Skip to main content
Springer logoLink to Springer
. 2019 Sep 24;145(2):265–276. doi: 10.1007/s11060-019-03292-y

Cognitive functioning and predictors thereof in patients with 1–10 brain metastases selected for stereotactic radiosurgery

Wietske C M Schimmel 1,2,3,4,, Karin Gehring 2,3, Patrick E J Hanssens 1,2, Margriet M Sitskoorn 2,3
PMCID: PMC6856035  PMID: 31552588

Abstract

Purpose

Information on predictive factors of cognitive functioning in patients with (multiple) brain metastases (BM) selected for radiosurgery may allow for more individual care and may play a role in predicting cognitive outcome after radiosurgery. The aim of this study was to evaluate cognitive performance, and predictors thereof, in patients with 1–10 BM before radiosurgery.

Methods

Cognition was measured before radiosurgery using a standardized neuropsychological test battery in patients with 1–10 BM (expected survival > 3 months; KPS ≥ 70; no prior BM treatment). Regression formulae were constructed to calculate sociodemographically corrected z scores. Group and individual cognitive functioning was analyzed. Multivariable regression was used to explore potential predictors.

Results

Patients (N = 92) performed significantly worse than controls (N = 104) on all 11 test variables (medium-large effect sizes for 8 variables). Percentages of impairment were highest for information processing (55.3%), dexterity (43.2%) and cognitive flexibility (28.7%). 62% and 46% of patients had impairments in at least two, or three test variables, respectively. Models including combinations of clinical and psychological variables were predictive of verbal memory, psychomotor speed, information processing and dexterity. Neither number nor volume of metastases predicted patients’ test performance.

Conclusions

Already before radiosurgery, almost half of the patients suffered from severe cognitive deficits in at least three test variables. At group and individual level, information processing, cognitive flexibility, and dexterity were most affected. These cognitive impairments may impair daily functioning and patients’ ability to make (shared) treatment decisions. Both clinical (symptomatic BM; timing of BM diagnosis) and psychological (mental fatigue) characteristics influenced cognitive performance.

Clinical trial information

Cognition and Radiation Study A (CAR-Study A; ClinicalTrials.gov Identifier: NCT02953756; Medical Ethics Committee file number: NL53472.028.15/P1515).

Electronic supplementary material

The online version of this article (10.1007/s11060-019-03292-y) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.

Keywords: Brain metastases, Cognitive functioning, Stereotactic radiosurgery, Gamma knife radiosurgery

Introduction

Stereotactic radiosurgery (SRS) is increasingly applied in patients with brain metastases (BM) as it is expected to cause less cognitive damage than whole brain radiation therapy (WBRT) because it allows precise radiation delivery to the BM only. Patients with newly diagnosed BM who are accepted for SRS alone represent a selective group of patients with a relatively good performance status (Karnofsky Performance Status ≥ 70) and an expected survival time of at least three months [1]. Nonetheless, before BM treatment, many patients experience cognitive impairments that may be caused by several factors, including the BM itself, medication use, the primary cancer, or side effects of systemic treatment [2]. Thorough assessment and understanding of these impairments is of high relevance because these impairments, e.g., slow processing of information, may negatively affect patients’ ability to reason through (shared) medical treatment decisions, daily functioning and ultimately patients’ quality of life [3]. In addition, pretreatment neuropsychological assessment is crucial for the evaluation of cognitive changes after SRS [4].

There have been relatively few studies in patients with newly diagnosed BM who undergo SRS that evaluated (baseline) cognitive functions with objective neuropsychological tests, as opposed to insensitive measures for this purpose such as the Mini-Mental Status Examination (MMSE) [5]. Moreover, in reports thereof, baseline test results were not the primary focus and were only (very) briefly discussed. The majority of patients (ranging from 53 to 67%) in these studies showed mild to severe impairments in at least one cognitive domain. Executive function, verbal learning and memory, dexterity, information processing, and visuoconstruction were the cognitive domains most frequently affected [610], which is in line with research in patients with BM in general [1115]. Previous studies, however, concerned patients with a limited number of BM 1–4 whereas the use of SRS is expanding to patients with multiple (> 4) BM [1618]. More recently, total volume of BM, as opposed to their number, has gained interest as a predictor for outcomes of patients with BM (including overall survival, local control and distant progression of BM) [1922]. However, thus far, only a few studies have examined the relationship between number and volume of BM and (pretreatment) cognitive functioning in patients with BM. In univariate analyses, a larger total volume of BM was suggested to be associated with worse baseline cognitive performance in four studies, including two small pilot studies [6, 8, 10, 15]. The number of BM was however not associated with cognitive performance in these studies, suggesting that cognitive functions are more affected by the total burden of BM than by the number of lesions [15]. To our knowledge only one previous study explored potential predictors of pretreatment cognition in patients with BM in a multivariable manner [15]. This study showed that total volume of BM was a predictor for baseline cognitive impairment in patients that were randomly assigned to WBRT with or without motexafin gadolinium.

In the current study, we investigated the incidence and severity of cognitive impairments in patients with 1 to 10 BM before Gamma Knife radiosurgery (GKRS). Both number and volume of BM are examined as potential predictors of baseline cognitive functioning. In addition, the role of other clinical variables (including KPS and diagnosis-specific graded prognostic assessment (DS-GPA [23]) and psychological variables, such as fatigue and symptoms of anxiety and depression, known to impact cognitive test performance [2426], were explored.

Methods and materials

Baseline test data of patients from the ongoing prospective longitudinal observational Cognition and Radiation Study A (CAR-Study A; ClinicalTrials.gov Identifier: NCT02953756) were analyzed. In addition, non-cancer controls were recruited. This study was approved by the Medical Ethics Committee Brabant (file NL53472.028.15/P1515).

Patients

Adult patients were recruited at the Elisabeth-TweeSteden Hospital (ETZ; Tilburg, the Netherlands). Eligibility criteria were previously described by Verhaak et al. [27]. Most important inclusion criteria included: 1–10 newly diagnosed BM on a diagnostic or referral MRI-scan from a histologically proven malignant cancer, KPS ≥ 70, total tumor volume ≤ 30 cm3, and expected survival > 3 months. Exclusion criteria included: active primary brain tumor, small cell lung cancer, leptomeningeal metastases, or progressive symptomatic systemic disease without further treatment options, prior treatment directed at the BM (e.g., radiation therapy or surgery). Patients were screened by the radiation-oncologist during the first consultation. Neuropsychological assessment (NPA) was performed by a trained neuropsychologist in the morning before treatment.

Non-cancer controls

A normative group of adult non-cancer controls, as previously described by Verhaak et al. [27], were recruited by convenience sampling from the general community and were selected to be, as much as possible, comparable to the general population and our patient-group, except for the fact that they were not allowed to have (a history of) cancer or severe cerebrovascular disease in the past year. Eligible controls received a study information letter and a medical checklist. All patients and controls signed informed consent before the NPA.

Measures

Medical records were consulted to extract patient characteristics. BM diagnosed > 30 days from the diagnosis of the primary tumor were considered metachronous (all other BM were considered synchronous). A well-established test battery [2, 28] was used that consisted of six neuropsychological tests, generating 11 test variables. In addition, three questionnaires [2931] were administered (Table 1). FACT-Br data was not evaluated in this study.

Table 1.

Neuropsychological test battery including questionnaires

Neuropsychological test Description/cognitive domain
Hopkins verbal learning test-revised (HVLT-R) Verbal memory test (12 target words, 6 parallel versions)
1. HVLT-R immediate recall Short-term verbal memory span
2. HVLT-R delayed recall Longer-term verbal memory
3. HVLT-R recognition Delayed verbal recognition (correct responses minus semantically related and unrelated false-positive errors)
Trail making test (TMT) Test of visual conceptual and visuomotor tracking
4. TMT A Psychomotor speed
5. TMT B Cognitive flexibility (aspect of executive functioning)

Controlled oral word association test

6. COWA

Speeded verbal fluency test (requires aspects of executive functioning; 2 parallel versions)
WAIS digit span Forward and backward repetitions of series of digits
7. Digit span forward Immediate attention
8. Digit span backward Working memory

WAIS digit symbol

9. Digit symbol

Symbol substitution test of information processing speed (requires visuomotor coordination and sustained attention)
Lafayette grooved pegboard (GP) A manipulative dexterity test
10. GP dominant hand Motor dexterity dominant hand
11. GP non-dominant hand Motor dexterity non-dominant hand
Questionnaire Description
Hospital and Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS) Symptoms of anxiety and depression
Multidimensional Fatigue Inventory (MFI) Symptoms of general fatigue, physical fatigue, reduced activation, reduced motivation and mental fatigue
Functional assessment of cancer therapy-brain (FACT-Br) General quality of life (QOL) questionnaire that reflects symptoms or problems associated with brain malignancies across five scales

WAIS Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale

Statistical analyses

Descriptive and comparative (Chi-square test; independent samples t-test) analyses were performed with respect to characteristics of patients and controls.

By means of multiple linear regression analyses, that regressed raw cognitive test scores of the control sample on age, sex and educational level, normative formulae were generated [32]. Raw Trails B scores were adjusted for sex, age, educational level and the Trails A score to derive the interference index. Sociodemographically-adjusted z scores were derived: Patients’ z score = patient’s raw score minus the predicted score divided by the SD of the control sample’s residuals. Higher z scores reflect better cognitive performance.

To compare cognitive performance between patients and controls, one-tailed one-sample z tests were performed. Patients’ mean z scores are equal to Glass’ delta effect sizes (MeanPatients − MeanControls / SDControls; [33]), where 0.2 = small, 0.5 = medium, and 0.8 = large effect [34]. Impaired cognitive performance was defined as a z-score ≤  − 1.5. Percentages of patients with impaired performance per test variable, and on one, two or more tests were calculated.

Correlations were explored of patients’ cognitive performances with clinical and psychological characteristics. A maximum of three additional predictors with the highest significant (p < 0.05) correlations were selected per test variable. Hierarchical multiple regression analyses were then performed to regress patients’ z scores on the selected predictors. In all models, number (dummy-coded) and volume of BM were entered separately in Block 1. To reduce false discovery rate (FDR) due to multiple testing, alpha’s were corrected per hypothesis, according to the Benjamini–Hochberg method [35]. All statistical analyses were performed with SPSS Statistics 25.0.

Results

Participants’ characteristics

In total, 92 patients and 104 controls were included. Patients and controls did not differ in terms of sex, age and education (Table 2). Forty percent of patients had more than three BM and the most common primary tumor was non-small cell lung cancer (NSCLC; 60%). Median total volume of BM was 5.64 cm3. For 16 patients (17.4%) and 5 controls (4.8%) scores on one or more tests were missing due to: invalid assessment (HVLT-R recognition, TMT), unfamiliarity with the alphabet (TMT), visual problems (TMT, Digit Symbol, GP), and impairments in dexterity (TMT, Digit Symbol, GP).

Table 2.

Characteristics of patients and controls

No. of patients (%) No. of controls (%) Test statistic p value
Number of participants 92 104
Sex χ2 = 0.18A 0.67
 Male 47 (51) 50 (48)
 Female 45 (49) 54 (52)

Age in years, mean ± SD

(range)

62 ± 10

(31–80)

59 ± 11

(31–87)

t = 1.53B 0.13
Educational level χ2 = 4.63A 0.10
 Low 28 (31) 25 (24)
 Middle 37 (40) 33 (32)
 High 27 (29) 46 (44)
KPS
 70–80 33 (36)
 90–100 59 (64) N/A
DS-GPA
 Class I (3.5–4 points) 8 (9) N/A
 Class II (2.5–3 points) 33 (35)
 Class III (1.5–2 points) 44 (48)
 Class IV (0–1 points) 7 (8)
Primary cancer
 Lung (NSCLC) 55 (60) N/A
 Renal 15 (16)
 Melanoma 12 (13)
 Other 10 (11)
Number of BM
 1 32 (35)
 2–4 29 (31) N/A
 5–10 31 (34)
BM volume by patient (cm3)
Median (range) 5.64 (0.02–31.15) N/A
Timing of BM diagnosis
 Synchronous 28 (30)
 Metachronous 64 (70)
Extracranial metastasesa
 Yes 66 (72)
 No 26 (28) N/A
BM Symptoms at diagnosis
 Symptomatic 64 (70)
 Asymptomatic 28 (30) N/A
Systemic therapy
 No 39 (42) N/A
 Yes 53 (58)
 Chemotherapyb 37 (40)
HADS scoresc, mean ± SD
 Anxiety subscale 7.3 ± 4.4 4.4 ± 2.8 t = 5.36B < 0.001
 Depression subscale 5.7 ± 4.1 3.5 ± 2.9 t = 4.37B < 0.001

Educational level according to Verhage (1964; 7 classes): low = 1–4, middle = 5, high = 6–7

N/A not applicable, KPS Karnofsky performance scale, DS-GPA diagnosis-specific graded prognostic assessment, NSCLC non-small cell lung cancer, BM brain metastases

aIncluding lymphatic metastases at baseline or before

bAlone or in combination with other systemic therapies

cHospital Anxiety and Depression Scale with two 7-item subscales; range 0–21 points; higher scores indicate more symptoms of anxiety or depression

AChi-square test of homogeneity

BIndependent-samples T test

Group-level cognitive performance

Patients performed significantly worse than non-cancer controls on all 11 test variables with medium to large effect sizes for 8 out of 11 variables (Table 3). Lowest performance was found on measures of psychomotor speed, cognitive flexibility, information processing, and dexterity of both dominant and non-dominant hand.

Table 3.

Cognitive performance at group and individual level

Test variables Group level Individual level
Mean Z scores of patients versus controlsa Impaired performance per test variableb
z score d z test p value Effect sizee Patients (%) Controls (%) χ2A p value
HVLT-R immediate recall − 0.52 − 4.95  < 0.001* − 0.52 (medium) 27.2 4.9 18.60 < 0.001*
HVLT-R delayed recall − 0.27 − 2.59 0.010* − 0.27 (small) 15.2 4.8 6.04 0.014*
HVLT-R recognition − 0.21 − 1.99 0.047* − 0.21 (small) 14.3 8.7 1.54 0.215
TMT A − 0.99 − 9.21  < 0.001* − 0.99 (large) 25.3 7.7 11.08 0.001*
TMT B|Ac − 1.49 − 13.35  < 0.001* − 1.49 (large) 28.8 5.8 17.99 < 0.001*
COWA − 0.63 − 6.06  < 0.001* − 0.63 (medium) 27.2 7.7 13.23  < 0.001*
Digit span forward − 0.43 − 4.10  < 0.001* − 0.43 (small) 10.9 5.8 1.64 0.200
Digit span backward − 0.78 − 7.51  < 0.001* − 0.78 (medium) 22.8 6.8 10.15 0.001*
Digit symbol − 1.49 − 13.78  < 0.001* − 1.49 (large) 55.3 6.7 54.05 < 0.001*
GP dominant hand − 1.43 − 13.42  < 0.001* − 1.43 (large) 27.3 6.9 14.41 < 0.001*
GP non-dominant hand − 1.63 − 15.25  < 0.001* − 1.63 (large) 43.2 5.9 36.94 < 0.001*

HVLT-R Hopkins verbal learning test revised, TMT trail making test, COWA Controlled Oral Word Association, GP grooved pegboard

*p ≤ 0.05 (group-level) and p ≤ 0.04 (individual-level): alpha was corrected using the Benjamini–Hochberg method Benjamini and Hochberg [35]

aOne-tailed one-sample z tests (N controls = 104; M = 0; SD = 1; N patients = 80–92)

bCognitive impairment was defined as a z score ≤ − 1.5 (N patients = 80–92; N controls = 102–104)

cTMT B|A: Trails B score adjusted for sex, age, educational level and the Trails A score

dHigher z scores reflect better performance

eGlass’ delta: Interpretable as Cohen’s d effect sizes: ≥ 0.20–0.49 = small, ≥ 0.50–0.79 = medium, ≥ 0.9 = large [34]

AChi-square test of homogeneity

Individual cognitive performance

Percentages of impairment on all 11 test variables were higher in patients than in non-cancer controls. This difference was statistically significant, except for verbal recognition and attention (Table 3). These percentages were highest for information processing (55.3%), dexterity (43.2%; non-dominant hand) and cognitive flexibility (28.8%). Compared to controls, more patients showed cognitive impairments in more tests (Table 4). Significantly more patients (62% and 46%) than controls (18% and 3%) had an impairment in at least two or three test variables respectively.

Table 4.

Cognitive performance at the individual level impairment on one or more test variables

No. of tests Patients (%) (n = 76) Controls (%) (n = 99) χ2b p value
≥ 1 test 76.3 43.4 19.05  < 0.001c
≥ 2 tests 61.8 18.2 35.10  < 0.001c
≥ 3 tests 46.1 3.0 46.81  < 0.001c
≥ 4 tests 36.8 3.0 33.72  < 0.001c
≥ 5 tests 23.7 0 26.14  < 0.001c
≥ 6 tests 14.5 0 15.29  < 0.001c
≥ 7 tests 11.8 0 12.36  < 0.001c
≥ 8 tests 6.6 0 6.71 0.010c
≥ 9 tests 0 0 N/A N/A
≥ 10 tests 0 0 N/A N/A
11 tests 0 0 N/A N/A

aImpaired performance (z score ≤ -− 1.5) of patients with complete test scores on all tests. For 16 patients (17.4%) and 5 controls (4.8%) scores on one or more tests were missing due to: invalid assessment (HVLT-R recognition, TMT), unfamiliarity with the alphabet (TMT), visual problems (TMT, Digit Symbol, GP), and impairments in manual dexterity (TMT, Digit Symbol, GP)

bChi-square test of homogeneity

cStatistical significance was considered as p ≤ 0.05: alpha was corrected according to the Benjamini–Hochberg method Benjamini and Hochberg [35]

Predictors of baseline cognitive performance

Supplementary Tables 1 and 2 present the results of the exploratory correlation analyses (Online Resource 1). A metachronous diagnosis of BM (compared to synchronous) was significantly associated with worse performance on 7 out of the 11 test variables. Chemotherapy was significantly negatively correlated with performance on 3 test variables (immediate and delayed memory and psychomotor speed). Mental fatigue was significantly negatively associated with psychomotor speed, information processing, and dexterity. Higher KPS was significantly associated with greater dexterity.

Four additional clinical (KPS; chemotherapy; symptomatic versus asymptomatic BM; timing of BM diagnosis) and four psychological predictors (Reduced Activation; Reduced Motivation; Mental Fatigue; symptoms of depression) were selected for the hierarchical multiple regression analyses. None of the initial regression models with only number and volume of the BM as predictors, nor the predictors themselves, were statistically significant (Table 5). The addition of the clinical and psychological predictors led to a statistically significant increase in explained variance in five models for measures of verbal memory, psychomotor speed, information processing and dexterity. In two models (delayed recognition and information processing), timing of BM diagnosis was the only significant predictor, whereby patients with metachronous BM performed worse. Post hoc descriptive analyses showed that of the patients with a metachronous diagnosis, 44% had NSCLC, 55% received (prior) chemotherapy and 53% had a high KPS of 90–100 (vs. 96%, 7% and 89% in the synchronous group, respectively). For immediate verbal memory, symptomatic (versus asymptomatic) BM was a significant predictor, whereby patients with symptomatic BM performed worse. For psychomotor speed, mental fatigue was the only significant predictor in the model, with slower psychomotor speed in patients with more symptoms of mental fatigue. A final significant model did not yield any significant individual predictors (dexterity non-dominant hand).

Table 5.

Multiple hierarchical regression predicting patients’ cognitive test performance

Test variable Model Predictor B SE B p* F(df) R2 ∆R2 p* (∆R2)
HVLT-R immediate recall Model 1 0.220 1.50 (3,88) 0.049
Number of BMSingle − 0.635 0.363 0.084
Number of BM5–10 0.023 0.366 0.949
Total volume of BM 0.002 0.020 0.922
Model 2 0.011* 2.96 (6,85) 0.173 0.124 0.008*
Number of BMSingle − 0.554 0.353 0.120
Number of BM5–10 − 0.081 0.352 0.818
Total volume of BM 0.014 0.020 0.474
Chemotherapy − 0.402 0.319 0.211
Symptomatic (y/n) − 0.743 0.318 0.022*
Timing of BM diagnosis − 0.472 0.343 0.173
HVLT-R delayed recall Model 1 0.046 2.77 (3,88) 0.086
Number of BMSingle − 0.540 0.323 0.098
Number of BM5–10 0.326 0.754
Total volume of BM 0.103 0.017 0.102
Model 2 0.013* 3.10 (5,86) 0.153 0.066 0.039*
Number of BMSingle − 0.388 0.320 0.229
Number of BM5–10 0.095 0.319 0.766
Total volume of BM − 0.030 0.017 0.079
Chemotherapy − 0.291 0.290 0.319
Timing of BM diagnosis − 0.533 0.308 0.087
HVLT-R recognition Model 1 0.426 0.94 (3,87) 0.031
Number of BMSingle − 0.166 0.356 0.642
Number of BM5–10 0.071 0.359 0.844
Total volume of BM − 0.028 0.019 0.149
Model 2 0.014* 3.04 (5,85) 0.151 0.120 0.004*
Number of BMSingle − 0.049 0.345 0.888
Number of BM5–10 − 0.049 0.342 0.887
Total volume of BM − 0.019 0.019 0.313
Symptomatic (y/n) − 0.568 0.308 0.068
Timing of BM diagnosis − 0.790 0.300 0.010*
TMT A Model 1 0.135 1.91 (3,82) 0.065
Number of BMSingle − 0.425 0.458 0.356
Number of BM5–10 0.541 0.459 0.242
Total volume of BM − 0.025 0.025 0.302
Model 2 0.005* 3.42 (6,79) 0.206 0.141 0.005*
Number of BMSingle − 0.172 0.438 0.695
Number of BM5–10 0.504 0.433 0.249
Total volume of BM − 0.020 0.023 0.389
Chemotherapy − 0.572 0.398 0.154
Timing of BM diagnosis − 0.299 0.434 0.492
Mental fatigue − 0.109 0.046 0.021*
TMT B|A Model 1 0.683 0.501 (3,76) 0.019
Number of BMSingle  − 0.567 0.723 0.435
Number of BM5-10  − 0.466 0.709 0.513
Total volume of BM  − 0.028 0.038 0.455
COWA Model 1 0.289 1.27 (3,87) 0.042
Number of BMSingle  − 0.515 0.315 0.106
Number of BM5-10  − 0.058 0.318 0.856
Total volume of BM  − 0.006 0.017 0.708
Model 2 0.091 1.97 (5,85) 0.104 0.062 0.059
Number of BMSingle  − 0.419 0.312 0.183
Number of BM5–10  − 0.036 0.311 0.908
Total volume of BM  − 0.010 0.017 0.562
Timing of BM diagnosis  − 0.360 0.275 0.195
Reduced motivation  − 0.059 0.033 0.078
Digit span forward Model 1 0.741 0.417 (3,88) 0.014
Number of BMSingle 0.015 0.240 0.950
Number of BM5–10 0.069 0.242 0.777
Total volume of BM  − 0.014 0.013 0.276
Digit span backward Model 1 0.163 1.75 (3,88) 0.083
Number of BMSingle  − 0.128 0.267 0.632
Number of BM5–10  − 0.144 0.269 0.594
Total volume of BM  − 0.029 0.014 0.046
Model 2 0.108 1.96 (4,87) 0.083 0.026 0.118
Number of BMSingle  − 0.167 0.266 0.532
Number of BM5–10  − 0.188 0.268 0.486
Total volume of BM  − 0.022 0.015 0.138
Symptomatic (y/n)  − 0.379 0.240 0.118
Digit symbol Model 1 0.518 0.764 (3,80) 0.028
Number of BMSingle 0.063 0.343 0.855
Number of BM5–10  − 0.033 0.353 0.925
Total volume of BM  − 0.028 0.019 0.150
Model 2 0.010* 3.03 (6,77) 0.191 0.163 0.003*
Number of BMSingle 0.226 0.324 0.488
Number of BM5–10  − 0.058 0.328 0.859
Total volume of BM  − 0.023 0.018 0.210
Timing of BM diagnosis  − 0.625 0.295 0.037*
Mental fatigue  − 0.041 0.038 0.281
Symptoms of depression  − 0.068 0.037 0.072
GP dominant hand Model 1 0.511 0.78 (3,83) 0.027
Number of BMSingle  − 0.720 0.771 0.353
Number of BM5–10  − 0.558 0.787 0.480
Total volume of BM  − 0.038 0.041 0.361
Model 2 0.194 1.56 (4,82) 0.070 0.043 0.054
Number of BMSingle  − 0.602 0.761 0.431
Number of BM5–10  − 0.555 0.774 0.475
Total volume of BM  − 0.030 0.041 0.466
Mental fatigue  − 0.152 0.078 0.054
GP non-dominant hand Model 1 0.977 0.07(3,84) 0.002
Number of BMSingle  − 0.238 0.601 0.693
Number of BM5–10  − 0.238 0.609 0.697
Total volume of BM  − 0.002 0.032 0.961
Model 2 0.018* 2.73(6, 81) 0.168 0.166 0.002*
Number of BMSingle  − 0.137 0.576 0.813
Number of BM5–10  − 0.312 0.571 0.587
Total volume of BM  − 0.002 0.030 0.949
KPS 0.031 0.029 0.284
Timing of BM diagnosis  − 1.03 0.526 0.054
Reduced activity  − 0.117 0.062 0.062

Bold values indicate the statistically significant difference

HVLT-R Hopkins verbal learning test revised, TMT trail making test, COWA Controlled Oral Word Association, GP grooved pegboard, BM brain metastases, KPS Karnofsky Performance Index, B unstandardized regression coefficient, SE B standard error B, df degrees of freedom

Coding of predictors: single BM: Number of BMsingle = 1; 2–4 BM: Number of BMsingle = 0, Number of BM5-10 = 0, 5–10 BM: Number of BM5-10 = 1; Symptomatic: yes = 1, no/asymptomatic = 0; Timing of BM diagnosis: synchronous = 0, metachronous = 1

*Statistical significance was considered as p ≤ 0.005 (models 1) and p ≤ 0.03 (models 2), alpha was corrected according to the Benjamini–Hochberg method [35] and as p ≤ 0.05 for the individual regression coefficients and change in R2 per model

Discussion

In this study we examined the incidence and severity of cognitive impairment, and clinical as well as psychological predictors thereof, in selected patients with 1–10 BM who were accepted for GKRS. Cognitive performance was measured with a well-established neuropsychological test battery. Previous studies on cognitive functioning were focused on patients with 1–4 BM or made use of an insensitive measure to assess cognitive test performance (the MMSE) [5].

At group level, we found lowest cognitive test performance (large effect sizes; means that ranged between − 1 and − 1.6 SD below the normative mean) on measures of psychomotor speed, cognitive flexibility, information processing, and dexterity of both dominant and non-dominant hand. At the individual level, cognitive performance was most frequently impaired with respect to measures of short-term verbal memory span, cognitive flexibility, information processing, and dexterity of both dominant and non-dominant hand. Although at group level, patients performed significantly worse than controls (with small effect sizes) on measures of verbal recognition and immediate attention. At the individual level, however, there were no significant differences in the frequencies of impairment for these two measures. These results are largely in line with previous studies in patients with BM: cognitive impairment in one or more tests before treatment of BM ranged between 53 and 80% (76% in our sample) and was most clearly demonstrated in the domains of executive functioning (including cognitive flexibility), verbal and visual memory, dexterity and psychomotor speed [6, 7, 9, 10, 36, 37].

We noted a degree of impairment in information processing in our study that is higher than in other studies. Some of these studies used different neuropsychological tests, however, both studies by Chang et al. [6, 7] used the WAIS Digit Symbol test as well. At baseline, only 7% of their patients showed impaired performance in the pilot study [6] and baseline z scores in the larger randomized trial ranged between − 0.1 and − 0.4 [7] whereas in our sample, 55% of patients had impaired performance on this test and the mean z score was − 1.5. This difference might be explained by differences in the study samples: compared to our study, their sample consisted of patients with fewer (1–3) BM, higher median KPS and lower median total BM volume BM. In addition, although having severe problems with dexterity was one of the exclusion criteria in our study, impairments in dexterity were (highly) prevalent in our patient sample: 27% of patients showed impaired dominant hand dexterity (the mean z score for this measure was − 1.43 in our study vs.  − 1.30 in the SRS-arm of Chang et al. [7]). These impairments may have influenced performance on the other measures with high dominant hand motor demands [38] and help explain the poor performance on information processing, psychomotor speed, and cognitive flexibility. The use of (additional) neuropsychological tests with minimal motor requirements should be considered in future trials in this patient population, as the assessment of speed (information processing or psychomotor) is aimed at understanding cognitive rather than physical function [38].

Multivariable regression was used to examine whether number or volume of BM was predictive of pretreatment cognitive test performance. Neither number nor volume of BM were significant predictors in any of these initial models. Similarly, in previous studies based on univariate analyses, number of BM was not associated with cognitive performance. However, the same studies found negative associations uncorrected for multiple testing between total BM volume and measures of attention, verbal memory, information processing and executive functions [6, 8, 10, 15]. We also found a significant negative univariate association between volume of BM and working memory but in multivariable analyses volume of BM was not a significant predictor of working memory.

Hierarchical multivariable models including clinical as well as psychological variables were predictive of performance on six measures of verbal memory, psychomotor speed, information processing, and dexterity. Timing of BM diagnosis was a significant individual predictor in two out of five significant regression models: patients with a synchronous (versus metachronous) diagnosis of BM performed better on verbal recognition and had higher information processing (speed). This might be explained by the fact that these patients were still largely treatment-naïve and were in a better overall (higher KPS), and cognitive condition. Patients with a metachronous diagnosis of BM on the other hand, already received various types of systemic treatment, including chemotherapy, for their primary tumor, which may have contributed to the cognitive impairments [39, 40] already before the diagnosis of the BM. These (cancer-related) cognitive impairments primarily involve the domains of memory, attention, executive functioning, and processing speed [41].

Despite the fact that the patients in our study had significantly more symptoms of anxiety and depression than our controls we found no evidence for a direct effect of anxiety and depression on cognitive test performance in our prediction models. This is in line with a previous study in patients with BM and indicates that anxiety and depression may not be (primary) contributors to cognitive impairment in these patients [37]. Mental fatigue however was predictive of reduced psychomotor speed. Efforts should be continued to investigate specific patient- and tumor-specific factors that can predict cognitive test performance. Identification of these characteristics allow for more individually tailored care for patients. In addition, thorough assessment of cognitive impairment, and understanding of the predictors thereof, is crucial for the evaluation of cognitive changes after SRS [4].

This study has some limitations to be considered. Our patients had BM originating from various primary tumor histologies. Since prognosis, systemic treatment, and timing of BM may vary with type of primary cancer [42], this might have affected cognitive test performance. However, as most BM originate from lung cancer, lung cancer patients represent the majority of patients with BM, both in clinical practice and in clinical trials (including this study). In addition, we did not examine or take into account the location(s) of the BM. Further study is required to examine the impact of BM location (e.g., supratentorial, cerebellar, brainstem and ‘other’) on cognitive test performance as cognitive impairment is related to the site of tumor growth [43]. Although we did not find a direct effect of number and volume of BM on cognitive test performance in our relatively large sample of patients with 1–10 BM, it is of interest to investigate whether change (reduction or progression) in number and volume influences change in cognitive test performances after SRS. Li et al. [44] showed that greater volume reduction in total volume of BM was associated with a delay in cognitive decline after WBRT [44].

Significant associations between cognitive test performance and daily functional independence have been found in brain tumor patients [45]. This study used mostly the same neuropsychological tests as the current study. Strongest associations were found for executive functioning (TMT B), language comprehension (COWA) and verbal learning and memory (HVLT-R). Patients with BM in our study showed significant impairments in all of these tests. These impairments may cause serious difficulties in day-to-day activities (e.g., daily chores, preparing dinner or communicating with family and friends). For example, patients may experience difficulties with the ability to plan ahead (related to impaired cognitive flexibility), slowness of comprehension and processing of information (related to impaired processing speed), and difficulties in learning and remembering new information (related to functions of memory), and difficulties in performing adequate movements appropriate to a certain task (related to impairments in dexterity and executive functioning). In addition, these difficulties in everyday living may increase the caregiver burden [45].

Assessment of cognitive deficits is also crucial in understanding patients’ ability in weighing the risks (cognitive impairment, distant recurrences, neurotoxicity) and benefits (cognitive preservation, local control, distant control) in coming to a treatment decision (e.g., WBRT, SRS or best supportive care) [46]. A previous study indicated that over half of the patients with BM (prior to BM treatment) had a diminished ability to reason through medical treatment decisions [47], this was associated (same study sample) with worse verbal memory and information processing [48, 49]. In our sample, 55% (information processing), 27% (immediate verbal memory and verbal fluency) and 23% (working memory) of patients had impairments in these cognitive domains, emphasizing the relevance of pretreatment neuropsychological assessment. Patients at risk may need additional (written) information and guidance through the process of understanding treatment choices. Early detection of these cognitive impairments may facilitate cognitive intervention planning. Intervention (e.g., cognitive rehabilitation programs; [50] at an early stage may benefit the quality of survival in these patients, which is of particular interest for the growing number of (subgroups of) patients with longer expected survival.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Funding

This study is funded by ZonMw, a Dutch organization for Health Research and Development (Project Number 842003006) and Tilburg University (The Netherlands).

Compliance with ethical standards

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Footnotes

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

References

  • 1.Loeffler JS (2019) Overview of the treatment of brain metastases. Wen PY, ed. UpToDate. Waltham, MA: UpToDate Inc.https://www.uptodate.com. Accessed June 14 2019
  • 2.Witgert ME, Meyers CA. Neurocognitive and quality of life measures in patients with metastatic brain disease. Neurosurg Clin N Am. 2011;22(1):79–85. doi: 10.1016/j.nec.2010.08.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Li J, Bentzen SM, Li J, Renschler M, Mehta MP. Relationship between neurocognitive function and quality of life after whole-brain radiotherapy in patients with brain metastasis. Int J Radiat Oncol Biol Phys. 2008;71(1):64–70. doi: 10.1016/j.ijrobp.2007.09.059. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Wefel JS, Parsons MW, Gondi V, Brown PD. Neurocognitive aspects of brain metastasis. Handb Clin Neurol. 2018;149:155–165. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-811161-1.00012-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Schimmel WCM, Gehring K, Eekers DBP, Hanssens PEJ, Sitskoorn MM. Cognitive effects of stereotactic radiosurgery in adult patients with brain metastases: a systematic review. Adv Radiat Oncol. 2018;3(4):568–581. doi: 10.1016/j.adro.2018.06.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Chang EL, Wefel JS, Maor MH, Hassenbusch SJ, Mahajan A, Lang FF, et al. A pilot study of neurocognitive function in patients with one to three new brain metastases initially treated with stereotactic radiosurgery alone. Neurosurgery. 2007;60(2):277–283. doi: 10.1227/01.NEU.0000249272.64439.B1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Chang EL, Wefel JS, Hess KR, Allen PK, Lang FF, Kornguth DG, et al. Neurocognition in patients with brain metastases treated with radiosurgery or radiosurgery plus whole-brain irradiation: a randomised controlled trial. Lancet Oncol. 2009;10(11):1037–1044. doi: 10.1016/S1470-2045(09)70263-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Onodera S, Aoyama H, Tha KK, Hashimoto N, Toyomaki A, Terae S, et al. The value of 4-month neurocognitive function as an endpoint in brain metastases trials. J Neurooncol. 2014;120(2):311–319. doi: 10.1007/s11060-014-1550-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Brown PD, Jaeckle K, Ballman KV, Farace E, Cerhan JH, Anderson SK, et al. Effect of radiosurgery alone vs radiosurgery with whole brain radiation therapy on cognitive function in patients with 1 to 3 brain metastases. JAMA. 2016;316(4):401–409. doi: 10.1001/jama.2016.9839. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Habets EJJ, Dirven L, Wiggenraad RG, Verbeek-de Kanter A, Lycklama A, Nijeholt GJ, Zwinkels H, et al. Neurocognitive functioning and health-related quality of life in patients treated with stereotactic radiotherapy for brain metastases: a prospective study. Neuro-oncology. 2016;18(3):435–444. doi: 10.1093/neuonc/nov186. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Mehta MP, Shapiro WR, Glantz MJ, Patchell RA, Weitzner MA, Meyers CA, et al. Lead-in phase to randomized trial of motexafin gadolinium and whole-brain radiation for patients with brain metastases: centralized assessment of magnetic resonance imaging, neurocognitive, and neurologic end points. J Clin Oncol. 2002;20(16):3445–3453. doi: 10.1200/JCO.2002.07.500. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Brown PD, Ballman KV, Cerhan JH, Anderson SK, Carrero XW, Whitton AC, et al. Postoperative stereotactic radiosurgery compared with whole brain radiotherapy for resected metastatic brain disease (NCCTG N107C/CEC·3): a multicentre, randomised, controlled, phase 3 trial. Lancet Oncol. 2017;18(8):1049–1060. doi: 10.1016/S1470-2045(17)30441-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Berger A, Strauss I, Moshe SB, Corn BW, Limon D, Shtraus N, et al. Neurocognitive evaluation of brain metastases patients treated with post-resection stereotactic radiosurgery: a prospective single arm clinical trial. J Neurooncol. 2018;140(2):307–315. doi: 10.1007/s11060-018-2954-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Welzel G, Fleckenstein K, Schaefer J, Hermann B, Kraus-Tiefenbacher U, Mai SK, et al. Memory function before and after whole brain radiotherapy in patients with and without brain metastases. Int J Radiat Oncol Biol Phys. 2008;72(5):1311–1318. doi: 10.1016/j.ijrobp.2008.03.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Meyers CA, Smith JA, Bezjak A, Mehta MP, Liebmann J, Illidge T, et al. Neurocognitive function and progression in patients with brain metastases treated with whole-brain radiation and motexafin gadolinium: results of a randomized phase III trial. J Clin Oncol. 2004;22(1):157–165. doi: 10.1200/JCO.2004.05.128. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Hunter GK, Suh JH, Reuther AM, Vogelbaum MA, Barnett GH, Angelov L, et al. Treatment of five or more brain metastases with stereotactic radiosurgery. Int J Radiat Oncol Biol Phys. 2012;83(5):1394–1398. doi: 10.1016/j.ijrobp.2011.10.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Limon D, McSherry F, Herndon J, Sampson J, Fecci P, Adamson J, et al. Single fraction stereotactic radiosurgery for multiple brain metastases. Adv Radiat Oncol. 2017;2(4):555–563. doi: 10.1016/j.adro.2017.09.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Lam T-C, Sahgal A, Chang EL, Lo SS. Stereotactic radiosurgery for multiple brain metastases. Expert Rev Anticancer Ther. 2014;14(10):1153–1172. doi: 10.1586/14737140.2014.940325. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Baschnagel AM, Meyer KD, Chen PY, Krauss DJ, Olson RE, Pieper DR, et al. Tumor volume as a predictor of survival and local control in patients with brain metastases treated with Gamma Knife surgery. J Neurosurg. 2013;119(5):1139–1144. doi: 10.3171/2013.7.JNS13431. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Emery A, Trifiletti DM, Romano KD, Patel N, Smolkin ME, Sheehan JP. More than just the number of brain metastases: evaluating the impact of brain metastasis location and relative volume on overall survival after stereotactic radiosurgery. World Neurosurg. 2017;99:111–117. doi: 10.1016/j.wneu.2016.11.119. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Kotecha R, Miller JA, Chao ST, Mohammadi AM, Murphy ES, Suh JH, et al. What drives patient outcomes in brain metastases: number, volume, or biology? J Clin Oncol Am Soc Clin Oncol. 2017;35(15_suppl):2071. [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Sharma M, Jia X, Ahluwalia M, Barnett GH, Vogelbaum MA, Chao ST, et al. Cumulative intracranial tumor volume and number of brain metastasis as predictors of developing new lesions after stereotactic radiosurgery for brain metastasis. World Neurosurg. 2017;106:666–675. doi: 10.1016/j.wneu.2017.07.048. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Sperduto PW, Kased N, Roberge D, Xu Z, Shanley R, Luo X, et al. Summary report on the graded prognostic assessment: an accurate and facile diagnosis-specific tool to estimate survival for patients with brain metastases. J Clin Oncol. 2012;30(4):419–425. doi: 10.1200/JCO.2011.38.0527. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Pulenzas N, Khan L, Tsao M, Zhang L, Lechner B, Thavarajah N, et al. Fatigue scores in patients with brain metastases receiving whole brain radiotherapy. Support Care Cancer. 2014;22(7):1757–1763. doi: 10.1007/s00520-014-2140-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Pendergrass JC, Targum SD, Harrison JE. Cognitive impairment associated with cancer: a brief review. Innov Clin Neurosci. 2018;15(1-2):36–44. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Thong MSY, Mols F, van de Poll-Franse LV, Sprangers MAG, van der Rijt CCD, Barsevick AM, et al. Identifying the subtypes of cancer-related fatigue: results from the population-based PROFILES registry. J Cancer Surviv. 2018;12(1):38–46. doi: 10.1007/s11764-017-0641-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Verhaak E, Gehring K, Hanssens PEG, Sitskoorn MM. Health-related quality of life of patients with brain metastases selected for stereotactic radiosurgery. J Neurooncol. 2019;4(5):289. doi: 10.1007/s11060-019-03186-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Wefel JS, Vardy J, Ahles T, Schagen SB. International Cognition and Cancer Task Force recommendations to harmonise studies of cognitive function in patients with cancer. Lancet Oncol. 2011;12(7):703–708. doi: 10.1016/S1470-2045(10)70294-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Zigmond AS, Snaith RP. The hospital anxiety and depression scale. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 1983;67(6):361–370. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1983.tb09716.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Smets EM, Garssen B, Cull A, De Haes JC. Application of the multidimensional fatigue inventory (MFI-20) in cancer patients receiving radiotherapy. Br J Cancer. 1996;73(2):241–245. doi: 10.1038/bjc.1996.42. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Weitzner MA, Meyers CA, Gelke CK, Byrne KS, Cella DF, Levin VA. The Functional Assessment of Cancer Therapy (FACT) scale. Development of a brain subscale and revalidation of the general version (FACT-G) in patients with primary brain tumors. Cancer. 1996;75(5):1151–1161. doi: 10.1002/1097-0142(19950301)75:5<1151::aid-cncr2820750515>3.0.co;2-q. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Oosterhuis HEM, van der Ark LA, Sijtsma K. Sample size requirements for traditional and regression-based norms. Assessment. 2015;23(2):191–202. doi: 10.1177/1073191115580638. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Glass GV, McGaw B, Smith ML. Meta-analysis in social research. Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications; 1981. p. 279. [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Cohen J (1988) Statistical power analysis for the behavioural sciences. Hillsdale.
  • 35.Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y, Sijtsma K. Controlling the False Discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J R Stat Soc Ser B. 2002;57(1):289–300. [Google Scholar]
  • 36.van der Meer PB, Habets EJJ, Wiggenraad RG, Verbeek-de Kanter A, Lycklama A, Nijeholt GJ, Zwinkels H, et al. Individual changes in neurocognitive functioning and health-related quality of life in patients with brain oligometastases treated with stereotactic radiotherapy. J Neurooncol. 2018;139(2):359–368. doi: 10.1007/s11060-018-2868-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Gerstenecker A, Nabors LB, Meneses K, Fiveash JB, Marson DC, Cutter G, et al. Cognition in patients with newly diagnosed brain metastasis: profiles and implications. J Neurooncol. 2014;120(1):179–185. doi: 10.1007/s11060-014-1543-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Low E, Crewther SG, Ong B, Perre D, Wijeratne T. Compromised motor dexterity confounds processing speed task outcomes in stroke patients. Front Neurol. 2017;8:484. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2017.00484. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Kesler SR, Blayney DW, Ong B, Perre D, Wijeratne T. Neurotoxic effects of anthracycline- vs nonanthracycline-based chemotherapy on cognition in breast cancer survivors. JAMA Oncol. 2017;2(2):185–192. doi: 10.1001/jamaoncol.2015.4333. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Schagen SB, Muller MJ, Boogerd W, Mellenbergh GJ, van Dam FSAM. Change in cognitive function after chemotherapy: a prospective longitudinal study in breast cancer patients. J Natl Cancer Inst. 2006;98(23):1742–1745. doi: 10.1093/jnci/djj470. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Wefel JS, Kesler SR, Noll KR, Schagen SB. Clinical characteristics, pathophysiology, and management of noncentral nervous system cancer-related cognitive impairment in adults. CA Cancer J Clin. 2015;65(2):123–138. doi: 10.3322/caac.21258. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Stelzer KJ. Epidemiology and prognosis of brain metastases. Surg Neurol Int. 2013;4(Suppl 4):S192–202. doi: 10.4103/2152-7806.111296. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Dwan TM, Ownsworth T, Chambers S, Walker DJ, Shum DHK. Neuropsychological assessment of individuals with brain tumor: comparison of approaches used in the classification of impairment. Front Oncol. 2015;5(6):56. doi: 10.3389/fonc.2015.00056. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Li J, Bentzen SM, Renschler M, Mehta MP. Regression after whole-brain radiation therapy for brain metastases correlates with survival and improved neurocognitive function. J Clin Oncol. 2007;25(10):1260–1266. doi: 10.1200/JCO.2006.09.2536. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Noll KR, Bradshaw ME, Weinberg JS, Wefel JS. Neurocognitive functioning is associated with functional independence in newly diagnosed patients with temporal lobe glioma. Neurooncol Pract. 2018;5(3):184–193. doi: 10.1093/nop/npx028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Zeng KL, Raman S, Sahgal A, Soliman H, Tsao M, Wendzicki C, et al. Patient preference for stereotactic radiosurgery plus or minus whole brain radiotherapy for the treatment of brain metastases. Ann Palliat Med. 2017;6(Suppl 2):S155–S160. doi: 10.21037/apm.2017.06.11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Triebel KL, Gerstenecker A, Meneses K, Fiveash JB, Meyers CA, Cutter G, et al. Capacity of patients with brain metastases to make treatment decisions. Psychooncology. 2015;24(11):1448–1455. doi: 10.1002/pon.3753. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Gerstenecker A, Duff K, Meneses K, Fiveash JB, Nabors LB, Triebel KL. Cognitive predictors of reasoning through treatment decisions in patients with newly diagnosed brain metastases. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2015;21(6):412–418. doi: 10.1017/S1355617715000478. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Gerstenecker A, Meneses K, Duff K, Fiveash JB, Marson DC, Triebel KL. Cognitive predictors of understanding treatment decisions in patients with newly diagnosed brain metastasis. Cancer. 2015;121(12):2013–2019. doi: 10.1002/cncr.29326. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Gehring K, Sitskoorn MM, Gundy CM, Sikkes SAM, Klein M, Postma TJ, et al. Cognitive rehabilitation in patients with gliomas: a randomized, controlled trial. J Clin Oncol. 2009;27(22):3712–3722. doi: 10.1200/JCO.2008.20.5765. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials


Articles from Journal of Neuro-Oncology are provided here courtesy of Springer

RESOURCES