Abstract
Identification of individuals at high risk of dementia has usually focused attention on the clinical concept of mild cognitive impairment (MCI), which captures an intermediate state between normal cognitive ageing and dementia. In many countries age specific risk of dementia has declined, but whether this is also the case for subclinical cognitive impairment is unknown. This has important implications for prevention, planning and policy. Here we describe subclinical cognitive impairment and mild dementia prevalence changes, in the UK, over 2 decades. The Cognitive Function and Ageing Studies have examined the full spectrum of cognition, from normal to dementia, in representative populations of people aged ≥ 65 years in the UK over the last 2 decades 7635 participants were interviewed in CFAS I in Cambridgeshire, Newcastle, and Nottingham in 1991, with 1457 being diagnostically assessed. In the same geographical areas, the CFAS II investigators interviewed 7796 individuals in 2011. Using established criteria, the population was categorised into seven groups: no cognitive impairment, Mild cognitive Impairment (defined using consensus criteria), other cognitive impairment no dementia without functional impairment, OCIND with functional impairment, cognitive impairment (MMSE < 24 and no functional impairment), mild dementia (MMSE < 24 with functional impairment, not captured by CFAS dementia criteria), and CFAS dementia criteria. Multinomial logistic regression, adjusted for age and sex, was used to estimate the prevalence of impairment in both studies. Results were standardized to the age-sex specific UK and global population. There is a clear increase in the prevalence of other cognitive Impairment no Dementia (without functional impairment), with the purer MCI remaining stable. In the UK, mild dementia is estimated to fall from 520,704 cases (5.7%, 95% CI 3.8, 8.1) in 1991 to 315,142 (3.0%, 95% CI 2.4, 3.8) in 2011, cognitive impairment, has fallen from 1,225,984 (13.5%, 95% CI 10.1, 17.5) to 654,436 (6.3%, 95% CI 5.4, 7.3) cases. Using additional categories which reflect the continuum of cognitive decline and impairment in populations we see that the mildest dementia declines, but that there is stability in estimates of those who meet MCI criteria. Increases were found in the Other Cognitive Impairment no Dementia group. The decline observed in severe impairment thus seems to have resulted in larger proportions of the population in milder forms, seen alongside physical illnesses.
Electronic supplementary material
The online version of this article (10.1007/s10654-019-00554-x) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.
Keywords: Mild cognitive impairment, Cognition, Dementia, Alzheimer’s disease, Epidemiology, MCI
Introduction
Mild cognitive impairment (MCI) represents an intermediate state of cognitive functioning between changes expected as a normal part of ageing and dementia [1]. MCI prevalence has been estimated to range between < 1 and 42% in older populations depending on the classification criteria used and setting (e.g. clinical vs. population based) [2–4]. Reported rates of progression of MCI to dementia have also varied and range from around 8–15% per year [2]. However, not all individuals with MCI experience decline; MCI has been shown to be a transient state, with some of the MCI population at risk of progressing to dementia at a later stage while others possibly regain cognitive function [5–9].
Defining subclinical cognitive impairment is complex and challenging due to a lack of standardised diagnostic criteria. Although the MCI criteria developed by Petersen and colleagues are an attempt to standardise subclinical impairment, concerns have emerged that the criteria are too restrictive [10, 11]. Population based studies have repeatedly shown that definitions of cognitive decline which cover a broader range of dysfunction, such as other cognitive impairment no dementia (OCIND) are more prevalent than more restrictive definitions of MCI and also have a high progression rate to dementia [6, 12]. There is also disagreement about the cut points used to define deficits in cognitive performance needed to distinguish MCI from minimal or questionable dementia [13, 14]. The longstanding approach of excluding individuals with medical comorbidities has also been challenged, with evidence suggesting that excluding this group could result in insufficient or biased MCI diagnosis, especially for clinical trials, thus restricting generalizability of results [15]. Consensus criteria proposed by the International Working Group on Mild Cognitive Impairment, in 2004, broadened the concept developed by Petersen and colleagues to include impairment in any cognitive domain as well as relax criteria focused on functional impairment [3]. In 2011, the National Institute on Aging and the Alzheimer’s Association (NIA-AA) refined the definition of MCI to account for possible aetiologies, including amnestic MCI (aMCI) and non-amnestic MCI (naMCI) thought to be associated with AD and vascular dementia, respectively [16]. In 2013 the new Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition (DSM-V) was released and included the diagnosis of mild neurocognitive disorder, a pre-dementia state based on MCI criteria [17]. Few community based populations have investigated DSM-V mild neurocognitive disorder, however a prevalence of 20% (95% CI 17.8–23.0) was reported in the LIFE-Adult-Study [18]. This study also found a 98.6% diagnostic overlap between the DSM-V and consensus MCI criteria.
Cohort effects have been observed in dementia. Indeed, in high income countries such as USA and UK, in the last 2 decades, it appears that risk of dementia is declining [19]. Whether cohort effects are seen in the prevalence of MCI and risk of progression to dementia is not known but has important public health implications (e.g. calculating population burden of disease). Further, determining how best to identify individuals at high risk of dementia will have important implications for intervention trials (e.g. clinical trial recruitment protocols), treatment (e.g. personalised medicine), public health surveillance (e.g. monitoring) and education (e.g. prevention programmes). Therefore, in this study, MCI (defined using consensus criteria) [20], in addition to other states of the entire cognitive spectrum, are investigated in the Cognitive Function and Ageing Studies (CFAS) in order to determine whether similar trends are seen for these states in the last 2 decades as those seen in dementia.
Methods
Setting, study design, and participants
Information on the CFAS I and II study designs has been published in detail previously [21, 22]. In brief, CFAS I baseline interviews were undertaken between 1989 and 1994. In total, 13,044 individuals aged 65 years and older were recruited in five geographical areas in England and Wales including: Cambridgeshire, Oxfordshire, Nottingham, Newcastle and Gwynedd. Between 2008 and 2011 a new cohort of individuals (N = 7796), aged 65 years and older were selected and interviewed from three of the original CFAS areas: Cambridgeshire, Newcastle, and Nottingham with the aim to provide contemporary evidence on dementia prevalence, incidence and risk factors. This study of prevalence includes all participants who underwent diagnostic assessment in CFAS I and CFAS II (Fig. 1).
Ethics
Fully informed written consent was sought, and when capacity was impaired procedures complied with the UK Mental Capacity Act 2005. Full details of ethical approvals in place for CFAS I and II can be found at: http://www.cfas.ac.uk/cfas-i/data/#cfasi-ethical-approval.
Outcome description
Full details of the cognitive outcomes are described in the Appendix. Briefly we sought to classify the entire cognitive spectrum of the older population from normal cognition, through mild cognitive impairment to dementia. Those without study diagnosis of dementia were classified into groups: No cognitive impairment, MCI, other cognitive impairment not dementia (OCIND—with and without functional impairment) and cognitive impairment with (mild dementia) and without functional impairment (Supplementary Table 1, Fig. 2).
Statistical analysis
Prevalence estimates were obtained using a multinomial logistic regression model, which predicted the probability of participants belonging to each group within the cognitive spectrum in one model. Participants with no cognitive impairment were treated as the reference group. This was repeated for men and women in each 5-year age band. Predicted probabilities were multiplied by UK population, stratified by age and sex to estimate population prevalence [22]. Inverse probability weights were used to adjust for non-response (CFAS I and CFAS II) and screen-assessment sampling (CFAS I), based on UK age, sex and deprivation structure. The three centres (Cambridgeshire, Newcastle and Nottingham) included in both CFAS I (n = 7635) and II (n = 7796) were used. Bootstrap resampling with 1000 replications was used to calculate 95% confidence intervals (95% CI) via the [bias corrected and accelerated] percentile method.
Age and sex specific prevalence estimates were combined with population data to obtain the number of individuals in the population in each cognitive group in 1991 (using CFAS I estimates) and in 2011 (using CFAS II estimates) [23]. Predicted estimates of the population assuming a constant prevalence over the 2 decades was calculated using the CFAS I estimates combined with the 2011 UK population data.
Results
Sample demographics
In CFAS I 7635 individuals took part in the baseline wave (80% response), with 1459 taking part in the wave 1 assessment interview (82% of those still alive), giving an overall response rate of 66% to the assessment interview. In CFAS II screen and assessment was undertaken in one stage and had a response rate of 56%. Figure 1 shows the number of individuals included in the baseline interviews in CFAS I and II. Demographic characteristics including age, sex, location, education, baseline cognitive function, Basic Activities of Daily Living/Instrumental Activities of Daily Living and dementia status are shown in Table 1.
Table 1.
CFAS I | CFAS II | ||
---|---|---|---|
Screening (%)a | Assessment (%)b | N (%)c | |
Sex | |||
Men | 3045 (39) | 531 (38) | 3550 (44) |
Women | 4590 (61) | 926 (62) | 4246 (56) |
Age group | |||
65–69 | 1981 (25) | 310 (23) | 1939 (23) |
70–74 | 1776 (23) | 320 (22) | 1874 (23) |
75–79 | 1725 (22) | 263 (23) | 1623 (21) |
80–84 | 1308 (18) | 291 (20) | 1289 (17) |
85–89 | 615 (9) | 186 (9) | 769 (11) |
> 90 | 230 (4) | 87 (3) | 302 (6) |
Education (years full time) | |||
0–9 | 5529 (74) | 1074 (80) | 1976 (26) |
10 | 644 (9) | 106 (8) | 2734 (36) |
> 10 | 1286 (17) | 172 (12) | 2909 (38) |
Geographical area | |||
Cambridgeshire | 2601 (34) | 465 (37) | 2558 (30) |
Newcastle | 2522 (33) | 499 (31) | 2616 (34) |
Nottingham | 2512 (33) | 493 (32) | 2622 (35) |
Residential status | |||
Community | 7281 (95) | 1269 (95) | 7599 (97) |
Care homes | 347 (5) | 183 (5) | 197 (3) |
Dementia | NA | 329 (8.6) | 461 (6) |
Characteristics (n (%)) including sex, age group, education (years full time), geographical area, residential status and dementia status. Percentages are backed weighted for initial non-response (CFAS I screening and CFAS II) and study design (CFAS I assessment)
Data are n (%)
CFAS, Cognitive Function and Ageing Study
aPercentages back-weighted for non-response
bPercentages back-weighted for sampling design and non-response
cResidential status missing for seven individuals for CFAS I (of whom five were also assessed)
UK population prevalence of each cognitive group
Figure 3 shows the prevalence of each cognitive group standardized to the age and sex structure of the UK in 1991 and 2011. The figure also shows the estimated prevalence for 2011 from age and sex-based predictions from the 1991 data. Cohort effects were observed in the definitions of mild dementia and cognitive impairment. Over the 2 decades, mild dementia has almost halved from 520,704 cases (5.7%, 95% CI 3.8, 8.1) in 1991 to 315,142 cases (3.0%, 95% CI 2.4, 3.8) in 2011 (Table 2). There has been a larger decrease in cognitive impairment, which has proportionally fallen by more than half from 1,225,984 (13.5%, 95% CI 10.1, 17.5) to 654,436 cases (6.3%, 95% CI 5.4, 7.3) (Table 2). This contrasts with the age-based projections which predicted that the prevalence of mild dementia and cognitive impairment would have remained relatively stable at 6.2% (95% CI 4.1, 8.7) and 13.4% (95% CI 9.9, 17.5), respectively (Table 2). The reduction in MCI prevalence over the 2 decades was small (2.4%) suggesting stable case numbers: 1,590,481 cases (17.6%, 95% CI 12.5, 22.9) versus 1,575,577 cases (15.2%, 95% CI 13.8, 16.6). In contrast between 1991 and 2011, OCIND (without FI) prevalence increased from 3,331,809 (36.8%, 95% CI 30.3, 43.6) to 4,191,265 cases (40.4%, 95% CI 38.5, 42.3) while prevalence of OCIND (with FI) remained stable (5.0%, 95% CI 2.6, 8.1 in 1991 vs. 5.6%, 95% CI 4.6, 6.5 in 2011) (Table 2). The numbers of people with no cognitive impairment increased from 1,291,396 cases (14.3%, 95% CI 9.3, 19.4) in 1991, to an estimated 2,376,070 cases (22.9%, 95% CI 21.3, 24.5), this result was also higher than the predicted age-based projections for 2011 of 1,518,700 cases (14.6%, 95% CI 9.6, 19.9).
Table 2.
Cognitive group | CFAS I | 2011 Age based projections | CFAS II | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
n | % (95% CI) | n | % (95% CI) | n | % (95% CI) | |
NCI | 1,291,396 | 14.3 (9.3, 19.4) | 1,518,700 | 14.6 (9.6, 19.9) | 2,376,070 | 22.9 (21.3, 24.5) |
OCIND | 3,331,809 | 36.8 (30.3, 43.6) | 3,695,737 | 35.6 (29.2, 42.5) | 4,191,265 | 40.4 (38.5, 42.3) |
MCI | 1,590,481 | 17.6 (12.5, 22.9) | 1,775,397 | 17.1 (12.1, 22.4) | 1,575,577 | 15.2 (13.5, 16.6) |
OCIND (with FI) | 456,253 | 5.0 (2.6, 8.1) | 522,149 | 5.0 (2.6, 8.1) | 577,559 | 5.6 (4.6, 6.5) |
Cognitive impairment | 1,225,984 | 13.5 (10.1, 17.5) | 1,395,954 | 13.4 (9.9, 17.5) | 654,436 | 6.3 (5.4, 7.3) |
Mild dementia | 520,704 | 5.7 (3.8, 8.1) | 638,522 | 6.2 (4.1, 8.7) | 315,142 | 3.0 (2.4, 3.8) |
Dementia | 642,238 | 7.1 (5.3, 9.2) | 833,426 | 8.0 (6.1, 10.4) | 690,051 | 6.6 (5.6, 7.8) |
Estimates were obtained using multiple regression modelling, with 95% CI’s obtained by bootstrap with 1000 replications via the percentile method. Age based projections for 2011 were obtained using 1991 estimates applied to 2011 population
Discussion
Using CFAS I and II the prevalence of the full spectrum of cognitive function including normal, MCI, OCIND and dementia (including its mildest forms) could be measured over 2 decades with population resampling. There is clear evidence of a cohort effect in the prevalence of mild dementia (decrease), cognitive impairment (decrease) and OCIND without functional impairment (increased). The numbers of people with no impairment have also increased relative to the age-based projections for 2011. Cohort effects are not seen in MCI and OCIND (with functional impairment), the prevalence of which appear to remain stable over the 2 decades.
There are a number of strengths to the study. This study is able to investigate cognitive impairment across the full spectrum (i.e. from no impairment to dementia), strengthened by the methodological approach of using repeated prevalence’s studies. CFAS give unique insight based on random sampling from three areas of England, allowing the examination of cross-generational cognitive impairment, being powered to detect changes and implementation of consistent methods and diagnostic procedures. There are also some limitations. First, although the key methodologies of CFAS I and II are identical, CFAS I comprised a two-stage design with the complete cognitive assessment on a subsample, though the analysis does take this fully into account. CFAS II was designed to avoid this complexity while retaining the ability to compare results across the two studies. Second, dementia was diagnosed algorithmically using the AGECAT validated with DSM-III-R. With the introduction of new DSM-V criteria, it is unknown whether prevalence of dementia or MCI may have differed had these criteria been applied. Understanding population change in MCI has been complicated by the lack of standardised cut-off scores, age norms, changing criteria including DSM-V creating an unstable environment. The advantage of using population based data is that it was collected without applying criteria and can be used to operationalise criteria that were not developed at the time of the study. Here we used consensus criteria for MCI which has been shown that when compared with the recent DSM-V criteria of mild neurocognitive disorder has high diagnostic overlap (98.6%) [17, 20].
While cohort effects were observed across the spectrum of cognitive impairment there were differences depending on group. Cognitive impairment and mild dementia do not share the same level of decrease as has been previously observed in dementia with both groups seeing a far greater reduction. When we combine the estimates of dementia, mild dementia and cognitive impairment in the older population together it corresponds to over one million people in 2018 with many likely to have very similar care needs to those with a diagnosis of dementia. In contrast, the proportion of cases with no cognitive impairment and OCIND (without FI) increased. A possible explanation for this is that the trends in population ageing along with improvements in education, health care and reduction of cardiovascular risk factors associated with cognitive impairment have led to a greater proportion of older people with no impairment. Further, where cognition has declined it remains mild rather than declining to the point of impairment. Interestingly the prevalence of MCI has remained stable since 1991. The estimated prevalence is consistent with other large cohort studies internationally, with the average prevalence of MCI from consensus criteria around 12% and 18% [24–28].
There is much debate in the literature about the variance in MCI prevalence estimates from epidemiological studies [2]. Evidence suggests that lack of consistency is largely due to a lack of standardised diagnostic criteria and homogenous study designs, methodology and populations [20]. For decades, the main area of disagreement has been around how to define mild impairment by neuropsychological testing; for example, tighter definitions such as scoring 1.5 standard deviations from normal on memory domain tests show much lower prevalence’s compared to the DSM-V criteria of between 1 and 2 standard deviations on non-specified domain testing [4]. Recently the criteria for MCI has narrowed with the introduction of consensus criteria [20] and different subtypes (amnestic MCI, non-amnestic MCI and multiple MCI) with the aim to help improve diagnostic accuracy [2]. Consensus criteria were selected for this study, for reasons stated earlier, and different changes over time may be obtained if a different definition were used. But this is beyond the scope of the current study.
The results show that since 1991 there has been a shift in the cognitive burden within the older population; although the prevalence of dementia and its mild dementia forms may be declining, OCIND is increasing and MCI is stable. These results have a number of implications for strategic planning for cognitive impairment and dementia. First, they provide further evidence alongside studies [29–31] that have found a decline in risk of cognitive impairment (measured using CIND) in older aged populations; extending the findings for the first time by showing that although severe cognitive impairment and the mildest forms of dementia have decreased, mild impairment in the form of OCIND has increased and MCI has remained stable. Strategic planning for the numbers of people with dementia will have to be adjusted to budget correctly as the prevalence of the disease declines, however, this analysis shows that despite these trends, there is still a significant number of people with cognitive impairment and mild dementia who may indeed be missed from formal dementia diagnosis but who have similar care needs. Planning for increasing numbers of people with OCIND and MCI is more complex, evidence suggests patients diagnosed with MCI prefer to undergo testing to predict dementia prevalence in the future; due to ever changing technologies, guidelines for diagnosis and management for MCI means testing is likely to be uncertain [32]. Diagnostic testing for MCI can incur heavy financial costs, and so far interventions to prevent or delay progression to dementia have limited efficacy [33, 34]. Planning for the future must involve policy makers and clinicians discussing the balance of risks and benefits of interventions, also involving patient’s views on interventions in later life.
In addition, our results suggest that changes seen in the population that has prevented dementia, has also impacted on severe cognitive impairment. However, what is not clear is whether lifestyle factors such as education and improved cardiovascular and metabolic health is having the same reductive effect on MCI [30, 35]. Indeed, the findings suggest that the increase in OCIND and stability of MCI could reflect “normal cognitive ageing” among a population which is substantially older. As well as the financial burden, a diagnosis of MCI can lead to emotional distress and social stigma; cognitive decline is a subject of fear among older people and patients may thus prefer foregoing the diagnosis altogether [33, 36]. In light of this care must be taken not to wrongly identify people with MCI as a clinical condition, which may be “normal cognitive ageing”. Clinical decision making regarding MCI will likely change in light of an effective disease modifying agent for pre-dementia being identified. In the meantime evidence suggests it is through healthy lifestyle behaviours, better educational attainment, minimizing risks from polypharmacy and comorbidities in the wider population which should be further encouraged to reduce risk of severe cognitive impairment [30].
Electronic supplementary material
Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.
Authors’ contribution
CR performed the data analysis under the supervision of BS and FM. CR drafted the first version of the manuscript. CB and LR were involved in defining cognitive groupings. All authors reviewed and edited the final manuscript. CB and FM obtained funding for CFAS I, including initial investigation of MCI. CB, LR, FM defining the aims and objectives for CFAS II, and obtained funding. FM designed CFAS II and the statistical analysis plan. CB, FM and BS attained funding for this project.
Funding
Connor Richardson was funded as part of the Alzheimer’s Society (UK) Cognitive Function and Ageing study doctoral training centre. CFAS II was funded by UK Medical Research Council (MRC; research Grant G0601022), Alzheimer’s Society UK and utilised resources of the UK National Institute of Health Research collaboration for leadership in applied health research and care for Cambridgeshire and Peterborough and the Cambridge Biomedical Research Centre infrastructures, Nottingham city and Nottinghamshire county NHS primary care trusts, and UK NIHR biomedical research centre for ageing and age-related disease award to Newcastle Upon Tyne hospital foundation trust. MRC CFAS (CFAS I) was funded by the MRC (including G0400077, G9901400) and the Department of Health. FEM is supported by the MRC Grants (MC_U105292687 and MC_UU_00002/12). We thank the participants, their families, the general practitioners and their staff, and the primary care trusts for their cooperation and support. We thank the all the fieldwork interviewers at Cambridge, Nottingham, and Newcastle for their valuable contribution.
Footnotes
C. Brayne and F. Matthews are joint senior authors.
Publisher's Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
- 1.Petersen RC, Caracciolo B, Brayne C, Gauthier S, Jelic V, Fratiglioni L. Mild cognitive impairment: a concept in evolution. J Intern Med. 2014;275(3):214–228. doi: 10.1111/joim.12190. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Petersen RC. Mild cognitive impairment. Contin Lifelong Learn Neurol. 2016;22(2 Dementia):404–418. doi: 10.1212/con.0000000000000313. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Ward A, Arrighi HM, Michels S, Cedarbaum JM. Mild cognitive impairment: disparity of incidence and prevalence estimates. Alzheimer’s Dement J Alzheimer’s Assoc. 2012;8(1):14–21. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2011.01.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Sachdev PS, Lipnicki DM, Kochan NA, et al. The prevalence of mild cognitive impairment in diverse geographical and ethnocultural regions: the COSMIC collaboration. PLoS ONE. 2015;10(11):e0142388. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0142388. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Stephan BC, Brayne C, McKeith IG, Bond J, Matthews FE. Mild cognitive impairment in the older population: Who is missed and does it matter? Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2008;23(8):863–871. doi: 10.1002/gps.2013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Stephan BC, Matthews FE, McKeith IG, Bond J, Brayne C. Early cognitive change in the general population: How do different definitions work? J Am Geriatr Soc. 2007;55(10):1534–1540. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2007.01386.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Matthews FE, Stephan BC, McKeith IG, Bond J, Brayne C. Two-year progression from mild cognitive impairment to dementia: to what extent do different definitions agree? J Am Geriatr Soc. 2008;56(8):1424–1433. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2008.01820.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Ganguli M, Snitz BE, Saxton JA, et al. Outcomes of mild cognitive impairment by definition: a population study. Arch Neurol. 2011;68(6):761–767. doi: 10.1001/archneurol.2011.101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Aretouli E, Okonkwo OC, Samek J, Brandt J. The fate of the 0.5 s: predictors of 2-year outcome in mild cognitive impairment. J Int Neuropsychol Soc JINS. 2011;17(2):277–288. doi: 10.1017/s1355617710001621. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Ritchie K, Artero S, Touchon J. Classification criteria for mild cognitive impairment: a population-based validation study. Neurology. 2001;56(1):37–42. doi: 10.1212/WNL.56.1.37. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Rasquin S, Lodder J, Visser P, Lousberg R, Verhey F. Predictive accuracy of MCI subtypes for Alzheimer’s disease and vascular dementia in subjects with mild cognitive impairment: a 2-year follow-up study. Dement Geriatr Cognit Disord. 2005;19(2–3):113–119. doi: 10.1159/000082662. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Hogan DB, Ebly EM. Primitive reflexes and dementia: results from the Canadian Study of Health and Aging. Age Ageing. 1995;24(5):375–381. doi: 10.1093/ageing/24.5.375. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Roth M, Tym E, Mountjoy C, et al. CAMDEX: a standardised instrument for the diagnosis of mental disorder in the elderly with special reference to the early detection of dementia. Br J Psychiatry. 1986;149(6):698–709. doi: 10.1192/bjp.149.6.698. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic Criteria from dsm-iv-tr, American Psychiatric Pub; 2000.
- 15.Stephan BCM, Brayne C, Savva GM, Matthews FE. Medical research council cognitive F, ageing S. Occurrence of medical co-morbidity in mild cognitive impairment: implications for generalisation of MCI research. Age Ageing. 2011;40(4):501–507. doi: 10.1093/ageing/afr057. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Albert MS, DeKosky ST, Dickson D, et al. The diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment due to Alzheimer’s disease: recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimer’s Dement J Alzheimer’s Assoc. 2011;7(3):270–279. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2011.03.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-5®), American Psychiatric Pub; 2013.
- 18.Luck T, Then FS, Schroeter ML, et al. Prevalence of DSM-5 mild neurocognitive disorder in dementia-free older adults: results of the population-based LIFE-adult-study. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2017;25(4):328–339. doi: 10.1016/j.jagp.2016.07.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Stephan BCM, Birdi R, Tang EYH, et al. Secular trends in dementia prevalence and incidence worldwide: a systematic review. J Alzheimer’s Dis JAD. 2018;66(2):653–680. doi: 10.3233/jad-180375. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Winblad B, Palmer K, Kivipelto M, et al. Mild cognitive impairment–beyond controversies, towards a consensus: report of the international working group on mild cognitive impairment. J Intern Med. 2004;256(3):240–246. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2796.2004.01380.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Matthews FE, Stephan BCM, Robinson L, et al. A 2 decade dementia incidence comparison from the Cognitive Function and Ageing Studies I and II. Nat Commun. 2016;7:11398. doi: 10.1038/ncomms11398. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Matthews FE, Arthur A, Barnes LE, et al. A 2-decade comparison of prevalence of dementia in individuals aged 65 years and older from three geographical areas of England: results of the Cognitive Function and Ageing Study I and II. The Lancet. 2013;382(9902):1405–1412. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(13)61570-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Office for National Statistics. Population Estimates. 2017. https://www.ons.gov.uk/peoplepopulationandcommunity/populationandmigration/populationestimates. Accessed 06 June/2017.
- 24.Busse A, Hensel A, Guhne U, Angermeyer MC, Riedel-Heller SG. Mild cognitive impairment: long-term course of four clinical subtypes. Neurology. 2006;67(12):2176–2185. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000249117.23318.e1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Di Carlo A, Lamassa M, Baldereschi M, et al. CIND and MCI in the Italian elderly: frequency, vascular risk factors, progression to dementia. Neurology. 2007;68(22):1909–1916. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000263132.99055.0d. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Ganguli M, Chang CC, Snitz BE, Saxton JA, Vanderbilt J, Lee CW. Prevalence of mild cognitive impairment by multiple classifications: The Monongahela-Youghiogheny Healthy Aging Team (MYHAT) project. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry Off J Am Assoc Geriatr Psychiatry. 2010;18(8):674–683. doi: 10.1097/JGP.0b013e3181cdee4f. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Lopez OL, Jagust WJ, DeKosky ST, et al. Prevalence and classification of mild cognitive impairment in the Cardiovascular Health Study Cognition Study: part 1. Arch Neurol. 2003;60(10):1385–1389. doi: 10.1001/archneur.60.10.1385. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Manly JJ, Tang MX, Schupf N, Stern Y, Vonsattel JP, Mayeux R. Frequency and course of mild cognitive impairment in a multiethnic community. Ann Neurol. 2008;63(4):494–506. doi: 10.1002/ana.21326. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Hall KS, Gao S, Baiyewu O, et al. Prevalence rates for dementia and Alzheimer’s disease in African Americans: 1992 versus 2001. Alzheimer’s Dement. 2009;5(3):227–233. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2009.01.026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Langa KM, Larson EB, Crimmins EM, et al. A comparison of the prevalence of dementia in the United States in 2000 and 2012. JAMA Intern Med. 2017;177(1):51–58. doi: 10.1001/jamainternmed.2016.6807. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Rocca WA, Petersen RC, Knopman DS, et al. Trends in the incidence and prevalence of Alzheimer’s disease, dementia, and cognitive impairment in the United States. Alzheimer’s Dement. 2011;7(1):80–93. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2010.11.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Wikler EM, Blendon RJ, Benson JM. Would you want to know? Public attitudes on early diagnostic testing for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Res Ther. 2013;5(5):43. doi: 10.1186/alzrt206. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Le Couteur DG, Doust J, Creasey H, Brayne C. Political drive to screen for pre-dementia: not evidence based and ignores the harms of diagnosis. BMJ. 2013;347:f5125. doi: 10.1136/bmj.f5125. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Brayne C, Davis D. Making Alzheimer’s and dementia research fit for populations. Lancet (Lond, Engl) 2012;380(9851):1441–1443. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(12)61803-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Geda YE, Roberts RO, Knopman DS, et al. Physical exercise, aging, and mild cognitive impairment: a population-based study. Arch Neurol. 2010;67(1):80–86. doi: 10.1001/archneurol.2009.297. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Small GW, Bookheimer SY, Thompson PM, et al. Current and future uses of neuroimaging for cognitively impaired patients. Lancet Neurol. 2008;7(2):161–172. doi: 10.1016/s1474-4422(08)70019-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.