Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2019 Nov 14;8:e50482. doi: 10.7554/eLife.50482

Shifts in myeloarchitecture characterise adolescent development of cortical gradients

Casey Paquola 1,†,, Richard AI Bethlehem 2,3,†,, Jakob Seidlitz 2,4, Konrad Wagstyl 2, Rafael Romero-Garcia 2, Kirstie J Whitaker 2,5, Reinder Vos de Wael 1, Guy B Williams 6; NSPN Consortium, Petra E Vértes 2,5, Daniel S Margulies 7, Boris Bernhardt 1,†,, Edward T Bullmore 2,6,†,
Editors: Joshua I Gold8, Theodore Satterthwaite9
PMCID: PMC6855802  PMID: 31724948

Abstract

We studied an accelerated longitudinal cohort of adolescents and young adults (n = 234, two time points) to investigate dynamic reconfigurations in myeloarchitecture. Intracortical profiles were generated using magnetization transfer (MT) data, a myelin-sensitive magnetic resonance imaging contrast. Mixed-effect models of depth specific intracortical profiles demonstrated two separate processes i) overall increases in MT, and ii) flattening of the MT profile related to enhanced signal in mid-to-deeper layers, especially in heteromodal and unimodal association cortices. This development was independent of morphological changes. Enhanced MT in mid-to-deeper layers was found to spatially co-localise specifically with gene expression markers of oligodendrocytes. Interregional covariance analysis revealed that these intracortical changes contributed to a gradual differentiation of higher-order from lower-order systems. Depth-dependent trajectories of intracortical myeloarchitectural development contribute to the maturation of structural hierarchies in the human neocortex, providing a model for adolescent development that bridges microstructural and macroscopic scales of brain organisation.

Research organism: Human

Introduction

Adolescence is a crucial phase in biological and psychosocial maturation and involves large-scale reconfigurations of brain anatomy (Paus et al., 2008). Previous histopathological and neuroimaging studies have shown marked age-related changes in brain structure during this sensitive period (Giedd et al., 2015; Raznahan et al., 2011; Sowell et al., 2004). This is particularly evident in magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) assessments of the macro-structural morphology of cortical regions, which have revealed regionally-variable dynamics of cortical thinning in adolescence (Giedd et al., 2015; Raznahan et al., 2011; Sowell et al., 2004). While these findings confirm the existence of strong biological forces shaping adolescent brain anatomy, morphometric analyses typically only quantify shape changes of the inner and outer cortical boundaries. In turn, these analyses may not be specific for microstructural changes occurring within the cortical mantle, which ultimately play key roles in adolescent development of cortical connectivity and function (Huntenburg et al., 2017). Several recent neuroimaging studies assessed intracortical microstructure in adolescence. One promising imaging technique is magnetisation transfer (MT), an MRI acquisition sequence that is sensitive to how water molecules interact with macromolecules in the brain, notably myelin (Schmierer et al., 2007). Although techniques such as MT cannot be equated with cortical myelin content per se (Serres et al., 2009a; Serres et al., 2009b), the MT parameter is dominated by myelin-related molecules making this technique a viable in vivo proxy for the contrast seen histologically in myelin basic protein (Koenig, 1991; Odrobina et al., 2005; Whitaker et al., 2016). A post mortem study in patients with multiple sclerosis has also shown that MT measures scale with demyelination and remyelination, suggesting dependence of this measure on myelin content (Schmierer et al., 2007). While correlated with alternative myelin-sensitive imaging, MT is arguably the strongest in vivo marker of myelin, based on its spatial correspondence with myelin basic protein (Whitaker et al., 2016). While recent studies confirmed associations between adolescence and changes in myelin-markers (Carey et al., 2018; Grydeland et al., 2019; Kwon et al., 2018; Whitaker et al., 2016), findings have been mainly cross-sectional, precluding inferences on intra-individual trajectories. Furthermore, studies have generally focused on specific depths or intracortical averages, ignoring depth-dependent dynamics and thus not addressing potential systematic shifts in cortical myeloarchitecture and lamination in adolescence.

Quantitative profiling of intracortical properties across cortical depths, and specifically parameterization using central moments (Papoulis and Unnikrishna Pillai, 2002), has been proposed to characterise cytoarchitecture in seminal histological work (Schleicher et al., 1999) and capture inter-individual variation (Amunts et al., 1999). In essence, studying the mean (first moment) of MT profiles perpendicular to the cortical mantle allows inferences on overall myelin content while higher order moments can address depth-dependent changes (Figure 1). Analysis of skewness (third moment) can contrast relative properties of deep and superficial cortical layers, and depth is a critical dimension of laminar differentiation that relates to architectural complexity (Zilles et al., 2002) and cortical hierarchy (Mesulam, 1998). Applied to adolescence, such an analysis offers a non-invasive window into cortical architecture, which may recapitulate and expand classical histological findings showing overall increases in cortical myelin in adolescence (Kaes, 1907) as well as transcriptional analyses suggesting that laminar signatures reflect cortical development (Miller et al., 2014). In addition to studying regional variations in cortical architecture, depth-dependent profiling theoretically lends a framework to tap into the large-scale coordination of different brain areas. One such approach, known as microstructure profile covariance (MPC), quantifies inter-regional similarity in microstructure at a single subject-level (Paquola et al., 2019). Previous research has demonstrated the utility of MPC for understanding large-scale patterns of cortical architecture, specifically illustrating a sensory-fugal gradient of microstructural differentiation in both post mortem and in vivo datasets (Paquola et al., 2019). This axis describes gradual transitions from primary sensory and motor regions with high laminar differentiation, through association cortex toward paralimbic areas with increasingly dysgranular appearance (Mesulam, 1998). Given prior evidence that microstructural similarity predicts cortico-cortical connectivity (Barbas and Rempel-Clower, 1997), tracking age-related changes in MPC provides an unprecedented way to probe coordinated maturation of microstructural networks during the critical adolescent period, moving towards a network perspective of structural brain development (Alexander-Bloch et al., 2013; Váša et al., 2018).

Figure 1. Intracortical MT depth profiling.

(A) Left. Equivolumetric surfaces overlaid on an MT image of a single subject, also showing an example vertex along which MT intensity is sampled (example MT profile in grey, with lighter tones represent higher MT intensity). Right. Schema of first and third moments. (B) Left. Examples of microscopic myelin-stained sections (inverted image shows myelin in lighter tones) (Vogt and Vogt, 1919; Vogt, 1911), with corresponding profiles and group-average MT profile from the same regions extracted in vivo. V1 = primary visual cortex. V2 = secondary visual area. SPL = superior parietal lobule. ACC = anterior cingulate cortex. Right. Dot-plots showing the moments for each exemplar profile. (C) Baseline group-average of first and third moment plotted on the cortical surface, and within levels of laminar differentiation (Mesulam, 2002; Paquola et al., 2019). The scatterplot shows the spatial overlap between first and third moment, coloured by level of laminar differentiation. Findings on the second and fourth moment are shown in Figure 1—figure supplement 1, and with an alternative cytoarchitectural taxonomy in Figure 1—figure supplement 2. (D) Microstructure profile covariance (MPC) was estimated between each pair of nodes based on the partial correlation of two nodes’ MT profiles (black), controlling for the cortex-wide mean intensity profile (grey dashed). Baseline MPC matrices were averaged across the group, and diffusion map embedding was employed to order regions according to the principal gradient of microstructural differentiation (G1MPC). Scatterplots show the relationship between node-wise loadings onto the principle gradient with MT moments.

Figure 1.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1. Baseline properties of SD and kurtosis.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1.

(A) Examples of hyper-stained myelin pictures (Nieuwenhuys and Broere, 2017; Vogt and Vogt, 1919; Vogt, 1911) with corresponding myelin profile and group-average MT profile from the same region. V1 = primary visual cortex. V2 = secondary visual area. SPL = superior parietal lobule. ACC = anterior cingulate cortex. Atlas of laminar differentiation projected on the cortical surface (Nieuwenhuys and Broere, 2017; Vogt, 1911). (B) Moment values across levels of laminar differentiation from the myelin pictures shown in (A). (C) Moment values from across levels of laminar differentiation from the MT profile shown in (A). (D) Group-average MT moment per node projected onto the cortical surface. SD captures a very similar pattern to the mean (spatial overlap: mean ~SD: r = 0.62, p<0.001), whereas kurtosis was correlated to skewness (r = 0.58, p<0.001). (E) Density plots depict the distribution of group-average MT moments for each node within levels of laminar differentiation.

Figure 1—figure supplement 2. Replication with different classification of cytoarchitectural complexity (Von Economo and Koskinas, 2008).

Figure 1—figure supplement 2.

(A) Distribution of values across each cortical class. (B) Z-scores of relative over or under-representation of parcels showing an age-related effect within each class. Classes showing a significant effect of age related changes are denoted by asterixis (***=p < 0.001, *=p < 0.05).

The present study examined adolescent maturation of cortical architecture and microstructure using an accelerated longitudinal NeuroScience in Psychiatry Network dataset (Kiddle et al., 2018; 234 participants scanned twice). We translated quantitative profile analysis via statistical moment parameterisation, previously proposed for histological data, to surface-based MT imaging data and cross-referenced findings against established atlases of cytoarchitectural complexity and laminar differentiation (Von Economo and Koskinas, 2008). We tracked longitudinal changes in MT profile moments using linear mixed effect models, which leverage subject- and group-level effects to estimate microstructural changes across the entire age range (Carey et al., 2018). Based on histological work (Palomero-Gallagher and Zilles, 2019; Zilles et al., 2002), we hypothesized that specifically the first (mean) and third (skewness) central moments of the MT profiles would capture different biological mechanisms and exhibit divergent maturational trajectories. The mean was expected to largely encompass similar changes to those captured by previous in vivo imaging studies on overall MT changes (Carey et al., 2018; Grydeland et al., 2019; Kwon et al., 2018; Whitaker et al., 2016) corresponding to a shift in the overall cortical myelin content. Conversely, we anticipated skewness would capture shifts of MT intensity in the depth dimension of the intensity profile. Intracortical MT profile analysis was complemented by a series of cortical thickness and transcriptomic overlap analyses to assess morphological correlates and molecular underpinnings. In addition to studying regional MT profiles, we leveraged the MPC framework to tap into cortex-wide coordination of adolescent MT profile changes between different brain areas and consolidate our findings at a system level.

Results

Characterization of intracortical MT profiles

We studied the NSPN dataset, an accelerated longitudinal cohort that aggregates multimodal imaging data from 234 adolescents and young adults aged 14–27 (for details on cohort selection, processing, and quality control, see Materials and methods). Briefly, we generated cortical surface models based on T1-weighted MRI and co-registered the corresponding MT volumes to these surfaces. We systematically generated equivolumetric intracortical surfaces and sampled MT intensities along matched vertices perpendicular to the cortical mantle to build intracortical MT profiles (Figure 1A). Our vertex-wise technique leverages equivolumetric transformations that critically adjust surface placement in accordance to the folded cortical sheet (Wagstyl et al., 2018), thereby better coinciding with the position of the putative cortical laminae than equidistant or Laplace-field guided techniques (Waehnert et al., 2014). Our intracortical approach thus offered better precision and biological validity over conventional voxel-based morphometry techniques that may be agnostic to cortical topology and intracortical architecture, and that may amplify mixing of different tissue types (Ziegler et al., 2019). MT profiles at each vertex were parcellated in 1012 approximately equal sized nodes and parameterized via central statistical moments (Zilles et al., 2002). We focused on the first moment (mean across all cortical depths) and third moment (skewness across cortical depths), which are readily interpretable in terms of, respectively, mean myelin content and the ratio of myelination in deeper compared to more superficial layers; and have been studied in prior histological work (Zilles et al., 2002).

Baseline MT profiles, moments and covariance

At baseline (i.e., using the average across timepoint 1), the cortex-wide average MT profile shows a non-linear increase in intensity from the superficial layers, adjacent to the pial boundary, towards the deeper layers approaching the white matter boundary, consistent with prior literature (Whitaker et al., 2016; Figure 1A). We first assessed the correspondence of intracortical MT profiles with myeloarchitecture by comparing MT profiles with histological myelin stains from four regions of interest, representing the four levels of laminar differentiation (Mesulam, 2002; Palomero-Gallagher and Zilles, 2019; Vogt and Vogt, 1919; Vogt, 1911; Figure 1B, see Materials and methods for details on profile quantification). Although post mortem myelin stained sections and in vivo MT profiles differ in terms of resolution and specificity to myelin, we observed similar variations in mean and skewness of profiles across levels of laminar differentiation (Figure 1B), supporting the extension of profile analysis from histology to in vivo MT imaging. At a whole cortex-level, mean MT was highest in idiotypic cortex and decreased with less laminar differentiation, while skewness exhibited an opposite pattern (spatial correlation = −0.23, pspin <0.001; Figure 1C, Figure 1—figure supplements 12, Appendix 1—figure 1). Negative or near-zero skewness was observed in idiotypic and unimodal areas, relating to more even distribution of MT across cortical depths, whereas more positive skewness was observed in heteromodal and paralimbic areas, related to higher MT intensities in infragranular compared to supragranular layers (Zilles et al., 2002; Figure 1C).

We explored the topology of intracortical MT patterns using microstructure profile covariance (MPC). By correlating depth-wise MT profiles, the MPC procedure estimates inter-regional microstructural similarity and has previously been validated using a combination of post mortem and in vivo data (Paquola et al., 2019; Figure 1D). Diffusion map embedding, a nonlinear dimensionality reduction technique, was employed to resolve the principal axis of microstructural differentiation. The relative positioning of nodes in this embedding space informs on similarity of their covariance patterns. In line with previous work (Paquola et al., 2019), the first principal gradient within the baseline cohort was anchored on one end by idiotypic sensory regions and by paralimbic regions on the other end. This sensory-fugal gradient reflects systematic variations in the MT profiles; it was strongly correlated with MT profile skewness (r = 0.91, pspin <0.001; Figure 1D) and weakly with mean MT (r = 0.19, pspin <0.001).

Age-related changes in cortical depth MT profiles

Studying age-related changes in MT moments, we observed that adolescence led to a significant increase in the mean (qFDR <0.00625, % rate of change: [4.8 14.7] 95% CIs; Figure 2, Figure 2—figure supplement 1), suggesting overall increases in intracortical myelin content. Conversely, we observed a unique spatial pattern of decreases in skewness (qFDR <0.00625, % rate of change: [−179.2–34.6] 95% CIs; Figure 2). Depth-wise changes in MT intensity suggest that these decreases in skewness reflected a preferential increases in MT in mid to deeper layers, suggesting accumulation in myelin. Controlling for overall mean MT trajectories resulted in virtually identical patterns of skewness change, suggesting relative independence between skewness and mean MT trajectories in adolescence (Appendix 1—figures 24). The differential impact of age across levels of laminar differentiation was subsequently assessed by spin permutations (Alexander-Bloch et al., 2018). Across all moments, age-related changes in skewness were preferentially located in heteromodal (z > 3.06, p<0.043) and unimodal cortex (z > 3.24, p<0.024), while idiotypic nodes were less prominently represented than expected by chance (Figure 2—figure supplement 1 and Supplementary file 1).

Figure 2. Age-related changes in MT moments.

(A) Shifts in MT profiles and moments from lowest to highest age strata shown for exemplar regions of laminar differentiation (Figure 1). (B) Upper. Age-related changes in MT moments (qFDR <0.00625). Middle. t-statistic (mean ± SD) of age-related changes in MT intensity at each intracortical surface across significant regions. Mean increases were balanced across surfaces, whereas decreases in skewness were driven by preferential intensity increases at mid-to-deeper surfaces. Lower. Individual changes across significant regions, with regression lines depicting age-related changes across the investigated range.

Figure 2.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1. Age-related changes in SD and kurtosis.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1.

(A) Upper t-statistics projected on the cortical surfaces depict significant age-related changes in MT moments. Lower Scatter plots depict individual changes in average moment value across significant regions, with regression lines depicting the age-related changes. SD showed a steady increase during adolescence and preferentially impacted heteromodal cortices (z = 3.42, p=0.012), while paralimbic cortex was under-represented (z = −5.60, p<0.001) as assessed using spin premutation and count tables of significant regions per class of laminar differentiation. Kurtosis showed a steady and robust decrease during adolescent development that preferentially affected heteromodal cortex (z = 8.80, p<0.001). (B) Bar plots show the z-scores of the number of regions passing FDR correction for each class relative to a null-distribution generated from spin permutations of the t-statistic map (Alexander-Bloch et al., 2018). Significance is indicated by asterixis and based on Bonferroni corrected p-values across all moments and classes (***=p < 0.001, *=p < 0.05). Positive significant z-scores indicate that the number of regions passing FDR for that class are significantly higher than expected by chance, negative significant z-scores indicate the number is significantly lower (tables provided below).

Independence of age-related intracortical MT profiles changes from cortical morphology

To examine the specificity of MT profile trajectories to intracortical variations, we assessed robustness of findings against changes in cortical morphology and boundary definition (Bernhardt et al., 2018; Whitaker et al., 2016). To this end, we computed residual MT moments by controlling for thickness or interface blurring at each vertex within node-wise linear models. The spatial distribution of MT profile mean and skewness were virtually unchanged when data were additionally controlled for morphological and intensity confounds (r > 0.99; Appendix 1—figures 24). Age-related changes in MT moments were also virtually identical when using MT profile data corrected for cortical thickness and interface blurring (r > 0.98, Appendix 1—figures 24), supporting that MT profile trajectories were primarily driven by intracortical factors. Furthermore, spatial correlations suggested that MT moments were relatively independent of regional variations in laminar thickness derived from a post mortem volumetric reconstruction of a Merker-stained human brain (Wagstyl et al., 2018) (mean: −0.06 < r < 0.05. skewness: −0.17 < r < 0.02; Appendix 1—figure 5).

Age-related change in MT profiles co-localised with expression of oligodendroglial genes

To explore molecular substrates of our imaging results, we referenced our findings against post mortem gene expression maps provided by the Allen Institute for Brain Sciences using Neurovault API gene decoding (Gorgolewski et al., 2015; Gorgolewski et al., 2014; Hawrylycz et al., 2012). We identified genes whose expression pattern spatially resembled the maps of the age-related change from our in vivo MT analysis (Figure 2B; Hawrylycz et al., 2012). Including only those genes that passed multiple comparisons corrections (pFDR <0.05), we conducted enrichment analysis of standard mammalian phenotypes (Smith and Eppig, 2012), cell-specific expression analysis and developmental expression analysis across developmental time windows (Dougherty et al., 2010).

The pattern of age-related change in skewness showed a significant and specific transcriptomic signature (for unthresholded lists, see Data Availability). Cell specific expression analysis (Dougherty et al., 2010) suggested enrichment exclusively with oligodendrocytes (p<0.001 [at a specificity index threshold of 0.0001]; Figure 3A), confirming the association between myelination and decreased MT skewness. Developmental expression analysis (Dougherty et al., 2010) showed selective enrichment for adolescence and young adulthood (Figure 3B). Both analyses thus confirmed a spatial overlap of our findings from NSPN with genes associated with myelin processes, that are also enriched during adolescence. It should be noted however that individual gene expression assays only provide a snapshot of inherently dynamic processes (ArnatkevicIūtė et al., 2019). We detected enrichment of adolescent-linked genes based on cross-sectional developmental gene expression data. Specifically, genes showing significant spatial overlap in adult post-mortem brains from the AIBS were identical to those showing significant expression differences during adolescence in the BrainSpan dataset. While there was evidence for an adolescent developmental signal results are nevertheless indirect, also given that glial and oligodendrocyte associated genes may undergo expression changes during other periods of the lifespan (Soreq et al., 2017). Furthermore, we performed gene ontology analysis using a modified Fishers exact test, which captures the deviation from the expected gene rank (Kuleshov et al., 2016), on genes associated with decreasing skewness. This indicated a negative association with demyelination (Z = −2.16, p=0.001) and axon degeneration (Z = −2.34, p=0.011), reinforcing associations to myelin. In sum, genes negatively associated with age-related skewness reduction were less likely to be enriched for demyelination and were exclusively associated with oligodendrocytes. Given that the present data are based on healthy cortex, those same areas are not high in expression of ‘demyelinating’ genes, but instead show a negative signal (e.g., negative z-scores). This is expected since those areas of high expressing myelin genes are most vulnerable (Seidlitz et al., 2019). Thus, MT skewness changes during adolescent development appear to be strongly reflective of changes in myelination. None of the other moments provided a list of genes passing the threshold set by the false discovery rate procedure. However, exploratory rank-based correlations on the weights of each gene indicated a small correlation between mean and skewness related genes (rho = 0.14, p<0.001), indicating mean and skewness share a modest transcriptomic signature. Finally, it should be noted that the same analysis on baseline data only (average across a single scan per subject) from this cohort did not identify a whole genome transcriptional profile that overlapped with any of the baseline MT moments (Figure 3—figure supplement 1), reinforcing the added value of developmental data (e.g. measuring an age-related change) in detecting human neurodevelopmental processes by myelin-sensitive MRI.

Figure 3. Gene decoding of age-related changes in skewness against the Allen Institute for Brain Sciences gene expression atlas.

Only genes negatively relating to age-related changes in skewness are shown as only these genes survived FDR < 0.05. No other moments had any significantly associated genes. Cell-type-specific expression, showing selective enrichment for oligodendrocytes, reinforcing the link between profile skewness and myelination. Hexagon rings denote significance at different pSI thresholds (from p<0.05 in the outer ring to 0.00001 in the centre). (B) Developmental cortical enrichment, showing enrichment specifically in adolescence and young adulthood.

Figure 3.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1. Gene ontology analyses from baseline profiles of the four moments of the intensity distribution.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1.

Results from this spatial overlap analysis show no clear or unique ontological profile for any of the genes and when p-values were corrected for FDR across the four moments and positive and negative gene-lists, none survived correction.

Adolescent reconfiguration of microstructure profile covariance networks

Adopting the MPC framework, we subsequently assessed inter-regional coordination of myeloarchitectural development. MPC networks were constructed for each participant, and we modelled age-related changes in microstructural similarity between all node pairs (Figure 4A). At the node-to-node edge-level, adolescent development was primarily related to increases in MPC (1564 increases vs 534 decreases at qFDR <0.025; Figure 4—figure supplement 1). To express the spatial pattern of myeloarchitectural development in a lower dimensional, and more readily interpretable space, we implemented diffusion map embedding (Margulies et al., 2016; Figure 4A). Nodes closer in this embedding space have more similar patterns of age-related change in MPC, whereas distant nodes undergo dissimilar development. The first principal component (G1DEV) illustrated a sensory-fugal gradient, explaining 33% variance (Figure 4A). On one end of the gradient, idiotypic sensory and motor areas became increasingly coupled in adolescence, and more segregated from the opposite anchor constituted mainly of paralimbic nodes. The concordance of the developmental gradient (G1DEV) with the baseline MPC gradient (r = 0.89, p<0.001, Figure 1D) suggested that the axis of microstructural differentiation expands during adolescence. To synoptically visualise these dimensional changes, we generated, aligned, and contrasted cross-sectional MPC gradients (G1MPC) within the youngest (<16 years) and oldest (>24 years) age strata. Similar results were observed using alternative age windows. Heteromodal and unimodal cortex, occupying central regions of the gradient, were drawn outwards towards one of the gradient anchors in older individuals (Figure 4B), forming a more bimodal distribution (Figure 4—figure supplement 2). Specifically, prefrontal and medial parietal areas were increasingly coupled with sensory areas, and temporal regions increased in similarity to paralimbic regions (Figure 4B). By relating the age-related shifts in MPC to anchors with age-related changes in MT moments, we found that greater decreases in MT skewness were related to sensory anchor coupling (r = −0.63, pspin <0.001; Figure 4C; Figure 4—figure supplement 3). Conversely, restricted age-related skewness changes were associated with paralimbic anchor coupling (r = 0.65, pspin <0.001). Together, these findings demonstrate how depth-dependent myelination during adolescence reshapes myeloarchitecture and underpins macro-scale reorganisation of cortical networks.

Figure 4. Age-related changes in microstructure profile covariance (MPC).

(A) Subject-specific MPC matrices (stacked) were used in mixed effect models to calculate age-related changes in microstructural similarity between each node pair, generating a t-statistic matrix (middle). Diffusion map embedding ordered this matrix along the principal axis (G1DEV) (right). Rows of the matrix were coloured according to G1DEV. Surface projection of G1DEV illustrates a transition from primary sensory (purple) through association (pink) to limbic cortices (yellow). (B) Left. Principal axis of microstructural differentiation within extreme age strata (G1MPC), show a sensory-fugal axis similar to the baseline and developmental analysis. Middle. Shifts in average G1MPC within ten discrete bins of G1DEV, and corresponding Cohen’s d effect size. Central regions of G1DEV, aligning with association cortices, expand from the centre of G1MPC, towards either sensory or paralimbic anchors. Right. Age-related shifts towards anchors visualised via age-strata difference in MPC. The average MPC to each anchor was calculated within each extreme age-strata then subtracted. Prefrontal and parietal areas increased in microstructural similarity with the sensory anchor (top), whereas temporal regions became more coupled with the paralimbic anchor. (C) Left. Age-related change in MT moments (unthresholded maps from Figure 2B). Middle. Age-related shifts in mean and skewness within each bin of the gradient. Right. Correlation betweeBn age-related change in MT moments (Figure 2B) and shifts to anchors (B right).

Figure 4.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1. Deconstructing age-related changes in MPC.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1.

(A) Age-related changes in MPC stratified by increases (red) and decreases (blue). (B) Edge-wise changes in MPC were stratified by level of laminar differentiation. Raincloud plots illustrate the range of t-statistic values within and between different classes. Lines depict the 25%, 50% and 75% quartiles. I = idiotypic. U = unimodal. H = heteromodal. p=paralimbic. (C) Ridgeplot depicting distribution of thresholded (z > 2) gradient values across NeuroSynth composite terms, ranked from high to low mean value.
Figure 4—figure supplement 2. Increasing bimodality of the principle gradient of microstructural differentiation.

Figure 4—figure supplement 2.

(Left) Matrices showing the Pearson correlation between average MT profiles within ten discrete bins of principal developmental gradient (G1Dev) for young (<16 years) and old (>24 years). (Right) Ridge plots of G1MPC by level of laminar differentiation show an increasingly bimodal distribution of G1MPC for association cortices.
Figure 4—figure supplement 3. Developmental gradients derived from moment difference.

Figure 4—figure supplement 3.

Absolute difference in MT moment values between each pair of regions was calculated for each participant and transformed into a node x node matrix. In the same manner as developmental gradient construction, linear mixed effects models estimated the age-related changes of each edge and the resultant t-statistic matrix was subjected to diffusion map embedding. Variance explained by diffusion embedding components, t-statistic matrices ordered by the first components, the first components projected onto the cortical surface, scatterplot depicting the similarity of moment-based developmental gradients (G1AGE-MOMENT) to MPC-based developmental gradients (G1AGE-MPC; Figure 4B).

Discussion

Our longitudinal analyses revealed marked alterations in intracortical myeloarchitecture during adolescence, which was accompanied by large-scale reorganisation of microstructural gradients. Changes to intracortical myelin can be generally characterised by two developmental processes, one involving overall increases in mean MT and one involving preferential MT increases in mid-to-deeper cortical layers. Both processes particularly affected unimodal and heteromodal association cortices that is areas between low-level sensory and motor areas on the one end of the cortical hierarchy and high-level transmodal and limbic regions on the other end. A series of complementary explorations, based on alternative neuroimaging features known to undergo large-scale changes in adolescence (Giedd et al., 2015; Raznahan et al., 2011; Sowell et al., 2004), demonstrated the independence of these intracortical microstructural changes from age-related effects on overall cortical morphology and tissue contrast. In addition, cross-referencing our findings against post mortem gene expression maps showed that patterns of age-related change overlapped with genes associated with oligodendrocyte processes, supporting the specificity of MT changes to myelin. Leveraging microstructure profile covariance analysis, we explored coordination of intracortical microstructure characteristics across distributed areas (Paquola et al., 2019). This showed that local processes were paralleled by a cortex-wide differentiation along the sensory-fugal axis. This axis ultimately underpins the segregation of lower- from higher-order components of the putative cortical hierarchy.

Inspired by classical histological studies operating on 2D sections (Amunts et al., 1999; Schleicher et al., 1999; Zilles et al., 2002), our work formulated a surface-based approach to quantify myeloarchitecture in vivo via systematic profiling of intracortical MT intensities in the direction of cortical columns and its parameterization via the profiles’ central moments. The skewness of the MT profile, reflecting nonlinear increases in myelin towards deeper layers (Dinse et al., 2015; Sprooten et al., 2019; Waehnert et al., 2014), tilts from infragranular-dominant in paralimbic regions towards a more even distribution across cortical depths in sensory regions. This graded shift parallels changes in the laminar origin of projections, where paralimbic regions engaged in feedback processing project from infragranular layers and sensory regions providing feedforward information project predominantly from supragranular layers (Felleman and Van Essen, 1991). Meanwhile, mean myelin increased from polar towards sensory regions, recapitulating prior post mortem evidence (Vogt and Vogt, 1919; Vogt, 1911), established atlases of cytoarchitecture (Von Economo and Koskinas, 2008), and resting-state functional connectivity gradients (Hong et al., 2017; Margulies et al., 2016). These baseline analyses demonstrate the power of this framework to profile human myeloarchitecture in vivo.

Tracking longitudinal change in intracortical MT profiles, we found concomitant but independent developmental patterns in the trajectory of mean and skewness. Both sets of age-related changes were strongly and preferentially located in heteromodal and unimodal association cortex; yet, they only partially overlapped, a finding also supported by gene expression analysis. Only skewness changes, but not mean MT changes, were spatially associated with oligodendrocyte gene expression patterns. Additional analyses indicated that skewness effects occurred rather independently of MRI-based measures of cortical interface blurring and variations in cortical thickness, supporting specificity to intracortical microstructure. Our findings may align with several mechanisms of experience-dependent myelination (de Faria et al., 2018). Increasing mean MT likely reflects myelination of previously unmyelinated axons. On the other hand, as skewness changes persisted even after controlling for changes in the mean, these more likely capture architectural reconfigurations or laminar specific changes in axonal myelination. Underlying mechanisms may include de novo generation of oligodendrocytes (Zuccaro and Arlotta, 2013), activity-dependent modifications in neuronal and oligodendrocyte precursors (Gibson et al., 2014), and tuning of the distance between nodes of Ranvier along both cortico-cortical as well as long-range axons (Arancibia-Cárcamo et al., 2017). The putative functional role of myelin in cortical grey matter may be to insulate fibres from making new synaptic connections, thus enhancing stability (Braitenberg, 1962; Braitenberg and Schüz, 1998; Micheva et al., 2016). Conversely, overall lower myelin content in transmodal regions might indicate a higher potential for plasticity (Huntenburg et al., 2017), possibly making these areas more suitable to support adaptive behaviour and learning that are core to many integrative functions that mature during adolescence. Rodent experiments suggest that experience-induced oligodendrocyte remodelling supports the development of positive social behaviour (Makinodan et al., 2012). Conversely, abnormal myelination has been implicated in several brain disorders, however, studies on intracortical myeloarchitecture are scarce (Mighdoll et al., 2015; Stedehouder and Kushner, 2017; Bernhardt et al., 2018). The architectural changes observed in the present cohort may represent a microstructural substrate that could determine overall cognitive and social capacities or susceptibility to a range of neuropsychiatric and neurological conditions occurring in late childhood and adolescence.

Adaptation of a recent microstructure profile covariance analysis framework to longitudinal MT data illustrated the macroscale impact of microstructural changes. Specifically, decreases in MT profile skewness pushed regions to a more sensory-like architecture (which was coupled with high mean MT). In contrast, increases in skewness pushed regions towards a paralimbic-like MT profile. These regions also underwent protracted increases in mean MT, but still did not reach the high levels observed in sensory regions. These two regionally distinct developmental patterns within the association cortex promoted a more bimodal distribution of myeloarchitectural types across the cortex. Similar properties of modular segregation in tractography-based networks have been shown to mediate age-related improvements in executive function throughout adolescence (Baum et al., 2017), reinforcing the importance of these processes for understanding psychosocial maturation. Drifts in association cortex towards either sensory-like or paralimbic-like architecture represents, thus, an expansion of the sensory-fugal gradient of microstructural differentiation (Paquola et al., 2019). Such a sensory-fugal gradient was previously described by Mesulam (1998) based on non-human primate research to encapsulate cortex-wide variations in architecture and connectivity, and has since been suggested to reflect increasing synaptic plasticity towards transmodal regions (García-Cabezas et al., 2017). Systematic variations in the degree of experience-dependent plasticity may explain why myelin-derived markers develop differently along the sensory-fugal gradient (de Faria et al., 2018), in contrast to other known developmental gradients such as the rostro-caudal timing of terminal neurogenesis (Rakic, 1974). Differentiation of association cortices also conforms with notions of the ‘tethering hypothesis’ of cortical evolution (Buckner and Krienen, 2013). According to this framework, intermediate regions of the cortical hierarchy are less constrained by extrinsic inputs and intrinsic signaling molecules, which has allowed for massive surface area expansion throughout mammalian evolution. Our findings suggest that reduced constraints on association cortices allow for protracted development of myeloarchitecture, whereas the sensory and limbic anchors are well-defined prior to adolescence.

Our work suggests that myelination during adolescence is unlikely to be a question of simply more or less. Instead, our longitudinal findings show that the type of change is topologically divergent when we take depth into consideration. Expanding upon previous evidence of adolescent increases in overall mean intracortical myelin content, our findings demonstrate a relative preferential specific accumulation of myelin towards mid-to-deeper infragranular layers mainly in association cortices. As our analyses have shown, these findings are not explainable by changes in overall cortical morphology during adolescence, but instead likely reflect architectural changes associated with oligodendrocyte related processes. The coordinated change patterns strengthen the notion that the forces of adolescent development further widen the existing axis of macroscale cortical organization, driving association cortex either closer towards sensory or limbic systems. Thus, our findings illustrate how cell-type and layer specific microstructural changes assessed in the direction of cortical columns contribute to the maturation of macroscale cortical organisation and suggest adolescent calibration of structural hierarchical gradients.

Materials and methods

Imaging acquisition and processing

The present study included a subset of individuals from the NeuroScience in Psychiatry Network (NSPN) study (Whitaker et al., 2016; Kiddle et al., 2018). For a visualization of the sampling design, see Appendix 1—figure 6. In brief, the NSPN study comprises a primary cohort of 2402 healthy young people, recruited from schools, colleges, NHS primary care services and direct advertisements in north London and Cambridgeshire. Participants were stratified into five age groups (14–15, 16–17, 18–19, 20–21 and 22–25 years) and each stratum was evenly balanced for sex and ethnicity. Primary participants completed demographic, medical, childhood trauma and mental health questionnaires by post. The secondary cohort sub-sampled approximately 60 individuals from each stratum in the primary cohort, maintaining the sex and ethnicity balance. Secondary participants completed MRI scanning as part of a whole-day assessment at one of two sites (Cambridge and London, UK), on at least two occasions. Cohort retention for the MRI follow-up was 74% (Kiddle et al., 2018). Further inclusion criteria for the present study were availability of T1w and MT data available at two timepoints, resulting in 234 healthy adolescents. Following additional quality control on raw images, surface reconstructions and MT profiles, detailed below, the sample included 180 individuals (age stratification at baseline, n = 39/41/39/33/26, inter-scan interval = 15.4 ± 3.5 months, 86 females).

T1w and MT imaging were acquired as part of the quantitative multiparameter mapping (MPM) sequence (Weiskopf et al., 2013) on three identical TIM Trio 3T scanners; two located in Cambridge and one located in London. The MPM sequence comprises several multi-echo 3D FLASH (fast low angle shot) scans (Weiskopf et al., 2013). MT-weighting was achieved by applying an off-resonance Gaussian-shaped RF pulse (4 ms duration, 220° nominal flip angle, 2 kHz frequency offset from water resonance) prior to the excitation with TR/α = 23.7 ms/6°. Multiple gradient echoes were acquired with alternating readout polarity at six equidistant echo times (TE) between 2.2 and 14.7 ms. for MT weighted acquisition. The MT saturation parameter decouples the MT signal from the longitudinal relaxation rate, making it a semi-quantitative measure that is robust to relaxation times and field inhomogeneities (Weiskopf et al., 2013; Hagiwara et al., 2018). Other acquisition parameters were: 1 mm isotropic resolution, 176 sagittal partitions, field of view (FOV) = 256 × 240 mm, matrix = 256 × 240×176, parallel imaging using GRAPPA factor two in phase-encoding (PE) direction (AP), 6/8 partial Fourier in partition direction, non-selective RF excitation, readout bandwidth BW = 425 Hz/pixel, RF spoiling phase increment = 50°.

All raw images were visually inspected by experienced researchers and 11 participants were excluded due to excessive motion artefacts. Surface reconstructions were visually inspected and manually edited for all scans, up to 10 times (Romero-Garcia et al., 2018). At each iteration, control points and grey/white matter edits were included, and the surface reconstruction was repeated. 10 participants were excluded due to poor surface reconstructions. Following cortical surface reconstruction and surface-based co-registration between T1w and MT weighted scans, we generated 14 equivolumetric cortical surfaces within the cortex (Wagstyl et al., 2018), and systematically sampled MT intensity along these surfaces (Paquola et al., 2019; Figure 1). Next, depth-wise MT profiles were calculated across all vertices in native space and MT profiles were averaged within 1012 equally sized nodes (Figure 1). Given 1 mm isotropic voxels and 1.4–4.2 mm cortical thickness in present dataset, we estimate that each vertex-wise depth profile contains approximately 2–5 voxels. Using trilinear volume interpolation to each intracortical surface allowed for greater nuance in the intensity estimates along the depth profile. In total, each parcel-wise profile contains 71 ± 5.4 voxels. The parcellation scheme was mapped from a standard space (fsaverage) to native space for each subject using surface-based registration (Hong et al., 2017). We corrected for depth-specific partial volume effects (PVE) of cerebrospinal fluid using a mixed tissue class model (Kim et al., 2005) to reduce potential bias of averaging MT values in voxels with CSF (Mossahebi et al., 2015). Specifically, we fitted a linear model at each node (n) and each surface (s) of the form

MT(n,s)b0+b1CSF(n,s)

where MT(n,s) and CSF(n,s) represent node-specific, surface-specific MT value and CSF partial volume effect estimates. Final CSF-corrected MT values were calculated as the sum of the residuals [MTc(n,s)=T1(n,s) – (b0+b1*CSF(n,s))] and the uncorrected group average MT value.

Baseline analysis of MT profiles

We characterised the MT profiles by the central moments of the intensity distributions; mean, standard deviation (SD), skewness, and kurtosis (Zilles et al., 2002). We focused our main analysis on the first and third moments (Figure 1), owing to the collinearity of SD with mean and kurtosis with skewness (see also Appendix 1—figure 1), the clearer biological interpretation of mean and skewness for the MT profiles, as well as how the first and third moments capture different dimensions of the intensity distribution. Prior to statistical assessment, we identified and removed outlier individuals (n = 33). Outliers were defined as individuals with >1% nodes that deviated from the age-stratified median of nodal mean MT by >3 interquartile ranges. In an effort to validate the relationship between MT and myelin, which is discussed in more detail elsewhere (Heath et al., 2018; Schmierer et al., 2007; Weiskopf et al., 2013), we examined MT-derived and post mortem derived myelin profiles in matched regions of the cortex. To further understand architectural and cellular underpinnings of the in vivo MT profiles, we assessed the similarity of the MT-derived profiles with intracortical myelin profiles computed from previously published post mortem myelin sections. While there are clear differences in the resolution and specificity to myelin between the histological staining and our in vivo proxy data, findings were overall supportive of a close association between in vivo MT profiles and those derived from myelin stained sections. This line of evidence supports the specificity of the MT features to intracortical myeloarchitecture and motivates efforts to increase the availability of post mortem histological measures for more detailed cross-validation studies of putative MRI markers of myelin. We selected hyper-stained myelin pictures from classical literature (Vogt and Vogt, 1919; Vogt, 1911) that represent different levels of cytoarchitectural complexity and have been well characterised in recent work (Palomero-Gallagher and Zilles, 2019). We extracted a rectangle section of each image (spanning from the pial layer to the white matter boundary; Figure 1A), inverted the colour to align with MT imaging (i.e.,: myelin is more white), obtained intensity values per pixel by reading the image into MATLAB (v2017b), then generated a region-specific intensity profile by averaging values row-wise. Finally, we calculated the MT moments for each region and contrasted these values with baseline group-average MT moments for matched regions. Additionally, we contrasted baseline group-average MT moments across levels of laminar differentiation (Mesulam, 2002; Paquola et al., 2019) and cytoarchitectonic type (Von Economo and Koskinas, 2008; Whitaker et al., 2016) to determine whether the in vivo derived MT moments recapitulate histological evidence of cytoarchitectonic variation. MPC networks were calculated as the pairwise Pearson correlation between nodal MT profiles, controlling for the average whole-cortex MT profile. In line with previous work (Paquola et al., 2019), we performed nonlinear dimensionality reduction to characterise the principle gradient of variance in MPC. We assessed the correspondence of the MPC gradient with the MT moments using a node-wise Spearman correlations, with p-values determined against 10000 null models obtained from spin permutations (Alexander-Bloch et al., 2018). All spatial correlations were estimated and tested in the same way throughout the study.

Longitudinal assessment of age-related changes in MT moments

Age-related changes in MT moments were estimated at each node within four linear mixed effect models (LME) using SurfStat (Worsley et al., 2009) for Matlab, accounting for the non-independence of subjects as well as sex, using the following model:

moment(n)b0+b1age+b2sex+(1|subject+ε)

where n represents the node. The addition of person-specific random intercepts significantly improves model fit in accelerated longitudinal designs. LMEs were performed on each node and t-statistics were projected onto the cortical surface. Additionally, we examined the relationship between level of laminar differentiation and adolescent development of MT moments. To assess the relative over- or under-representation of significant regions per laminar class we generated 10,000 spin permutations of the t-maps, which controls for spatial contiguity and hemispheric symmetry across permutations (Alexander-Bloch et al., 2018; Seidlitz et al., 2019; Váša et al., 2018). Specifically, we differently rotated the t-maps 10,000 times and for each permutation of this spin or rotation we computed counts per laminar class that passed a false discovery rate (FDR) correction for multiple comparisons (Benjamini and Hochberg, 1995) to create a null-distribution of the counts table. Based on the null distribution, we computed Z-scores and two-sided p-values for the actual count table and corrected for multiple comparisons across the entire table using conservative Bonferroni correction.

Gene expression analyses

We measured the spatial overlap between our baseline maps of MT moments (e.g., one scan per subject; Figure 1E) and t-statistics (Figure 2A) and maps of post mortem gene expression from the Allen Institute for Brain Sciences (AIBS). We used Neurovault gene decoding of the AIBS dataset to identify the significant associations of spatial gene expression with t-maps (Hawrylycz et al., 2012; Gorgolewski et al., 2015). Neurovault implements a non-linear co-registration of each AIBS donor brain to a standard MNI template. Visual inspection revealed that one donor required additional manual correction due to a cerebellar deformations. Then, 4 mm spheres were drawn around each probe coordinate and values from the input t-maps, already in MNI space, were averaged within each sphere. Mixed effect analysis modelled the associations between each individual gene across the six donors and each input t-map. Meta-data from the gene probes (i.e., their EntrezID’s) were downloaded within the same pipeline and thus always had the latest information available from AIBS. Genes without EntrezID were excluded from subsequent analysis. Filtering was omitted on the genes to ensure a fully data-driven approach. Resultant gene associations p-values were corrected at an FDR level of 0.05 across all included genes and only genes passing correction were included in subsequent analyses. Enrichment analyses were conducted using Enrichr (Chen et al., 2013; Kuleshov et al., 2016), using a Z-score modification of Fisher’s exact test and FDR correction. It should be noted that the AIBS data are based on adult post-mortem and that the developmental associations are indirect associations between the enriched gene-set from spatial overlap analysis and a different developmental gene-expression dataset. Specifically, the t-statistic maps of age-related changes in the moments were spatially correlated with gene expression maps from the AIBS dataset using Neurovault (https://github.com/NeuroVault/NeuroVault/tree/master/ahba_docker). The genes derived from this spatial analysis were subsequently compared against developmental expression profiles from the BrainSpan dataset (http://www.brainspan.org/) using the CSEA developmental expression tool developed by the Dougherty lab (http://genetics.wustl.edu/jdlab/csea-tool-2/).

Spatial topography of age-related changes in MT profiles

Age-related changes in inter-regional microstructural similarity were assessed by applying the same LME to individual edges of subject specific MPC networks (Paquola et al., 2019). Age-related changes were deemed significant using a two-tailed qFDR <0.05. Diffusion map embedding, a non-linear dimensionality reduction technique (Coifman and Lafon, 2006), was applied to the resultant t-statistic matrix to discern the spatial topography of age-related changes. The first principal component represents the principal axis of variation in age-related changes in MPC. Nodal loadings onto the principal component, otherwise known as gradient values (G1DEV), depict the similarity of nodal patterns of MPC change. Similar to previous analyses (Paquola et al., 2019), we examined whether G1DEV differed across levels of laminar differentiation using ridge plots (Wilke, 2018). For closer inspection of MT profile changes across the developmental gradient, we generated group-average MT profiles for the youngest (<16 years, n = 43) and oldest (>25 years, n = 30) age strata. To probe the impact of developmental differentiation on the maturity of the underlying microstructure map, we generated group-average MPC matrices within the youngest, oldest as well as a mid-range (20–22 years) age-strata. MPC matrices were subjected to diffusion map embedding (Vos de Wael et al., 2019), then the young and old embeddings were aligned to the mid-range embedding using Procrustes rotation (Langs et al., 2015). We rank ordered and binned G1into ten, approximately equal sized bins (nodes per bin ~110). Age-related differences in MPC gradients were assessed by (i) bin-wise difference average G1MPC values, (ii) bin-wise Cohen’s d effect size change of gradient values, (iii) node-wise difference in MPC to the gradient anchors, that is the extreme bins, and (iv) bin-wise difference in MT moment values. To reconcile macroscale changes in the MPC gradients to microstructural alterations, we performed a spatial Pearson correlation between unthresholded t-statistic maps of age-related changes in MT mean and skewness (from Figure 2B) with shifts relative to anchors (map iii, Figure 4B left).

Data and code availability

Preprocessed microstructure profiles are available via GitHub: https://github.com/MICA-MNI/micaopen/tree/master/a_moment_of_change (Paquola, 2019; copy archived at https://github.com/elifesciences-publications/micaopen/tree/master/a_moment_of_change).

The repository also includes Matlab and R scripts to reproduce the primary analyses and figures.

Acknowledgements

CP is supported by the Fonds de la Recherche du Quebec – Santé (FRQS), RAIB is supported by a British Academy Postdoctoral fellowship and Autism Research Trust. CP, RAIB, GW, BB are supported by a Cambridge-MNI collaborative research grant. RRG was funded by the NSPN and the Guarantors of Brain. JS was supported by the NIH Oxford-Cambridge Scholars’ Program. KW was supported by the Health Brain Healthy Lives (HBHL) Initiative. KJW was funded by a The Alan Turing Institute under the EPSRC grant EP/N510129/1. PEV was supported by the MRC (MR/K020706/1), is a Fellow of MQ: Transforming Mental Health (MQF17/24) and is a Fellow of the Alan Turing Institute funded under EPSRC (EP/N510129/1). BB is supported by National Science and Engineering Research Council of Canada (NSERC, Discovery-1304413), the Canadian Institutes of Health Research (CIHR, FDN-154298), the Azrieli Center for Autism Research of the Montreal Neurological Institute (ACAR), SickKids Foundation (NI17-039), and received salary support from FRQS (Chercheur Boursier Junior 1). EB is supported by a NIHR Senior Investigator award.

Data were curated and analysed using a computational facility funded by an MRC research infrastructure award (MR/M009041/1) and supported by the NIHR Cambridge BRC. This work was supported by the Neuroscience in Psychiatry Network (NSPN) Consortium, a strategic award from the Wellcome Trust to the University of Cambridge and University College London (095844/Z/11/Z); by the Cambridge NIHR Biomedical Research Centre. The views expressed are those of the authors and not necessarily those of the NHS, the NIHR or the Department of Health and Social Care.

Appendix 1

Appendix 1—figure 1. Matrices depict the strength of correlations (r values) between (A) baseline maps of MT moments and (B) t-statistic maps of age-related changes in MT moments.

Appendix 1—figure 1.

Appendix 1—figure 2. Independence of effects controlling for cortical thickness.

Appendix 1—figure 2.

(A) Spatial distribution of MT moments corrected for cortical thickness (moment(n)~b0 + b1thickness(n) + ε). (B) t-statistics representing the age-related changes in MT moments, with cortical thickness included in the linear mixed effect model as a covariate (moment(n) ~ b0 + b1thickness(n) + b2age + b3sex + (1|subject) + ε).

Appendix 1—figure 3. Independence of effects controlling for interface blurring.

Appendix 1—figure 3.

(A) Spatial distribution of MT moments corrected for interface blurring (moment(n)~b0 + blurring(n) + ε). (B) t-statistics representing the age-related changes in MT moments, with interface blurring included in the linear mixed effect model as a covariate (moment(n) ~ b0 + b1blurring(n) + b2age + b3sex + (1|subject) + ε).

Appendix 1—figure 4. Independence of effects controlling for mean MT.

Appendix 1—figure 4.

(A) Spatial distribution of MT moments corrected for mean MT (moment(n)~b0 + b1meanMT(n) + ε). (B) t-statistics representing the age-related changes in MT moments, with mean MT included in the linear mixed effect model as a covariate (moment(n) ~ b0 + b1meanMT(n) + b2age + b3sex + (1|subject) + ε).

Appendix 1—figure 5. Association of laminar thickness with MT moments.

Appendix 1—figure 5.

(Left) Laminar thickness, derived from a post mortem volumetric reconstruction of a Merker stained human brain (Wagstyl et al., 2018). (Right) Spearman correlation coefficients between spatial maps of baseline MT moments with laminar thicknesses.

Appendix 1—figure 6. Sampling design.

Appendix 1—figure 6.

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Contributor Information

Casey Paquola, Email: casey.paquola@gmail.com.

Richard AI Bethlehem, Email: rb643@medschl.cam.ac.uk.

Boris Bernhardt, Email: boris.bernhardt@mcgill.ca.

Edward T Bullmore, Email: etb23@cam.ac.uk.

Joshua I Gold, University of Pennsylvania, United States.

Theodore Satterthwaite, University of Pennsylvania, United States.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grants:

  • Wellcome 095844/Z/11/Z to Rafael Romero-Garcia, Edward T Bullmore.

  • Fonds de la recherche en sante du Quebec to Casey Paquola.

  • Medical Research Council MR/K020706/1 to Petra E Vértes.

  • British Academy to Richard AI Bethlehem.

  • Autism Research Trust to Richard AI Bethlehem.

  • Cambridge-Montreal Neurological Institute and Hospital to Casey Paquola, Richard AI Bethlehem, Guy B Williams, Boris Bernhardt.

  • Guarantors of Brain to Rafael Romero-Garcia.

  • National Institutes of Health Oxford-Cambridge Scholars' Program to Jakob Seidlitz.

  • Healthy Brains, Healthy Lives to Konrad Wagstyl.

  • Engineering and Physical Sciences Research Council EP/N510129/1 to Kirstie J Whitaker, Petra E Vértes.

  • MQ: Transforming Mental Health MQF17/24 to Petra E Vértes.

  • Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada Discovery-1304413 to Boris Bernhardt.

  • Canadian Institutes of Health Research FDN-154298 to Boris Bernhardt.

  • Azrieli Center for Autism Research of the Montreal Neurological Institute to Boris Bernhardt.

  • Sick Kids Foundation NI17-039 to Boris Bernhardt.

  • Fonds de Recherche du Québec - Santé Chercheur Boursier Junior 1 to Boris Bernhardt.

  • National Institute for Health Research Senior Investigator Award to Edward T Bullmore.

  • Cambridge NIHR Biomedical Research Centre to Edward T Bullmore.

Additional information

Competing interests

No competing interests declared.

Author contributions

Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Visualization, Methodology, Writing—original draft, Writing—review and editing.

Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Investigation, Visualization, Writing—original draft, Writing—review and editing.

Formal analysis, Methodology, Writing—review and editing.

Investigation, Methodology, Writing—review and editing.

Methodology, Writing—review and editing.

Data curation, Writing—review and editing.

Software, Methodology.

Funding acquisition, Writing—review and editing.

Funding acquisition; Data curation.

Data curation, Writing—review and editing.

Methodology, Writing—review and editing.

Conceptualization, Resources, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Visualization, Methodology, Writing—review and editing.

Conceptualization, Resources, Data curation, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Writing—review and editing.

Ethics

Human subjects: Participants provided informed written consent for each aspect of the study, and parental consent was obtained for those aged 14-15 y old. Ethical approval was granted for this study by the NHS NRES Committee East of England-Cambridge Central (project ID 97546). The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.

Additional files

Supplementary file 1. Supplementary materials.
elife-50482-supp1.docx (16.1KB, docx)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.50482.013
Transparent reporting form
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.50482.014

Data availability

Preprocessed microstructure profiles are available via GitHub: https://github.com/MICA-MNI/micaopen/tree/master/a_moment_of_change (copy archived at https://github.com/elifesciences-publications/micaopen/tree/master/a_moment_of_change). The repository also includes Matlab and R scripts to reproduce the primary analyses and figures.

The following datasets were generated:

References

  1. Alexander-Bloch A, Raznahan A, Bullmore E, Giedd J. The convergence of maturational change and structural covariance in human cortical networks. Journal of Neuroscience. 2013;33:2889–2899. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3554-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Alexander-Bloch AF, Shou H, Liu S, Satterthwaite TD, Glahn DC, Shinohara RT, Vandekar SN, Raznahan A. On testing for spatial correspondence between maps of human brain structure and function. NeuroImage. 2018;178:540–551. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.05.070. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Amunts K, Schleicher A, Bürgel U, Mohlberg H, Uylings HB, Zilles K. Broca's region revisited: cytoarchitecture and intersubject variability. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 1999;412:319–341. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1096-9861(19990920)412:2&#x0003c;319::AID-CNE10&#x0003e;3.0.CO;2-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Arancibia-Cárcamo IL, Ford MC, Cossell L, Ishida K, Tohyama K, Attwell D. Node of ranvier length as a potential regulator of myelinated axon conduction speed. eLife. 2017;6:e23329. doi: 10.7554/eLife.23329. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. ArnatkevicIūtė A, Fulcher BD, Fornito A. A practical guide to linking brain-wide gene expression and neuroimaging data. NeuroImage. 2019;189:353–367. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.01.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Barbas H, Rempel-Clower N. Cortical structure predicts the pattern of corticocortical connections. Cerebral Cortex. 1997;7:635–646. doi: 10.1093/cercor/7.7.635. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Baum GL, Ciric R, Roalf DR, Betzel RF, Moore TM, Shinohara RT, Kahn AE, Vandekar SN, Rupert PE, Quarmley M, Cook PA, Elliott MA, Ruparel K, Gur RE, Gur RC, Bassett DS, Satterthwaite TD. Modular segregation of structural brain networks supports the development of executive function in youth. Current Biology. 2017;27:1561–1572. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.04.051. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y. Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series B. 1995;57:289–300. doi: 10.2307/2346101. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bernhardt BC, Fadaie F, Vos de Wael R, Hong SJ, Liu M, Guiot MC, Rudko DA, Bernasconi A, Bernasconi N. Preferential susceptibility of limbic cortices to microstructural damage in temporal lobe epilepsy: a quantitative T1 mapping study. NeuroImage. 2018;182:294–303. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.06.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Braitenberg V. A note on myeloarchitectonics. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 1962;118:141–156. doi: 10.1002/cne.901180202. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Braitenberg V, Schüz A. Statistics Cortex: andAnd Geometry of Neuronal Connectivity. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer; 1998. Cyto- and Myeloarchitectonics: Two Aspects of the same Reality; pp. 151–158. [Google Scholar]
  12. Buckner RL, Krienen FM. The evolution of distributed association networks in the human brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2013;17:648–665. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.09.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Carey D, Caprini F, Allen M, Lutti A, Weiskopf N, Rees G, Callaghan MF, Dick F. Quantitative MRI provides markers of intra-, inter-regional, and age-related differences in young adult cortical microstructure. NeuroImage. 2018;182:429–440. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.11.066. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Chen EY, Tan CM, Kou Y, Duan Q, Wang Z, Meirelles GV, Clark NR, Ma'ayan A. Enrichr: interactive and collaborative HTML5 gene list enrichment analysis tool. BMC Bioinformatics. 2013;14:128. doi: 10.1186/1471-2105-14-128. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Coifman RR, Lafon S. Diffusion maps. Applied and Computational Harmonic Analysis. 2006;21:5–30. doi: 10.1016/j.acha.2006.04.006. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  16. de Faria O, Pama EAC, Evans K, Luzhynskaya A, Káradóttir RT. Neuroglial interactions underpinning myelin plasticity. Developmental Neurobiology. 2018;78:93–107. doi: 10.1002/dneu.22539. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Dinse J, Härtwich N, Waehnert MD, Tardif CL, Schäfer A, Geyer S, Preim B, Turner R, Bazin PL. A cytoarchitecture-driven myelin model reveals area-specific signatures in human primary and secondary Areas using ultra-high resolution in-vivo brain MRI. NeuroImage. 2015;114:71–87. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.04.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Dougherty JD, Schmidt EF, Nakajima M, Heintz N. Analytical approaches to RNA profiling data for the identification of genes enriched in specific cells. Nucleic Acids Research. 2010;38:4218–4230. doi: 10.1093/nar/gkq130. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Felleman DJ, Van Essen DC. Distributed hierarchical processing in the primate cerebral cortex. Cerebral Cortex. 1991;1:1–47. doi: 10.1093/cercor/1.1.1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. García-Cabezas MÁ, Joyce MKP, John YJ, Zikopoulos B, Barbas H. Mirror trends of plasticity and stability indicators in primate prefrontal cortex. The European Journal of Neuroscience. 2017;46:2392–2405. doi: 10.1111/ejn.13706. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Gibson EM, Purger D, Mount CW, Goldstein AK, Lin GL, Wood LS, Inema I, Miller SE, Bieri G, Zuchero JB, Barres BA, Woo PJ, Vogel H, Monje M. Neuronal activity promotes oligodendrogenesis and adaptive myelination in the mammalian brain. Science. 2014;344:1252304. doi: 10.1126/science.1252304. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Giedd JN, Raznahan A, Alexander-Bloch A, Schmitt E, Gogtay N, Rapoport JL. Child psychiatry branch of the national institute of mental health longitudinal structural magnetic resonance imaging study of human brain development. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2015;40:43–49. doi: 10.1038/npp.2014.236. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Gorgolewski KJ, Fox AS, Chang L, Schäfer A, Arélin K, Burmann I, Sacher J, Margulies DS. Tight fitting genes: finding relations between statistical maps and gene expression patterns. 20th Annual Meeting of the Organization for Human Brain Mapping.2014. [Google Scholar]
  24. Gorgolewski KJ, Varoquaux G, Rivera G, Schwarz Y, Ghosh SS, Maumet C, Sochat VV, Nichols TE, Poldrack RA, Poline JB, Yarkoni T, Margulies DS. NeuroVault.org: a web-based repository for collecting and sharing unthresholded statistical maps of the human brain. Frontiers in Neuroinformatics. 2015;9:8. doi: 10.3389/fninf.2015.00008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Grydeland H, Vértes PE, Váša F, Romero-Garcia R, Whitaker K, Alexander-Bloch AF, Bjørnerud A, Patel AX, Sederevicius D, Tamnes CK, Westlye LT, White SR, Walhovd KB, Fjell AM, Bullmore ET. Waves of maturation and senescence in Micro-structural MRI markers of human cortical myelination over the lifespan. Cerebral Cortex. 2019;29:1369–1381. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhy330. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Hagiwara A, Hori M, Kamagata K, Warntjes M, Matsuyoshi D, Nakazawa M, Ueda R, Andica C, Koshino S, Maekawa T, Irie R, Takamura T, Kumamaru KK, Abe O, Aoki S. Myelin measurement: comparison between simultaneous tissue relaxometry, magnetization transfer saturation index, and T1w/T2w Ratio Methods. Scientific Reports. 2018;8:10554. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-28852-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Hawrylycz MJ, Lein ES, Guillozet-Bongaarts AL, Shen EH, Ng L, Miller JA, van de Lagemaat LN, Smith KA, Ebbert A, Riley ZL, Abajian C, Beckmann CF, Bernard A, Bertagnolli D, Boe AF, Cartagena PM, Chakravarty MM, Chapin M, Chong J, Dalley RA, Daly BD, Dang C, Datta S, Dee N, Dolbeare TA, Faber V, Feng D, Fowler DR, Goldy J, Gregor BW, Haradon Z, Haynor DR, Hohmann JG, Horvath S, Howard RE, Jeromin A, Jochim JM, Kinnunen M, Lau C, Lazarz ET, Lee C, Lemon TA, Li L, Li Y, Morris JA, Overly CC, Parker PD, Parry SE, Reding M, Royall JJ, Schulkin J, Sequeira PA, Slaughterbeck CR, Smith SC, Sodt AJ, Sunkin SM, Swanson BE, Vawter MP, Williams D, Wohnoutka P, Zielke HR, Geschwind DH, Hof PR, Smith SM, Koch C, Grant SGN, Jones AR. An anatomically comprehensive atlas of the adult human brain transcriptome. Nature. 2012;489:391–399. doi: 10.1038/nature11405. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Heath F, Hurley SA, Johansen-Berg H, Sampaio-Baptista C. Advances in noninvasive myelin imaging. Developmental Neurobiology. 2018;78:136–151. doi: 10.1002/dneu.22552. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Hong SJ, Bernhardt BC, Gill RS, Bernasconi N, Bernasconi A. The spectrum of structural and functional network alterations in malformations of cortical development. Brain. 2017;140:2133–2143. doi: 10.1093/brain/awx145. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Huntenburg JM, Bazin PL, Goulas A, Tardif CL, Villringer A, Margulies DS. A systematic relationship between functional connectivity and intracortical myelin in the human cerebral cortex. Cerebral Cortex. 2017;27:981–997. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhx030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Kaes T. Die Grosshirnrinde Des Menschen in Ihren Massen Und in Ihrem Fasergehalt: Ein Gehirnanatomischer Atlas Mit Erläuterndem Text Und Schematischer Zeichnung, 16 Tabellen, 15 Kurven Und 79 Farbigen Tafeln. G. Fischer; 1907. [Google Scholar]
  32. Kiddle B, Inkster B, Prabhu G, Moutoussis M, Whitaker KJ, Bullmore ET, Dolan RJ, Fonagy P, Goodyer IM, Jones PB. Cohort profile: the NSPN 2400 cohort: a developmental sample supporting the wellcome trust NeuroScience in psychiatry network. International Journal of Epidemiology. 2018;47:18–19. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyx117. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Kim JS, Singh V, Lee JK, Lerch J, Ad-Dab'bagh Y, MacDonald D, Lee JM, Kim SI, Evans AC. Automated 3-D extraction and evaluation of the inner and outer cortical surfaces using a laplacian map and partial volume effect classification. NeuroImage. 2005;27:210–221. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.03.036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Koenig SH. Cholesterol of myelin is the determinant of gray-white contrast in MRI of brain. Magnetic Resonance in Medicine. 1991;20:285–291. doi: 10.1002/mrm.1910200210. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Kuleshov MV, Jones MR, Rouillard AD, Fernandez NF, Duan Q, Wang Z, Koplev S, Jenkins SL, Jagodnik KM, Lachmann A, McDermott MG, Monteiro CD, Gundersen GW, Ma'ayan A. Enrichr: a comprehensive gene set enrichment analysis web server 2016 update. Nucleic Acids Research. 2016;44:W90–W97. doi: 10.1093/nar/gkw377. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Kwon D, Pfefferbaum A, Sullivan EV, Pohl KM. Regional growth trajectories of cortical myelination in adolescents and young adults: longitudinal validation and functional correlates. Brain Imaging and Behavior. 2018;38:1–25. doi: 10.1007/s11682-018-9980-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Langs G, Golland P, Ghosh SS. Predicting activation across individuals with Resting-State functional connectivity based Multi-Atlas label fusion. Medical Image Computing and Computer-Assisted Intervention : MICCAI ... International Conference on Medical Image Computing and Computer-Assisted Intervention. 2015;9350:313–320. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-24571-3_38. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Makinodan M, Rosen KM, Ito S, Corfas G. A critical period for social experience-dependent oligodendrocyte maturation and myelination. Science. 2012;337:1357–1360. doi: 10.1126/science.1220845. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Margulies DS, Ghosh SS, Goulas A, Falkiewicz M, Huntenburg JM, Langs G, Bezgin G, Eickhoff SB, Castellanos FX, Petrides M, Jefferies E, Smallwood J. Situating the default-mode network along a principal gradient of macroscale cortical organization. PNAS. 2016;113:12574–12579. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1608282113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Mesulam M. From sensation to cognition. Brain. 1998;121:1013–1052. doi: 10.1093/brain/121.6.1013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Mesulam M-M. The Human Frontal Lobes: Transcending the Default Mode Through Contingent Encoding. In: Stuss DT, Knight RT, editors. Principles of Frontal Lobe Function. Oxford University Press; 2002. pp. 8–30. [Google Scholar]
  42. Micheva KD, Wolman D, Mensh BD, Pax E, Buchanan J, Smith SJ, Bock DD. A large fraction of neocortical myelin ensheathes axons of local inhibitory neurons. eLife. 2016;5:e15784. doi: 10.7554/eLife.15784. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Mighdoll MI, Tao R, Kleinman JE, Hyde TM. Myelin, myelin-related disorders, and psychosis. Schizophrenia Research. 2015;161:85–93. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2014.09.040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Miller JA, Ding SL, Sunkin SM, Smith KA, Ng L, Szafer A, Ebbert A, Riley ZL, Royall JJ, Aiona K, Arnold JM, Bennet C, Bertagnolli D, Brouner K, Butler S, Caldejon S, Carey A, Cuhaciyan C, Dalley RA, Dee N, Dolbeare TA, Facer BA, Feng D, Fliss TP, Gee G, Goldy J, Gourley L, Gregor BW, Gu G, Howard RE, Jochim JM, Kuan CL, Lau C, Lee CK, Lee F, Lemon TA, Lesnar P, McMurray B, Mastan N, Mosqueda N, Naluai-Cecchini T, Ngo NK, Nyhus J, Oldre A, Olson E, Parente J, Parker PD, Parry SE, Stevens A, Pletikos M, Reding M, Roll K, Sandman D, Sarreal M, Shapouri S, Shapovalova NV, Shen EH, Sjoquist N, Slaughterbeck CR, Smith M, Sodt AJ, Williams D, Zöllei L, Fischl B, Gerstein MB, Geschwind DH, Glass IA, Hawrylycz MJ, Hevner RF, Huang H, Jones AR, Knowles JA, Levitt P, Phillips JW, Sestan N, Wohnoutka P, Dang C, Bernard A, Hohmann JG, Lein ES. Transcriptional landscape of the prenatal human brain. Nature. 2014;508:199–206. doi: 10.1038/nature13185. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Mossahebi P, Alexander AL, Field AS, Samsonov AA. Removal of cerebrospinal fluid partial volume effects in quantitative magnetization transfer imaging using a three-pool model with nonexchanging water component. Magnetic Resonance in Medicine. 2015;74:1317–1326. doi: 10.1002/mrm.25516. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Nieuwenhuys R, Broere CA. A map of the human neocortex showing the estimated overall myelin content of the individual architectonic areas based on the studies of adolf hopf. Brain Structure and Function. 2017;222:465–480. doi: 10.1007/s00429-016-1228-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Odrobina EE, Lam TY, Pun T, Midha R, Stanisz GJ. MR properties of excised neural tissue following experimentally induced demyelination. NMR in Biomedicine. 2005;18:277–284. doi: 10.1002/nbm.951. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Palomero-Gallagher N, Zilles K. Cortical layers: cyto-, myelo-, receptor- and synaptic architecture in human cortical Areas. NeuroImage. 2019;197:716–741. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.08.035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Papoulis A, Unnikrishna Pillai S. Probability, Random Variables and Stochastic Processes. 4th edition. McGraw-Hill Europe; 2002. [Google Scholar]
  50. Paquola C, Vos De Wael R, Wagstyl K, Bethlehem RAI, Hong SJ, Seidlitz J, Bullmore ET, Evans AC, Misic B, Margulies DS, Smallwood J, Bernhardt BC. Microstructural and functional gradients are increasingly dissociated in transmodal cortices. PLOS Biology. 2019;17:e3000284. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.3000284. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Paquola C. a_moment_of_change. Github. 2019 https://github.com/MICA-MNI/micaopen/tree/master/a_moment_of_change
  52. Paus T, Keshavan M, Giedd JN. Why do many psychiatric disorders emerge during adolescence? Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2008;9:947–957. doi: 10.1038/nrn2513. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Rakic P. Neurons in rhesus monkey visual cortex: systematic relation between time of origin and eventual disposition. Science. 1974;183:425–427. doi: 10.1126/science.183.4123.425. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Raznahan A, Shaw P, Lalonde F, Stockman M, Wallace GL, Greenstein D, Clasen L, Gogtay N, Giedd JN. How does your cortex grow? Journal of Neuroscience. 2011;31:7174–7177. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0054-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Romero-Garcia R, Whitaker KJ, Váša F, Seidlitz J, Shinn M, Fonagy P, Dolan RJ, Jones PB, Goodyer IM, Bullmore ET, Vértes PE, NSPN Consortium Structural covariance networks are coupled to expression of genes enriched in supragranular layers of the human cortex. NeuroImage. 2018;171:256–267. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.12.060. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Schleicher A, Amunts K, Geyer S, Morosan P, Zilles K. Observer-independent method for microstructural parcellation of cerebral cortex: a quantitative approach to cytoarchitectonics. NeuroImage. 1999;9:165–177. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1998.0385. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Schmierer K, Tozer DJ, Scaravilli F, Altmann DR, Barker GJ, Tofts PS, Miller DH. Quantitative magnetization transfer imaging in postmortem multiple sclerosis brain. Journal of Magnetic Resonance Imaging. 2007;26:41–51. doi: 10.1002/jmri.20984. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Seidlitz J, Nadig A, Liu S, Bethlehem RAI, Vertes PE, Morgan SE, Vasa F, Romero-Garcia R, Lalonde FM, Clasen LS, Blumenthal JD, Paquola C, Bernhardt B, Wagstyl K, Polioudakis D, de la Torre-Ubieta L, Geschwind DH, Han JC, Lee NR, Murphy DG, Bullmore ET, Raznahan A. Transcriptomic and cellular decoding of regional brain vulnerability to neurodevelopmental disorders. bioRxiv. 2019 doi: 10.1101/573279. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  59. Serres S, Anthony DC, Jiang Y, Broom KA, Campbell SJ, Tyler DJ, van Kasteren SI, Davis BG, Sibson NR. Systemic inflammatory response reactivates immune-mediated lesions in rat brain. Journal of Neuroscience. 2009a;29:4820–4828. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0406-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Serres S, Anthony DC, Jiang Y, Campbell SJ, Broom KA, Khrapitchev A, Sibson NR. Comparison of MRI signatures in pattern I and II multiple sclerosis models. NMR in Biomedicine. 2009b;22:1014–1024. doi: 10.1002/nbm.1404. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Smith CL, Eppig JT. The mammalian phenotype ontology as a unifying standard for experimental and high-throughput phenotyping data. Mammalian Genome. 2012;23:653–668. doi: 10.1007/s00335-012-9421-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Soreq L, Rose J, Soreq E, Hardy J, Trabzuni D, Cookson MR, Smith C, Ryten M, Patani R, Ule J, UK Brain Expression Consortium, North American Brain Expression Consortium Major shifts in glial regional identity are a transcriptional hallmark of human brain aging. Cell Reports. 2017;18:557–570. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2016.12.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Sowell ER, Thompson PM, Leonard CM, Welcome SE, Kan E, Toga AW. Longitudinal mapping of cortical thickness and brain growth in normal children. Journal of Neuroscience. 2004;24:8223–8231. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1798-04.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Sprooten E, O'Halloran R, Dinse J, Lee WH, Moser DA, Doucet GE, Goodman M, Krinsky H, Paulino A, Rasgon A, Leibu E, Balchandani P, Inglese M, Frangou S. Depth-dependent intracortical myelin organization in the living human brain determined by in vivo ultra-high field magnetic resonance imaging. NeuroImage. 2019;185:27–34. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.10.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Stedehouder J, Kushner SA. Myelination of parvalbumin interneurons: a parsimonious locus of pathophysiological convergence in schizophrenia. Molecular Psychiatry. 2017;22:4–12. doi: 10.1038/mp.2016.147. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Váša F, Seidlitz J, Romero-Garcia R, Whitaker KJ, Rosenthal G, Vértes PE, Shinn M, Alexander-Bloch A, Fonagy P, Dolan RJ, Jones PB, Goodyer IM, Sporns O, Bullmore ET, NSPN consortium Adolescent tuning of association cortex in human structural brain networks. Cerebral Cortex. 2018;28:281–294. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhx249. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Vogt O. Die myeloarchitektonik des isocortex parietalis. Journal für Psychologie und Neurologie. 1911;18:379–390. [Google Scholar]
  68. Vogt C, Vogt O. Allgemeinere ergebnisse unserer hirnforschung. Journal für Psychologie und Neurologie. 1919;25:279–461. [Google Scholar]
  69. Von Economo C, Koskinas GN. Atlas of Cytoarchitectonics of the Adult Human Cerebral Cortex, 1st Ed. Thessaloniki: Karger; 2008. [Google Scholar]
  70. Vos de Wael R, Benkarim O, Paquola C, Lariviere S, Royer J, Tavakol S, Xu T, Hong S-J, Valk SL, Misic B, Milham MP, Margulies DS, Smallwood J, Bernhardt BC. BrainSpace: a toolbox for the analysis of macroscale gradients in neuroimaging and connectomics datasets. bioRxiv. 2019 doi: 10.1101/761460. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  71. Waehnert MD, Dinse J, Weiss M, Streicher MN, Waehnert P, Geyer S, Turner R, Bazin P-L. Anatomically motivated modeling of cortical laminae. NeuroImage. 2014;93:210–220. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.03.078. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Wagstyl K, Paquola C, Bethlehem R, Huth A. kwagstyl/surface_tools. Initial Release of Equivolumetric Surfaces 2018
  73. Weiskopf N, Suckling J, Williams G, Correia MM, Inkster B, Tait R, Ooi C, Bullmore ET, Lutti A. Quantitative multi-parameter mapping of R1, PD(*), MT, and R2(*) at 3T: a multi-center validation. Frontiers in Neuroscience. 2013;7:95. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2013.00095. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Whitaker KJ, Vértes PE, Romero-Garcia R, Váša F, Moutoussis M, Prabhu G, Weiskopf N, Callaghan MF, Wagstyl K, Rittman T, Tait R, Ooi C, Suckling J, Inkster B, Fonagy P, Dolan RJ, Jones PB, Goodyer IM, Bullmore ET, NSPN Consortium Adolescence is associated with genomically patterned consolidation of the hubs of the human brain connectome. PNAS. 2016;113:9105–9110. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1601745113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Wilke CO. Introduction to ggridges. 2018 https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/ggridges/vignettes/introduction.html
  76. Worsley KJ, Taylor JE, Carbonell F, Chung MK, Duerden E, Bernhardt B, Lyttelton O, Boucher M, Evans AC. SurfStat: a matlab toolbox for the statistical analysis of univariate and multivariate surface and volumetric data using linear mixed effects models and random field theory. NeuroImage. 2009;47:S102. doi: 10.1016/S1053-8119(09)70882-1. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  77. Ziegler G, Hauser TU, Moutoussis M, Bullmore ET, Goodyer IM, Fonagy P, Jones PB, Lindenberger U, Dolan RJ, NSPN Consortium Compulsivity and impulsivity traits linked to attenuated developmental frontostriatal myelination trajectories. Nature Neuroscience. 2019;22:992–999. doi: 10.1038/s41593-019-0394-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Zilles K, Schleicher A, Palomero-Gallagher N, Amunts K. Quantitative analysis of cyto- and receptor architecture of the human brain. Brain Mapping: The Methods. 2002:573–602. doi: 10.1016/B978-012693019-1/50023-X. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  79. Zuccaro E, Arlotta P. The quest for myelin in the adult brain. Nature Cell Biology. 2013;15:572–575. doi: 10.1038/ncb2750. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision letter

Editor: Theodore Satterthwaite1
Reviewed by: Kate Mills2, Ben Fulcher3

In the interests of transparency, eLife includes the editorial decision letter and accompanying author responses. A lightly edited version of the letter sent to the authors after peer review is shown, indicating the most substantive concerns; minor comments are not usually included.

Acceptance Summary:

This novel and important study capitalizes upon a large sample of youths scanned twice with magnetization transfer imaging, in order to map developmental profiles of cortical myelin. The authors use this approach to demonstrate that the human cortex undergoes dissociable, depth-specific changes in cortical myeloarchitecture during development. Critically, greater imaging markers of myelin were spatially associated with gene expression markers of oligodentrocytes, and align with a gradient ranging from primary sensory to higher-order association cortex. Together, this approach provides a new understanding of how cortical myelin develops during the critical period of adolescence. Furthermore, this data has numerous implications for understanding both healthy brain maturation and abnormal brain development associated with neuropsychiatric syndromes.

Decision letter after peer review:

Thank you for submitting your article "Shifts in myeloarchitecture characterise adolescent development of cortical gradients" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by Joshua Gold as the Senior Editor, a Reviewing Editor, and two reviewers. The following individuals involved in review of your submission have agreed to reveal their identity: Kate Mills (Reviewer #1); Ben Fulcher (Reviewer #2).

The reviewers have discussed the reviews with one another and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this decision to help you prepare a revised submission.

Summary:

This is a study investigating intracortical microstructure in a sample of 223 individuals scanned twice between ages 14-27 years. Authors estimated myelin content in the cortex of magnetic resonance images using magnetization transfer, and by using central moments to test the hypothesis that mean and skewness of the magnetization transfer profiles would show different developmental trajectories and relate to different gene expression patterns. The study is comprehensive and the methods are sound. Writing and presentation are excellent, and code for reproducing the analyses is made available (and processed data where possible), further enhancing the impact of this work. Nonetheless, reviewers felt that several aspects of the work could be strengthened on revision.

Essential revisions:

1) NSPN sample. More details about the full sample (the full NSPN sample) compared to the included sample for this study would be useful. A visualization of the sampling design would be helpful.

2) Quality control. More details regarding the exclusion criteria and brain image quality control procedures are needed. What was the procedure for inspection, criteria used to determine exclusion, and number of scans excluded? Were both the raw images and processed images inspected for quality? Please describe the extent of manual intervention of processed images, including the protocol and number of scans that were successfully processed post-intervention and included in analyses.

3) AIBS. Further details regarding the processing of the gene expression maps is necessary (see: Arnatkeviciūtė et al., 2019). Furthermore, authors should clearly state that AIBS data are based on adult postmortem oligodendrocyte density. As such, it seems warranted to temper their claims (example: "confirmed a spatio-temporal overlap of our findings from NSPN with myelin processes during adolescence").

4) MT and myelin. The authors should clarify how sensitive is MT to myelin. The strength of this link is crucial to a lot of the interpretation and biological novelty of the results. MT (like T1w:T2w) may be sensitive not just to myelin, but also potentially other biological variables. Some evidence/discussion of this, and the extent to which the reader can safely make the association to myelin content would be helpful in the introduction. Furthermore, many features are different between the myelin stains and MT profiles (Figure 1B). Some brief discussion of the differences would be helpful. Finally, it would be useful to clarify if to the author's knowledge this is the first time anyone has matched MT depth profiles to a histological measurement (or discuss relevant literature of past efforts).

5) MT skewness and relative layer thickness. MT skewness may depend on the relative thickness of the different layers. It would be useful to clarify how much results explain the data beyond a simple cytoarchitectural analysis (e.g., are the depth profiles acting as a quantitative substitute for cytoarchitecture 'types', or is the inferred connection to myeloarchitecture relevant?) Furthermore, it would be useful to clarify if every node is squashed to the same absolute depth? This could impact interpretation: two areas with an identical ratio of myelination in deep relative to superficial layers would have different skewnesses if their middle layer was made thicker or thinner (thicker middle layer could push out the skewness). If this is the case, MT skewness could not simply be interpreted as a "ratio of myelination in deeper compared to more superficial layers"; it would more represent a measure of the preferential distribution of MT closer to the white matter boundary. Clarification is needed.

6) Statistical testing. Every time a statistical analysis is performed (and a test statistic/p-value quoted), it should be clearly stated to the reader what it is. Non-normal distributions seem common in this data; Spearman's correlations may be appropriate. Furthermore, it appears that the spin-test based permutations are only used for some analyses but not others (e.g., only from the "Age-related changes… MT profiles" section onwards). Were spin tests used for earlier spatial association analyses? If not, can this be justified?

7) Utility of low-dimensional embedding. Regarding this statement: "Nodes closer in this embedding space increase in microstructural similarity during adolescence, whereas distant nodes decouple", it is unclear why a low-dimensional embedding is useful a direct measurement of change is available. Couldn't you more easily do an analysis directly (e.g., investigate the structure of the 'increasing' edges?) What is gained from this harder-to-interpret nonlinear embedding that helps test/interpret your hypotheses? Perhaps more importantly, it is unclear if this statement is supported by the data: Wouldn't the embedding put nodes close in the space that have similar "patterns" of age-related MPC change with other network nodes? In the plot of the behavior of G1_dev, it does not display the claimed behavior (Figure 4B) – instead, it seems like the largest values of G1_dev already have ~maximal G1_MPC, and thus barely change at all between the <16 and >24 groups. Clarification is warranted.

8) Biological interpretation. In the discussion, the biological interpretation of the results at times is rather thin. If MT is a strong myelin marker, then why would myeloarchitecture be reorganizing like this (e.g., to support what behavior)? Axonal myelination improves transmission speeds and prevents the formation of new synaptic connections. Could you speculate on how this might alter behavior relevant to the longitudinal changes analyzed? Relevant papers to consider include:

*Micheva et al., (2016).

*Braitenberg, (1962).

*Huntenburg et al., (2017).

eLife. 2019 Nov 14;8:e50482. doi: 10.7554/eLife.50482.024

Author response


Summary:

This is a study investigating intracortical microstructure in a sample of 223 individuals scanned twice between ages 14-27 years. Authors estimated myelin content in the cortex of magnetic resonance images using magnetization transfer, and by using central moments to test the hypothesis that mean and skewness of the magnetization transfer profiles would show different developmental trajectories and relate to different gene expression patterns. The study is comprehensive and the methods are sound. Writing and presentation are excellent, and code for reproducing the analyses is made available (and processed data where possible), further enhancing the impact of this work. Nonetheless, reviewers felt that several aspects of the work could be strengthened on revision.

We thank the reviewers and Editor for recognising the quality and impact of our work. We are grateful to the reviewers for their constructive comments, which we feel have improved the manuscript. We have addressed all comments point-by-point.

Essential revisions:

1) NSPN sample. More details about the full sample (the full NSPN sample) compared to the included sample for this study would be useful. A visualization of the sampling design would be helpful.

We thank the reviewers for this suggestion and have provided greater characterisation of our cohort in the Materials and methods section. A visualization is provided in the new Appendix—figure 6.

“The present study included a subset of individuals from the NeuroScience in Psychiatry Network (NSPN) study (Whitaker et al., 2016; Kiddle et al., 2018). […] Secondary cohort data were eligible for inclusion in this study if T1w and MT data were available at two timepoints, resulting in N=234 healthy adolescents. Following additional quality control on raw images, surface reconstructions and MT profiles, detailed below, the evaluable sample included N=180 individuals (age stratification at baseline, n=39/41/39/33/26, inter-scan interval=15.4 ± 3.5 months, 86 females).”

2) Quality control. More details regarding the exclusion criteria and brain image quality control procedures are needed. What was the procedure for inspection, criteria used to determine exclusion, and number of scans excluded? Were both the raw images and processed images inspected for quality? Please describe the extent of manual intervention of processed images, including the protocol and number of scans that were successfully processed post-intervention and included in analyses.

To clarify, quality control and exclusion criteria were conducted at three stages: (a) on the raw images, (b) at the level surface reconstructions, and c) with respect to the MT profiles. As suggested, we have now provided more specifics in the revised Materials and methods section:

“All raw images were visually inspected by experienced researchers and 11 participants were excluded due to excessive motion artefacts. Surface reconstructions were visually inspected and manually edited for all scans, up to 10 times (Romero-Garcia et al., 2018). At each iteration, control points and grey/white matter edits were included, and the surface reconstruction was repeated. 10 participants were excluded due to poor surface reconstructions.”

“Prior to statistical assessment, we identified and removed outlier individuals (n=33). Outliers were defined as individuals with >1% nodes that deviated from the age-stratified median of nodal mean MT by >3 interquartile ranges.”

3) AIBS. Further details regarding the processing of the gene expression maps is necessary (see: Arnatkeviciūtė et al., 2019). Furthermore, authors should clearly state that AIBS data are based on adult postmortem oligodendrocyte density. As such, it seems warranted to temper their claims (example: "confirmed a spatio-temporal overlap of our findings from NSPN with myelin processes during adolescence").

We thank the reviewers for this important comment and are happy to provide further clarifications. The present analysis relied on the Neurovault implementation of AIBS gene expression processing. More details regarding the automated gene decoding are provided in the Materials and methods section:

“We measured the spatial overlap between our baseline maps of MT moments (e.g., one scan per subject; Figure 1E) and t-statistics (Figure 2A) and maps of post mortem gene expression from the Allen Institute for Brain Sciences (AIBS). […] Resultant gene associations p-values were corrected at an FDR level of 0.05 across included genes and only genes passing correction were included.”

As suggested, we tempered claims regarding the adult post-mortem nature of gene expression data in the updated Results section:

“Both analyses thus confirmed a spatial overlap of our findings from NSPN with genes associated with myelin processes that are also enriched during adolescence. […] While there was thus evidence for an adolescent developmental signal, results are nevertheless indirect also given that glial and oligodendrocyte associated genes may undergo expression changes during other periods of the lifespan (Soreq et al., 2017).”

We also added the following caveat to the Materials and methods section:

“It should be noted that the AIBS data is based on adult post-mortem and that the developmental associations are indirect associations between the enriched gene-set from spatial overlap analysis and a different developmental gene-expression dataset. Specifically, the t-statistic maps of age-related changes in the moments were spatially correlated with gene expression maps from the AIBS dataset using Neurovault (https://github.com/NeuroVault/NeuroVault/tree/master/ahba_docker). The genes derived from this spatial analysis were subsequently compared against developmental expression profiles from the BrainSpan dataset (http://www.brainspan.org/) using the CSEA developmental expression tool developed by the Dougherty lab (http://genetics.wustl.edu/jdlab/csea-tool-2/).”

4) MT and myelin. The authors should clarify how sensitive is MT to myelin. The strength of this link is crucial to a lot of the interpretation and biological novelty of the results. MT (like T1w:T2w) may be sensitive not just to myelin, but also potentially other biological variables. Some evidence/discussion of this, and the extent to which the reader can safely make the association to myelin content would be helpful in the introduction. Furthermore, many features are different between the myelin stains and MT profiles (Figure 1B). Some brief discussion of the differences would be helpful. Finally, it would be useful to clarify if to the author's knowledge this is the first time anyone has matched MT depth profiles to a histological measurement (or discuss relevant literature of past efforts).

The updated manuscript elaborated more on the relationship between MT, myelin, and the link between MT profiles and intracortical architecture. As suggested, we also extended our Discussion section on our novel comparison of MT profiles with myelin sections, specifically highlighting that differences in resolution and specificity to myelin produce differences in the profiles.

Updates were made to the Introduction, Results section and Materials and methods section:

“Several recent neuroimaging studies assessed intracortical microstructure in adolescence. One promising imaging technique is magnetisation transfer (MT), an MRI acquisition sequence that is sensitive to how water molecules interact with macromolecules in the brain, notably myelin (Schmierer et al., 2007). Although techniques such as MT cannot be equated with cortical myelin content per se (Serres et al., 2009a; Serres et al., 2009b), the MT parameter is dominated by myelin-related molecules making this technique a viable in vivo proxy for the contrast seen histologically in myelin basic protein (Koenig, 1991; Odrobina et al., 2005; Whitaker et al., 2016). A post mortem study in patients with multiple sclerosis has also shown that MT measures scale with demyelination and remyelination, suggesting dependence of this measure on myelin content (Schmierer et al., 2007).”

“Although post mortem myelin-stained sections and in vivo MT profiles differ in terms of resolution and specificity to myelin, we observed similar variations in mean and skewness of profiles across levels of laminar differentiation (Figure 1B), supporting the extension of profile analysis from histology to in vivo MT imaging.”

“The MT saturation parameter decouples the MT signal from the longitudinal relaxation rate, making it a semi-quantitative measure that is robust to relaxation times and field inhomogeneities (Weiskopf et al., 2013; Hagiwara et al., 2018).”

“To further understand architectural and cellular underpinnings of in vivo MT profiles, we assessed the similarity of the MT-derived profiles with intracortical myelin profiles computed from previously published post mortem myelin sections. While there are clear differences in the resolution and specificity to myelin between the histological staining and our in vivo proxy data, findings were overall supportive of a close association between in vivo MT profiles and those derived from myelin stained sections. This line of evidence supports the specificity of the MT features to intracortical myeloarchitecture and motivates efforts to increase the availability of post mortem histological measures for more detailed cross-validation studies of putative MRI markers of myelin.”

5) MT skewness and relative layer thickness. MT skewness may depend on the relative thickness of the different layers. It would be useful to clarify how much results explain the data beyond a simple cytoarchitectural analysis (e.g., are the depth profiles acting as a quantitative substitute for cytoarchitecture 'types', or is the inferred connection to myeloarchitecture relevant?) Furthermore, it would be useful to clarify if every node is squashed to the same absolute depth? This could impact interpretation: two areas with an identical ratio of myelination in deep relative to superficial layers would have different skewnesses if their middle layer was made thicker or thinner (thicker middle layer could push out the skewness). If this is the case, MT skewness could not simply be interpreted as a "ratio of myelination in deeper compared to more superficial layers"; it would more represent a measure of the preferential distribution of MT closer to the white matter boundary. Clarification is needed.

We thank the reviewers for this interesting question. We have carried out several precautions and additional analyses to dispel an interaction of thickness with MT profile results. First, please note that our MT sampling approach leveraged equivolumetric transformations for intracortical surface constructions (Waehnert et al., 2014), which accounted for variations in the laminar thickness with respect to curvature. Secondly, we have regressed out overall cortical thickness from the MT findings in Appendix-figure 2 and observed no major changes to the spatial pattern of MT moments or age related changes in MT moments when including thickness in the regression model. The revised paper furthermore tested whether laminar thickness had an effect on MT skewness. Specifically, we extracted laminar thickness from a recent segmentation of cortical layers in a post mortem volumetric reconstruction of a human brain (Wagstyl et al., 2019). We found that laminar thicknesses did not exhibit systematic relationships with skewness (-0.17<r<0.02). We have updated the Results section and added these findings into a new Appendix-figure 5:

“Furthermore, spatial correlation analysis suggested that MT moments were relatively independent of regional variations in laminar thickness derived from a post mortem volumetric reconstruction of a Merker-stained human brain (Wagstyl et al., 2019) (mean: -0.06<r<0.05. skewness: -0.17<r<0.02; Appendix—figure 5).”

6) Statistical testing. Every time a statistical analysis is performed (and a test statistic/p-value quoted), it should be clearly stated to the reader what it is. Non-normal distributions seem common in this data; Spearman's correlations may be appropriate. Furthermore, it appears that the spin-test based permutations are only used for some analyses but not others (e.g., only from the "Age-related changes… MT profiles" section onwards). Were spin tests used for earlier spatial association analyses? If not, can this be justified?

We thank the reviewers for this suggestion. In the updated manuscript, all spatial correlation analyses utilized Spearman correlations and corresponding p-values were determined via spin tests (AlexanderBloch et al., 2018). We have updated Figure 1 and Figure 4 to reflect the slight changes in statistics. The Results section and Materials and methods section have also been amended as follows:

“At a whole cortex-level, mean MT was highest in idiotypic cortex and decreased with less laminar differentiation, while skewness exhibited an opposite pattern (spatial correlation=-0.23, pspin<0.001; Figure 1C).”

“This sensory-fugal gradient reflects systematic variations in the MT profiles; it was strongly correlated with MT profile skewness (r=0.91, pspin<0.001; Figure 1D) and weakly with mean MT (r=0.19, pspin<0.001).”

“By relating age-related shifts in MPC to anchors with age-related changes in MT moments, we found that greater decreases in MT skewness were related to sensory anchor coupling (r=-0.63, pspin<0.001; Figure 4C). Conversely, restricted age-related skewness changes were associated with paralimbic anchor coupling (r=0.65, pspin<0.001).”

“We assessed the correspondence of the MPC gradient with the MT moments using node-wise Spearman correlations, with p-values determined against 10000 null models obtained from spin permutations (Alexander-Bloch et al., 2018). All spatial correlations were estimated and tested in the same way throughout the study.”

7) Utility of low-dimensional embedding. Regarding this statement: "Nodes closer in this embedding space increase in microstructural similarity during adolescence, whereas distant nodes decouple", it is unclear why a low-dimensional embedding is useful a direct measurement of change is available. Couldn't you more easily do an analysis directly (e.g., investigate the structure of the 'increasing' edges?) What is gained from this harder-to-interpret nonlinear embedding that helps test/interpret your hypotheses? Perhaps more importantly, it is unclear if this statement is supported by the data: Wouldn't the embedding put nodes close in the space that have similar "patterns" of age-related MPC change with other network nodes? In the plot of the behavior of G1_dev, it does not display the claimed behavior (Figure 4B) – instead, it seems like the largest values of G1_dev already have ~maximal G1_MPC, and thus barely change at all between the <16 and >24 groups. Clarification is warranted.

We agree with the reviewers’ recommendation that the G1_dev would be better described as “Nodes closer in this embedding space have more similar patterns of age-related change in MPC, whereas distant nodes undergo dissimilar development” (updated in the Results section). It follows from this definition that positioning of G1_dev reflects the principal axis of variance in developmental changes, but that does not necessarily correspond to changes in the values of G1_MPC.

Low-dimensional embedding was used to synoptically characterise coordinated age-related changes in MPC. We noted many significant age-related changes in MPC (1564 increases vs 534 decreases at qFDR<0.025), but they were not clearly circumscribed to a set of brain regions or level of laminar differentiation (Figure 4—figure supplement 1), therefore the low-dimensional embedding was helpful to visualise and interpret the patterns of change. In fact, the embedding approach led to the discovery that adolescent development expanded the existing sensory-fugal gradient of microstructural differentiation, and this would not have been easily observable when using the high-dimensional representations.

8) Biological interpretation. In the discussion, the biological interpretation of the results at times is rather thin. If MT is a strong myelin marker, then why would myeloarchitecture be reorganizing like this (e.g., to support what behavior)? Axonal myelination improves transmission speeds and prevents the formation of new synaptic connections. Could you speculate on how this might alter behavior relevant to the longitudinal changes analyzed? Relevant papers to consider include:

*Micheva et al., (2016).

*Braitenberg, (1962).

*Huntenburg et al., (2017).

We have expanded the biological interpretation of our findings and provided further speculation on potential implications for behaviour, also citing the suggested papers.:

“Both sets of age-related changes were strongly and preferentially located in heteromodal and unimodal association cortex; yet, they only partially overlapped, a finding also supported by gene expression analysis. […] The architectural changes observed in the present cohort may represent a microstructural substrate that could determine overall cognitive and social capacities or susceptibility to a range of neuropsychiatric and neurological conditions occurring in late childhood and adolescence.”

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    Supplementary file 1. Supplementary materials.
    elife-50482-supp1.docx (16.1KB, docx)
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.50482.013
    Transparent reporting form
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.50482.014

    Data Availability Statement

    Preprocessed microstructure profiles are available via GitHub: https://github.com/MICA-MNI/micaopen/tree/master/a_moment_of_change (Paquola, 2019; copy archived at https://github.com/elifesciences-publications/micaopen/tree/master/a_moment_of_change).

    The repository also includes Matlab and R scripts to reproduce the primary analyses and figures.

    Preprocessed microstructure profiles are available via GitHub: https://github.com/MICA-MNI/micaopen/tree/master/a_moment_of_change (copy archived at https://github.com/elifesciences-publications/micaopen/tree/master/a_moment_of_change). The repository also includes Matlab and R scripts to reproduce the primary analyses and figures.

    The following datasets were generated:


    Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

    RESOURCES