Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2019 Nov 15.
Published in final edited form as: Am J Psychiatry. 2008 Aug 15;165(11):1465–1472. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.07111806

Self and Other in Schizophrenia: A Cognitive Neuroscience Perspective

Melissa Fisher 1, Kelly McCoy 1, John H Poole 2, Sophia Vinogradov 1
PMCID: PMC6857930  NIHMSID: NIHMS1057989  PMID: 18708487

Abstract

Objective:

Recent basic science data indicate that, in healthy individuals, self-referential processing and social cognition rely on common neural substrates. We assessed self-referential source memory and social cognition in a large sample of schizophrenia outpatients and healthy comparison subjects in order to test and contrast how these critical processes are associated in the two groups.

Methods:

Ninety-one schizophrenia outpatients and 30 healthy comparison subjects were assessed on measures of basic social cognition and source memory for previously learned word items: self-generated, externally-presented, and new words. Partial correlations and multiple regression analysis tested the association of social cognition measures to source memory performance, and the contributions of source memory and general cognitive abilities to a social cognition composite score.

Results:

Schizophrenia subjects demonstrated significantly lower source memory for self-generated items (self-referential source memory), but intact external source memory. In both groups, self-referential source memory and social cognition showed strong correlations. When the effects of general cognitive abilities were controlled, these correlations were attenuated in the schizophrenia subjects. Regression analysis revealed discrepancies between groups in the cognitive functions contributing to social cognition performance.

Conclusions:

Impaired self-referential source memory represents a unique cognitive deficit in schizophrenia. Further, the strong association between self-referential source memory and social cognition seen in healthy subjects is reduced in schizophrenia and is moderated by general cognitive abilities. Impairments in the neurocognitive system that underlies both self-referential and social cognition provide a parsimonious explanation for the disturbances in the sense of “self” and “other” that characterize schizophrenia.

Keywords: facial recognition, emotion identification, self-reference effect, medial prefrontal cortex

Introduction

The difficulty the patient experiences in dealing with other human beings … is the external counterpart of what goes on inside himself… [A] disturbed inner life causes alterations in relating to others.

—Silvano Arieti (1)

Deficits in social cognitive abilities and interpersonal functioning are among the most dramatic features of schizophrenia, have been described and interpreted for over 150 years, and are a key determinant of functional outcome. And yet, the component processes that contribute to social cognitive impairments in schizophrenia are not fully understood (2). The well-known deficits in general neurocognition that characterize this disorder show only moderate bivariate correlations with performance on tasks involving emotion identification, social perception, and mentalizing about the states of others (35). Further, as noted by the Measurement and Treatment Research to Improve Cognition in Schizophrenia Committee (2), aspects of social cognition have a distinct neural substrate from non-social cognitive domains such as attention, memory, and executive functions (6).

Does Self-Referential Processing Play a Role in Social Cognition?

Given that the processing of social information involves distinct neural systems, what specific operations might contribute to the impaired social cognitive performance seen in schizophrenia? Interestingly, recent functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) evidence from healthy subjects suggests that a common neural substrate, the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC), is recruited during both social cognition tasks and self-referential processing (7). Specifically, mPFC shows relative activation during a wide range of social cognitive processes, such as viewing faces, social scenes, and social interactions (810) as well as mentalizing about the thoughts, intentions, or feelings of others (11). Subregions of mPFC also show activation in response to self-referential cognitive processes: during self-referential memory (12, 13) and during reflection on one’s own personality characteristics and emotional states (1416).

These imaging findings suggest that there is a fundamental relationship between social and self-referential cognition, and raise the intriguing possibility that alterations in this association might play a role in schizophrenia. Yet, with few exceptions, the evidence that self-referential and social cognitions share common processes is based on comparisons across imaging studies of healthy subjects, rather than investigations of both sets of operations within the same sample of subjects (7). Furthermore, these questions have not been systematically broached in schizophrenia patients, even though cognitive theory and psychoanalytic thinkers have for decades posited that cognitive biases in the internal representation of self and other are central to models of severe psychopathology (17, 18).

Can We Use a Self-Referential Source Memory Task to Investigate Social Cognition in Schizophrenia?

Guided by the neuroimaging findings in healthy individuals, and having recently demonstrated in an fMRI experiment that subjects with schizophrenia do not show the normal activation of mPFC seen in healthy subjects during a self-referential memory task (19), we were prompted to ask: is it possible that the deficits in social cognition that are observed in schizophrenia are related to impairments in self-referential processing?

We thus sought to investigate, in a large sample of stable, well-characterized outpatients, the behavioral relationship between performance on a set of basic social cognition measures—facial memory, facial emotion identification, and vocal prosody recognition-- and a specific emotionally-neutral self-referential cognitive process under study in our laboratory: source memory for self-generated information (20). We defined social cognition by performance on basic tasks of emotional recognition and social perception that are known to be associated with functional outcome in schizophrenia (and that contribute to, and can be contrasted with, more complex constructs, such as mentalizing about others) (2, 11).

In order to proceed with this line of inquiry, however, we were first obliged to demonstrate that self-referential source memory in schizophrenia is a unique cognitive process, suitable for use as a probe into social cognition, and not merely a reflection of an overall problem in source memory or other more general cognitive abilities. We previously reported the independence of source memory for self-generated information from executive function, but did not test its association to attention or verbal memory (21); with some exceptions (22, 23), source memory deficits in schizophrenia have not been examined in relation to general neurocognition.

A number of prior studies have shown that symptomatic schizophrenia subjects manifest poor memory for the source of all item types, not just self-generated information, and that patients with severe psychosis also demonstrate an external response bias or tendency to attribute information to an external source (2229). However, more recent studies have identified source monitoring deficits among patients without acute positive symptoms, who do not show the external response bias observed in more symptomatic patients (30, 31); furthermore, source memory performance of siblings has been shown to be intermediate between that of patients and healthy controls (30). Taken together, the recent evidence suggests that source monitoring deficits represent a trait marker for schizophrenia, above and beyond the external response bias described as state markers in earlier studies.

These two considerations – the specific nature of self-referential source memory performance in clinically stable schizophrenia patients; and its potential utility as a probe into the component processes underlying social cognitive abilities – formed the rationale for the present study, with several direct theoretical and clinical implications. Chronic psychosis has long been conceptualized as a disorder of the self and the self’s relationship to the other – aspects of the illness that are most troubling to patients and to society at large, and thus clearly deserving of further study. Additionally, social cognitive performance is now recognized as a crucial factor mediating functional outcome for patients, and must increasingly become a focus of treatment, requiring a thorough understanding of its significant component processes. We thus conducted the present study in order to address the following questions:

  1. Do individuals with schizophrenia manifest specific deficits in remembering the source of self-generated information in the presence of intact source memory for external information and intact recognition of new items?

  2. As would be predicted by the functional imaging literature, do we find that self-referential source memory performance is uniquely related to social cognition in healthy comparison subjects and in schizophrenia subjects?

  3. What role do general cognitive abilities play in this relationship?

Methods

Subjects

Ninety-one schizophrenia outpatients were recruited from psychiatric outpatient clinics in the San Francisco Bay area. Healthy comparison subjects (N=30) were recruited from the community and were matched to the schizophrenia group on age, gender ratio, ethnicity, premorbid IQ, and parental education (see Table 1). (Data on the healthy subjects has been presented in Fisher et al., under review.) The groups differed significantly in terms of education, t(116)= 3.45, and current IQ, t(116) = 4.62. A diagnosis of schizophrenia was confirmed with the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (32), the Positive and Negative Symptom Scale-Extended (3, 33), and a medical chart review, while healthy participants underwent screening to rule out lifetime history of mental illness and family history of schizophrenia. Additional exclusion criteria for all subjects included history of head trauma, substance abuse during the past 6 months, neurological disorders, and English as a second language. All schizophrenia subjects were clinically stable at the time of testing. Symptom severity varied broadly, with average PANSS-E ratings in the mild range (Table 1). Subjects provided written informed consent after the experimental procedures were fully explained, and received nominal payment for their participation in this study.

Table 1.

Demographic and Clinical Characteristics of Patients with Schizophrenia and Healthy Comparison Subjects in a Study of Self-Referential Processing and Social Cognition

DEMOGRAPHIC, COGNITIVE, AND CLINICAL MEASURES HEALTHY COMPARISON SUBJECTS N=30 MEAN ± SD SCHIZOPHRENIA SUBJECTS N=91 MEAN ± SD P VALUE
Age1 39.7 ± 13.9 39.9 ± 11.4 .939
Gender (% Male)2 67% 75% .392
Ethnicity (%Euro-Amer.)2 67% 69% .143
Premorbid IQ Estimate3 Mdn=108 (IQR=15) Mdn=104 (IQR=18) .093
Current IQ Estimate1 108 ± 12 96 ± 12 <.001
Education (Yrs)1 15 ± 2 13 ± 2 <.001
Parental Education (Yrs)1 13 ± 3 13 ± 3 .470
PANSS-E
 Positive -- 3 ± 1
 Negative -- 3 ± 1
 Disorganized -- 3 ± 1
1

T-test (2-tailed).

2

Chi-Square Test (2-tailed).

3

Mann-Whitney U-Test (2-tailed).

Measures

Information Source Memory Task

The Information Source Memory Task, (21), was used to assess source memory for three word item types: self-generated, experimenter-presented, and new.

Study phase.

The subject is presented with a list of 40 sentences each containing a noun and a verb followed either by a target word (underlined) or by a fill-in-the-blank space. The sentences alternate (“The boy threw the ball”; “The captain sailed the ___”; “The queen ruled the land”; “The cat chased the ___.”) The subject is asked to read each sentence aloud and to “make up a word and say it aloud” for each blank. The experimenter prints the subject’s self-generated target words on a “test list.” The subject thus reads aloud 20 experimenter-generated target words and must self-generate and speak aloud 20 target words. The order of the sentence stimuli is varied among subjects.

Testing phase.

The experimenter presents the test list to the subject approximately 2 hours later. This list contains 40 word pairs from the original sentence stimuli, with target words that were generated by either the experimenter or the research subject (e.g., boy - ball, captain - ship, queen - land, cat - mouse). The test list also contains 20 new word pairs containing words that are highly associated (e.g., bird - nest). The 60 word pairs on the test list are presented in pseudorandom order. The research subject is told that the test list contains word pairs from the sentences that he or she read aloud in part 1 as well as some new word pairs. The subject is asked to determine, for each word pair, whether the target word is “brand new,” whether it is a word that he or she “made up and said aloud” when reading the original sentence, or whether it is a word that was underlined and that he or she simply “read aloud.”

Social Cognition Measures

Immediate and delayed recognition of emotionally neutral faces was assessed using the TOMAL Facial Memory Test (34). The Facial Affect Recognition test assessed basic emotion identification, while Neutral Prosody and Emotional Prosody were measured using subtests from the Florida Affect Battery. See (3) for a full description of these measures.

General Cognitive Factors: Attention, Verbal Memory, Executive Functioning, and IQ

Age-adjusted z-scores from the following measures were used: 1) WAIS-R Freedom from Distractibility Index, 2) California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT), Immediate Recall Total (Trials 1–5), 3) Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST), Modified Nelson Version, composite score of the total number of categories and total number of errors, 4) WAIS-R Full Scale IQ.

Data Analyses

The dispersion of all variables was examined for “restriction of range”. All variables were normally distributed with the exception of False Positive errors on the Source Memory task (right-skewed) and Neutral Prosody (left-skewed). Data for these measures were analyzed with both parametric and non-parametric procedures with identical findings. Parametric statistical procedures were used to analyze all other measures. Missing values (for one to two measures) among four schizophrenia subjects and three healthy comparison subjects were replaced by sample means. Only slight, nonsignificant changes occurred when these variables were reanalyzed without mean substitution.

A Social Cognition Composite score was calculated for each subject as the average z-score across the five social cognition measures (Facial Memory Test Immediate Recognition; Facial Memory Test Delayed Recognition; Facial Affect Recognition; Neutral Prosody; Emotional Prosody). The Social Cognition Composite was normally distributed, with acceptable internal-consistency reliability (Cronbach’s α = .72). Multivariate Analysis of Covariance (MANCOVA) was used to test between-group differences on the three source memory measures (hit rates for self, experimenter, and new), and to test group differences on the five measures of social cognition. Univariate Analysis of Covariance (ANCOVA) assessed group differences on the Social Cognition Composite score. Age was entered as a covariate in all analyses.

Zero-order correlations assessed the association of social cognition (the composite score, and the five individual social cognition measures) to source memory (self-generated, experimenter-presented, and new hit rates). These were followed by partial correlations, controlling for the effects of attention, verbal memory, executive functioning and IQ. Fisher’s r-to-z transformations allowed for testing of between-group differences of the magnitude of these associations. All tests of significance were two-tailed. A multiple regression analysis was conducted to determine the unique contributions of self-referential source memory, attention (WAIS-R Attention Index), verbal memory (CVLT Immediate Recall Total, Trials 1–5), executive functioning (WCST composite score), and IQ to the Social Cognition Composite score.

Results

Between-Group Analyses: Source Memory and Social Cognition Measures

The source memory MANCOVA revealed a significant omnibus F-test, F(3, 116) = 3.58, p = .016, with significantly lower performance on self-referential source memory among schizophrenia subjects compared to healthy subjects, while controlling for the effects of age, F(1, 118) = 9.89 (see Table 2 for significance levels). The magnitude of this effect was moderate to large (Cohen’s d = .79). In contrast, the groups did not differ significantly on source memory for externally-presented items, recognition memory (correct detection of new items), or on false positive error measures.

Table 2.

Performance on Source Memory, Social Cognition, and General Cognition Measures among Healthy Comparison and Schizophrenia Subjects.

HEALTHY COMPARISON SUBJECTS SCHIZOPHRENIA SUBJECTS
Source Memory Task RATES (MEAN ± SD) p
Correct Hits1
 Self-generated .86 ± .14 .68 ± .29 .002
 Experimenter-presented .48 ± .16 .42 ± .21 .184
 New .74 ± .15 .72 ± .23 .725
False Positive Errors2
 Self-generated .08 ± .06 .11 ± .11 .273
 Experimenter-presented .13 ± .09 .17 ± .17 .292
 New .24 ± .08 .30 ± .18 .110
Social Cognition RATES (MEAN ± SD)
Facial Memory Test1
 Immediate Recognition .78 ± .10 .65 ± .11 <.001
 Delayed Recognition .81 ± .08 .72 ± .14 <.001
Facial Affect Recognition .71 ± .09 .70 ± .10 .682
Neutral Prosody2 .99 ± .02 .95 ± .09 .035
Emotional Prosody1 .92 ± .07 .82 ± .15 <.001
Social Cognition Index1, 4 .23 ± .42 −.48 ± .83 <.001
General Cognition3 INDEX/Z-SCORE (MEAN ± SD)
WAIS-R Attention Index (Freedom from Distractibility) 107.5 ± 10.0 93.3 ± 8.7 <.001
Verbal Memory (CVLT Immediate Recall, Trials 1–5) .55 ± .82 −1.10 ± 1.30 <.001
Executive Functioning Index5 .45 ± .41 −.54 ± 1.09 <.001
WAIS-R Full-Scale IQ 107.61 ± 11.53 96.38 ± 11.56 <.001
1

ANCOVA (2-tailed).

2

Mann Whitney U-Test (2-tailed).

3

T-test (2-tailed).

4

Composite Index = average z-score across social cognition measures.

5

Composite Index = average z-score across WCST number of categories and number of errors.

The social cognition MANCOVA omnibus F-test was also significant, F(5, 114) = 10.41, p < .001. Schizophrenia subjects performed significantly lower on the following measures of social cognition (see Table 2): Facial Memory Immediate Recognition, F(1, 118) = 42.52, Facial Memory Delayed Recognition, F(1, 118) = 12.40, Emotional Prosody, F(1, 118) = 14.71, and Neutral Prosody F(1, 118) = 4.53. The groups did not differ significantly on Facial Affect Recognition. Schizophrenia subjects also performed significantly lower on the Social Cognition Composite score, F(1, 118) = 21.32, p < .001.

Within-Group Contrasts: The Self-Generation Effect in Memory

The self-generation effect, (enhanced memory for self-generated information, such as words, pictures, objects, etc., (35)) was evident among healthy comparison subjects: source memory for self-generated items was significantly greater than source memory for externally-presented items, F(1, 29) = 93.70, p < .001, and recognition memory (correct detection of new items) F(1, 29) = 12.06, p = .002. In the schizophrenia sample, source memory for self-generated items was also significantly greater than source memory for externally-presented items, F(1, 29) = 62.61, p < .001, but did not differ significantly from recognition memory (detection of new items).

Associations of Social Cognition to Self-Referential Source Memory versus Source Memory for Externally-Presented Items

All zero-order correlations of social cognition measures to self-referential source memory were significant among both healthy comparison subjects and schizophrenia subjects (see Table 3). In contrast, all correlations of social cognition measures to source memory for externally-presented items were not significant in healthy comparison subjects, while in the schizophrenia subject sample, two of the five social cognition measures showed small but significant correlations to source memory for externally-presented items (see Table 3). Correlations of all social cognition measures and recognition memory (correct detection of new items) were not significant in either subject sample. Figure 1 illustrates the associations of the Social Cognition Composite score to source memory measures in both subject groups. We note that none of the variable distributions showed evidence of restriction of range in either subject group, despite the greater variance observed on performance measures among the schizophrenia subject group.

Table 3.

Zero-Order Correlations and Partial Correlations of Social Cognition to Source Memory Measures, Controlling for Attention, Verbal Memory, Executive Functioning, and IQ.

Zero-Order Correlations Social Cognition Composite Affect Recognition Faces Immediate Recognition Faces Delayed Recognition Emotional Prosody 1Neutral Prosody
Healthy Control Group
 Self .66 ** .50 ** .52 ** .51** .40 * --
 Experimenter .19 .07 .32 .33 −.13 --
 New .16 .04 .28 .12 .18 --
Schizophrenia Group
 Self .52 ** .23 * .25 * .37 ** .38 ** .45 **
 Experimenter .22 * .34 ** .11 .23 * .13 .06
 New .06 −.17 .12 .16 −.002 .11
Partial Correlations 2Social Cognition Composite 2Affect Recognition 2Faces Immediate Recognition Faces Delayed Recognition Emotional Prosody 1Neutral Prosody
Healthy Control Group
 Self .67 ** .48 * .51 ** .46 * .41 * --
 Experimenter .03 −.04 .16 .30 -.26 --
Schizophrenia Group
 Self .31 ** .04 .09 .18 .18 .30 **
 Experimenter .14 .31 ** .03 .15 .05 −.02
**

p < .01,

*

p < .05: Partial Correlation significance level (2-tailed).

1

Unable to calculate among healthy comparison subjects due to restriction of range (ceiling effects).

2

p < .05: Fisher’s r-to-z test significance level (contrast of self-referential source memory coefficients between groups).

Figure 1. Correlation of Social Cognition with Source Memory for Self-Generated, Experimenter-Presented, and New Items among two Subject Groups.

Figure 1.

* Self-Generated coefficient p < .001, both groups. Experimenter-presented coefficient n.s. for healthy control group, and p = .036 for schizophrenia subject group. New items coefficient n.s., both groups.

The partial correlations (Table 3) revealed that the Social Cognition Composite score and individual measures of social cognition were significantly correlated to self-referential source memory in the healthy comparison group, independent of the effects of attention, verbal memory, executive functioning, and IQ. In contrast, in the schizophrenia group, associations between social cognition measures and self-referential source memory were significantly attenuated when the effects of attention, verbal memory, executive functioning, and IQ were removed.

Fisher’s r-to-z transformations revealed that the association between measures of social cognition and self-referential source memory were significantly greater among healthy subjects compared to schizophrenia subjects for the following variables: Social Cognition Composite, Affect Recognition, and TOMAL Facial Memory Immediate Recognition. Partial correlations of social cognition measures to source memory for externally-presented items were non-significant in both subject groups, with the exception of Facial Affect Recognition in the schizophrenia subject group.

The Relationship of Self-Referential Source Memory, Social Cognition, and General Cognitive Abilities

In the healthy comparison group, the Social Cognition Composite score was not significantly correlated to measures of general cognition (attention, verbal memory, executive functioning, IQ). In contrast, in the schizophrenia group, the Social Cognition Composite score showed significant correlations to attention (r = .57), verbal memory (r = .41), executive functioning (r = .46), and IQ (r = .51) (p < .001, two-tailed). The multiple regression analysis (Table 4) revealed distinct models accounting for approximately the same amount of variance in social cognition among healthy comparison subjects (56%) and schizophrenia subjects (50%). In healthy comparison subjects, self-referential source memory was the sole, significant predictor of social cognition in the model, with a trend level contribution of verbal memory. In contrast, in the schizophrenia subject group, self-referential source memory, attention, and executive functioning contributed approximately equally to the variance in social cognition, while the effects of verbal memory and IQ were non-significant.

Table 4.

Self-Referential Source Memory, Attention, Verbal Memory, Executive Functioning, and IQ Regressed on Social Cognition among Healthy Comparison and Schizophrenia Subjects.

HEALTHY COMPARISON SUBJECTS
Dependent Variable Model Summary
Social Cognition R R2 Std. Error F(5, 24) p
.752 .565 .306 6.24 .001
Predictors Std. β t df p Partial r2
Self Source Memory .62 4.42 24 < .001 .45
Attention .05 0.25 24 .803 <.01
Verbal Memory .27 1.97 24 .060 .14
Executive Functioning .19 1.28 24 .213 .06
IQ −.02 −.11 24 .913 <.01
SCHIZOPHRENIA SUBJECTS
Dependent Variable Model Summary
Social Cognition R R2 Std. Error F(5, 85) p
.707 .500 .601 17.01 <.001
Predictors Std. β t df p Partial r2
Self Source Memory .274 2.95 85 .004 .09
Attention .286 2.90 85 .005 .09
Verbal Memory .077 0.92 85 .362 <.01
Executive Functioning .242 2.78 85 .007 .08
IQ .097 0.94 85 .350 .01

Discussion

Self-referential Source Memory in Schizophrenia Represents a Unique Cognitive Deficit

Using a large sample of stable schizophrenia outpatients, we found that participants showed deficits in self-referential source memory in the presence of intact external source memory and intact recognition memory, with no external response bias. In other words, compared to healthy subjects, individuals with schizophrenia had a specific difficulty in recognizing that they themselves were the source of words generated during an earlier sentence completion task. Overall, our results are consistent with the notion that processing information with respect to the “self” makes use of cognitive operations that are in part distinct from those underlying general memory abilities for external information, and that individuals with schizophrenia have difficulty recruiting these specific operations. The effect size of this self-referential source memory impairment in the schizophrenia sample was medium to large (Cohen’s d = .79).

Earlier studies have tended to show poor memory for the source of all item types among symptomatic inpatient schizophrenia subgroups relative to healthy comparison subjects, as well as an external response bias (2229). More recent data from clinically remitted samples indicate that impaired source monitoring is a trait marker of the illness, rather than a state-dependent feature (30), and that internal source monitoring deficits are evident without response bias (31). Our finding of a dissociation between self-referential and external source memory, in the absence of response bias, may reflect our sample of clinically stable outpatients and/or the sensitivity of our measure (measures of source memory vary extensively across studies). Interestingly, one previous study detected a dissociation of self-referential versus external source memory in both mild and severely ill schizophrenia patients compared to healthy subjects with the aid of multinomial modeling techniques (23). The sum of the evidence thus suggests that impaired self-referential source memory represents a unique cognitive impairment in schizophrenia.

The Association Between Self-Referential Source Memory and Social Cognition Seen in Healthy Subjects is Attenuated in Schizophrenia

In both the schizophrenia and the healthy subjects, self-referential source memory showed unique associations with social cognition, compared either to memory for externally-presented information or to simple recognition memory. In other words, the ability to remember that “self was source” on a sentence completion task was strongly related to basic social cognitive processes of face recognition and emotion identification. In contrast, general memory for “external” target words from the sentences or recognition of new items were not related to these social cognition abilities in either subject group. Further, our data indicate that this strong relationship was not mediated by the emotional valence of the stimuli, as our emotionally-neutral self-referential source memory task showed significant associations to both emotionally-laden and emotionally-neutral measures of basic social cognition.

Simulation theory (36) and perception-action based theories posit that one’s internal representations of actions and feelings (“self”) are activated when perceiving such states in the “other” (37, 38). At the behavioral level, for example, subjects demonstrate unconscious mimicry of the facial expressions they observe in others (e.g.,(39)). At the neural level, facial and prosodic emotion perception activate brain regions also implicated during the active experience of emotion (e.g. (40)). As Brunet-Gouet and Decety (11) hypothesize, the inability to track the sense of “self” or agency may lead to a profound impairment in the processing of emotional and social information. Our behavioral data indicate that such a sense of self-agency is indeed strongly associated to social and emotional information processing in healthy subjects, and that deficits in this operation are associated with impaired processing of social and emotional information in schizophrenia subjects.

In the healthy comparison group, the associations between self-referential source memory and social cognition remained unchanged while controlling for the effects of attention, verbal memory, executive functioning, and IQ. This highlights the distinct nature of the relationship between self-referential and social cognition and its relative “dissociation” from other general cognitive abilities, consistent with the distinct neural correlates for these processes observed in neuroimaging studies of healthy subjects (6, 16). However, in stark contrast to the healthy comparison group, when the effects of attention, verbal memory, executive functioning, and IQ were controlled in the schizophrenia sample, the magnitude of the associations between self-referential source memory and social cognition measures was reduced, and significantly different from the healthy comparison subjects.

Taken together, these data indicate that, although social and self-referential cognition are uniquely associated in both subject groups, this relationship is attenuated in the schizophrenia subjects, and mediated by general cognitive abilities as further indicated by the multiple regression analysis. Among healthy subjects, approximately 45% of the variance in social cognition was accounted for by the independent contribution of self-referential source memory, while the contributions of the general cognitive factors were non-significant. In marked counterpoint, among schizophrenia subjects, the variance in social cognition was accounted for by the independent contributions of self-referential source memory (9%), attention (9%), and executive functions (8%).

The Disturbed Processing of “Self” and “Other” in Schizophrenia Has Neurocognitive Underpinnings

In sum, we find that individuals with schizophrenia are impaired in their ability to recognize that their “self” was the source of an earlier mental event. In healthy subjects, this specific ability to recognize “self as source” is strongly and uniquely related to their ability to identify facial and vocal emotion and to recognize faces, likely due to the fact that we normally process social stimuli (the “other”) in part by activating internal representations of our own sense of “self.” Subjects with schizophrenia, however, are impaired in this process—due, we suggest, to disturbances in the neurocognitive systems that normally facilitate the accurate processing of both self-referential and social information (11, 19, 41). They thus must rely on the contributions of other general cognitive operations, such as attention and executive functions, in order to accomplish social cognitive tasks.

Such a cognitive “solution” is likely to be less efficient, less accurate, and less reliable, leading to distortions in the representation of both self-referential and social (interpersonal) information. In the former case, this might contribute to the impaired sense of agency, decreased insight, and deficits in self-monitoring that are known to characterize schizophrenia (11, 42). In the latter instance, these could play a role in difficulties with empathy (38), mentalization (making attributional judgments about the mental states of others, (11)), and emotional and social perception (e.g., perception and interpretation of intimacy, status, mood state, veracity, etc. (2)). The ongoing clinical effects of such distortions are thus potentially quite profound, and suggest that the neurocognitive associations we present here provide a parsimonious explanation for the observed disturbances seen in the sense of “self” and “other” in schizophrenia. A greater understanding of these associations will lead to the development of more informed treatment approaches and ultimately, to improved quality of life for individuals with the illness.

Patient Perspectives.

Ms. M is a 41 year old woman diagnosed with schizophrenia at age 16. She is currently treated with lorazapam medication, and works part-time as an aide at a childcare center.

Ms. M describes difficulties making the distinction between “self” (her own mental contents, or self-generated information), and “other” (real events in the outside world, or external information):

“I can be at a party, and I might have the thought that this party is fun, and maybe I’m wearing some pretty earrings that look nice-- and if everyone is talking and laughing, then I think it’s because they are sharing my same thoughts and that’s why they are laughing. I don’t have to say it because they already know it – we are communicating mentally, like mind-reading, and they are having the same reactions I am.”

Ms. M goes on to describe how these experiences are related to difficulties she has with interpreting social interactions.

“Sometimes I have trouble knowing what a person’s face really means. I thought this one person was reading my mind because he made strong eye contact with me and I had to close my eyes and block my thoughts so he wouldn’t know what I was thinking. A lot of times I think someone has an angry face and is annoyed at me when they’re not.

“It used to be worse. I couldn’t figure out what people really meant, so I used to think that I was a secret agent and that there were secret messages that people were trying to tell me. If someone would smile and say, “Have a nice day,” I would think that they really were communicating something else, like a code I had to figure out.”

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by the Department of Veterans Affairs and by NIMH RO1 grant MH068725–01.

References

  • 1.Arieti S: Interpretation of Schizophrenia. New York, Basic Books, 1974 [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Green MF, Olivier B, Crawley JN, Penn DL, Silverstein S: Social cognition in schizophrenia: recommendations from the measurement and treatment research to improve cognition in schizophrenia new approaches conference. Schizophr Bull 2005; 31(4):882–7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Poole JH, Tobias FC, Vinogradov S: The functional relevance of affect recognition errors in schizophrenia. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2000; 6(6):649–58 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Bryson G, Bell M, Lysaker P: Affect recognition in schizophrenia: a function of global impairment or a specific cognitive deficit. Psychiatry Res 1997; 71(2):105–13 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Brune M: Emotion recognition, ‘theory of mind,’ and social behavior in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res 2005; 133(2–3):135–47 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Gilbert S, Spengler S, Simons JS, Frith CD, Burgess PW: Differential functions of lateral and medial rostral prefrontal cortex (area 10) revealed by brain-behavior associations. Cereb Cortex 2006; 16(12):1783–1789 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Ochsner KN, Knierim K, Ludlow DH, Hanelin J, Ramachandran T, Glover G, Mackey SC: Reflecting upon feelings: an fMRI study of neural systems supporting the attribution of emotion to self and other. J Cogn Neurosci 2004; 16(10):1746–72 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Mitchell JP, Macrae CN, Banaji MR: Encoding-specific effects of social cognition on the neural correlates of subsequent memory. J Neurosci 2004; 24(21):4912–7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Harvey PO, Fossati P, Lepage M: Modulation of memory formation by stimulus content: specific role of the medial prefrontal cortex in the successful encoding of social pictures. J Cogn Neurosci 2007; 19(2):351–62 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Iacoboni M, Lieberman MD, Knowlton BJ, Molnar-Szakacs I, Moritz M, Throop CJ, Fiske AP: Watching social interactions produces dorsomedial prefrontal and medial parietal BOLD fMRI signal increases compared to a resting baseline. Neuroimage 2004; 21(3):1167–73 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Brunet-Gouet E, Decety J: Social brain dysfunctions in schizophrenia: a review of neuroimaging studies. Psychiatry Res 2006; 148(2–3):75–92 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Vinogradov S, Luks TL, Simpson GV, Schulman BJ, Glenn S, Wong AE: Brain activation patterns during memory of cognitive agency. Neuroimage 2006; 31(2):896–905 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Simons JS, Davis SW, Gilbert SJ, Frith CD, Burgess PW: Discriminating imagined from perceived information engages brain areas implicated in schizophrenia. Neuroimage 2006; 32(2):696–703 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Ochsner KN, Beer JS, Robertson ER, Cooper JC, Gabrieli JD, Kihsltrom JF, D’Esposito M: The neural correlates of direct and reflected self-knowledge. Neuroimage 2005; 28(4):797–814 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Macrae CN, Moran JM, Heatherton TF, Banfield JF, Kelley WM: Medial prefrontal activity predicts memory for self. Cereb Cortex 2004; 14(6):647–54 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Gusnard DA, Akbudak E, Shulman GL, Raichle ME: Medial prefrontal cortex and self-referential mental activity: relation to a default mode of brain function. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2001; 98(7):4259–64 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Beck A, Rector AR: Cognitive Approaches to Schizophrenia: Theory and therapy. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology 2005; 1:577–606 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Ogden T: The Primitive Edge of Experience. Northvale, Jason Aronson, 1989 [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Vinogradov S, Luks TL, Schulman BJ, Simpson GV: Deficits in a neural correlate of reality montiroing in schizophrenia patients. Cereb Cortex; Advanced Access Published March 4, 2008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Johnson MK: Memory and reality. Am Psychol 2006; 61(8):760–71 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Vinogradov S, Willis-Shore J, Poole JH, Marten E, Ober BA, Shenaut GK: Clinical and neurocognitive aspects of source monitoring errors in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 1997; 154(11):1530–7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Seal MC SF, Cheung P: Deficits in source monitoring in participants with auditory hallucinations may be due to differences in verbal intelligence and verbal memory. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry 1997; 2(4):273–290 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Keefe RS, Arnold MC, Bayen UJ, McEvoy JP, Wilson WH: Source-monitoring deficits for self-generated stimuli in schizophrenia: multinomial modeling of data from three sources. Schizophr Res 2002; 57(1):51–67 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Bentall RP, Baker GA, Havers S: Reality monitoring and psychotic hallucinations. Br J Clin Psychol 1991; 30 (Pt 3):213–22 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.McGuire PK, Silbersweig DA, Wright I, Murray RM, Frackowiak RS, Frith CD: The neural correlates of inner speech and auditory verbal imagery in schizophrenia: relationship to auditory verbal hallucinations. Br J Psychiatry 1996; 169(2):148–59 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Morrison AP, Haddock G: Cognitive factors in source monitoring and auditory hallucinations. Psychol Med 1997; 27(3):669–79 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Allen PP, Johns LC, Fu CH, Broome MR, Vythelingum GN, McGuire PK: Misattribution of external speech in patients with hallucinations and delusions. Schizophr Res 2004; 69(2–3):277–87 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Johns LC, Gregg L, Allen P, McGuire PK: Impaired verbal self-monitoring in psychosis: effects of state, trait and diagnosis. Psychol Med 2006; 36(4):465–74 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Anselmetti S, Cavallaro R, Bechi M, Angelone SM, Ermoli E, Cocchi F, Smeraldi E: Psychopathological and neuropsychological correlates of source monitoring impairment in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res 2007; 150(1):51–9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Brunelin J, d’Amato T, Brun P, Bediou B, Kallel L, Senn M, Poulet E, Saoud M: Impaired verbal source monitoring in schizophrenia: an intermediate trait vulnerability marker? Schizophr Res 2007; 89(1–3):287–92 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Henquet C, Krabbendam L, Dautzenberg J, Jolles J, Merckelbach H: Confusing thoughts and speech: source monitoring and psychosis. Psychiatry Res 2005; 133(1):57–63 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.First M, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Williams JBW: Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders (SCID) New York, Biometrics Research Division, New York State Psychiatric Institute, 1995 [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Kay S, Fiszbein A, Opler LA: The Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 1987; 13(261–276) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Reynolds C, Bigler ED: Test of Memory and Learning Examiner’s Manual. Austin, Texas, 1994 [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Mulligan N, Lozito JP: Self-Generation and Memory, in The Psychology of Learning and Motivation: Advances in Research and Theory, vol 45 Edited by Ross B. Urbana, Elsevier Academic Press, 2004, pp 175–210 [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Davies M, Stone T: Mental Simulation: Evaluations and Applications. Oxford, Blackwell Publishers, 1995 [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Preston SD, de Waal FB: Empathy: Its ultimate and proximate bases. Behav Brain Sci 2002; 25(1):1–20; discussion 20–71 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Decety J, Jackson PL: A social neuroscience perspective on empathy. Current Directions in Psychological Science 2006; 15(2):54–58 [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Dimberg U, Thunberg M, Elmehed K: Unconscious facial reactions to emotional facial expressions. Psychol Sci 2000; 11(1):86–89 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Adolphs R, Damasio H, Tranel D, Cooper G, Damsio AR: A role for somatosensory cortices in the visual recognition of emotion as revealed by three-dimensional lesion mapping. J Neurosci 2000; 20(7):2683–90 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Garrity AG, Pearlson GD, McKiernan K, Lloyd D, Kiehl KA, Calhoun VD: Aberrant “default mode” functional connectivity in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 2007; 164(3):450–7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Shad MU, Tamminga CA, Cullum M, Haas GL, Keshavan MS: Insight and frontal cortical function in schizophrenia: a review. Schizophr Res 2006; 86(1–3):54–70 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES