Abstract
Neural activity fluctuates over time, creating considerable variability across trials. This trial-by-trial neural variability is dramatically reduced (“quenched”) after the presentation of sensory stimuli. Likewise, the power of neural oscillations, primarily in the alpha-beta band, is also reduced after stimulus onset. Despite their similarity, these phenomena have so far been studied and discussed independently. We hypothesized that the two phenomena are tightly coupled in electrophysiological recordings of large cortical neural populations. To test this, we examined magnetoencephalography (MEG) recordings of healthy subjects viewing repeated presentations of a visual stimulus. The timing, amplitude, and spatial topography of variability-quenching and power-suppression were remarkably similar. Neural variability quenching was eliminated by excluding the alpha-beta band from the recordings, but not by excluding other frequency-bands. Moreover, individual magnitudes of alpha-beta band-power explained 86% of between-subject differences in variability quenching. An alternative mechanism that may generate variability quenching is increased phase alignment across trials. However, changes in inter-trial-phase-coherence (ITPC) exhibited distinct timing and no correlations with the magnitude of variability quenching in individual participants. These results reveal that neural variability quenching is tightly coupled with stimulus-induced changes in the power of alpha-beta band oscillations, associating two phenomena that have so far been studied in isolation.
Subject terms: Perception, Cognitive neuroscience, Sensory processing, Visual system
Introduction
Neural activity is highly variable, such that repeated presentations of an identical stimulus result in variable neural responses across trials1–6. This trial-by-trial variability is relatively large before stimulus presentation, and strongly reduced (quenched) approximately 200 ms after stimulus presentation7. Neural variability quenching is a robust phenomenon that has been reported in intracellular membrane potential recordings in cats, extracellular recordings of spiking activity in monkeys7,8, and in human electroencephalography (EEG)9–12, electrocorticography (ECOG)13, magnetoencephalography (MEG)11, and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) recordings14,15. Furthermore, the phenomenon was reported during both awake and anaesthetized states, and in several cortical areas7,15 using a variety of sensory stimuli7,10. Neural variability quenching seems to be a network phenomenon that is apparent across large populations of neighboring neurons regardless of their firing rates or stimulus selectivity7,16.
Another robust phenomenon that is apparent in recordings of electrophysiological mass activity is the reduction of induced oscillatory power approximately 200 ms after stimulus presentation. This power suppression predominates in the alpha (8–13 Hz) and beta (14–25 Hz) bands, and is often referred to as event related desynchronization (ERD)17,18. It is evident in a spatially selective manner corresponding to the sensory-activated cortical areas19, and coincides with increases in gamma power (>30 Hz) and population spiking activity20. It is, therefore, commonly assumed that reductions in alpha power indicate an increase in cortical activity21.
In the current study we examined neural variability quenching in the context of mass electrophysiological recordings using MEG. In such recordings, trial-by-trial variability may be driven by two independent mechanisms (Fig. 1). First, a stimulus-induced decrease in oscillatory power/amplitude would yield fewer trail-by-trial differences regardless of the precise timing of these oscillations (Fig. 1A). Second, a stimulus-evoked increase in phase coherence across trials (i.e., better phase locking across trials) would also yield fewer trial-by-trial differences (Fig. 1B). The two mechanisms are not mutually exclusive and may both contribute to the variability quenching phenomenon as recorded in EEG or MEG studies22. Note that the two mechanisms may also counteract each other such that, for example, variability may increase despite a partial increase in phase coherence, if it is accompanied by a simultaneous large increase in oscillatory power.
In the current study we quantified the relationships between spectral power and trial-by-trial neural variability in several ways. First, we extracted specific frequency bands from the MEG data and determined the effect that this had on neural variability magnitudes. Second, we examined whether individual subject differences in spectral power could explain individual differences in neural variability. Finally, we examined whether individual subject differences in inter-trial phase coherence (ITPC) could explain individual differences in neural variability. These analyses were performed using MEG recordings from an experiment with a coherently moving stimulus (henceforth called ‘moving pattern’) presented for a relatively long duration (750 ms), which is known to induce reliable sustained gamma band responses in MEG recordings23. Such gamma band responses are difficult to detect with other techniques or stimuli24,25.
Materials and Methods
The current study utilized a subset of MEG recordings that were part of a previously published study regarding perceptual decision making23.
Subjects
23 subjects (13 females; age range, 20–54; mean age, 26.6 years; SD, 7.5 years) were included in the current study. All subjects had normal or corrected-to-normal vision and no known history of neurological disorders. The experiment was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the local ethics committee of the Hamburg Medical Association. Each subject gave written informed consent.
Experimental design
Subjects passively viewed a repeating visual stimulus while MEG data was recorded (Fig. 2A). The stimulus consisted of a large and salient, full-field grid of white crosses (17° × 17°) that rotated in the clock-wise or counter-clockwise direction (speed: 160°/s). This moving pattern surrounded a small Gabor patch (diameter, 2°; two cycles), located in the lower right or left visual field quadrant (counterbalanced between subjects) that was modulated at a temporal frequency of 10 Hz. Subjects fixated on a fixation mark (0.8° width and length) in the middle of the screen to suppress potential eye movements. Stimuli were presented using the Presentation Software (NeuroBehavioral Systems Inc.). Stimuli were back-projected on a transparent screen using a Sanyo PLC-XP51 projector with a resolution of 1024 × 768 pixels at 60 Hz. Subjects were seated 58 cm from the screen in a whole-head magnetoencephalography (MEG) scanner setup in a dimly lit room. Each trial started with the presentation of the fixation mark for 250 ms, followed by fixation mark and full stimulus for 750 ms, fixation mark only for 750 ms, and an inter-trial-interval of 750 ms containing a blank screen. This experiment was used as a localizer for quantifying sensory responses to the moving pattern and Gabor patch in a previous study23. This previous study examined perceptual decision making during motion induced blindness26, a phenomenon where the moving pattern induces the illusory disappearances of small but salient static stimuli (i.e., the Gabor patch). The flicker of the Gabor patch in this localizer, however, was specifically implemented to prevent the occurrence of motion induced blindness23. The large sensory cortical response induced by this compound stimulus was mostly due to the larger moving pattern rather than the small Gabor patch27.
Data acquisition
MEG data were acquired using a 275-channel MEG system (VSM/CTF Systems) with a sample rate of 1200 Hz, while subjects were in a seated position. The location of each subjects’ head was measured throughout the experiment using three fiducial markers placed on both ears and the nasal bridge to control for excessive movement. Furthermore, electrooculography and electrocardiography were recorded to aid post-hoc artifact rejection.
Preprocessing
MEG data were analyzed in MATLAB (MathWorks Inc., USA) using the Fieldtrip toolbox28, EEG toolbox29, and custom-written software. Each trial was defined as an epoch that started 800 ms before stimulus onset and lasted until 1000 ms after stimulus offset (i.e., −800 to 1750 ms with respect to stimulus onset). Artifacts related to environmental noise, eye and muscle activity, and squid jumps were detected using standard automated artifact rejection methods included in the Fieldtrip toolbox. Trials containing artifacts were excluded and remaining data was down sampled to 500 Hz. The final analysis was conducted, on average, with 156 trials (SD = 54.6) per subject. We focused our analysis on the cortical regions processing the physical stimulus (i.e., visual cortex). We, therefore, selected 25 occipital and parietal sensors that exhibited the strongest stimulus-induced response, as defined and previously reported by Meindertsma et al. (2017) (Fig. 2B). One sensor was missing in many subjects, and therefore removed from the data of all subjects, resulting in 24 sensors of interest. We also present topographical displays of our findings, which demonstrate the spatial selectivity of the results.
Spectral analyses
Spectral decomposition of MEG recordings was performed using a sliding Hamming windowed Fourier transform (step size: 20 samples, window length: 250 samples, centered in the middle), as implemented in EEGLAB29, and performed separately for each trial, sensor, and subject. This window size enabled us to estimate power in the delta-band (2–4 Hz), while retaining a reasonable temporal resolution for assessing stimulus-induced changes in power. The same window properties were used across all frequency bands to allow comparison of their temporal dynamics.
Power was calculated for each time-frequency segment by computing the absolute values of the Fourier coefficients. The resulting time–frequency power estimates (i.e., spectrograms) were averaged across the 24 sensors described above and then across trials to obtain a spectrogram for each subject. These were then averaged to create a spectrogram for the entire group. We also isolated power changes in specific frequency bands, which included the delta (2–4 Hz), alpha-beta (5–25 Hz), and gamma (60–120 Hz) bands, using band-pass filters. This was implemented by applying one low-pass and one high-pass Hamming-windowed FIR filter for each of the frequency ranges, with cutoff frequencies equal to the upper and lower limits of each frequency range, respectively. Topographic plots of changes in power were obtained by computing the mean power of each frequency band 200–700 ms after stimulus onset, relative to baseline (−250 ms to stimulus onset), in units of percent signal change (see below). These power changes were then averaged across subjects to create a single topographic plot.
To estimate the effect of extracting each of the frequency bands on trial-by-trial variability we used band-stop finite impulse response (FIR) filters with a Hamming-window. The cutoff frequencies of each filter were equal to the lower and upper limits of each frequency range described above.
Relative stimulus-induced change in power was normalized into units of percent signal change. We calculated the power in the pre-stimulus period (−250 ms to stimulus onset) and post-stimulus period (200–700 ms after stimulus onset) and computed percent signal change as follows:
Finally, we also estimated inter-trial phase coherence (ITPC) across trials, for each frequency and sensor29. This measure reflects the degree to which the phase of each frequency is aligned across trials. The ITPC measure was calculated as follows:
Raw ITPC was calculated for each frequency (f) in a sliding time window (t) that was equivalent to that described above for the frequency analysis. F stands for the Fourier transform, k is the trial number, and | | is the complex norm. The raw ITPC values in each time window can range from 0 (indicating complete lack of phase alignment across trials) to 1 (indicating perfect phase alignment). ITPC values were then normalized to percent change units with respect to the pre-stimulus period as described above for the power calculations.
Analyses of trial-by-trial variability
Trial-by-trial variability was computed across trials for each time point in every sensor. Absolute trial-by-trial variability in the pre-stimulus () and post-stimulus () periods were computed by averaging across the relevant time-points (−250 ms to stimulus onset, and 200–700 ms after stimulus onset, respectively). Relative change in trial-by-trial variability (i.e., neural variability quenching) was then estimated by dividing the variability in the post-stimulus period by the pre-stimulus period and adjusting to percentage change units, as follows:
To isolate the contribution of each frequency band to the magnitude of variability quenching, we used Hamming windowed FIR band-stop filters to exclude data in each of the frequency bands as described above.
Head motion control analysis
To control for potential head motion artifacts, we computed the three-dimensional position of the head in every time-point. We then quantified head-motion by computing the mean absolute difference in position from each time-point to the next. Estimated magnitudes of head-motion were then correlated with individual measures of neural variability quenching to determine potential relationships or lack there-of.
Statistical analysis
To identify statistically significant changes in oscillatory power or trial-by-trial variability, while correcting for multiple comparisons, we used two-tailed cluster-based permutation tests30. This involved identifying time-points with an un-corrected p-value smaller than 0.05 when applying a paired sample t-test. Consecutive time-points that exceeded the threshold formed candidate clusters and the sum of each cluster’s t-values was computed. We then used a Monte-Carlo permutation with 1000 iterations to define a probability distribution of t-value sums from clusters in randomly drawn sets of time-points30. The corrected p-value was defined by the relative percentile of the actual cluster statistic relative to this null distribution of random cluster statistics.
We computed Pearson’s correlation coefficients to assess potential relationships between neural variability quenching and oscillatory power or ITPC, across subjects. The same analysis was performed in the control analysis with estimates of head motion. We also used a partial-correlation analysis to estimate the relative contribution of oscillatory power in each frequency band to the magnitude of neural variability. This eliminated inter-dependencies across frequency bands, thereby isolating the contribution of each frequency band from that of the others. We performed a regression analysis to explain between-subject-differences in the magnitude of variability quenching, using the subjects’ change in power as measured in each frequency band separately. We then computed penalized-likelihood criteria for comparison between the regression models using both the Akaike Information Criterion (AIC) and Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC) as implemented in the MATLAB fitlm function. Spatial correlations between changes in power and variability quenching were calculated for each subject, and then averaged across subjects.
Results
Subjects exhibited clear stimulus-evoked ERF responses (Fig. 3A) with commonly reported negative and positive peaks that were locked to stimulus onset and offset. In addition, subjects exhibited strong stimulus-induced responses (Fig. 3B) with a characteristic time-frequency and spatial signature31. An initial broadband power increase in all frequencies was followed by different frequency-band specific dynamics. Power in the delta (2–4 Hz) band increased dramatically with stimulus presentation, peaking approximately 200 ms after stimulus onset. Power in the alpha-beta frequency band (5–25 Hz) increased transiently and then decreased to negative values ~200 ms after stimulus presentation (Fig. 3C). Power in the gamma (60–120 Hz) band increased after stimulus presentation in a sustained manner and returned to pre-stimulus levels ~250 ms after stimulus offset.
Note the similar temporal dynamics of power across the alpha-beta (5–25 Hz) frequency range (Fig. 3B). This motivated us to simplify all further analyses by aggregating this frequency range together, as performed in other recent studies32,33. Importantly, analyzing the alpha and beta bands separately did not change any of the reported conclusions. Power changes in the three selected frequency bands, 200–700 ms after stimulus presentation, exhibited different spatial characteristics (Fig. 3D). Power reduction in the alpha-beta band and power enhancement in the gamma band were specific to sensors located over occipital and parietal cortices, while changes in the delta band were more diffused and distributed.
Neural variability quenching
Subjects exhibited robust reductions in trial-by-trial neural variability 200–700 ms after stimulus presentation in comparison to the pre-stimulus period (Fig. 4A). To determine the relationship between variability quenching and the activity of specific frequency bands, we re-computed neural variability after isolating each frequency-band using band-pass filters (Fig. 4B). This revealed that variability quenching was remarkably strong 200–700 ms after stimulus presentation in isolated alpha-beta band activity, where variability quenching reached a mean value of −54.42% (blue line, Fig. 4B). In contrast, neural variability was enhanced, rather than quenched in isolated gamma band activity (+6.95%, orange line, Fig. 4B). In isolated delta band activity, there was an initial transient increase in variability, followed by a reduction in variability from approximately 150 ms after stimulus onset, reaching a mean value of −20.32% (green line, Fig. 4B). This was similar to the magnitude of variability quenching in the original, un-filtered data (−20.29%, yellow line, Fig. 4B).
In a complementary analysis we used band-stop filters to eliminate specific frequency bands in the MEG data (see Materials and Methods) and then re-computed trial-to-trial variability (Fig. 4C). Eliminating the alpha-beta frequency band dramatically altered variability quenching from a mean value of −20.29% in the original signal (yellow line, Fig. 4C) to variability enhancement with a mean value of +31.67% (blue line, Fig. 4C). In contrast, eliminating the Delta or Gamma bands had minor effects on variability quenching, which had values of −19.26% and −20.83% (green and orange lines, Fig. 4C) respectively. Taken together, these results demonstrate that variability quenching is strongly dependent on neural activity changes that are specific to the alpha-beta band. Note that this analysis estimated the mean response across subjects and disregarded individual differences.
The spatial topography of changes in neural variability was selective to occipital and parietal sensors in isolated delta, alpha-beta, and gamma band activity. The band-specific spatial changes in neural variability (Fig. 4D) were very similar to the spatial changes in power (Fig. 3D). This was apparent in a moderate spatial correlation in the delta (r(268) = 0.57, p = 0.02) band and very strong correlations in the alpha-beta (r(268) = 0.95, p < 0.001) and gamma (r(268) = 0.95, p < 0.001) bands.
Individual differences
In line with previous studies10, individual subjects exhibited distinct magnitudes of neural variability quenching. These individual differences were strongly correlated with the magnitudes of stimulus-induced power changes (Fig. 5) in the delta (r(23) = 0.59, p = 0.003) and alpha-beta (r(23) = 0.93, p < 0.001) frequency-bands, but not in the gamma band (r(23) = −0.17, p = 0.44). These results demonstrate that individual differences in variability quenching were best explained by individual differences in alpha-beta band power reductions, which explained 86% of between-subject differences (adjusted R2 = 0.86, AIC = 176.3, BIC = 180.9). To examine the combined predictive value of power changes in all three frequency bands on variability quenching magnitudes, we also performed a multiple regression analysis. The regression model included three predictors containing the individual subjects’ power changes in each frequency band. This regression model yielded an adjusted R2 value of 0.87 (AIC = 176.5, BIC = 178.8), suggesting that adding the delta and gamma power-changes did very little to improve the ability to predict individual variability quenching magnitudes.
Variability quenching is not associated with the timing of neural responses
The results presented thus far establish that stimulus-induced modulations in alpha-beta band oscillatory power contribute to neural variability quenching. Trial-to-trial variability, however, is governed not only by the amplitude of neural oscillations (Fig. 1A), but also by their phase (i.e., timing) relative to stimulus presentation (Fig. 1B). We, therefore, tested whether variability quenching was also associated with an increase in inter trial phase coherence (ITPC). ITPC increased transiently after stimulus onset and offset (Fig. 6A,B) with a time course that corresponded to the appearance of the ERF peaks (Fig. 3A). This ITPC time course was distinct from that of variability quenching, which was sustained throughout the stimulus presentation (Fig. 4). Furthermore, individual magnitudes of variability quenching were not significantly correlated with ITPC changes in any of the examined frequency bands (Fig. 6C): delta (r(23) = −0.16, p = 0.46), alpha-beta (r(23) = −0.30, p = 0.17), or gamma (r(23) = −0.03, p = 0.91). Taken together, these results demonstrate that variability quenching is not associated with stimulus-evoked changes in the phase of neural oscillations.
Control analyses
To exclude alternative explanations of the data we examined whether head motion affected our measures of neural variability. We computed a mean measure of head motion magnitude for each subject (see Methods) and found that there was no significant correlation with neural variability quenching (r(23) = 0.12, p = 0.54). Between-subject differences in variability quenching were, therefore, unrelated to individual differences in head movements.
Discussion
Our results demonstrate that neural variability quenching is tightly coupled to reductions in alpha-beta band power, as measured by MEG. The timing, amplitude, and spatial topography of variability quenching and decreases in alpha-beta band power were remarkably similar (Figs 3, 4), and removing the alpha-beta band from the data entirely eliminated the variability quenching phenomenon (Fig. 4C). The strong relationship between changes in oscillatory power in this frequency band and neural variability were also apparent when examining individual subject differences (Fig. 5). Specifically, individual magnitudes of alpha-beta band power changes explained 86% of between-subject differences in neural variability quenching. In contrast, removing the delta or gamma band from the data had little effect on the magnitude of variability quenching (Fig. 4), and adding individual differences in delta or gamma band power as predictors in a multiple regression model did not increase the explanatory power beyond what was already described with alpha-beta band changes (adjusted R2 = 0.87). Taken together, these findings suggest that neural variability quenching is tightly coupled with changes in alpha-beta band power that are induced by the presentation of a stimulus.
While neural variability quenching was strongly related to the amplitude of induced oscillatory power, it was not related to the phase-locking of neural oscillations. The time course of ITPC changes did not correspond to the time course of neural variability quenching, and individual subject ITPC magnitudes were not correlated with neural variability quenching magnitudes (Fig. 6). Hence, cortical responses to sensory stimuli are characterized by relatively high reproducibility (i.e., low trial-by-trial variability) that is dependent on stimulus-induced decreases in alpha-beta band power rather than stimulus-evoked phase resetting.
Event related desynchronization and synchronization
Stimulus or task induced reductions in oscillatory power, primarily in the alpha-beta band, are commonly referred to as ERD17,18, which is thought to coincide with increased synchronization in gamma band oscillations and increased multi-unit activity20. These concomitant changes in cortical oscillatory power are thought to represent the transition of cortical state from an idle “resting-state” to an active state of anticipation, sensory processing, and/or task initiation. It is believed that such oscillatory changes are essential for synchronizing the activity of task-related cortical neural ensembles34,35. Previous studies have not examined how these changes in oscillatory power relate to measures of trial-by-trial neural variability.
We selected the current experiment, utilizing MEG recordings and an experimental design with a relatively long, salient, moving stimulus, because it is particularly useful for identifying sustained gamma band responses23 that are difficult to identify with other techniques and experimental designs24,25. Indeed, our results revealed clear sustained gamma band responses with focal topography in occipital and parietal sensors (Fig. 3).
We initially hypothesized that we would find a positive correlation between the magnitude of ERD and neural variability quenching as well as a negative correlation between the magnitude of gamma band power and neural variability quenching. This was expected given the inverse relationship between stimulus-induced ERD and gamma power. However, we did not find a significant relationship between gamma power and neural variability quenching (Fig. 5). This suggests that individual differences in neural variability, in MEG recordings, are mostly associated with differences in alpha-beta band oscillations, which are larger in amplitude than gamma band oscillations. Note that gamma band responses have a substantially higher signal to noise ratio in invasive recordings of cortical mass activity33. Studying the relationship between neural oscillations and variability quenching using intra-cranial recordings is, therefore, highly warranted.
Ongoing neural activity
Ongoing neural activity continuously changes and fluctuates in the absence of stimuli or tasks thereby creating considerable moment-by-moment neural variability4,36,37. Some studies have suggested that these ongoing fluctuations persist during the processing of stimuli4 and execution of tasks38,39 such that stimulus evoked responses are linearly superimposed on ongoing fluctuations. In such a case one would expect similar trial-by-trial neural variability to exist before and after stimulus presentation. Many recent studies, however, have shown that trial-by-trial neural variability is dramatically reduced following stimulus presentation7,9–11,14–16. This suggests that ongoing neural fluctuations do not persist, but are instead altered by the presentation of a stimulus such that trial-by-trial variability is reduced/quenched.
In mass electrophysiological recordings such as MEG, trial-by-trial variability may be governed by two distinct factors (Fig. 1). The first is the overall power of ongoing neural oscillations with random phase from trial to trial: the larger the power in each of the trials, the larger the variance across trials (Fig. 1A). The second is the phase coherence across trials: the larger the coherence, the smaller the variance across trials. Note that these two factors are not mutually exclusive (Fig. 1B)22.
Our results suggest that the presentation of a visual stimulus quenches trial-by-trial neural variability by reducing induced oscillatory power in the alpha-beta band, rather than by evoking a reproducible phase locked response (Figs 3–6). The strong relationship between the ERD and the variability quenching phenomena suggests that ongoing neural activity fluctuations are actively suppressed after the presentation of a sensory stimulus, perhaps to achieve a more reproducible and stable cortical state during sensory processing11.
Behavioral significance
There are several similarities in the behavioral significance that has been assigned to the ERD and neural variability phenomena. For example, some have reported that allocating attention reduces neural variability across trials14,40–42 while others have reported that allocating attention creates ERD43–45. Similarly, some have reported that threshold-level stimuli are accurately perceived on trials with reduced neural variability11,46 while others have reported the same on trials with larger ERD47. Finally, individuals with lower contrast discrimination thresholds exhibited larger magnitudes of neural variability quenching, which coincided with larger ERD magnitudes9. All studies, except the last one, have reported one measure or the other and none have directly compared ERD and neural variability measures. We speculate that these independent studies may be reporting strongly correlated measures that seem to describe two tightly coupled phenomena.
Limitations
Quenching of trial-by-trial neural variability has been studied with a variety of techniques including intra-cellular recordings of membrane potentials, extracellular recordings of single-unit spiking activity in animals7, and extra-cranial recordings in humans9,10. While all of these studies have shown similar variability quenching approximately 200 ms after stimulus presentation, the specific mechanisms that influence trial-by-trial variability in the spiking of single neurons may differ from those that influence trial-by-trial variability of large neural populations as measured with MEG. With that said, one recent study reported that reductions in trial-by-trial spike count variability (i.e., Fano factor) are significantly correlated with reductions in beta-band power of the local field potential, in macaque motor cortex during movement execution48. Further studies are necessary to bridge the gaps between different manifestations of neural variability as they appear in single cell spiking activity, local field potentials, EEG/MEG, and fMRI.
Another import limitation of the current study is the specific experimental setup and stimulus that was used. It may be the case that when employing other tasks or stimuli, the relationship between variability quenching and alpha-beta band power may change to some degree. For example, stimulating subjects with steady-state visual or auditory stimuli may alter the relative power of specific frequency bands and change the relative contribution of specific frequency bands to variability quenching49. In addition, changes in delta or gamma power may have a more pronounced impact on variability quenching in other experimental contexts.
Conclusions
Our results demonstrate that stimulus-induced reductions in alpha-beta band power are tightly coupled with neural variability quenching as apparent in MEG recordings. The suppression of these oscillations, and subsequent reduction in trial-by-trial variability, may enable sensory cortices to generate more stable and reproducible neural representations of a stimulus across trials, which is likely to be beneficial for accurate perception. This appealing conceptual framework, whereby cortical responses involve an active reduction of neural “noise” (i.e., neural activity that is not related to the stimulus), is in line with signal detection theory principles50, and fits well with the existing literature regarding neural variability quenching and ERD. Quantifying oscillatory power, inter-trial phase coherence, and trial-by-trial variability in the same experiments will enable future studies to assess the robustness and validity of this conceptual framework across different stimuli, tasks, and recording techniques, and determine its behavioral significance.
Acknowledgements
This work was supported by the Israeli Science Foundation (Grant 961/14 to I.D.), the Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (NWO, 406-14-016 to T.H.D. and T.M.) and the German Research Foundation (DFG, Heisenberg Professorship DO 1240/3-1 and DO 1240/4-1 to T.H.D.).
Author contributions
T.M. designed study, collected and preprocessed data, and assisted with data analysis. E.D. conducted all analyses and data visualizations, and wrote the main manuscript text. A.A. contributed to the data analysis. T.H.D. contributed to the design of the study, and offered helpful insights for data interpretation. I.D. guided the data analyses process and edited the manuscript. All authors discussed the results and contributed to the final manuscript.
Data availability
The MEG recordings utilized in the current study along with an explanation regarding their structure will be uploaded to an open access server and will be available to anyone who would like to re-analyze them.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
These authors contributed equally: Edan Daniel and Thomas Meindertsma.
References
- 1.Shadlen MN, Newsome WT. The variable discharge of cortical neurons: implications for connectivity, computation, and information coding. J. Neurosci. 1998;18:3870–3896. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-10-03870.1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Tolhurst DJ, Movshon JA, Dean AF. The statistical reliability of single neurons in cat and monkey visual cortex. Vision Res. 1983;23:775–785. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(83)90200-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Tomko GJ, Crapper DR. Neuronal variability: non-stationary responses to identical visual stimuli. Brain Res. 1974;79:405–418. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(74)90438-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Arieli, A., Sterkin, A., Grinvald, A. & Aertsen, A. Dynamics of Ongoing Activity: Explanation of the Large Variability in Evoked Cortical Responses. 273, 1868–1871 (1996). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 5.Faisal A, Selen L, Wolpert D. Noise in the nervous system. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2008;9:292–303. doi: 10.1038/nrn2258. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Werner G, Mountcastle VB. the Variability of Central Neural Activity in a Sensory System, and Its Implications for the Central Reflection of Sensory Events. J. Neurophysiol. 1963;26:958–977. doi: 10.1152/jn.1963.26.6.958. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Churchland MM, et al. Stimulus onset quenches neural variability: A widespread cortical phenomenon. Nat. Neurosci. 2010;13:369–378. doi: 10.1038/nn.2501. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Churchland MM, Yu BM, Ryu SI, Santhanam G, Shenoy KV. Neural Variability in Premotor Cortex Provides a Signature of Motor Preparation. J. Neurosci. 2006;26:3697–3712. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3762-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Arazi A, Censor N, Dinstein I. Neural Variability Quenching Predicts Individual Perceptual Abilities. J. Neurosci. 2017;37:97–109. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1671-16.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Arazi A, Gonen-Yaacovi G, Dinstein I. The Magnitude of Trial-By-Trial Neural Variability Is Reproducible over Time and across Tasks in Humans. eNeuro4. ENEURO.0292–. 2017;17:2017. doi: 10.1523/ENEURO.0292-17.2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Schurger A, Sarigiannidis I, Naccache L, Sitt JD, Dehaene S. Cortical activity is more stable when sensory stimuli are consciously perceived. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 2015;112:E2083–E2092. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1418730112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Arazi Ayelet, Yeshurun Yaffa, Dinstein Ilan. Neural Variability Is Quenched by Attention. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2019;39(30):5975–5985. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0355-19.2019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.He, B. J. & Zempel, J. M. Average Is Optimal: An Inverted-U Relationship between Trial-to-Trial Brain Activity and Behavioral Performance. PLoS Comput. Biol. 9 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 14.Broday-Dvir R, Grossman S, Furman-Haran E, Malach R. Quenching of spontaneous fluctuations by attention in human visual cortex. Neuroimage. 2018;171:84–98. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.12.089. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.He BJ. Spontaneous and Task-Evoked Brain Activity Negatively Interact. J. Neurosci. 2013;33:4672–4682. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2922-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Goris RLT, Movshon JA, Simoncelli EP. Partitioning neuronal variability. Nat. Neurosci. 2014;17:858–865. doi: 10.1038/nn.3711. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Pfurtscheller G, Lopes da Silva FH. Event-related EEG/MEG synchronization and desynchronization: basic principles. Clin. Neurophysiol. 1999;110:1842–1857. doi: 10.1016/S1388-2457(99)00141-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Pfurtscheller G, Aranibar A. Event-related cortical desynchronization detected by power measurements of scalp EEG. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 1977;42:817–826. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(77)90235-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Jensen O, Mazaheri A. Shaping Functional Architecture by Oscillatory Alpha Activity: Gating by Inhibition. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2010;4:186. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2010.00186. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Mukamel R, et al. Coupling between neuronal firing, field potentials, and fMRI in human auditory cortex. Science (80-.). 2005;309:951–954. doi: 10.1126/science.1110913. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Neuper Christa, Wörtz Michael, Pfurtscheller Gert. Progress in Brain Research. 2006. ERD/ERS patterns reflecting sensorimotor activation and deactivation; pp. 211–222. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Dinstein I, Heeger DJ, Behrmann M. Neural variability: friend or foe? Trends Cogn. Sci. 2015;19:322–8. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2015.04.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Meindertsma T, Kloosterman NA, Nolte G, Engel AK, Donner TH. Multiple Transient Signals in Human Visual Cortex Associated with an Elementary Decision. J. Neurosci. 2017;37:5744–5757. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3835-16.2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Whitham EM, et al. Scalp electrical recording during paralysis: Quantitative evidence that EEG frequencies above 20 Hz are contaminated by EMG. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2007;118:1877–1888. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2007.04.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Yuval-Greenberg S, Tomer O, Keren AS, Nelken I, Deouell LY. Transient Induced Gamma-Band Response in EEG as a Manifestation of Miniature Saccades. Neuron. 2008;58:429–441. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.03.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Bonneh YS, Cooperman A, Sagi D. Motion-induced blindness in normal observers. Nature. 2001;411:798–801. doi: 10.1038/35081073. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Kloosterman NA, et al. Top-down modulation in human visual cortex predicts the stability of a perceptual illusion. J. Neurophysiol. 2015;113:1063–1076. doi: 10.1152/jn.00338.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Oostenveld R, Fries P, Maris E, Schoffelen JM. FieldTrip: Open source software for advanced analysis of MEG, EEG, and invasive electrophysiological data. Comput. Intell. Neurosci. 2011;1:1–9. doi: 10.1155/2011/156869. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Delorme A, Makeig S. EEGLAB: An open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. J. Neurosci. Methods. 2004;134:9–21. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.10.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Efron, B. & Tibshirani, R. An introduction to the bootstrap. (Chapman and Hall/CRC, 1994).
- 31.Donner Tobias H., Siegel Markus. A framework for local cortical oscillation patterns. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2011;15(5):191–199. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.03.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Michalareas G, et al. Alpha-Beta and Gamma Rhythms Subserve Feedback and Feedforward Influences among Human Visual Cortical Areas. Neuron. 2016;89:384–397. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.12.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Fries Pascal. Rhythms for Cognition: Communication through Coherence. Neuron. 2015;88(1):220–235. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Siegel M, Donner TH, Engel AK. Spectral fingerprints of large-scale neuronal interactions. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2012;13:121–34. doi: 10.1038/nrn3137. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Engel Andreas K., Fries Pascal, Singer Wolf. Dynamic predictions: Oscillations and synchrony in top–down processing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2001;2(10):704–716. doi: 10.1038/35094565. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Fox MD, Raichle ME. Spontaneous fluctuations in brain activity observed with functional magnetic resonance imaging. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2007;8:700–11. doi: 10.1038/nrn2201. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Biswal Bharat, Zerrin Yetkin F., Haughton Victor M., Hyde James S. Functional connectivity in the motor cortex of resting human brain using echo-planar mri. Magnetic Resonance in Medicine. 1995;34(4):537–541. doi: 10.1002/mrm.1910340409. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Fox Michael D., Snyder Abraham Z., Vincent Justin L., Raichle Marcus E. Intrinsic Fluctuations within Cortical Systems Account for Intertrial Variability in Human Behavior. Neuron. 2007;56(1):171–184. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.08.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Becker R, Reinacher M, Freyer F, Villringer A, Ritter P. How ongoing neuronal oscillations account for evoked fMRI variability. J. Neurosci. 2011;31:11016–27. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0210-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Mitchell Jude F., Sundberg Kristy A., Reynolds John H. Differential Attention-Dependent Response Modulation across Cell Classes in Macaque Visual Area V4. Neuron. 2007;55(1):131–141. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.06.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Mitchell JF, Sundberg KA, Reynolds JH. Spatial Attention Decorrelates Intrinsic Activity Fluctuations in Macaque Area V4. Neuron. 2009 doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.09.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Cohen Marlene R, Maunsell John H R. Attention improves performance primarily by reducing interneuronal correlations. Nature Neuroscience. 2009;12(12):1594–1600. doi: 10.1038/nn.2439. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Gregor T, et al. Alpha Band Electroencephalographic Activity over Occipital Cortex Indexes Visuospatial Attention Bias and Predicts Visual Target Detection. J. Neurosci. 2006;26:9494–9502. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0875-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Siegel M, Donner TH, Oostenveld R, Fries P, Engel AK. Neuronal Synchronization along the Dorsal Visual Pathway Reflects the Focus of Spatial Attention. Neuron. 2008;60:709–719. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.09.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Ikkai A, Dandekar S, Curtis CE. Lateralization in Alpha-Band Oscillations Predicts the Locus and Spatial Distribution of Attention. PLoS One. 2016;11:e0154796. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0154796. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Schurger A, Pereira F, Treisman A, Cohen JD. Reproducibility distinguishes conscious from nonconscious neural representations. Science. 2010;327:97–99. doi: 10.1126/science.1180029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.van Dijk H, Schoffelen J-M, Oostenveld R, Jensen O. Prestimulus Oscillatory Activity in the Alpha Band Predicts Visual Discrimination Ability. J. Neurosci. 2008 doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1853-07.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Riehle, A., Brochier, T., Nawrot, M. & Grün, S. Behavioral Context Determines Network State and Variability Dynamics in Monkey Motor Cortex. Front. Neural Circuits, 10.3389/fncir.2018.00052 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 49.Keitel Christian, Keitel Anne, Benwell Christopher S.Y., Daube Christoph, Thut Gregor, Gross Joachim. Stimulus-Driven Brain Rhythms within the Alpha Band: The Attentional-Modulation Conundrum. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2019;39(16):3119–3129. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1633-18.2019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Green DM, Swets JA. Signal detection theory and psychophysics. Society. 1966 doi: 10.1901/jeab.1969.12-475. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
The MEG recordings utilized in the current study along with an explanation regarding their structure will be uploaded to an open access server and will be available to anyone who would like to re-analyze them.