomes found across studies are due to differences in measurement or to real differences in the behavioural correlates of cancer worry frequency versus intensity.
To our knowledge, only three large population-based studies have simultaneously included items that can be considered as measures of cancer worry frequency and cancer worry intensity to examine associations with uptake of CRC screening,16–18 and thus allow comparisons between these two measures and their association with cancer screening behaviour. Using data from the US Health Information National Trends Survey (HINTS), McQueen et al.16 did not find an association between cancer worry frequency (i.e. “How often do you worry about getting colon cancer?”) and uptake of FOBt or endoscopic screening. However, being afraid of finding cancer, if tested (i.e. agreeing to the statement “You are afraid of finding colon cancer if you were checked”), was associated with a higher likelihood of endoscopic screening in women, and also seemed to be associated with higher uptake of FOBt, although this did not reach statistical significance. Wong et al.18 used similar measures of cancer worry frequency (i.e. (dis)agreement with the statement “I never worry about getting CRC”) and fear of finding cancer (i.e. “I am afraid of finding out if I have CRC”) in a population-representative survey in Singapore to examine associations with CRC screening uptake (either FOBT or endoscopic screening, although FOBt was reportedly used most often as test modality). Their findings showed that more frequent worry about getting CRC was associated with higher uptake of screening, while fear of finding out about CRC was associated with lower uptake.18 Finally, in a prospective study of flexible sigmoidoscopy (FS) screening in the UK, Vrinten et al.17 found that worrying a lot about cancer (i.e. agreement with the statement “I worry a lot about cancer”) was associated with a higher likelihood of intending to attend screening, but did not predict actual attendance, while feeling uncomfortable when thinking about cancer (i.e. “It makes me uncomfortable to think about cancer”) was associated with a lower likelihood of attending FS screening.17 These mixed findings suggest that there may be differences between cancer worry frequency and intensity measures and their relationship with screening intentions and uptake, although the study samples were very different and no firm conclusions can therefore be drawn from these previous findings. In addition, these studies only reported the behavioural associations for items separately, and did not examine whether the behavioural associations of cancer worry are even stronger for a combined index of frequency and intensity of cancer worry, as may be hypothesized based on the characterization of clinically relevant anxiety as described above.
To better understand how cancer worry can best be measured in population-based studies that seek to examine the association of cancer worry with cancer-related behavioural outcomes, this study compared measures of cancer worry frequency, cancer worry intensity and a combination of both, and their association with intention and uptake of the FOBT in a population-based sample of screening-eligible English adults aged 60–70.
Methods
Design
We combined data from all four waves of the Attitudes, Behaviour and Cancer UK Survey (Attitudes, Behaviour and Cancer UK Survey [ABACUS]; 2014–2016), which were collected by TNS Research International as part of their weekly omnibus survey using face-to-face, computer-assisted personal interviews. The TNS omnibus survey defines sample points using 2001 Census small-area statistics and the Postcode Address File (stratified by social grade and Government Office Region), which are used for random location sampling. To ensure a population-representative sample, quotas are set for age, sex, children in the home, and working status, and weights are provided by the survey company. An adjustment weight was assigned to each case during analysis, so that respondents with characteristics (age, sex, social grade, region) who were underrepresented (relative to the national population) were given a higher weight (>1) than those who were relatively overrepresented (weight <1). The ABACUS survey is a series of four population-based surveys designed to assess attitudes to cancer and cancer screening in England from 2014 to 2016. The first (2014; N = 1675) focused mainly on colorectal cancer screening using FOBt and included men and women aged 58–70. The second (2015; N = 1464) focused on colorectal cancer screening using FOBt and FS and included a wider age range (men and women aged 50–70), while the third (N = 2111) and fourth surveys (N = 2048; both collected in 2016) focused on various beliefs and attitudes about cancer in the general population, and included men and women aged 18–70. The upper age limit for the survey was chosen because this is the age at which people tend to stop being invited to cancer screening in England; the extension of the colorectal cancer screening programme to age 74 had not been fully rolled out across England at the time of the surveys. Several other papers have been published from these datasets, including some that examine different objects of cancer worry (e.g. fear of death) in relation to cancer screening (ABACUS wave 3),10,30 but this is the first time that we have combined all four datasets to obtain a large enough sample to examine the role of cancer worry frequency and intensity on colorectal cancer screening uptake. All respondents resided in England at the time of the interview.
Participants
The sample for these analyses consisted of 2878 men and women aged 60–70. We excluded participants who were outside this age range and those who had a previous diagnosis of cancer. All participants consented at the start of the interview.
Measures
Sociodemographics, including the respondents’ age, gender, ethnicity, marital status, and social grade were recorded using simple questions. Ethnicity was measured using 16 categories from the UK Census,31 but were dichotomized into White vs. non-White (including mixed ethnic backgrounds) due to small numbers in the ethnic minority groups. Marital status was recorded as “married or living as married,” “single,” and “widowed, separated, or divorced.” Social grade was used as an indicator of socioeconomic status and was recorded using the categories from the National Readership Survey.32 These are based on the occupation of the chief wage earner in each household. The categories were: A (higher managerial, administrative, or professional), B (intermediate managerial, administrative, or professional), C1 (supervisory, clerical or junior managerial, administrative, or professional), C2 (skilled manual), D (semiskilled or unskilled manual), or E (state pensioners, casual/lowest grade workers, or unemployed with state benefits only). Grades A and B were combined to create more equal sized groups, as were groups D and E.
Cancer worry was assessed with two items, one assessing cancer worry frequency and one assessing cancer worry intensity. These questions were the same across all four surveys. Cancer worry frequency was measured with the item: ‘How often do you worry about your chance of getting cancer yourself?’, with response options ‘never’, ‘occasionally’, ‘sometimes’, ‘often’, and ‘very often’, which was adapted from the US HINTS.33 Cancer worry intensity was measured with the item: ‘How anxious do you feel when you think about cancer?’, with response options ‘not at all’, ‘slightly’, ‘quite a bit’, and ‘extremely’. The two items were significantly correlated (rs[2632] = 0.573, p < 0.001). It should be noted that these items are about cancer in general, and are not specific to colorectal cancer. In addition, we combined these two items to create a conceptual categorization of cancer worry. In accordance with accepted definitions of clinically relevant anxiety, which differentiates anxiety disorders from developmentally normative fear as being “excessive or persisting beyond developmentally appropriate periods”29 (i.e. as a function of intensity or frequency/duration of the anxiety), we conceptualized high cancer worry as worry that was very high in intensity, or very frequent. Consistent with some of our previous studies,34,35 we categorized this combined measure in three levels: “no cancer worry” (those who “never” worried and were “not at all” anxious), moderate cancer worry (those who worried “occasionally” or “sometimes”, or who were “slightly anxious”), and high cancer worry (those who worried “often” or “very often”, or who were “quite a bit” or “extremely” anxious). We have favoured a conceptual categorization in three categories (to allow for exploration of curvilinear effects) rather than combining the two items by adding or averaging the scores, because scores in the middle of the range for sum or average scores may be difficult to interpret. For example, they could result from scores in the middle of the range for both items, or from scores at the top end of the range for one item and the bottom end of the range for the other item. Conceptually, these two scenarios are very different from each other, and we have therefore favoured a conceptual classification, whereby high scores on either (or both) item(s) would always result in being classified as being ‘high’ in cancer worry, while only scoring the lowest score on both items (‘not at all anxious’ and ‘never worried’) results in being classified as having ‘no’ cancer worry.
Intentions to do the FOB test in the future were assessed in all surveys using variations of the same question: “Will you do the stool test when you are next sent one/the next time you are sent a kit/the next time you are invited?” Response options were the same for all versions of this question, and consisted of “definitely not,” “probably not,” “yes, probably,” “yes, definitely,” which were categorized as ‘not intending to complete FOBt screening’ (“definitely not” and “probably not”) and ‘intending to complete FOBt screening’ (“yes, probably” and “yes, definitely”). “Don’t know/not sure” responses were coded as missing.
Self-reported screening uptake among screening-eligible respondents was recorded slightly differently in the four waves of the ABACUS survey, but for the purpose of this study, all responses were dichotomized as ‘never’ (as in never having returned one of the routine colorectal cancer screening test kits sent by the National Health Service BCSP) vs. ‘ever’ screening (i.e. having returned at least one test kit). In survey 1, respondents were asked whether they had ever been invited to do a stool test for the National Health Service BCSP. Respondents answering ‘yes’, were asked additional questions about how many times they had received a stool kit and how many times they had taken part. All participants who indicated that they had done at least one test kit were coded as ‘ever’ having participated. In survey 2, respondents were asked, ‘Have you ever done the stool test?’ (yes, no, don’t know), with ‘yes’ responses coded as ‘ever’ (1) and ‘no’ responses coded as ‘never’ (0). In surveys 3 and 4, respondents were asked which of five statements best described them. The statements were based on the stages of the Precaution Adoption Process Model36 and were dichotomized to characterize ‘never’ participants as those who were eligible for FOBt screening but reported never having heard of it, never having been invited, or never having done it, and ‘ever’ participants as those who reported having completed the test at least once. Respondents stating ‘don’t know’ or ‘refused’ were coded as missing. A previous study has shown that self-reported ever uptake of screening is highly reliable.37
We excluded those with missing data on the cancer worry, intention, and self-reported uptake variables. We report descriptive statistics for all variables for the total sample. We then used logistic regression to examine the relationship between the sociodemographic and cancer worry variables, and FOBt screening intention and uptake. In addition to the unadjusted associations, we compared a series of logistic regression models containing only the sociodemographic variables and ABACUS Wave (Model 1) with four models adding cancer worry frequency (Model 2), cancer worry intensity (Model 3), both (Model 4), or the combined measure of cancer worry (Model 5). We used weighted data for descriptive analyses, as sample weights are highly relevant for prevalence estimation, but relied on unweighted data to test associations, as weights typically do not alter the results of coefficients, and unweighted analysis has been recommended because of being simpler, more transparent, and more accurate (e.g. reduced standard error, reduced risk of overfitting).38 As a sensitivity analysis, we also performed the same analyses using the continuous cancer worry variables. For these analyses, we also tested the interaction effect for cancer worry frequency and intensity, and curvilinear effects of cancer worry frequency and intensity on screening intentions and uptake. Stata 12 SE was used for all analyses and alpha levels of 0.05 or less indicated statistical significance.
Results
The weighted sample characteristics for each wave and for the combined sample are reported in Table 1 (the unweighted sample characteristics are reported in Table S1 in the Online Supplement). The mean age of the sample was 65.2 (SD = 3.3), and 47% were men. Similar to the general population estimates for this age group in England,39 the majority of respondents were married or living as married (67%) and of White ethnic origin (95%). About a quarter of respondents (24%) came from social grades D and E, 21% were in grade C2, 24% in grade C1, and 31% in grade A/B. Across all four waves, 69% had participated in FOBt screening at least once, and 81% intended to participate when next invited.
Table 1.
ABACUS 1 | ABACUS 2 | ABACUS 3 | ABACUS 4 | TOTAL | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
N=1381 | N=719 | N=369 | N=409 | N=2878 | ||||||
Age (mean and SD) | 65.14 | (3.20) | 65.06 | (3.19) | 65.58 | (3.27) | 65.34 | (3.47) | 65.21 | (3.25) |
Gender: n (%) | ||||||||||
Male | 701 | (45.7%) | 353 | (48.7%) | 201 | (50.1%) | 200 | (46.2%) | 1455 | (47.1%) |
Female | 680 | (54.3%) | 366 | (51.3%) | 168 | (49.9%) | 209 | (53.8%) | 1423 | (52.9%) |
Ethnicity: n (%) | ||||||||||
White | 1316 | (96.1%) | 681 | (94.5%) | 346 | (94.5%) | 385 | (93.8%) | 2728 | (95.2%) |
Other | 65 | (3.9%) | 38 | (5.5%) | 23 | (5.5%) | 24 | (6.2%) | 150 | (4.8%) |
Marital status: n (%) | ||||||||||
Married or living as married | 901 | (69.7%) | 442 | (64.7%) | 236 | (66.7%) | 254 | (64.8%) | 1833 | (67.4%) |
Single | 122 | (7.4%) | 78 | (10.2%) | 50 | (11.7%) | 47 | (10.8%) | 297 | (9.1%) |
Widowed, separated or divorced | 358 | (22.9%) | 199 | (25.1%) | 83 | (21.6%) | 108 | (24.4%) | 748 | (23.5%) |
Social grade: n (%) | ||||||||||
A/B | 347 | (30.9%) | 153 | (31.3%) | 85 | (31.1%) | 84 | (31.3%) | 669 | (31.1%) |
C1 | 301 | (26.5%) | 142 | (21.6%) | 81 | (23.2%) | 91 | (21.6%) | 615 | (24.2%) |
C2 | 255 | (21.4%) | 152 | (23.0%) | 60 | (16.4%) | 43 | (17.8%) | 510 | (20.7%) |
D/E | 478 | (21.2%) | 272 | (24.1%) | 143 | (29.3%) | 161 | (29.3%) | 1054 | (24.0%) |
Cancer worry frequency: n (%) | ||||||||||
Never | 484 | (32.9%) | 360 | (49.2%) | 142 | (36.4%) | 176 | (42.6%) | 1162 | (38.7%) |
Occasionally | 382 | (28.6%) | 237 | (34.7%) | 150 | (42.0%) | 145 | (36.4%) | 914 | (32.8%) |
Sometimes | 257 | (19.5%) | 80 | (10.5%) | 57 | (16.2%) | 63 | (15.4%) | 457 | (16.3%) |
Often | 66 | (5.1%) | 12 | (1.4%) | 8 | (2.3%) | 11 | (2.6%) | 97 | (3.5%) |
Very often | 21 | (1.5%) | 6 | (0.8%) | 3 | (0.9%) | 4 | (0.8%) | 34 | (1.2%) |
Missing | 171 | (12.3%) | 24 | (3.4%) | 9 | (2.4%) | 10 | (2.3%) | 214 | (7.5%) |
Cancer worry intensity: n (%) | ||||||||||
Not at all | 461 | (32.3%) | 315 | (43.7%) | 138 | (36.1%) | 165 | (39.7%) | 1079 | (36.6%) |
Slightly | 511 | (38.4%) | 283 | (39.9%) | 164 | (46.4%) | 172 | (43.3%) | 1130 | (40.4%) |
Quite | 244 | (18.4%) | 75 | (10.4%) | 38 | (9.6%) | 51 | (12.4%) | 408 | (14.5%) |
Extremely | 110 | (7.6%) | 18 | (2.3%) | 19 | (5.4%) | 11 | (2.4%) | 158 | (5.3%) |
Missing | 55 | (3.4%) | 28 | (3.7%) | 10 | (2.5%) | 10 | (2.2%) | 103 | (3.2%) |
Cancer worry (combined measure): n (%) | ||||||||||
No worry | 323 | (21.8%) | 267 | (37.1%) | 103 | (26.8%) | 130 | (31.1%) | 823 | (27.5%) |
Moderate worry | 545 | (41.2%) | 322 | (45.3%) | 196 | (55.1%) | 203 | (50.8%) | 1266 | (45.2%) |
High worry | 328 | (23.8%) | 93 | (12.7%) | 57 | (15.0%) | 65 | (15.7%) | 543 | (18.9%) |
Missing | 185 | (13.2%) | 37 | (4.9%) | 13 | (3.2%) | 11 | (2.4%) | 246 | (8.5%) |
Uptake: n (%) | ||||||||||
No | 214 | (14.7%) | 122 | (16.1%) | 106 | (27.0%) | 112 | (26.5%) | 554 | (18.3%) |
Yes | 913 | (68.2%) | 480 | (68.3%) | 252 | (70.5%) | 273 | (68.1%) | 1918 | (68.5%) |
Missing | 254 | (17.1%) | 117 | (16.6%) | 11 | (2.6%) | 24 | (5.4%) | 406 | (13.2%) |
Screening intentions: n (%) | ||||||||||
No | 180 | (12.3%) | 105 | (14.1%) | 58 | (12.9%) | 48 | (10.3%) | 391 | (12.6%) |
Yes | 1076 | (79.7%) | 555 | (79.3%) | 298 | (82.6%) | 334 | (83.8%) | 2263 | (80.5%) |
Missing | 125 | (8.0%) | 59 | (6.6%) | 20 | (4.5%) | 27 | (5.9%) | 231 | (6.9%) |
In terms of frequency of worry about cancer, more than a third (39%) never worried about getting cancer, nearly half of the sample (49%) worried occasionally or sometimes, and a minority (5%) worried often or very often (Table 1). In terms of cancer worry intensity, just over a third (37%) did not feel anxious at all when thinking about cancer, 40% were slightly anxious, and 20% were quite or extremely anxious. When these two measures were combined, 28% of the sample was classified as having “no cancer worry,” 45% as having “moderate cancer worry,” and 20% as having “high cancer worry.”
After excluding those with missing data on the cancer worry and intention variables, a sample of N = 2463 remained for analysis (Table 2). Of these, 86% intended to take the stool test when they were next invited. FOBt screening intentions were significantly lower in those who were single (79%) or those who were widowed, separated, or divorced (83%), than in those who were married or living as married (88%). There was also a significant social gradient in screening intentions, with fewer of those from lower social grades intending to do the FOBt (C1: 87%, C2: 85%, DE: 81%) compared with those from the highest social grade (AB: 92%), and these associations remained significant when all other sociodemographic variables were adjusted for in Model 1 (Table 2). Model 1 explained 4.2% of variance in the data.
Table 2.
Total | Unadjusted |
Model 1 |
Model 2 |
Model 3 |
Model 4 |
Model 5 |
|||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
(%) | OR | CI | OR | CI | OR | CI | OR | CI | OR | CI | OR | CI | |
Overall | 85.8 | ||||||||||||
Age | 0.986 | 0.952–1.021 | 0.992 | 0.957–1.028 | 0.995 | 0.960–1.032 | 0.993 | 0.958–1.030 | 0.994 | 0.958–1.031 | 0.994 | 0.959–1.031 | |
Gender | |||||||||||||
Male | 87.0 | Ref | Ref | Ref | Ref | Ref | Ref | ||||||
Female | 84.5 | 0.814 | 0.649–1.021 | 0.855 | 0.676–1.081 | 0.811 | 0.640–1.028 | 0.803 | 0.633–1.019 | 0.797 | 0.628–1.011 | 0.812 | 0.641–1.030 |
Ethnicity | |||||||||||||
White | 86.0 | Ref | Ref | Ref | Ref | Ref | Ref | ||||||
Non-white | 80.2 | 0.658 | 0.407–1.064 | 0.671 | 0.411–1.094 | 0.702 | 0.429–1.148 | 0.701 | 0.428–1.148 | 0.707 | 0.431–1.159 | 0.693 | 0.423–1.135 |
Marital status | |||||||||||||
Married/living as married | 88.1 | Ref | Ref | Ref | Ref | Ref | Ref | ||||||
Single | 78.8 | 0.499 | 0.353–0.704** | 0.556 | 0.391–0.792** | 0.551 | 0.387–0.785** | 0.552 | 0.387–0.787** | 0.550 | 0.386–0.785** | 0.551 | 0.387–0.785** |
Wid/sep/div | 82.5 | 0.636 | 0.494–0.820** | 0.749 | 0.575–0.975* | 0.760 | 0.583–0.990* | 0.759 | 0.582–0.991* | 0.761 | 0.583–0.993* | 0.760 | 0.583–0.991* |
Social grade | |||||||||||||
A/B | 92.4 | Ref | Ref | Ref | Ref | Ref | Ref | ||||||
C1 | 86.8 | 0.544 | 0.367–0.805** | 0.572 | 0.385–0.850** | 0.587 | 0.395–0.873** | 0.584 | 0.392–0.869** | 0.587 | 0.394–0.873** | 0.583 | 0.392–0.867** |
C2 | 84.9 | 0.463 | 0.312–0.687** | 0.476 | 0.320–0.708** | 0.496 | 0.333–0.738** | 0.478 | 0.321–0.712** | 0.483 | 0.324–0.721** | 0.483 | 0.324–0.719** |
D/E | 81.0 | 0.352 | 0.248–0.499** | 0.401 | 0.280–0.573** | 0.418 | 0.292–0.598** | 0.412 | 0.287–0.590** | 0.416 | 0.290–0.597** | 0.411 | 0.287–0.588** |
ABACUS wave | |||||||||||||
Wave 1 | 85.8 | Ref | Ref | Ref | Ref | Ref | Ref | ||||||
Wave 2 | 84.8 | 0.925 | 0.703–1.218 | 0.962 | 0.728–1.271 | 1.008 | 0.761–1.335 | 1.004 | 0.757–1.331 | 1.012 | 0.762–1.343 | 1.013 | 0.764–1.343 |
Wave 3 | 85.4 | 0.973 | 0.690–1.372 | 1.014 | 0.715–1.439 | 0.999 | 0.703–1.421 | 1.010 | 0.710–1.438 | 1.004 | 0.705–1.430 | 1.008 | 0.709–1.434 |
Wave 4 | 87.6 | 1.172 | 0.827–1.662 | 1.254 | 0.881–1.785 | 1.275 | 0.894–1.818 | 1.291 | 0.903–1.845 | 1.291 | 0.903–1.845 | 1.286 | 0.901–1.836 |
Cancer worry frequency | |||||||||||||
Never | 82.3 | Ref | Ref | Ref | |||||||||
Occasionally/sometimes | 88.4 | 1.641 | 1.300–2.072** | 1.568 | 1.234–1.992** | 1.151 | 0.860–1.541 | ||||||
Often/very often | 86.8 | 1.408 | 0.813–2.439 | 1.481 | 0.847–2.590 | 1.135 | 0.608–2.121 | ||||||
Cancer worry intensity | |||||||||||||
Not at all | 81.0 | Ref | Ref | Ref | |||||||||
Slightly | 89.7 | 2.033 | 1.570–2.633** | 1.993 | 1.532–2.593** | 1.841 | 1.350–2.510** | ||||||
Quite/extremely | 86.6 | 1.510 | 1.112–2.051** | 1.611 | 1.175–2.208** | 1.482 | 1.020–2.154* | ||||||
Cancer worry (combined) | |||||||||||||
None | 81.3 | Ref | Ref | ||||||||||
Moderate | 88.2 | 1.717 | 1.332–2.212** | 1.665 | 1.285–2.158** | ||||||||
High | 86.6 | 1.478 | 1.081–2.020* | 1.556 | 1.127–2.150** | ||||||||
R2 (Nagelkerke) | 0.042 | 0.052 | 0.062 | 0.062 | 0.053 |
wid: widowed; sep: separated; div: divorced.
* p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01.
We then compared Model 1 with the four models that contained the sociodemographic variables as well as cancer worry frequency (Model 2), cancer worry intensity (Model 3), both (Model 4), or the combined cancer worry measure (Model 5; see Table 2). All models explained significantly more variance than Model 1 (Model 2 (2) = 14.0, p < 0.001, Model 3 (2)=28.3, p < 0.001, Model 4 (2) = 29.2, p < 0.001, and Model 5 (2)=15.8, p < 0.001).
Compared with never worrying, worrying about cancer occasionally or sometimes was significantly and positively associated with FOBt screening intentions when all sociodemographic variables were adjusted for (82% vs. 88% reported positive intention; OR = 1.57, 95% CI 1.23–1.99; Model 2), but there was no association with worrying often or very often. Similarly, compared with not being anxious (81% reported positive intention), reporting slight vs. quite a bit or extreme cancer worry intensity increased the odds of intending to do the FOBt (90%; OR = 1.99, 95% CI 1.53–2.59; 87%; OR = 1.61, 95% CI 1.18–2.21, respectively; Model 3). When both measures were combined in a single model (Model 4), cancer worry frequency was no longer significantly associated with FOBt screening intentions, but cancer worry intensity remained significantly associated. For the combined measure (Model 5), both moderate and high levels of cancer worry were significantly and positively associated with FOBt screening intentions, but this model explained less variance (Nagelkerke’s R2 = .053) than Models 3 or 4 (Nagelkerke’s R2 = .062 and .062, respectively). The difference between Models 3 and 4 was non-significant (χ2 diff(2) = 0.90, p = 0.64). Thus, Model 3 (cancer worry intensity) appeared the most parsimonious.
After excluding those with missing data on the cancer worry and uptake variables, a sample of N = 2318 remained for analysis (Table 3). Overall, 78% of respondents reported having done the stool test at least once. Cancer screening uptake was significantly and positively associated with age. Self-reported past FOBt uptake was significantly lower in those who were single (63%) compared with those who were married or living as married (80%), and in those from lower social grades (C2: 78%, DE: 72%) compared with those from the highest social grade (AB: 83%). These associations remained significant when all other sociodemographic variables were adjusted for in Model 1 (Table 3). Model 1 explained 5.8% of the variance in the data.
Table 3.
Total | Unadjusted |
Model 1 |
Model 2 |
Model 3 |
Model 4 |
Model 5 |
|||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
(%) | OR | CI | OR | CI | OR | CI | OR | CI | OR | CI | OR | CI | |
Overall | 77.5 | ||||||||||||
Age | 1.049 | 1.018–1.082** | 1.059 | 1.026–1.092** | 1.059 | 1.027–1.093** | 1.060 | 1.027–1.094** | 1.058 | 1.026–1.092** | 1.060 | 1.028–1.094** | |
Gender | |||||||||||||
Male | 76.9 | Ref | Ref | Ref | Ref | Ref | Ref | ||||||
Female | 78.2 | 1.078 | 0.887–1.310 | 1.108 | 0.904–1.358 | 1.091 | 0.889–1.339 | 1.060 | 0.863–1.303 | 1.068 | 0.869–1.312 | 1.071 | 0.872–1.315 |
Ethnicity | |||||||||||||
White | 77.8 | Ref | Ref | Ref | Ref | Ref | Ref | ||||||
Non-white | 69.4 | 0.645 | 0.415–1.003 | 0.721 | 0.459–1.135 | 0.726 | 0.461–1.143 | 0.741 | 0.470–1.169 | 0.732 | 0.463–1.157 | 0.733 | 0.465–1.155 |
Marital status | |||||||||||||
Married/living as married | 80.1 | Ref | Ref | Ref | Ref | Ref | Ref | ||||||
Single | 62.7 | 0.417 | 0.309–0.562** | 0.483 | 0.355–0.656** | 0.484 | 0.356–0.659** | 0.484 | 0.356–0.659** | 0.485 | 0.357–0.661** | 0.484 | 0.356–0.658** |
Wid/sep/div | 76.5 | 0.810 | 0.645–1.016 | 0.867 | 0.682–1.101 | 0.871 | 0.685–1.107 | 0.879 | 0.691–1.117 | 0.874 | 0.687–1.112 | 0.877 | 0.690–1.115 |
Social grade | |||||||||||||
A/B | 83.3 | Ref | Ref | Ref | Ref | Ref | Ref | ||||||
C1 | 80.2 | 0.815 | 0.598–1.111 | 0.828 | 0.604–1.136 | 0.832 | 0.607–1.141 | 0.846 | 0.616–1.161 | 0.839 | 0.611–1.153 | 0.841 | 0.613–1.154 |
C2 | 77.5 | 0.691 | 0.504–0.947* | 0.696 | 0.506–0.959* | 0.699 | 0.507–0.964* | 0.701 | 0.508–0.966* | 0.690 | 0.500–0.952* | 0.702 | 0.509–0.967* |
D/E | 71.7 | 0.508 | 0.388–0.665** | 0.540 | 0.408–0.716** | 0.542 | 0.408–0.719** | 0.551 | 0.415–0.732** | 0.540 | 0.406–0.718** | 0.545 | 0.411–0.723** |
ABACUS wave | |||||||||||||
Wave 1 | 80.7 | Ref | Ref | Ref | Ref | Ref | Ref | ||||||
Wave 2 | 80.2 | 0.972 | 0.751–1.257 | 1.034 | 0.796–1.343 | 1.057 | 0.813–1.375 | 1.079 | 0.829–1.406 | 1.072 | 0.823–1.397 | 1.080 | 0.829–1.407 |
Wave 3 | 70.7 | 0.580 | 0.439–0.765** | 0.610 | 0.459–0.810** | 0.616 | 0.463–0.819** | 0.616 | 0.463–0.821** | 0.624 | 0.468–0.831** | 0.618 | 0.464–0.823** |
Wave 4 | 71.2 | 0.593 | 0.452–0.778** | 0.626 | 0.475–0.825** | 0.634 | 0.480–0.835** | 0.642 | 0.486–0.848** | 0.643 | 0.487–0.850** | 0.642 | 0.486–0.848** |
Cancer worry frequency | |||||||||||||
Never | 76.1 | Ref | Ref | Ref | |||||||||
Occasionally or sometimes | 78.1 | 1.122 | 0.919–1.370 | 1.093 | 0.889–1.344 | 0.797 | 0.617–1.031 | ||||||
Often or very often | 83.0 | 1.540 | 0.920–2.577 | 1.485 | 0.878–2.513 | 1.013 | 0.566–1.814 | ||||||
Cancer worry intensity | |||||||||||||
Not at all | 72.6 | Ref | Ref | Ref | |||||||||
Slightly | 80.5 | 1.558 | 1.254–1.935** | 1.515 | 1.213–1.892** | 1.722 | 1.317–2.251** | ||||||
Quite and extremely | 80.6 | 1.563 | 1.188–2.056** | 1.545 | 1.163–2.053** | 1.702 | 1.216–2.384** | ||||||
Cancer worry (combined) | |||||||||||||
None | 73.7 | Ref | Ref | ||||||||||
Moderate | 78.6 | 1.314 | 1.055–1.636* | 1.291 | 1.030–1.618* | ||||||||
High | 80.5 | 1.476 | 1.114–1.955** | 1.457 | 1.088–1.952* | ||||||||
R2 (Nagelkerke) | 0.058 | 0.059 | 0.068 | 0.070 | 0.063 |
wid: widowed; sep: separated; div: divorced.
*p < 0.05; **p < 0.01.
We then compared Model 1 with the other four models. Model 3 ((2)=16.5, p < 0.001), Model 4 ((2)=20.0, p < 0.001), and Model 5 ((2)=7.79, p=0.02) explained significantly more variance than Model 1, but Model 2 did not ((2)=2.57, p=0.28), i.e. cancer worry frequency was not associated with self-reported past uptake of FOBt (Table 3). In Model 3, compared with not being anxious at all (73% uptake), being slightly, or quite a bit or extremely anxious about cancer was associated with increased odds of FOBt uptake (81%; OR = 1.52, 95% CI 1.21–1.89; and 81%; OR = 1.55, 95% CI 1.16–2.05, respectively). Cancer worry frequency was not significantly associated with FOBt uptake in Model 4 when cancer worry intensity was also included in the model. For the combined cancer worry measure, moderate and high levels of worry were significantly associated with FOBt screening uptake (Model 5), but this model explained less variance than Models 3 and 4, which again explained most variance (Nagelkerke’s R2=.068 and .070, respectively). There was no difference between these models in the amount of variance explained ((2)=3.49, p=0.18). Thus, Model 3 (cancer worry intensity) again appeared the most parsimonious.
We repeated all analyses using the continuous cancer worry variables and an interaction term for cancer worry frequency and intensity to test their additive effects. We also tested for curvilinear effects in these analyses. The full results are reported in the Online Supplement. Briefly, both cancer worry frequency and intensity were significantly associated with FOBt screening intentions, both linearly and curvilinearly (Online Supplement Table S2). When both were combined into a single model, either separately or their interaction, only cancer worry intensity remained significantly associated with screening intentions, while the variance explained increased only marginally compared with a model with only cancer worry intensity (R2=.066 vs. R2=.065). The results were very similar for self-reported FOBt screening uptake (Table 2, and Online Supplement Table S3). The results from these sensitivity analyses therefore also suggest that the most parsimonious model of FOBt screening intentions and uptake is one with cancer worry intensity.
Discussion
This study found that being at least slightly and at least occasionally worried about cancer was relatively common, with nearly two out of three people being either moderately or very worried on the measure that combined frequency and intensity of cancer worry. A comparison of the two measures indicated that respondents did not necessarily worry about cancer often, but that many experienced moderate to high levels of worry when they thought about it. Furthermore, it was this level of cancer worry intensity, rather than the frequency of worrying, that seemed to be associated with CRC screening intention and uptake. Our findings therefore seem to suggest that, contrary to common practice, a single item on cancer worry intensity rather than frequency could be used when trying to explain variance in uptake of colorectal cancer screening.
This is the first study of FOBt screening in the UK to include measures of both cancer worry frequency and intensity. It is also the first study to explicitly examine which of these measures is more strongly associated with intentions and with (self-reported) screening uptake.
It should be noted that previous research did not just find differences between different aspects of cancer worry, but also important inconsistencies according to screening modality (FOBt vs. endoscopic screening), programme (cervical vs. breast vs. colorectal), population (US vs. UK vs. Singapore), and whether measures of screening intentions or uptake are used. For this reason, it is difficult to make strong claims about the practical implications of the findings. Future research should investigate psychological antecedents of cancer worry intensity. While simply increasing worry about cancer would be potentially unethical and an inappropriate way to increase engagement with cancer screening, it is possible that what underlies our observation relates to more general perceptions of cancer, or indeed cancer screening (e.g. lack of perceived risk and optimistic bias), which could be addressed without necessarily ‘scaring’ people into attending cancer screening.
The sociodemographic patterns in FOBt screening intentions and uptake in our study were similar to previous studies,6,7 with intentions and uptake lower in those from more deprived backgrounds and those who are single. Screening intentions and uptake were also lower in those from non-White ethnic backgrounds, although these differences were not significant in our study. Our study may have been underpowered to detect these differences due to the small percentage of those from a non-White ethnic background in our sample (5%), which is nevertheless consistent with the ethnic distribution for this age group.39 As would be expected, we found a small but statistically significant positive association between age and ‘ever’ having done the FOB test, as those who are older would have received more invitations to complete the test, but we did not find an association between age and intentions to do the test when next invited. Self-reported ‘ever’ uptake of the FOBt (69%) was similar to self-reported ever uptake in other studies in the UK (70%),37,40 and has been shown to be highly consistent with actual uptake of FOBt screening.37
This study had some limitations. It is the first study to suggest that cancer worry intensity, rather than frequency, may be more strongly associated with uptake of screening for colorectal cancer, although these constructs were measured using single items that have inherent validity problems, and face validity was the only accessible psychometric property of the measures used in the current study. More work is needed to validate measures of cancer worry frequency and intensity, and future studies should try to replicate our findings to see if measures of cancer worry in large population-based studies should be changed from frequency to intensity items. We note that the single item measure relating to cancer worry in the US HINTS has already been changed from a frequency to an intensity-based item from the 2003, 2005, and 2008 to the 2011–2013 and 2017 surveys.33 The questions about cancer worry frequency and cancer worry intensity were about cancer in general, not specifically about colorectal cancer. This may have affected the strength of the associations, as previous research shows that attitudes may differ between cancer types,5,41 and future studies should use measures specific to colorectal cancer. In addition, few people scored the highest two levels of cancer worry frequency (often and very often). We therefore chose to collapse these categories, but the small sample size, even in the collapsed category, may have limited the power to detect any effects of frequent cancer worry on screening intention and uptake, and future studies may wish to oversample these levels of cancer worry frequency to formally test these associations. A further limitation is that this study only measured intensity and frequency of cancer worry, while various literature reviews of cancer worry in the general population suggest that there may be a variety of sources of cancer worry that may have different behavioural effects.8,25,26 In our recent review of the qualitative literature, we provide a comprehensive taxonomy of cancer worries related to cancer screening, including cancer worry as a general and diffuse worry, and worries about specific aspects of cancer, such as treatment, death, or the social consequences of a diagnosis.25 A recent population-based survey of self-reported cancer screening uptake found differential behavioural associations by type of cancer worry,30 and future studies should therefore include more comprehensive measures of cancer worry whenever possible. Items used to assess general feelings of cancer worry, such as those used in the current study, may nevertheless give a meaningful general overview of whether the cancer worry emotion tends to favour uptake of a particular type of screening or not. Furthermore, excluding people with missing data introduced a certain amount of selection bias, as indicated by the level of intention and uptake in the final analytic sample (86% and 77% respectively). We adjusted for the effect of sociodemographic confounders, but there may be other confounding variables, such as family history of colorectal cancer and physician recommendation, that may influence the association between cancer worry and screening uptake, and could be included in future studies. Finally, due to the cross-sectional nature of the study, it is not possible to draw any conclusions about the causal nature of the relationships or their temporal development. Prospective, longitudinal studies could examine how cancer worry frequency and intensity develop over time and in response to screening attendance.
Conclusion
This large cross-sectional analysis was the first to compare cancer worry frequency versus cancer worry intensity in the context of CRC screening. Our findings suggest that cancer worry intensity, rather than frequency, is informative when understanding engagement with CRC screening using gFOBt. Future studies should use prospective designs and objective uptake across different CRC screening modalities, and other cancer screening programmes.
Supplemental Material
Supplemental Material for Cancer worry frequency vs. intensity and self-reported colorectal cancer screening uptake: A population-based study by Charlotte Vrinten, Sandro Stoffel, Rachael H Dodd, Jo Waller, Yoryos Lyratzopoulos and Christian von Wagner in Journal of Medical Screening
Acknowledgements
We acknowledge the intellectual contribution to the ABACUS surveys made by Professor Jane Wardle, before her death in October 2015. We also thank Miss Evelina Baltrukaityte, who performed some preliminary analyses on part of the current dataset as part of her MSc dissertation.
Declaration of conflicting interests
The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
Funding
The author(s) disclosed receipt of the following financial support for the research, authorship and/or publication of this article: This work was supported by Cancer Research UK (Grant Number C1418/A14134).
Supplemental Material
Supplemental material is available for this article online.
References
- 1.Ferlay J, Soerjomataram I, Dikshit R, et al. Cancer incidence and mortality worldwide: sources, methods and major patterns in GLOBOCAN 2012. Int J Cancer 2015; 1: 136. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Cancer Research UK. Bowel cancer statistics, www.cancerresearchuk.org/health-professional/cancer-statistics/statistics-by-cancer-type/bowel-cancer (accessed 15 November 2018).
- 3.Hewitson P, Glasziou P, Watson E, et al. Cochrane systematic review of colorectal cancer screening using the fecal occult blood test (Hemoccult): an update. Am J Gastroenterol 2008; 103: 1541–1549. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Berkowitz Z, Hawkins NA, Peipins LA, et al. Beliefs, risk perceptions, and gaps in knowledge as barriers to colorectal cancer screening in older adults. J Am Geriatr Soc 2008; 56: 307–314. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Moser RP, McCaul K, Peters E, et al. Associations of perceived risk and worry with cancer health-protective actions data from the Health Information National Trends Survey (HINTS). J Health Psychol 2007; 12: 53–65. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Power E, Miles A, von Wagner C, et al. Uptake of colorectal cancer screening: system, provider and individual factors and strategies to improve participation. Future Oncol 2009; 5: 1371–1388. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.von Wagner C, Baio G, Raine R, et al. Inequalities in participation in an organized national colorectal cancer screening programme: results from the first 2.6 million invitations in England. Int J Epidemiol 2011; 40: 712–718. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Hay JL, Buckley TR, Ostroff JS. The role of cancer worry in cancer screening: a theoretical and empirical review of the literature. Psychooncology 2005; 14: 517–534. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Vrinten C, van Jaarsveld CHM, Waller J, et al. The structure and demographic correlates of cancer fear. BMC Cancer 2014; 14: 597. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Murphy PJ, Marlow LAV, Waller J, et al. What is it about cancer that would worry people? A population-based survey of adults in England. BMC Cancer 2018; 18: 86. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Choi E, Lee YY, Suh M, et al. Associations of perceived risk and cancer worry for colorectal cancer with screening behaviour. J Health Psychol 2018; 23: 840–852. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Ferrer RA, Hall KL, Portnoy DB, et al. Relationships among health perceptions vary depending on stage of readiness for colorectal cancer screening. Health Psychol 2011; 30: 525–535. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Mack LA, Cook LS, Temple WJ, et al. Colorectal cancer screening among first-degree relatives of colorectal cancer patients: benefits and barriers. Ann Surg Oncol 2009; 16: 2092–2100. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Robb KA, Power E, Atkin W, et al. Ethnic differences in participation in flexible sigmoidoscopy screening in the UK. J Med Screen 2008; 15: 130–136. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Senore C, Armaroli P, Silvani M, et al. Comparing different strategies for colorectal cancer screening in Italy: predictors of patients’ participation. Am J Gastroenterol 2010; 105: 188–198. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.McQueen A, Vernon SW, Meissner H, et al. Are there gender differences in colorectal cancer test use, prevalence and correlates? Cancer Epidemiol Biomarkers Prev 2006; 15: 782–791. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Vrinten C, Waller J, Von Wagner C, et al. Cancer fear: facilitator and deterrent to participation in colorectal cancer screening. Cancer Epidemiol Biomarkers Prev 2015; 24: 400–405. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Wong RK, Wong ML, Chan YH, et al. Gender differences in predictors of colorectal cancer screening uptake: a national cross sectional study based on the health belief model. BMC Public Health 2013; 13: 677. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Attarabeen OF, Sambamoorthi U, Larkin KT, et al. Colon cancer worry in Appalachia. J Community Health 2018; 43: 79–88. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Azaiza F, Cohen M. Colorectal cancer screening, intentions, and predictors in Jewish and Arab Israelis: a population-based study. Health Educ Behav 2008; 35: 478–493. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Boonyasiriwat W, Hung M, Hon SD, et al. Intention to undergo colonoscopy screening among relatives of colorectal cancer cases: a theory-based model. Ann Behav Med 2014; 47: 280–291. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Kremers SP, Mesters I, Pladdet IE, et al. Participation in a sigmoidoscopic colorectal cancer screening program: a pilot study. Cancer Epidemiol Biomarkers Prev 2000; 9: 1127–1130. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Llanos AA, Pennell ML, Young GS, et al. No association between colorectal cancer worry and screening uptake in Appalachian Ohio. J Public Health 2015; 37: 322–327. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Miles A, Rainbow S, von Wagner C. Cancer fatalism and poor self-rated health mediate the association between socioeconomic status and uptake of colorectal cancer screening in England. Cancer Epidemiol Biomarkers Prev 2011; 20: 2132–2140. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Vrinten C, McGregor LM, Heinrich M, et al. What do people fear about cancer? A systematic review and meta-synthesis of cancer fears in the general population. Psychooncology 2017; 25: 1070–1079. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Consedine NS, Magai C, Krivoshekova YS, et al. Fear, anxiety, worry, and breast cancer screening behaviour: a critical review. Cancer Epidemiol Biomarkers Prev 2004; 13: 501–510. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Clemow L, Costanza ME, Haddad WP, et al. Underutilizers of mammography screening today: characteristics of women planning, undecided about, and not planning a mammogram. Ann Behav Med 2000; 22: 80–88. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Thewes B, Butow P, Zachariae R, et al. Fear of cancer recurrence: a systematic literature review of self-report measures. Psychooncology 2012; 21: 571–587. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 5th ed Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing, 2013. [Google Scholar]
- 30.Quaife SL, Waller J, von Wagner C, et al. Cancer worries and uptake of breast, cervical, and colorectal cancer screening: a population based survey in England. J Med Screen 2019; 26: 3–10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Office for National Statistics, 2011 Census questionnaire for England, http://webarchive.nationalarchives.gov.uk/20160105225826/http://www.ons.gov.uk/ons/guide-method/census/2011/how-our-census-works/how-we-took-the-2011-census/how-we-collected-the-information/questionnaires–delivery–completion-and-return/index.html (2011, accessed 15 November 2018).
- 32.National Readership Survey (n.. d.). Social grade, www.nrs.co.uk/nrs-print/lifestyle-and-classification-data/social-grade/ ( accessed 15 November 2018).
- 33.National Cancer Institute (n.. d.). Health Information National Trends Survey (HINTS), https://hints.cancer.gov (accessed 15 November 2018).
- 34.Agustina E, Dodd R, Waller J, et al. Understanding middle-aged and older adults’ first associations with the word ‘cancer’: a mixed-methods study in England. Psychooncology 2018; 27: 309–315. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Vrinten C, Boniface D, Lo SH, et al. Does psychosocial stress exacerbate avoidant responses to cancer information in those who are afraid of cancer? A population-based survey among older adults in England. Psychol Health 2018; 33: 117–129. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Weinstein ND, Sandman PM. Chapter two: the precaution adoption process model and its application In: DiClemente RJ, Crosby RA, Kegler MC. (eds) Emerging theories in health promotion practice and research: strategies for improving public health. San Francisco, CA, USA: Jossey-Bass, 2002, pp.16–39. [Google Scholar]
- 37.Lo SH, Waller J, Vrinten C, et al. Self-reported and objectively recorded colorectal cancer screening participation in England. J Med Screen 2016; 23: 17–23. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Korn EL, Graubard BI. Epidemiologic studies utilizing surveys: accounting for the sampling design. Am J Public Health 1991; 81: 1166–1173. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Office for National Statistics. 2001 Census: national report for England and Wales, http://webarchive.nationalarchives.gov.uk/20151013225514/http://www.ons.gov.uk/ons/rel/census/census-2001-national-report-for-england-and-wales/national-report-for-england-and-wales-part-1/index.html (2003, accessed 15 November 2018).
- 40.Lo SH, Halloran S, Snowball J, et al. Colorectal cancer screening uptake over three biennial invitation rounds in the English bowel cancer screening programme. Gut 2015; 64: 282–291. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Baron-Epel O, Klin A. Cancer as perceived by a middle-aged Jewish urban population in Israel. Oncol Nurs Forum 2009; 36: E326–E334. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Supplemental Material for Cancer worry frequency vs. intensity and self-reported colorectal cancer screening uptake: A population-based study by Charlotte Vrinten, Sandro Stoffel, Rachael H Dodd, Jo Waller, Yoryos Lyratzopoulos and Christian von Wagner in Journal of Medical Screening