Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2019 Nov 12;8:e49511. doi: 10.7554/eLife.49511

Motor cortex can directly drive the globus pallidus neurons in a projection neuron type-dependent manner in the rat

Fuyuki Karube 1,, Susumu Takahashi 1,2,, Kenta Kobayashi 3, Fumino Fujiyama 1,
Editors: Naoshige Uchida4, Floris P de Lange5
PMCID: PMC6863630  PMID: 31711567

Abstract

The basal ganglia are critical for the control of motor behaviors and for reinforcement learning. Here, we demonstrate in rats that primary and secondary motor areas (M1 and M2) make functional synaptic connections in the globus pallidus (GP), not usually thought of as an input site of the basal ganglia. Morphological observation revealed that the density of axonal boutons from motor cortices in the GP was 47% and 78% of that in the subthalamic nucleus (STN) from M1 and M2, respectively. Cortical excitation of GP neurons was comparable to that of STN neurons in slice preparations. FoxP2-expressing arkypallidal neurons were preferentially innervated by the motor cortex. The connection probability of cortico-pallidal innervation was higher for M2 than M1. These results suggest that cortico-pallidal innervation is an additional excitatory input to the basal ganglia, and that it can affect behaviors via the cortex-basal ganglia-thalamus motor loop.

Research organism: Rat

Introduction

Parallel loops of neural connections among the cerebral cortex, basal ganglia, and thalamus contribute to multiple aspects of behavior (Alexander et al., 1986; Nambu, 2008; Wei and Wang, 2016). The functions mediated by these loops depend on relevant cortical areas and brain regions receiving outputs of the basal ganglia (Hikosaka, 2007; Middleton and Strick, 2000). The loop containing the motor cortex is crucial for appropriate motor control, action selection, and movement-related learning. Dysfunction of the motor loop leads to movement disorders such as those seen in Parkinsonian disease (Albin et al., 1989; DeLong, 1990; Middleton and Strick, 2002; Nambu, 2008; Nambu et al., 2000; Parent and Hazrati, 1995a; Redgrave et al., 2010; Wichmann and DeLong, 1996). Cortical projections drive three pathways in the basal ganglia: the direct, the indirect, and the hyperdirect pathways (Bolam et al., 2000; Smith et al., 1998). The direct and indirect pathways originate from two distinct types of striatal medium spiny neurons (MSNs), termed the direct- and indirect-pathway MSNs (dMSNs and iMSNs). dMSNs project to the output nuclei of the basal ganglia, namely the substantia nigra (SN) pars reticulata (SNr), and the globus pallidus internal segment, the latter being the homologue of the entopeduncular nucleus (EP) in rodents. iMSNs project to the globus pallidus external segment (GP in rodents), which interconnects with the subthalamic nucleus (STN). In turn, both the STN and GP also innervate the output nuclei. The hyperdirect pathway involves direct cortical projections to the STN (Nambu et al., 2002), which provides the fastest information flow among the three pathways (Nambu et al., 2000). The behavioral functions of these pathways are gradually being elucidated, although recent findings propose refinement and reappraisal of the classical views of the functional roles of distinct MSNs (Calabresi et al., 2014; Cui et al., 2013; Isomura et al., 2013; Vicente et al., 2016). According to the traditional model, the direct pathway promotes the execution of desired actions, whereas the indirect pathway prevents the execution of competing actions (Friend and Kravitz, 2014; Nambu, 2007; Vicente et al., 2016), and the hyperdirect pathway emergently cancels or switches imminent movements (Frank et al., 2007; Isoda and Hikosaka, 2008; Nambu et al., 2002; Schmidt et al., 2013). Massive connections between the STN and GP illustrate that the hyperdirect and the indirect pathways operate in a coordinated manner.

Recently, two types of GP neurons have been identified: one projects to the STN with or without projection to the striatum, while the other only projects to the striatum (Abdi et al., 2015; Dodson et al., 2015; Hernández et al., 2015; Mallet et al., 2012; Mastro et al., 2014), which has been implicated in the rewriting of neural circuitry involved in the GP. The GP is now considered the hub of the basal ganglia, not simply a relay nucleus (Hegeman et al., 2016). Not only the circuitry, but also the functional significance of these neuron types and the entire GP have gradually been uncovered (Mallet et al., 2016; Mastro et al., 2017); however, how their activities are controlled remains unclear. In 1994, Naito and Kita reported cortical projections to the GP, which raised the possibility of an additional neural pathway that can modulate the basal ganglia activities. Although some studies have proposed more recent evidence on cortico-pallidal projection (Hunt et al., 2018; Abecassis et al., 2019; Magno et al., 2019), a detailed morphological and electrophysiological analysis of this connection has not been conducted so far (Milardi et al., 2015; Smith and Wichmann, 2015).

Using a combination of neural tracing with immunostaining, we here demonstrate that M1 and M2 directly project to the GP, and that cortical axon collaterals and boutons have topographic distributions that depend on the cortical area of origin (Naito and Kita, 1994). Using morphological and electrophysiological experiments combined with optogenetics, we demonstrate that cortico-pallidal synapses are effective and dependent on post-synaptic GP cell types. We discuss the potential roles of cortico-pallidal projections in relation to other basal ganglia nuclei. Part of this research has been presented in abstract form earlier (Karube and Fujiyama, 2017).

Results

Motor cortex innervates the GP

We observed cortical axons in the GP using conventional tracers (BDA or PHA-L; Figure 1A, B and C; Figure 2, Figure 1—figure supplements 1, 2 and 3; Figure 2—figure supplement 1) or adenoassociated virus (AAV) vectors (Figure 1D; Figure 2—figure supplements 1, 2), as reported previously (Naito and Kita, 1994). For both M1 and M2 projections, cortical axons were dense in calbindin D-28k (CB) -negative [CB(-)] regions of the GP. The appearance of cortical axon varicosities in the GP was similar to those in the striatum and STN (Figure 1A2 and C, Figure 1—figure supplements 1 and 2). Some axons in the GP were emitted from thick trunks, and often elongated along the dorso-ventral axis (Figure 1B). Cortico-pallidal projections were exclusively ipsilateral (Figure 1D), implying that they did not stem from the intratelencephalon (IT) type of pyramidal neurons. In addition, we found that cingulate area (Cg) and lateral orbitofrontal area (LO) also projected to the GP (Figure 2—figure supplements 1 and 2), although LO provided fewer axons than motor areas. Cortical axons were differentially distributed in the GP, depending on the cortical area of origin (Figure 1—figure supplements 1 and 2; Figure 2—figure supplements 1 and 2). M1 and M2 axons were observed across the broad extent of the medio-lateral axis (range: M/L 2.4–4.2 mm from the midline; see also Figure 2A, B and C), but relatively sparse in the medial and lateral portions in most cases (Figure 2C). LO axons were often located in the ventral part of the GP, whereas Cg axons were dense in the more medial GP than M1/M2 axons.

Figure 1. Motor cortical axons project to the globus pallidus (GP).

(A) Representative images of axons in the GP, striatum, and STN originated from the primary motor cortex (M1), labeled with biotinylated dextran amine (BDA; visualized in black). Cortical axons in the GP are predominantly distributed in the calbindin (CB)-negative regions. CB is visualized in brown. Irrespective of differences in axon density among the GP, striatum, and STN, the morphology of the axons and varicosities is similar (A2). (B) Drawings and an image of cortical axons in the GP. The collaterals (thin black lines in B1) were issued from thick axon trunks (thick gray lines in B1; arrowhead in B2). (C) Magnified views of cortical axon varicosities (arrowheads) in the GP; left, M1 axons; right, M2 axons. The images are composites from multiple focal planes. The sections were counter-stained with an anti-parvalbumin (PV) antibody, visualized in brown. (D), Cortical axons in the GP were found exclusively in the ipsilateral hemisphere. M2 axons were visualized with AAV. Similar to axons in the STN, fluorescent signals were not detected in the contralateral GP even by over-exposure.

Figure 1.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1. Example images of M1 projections to the GP, STN, and striatum (related to Figure 1).

Figure 1—figure supplement 1.

(A), Using BDA injections into M1, labeled axons are visualized in black and CB immunoactivity in brown. Axons are distributed in the GP lateral to the midline (M/L 2.9–4.2 mm). (B), In the same specimen, cortical axons in the STN were densely distributed at M/L 2.6 and 2.9 mm. The spatial extent of axons was restricted to the medio-caudal position of the STN. (C), M1 axons in the striatum. Note that the scale is different from A and B. (D), Magnified images of the black rectangle area, which is the ROI used for the axon varicosity count, shown in A, B, and C. Note that the size of the ROI is large enough to sample the densely innervated area.
Figure 1—figure supplement 2. Example images of M2 projections to the GP, striatum, and STN (related to Figure 1).

Figure 1—figure supplement 2.

(A), Using BDA injections into M2, labeled axons are visualized in black and CB immunoactivity in brown. Axons are distributed in the GP (M/L 2.6–3.2 mm). (B), In the same specimen, cortical axons in the STN were densely distributed at M/L 2.4 and 2.6 mm. The spatial extent of axons was restricted to the medio-caudal position of the STN. (C), M2 axon in the striatum. Note that the scale is different from A and B. (D), Magnified images of the black rectangle area, which is the ROI used for the axon varicosity count, shown in A, B, and C. (E), Normalized bouton density in the STN.
Figure 1—figure supplement 3. Example images of M2 projections to the striatum and GP (related to Figure 1).

Figure 1—figure supplement 3.

Using BDA injections into M2, labeled axons are visualized in black and CB immunoactivity in brown. Axons are densely distributed in the dorso-lateral striatum (M/L 2.1–4.3 mm). In more lateral sections, axons were denser in CB(-) subregions, and were also found in striosomes [white arrowheads; small CB(-) subregions] throughout the entire projection field. In medial sections, M2 axons were observed in both CB(+) and CB(-) subregions. M2 axons were only faintly detected in ventral striosomes (filled arrowheads), whereas they were relatively dense in dorsal striosomes (white arrowheads). Note also the presence of M2 axons in CB(-) portions of the GP (arrows).

Figure 2. Comparison of axonal bouton density in the GP, striatum, and STN.

(A), Images and drawings of BDA injection sites into the primary and secondary motor areas (M1 and M2, respectively). Tracer was deposited across the entire thickness of the cortex. (B) Images of axon distributions in the GP, striatum, and STN from M1 and M2. To clearly represent axon density, the images are composites from multiple focal planes. (C) Comparison of axon varicosity density in the GP with that in the STN along the M/L dimension (N = 4 animals for each M1 and M2). In most cases, varicosities in the STN showed a sharp peak at a given M/L position. In the GP, M1 axon varicosities were broadly distributed along the M/L dimension. M2 axons tended to be densely distributed in the medial portion. (D) The axon varicosity density in the GP was normalized to that in the striatum or the STN (E).

Figure 2—source data 1. Source data for Figure 2C.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.49511.009
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.49511.010

Figure 2.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1. LO projections to the basal ganglia (related to Figure 2).

Figure 2—figure supplement 1.

(A) LO projections to the striatum. (A1) An AAV injection site in the LO. (A2) A representative LO projection to the striatum. LO axons (red) are distributed in the caudal and ventral part of the dorsal striatum (M/L 2.6 mm). (A3) Immunofluorescence for μ-opioid receptor (MOR, green). Arrowheads indicate the striosomes, which are MOR (+), innervated by the LO axons seen in A2 and A3. The square surrounded by a dotted line in A2 and A3 is magnified in the right-most column. (B) BDA injection into the LO. (C) Brightfield images of LO axons in the GP, striatum (CPu), and STN. Note that axon and terminal densities in the GP were lower than those in the striatum or STN. (D) Quantitative comparison of axon density in the GP normalized to that in the STN (D1) and striatum (D2).
Figure 2—figure supplement 2. Cingulate cortex (Cg) projections to the basal ganglia (related to Figure 2).

Figure 2—figure supplement 2.

(A) Cg projections to the striatum (CPu) in sagittal sections (M/L 2.1 mm). Cg axons are dense in the CB-negative subregions of the striatum. Cg axons were less dense in the CB-negative striosomes. (B) Cg projections to the GP. Cg axon collaterals were dense in the CB (-) central GP. Thick axon bundles pass through the dorsal side of the medial GP (M/L 2.4 mm). (C) Cg projections to the STN. Cg axons are localized in the caudal part of the medial STN (arrowheads; M/L 2.4 mm).

We compared the density of cortical axonal varicosities (boutons) in the GP, STN, and striatum. The number of varicosities in the GP varied with the M/L dimension, as in the case of varicosities in the STN for both M1 and M2 projections (Figure 2C; N = 4 rats for each). Varicosity density was apparently greater in the striatum than in the GP or STN (Figures 1A and 2B, Figure 1—figure supplements 1 and 2Table 1). Since the size and efficacy of the tracer labeling were not uniform across animals, the maximum bouton density in the GP was normalized to that in the striatum or STN for each injection. Using this normalization, the bouton density was found to be significantly higher in the striatum than in the GP (Figure 2D; p=0.013, Kruskal Wallis test; p<10−7 for both M1 and M2 projections, Tukey-HSD test; not significant between M1 and M2, p=0.18), or in the STN (Figure 1—figure supplement 2E; p=0.016, Kruskal Wallis test; p=0.00003 for M1; p=0.000008 for M2, Tukey-HSD test; not significant between M1 and M2, p=0.296). Normalized bouton densities in the GP were significantly different from those in the STN (Figure 2E; p=0.032, Kruskal-Wallis test). For M1 projections, STN-normalized bouton density was significantly smaller in the GP (0.496 ± 0.12) than in the STN (p=0.0007, Tukey-HSD test). Since M2 had more boutons in the GP (0.778 ± 0.207) than M1 (p=0.022), M2 bouton density did not significantly differ between the STN and the GP (p=0.09).

Table 1. Electrophysiological properties of globus pallidus (GP) neurons.

GPSTN (N = 108) GPCPu (N = 65) p-value
Mean membrane potential (mV) −46.63 ± 4.95
(−34.2– −61.53)
−46.36 ± 5.9
(−33.59– −60.53)
0.682
Input resistance (MΩ) 232.35 ± 131.87
(33.1–887.45)
329.97 ± 168.19
(32.8–828.3)
6.8 × 10−05 ***
Time constant (ms) 12.94 ± 10.23
(2.25–79.75)
21.92 ± 13.11
(2.42–55.67)
2.1 × 10−07 ***
Sag potential (mV) 7.23 ± 4.52
(1.25–22.02)
8.71 ± 7
(0.63–35.18)
0.3432
Spike frequency at 100 pA depolarization (Hz) 48.46 ± 22.94
(0–103)
35.81 ± 19.6
(2–85)
0.00041 ***
Spike frequency at 500 pA depolarization (Hz) 99.14 ± 66.87
(1–244)
35.24 ± 39.24
(1–172)
1.8 × 10−09 ***
Maximum spike frequency (Hz) 135.04 ± 65.42
(11–263)
72.14 ± 36.12
(2–172)
1.0 × 10−09 ***
Current at maximum spike frequency (pA) 578.63 ± 277.97
(50–1000)
345.24 ± 219
(50–1000)
6.9 × 10−08 ***
Spike height (mV) 74.06 ± 9.77
(47.04–96.98)
76.55 ± 11.69
(38.84–95.97)
0.051
Spike width (ms) 0.95 ± 0.29
(0.63–2.29)
1.19 ± 0.32
0.7–2.17
1.6 × 10−09 ***
Spike threshold (mV) −37.57 ± 4.79
(−24.09– −48.11)
−37.7 ± 5.32
(−23.14– −46.03)
0.5351
fAHP amplitude (mV) 21.16 ± 5.14
(9.1–38.72)
18.09 ± 4.57
(11.88–36.34)
1.3 × 10−05 ***
fAHP delay after spike peak (ms) 0.98 ± 0.39
(0.55–2.7)
1.31 ± 0.55
(0.6–3.8)
3.4 × 10−07***
sAHP amplitude (mV) 18.26 ± 4.2
(9.47–32.87)
16.54 ± 4.71
(10.38–29.78)
0.0035 **
sAHP delay after spike peak (ms) 9.63 ± 3.32
(2.25–16.95)
22.58 ± 13.54
(2.25–65.5)
4.5 × 10−12 ***
On cell mode spontaneous firing frequency (Hz) 20.79 ± 18.74
(0–90.53)
5.47 ± 9.22
(0–35.68)
1.2 × 10−08 ***
Whole cell mode spontaneous firing frequency (Hz) 19.02 ± 13.74
(0–61.74)
9.41 ± 9.44
(0–31.85)
4.0 × 10−06 ***

GPSTN, GP neurons projecting to the subthalamic nucleus; GPCPu, GP neurons projecting to the striatum; fAHP, fast afterhyperpolarization; sAHP, slow afterhyperpolarization; **, p<0.01; ***, p<0.001. Statistical significance was examined using the Wilcoxon rank sum test. The range of each parameter is shown in parentheses.

These findings suggest that the motor cortices provide a substantial number of boutons in the GP, and can thus modulate GP neuronal activity as they affect the striatum and the STN. However, because bouton density does not directly indicate synaptic efficacy, we also evaluated the electrophysiological features of cortico-pallidal projections.

Cortico-GP terminals elicit monosynaptic EPSCs

We conducted whole-cell patch clamp recordings combined with optogenetic stimulation of cortical terminals using in vitro slice preparations. Channelrhodopsin 2 (ChR2) was introduced into cortical neurons using AAV-hSyn-H134R-mCherry injections into either M1 or M2 (Figure 3A). In voltage clamp mode at a holding potential of −60 mV, stimulation with a brief light pulse (5 ms, 470 nm) elicited inward currents in GP neurons (Figure 3B1). The response was stable over repetitive stimulation (10 pulses at 2–10 Hz; Figure 3B1). In current clamp mode, photoactivation elicited action potentials, although the action potential probability was affected by the spontaneous oscillation of the membrane potential (Figure 3B2). To confirm that the photoactivated current that elicited action potentials was within the physiological range, we measured the minimum current required to induce action potentials (rheobase current) in GP neurons using 5 ms depolarizing pulses (Figure 3C, inset). In half of the GP neurons, the rheobase was less than 30 pA, and most GP neurons could emit an action potential with less than 100 pA of depolarization (N = 100 neurons; Figure 3C). A depolarized membrane potential and a high input resistance (Table 1) led to easy induction of action potentials by small excitation.

Figure 3. Photoactivation of motor cortical terminals evokes excitation in GP neurons.

Figure 3.

(A) Schematic (top) of AAV encoding channelrhodopsin 2 and mCherry injection into the motor cortex for ex vivo recordings using coronal slices. Examples of AAV injection sites are shown in the middle panels (red). Images of immunofluorescence for neurofilament 200 kDa (N200, bottom), used for identification of the M1/M2 border (white dotted lines). (B1) A representative voltage clamp trace (held at −60 mV) showing inward currents in GP neurons elicited by 5 ms blue light pulses (470 nm). (B2) A representative current clamp trace showing photoinduced action potentials and excitatory postsynaptic potentials (EPSPs, arrowheads). (C) Cumulative histogram of the rheobase current of GP neurons. Note that 25 to 30 pA is sufficient to elicit action potentials in half of GP neurons (N = 100). (D) Proportion of GP neurons innervated by M1 or M2 terminals. The number of neurons is shown in bars. M2 more frequently innervated the GP than did M1. (E) Location of GP neurons innervated by M1 (red circle) or M2 (blue circle). Note the topographic distribution of M1 and M2 innervation. The size of circles represents the amplitude of optically evoked currents.

Not all GP neurons exhibited inward photocurrents, a total of 67/159 and 151/248 neurons did so during M1 and M2 stimulation, respectively (Figure 3D). The locations of the GP neurons in which inward currents were observed were plotted (Figure 3E). Consistent with the distribution of cortical axons, these locations were frequently around the center of the GP in coronal slices. Responsive neurons were similarly concentrated around the center of the GP along the rostro-caudal axis. Neurons responding to M1 terminal stimulation tended to be located in the dorsal GP, whereas those responding to M2 terminal stimulation were clustered in the ventral GP (Figure 3E).

It is possible that the observed EPSCs were elicited by the STN via a di-synaptic circuit. However, we used coronal slices with an anteroposterior position of 0.6 mm rostral (r0.6)–2.2 mm caudal (c2.2) to bregma, which did not include the STN (Paxinos and Watson, 2007). Bath application of the sodium channel blocker tetrodotoxin (TTX) at 1 µM completely prevented inward currents (Figure 4A). Additional application of the potassium channel blocker 4-aminopyridine (4AP) at 1 mM recovered the currents to up to 60% of control on average (Figure 4A), indicating that the current was monosynaptic (Gradinaru et al., 2009; Shu et al., 2007). A GABAA receptor antagonist (gabazine, 20 µM) did not block the current (Figure 4A); conversely, glutamate receptor antagonists (CNQX, 10 μM and AP-5, 20 μM) almost completely abolished the current (Figure 4A). Thus, the inward current was mediated by glutamatergic excitatory postsynaptic currents (EPSCs). We compared the time course of EPSCs between STN and GP neurons. The latency (delay) of current onset after the photic stimulus in GP neurons did not differ from that in STN neurons (Figure 4B), which indicates that both EPSCs are directly elicited by cortical terminal activation. The 20–80% rise time and decay constant tended to be longer in GP neurons than in STN neurons (Figure 4B). Taken together, these results indicate that the inward current observed in GP neurons was a monosynaptic, glutamatergic EPSC directly elicited by cortical terminals. Hereinafter, we refer to this current as an optically evoked EPSC (oEPSC). In some experiments, both GP and STN neurons were recorded from the same animal, and the amplitude of oEPSC in GP neurons was normalized to the mean of oEPSCs from STN neurons (Figure 4C; N = 10 GP neurons from three rats for M1 stimulation and N = 13 GP neurons from two rats for M2 stimulation). The normalized mean values were not significantly different from 1 (p>0.1, signed rank test), suggesting that the motor cortex could innervate GP and STN using similar weights, however, as the oEPSC amplitude largely varied across cells in both GP and STN neurons, the interpretation of this comparison must be made with caution.

Figure 4. Cortico-pallidal connections are monosynaptic and glutamatergic.

Figure 4.

(A) The current induced by optogenetic stimulation of cortical terminals is monosynaptic and glutamatergic. (A1) Representative traces of pharmacological effects on the light-induced inward current. Top, no treatment; Middle, effects of TTX, 4AP, and gabazine; Bottom, effects of additional application of glutamate receptor antagonists (CNQX, AP5). (A2) A summary plot of pharmacological treatments (N = 10 GP neurons). (B) Comparison of the time courses of optogenetically evoked EPSCs (oEPSCs). (B1) Representative traces recorded from STN (black) and GP (red) neurons. The GP and STN neurons were not recorded simultaneously. (B2-4) Summary plots of oEPSC time courses in STN and GP neurons. (B2) The latency from light onset to oEPSC onset does not differ between STN and GP neurons (both N = 15 neurons). The rise time (B3) and the decay constant (B4) of oEPSCs tended to be longer in GP neurons (N = 15 GP neurons and 12 STN neurons). (C), Comparison of oEPSC amplitude between GP and STN neurons, derived from the same animal. The amplitude in the GP neurons was normalized to that in the STN. No significant difference was observed between GP and STN neurons.

Figure 4—source data 1. Source data for Figure 4A.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.49511.014
Figure 4—source data 2. Source data for Figure 4B and C.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.49511.015

M1 and M2 more frequently innervate GP neurons projecting to the striatum than those projecting to the STN

The results shown in Figure 3D raised the question of whether the cortical innervation of GP neurons follows any specific rules. To determine whether cortical innervation depends on the GP-neuron projection type, we conducted in vitro whole cell recordings from retrogradely labeled GPSTN or GPCPu neurons (Figure 5A). Occasionally, we recorded large GP neurons with no or very little spontaneous activity (N = 6, 1.20 ± 1.79 Hz during on-cell recording mode), which exhibited a distinct action potential shape. Based on earlier reports (Bengtson and Osborne, 2000; Hernández et al., 2015), these were most likely cholinergic neurons and were excluded from subsequent analysis, although they were found to be innervated by the cortex (N = 5/6). In addition to the molecular profiles, the electrophysiological properties of GPCPu and GPSTN neurons also differed. GPSTN neurons usually showed spontaneous repetitive firing (~20 Hz; Table 1), whereas many GPCPu neurons were silent. Firing frequencies induced by depolarizing current pulses were higher in GPSTN than in GPCPu neurons, and spike width was narrower in GPSTN neurons (Figure 5B; see Table 1 for other electrophysiological parameters and quantitative comparisons). We discovered that GPCPu neurons (51/62) responded more frequently to opotogenetic activation of motor cortical afferents than GPSTN neurons (53/126), as shown in Figure 5C. The oEPSC amplitude was also larger in GPCPu neurons (Figure 5D,E,F). The distribution of oEPSC amplitudes recorded from GPCPu neurons seemed bimodal; the smaller-amplitude group was similar to GPSTN neurons (Figure 5D). For comparison, MSNs (N = 11) and STN neurons (N = 18) were also recorded. One-way ANOVA followed by post-hoc Tukey tests revealed that the oEPSC amplitude was significantly larger in MSNs than in STN or GP neurons (for all combinations of comparisons, p<9 × 10−7). The oEPSC amplitude of STN neurons was not significantly different from that of GPSTN (p=0.109) or GPCPu neurons (p=0.999). GPCPu neurons exhibited significantly greater amplitudes than GPSTN neurons (p=0.0021; Figure 5D). We observed relatively hyperpolarized membrane potentials and lower membrane input resistances in MSNs compared with GP or STN neurons. The mean membrane potential (Vmean) was −73.45 ± 5.3 mV, and input resistance (Rin) was 79.17 ± 28.38 MΩ for 10 MSNs, whereas Vmean was −45.43 ± 7.47 mV and Rin was 253.14 ± 156.31 MΩ for 15 STN neurons (see Table 1 for GP neurons). Thus, despite the larger EPSCs, the ease of induction of action potentials was lower in MSNs than in GP or STN neurons, at least in slice preparations. Actually, the median of the rheobase current was 695 pA for MSNs (N = 10; range, 450–1415 pA), 55 pA for STN neurons (N = 30; range, 10–545 pA) and 30 pA for GP neurons (N = 100; range, 5–250 pA).

Figure 5. Cell type-dependent cortical innervation of pallidal neurons.

Figure 5.

(A) Schematic of ex vivo recordings from retrogradely labeled GP neurons to investigate the effect of GP neuron projection type on cortical innervation. (B) Electrophysiological differences between GP neurons projecting to the striatum (GPCPu) and STN (GPSTN) (see also Table 1). (C) The proportion of GP neurons innervated by M1 or M2 is correlated with projection type. GPCPu neurons were more often innervated by M1 or M2 than GPSTN neurons. Significance was examined using Fisher’s exact test with a Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons (***, p<0.00025; **, p<0.0025; *, p<0.0125). (D) Amplitudes of oEPSCs in GP, STN, and striatal neurons. The amplitude in GP neurons is similar to that in STN neurons but smaller than that in striatal medium spiny neurons (MSNs). Data obtained from M1 and M2 stimulation are summed. (E) Cumulative histograms of oEPSC amplitude in GPCPu and GPSTN neurons. GPCPu neurons exhibit a greater optically evoked EPSC (oEPSC) amplitude. (F) Left, amplitudes of oEPSCs induced in GP neurons by M1 terminal stimulation. The GPCPu group shows larger oEPSC amplitudes than the GPSTN group. Right, amplitudes of oEPSCs induced by M2 terminal stimulation. Significantly larger oEPSCs were again recorded in the GPCPu group (p=0.00084), but the difference is small.

Figure 5—source data 1. Source data for Figure 5B.
The file also contained source data for Table 1.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.49511.017
Figure 5—source data 2. Source data for Figures 5C, D, E and F.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.49511.018

GPCPu neurons were frequently innervated by either M1 (28/35) or M2 (23/27). In contrast, only a small fraction of GPSTN neurons received M1 (17/51) or M2 (36/75) inputs (Figure 5C). The amplitude of M1-induced oEPSCs was significantly larger in the GPCPu neurons than in the GPSTN neurons (p=2.1 × 10−8, Wilcoxon rank sum test). This was also the case for M2-induced oEPSCs (p=0.00084). Therefore, both motor areas preferentially innervated GPCPu neurons, although GPSTN was more effectively innervated by M2 (Figure 5C F). Indeed, the mean oEPSC amplitude in GPSTN was larger with M2 stimulation than with M1 stimulation (p=0.0028), but no significant difference between cortical sites was observed in GPCPu neurons (p=0.9595). M2 stimulation frequently evoked oEPSCs with initially smaller amplitudes, especially for the first light pulse, although repetitive light pulses at 10 Hz often augmented the oEPSC amplitude, similar to M1 stimulation. The paired-pulse ratio for second-to-first oEPSC in GPSTN neurons was 1.27 ± 0.81 for M2 stimulation (N = 38) and 1.45 ± 1.20 for M1 stimulation (N = 14); in the GPCPu neurons, it was 1.25 ± 0.62 (N = 21) for M2 stimulation and 1.31 ± 0.79 (N = 28) for M1 stimulation.

It is important to uncover if any innervation bias exists in GPCPu neurons, because GPCPu neurons should be composed of at least two neuron types, prototypic neurons and arkypallidal neurons. Prototypic neurons are composed of parvalbumin (PV)- and/or LIM homeobox 6 (Lhx6)- expressing neurons, and Lhx6 neurons provide more axons into the striatum than PV neurons (Hernández et al., 2015; Mizutani et al., 2017; Oh et al., 2017). In contrast, arkypallidal neurons express forkhead box protein 2 (FoxP2), and exclusively project to the striatum. Electrophysiological membrane properties are known to differentiate between arkypallidal and prototypic neurons (Abdi et al., 2015; Hernández et al., 2015; Mallet et al., 2012; Mastro et al., 2014), however, as shown in Figure 5B, the range of parameter values of GPSTN and GPCPu neurons overlapped, indicating that they do not allow to distinguish cell types strictly. In addition, we found that immunodetection of Lhx6 was somewhat unstable after in vitro slice recordings.

Since at least a part of the Lhx6 neurons projects to both the striatum and the STN, we hypothesized that double retrograde labeling could distinguish them from other GP neuron types. To confirm this, using retrograde labeling of GP neurons and immunofluorescence against PV, Lhx6, and FoxP2, we examined the molecular profiles of GP neuron types in Wistar rats (Fujiyama et al., 2016). We confirmed that GPCPu neurons frequently expressed FoxP2 or Lhx6, but not PV (N = 659 GPCPu neurons in nine sections from three rats; Figure 6A), in agreement with previous studies in mice (Dodson et al., 2015; Hernández et al., 2015; Mastro et al., 2014; Mizutani et al., 2017), Long-Evans rats (Oh et al., 2017), and Sprague-Dawley rats (Abdi et al., 2015; Kita and Kita, 2001). The expression of FoxP2 and Lhx6 was almost mutually exclusive. Most PV(+) GPCPu neurons co-expressed Lhx6. GPSTN neurons lacked expression of FoxP2 but expressed PV and/or Lhx6 (N = 727 GPSTN neurons in nine sections from three rats; Figure 6A). Triple immunofluorescence combined with a single retrograde tracer injection into the striatum was conducted to further elucidate the molecular identity of GPCPu neurons. The expression of Lhx6 (333/665) or FoxP2 (328/665) was again almost mutually exclusive. Only a small fraction (14.7%) of Lhx6-expressing neurons co-expressed PV (49/333) (Figure 6B). Therefore, GPSTN neurons comprised PV(+) and/or Lhx6(+) prototypic neurons, whereas GPCPu neurons comprised arkypallidal neurons expressing FoxP2 and prototypic neurons expressing Lhx6 (Fujiyama et al., 2016; Mallet et al., 2012). Occasionally, double-retrogradely labeled neurons, namely bi-directionally projecting GP neurons (GPBi), which innervate both the STN and the striatum, were observed (N = 113; Figure 6A3). Lhx6 was expressed in most GPBi neurons (61/69), PV less frequently (19/72), and FoxP2 rarely (2/85), as expected. GPBi neurons that expressed PV co-expressed Lhx6, although the number of neurons examined was small (5/6). These observations show that selective electrophysiological recordings from putative Lhx6(+) neurons can be conducted by targeting GPBi neurons. We found that GPBi neurons received cortical inputs (7/10 for M1 and 7/9 for M2; N = 3 rats for each), although most of them exhibited oEPSCs with small amplitudes (Figure 6C). In fact, the frequency distributions of oEPSC amplitudes significantly differed between GPBi and GPCPu neurons (Figure 6D; p=2.2 × 10−16, Kolmogorov-Smirnov test). To clarify whether arkypallidal neurons are innervated by the motor cortex, the GPCPu neurons, in which oEPSCs were detected, were examined for immunofluorescence against FoxP2 (N = 9). We found that eight out of nine GPCPu neurons expressed FoxP2 (Figure 6E), suggesting that arkypallidal neurons are preferentially innervated by the motor cortex. Again, although the comparison of oEPSC amplitudes among experiments is difficult to interpret, this could imply that arkypallidal neurons receive larger cortical inputs.

Figure 6. Cortical inputs on bi-directional projection GP neurons (GPBi) and arkypallidal neurons.

Figure 6.

Two distinct retrograde tracers were injected into the STN and striatum (CPu), respectively. (A) Three combinations of double immunofluorescence (PV/Lhx6, PV/FoxP2, and Lhx6/FoxP2) were applied. CPu-projecting GP neurons (GPCPu) expressed either FoxP2 or Lhx6 exclusively (A1), whereas STN-projecting GP neurons (GPSTN) were mainly composed of PV- and/or Lhx6-expressing neurons (A2). (A3) GP neurons projecting to both the CPu and STN (GPBi) frequently expressed Lhx6, but not FoxP2. (B) Triple immunofluorescence for GPCPu neurons. Only a single retrograde tracer was injected into the striatum. (C) oEPSC amplitudes of GPBi neurons. Most GPBi neurons exhibited small amplitudes of oEPSCs. Green and red retrobeads were injected into the CPu and STN, respectively. Confocal images of a biocytin-filled GPBi neuron (arrowheads) are shown. AAV-labeled cortical axons also show red fluorescence. (D) Cumulative histograms of oEPSC amplitudes in GPBi and GPCPu neurons. The distributions are significantly different (p=2.2 × 10−16 Kolmogorov-Smirnov test). The histogram of GPCPu neurons is the same as that shown in Figure 5E. (E) An example of confocal images of GPCPu neurons innervated by the motor cortex. Upper, the first imaging session before immunoreaction to identify a biocytin-filled neuron that is labeled with retrobeads. The slice was cleared with CUBIC. Bottom, the second imaging session of the neuron after immunoreaction against FoxP2. The focal plane was slightly shifted from the first to the second session, because retrobeads are accumulated in the cytosol, whereas FoxP2 is localized in the nucleus.

Figure 6—source data 1. Source data for Figure 6A and B.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.49511.021
Figure 6—source data 2. Source data for Figure 6C and D.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.49511.022

Discussion

To summarize, we here report on the direct motor cortical innervation of GP neurons. Morphologically, the axon varicosity density in the GP reached 47% and 78% of that in the STN for M1 and M2 projections, respectively. Pallidostriatal neurons were more frequently innervated by the cortex than pallidosubthalamic neurons, and they often expressed FoxP2, indicating that they were arkypallidal neurons (see Figure 7 for the schematic). To date, the fast excitation observed in the GP following cortical stimulation has been considered disynaptic excitation via the STN (Nambu et al., 2000). This does not fully contradict the present findings, since in a larger population of GP neurons, GPSTN (or prototypic neurons), only 33% (from M1) and 52% (from M2) received motor cortical inputs. In addition, it is possible that extracellular unit recordings are biased towards neurons with relatively high firing frequencies, which include GPSTN neurons (Figure 5B, Table 2). A question that remains is whether cortico-pallidal projections exist in primates including humans, as has been suggested (Milardi et al., 2015; Smith and Wichmann, 2015).

Figure 7. Diagram of cortico-basal ganglia-thalamus circuitry.

Figure 7.

The dotted lines represent the relatively weak innervation reported in this study for cortex to GPSTN, and for GPCPu to dMSNs (Glajch et al., 2016). The numbers in circles indicate estimated conduction times in milliseconds (ms) (Jaeger and Kita, 2011). (A) M1 activation induces excitation in the striatum, STN, and GPCPu. Due to the electrophysiological properties of the neurons, STN and GP may be activated faster than striatal neurons. (B) M2 activation conveys additional excitation to the GPSTN neurons. The possible information flow related to the present study is indicated by red lines. Note that inhibition from the GP is here considered faster (1 ms) than excitation from the STN (2–3 ms) (Jaeger and Kita, 2011). The actual timing of spike activity depends on neuron type, and excitation/inhibition interactions are complex.

Table 2. The maximum number of cortical boutons (/100 × 100 μm2) in the basal ganglia nuclei.

Cortical area GP Striatum STN
M1 (N = 4 rats) 267, 200, 289, 141 911, 1300, 1464, 657, 506, 432, 528, 408
M2 (N = 4 rats) 317, 737, 513, 92 1300, 2021, 1829, 398 420, 717, 637, 177

Methodological consideration

In the present study, we employed tracer or viral vector injections to label cortico-pallidal projections in both morphological and electrophysiological experiments. Injections can extend to nearby areas, and some tracers can be transmitted both anterogradely and retrogradely, which results in the contamination of multiple projection pathways. This was however not the case in this study. First, as we show in Figures 2 and 3, cortical tracer injections into the motor area were restricted to single areas. Second, we also examined well characterized cortico-striatal and cortico-subthalamic projections (preliminary data can be found in Karube et al., 2019), and we found that the distributions of M1, M2, LO, and Cg axons differed in the striatum and STN, in line with earlier reports (Afsharpour, 1985; Alloway et al., 2010; Brown et al., 1998; Ebrahimi et al., 1992; Hoover and Vertes, 2011; Janssen et al., 2017; Kincaid and Wilson, 1996; Kolomiets et al., 2001; Mailly et al., 2013; Reep and Corwin, 1999; Reep et al., 2008). Third, we occasionally found some retrogradely labeled neurons in remote cortical areas, mainly in the temporal/insular area and posterior parietal area after injection into the motor areas. However, in approximately three-fourths of sections, no retrogradely labeled neuron was found. In the remaining sections, only less than eight pyramidal neurons per section were sparsely scattered. Thus, their contribution to axonal projections is likely to be negligible.

Another technical limitation that needs to be mentioned is the difficulty to precisely compare oEPSC amplitudes among experiments. As shown in Figure 5D, oEPSC amplitudes in the striatum and STN also varied among neurons. We cannot exclude the possibility that amplitude differences between GPSTN and GPCPu or between M1-GPCPu and M2-GPCPu may be caused by inconsistencies in AAV infections, even though there were significant differences. We think that the potential variance in oEPSC amplitudes that stems from differences in the efficiency of viral vector infections is evened out by the large number of GP neurons examined, but further research will be needed to come to a definite conclusion.

Possible effects of cortical innervation in GP neurons

Cortico-pallidal projections were topographically organized; M1 and M2 axons were densely distributed in the CB(-) central part of the GP (Figures 1 and 2, Figure 1—figure supplements 1 and 2). Cg axons projected to the GP in a similar manner, but dense in the more medial GP. In contrast, LO provided fewer pallidal collaterals (Figure 2—figure supplements 1 and 2). Thus, each frontal cortical area likely has its own regions of interest in the GP. Our findings may explain the heterogeneous activity of GP neurons during movement (Arkadir et al., 2004; DeLong, 1971; Dodson et al., 2015; Goldberg and Bergman, 2011; Mink and Thach, 1991a; Mink and Thach, 1991b; Turner and Anderson, 1997). The firing phases of prototypic neurons (GPSTN) and arkypallidal neurons corresponding to FoxP2(+) GPCPu differ with regard to cortical activity (Abdi et al., 2015; Mallet et al., 2012; Mallet et al., 2016). This can be due to biased cortical innervation of the GPCPu (Figure 5C, D, E, F, Figure 6C, E). In addition, the above reports revealed that the firing phase was heterogeneous in prototypic neurons, too. We show here that one-third to half of all prototypic neurons received cortical inputs, which can contribute to their heterogeneity. The axon terminal density of cortico-pallidal projections was much sparser than that of cortico-striatal projections, but reached 50–80% of that of cortico-subthalamic projections. Considering that even in the connection between the STN and GP, which is one of the main synaptic connections in the indirect pathway, only 1–2% of all GP neurons converge onto a single STN neuron and vice versa (Baufreton et al., 2009; Goldberg and Bergman, 2011), sparse connections can still functionally work. Moreover, even if excitatory synaptic inputs did not reach action potential thresholds, the timing of subsequent action potentials can be altered (Ermentrout, 1996; Schultheiss et al., 2010). In addition, the expression of sodium channel characteristics of GP neurons can boost excitatory synaptic inputs (Edgerton et al., 2010; Hanson et al., 2004). Thus, the relatively sparse cortical innervation to the GPSTN neurons could nevertheless affect a small but specific component of the basal ganglia circuitry.

Cortical innervation could affect the synchronization of GP neurons. During spontaneous firing, even most neighboring GP neuron pairs do not exhibit real correlation (Bar-Gad et al., 2003; Goldberg and Bergman, 2011; Stanford, 2003), even for pause periods (Elias et al., 2007). However, it has been reported that excitatory inputs may synchronize GP neurons effectively, as GP neurons tend to align with STN inputs rather than with striatal inhibitory inputs (Goldberg et al., 2003). Monosynaptic cortico-pallidal inputs, which are also excitatory, probably precede the inputs from the STN and can contribute to synchronization of GP neurons, which would strengthen STN-GP coupling (Bevan et al., 2002a; Bevan et al., 2002b; Parent and Hazrati, 1995b), thereby affecting the oscillatory phase and synchronization of the basal ganglia.

Potential roles of the cortico-pallidal pathway in the cortex-basal ganglia-thalamus loop

We could not identify the origin of cortical neurons that provide axons in the GP. Pyramidal tract (PT) type pyramidal neurons are potential candidates, because some collaterals were emitted from thick axon trunks passing the GP (Figure 1B). The most recent finding in mice suggested that layers 5 and 6 cortical neurons project to the GP (Abecassis et al., 2019), although it is not clear whether they project only to the GP. PT neurons innervate multiple brain regions including the striatum and STN in rodents, however, their axon collaterals in the GP have not been reported yet (Kita and Kita, 2012; Shepherd, 2013; Shibata et al., 2018). The combinations of multiple cortical innervation onto the basal ganglia nuclei must influence information flow. Thus, it needs to be clarified whether the cortical neurons innervating the GP also innervate other basal ganglia nuclei.

To speculate potential effect of cortico-pallidal projections, we follow the related neural circuitries in the basal ganglia. Once M1 and/or M2 are activated, membrane properties of STN and GP neurons will contribute to earlier and more efficient excitation than those of striatal neurons (Jaeger and Kita, 2011; Figure 7). Since GPCPu neurons are activated by direct M1 inputs, they may increase the firing more significantly than GPSTN neurons and be more in phase with cortical excitation. With regard to synaptic targets of GPCPu neurons, Mallet et al. (2012) reported that both MSNs and interneurons in the striatum are innervated by arkypallidal neurons. In mice, Npas1(+) neurons, which overlap with Lhx6(+) and FoxP2(+) GPCPu neurons, synapse onto iMSNs and dMSNs. The average inhibitory postsynaptic current amplitude evoked by stimulation of axon terminals of Npas1(+) neurons is significantly larger in iMSNs than dMSNs (Glajch et al., 2016). Therefore, the inhibition of MSNs by GPCPu could counteract corticostriatal excitation, and weaken iMSN activity. Because iMSNs are likely to inhibit all GP neuron types equally (Hernández et al., 2015), decreased iMSN activity may reduce the inhibition of GP neurons, thereby augmenting the inhibition of STN, EP, and SNr. As the operation of this trisynaptic circuit should be slower than monosynaptic cortico-STN excitation (Figure 7), the M1 cortico-pallidal pathway may act as a delayed terminator of the hyperdirect pathway. GPCPu can also inhibit dMSNs, which in turn disinhibit the EP/SNr.

If iMSN activity is suppressed by the GPCPu, what controls or suppresses GPCPu excitation? The most parsimonious explanation is that decreased cortico-pallidal activity can weaken GP excitation. In addition, it is noteworthy that striatal neurons receive contralateral cortical excitation as well as ipsilateral input (Wilson, 1986; Wilson, 1987), whereas the GP and STN receive only ipsilateral excitation (Figure 1D). Thus, it could be possible that the accumulating cortico-striatal excitation evoked by both hemispheres may overcome the pallido-striatal inhibition evoked by ipsilateral cortico-pallidal and cortico-STN excitation. If that was the case, then, the iMSNs can suppress GP neurons. It is also possible that axon collaterals of dMSNs, which would receive weaker inhibition from GP than iMSNs, inhibit the GP (Fujiyama et al., 2011; Kawaguchi et al., 1990; Lévesque et al., 2003; Wu et al., 2000). In addition, pallidostriatal inhibition of striatal interneurons can possibly disinhibit MSN activity. Finally, via local axons of GP neurons (Fujiyama et al., 2016; Mallet et al., 2012), mutual inhibition (Bugaysen et al., 2013; Mastro et al., 2017; Sadek et al., 2007) may work to terminate transient excitation of the GP.

In the case of M2 inputs to the GP, the GPSTN pathway can also be activated, as well as the GPCPu pathway described above (Figure 7B). M2-GPSTN circuitry may be more sensitive to timing, because the GP and STN form bidirectional connections, and the cortico-pallidal pathway can act as fast as the hyperdirect pathway. These two pathways may compete: if GPSTN is activated first, it will suppress STN; if STN is activated first, GP will be excited directly by the cortex and via the STN, in turn inhibiting the STN. Taken together, it is implied that the net effect of M2-GPSTN pathway activity is likely to be suppression of the STN. However, basal ganglia activity may be strongly affected by competition between M2-pallidal and hyperdirect pathways. Because the hyperdirect pathway contributes to the cessation of ongoing actions, the cortico-pallidal pathway may cancel this cessation signal. Mallet et al. (2016) proposed a two-step model for cancellation via cooperation between the STN and GP, especially arkypallidal neurons. Our current findings shed light on the linking of complex stop/cancel sequences. In addition, GPSTN neurons innervate SNc dopaminergic neurons in a cell type specific manner (Mastro et al., 2014; Oh et al., 2017). Based on our results indicating that M2 but not M1 projects to the dorsolateral striosomes (Figure 1—figure supplement 3) and on the fact that striosomal dMSNs innervate the SNc (Fujiyama et al., 2011; Gerfen, 1985), M2-driven pathways may modulate dopamine signaling via the striatum and GPSTN. Recently, Magno et al. (2019) revealed that M2 activation can relieve the motor dysfunction of Parkinson’s disease in mice. The neural circuitry proposed here is likely to contribute to such an effect.

The findings of recent in vivo experiments in rodents have revealed functions of M2. M2 contributes to the preparatory function (Li et al., 2015; Svoboda and Li, 2018), as observed in primates (Wise, 1985), or increases its influence on other cortical areas during the preparatory phase (Makino et al., 2016; Makino et al., 2017). GP neurons are also suggested to compute action selection (Bogacz et al., 2016; Goldberg and Bergman, 2011), which likely relates to the preparation of movement, and the M2-pallidal pathway may therefore contribute. It has been also suggested that M2 is involved in the integration of movement with sensory or internal information (Barthas and Kwan, 2017; Saiki et al., 2014), including posture coding (Mimica et al., 2018). It is possible that M2-GP innervation reflects sensory signal information and contributes to the fine tuning of ongoing movement such as the adaptation of chosen behaviors. Along with possibly involving the dopamine system, the M2-GP pathway may relate more to the plastic and integrated phases of movement, which may require sensitive control using two GP cell types. However, functional differentiation between M1 and M2 must be more complex, since concurrent and similar activities of M1 and M2 neurons have been reported, at least for certain movements (Saiki et al., 2014; Soma et al., 2017). Parallel connections between M1 and M2 also affect the aforementioned neural circuitry (Ueta et al., 2013; Ueta et al., 2014). To conclude, the functional relevance and importance of cortico-pallidal projections from M1/M2 remain to be resolved in detail.

Materials and methods

Animal experiments were approved and performed in accordance with the guidelines for the care and use of laboratory animals established by the Committee for Animal Care and Use (Permit Number: A16008, A17001, A18001, A19036) and the Committee for Recombinant DNA Study (Permit Number: D16008, D17001, D18001, D19036) of Doshisha University. All efforts were made to minimize animal suffering and the number of animals used.

Animal surgery for injection of neural tracers and viral vectors

Wistar SLC rats (Japan SLC Inc, Hamamatsu, Japan; N = 52 of both sexes for electrophysiological experiments and N = 16 male rats for morphological experiments) were anesthetized with intramuscular injection of a mixture of ketamine (Ketalar; Daiichi-Sankyo, Tokyo, Japan; 40 mg/kg) and xylazine (Bayer HealthCare, Tokyo, Japan; 4 mg/kg). A small amount (0.05 mL) of the mixture was additionally injected every 15 min during any prolonged surgery (>1 hr). Body temperature was monitored and controlled at 37°C with the aid of a heating device (World Precision Instruments [WPI], Sarasota, FL, USA). Craniotomy was performed with a drill at an appropriate position on the skull based on the rat brain atlas (Paxinos and Watson, 2007). A glass pipette (tip diameter, 30–60 µm) was used for all injections. For anterograde tracers, biotinylated dextran amine (BDA, 10 kDa; 10% solution dissolved in PB; Thermo Fisher Scientific, Waltham, MA, USA) was injected using either a brief air pulse (10–20 psi for 40–100 ms) controlled with a coordinated valve system (PV 820, WPI) or using electrophoresis (0.5–2 μA of positive current, 7 s ON/7 s OFF for 80 cycles) with a current controller (WPI). PHA-L (Vector Laboratories, Burlingame, CA, USA; 2.5% dissolved in 10 mM Na2HPO4, pH 8.0) was injected using electrophoresis (1–5 μA positive current, 7 s ON/7 s OFF for 80 cycles). AAV vectors encoding fluorophores were injected using air pressure (1.5 × 1010 vg [vector genomes]/µL, AAVdj-hSyn-hChR2(H134R)-mCherry; the plasmid a gift from Karl Deisseroth; Addgene plasmid #26976; http://n2t.net/addgene:26976; RRID: Addgene_26976). For retrograde tracers, cholera toxin subunit B (CTB) conjugated with Alexa Fluor 488 or 555 (Thermo Fisher Scientific) and fluorophore-labeled beads (Green Retrobeads IX, LumaFluor Inc, Durham, NC, USA; FluoSphere Orange 0.04 µm, Thermo Fisher Scientific) were injected using air pressure. For labeling cortical neurons, injections were performed at two to three depths typically at 400 μm-intervals. The stereotaxic coordinates (Paxinos and Watson, 2007) of injections in the centers of each cortical area were as follows (the actual injection locations were normalized based on skull size using the distance between bregma and lambda): for rostral M1, 2.0 mm rostral from bregma (A/P r2.0) and 2.5 mm lateral from the midline (M/L 2.5), depth 0.5–1.2 mm from cortical surface; for caudal M1: A/P 0.0, M/L 2.6, depth 0.5–1.2; for M2: A/P r4.2, M/L 1.9, depth 0.5–1.2 at a 30° angle rostral from vertical; for lateral orbitofrontal area (LO): A/P r3.4, M/L 2.9, depth 4.0. Because the Cg is located at the medial surface of the frontal cortex and is enclosed by M2, contamination of M2 following vertical injections into the Cg would have been unavoidable. Therefore, for injections into the Cg, the contralateral medial frontal area was removed using an aspiration needle and vacuum pump to expose the medial surface of the frontal cortex of the targeted hemisphere. An injection electrode was then inserted into the Cg at an angle of 45° (A/P r1.8, M/L 0.5) and the contents of the electrode ejected into the Cg at two depths (0.5 and 1.0) using air pressure. Tracer injections into the striatum were performed to efficiently label GP neurons projecting to the dorsal striatum, pressure injection was applied in three tracks (two depths for each track) at A/P r2.0, M/L 2.5, depth 3.8; A/P r1.0, M/L 3.5, depth 4.2; and A/P 0.0, M/L 3.7, depth 4.0. After injections, the skin was sutured and 2.5 mg/kg of butorphanol (Vetorphale, Meiji Seika Pharma, Tokyo, Japan) was subcutaneously injected as an analgesic. The animals were allowed to recover before further experimentation. We allowed 2–4 d of survival time for BDA or retrograde tracers, 5–8 d for PHA-L, and 2–4 weeks for AAV. The age of the rats used for the morphological experiments ranged from 7 to 12 weeks.

Immunohistochemistry

After the survival period, rats were deeply anesthetized with an intraperitoneal injection of sodium pentobarbital (100 mg/kg; Kyoritsu Seiyaku Corporation, Tokyo, Japan) and perfused with pre-fixative (sucrose 8.5% w/v, MgCl5 mM; dissolved in 20 mM PB) followed by fixative (2 or 4% paraformaldehyde and 0.2% picric acid with or without 0.05% glutaraldehyde in 0.1 M PB, pH 7.4) through the cardiac artery. The brains were post-fixed in situ for 2–3 hr, and then removed and washed with PB several times. Sagittal or coronal sections 50 µm thick were cut using a vibratome (Leica VT1000, Leica Instruments, Wetzlar, Germany) or freezing microtome (Leica SM 2000R), and stored in PB containing 0.02% NaN3 until further use.

For immunoreaction, sections were incubated with primary antibody diluted in incubation buffer consisting of 10% normal goat serum, 2% bovine serum albumin, and 0.5% Triton X in 0.05 M Tris buffered saline (TBS) overnight at room temperature (RT) or for 2–3 d at 4°C. For immunofluorescence, after rinsing with TBS three times, the sections were incubated with the secondary antibodies conjugated with fluorophores for 3 hr at RT. After three rinses, the sections were dried on glass slides and coverslipped with antifade mounting medium (ProLong Gold, Vector, Burlingame, CA). For brightfield specimens, the sections were incubated with a biotinylated secondary antibody followed by rinses and then reacted with ABC solution (1:200 dilution; Vector Elite) for 3 hr at RT, and visualized with 3,3′-diaminobenzidine (DAB) and/or Ni-DAB. The sections were dehydrated with a graded series of ethanol, delipidated with xylene, and finally embedded with M·X (Matsunami Glass Ind., Ltd., Osaka, Japan). The primary and secondary antibodies used in this study are listed in Supplementary file 1 Tables 1 and 2.

Image acquisition

Photomicrographs of brightfield specimens were captured using a CCD camera (DP-73, Olympus, Tokyo, JAPAN) equipped with a BX-53 microscope (Olympus) using 4× (numerical aperture [N.A.] 0.13), 10× (N.A. 0.3), 40× (N.A. 0.75), 60× (N.A. 0.9) or 100× (N.A. 1.4; oil-immersion) objectives. Photomicrographs were analyzed using Fiji (a distribution of Image J) (Schindelin et al., 2012), and Adobe photoshop (Adobe Systems Incorporated, Sam Jose, CA, USA). The brightness of digitized images was adjusted using the adjust-level function of these applications. To obtain a multifocus image, images were captured with 1 μm steps and processed with the ‘extended depth of focus’ Fiji plugin. Fluorescent images were captured using an Orca Spark CMOS camera (Hamamatsu photonics, Hamamatsu, Japan) or a DP-73 camera equipped with a BX-53 microscope. To quantify the molecular expression patterns of GP neuron types, immunofluorescent images were acquired using a confocal microscope (FV1200, Olympus) with 40× (N.A. 0.95) or 100× (N.A. 1.35; silicon oil immersion) objectives.

In vitro slice recordings

Basal ganglia neurons were recorded using in vitro on cell and whole cell patch clamp. Rats of both sexes (N = 52; postnatal 30–65 d) were deeply anesthetized with isoflurane and perfused with 25 mL of ice-cold modified artificial cerebrospinal fluid (ACSF; N-methyl-D-glucamine, 93; KCl, 2.5; NaH2PO4, 1.2; NaHCO3, 30; HEPES, 20; glucose, 25; sodium ascorbate, 5; thiourea, 2; sodium pyruvate, 3; MgCl2, 10; and CaCl2, 0.5; all in mM; pH was adjusted to 7.3 with HCl). All ACSFs were continuously aerated with 95/5% O2/CO2. Brains were removed and immersed in ice-cold modified ACSF for 2 min. Coronal slices 300 μm thick were cut using a vibratome (7000smz-2, Campden, Leicestershire, UK) and incubated with modified ACSF at 32°C for 15 min. The slices were transferred to normal ACSF (NaCl, 125; KCl, 2.5; CaCl2, 2.4; MgCl2, 1.2; NaHCO3, 25; glucose, 15; NaH2PO4, 1.25; pyruvic acid, 2; lactic acid, 4; all in mM) at RT. After 1 hr of recovery, slices were moved into a recording chamber thermostatted at 30°C. A whole-cell glass pipette of 4–6 MΩ was filled with intracellular solution (K-gluconate, 130; KCl, 2; Na2ATP, 3; NaGTP, 0.3; MgCl2, 2; Na4EGTA, 0.6; HEPES, 10; biocytin, 20.1; all in mM). The pH was adjusted to 7.3 with KOH, and the osmolality was adjusted to ~290 mOsm. Target brain regions were identified with the aid of a fluorescence microscope (BX-51WI, Olympus) using a × 40 water-immersed objective lens. Voltage and current clamp recordings were low-pass filtered at 10 kHz and recorded using EPC10 (HEKA Elektronik Dr. Schulze GmbH, Lambrecht/Pfalz, Germany) with a sampling rate of 20 kHz. Spontaneous firings were recorded in an on-cell mode for at least 2 min, then neurons were recorded in a whole-cell mode. The series resistance was examined by applying a brief voltage pulse of −10 mV for 10 ms and was confirmed to be less than 25 MΩ during recording. Shortly (less than 1 min) after achieving whole cell configuration, the firing responses to 1 s depolarizing current pulses (maximum intensity was 1000 pA, increasing in 50 pA steps) were recorded in current clamp mode. Passive membrane properties were monitored as responses to 1 s hyperpolarizing current pulses.

For photoactivation of ChR2, a 470 nm light-emitting diode (LED; BLS-LCS-0470-50-22, Mightex Systems, Pleasanton, CA, USA) was used at full field illumination through a 40 × water immersion objective. Five-millisecond blue light pulses were applied at a maximum total power of ~4 mW, at which neuronal responses were saturated. One photo stimulation sequence was composed of 10 light pulses at 10 Hz, and repeated for 10 to 15 times with an interval of 1 s. In some experiments, low concentrations of TTX, 1 μM, and 4-amino pyridine (1 mM) were added to the ACSF to isolate monosynaptic currents (Petreanu et al., 2009; Shu et al., 2007). CNQX (10 μM) and AP5 (20 μM) were applied to inhibit glutamatergic synaptic currents (N = 10), and SR95531 (gabazine; 20 μM) was applied (N = 10) to prevent GABAA receptor-mediated synaptic currents. All pharmacological reagents were purchased from Tocris Bioscience (Bristol, UK).

Slices were then fixed with a mixture of 4% paraformaldehyde, 0.05% glutaraldehyde, and 0.2% picric acid in 0.1 M PB overnight at 4°C. Fixed slices were rinsed with PB (3 × 10 min). In some cases, they were then re-sectioned into 50 μm-thick sections. The sections were incubated with 1% H2O2 in 0.05 M TBS for 30 min at RT to deplete endogenous peroxidases, then rinsed with TBS three times. The sections were incubated with CF350-conjugated streptavidin for 2 hr (1:3000; Biotium, Inc, Fremont, CA) for fluorescent investigation of biocytin-filled neurons. For immunoreaction against FoxP2, the slices were treated using the CUBIC protocol for tissue clearing (Susaki et al., 2015). Briefly, the slices were incubated with 15% sucrose in PB for 3 hr, followed by 30% sucrose in PB for 3 hr for cryoprotection. After two rounds of freezing and thawing with dry ice, the slices were cleared with CUBIC-1 solution overnight, composed of 25% urea, 25% N,N,N',N'-tetrakis-(2-hydroxypropyl)-ethylenediamine and 15% polyethylene glycol mono-p-isooctylphenyl ether in DW. After washing with PBS containing 0.3% Triton X (PBS-X), the slices were incubated with CF350 conjugated streptavidin overnight. After washing, the slices were incubated overnight with CUBIC-2 solution (10% 2,2',2''-nitrilotriethanol, 50% sucrose, and 25% urea in DW). The cleared sections were investigated using confocal microscopy with a long working distance lens to capture the image of biocytin-filled neurons and its retrobeads labeling. Once recorded neurons were identified, the slices were washed with PBS-X and incubated with anti-FoxP2 antibody overnight. Due to the limited number of fluorescent channels, FoxP2-immunoreaction was detected using the same wavelength as fluorescence of one of the retrobeads. We confirmed that the fluorescent signals of retrobeads could be easily distinguished from those of FoxP2, with FoxP2 localized in the nucleus and retrobeads concentrated in the cytosol, and the signal size larger for FoxP2 than for retrobeads (Figure 4E). Usually, fluorescence of retrobeads is more or less diminished at the time of observation of FoxP2 immunofluorescence (Figure 4E). For brightfield microscopy, the sections were incubated with ABC (1:200) overnight at 4°C. Biocytin-filled neurons were visualized for light microscopy with Ni-DAB using H2O2 at a final concentration of 0.01%. The sections were dried on a glass slide and coverslipped with EcoMount (Biocare Medical, LLC, Concord, CA) or Mount-Quick (Daido Sangyo, Toda, Japan).

Data analysis

Quantification of cortico-pallidal axon density

Many thick cortical axon bundles with strong fluorescence passed through the GP, which largely disrupted proportional relationships between fluorescence intensity and axon varicosity (or bouton) density. Therefore, we manually counted axonal varicosities in each ROI using brightfield microscopic images. ROI size was 233 × 173 μm2, which was enough to sample the region with densest axon terminals (Figure 1—figure supplements 1 and 2). Because cortical boutons were observed in the striatum and STN of the same animal, these were also counted, and the resultant bouton density was compared with that of the GP to decrease the potential effect of tracer injection variability.

Analysis of electrophysiological data

Our analysis method has been previously described (Mizutani et al., 2017; Oh et al., 2017). Slice recording data were analyzed using Igor Pro 7 (Wave metrics Inc, Portland, OR) with the aid of the Neuromatic plugin (http://www.neuromatic.thinkrandom.com) (Rothman and Silver, 2018) and custom-built procedures. The input resistance was determined via the linear fitting of voltage responses to hyperpolarized current pulses (−20 to −100 pA with 20 pA steps). The membrane time constant was calculated from the voltage response to a −50 pA current pulse. To identify oEPSCs, each trace was smoothed with a 0.2 ms moving time window (moving average of 4 consecutive recording points), and all traces were aligned with the onset of each photo stimulation to calculate the average trace. The baseline was defined as the mean current trace over a 50 ms period prior to each photo stimulation. the amplitude of the inward current with a stable delay after the onset of photo stimulation was measured from the baseline to the peak of the current. The inward current was identified as an oEPSC if the peak amplitude was 3-fold larger than the standard deviation of the baseline. The rising phase of the oEPSC was fitted linearly and extrapolated and the intersection point with the baseline was determined. The time of this intersection point was defined as the onset of the oEPSC. Latency was measured as the time from the onset of each photo stimulation to that of the oEPSC. Neurons in the striatum, basal nucleus of Meynert, and GP possessed distinct firing properties and were readily distinguished. In the GP, GABAergic projection neurons and putative cholinergic neurons were distinguishable by cell morphology and firing properties. Putative cholinergic neurons were excluded from the present data. The locations of recorded cells were visualized with biocytin and manually plotted on a digital image.

Statistical comparisons

Averaged data are provided as mean ± standard deviation unless otherwise noted. Data comparison among more than two groups was performed using one-way ANOVA followed by post hoc Tukey tests, using R software (http://www.r-project.org/; R Project for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria). For small samples, the Kruskal-Wallis test was applied instead of ANOVA. Data comparison between two groups was performed using the Wilcoxon rank sum test. For comparison of proportion values, Fisher’s exact test was used. To examine whether the mean of normalized values was significantly different from 1, a signed rank test for the mean was applied. For comparison of cumulative histograms, the Kolmogorov-Smirnov test was applied. Differences in data values were considered significant if p<0.05. Significant differences are indicated using asterisks (*, p<0.05; **, p<0.01; ***, p<0.001). All p-values are reported.

Acknowledgements

The authors thank Dr. Yasuharu Hirai for helpful discussion and comments.

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Contributor Information

Fuyuki Karube, Email: fkarube@mail.doshisha.ac.jp.

Fumino Fujiyama, Email: ffujiyam@mail.doshisha.ac.jp.

Naoshige Uchida, Harvard University, United States.

Floris P de Lange, Radboud University, Netherlands.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grants:

  • Japan Society for the Promotion of Science Grant-in-Aid for Scientific Research (C) 26350983 to Fuyuki Karube.

  • Japan Society for the Promotion of Science Grant-in-Aid for Scientific Research on Innovative Areas 16H01622 to Fuyuki Karube.

  • Japan Society for the Promotion of Science Grant-in-Aid for Scientific Research on Innovative Areas 16H06543 to Susumu Takahashi.

  • Japan Society for the Promotion of Science Grant-in-Aid for Scientific Research (B) 16H02840 to Susumu Takahashi.

  • Japan Society for the Promotion of Science Grant-in-Aid for Scientific Research (B) 16H03299 to Fumino Fujiyama.

  • Japan Society for the Promotion of Science Grant-in-Aid for Challenging Exploratory Research 15K12770 to Fumino Fujiyama.

  • Japan Society for the Promotion of Science Scientific Researches on Innovative Areas 26112006 to Fumino Fujiyama.

  • Japan Society for the Promotion of Science Grant-in-Aid for Scientific Research (A) 19H01131 to Susumu Takahashi.

Additional information

Competing interests

No competing interests declared.

Author contributions

Conceptualization, Resources, Data curation, Software, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Validation, Investigation, Visualization, Methodology, Writing—original draft, Project administration, Writing—review and editing.

Resources, Funding acquisition, Writing—original draft.

Resources.

Resources, Funding acquisition, Writing—original draft.

Ethics

Animal experimentation: Animal experiments were approved and performed in accordance with the guidelines for the care and use of laboratory animals established by the Committee for Animal Care (Permit Number: A16008, A17001, A18001, A19036) and Use and the Committee for Recombinant DNA Study (Permit Number: D16008, D17001, D18001, D19036) of Doshisha University. All efforts were made to minimize animal suffering and the number of animals used.

Additional files

Supplementary file 1. Primary and secondary antibodies used in this study.
elife-49511-supp1.docx (19.6KB, docx)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.49511.024
Transparent reporting form
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.49511.025

Data availability

All data generated or analysed during this study are included in the manuscript and supporting files. Source data files in a Microsoft Excel format are provided for Table 2, for Figures 2C, 2D, 2E, 4A2, 4B2, 4B3, 4B4, 4C, 5B, 5C, 5D, 5F, 6A, 6B, 6C, 6D, and also for Figure 1-Figure supplement 2E, and Figure 2-Figure supplement 1D.

References

  1. Abdi A, Mallet N, Mohamed FY, Sharott A, Dodson PD, Nakamura KC, Suri S, Avery SV, Larvin JT, Garas FN, Garas SN, Vinciati F, Morin S, Bezard E, Baufreton J, Magill PJ. Prototypic and arkypallidal neurons in the dopamine-intact external globus pallidus. Journal of Neuroscience. 2015;35:6667–6688. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4662-14.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Abecassis A, Berceau B, Win J, Garcia D, Xenias H, Cui Q, Pamucku A, Hernández V, Chon U, Lim B, Kim Y, Justice N, Awatramani R, Chan S. Npas1+-Nkx2.1+ neurons form a unique pallidal neuron subclass. bioRxiv. 2019 doi: 10.1101/644674. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  3. Afsharpour S. Topographical projections of the cerebral cortex to the subthalamic nucleus. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 1985;236:14–28. doi: 10.1002/cne.902360103. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Albin RL, Young AB, Penney JB. The functional anatomy of basal ganglia disorders. Trends in Neurosciences. 1989;12:366–375. doi: 10.1016/0166-2236(89)90074-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Alexander GE, DeLong MR, Strick PL. Parallel organization of functionally segregated circuits linking basal ganglia and cortex. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 1986;9:357–381. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.09.030186.002041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Alloway KD, Smith JB, Beauchemin KJ. Quantitative analysis of the bilateral brainstem projections from the whisker and forepaw regions in rat primary motor cortex. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 2010;518:4546–4566. doi: 10.1002/cne.22477. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Arkadir D, Morris G, Vaadia E, Bergman H. Independent coding of movement direction and reward prediction by single pallidal neurons. Journal of Neuroscience. 2004;24:10047–10056. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2583-04.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bar-Gad I, Heimer G, Ritov Y, Bergman H. Functional correlations between neighboring neurons in the primate globus pallidus are weak or nonexistent. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2003;23:4012–4016. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-10-04012.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Barthas F, Kwan AC. Secondary motor cortex: where 'Sensory' Meets 'Motor' in the Rodent Frontal Cortex. Trends in Neurosciences. 2017;40:181–193. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2016.11.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Baufreton J, Kirkham E, Atherton JF, Menard A, Magill PJ, Bolam JP, Bevan MD. Sparse but selective and potent synaptic transmission from the globus pallidus to the subthalamic nucleus. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2009;102:532–545. doi: 10.1152/jn.00305.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Bengtson CP, Osborne PB. Electrophysiological properties of cholinergic and noncholinergic neurons in the ventral pallidal region of the nucleus basalis in rat brain slices. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2000;83:2649–2660. doi: 10.1152/jn.2000.83.5.2649. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Bevan MD, Magill PJ, Hallworth NE, Bolam JP, Wilson CJ. Regulation of the timing and pattern of action potential generation in rat subthalamic neurons in vitro by GABA-A IPSPs. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2002a;87:1348–1362. doi: 10.1152/jn.00582.2001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Bevan MD, Magill PJ, Terman D, Bolam JP, Wilson CJ. Move to the rhythm: oscillations in the subthalamic nucleus-external globus pallidus network. Trends in Neurosciences. 2002b;25:525–531. doi: 10.1016/S0166-2236(02)02235-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Bogacz R, Martin Moraud E, Abdi A, Magill PJ, Baufreton J. Properties of neurons in external globus pallidus can support optimal action selection. PLOS Computational Biology. 2016;12:e1005004. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1005004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Bolam JP, Hanley JJ, Booth PA, Bevan MD. Synaptic organisation of the basal ganglia. Journal of Anatomy. 2000;196:527–542. doi: 10.1046/j.1469-7580.2000.19640527.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Brown LL, Smith DM, Goldbloom LM. Organizing principles of cortical integration in the rat neostriatum: corticostriate map of the body surface is an ordered lattice of curved laminae and radial points. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 1998;392:468–488. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1096-9861(19980323)392:4&#x0003c;468::AID-CNE5&#x0003e;3.0.CO;2-Z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Bugaysen J, Bar-Gad I, Korngreen A. Continuous modulation of action potential firing by a unitary GABAergic connection in the globus pallidus in vitro. Journal of Neuroscience. 2013;33:12805–12809. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1970-13.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Calabresi P, Picconi B, Tozzi A, Ghiglieri V, Di Filippo M. Direct and indirect pathways of basal ganglia: a critical reappraisal. Nature Neuroscience. 2014;17:1022–1030. doi: 10.1038/nn.3743. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Cui G, Jun SB, Jin X, Pham MD, Vogel SS, Lovinger DM, Costa RM. Concurrent activation of striatal direct and indirect pathways during action initiation. Nature. 2013;494:238–242. doi: 10.1038/nature11846. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. DeLong MR. Activity of pallidal neurons during movement. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1971;34:414–427. doi: 10.1152/jn.1971.34.3.414. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. DeLong MR. Primate models of movement disorders of basal ganglia origin. Trends in Neurosciences. 1990;13:281–285. doi: 10.1016/0166-2236(90)90110-V. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Dodson PD, Larvin JT, Duffell JM, Garas FN, Doig NM, Kessaris N, Duguid IC, Bogacz R, Butt SJ, Magill PJ. Distinct developmental origins manifest in the specialized encoding of movement by adult neurons of the external globus pallidus. Neuron. 2015;86:501–513. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.03.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Ebrahimi A, Pochet R, Roger M. Topographical organization of the projections from physiologically identified Areas of the motor cortex to the striatum in the rat. Neuroscience Research. 1992;14:39–60. doi: 10.1016/S0168-0102(05)80005-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Edgerton JR, Hanson JE, Günay C, Jaeger D. Dendritic sodium channels regulate network integration in globus pallidus neurons: a modeling study. Journal of Neuroscience. 2010;30:15146–15159. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2662-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Elias S, Joshua M, Goldberg JA, Heimer G, Arkadir D, Morris G, Bergman H. Statistical properties of pauses of the high-frequency discharge neurons in the external segment of the globus pallidus. Journal of Neuroscience. 2007;27:2525–2538. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4156-06.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Ermentrout B. Type I membranes, phase resetting curves, and synchrony. Neural Computation. 1996;8:979–1001. doi: 10.1162/neco.1996.8.5.979. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Frank MJ, Samanta J, Moustafa AA, Sherman SJ. Hold your horses: impulsivity, deep brain stimulation, and medication in parkinsonism. Science. 2007;318:1309–1312. doi: 10.1126/science.1146157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Friend DM, Kravitz AV. Working together: basal ganglia pathways in action selection. Trends in Neurosciences. 2014;37:301–303. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2014.04.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Fujiyama F, Sohn J, Nakano T, Furuta T, Nakamura KC, Matsuda W, Kaneko T. Exclusive and common targets of neostriatofugal projections of rat striosome neurons: a single neuron-tracing study using a viral vector. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2011;33:668–677. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07564.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Fujiyama F, Nakano T, Matsuda W, Furuta T, Udagawa J, Kaneko T. A single-neuron tracing study of arkypallidal and prototypic neurons in healthy rats. Brain Structure and Function. 2016;221:4733–4740. doi: 10.1007/s00429-015-1152-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Gerfen CR. The neostriatal mosaic. I. compartmental organization of projections from the striatum to the Substantia nigra in the rat. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 1985;236:454–476. doi: 10.1002/cne.902360404. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Glajch KE, Kelver DA, Hegeman DJ, Cui Q, Xenias HS, Augustine EC, Hernández VM, Verma N, Huang TY, Luo M, Justice NJ, Chan CS. Npas1+ pallidal neurons target striatal projection neurons. Journal of Neuroscience. 2016;36:5472–5488. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1720-15.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Goldberg JA, Kats SS, Jaeger D. Globus pallidus discharge is coincident with striatal activity during global slow wave activity in the rat. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2003;23:10058–10063. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-31-10058.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Goldberg JA, Bergman H. Computational physiology of the neural networks of the primate globus pallidus: function and dysfunction. Neuroscience. 2011;198:171–192. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2011.08.068. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Gradinaru V, Mogri M, Thompson KR, Henderson JM, Deisseroth K. Optical deconstruction of parkinsonian neural circuitry. Science. 2009;324:354–359. doi: 10.1126/science.1167093. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Hanson JE, Smith Y, Jaeger D. Sodium channels and dendritic spike initiation at excitatory synapses in globus pallidus neurons. Journal of Neuroscience. 2004;24:329–340. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3937-03.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Hegeman DJ, Hong ES, Hernández VM, Chan CS. The external globus pallidus: progress and perspectives. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2016;43:1239–1265. doi: 10.1111/ejn.13196. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Hernández VM, Hegeman DJ, Cui Q, Kelver DA, Fiske MP, Glajch KE, Pitt JE, Huang TY, Justice NJ, Chan CS. Parvalbumin+ neurons and Npas1+ neurons are distinct neuron classes in the mouse external globus pallidus. Journal of Neuroscience. 2015;35:11830–11847. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4672-14.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Hikosaka O. GABAergic output of the basal ganglia. Progress in Brain Research. 2007;160:209–226. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(06)60012-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Hoover WB, Vertes RP. Projections of the medial orbital and ventral orbital cortex in the rat. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 2011;519:3766–3801. doi: 10.1002/cne.22733. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Hunt AJ, Dasgupta R, Rajamanickam S, Jiang Z, Beierlein M, Chan CS, Justice NJ. Paraventricular hypothalamic and amygdalar CRF neurons synapse in the external globus pallidus. Brain Structure and Function. 2018;223:2685–2698. doi: 10.1007/s00429-018-1652-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Isoda M, Hikosaka O. Role for subthalamic nucleus neurons in switching from automatic to controlled eye movement. Journal of Neuroscience. 2008;28:7209–7218. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0487-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Isomura Y, Takekawa T, Harukuni R, Handa T, Aizawa H, Takada M, Fukai T. Reward-modulated motor information in identified striatum neurons. Journal of Neuroscience. 2013;33:10209–10220. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0381-13.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Jaeger D, Kita H. Functional connectivity and integrative properties of globus pallidus neurons. Neuroscience. 2011;198:44–53. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2011.07.050. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Janssen MLF, Temel Y, Delaville C, Zwartjes DGM, Heida T, De Deurwaerdère P, Visser-Vandewalle V, Benazzouz A. Cortico-subthalamic inputs from the motor, Limbic, and associative Areas in normal and dopamine-depleted rats are not fully segregated. Brain Structure and Function. 2017;222:2473–2485. doi: 10.1007/s00429-016-1351-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Karube F, Takahashi S, Fujiyama F. Motor cortex can directly drive the globus pallidal neurons in a projection neuron type dependent manner in rat. bioRxiv. 2019 doi: 10.1101/677377. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  47. Karube F, Fujiyama F. Morphological and electrophysiological evaluation of cortico-pallidal pathway in rodent. Journal of the Neurological Sciences. 2017;381:494–495. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2017.08.3601. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  48. Kawaguchi Y, Wilson CJ, Emson PC. Projection subtypes of rat neostriatal matrix cells revealed by intracellular injection of biocytin. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1990;10:3421–3438. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.10-10-03421.1990. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Kincaid AE, Wilson CJ. Corticostriatal innervation of the patch and matrix in the rat neostriatum. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 1996;374:578–592. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1096-9861(19961028)374:4&#x0003c;578::AID-CNE7&#x0003e;3.0.CO;2-Z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Kita H, Kita T. Number, origins, and chemical types of rat pallidostriatal projection neurons. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 2001;437:438–448. doi: 10.1002/cne.1294. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Kita T, Kita H. The subthalamic nucleus is one of multiple innervation sites for long-range corticofugal axons: a single-axon tracing study in the rat. Journal of Neuroscience. 2012;32:5990–5999. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5717-11.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Kolomiets BP, Deniau JM, Mailly P, Ménétrey A, Glowinski J, Thierry AM. Segregation and convergence of information flow through the cortico-subthalamic pathways. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2001;21:5764–5772. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-15-05764.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Lévesque M, Bédard A, Cossette M, Parent A. Novel aspects of the chemical anatomy of the striatum and its efferents projections. Journal of Chemical Neuroanatomy. 2003;26:271–281. doi: 10.1016/j.jchemneu.2003.07.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Li N, Chen TW, Guo ZV, Gerfen CR, Svoboda K. A motor cortex circuit for motor planning and movement. Nature. 2015;519:51–56. doi: 10.1038/nature14178. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Magno LAV, Tenza-Ferrer H, Collodetti M, Aguiar MFG, Rodrigues APC, da Silva RS, Silva JDP, Nicolau NF, Rosa DVF, Birbrair A, Miranda DM, Romano-Silva MA. Optogenetic stimulation of the M2 cortex reverts motor dysfunction in a mouse model of Parkinson's Disease. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2019;39:3234–3248. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2277-18.2019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Mailly P, Aliane V, Groenewegen HJ, Haber SN, Deniau JM. The rat prefrontostriatal system analyzed in 3D: evidence for multiple interacting functional units. Journal of Neuroscience. 2013;33:5718–5727. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5248-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Makino H, Hwang EJ, Hedrick NG, Komiyama T. Circuit mechanisms of sensorimotor learning. Neuron. 2016;92:705–721. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.10.029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Makino H, Ren C, Liu H, Kim AN, Kondapaneni N, Liu X, Kuzum D, Komiyama T. Transformation of Cortex-wide emergent properties during motor learning. Neuron. 2017;94:880–890. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.04.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Mallet N, Micklem BR, Henny P, Brown MT, Williams C, Bolam JP, Nakamura KC, Magill PJ. Dichotomous organization of the external globus pallidus. Neuron. 2012;74:1075–1086. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.04.027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Mallet N, Schmidt R, Leventhal D, Chen F, Amer N, Boraud T, Berke JD. Arkypallidal cells send a stop signal to striatum. Neuron. 2016;89:308–316. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.12.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Mastro KJ, Bouchard RS, Holt HA, Gittis AH. Transgenic mouse lines subdivide external segment of the globus pallidus (GPe) neurons and reveal distinct GPe output pathways. Journal of Neuroscience. 2014;34:2087–2099. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4646-13.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Mastro KJ, Zitelli KT, Willard AM, Leblanc KH, Kravitz AV, Gittis AH. Cell-specific pallidal intervention induces long-lasting motor recovery in dopamine-depleted mice. Nature Neuroscience. 2017;20:815–823. doi: 10.1038/nn.4559. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Middleton FA, Strick PL. Basal ganglia and cerebellar loops: motor and cognitive circuits. Brain Research Reviews. 2000;31:236–250. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0173(99)00040-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Middleton FA, Strick PL. Basal-ganglia 'projections' to the prefrontal cortex of the primate. Cerebral Cortex. 2002;12:926–935. doi: 10.1093/cercor/12.9.926. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Milardi D, Gaeta M, Marino S, Arrigo A, Vaccarino G, Mormina E, Rizzo G, Milazzo C, Finocchio G, Baglieri A, Anastasi G, Quartarone A. Basal ganglia network by constrained spherical deconvolution: a possible cortico‐pallidal pathway? Movement Disorders. 2015;30:342–349. doi: 10.1002/mds.25995. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Mimica B, Dunn BA, Tombaz T, Bojja V, Whitlock JR. Efficient cortical coding of 3D posture in freely behaving rats. Science. 2018;362:584–589. doi: 10.1126/science.aau2013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Mink JW, Thach WT. Basal ganglia motor control. I. nonexclusive relation of pallidal discharge to five movement modes. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1991a;65:273–300. doi: 10.1152/jn.1991.65.2.273. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Mink JW, Thach WT. Basal ganglia motor control. II. late pallidal timing relative to movement onset and inconsistent pallidal coding of movement parameters. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1991b;65:301–329. doi: 10.1152/jn.1991.65.2.301. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Mizutani K, Takahashi S, Okamoto S, Karube F, Fujiyama F. Substance P effects exclusively on prototypic neurons in mouse globus pallidus. Brain Structure and Function. 2017;222:4089–4110. doi: 10.1007/s00429-017-1453-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Naito A, Kita H. The cortico-pallidal projection in the rat: an anterograde tracing study with biotinylated dextran amine. Brain Research. 1994;653:251–257. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(94)90397-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Nambu A, Tokuno H, Hamada I, Kita H, Imanishi M, Akazawa T, Ikeuchi Y, Hasegawa N. Excitatory cortical inputs to pallidal neurons via the subthalamic nucleus in the monkey. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2000;84:289–300. doi: 10.1152/jn.2000.84.1.289. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Nambu A, Tokuno H, Takada M. Functional significance of the cortico-subthalamo-pallidal 'hyperdirect' pathway. Neuroscience Research. 2002;43:111–117. doi: 10.1016/S0168-0102(02)00027-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Nambu A. Globus pallidus internal segment. Progress in Brain Research. 2007;160:135–150. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(06)60008-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Nambu A. Seven problems on the basal ganglia. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2008;18:595–604. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2008.11.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Oh YM, Karube F, Takahashi S, Kobayashi K, Takada M, Uchigashima M, Watanabe M, Nishizawa K, Kobayashi K, Fujiyama F. Using a novel PV-Cre rat model to characterize pallidonigral cells and their terminations. Brain Structure & Function. 2017;222:2359–2378. doi: 10.1007/s00429-016-1346-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Parent A, Hazrati LN. Functional anatomy of the basal ganglia. I. the cortico-basal ganglia-thalamo-cortical loop. Brain Research Reviews. 1995a;20:91–127. doi: 10.1016/0165-0173(94)00007-C. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Parent A, Hazrati LN. Functional anatomy of the basal ganglia. II. the place of subthalamic nucleus and external pallidum in basal ganglia circuitry. Brain Research Reviews. 1995b;20:128–154. doi: 10.1016/0165-0173(94)00008-D. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Paxinos G, Watson C. The Rat Brain in Stereotaxic Coordinates. San Diego: Academic Press; 2007. [Google Scholar]
  79. Petreanu L, Mao T, Sternson SM, Svoboda K. The subcellular organization of neocortical excitatory connections. Nature. 2009;457:1142–1145. doi: 10.1038/nature07709. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Redgrave P, Rodriguez M, Smith Y, Rodriguez-Oroz MC, Lehericy S, Bergman H, Agid Y, DeLong MR, Obeso JA. Goal-directed and habitual control in the basal ganglia: implications for parkinson's disease. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2010;11:760–772. doi: 10.1038/nrn2915. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Reep RL, Wu JH, Cheatwood JL, Corwin JV, Kartje GL, Mir A. Quantification of synaptic density in corticostriatal projections from rat medial agranular cortex. Brain Research. 2008;1233:27–34. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2008.07.059. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Reep RL, Corwin JV. Topographic organization of the striatal and thalamic connections of rat medial agranular cortex. Brain Research. 1999;841:43–52. doi: 10.1016/S0006-8993(99)01779-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Rothman JS, Silver RA. NeuroMatic: an integrated Open-Source software toolkit for acquisition, analysis and simulation of electrophysiological data. Frontiers in Neuroinformatics. 2018;12:14. doi: 10.3389/fninf.2018.00014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Sadek AR, Magill PJ, Bolam JP. A single-cell analysis of intrinsic connectivity in the rat globus pallidus. Journal of Neuroscience. 2007;27:6352–6362. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0953-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Saiki A, Kimura R, Samura T, Fujiwara-Tsukamoto Y, Sakai Y, Isomura Y. Different modulation of common motor information in rat primary and secondary motor cortices. PLOS ONE. 2014;9:e98662. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0098662. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Schindelin J, Arganda-Carreras I, Frise E, Kaynig V, Longair M, Pietzsch T, Preibisch S, Rueden C, Saalfeld S, Schmid B, Tinevez JY, White DJ, Hartenstein V, Eliceiri K, Tomancak P, Cardona A. Fiji: an open-source platform for biological-image analysis. Nature Methods. 2012;9:676–682. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.2019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Schmidt R, Leventhal DK, Mallet N, Chen F, Berke JD. Canceling actions involves a race between basal ganglia pathways. Nature Neuroscience. 2013;16:1118–1124. doi: 10.1038/nn.3456. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Schultheiss NW, Edgerton JR, Jaeger D. Phase response curve analysis of a full morphological globus pallidus neuron model reveals distinct perisomatic and dendritic modes of synaptic integration. Journal of Neuroscience. 2010;30:2767–2782. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3959-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Shepherd GM. Corticostriatal connectivity and its role in disease. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2013;14:278–291. doi: 10.1038/nrn3469. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Shibata KI, Tanaka T, Hioki H, Furuta T. Projection patterns of corticofugal neurons associated with vibrissa movement. Eneuro. 2018;5:ENEURO.0190-18.2018. doi: 10.1523/ENEURO.0190-18.2018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Shu Y, Yu Y, Yang J, McCormick DA. Selective control of cortical axonal spikes by a slowly inactivating K+ current. PNAS. 2007;104:11453–11458. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0702041104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Smith Y, Bevan MD, Shink E, Bolam JP. Microcircuitry of the direct and indirect pathways of the basal ganglia. Neuroscience. 1998;86:353–387. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(98)00004-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Smith Y, Wichmann T. The cortico-pallidal projection: an additional route for cortical regulation of the basal ganglia circuitry. Movement Disorders. 2015;30:293–295. doi: 10.1002/mds.26095. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Soma S, Saiki A, Yoshida J, Ríos A, Kawabata M, Sakai Y, Isomura Y. Distinct laterality in Forelimb-Movement representations of rat primary and secondary motor cortical neurons with intratelencephalic and pyramidal tract projections. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2017;37:10904–10916. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1188-17.2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Stanford IM. Independent neuronal oscillators of the rat globus pallidus. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2003;89:1713–1717. doi: 10.1152/jn.00864.2002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Susaki EA, Tainaka K, Perrin D, Yukinaga H, Kuno A, Ueda HR. Advanced CUBIC protocols for whole-brain and whole-body clearing and imaging. Nature Protocols. 2015;10:1709–1727. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2015.085. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Svoboda K, Li N. Neural mechanisms of movement planning: motor cortex and beyond. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2018;49:33–41. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2017.10.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Turner RS, Anderson ME. Pallidal discharge related to the kinematics of reaching movements in two dimensions. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1997;77:1051–1074. doi: 10.1152/jn.1997.77.3.1051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Ueta Y, Hirai Y, Otsuka T, Kawaguchi Y. Direction- and distance-dependent interareal connectivity of pyramidal cell subpopulations in the rat frontal cortex. Frontiers in Neural Circuits. 2013;7:164. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2013.00164. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Ueta Y, Otsuka T, Morishima M, Ushimaru M, Kawaguchi Y. Multiple layer 5 pyramidal cell subtypes relay cortical feedback from secondary to primary motor Areas in rats. Cerebral Cortex. 2014;24:2362–2376. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht088. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Vicente AM, Galvão-Ferreira P, Tecuapetla F, Costa RM. Direct and indirect dorsolateral striatum pathways reinforce different action strategies. Current Biology. 2016;26:R267–R269. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2016.02.036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Wei W, Wang XJ. Inhibitory control in the Cortico-Basal Ganglia-Thalamocortical loop: complex regulation and interplay with memory and decision processes. Neuron. 2016;92:1093–1105. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.10.031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Wichmann T, DeLong MR. Functional and pathophysiological models of the basal ganglia. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 1996;6:751–758. doi: 10.1016/S0959-4388(96)80024-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Wilson CJ. Postsynaptic potentials evoked in spiny neostriatal projection neurons by stimulation of ipsilateral and contralateral neocortex. Brain Research. 1986;367:201–213. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(86)91593-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Wilson CJ. Morphology and synaptic connections of crossed corticostriatal neurons in the rat. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 1987;263:567–580. doi: 10.1002/cne.902630408. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Wise SP. The primate premotor cortex: past, present, and preparatory. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 1985;8:1–19. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.08.030185.000245. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Wu Y, Richard S, Parent A. The organization of the striatal output system: a single-cell juxtacellular labeling study in the rat. Neuroscience Research. 2000;38:49–62. doi: 10.1016/S0168-0102(00)00140-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision letter

Editor: Naoshige Uchida1

In the interests of transparency, eLife publishes the most substantive revision requests and the accompanying author responses.

Acceptance summary:

This study found a significant direct projection from the cortex to the globus pallidus (GP), an overlooked input from the cortex to the basal ganglia circuit. This projection is quantitatively comparable to the cortical projection to the subthalamic nucleus (hyperdirect pathway). The authors have also found that this projection targets GP in a cell type specific manner. These findings prompt a revision of a commonly held view that the striatum and subthalamic nucleus are the predominant input structures of the cortico-basal ganglia circuit.

Decision letter after peer review:

Thank you for submitting your article "Motor cortex can directly drive the globus pallidal neurons in a projection neuron type dependent manner in rat" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by three peer reviewers, and the evaluation has been overseen by a Reviewing Editor and Floris de Lange as the Senior Editor. The reviewers have opted to remain anonymous.

The reviewers have discussed the reviews with one another and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this decision to help you prepare a revised submission.

Summary:

The striatum has been often regarded as the sole input structure of the basal ganglia. This study shows that the premotor and motor cortices (M1 and M2, respectively) directly project to the globus pallidus. Although a previous study (Naito and Kita, 1994) reported this connection, this observation has been largely neglected in the field. The present study re-examines this connection using anterograde anatomical tracing and channelrhodopsin-2-assisted circuit mapping in slice, and concludes that the cortico-pallidal projection is as strong as the hyperdirect pathway (cortico-subthalamic projection).

All the reviewers thought that this is a potentially very important finding. However, the reviewers raised substantive concerns regarding the clarity of the manuscript and some technical issues. As you will see below, the original comments by the reviewers somewhat varied in terms of the points raised and enthusiasm. After discussions, however, we were able to narrow the essential points that the authors should address. We believe that the authors should be able to address these essential points with the current data although the reviewers pointed out various experiments that can improve the quality of the work. Although these additional experiments are optional following eLife's policy, please consider these experiments as much as you can or provide use with your responses.

Essential revisions:

1) The reviewers thought that the current manuscript is difficult to read because it contains many results that are not directly related to the main story and that are, at least in part, reported in previous studies (reviewer 1, the point starting with "Finally, the paper jumps around quite a bit" and reviewer 3, point #1).

a) Significance and novelty of Figure 5 were not very clear. Please explain these more clearly.

b) Please remove Figure 6 (M1, M2 projections to the striatum), Figure 7 (distribution of STN-projecting cortical neurons), and Figure 8 (spatial distribution of cortico-STN axons).

2) The authors conclude that "the cortical excitation in the GP was as strong as that in the STN" but the evidence is not very convincing.

a) Please add a density analysis using wider regions of interest (ROIs) that encompass all of the GP and STN (reviewer 1, the point starting with "First, with the regard to").

b) Please interpret the electrophysiology results comparing inputs from M1 and M2 more quantitatively (reviewer 3, point 3).

c) (optional) For comparison, please perform electrophysiological characterization of cortico-STN connection (reviewer 1, the point starting with "The experiments in Figure 2 are meant to show").

3) The authors speculate that pyramidal tract (PT) neurons appear to be the source of input to GP neurons. It is important to know whether this is true or whether it is a different (new) population.

a) Retrograde labeling might clarify which neurons in the cortex are projecting to the GP (reviewer 1, the point starting with "There is a missing, obvious anatomical experiment"). During discussion, the reviewers thought that knowing this would increase the significance of this work. However, it is also pointed out that retrograde tracing may not be very straightforward with a conventional tracer due to passing fibers etc. and may require a different method such as rabies virus-mediated tracing. Due to these complexities, we thought that retrograde tracing could be optional. However, if you choose to do so, please reduce the emphasis on PT and discuss this point more carefully.

4) The authors suggest that "arkypallidal neurons, which do not send axons to the STN, could be a principle target of cortico-pallidal innervation". Reviewer 3 raised this issue but during discussion, it was discussed that FoxP2 is a nuclear protein and it might be difficult to detect in the proposed experiment. Thus, an EM investigation could be combined with retrograde labeling from the striatum (CPU). Although we thought this could be useful, it was also pointed out that the results do not advance beyond what the authors have done with slice electrophysiology. Please explain your view on this in the rebuttal letter.

The details of the above issues in the original review comments can be found below. Please use the essential points above when reading the original comments because the tone and the suggested experiments and analyses have been changed in some points after our discussions.

Reviewer #1:

The goal of this paper is characterize the direct projection between the cortex and the globus pallidus, and to place that projection within the broader context of cortico-basal ganglia circuitry. The authors conclude that the cortico-pallidal projection, at least from areas M1 and M2 in the rat, is just as strong as the hyperdirect pathway (cortico-STN projection). To my knowledge, no lab has replicated the important work of Naito and Kita demonstrating the cortico-pallidal projection, so this is a significant claim. My impression has been that this projection is largely ignored in studies of the basal ganglia, perhaps because people believe that it is too sparse to be very significant. Therefore, if it is, in fact, a projection equal to the one to the STN, the paper would be quite significant for our understanding of cortico-basal ganglia circuitry.

However, with what the authors are showing, I just don't think the case is strong enough yet. I think substantial revisions could help, but this involves collecting a moderate amount of additional data, and may be beyond the scope of eLife's revisions policy.

First, with the regard to the data shown in Figure 1, I'm not sure that counting axon varicosity density within a small ROI at the point of densest projection is the best way to assess impact. These are small enough structures, I think the authors do need to count across the entire GP and STN (sampling stereologically, so it's not as great a burden). Isn't it possible that the projection to the STN is much wider-spread, so the overall impact is greater, but at its densest point, it will not have more varicosities? Also, it looks as though the "densest ROI" was chosen before counting, so couldn't this have been conflated across regions by passing axons, since the eye could not distinguish? What if varicosities are actually denser elsewhere? Finally, experimenter bias, however unconscious, may be a concern here based on how the ROI was selected.

Figure 1E1 is also the basis for the authors to conclude that the cortico-pallidal projection is just as strong as the cortico-STN projection. But with a p-value of 0.16, and only 3 subjects per injection, I have to wonder whether this analysis was drastically underpowered.

There is a missing, obvious anatomical experiment here that could substantiate their claims, and clarify the circuit The authors claim that these are likely collaterals off of PT neurons. They refer to the pallidal projections as collaterals throughout. I think this makes sense, and seems like the most logical circuit, but there aren't actually data to show that this is the case. They should do some retrograde injections in the GP to show the layer these cells are coming from, and to understand the density. They could compare to density with STN projections. Then some double injections in the GP and striatum to show that cortico-pallidal projections are also cortico-striatal projections. For all we know, at this point, this could be an entirely new population of projecting cells in M1/M2, not PT neurons at all!

The experiments in Figure 2 are meant to show that the cortico-pallidal pathway is a functional connection. But it seems like there's a big missing piece of the puzzle, and that's what that figure looks like for the cortico-STN projection. If the metrics look similar for the hyperdirect and cortico-pallidal pathways, it greatly bolsters the authors' argument. Hyperdirect experiments would also act as an internal control: I have some concern that the addition of the ChR2 is inflating the strength of the connection. And how should we judge the measures of strength shown? Is this strong? Is it weak?

Finally, the paper jumps around quite a bit, and there are several experiments, namely those from Figures 6-9, as well as half of Figure 5, that just don't seem to belong here. They don't relate the central point of the cortico-pallidal circuit, and just provide minor replications of prior work. I recommend the authors remove them entirely, and perhaps start building a second paper about circuit topographies. The current paper should instead be filled out with the revisions described above.

- The experiments shown in Figure 5A-B seem out of place. The authors are showing the proportion of GP-str and GP-stn cells that also label with PV, Lhx6, and FoxP2, but there is no reference to whether these GP cells are receiving direct cortical input. It doesn't fit with the other experiments.

- The experiments shown in Figure 6 also do not belong. They simply replicate abundant prior work showing the relationship between M1/M2 projections and striosome/matrix (Donoghue and Herkenham, 1986; Gerfen, 1989; Ebrahimi et al., 1992; and so on).

- Similarly, Figure 7, which demonstrates the topography of the hyperdirect pathway, is not strongly related to the goals of the paper. And the experiments have been done before (Janssen et al., 2010; Canteras et al., 1990; Kitai and Denieu, 1981; and so on). Instead, the figures showing the LO-pallidal and CG-pallidal projections should be pulled out of the supplement and put in the main text.

- Same with Figures 8 and 9.

Reviewer #2:

The manuscript submitted by Karube et al., characterized the cortical innervation principally coming from the primary (M1), the secondary (M2) motor cortex to the basal ganglia, in particular, the globus pallidus (GP) and the subthalamic nucleus (STN). The authors also looked at the projection from the orbitofrontal (OF), and the cingulate cortex (Cg) to the GP, the STN, and the striatum. The cortico-pallidal projection has been known for some time but poorly characterized. Here the authors show that the cortical inputs principally excite the striatum-projecting GP neurons (GP-CPU) over the STN-projecting neurons (GP-STN). They also demonstrate that the GP-CPU neurons can be further subdivided into FoxP2+ (i.e. the arkypallidal neurons) and Lhx6+ cells (that represent the main population of GP neurons, the so-called prototypic cells) and that both these populations receive functional inputs from the cortex. The authors also characterize some interesting topographical organization between the different cortical areas and the GP or the STN. Overall this is a nice and interesting story that add novel findings highlighting the anatomical and functional complexity of these basal ganglia circuits. These findings also challenge the classic view that cortical inputs are exclusively integrated by the striatum and the subthalamic nucleus in the basal ganglia. Indeed, here the authors show that the density of varicosity in STN is similar to the one in GP.

I found that the only small/moderate limitation of the work is that the origin and cellular identity of this cortico-pallidal inputs could have been better characterized. Indeed, the authors mention (subsection “Motor cortex innervates the GP”, second paragraph) that the GP projections are exclusively ipsilateral implying that the cortical neurons providing these inputs are pyramidal tract (PT) neurons. However, previous work that has investigated the projection sites of PT neurons have not seen any collaterals to GP (Kita and Kita, 2012). What could explain these differences? It would have been nice to clarify this issue in the paper. Interestingly, two population of PT neurons have been identified based on their projection site (i.e. thalamus or medulla projecting, see Economo et al., 2018 DIO: 10.1038/s41586-018-0642-9) and it is possible that this PT neuronal heterogeneity is at the origin of a separate pathway to the GP.

Reviewer #3:

This is an interesting paper that addresses an important issue that can have a significant impact on our current view of information processing through the basal ganglia circuits in normal and diseased states. The main finding of this study is evidence for the existence of a direct monosynaptic cortico-pallidal projection that innervates preferentially arkypallidal neurons in the rat globus pallidus (GP). Because the GP is not recognized as a direct target of cortical afferents in current schemes of the basal ganglia circuitry, the existence of this connection will generate significant interest in the field and may open up new critical thoughts about substrates of basal ganglia function and dysfunction. However, the manuscript in its present form suffers of major organizational problems and need additional data to support more strongly the authors' conclusions. Some of the substantive concerns that must be addressed include:

1) In its present form, the paper is very hard to read and the main message (i.e. existence of the cortico-pallidal inputs to arkypallidal neurons) is lost into a series of other anatomical data that have some value, but not for this paper. I suggest that the authors significantly streamline their paper to make it focused solely on the cortico-pallidal projection.

2) The evidence for a monosynaptic M1/M2 inputs to arkypallidal neurons should be supported by electron microscopic studies of synaptic connections between M1/M2 terminals and arkypallidal neurons. The addition of solid EM data to this study would strengthen significantly the authors' conclusions.

3) The results of optogenetic studies shown in Figure 2 and 4 are hard to interpret because the amplitude of EPSC evoked in the different populations of GP neurons and the number of responding neurons to M1 or M2 terminals is highly dependent on the number of cortical terminals that express ChR2 in each area of light stimulation. Because the number of transfected terminals in the GP is variable between animals due to inherent technical issues related to the characteristics of the injection sites, extent of uptake of the virus by cortical neurons and efficiency of anterograde transport along cortico-pallidal axons, quantitative data that compare M1- and M2-mediated effects on GP neurons are difficult to interpret. A more rigorous analysis that takes into consideration these technical issues must be done to validate these data.

eLife. 2019 Nov 12;8:e49511. doi: 10.7554/eLife.49511.028

Author response


Essential revisions:

1) The reviewers thought that the current manuscript is difficult to read because it contains many results that are not directly related to the main story and that are, at least in part, reported in previous studies (reviewer 1, the point starting with "Finally, the paper jumps around quite a bit" and reviewer 3, point #1).

a) Significance and novelty of Figure 5 were not very clear. Please explain these more clearly.

This point deeply relates to the point 4. Please refer below. We modified the corresponding text to explain the point (subsection “M1 and M2 more frequently innervate GP neurons projecting to the striatum than those projecting to STN”).

b) Please remove Figure 6 (M1, M2 projections to the striatum), Figure 7 (distribution of STN-projecting cortical neurons), and Figure 8 (spatial distribution of cortico-STN axons).

We omitted the corresponding figures and texts. One of the reasons why we had added those figures is confirmation of our injection quality. As the reviewer 1 commented, our data for cortico-striatal and cortico-STN projection patterns are similar with previous qualitative investigations. We appreciated if you keep it in mind (since the reviewer unfairly suspected our morphological data on GP, irrespective of his acceptance of cortico-striatal and cortico-STN data derived from the same samples as cortico-GP data are obtained). Anyway, they were gone in the revision.

2) The authors conclude that "the cortical excitation in the GP was as strong as that in the STN" but the evidence is not very convincing.

a) Please add a density analysis using wider regions of interest (ROIs) that encompass all of the GP and STN (reviewer 1, the point starting with "First, with the regard to").

As reviewer 1 pointed out, the location of ROI was not clearly shown, which lead difficulty for understanding. Thus, we added the images with low magnification to represent ROI location and topographical distribution of cortical axons in the STN and GP as shown in Figure 1—figure supplement 1 and 2, which can support the reasonability of the ROI size, 233 µm x 173 µm. In addition, we counted another sample for both M1 and M2 projections (now N = 4 in the revision; Figure 2C, D, E). Also, our presentation in the previous Figure 1—figure supplement 3 maybe not adequate. We counted additional sections along with the M/L dimension, and modified the figure as shown in Figure 2C, D.

We argued against the scientific importance of counting boutons in the entire area of the GP and STN. First, as mentioned in Materials and methods and Figure 1—figure supplement 1 and 2, the ROI size is enough large to sample the densest cortical projection field in the GP and especially in the STN, since, as we showed, those projections are highly topographic. Extending ROI further means counting the bouton number in less innervated regions, and we considered it is not important. Second, bouton count is highly time consuming, because the size of boutons is close to light microscope resolution which prevents reliable segmentation by any algorithms (hopefully AI will help in the near future), and bouton count has to be done completely manually.

b) Please interpret the electrophysiology results comparing inputs from M1 and M2 more quantitatively (reviewer 3, point 3).

This is an actually critical point. We added “methodological consideration” in Discussion, and state that comparing the oEPSC amplitude is difficult and not fully conclusive (subsection “Cortico-GP terminals elicit monosynaptic EPSCs”, last paragraph; subsection “M1 and M2 more frequently innervate GP neurons projecting to the striatum than those projecting to STN, last paragraph and subsection “Methodological consideration”, last paragraph).

c) (optional) For comparison, please perform electrophysiological characterization of cortico-STN connection (reviewer 1, the point starting with "The experiments in Figure 2 are meant to show").

We add comparison of oEPSC amplitude between STN and GP neurons derived from the same animal, the data are represented in Figure 4C and subsection “Cortico-GP terminals elicit monosynaptic EPSCs”, last paragraph. By the signed rank test for the mean, normalized oEPSC amplitude in GP neurons are not significantly different from 1, namely, the mean amplitude of STN neurons. We had already showed comparison of oEPSC time course in the previous Figure 3. However, as shown in the previous Figure 4 (Figure 5 in the revision), oEPSC amplitudes in STN neurons themselves largely varied from cell to cell. Thus, I wonder the editors and other reviewers could accept the above comparison (if not, we will omit it).

3) The authors speculate that pyramidal tract (PT) neurons appear to be the source of input to GP neurons. It is important to know whether this is true or whether it is a different (new) population.

a) Retrograde labeling might clarify which neurons in the cortex are projecting to the GP (reviewer 1, the point starting with "There is a missing, obvious anatomical experiment"). During discussion, the reviewers thought that knowing this would increase the significance of this work. However, it is also pointed out that retrograde tracing may not be very straightforward with a conventional tracer due to passing fibers etc. and may require a different method such as rabies virus-mediated tracing. Due to these complexities, we thought that retrograde tracing could be optional. However, if you choose to do so, please reduce the emphasis on PT and discuss this point more carefully.

This is an important issue, and we admit lack of enough evidences. We added images and drawings of partial reconstruction of cortico-GP projections, which represent axon branches issue from thick main axons (Figure 1B; subsection “Motor cortex innervates the GP”, first paragraph). However, still it is not clear whether single pyramidal cells emit all collaterals in the striatum, GP, and STN. Therefore, as the above comment, we carefully discussed on the point (subsection “Potential roles of the cortico-pallidal pathway in the cortex-basal ganglia-thalamus loop”, second paragraph).

We examined AAV-retro injection into GP and STN, or into GP and pontine nucleus. We will prepare a new paper, in which the data and data removed from the revision will be included.

4) The authors suggest that "arkypallidal neurons, which do not send axons to the STN, could be a principle target of cortico-pallidal innervation". Reviewer 3 raised this issue but during discussion, it was discussed that FoxP2 is a nuclear protein and it might be difficult to detect in the proposed experiment. Thus, an EM investigation could be combined with retrograde labeling from the striatum (CPU). Although we thought this could be useful, it was also pointed out that the results do not advance beyond what the authors have done with slice electrophysiology. Please explain your view on this in the rebuttal letter.

The details of the above issues in the original review comments can be found below. Please use the essential points above when reading the original comments because the tone and the suggested experiments and analyses have been changed in some points after our discussions.

Thanks to the comment, now we successfully detected immunoreaction for FoxP2 in recorded neurons using CUBIC, a tissue clearing method. Thus, we clarified arkypallidal neurons actually receive cortical inputs. We added the images in Figure 6E and modified the text (subsection “M1 and M2 more frequently innervate GP neurons projecting to the striatum than those projecting to STN”, last paragraph).

More difficulty we face is immunoreaction of Lhx6, which is occasionally unstable after slice recording. That is the reason why we did experiments shown in the previous Figure 5 (Figure 6 in the revision), to identify putative Lhx6 neurons without immunoreaction. We modified the corresponding text to explain the point (see the last two paragraphs of the aforementioned subsection).

Reviewer #1:

[…] First, with the regard to the data shown in Figure 1, I'm not sure that counting axon varicosity density within a small ROI at the point of densest projection is the best way to assess impact. These are small enough structures, I think the authors do need to count across the entire GP and STN (sampling stereologically, so it's not as great a burden). Isn't it possible that the projection to the STN is much wider-spread, so the overall impact is greater, but at its densest point, it will not have more varicosities? Also, it looks as though the "densest ROI" was chosen before counting, so couldn't this have been conflated across regions by passing axons, since the eye could not distinguish? What if varicosities are actually denser elsewhere? Finally, experimenter bias, however unconscious, may be a concern here based on how the ROI was selected.

Figure 1E1 is also the basis for the authors to conclude that the cortico-pallidal projection is just as strong as the cortico-STN projection. But with a p-value of 0.16, and only 3 subjects per injection, I have to wonder whether this analysis was drastically underpowered.

We added samples and added the sections for counting, and re-formulate Figure 1, 2, Figure 1—figure supplement 1, 2.

There is a missing, obvious anatomical experiment here that could substantiate their claims, and clarify the circuit The authors claim that these are likely collaterals off of PT neurons. They refer to the pallidal projections as collaterals throughout. I think this makes sense, and seems like the most logical circuit, but there aren't actually data to show that this is the case. They should do some retrograde injections in the GP to show the layer these cells are coming from, and to understand the density. They could compare to density with STN projections. Then some double injections in the GP and striatum to show that cortico-pallidal projections are also cortico-striatal projections. For all we know, at this point, this could be an entirely new population of projecting cells in M1/M2, not PT neurons at all!

We thank the reviewer for this important comment. We had conducted double retrograde tracer injections, but cannot exclude the possibility of labeling from passing fibers in the GP. We carefully described this point in the revision (subsection “Motor cortex innervates the GP”, first paragraph; subsection “Potential roles of the cortico-pallidal pathway in the cortex-basal ganglia-thalamus loop”, second paragraph). We had started double retrograde labeling using AAV-retro and/or rabies viral vectors, which will be a piece of a new paper.

The experiments in Figure 2 are meant to show that the cortico-pallidal pathway is a functional connection. But it seems like there's a big missing piece of the puzzle, and that's what that figure looks like for the cortico-STN projection. If the metrics look similar for the hyperdirect and cortico-pallidal pathways, it greatly bolsters the authors' argument. Hyperdirect experiments would also act as an internal control: I have some concern that the addition of the ChR2 is inflating the strength of the connection. And how should we judge the measures of strength shown? Is this strong? Is it weak?

We had already shown the comparison of cortico-GP and cortico-STN EPSCs in the previous Figure 3, 4 (Figure 4, 5 in the revision, respectively), as the comment, we intended to employ them as internal control, since cortico-striatal and cortico-STN synapses are broadly accepted as functional.

Honestly saying, we cannot understand why “that's what that figure looks like for the cortico-STN projection“, since no critical reason was raised. We added the comparison of EPSC amplitude between GP and STN (Figure 4C and subsection “Cortico-GP terminals elicit monosynaptic EPSCs”, last paragraph). By the signed rank test for the mean, normalized oEPSC amplitude in GP neurons are not significantly different from 1, namely, the mean amplitude of STN neurons.

Finally, the paper jumps around quite a bit, and there are several experiments, namely those from Figures 6-9, as well as half of Figure 5, that just don't seem to belong here. They don't relate the central point of the cortico-pallidal circuit, and just provide minor replications of prior work. I recommend the authors remove them entirely, and perhaps start building a second paper about circuit topographies. The current paper should instead be filled out with the revisions described above.

We had planned to place our findings in the context of whole basal ganglia pathways, also these data could support our morphological analysis quality, since cortico-striatal projections have been widely examined, and we got similar results (although the quantitative data we had shown is not reported so far). However, the comment is reasonable, and we excluded Figure 6-9 from the current manuscript.

- The experiments shown in Figure 5A-B seem out of place. The authors are showing the proportion of GP-str and GP-stn cells that also label with PV, Lhx6, and FoxP2, but there is no reference to whether these GP cells are receiving direct cortical input. It doesn't fit with the other experiments.

We thank the comment, and our description on the point was not enough. We think these data are necessary to study GP neuron types which receives cortical inputs. Since immunohistochemistry for Lhx6 after in vitro slice recordings were unstable, we cannot directly examine that GP neurons receiving cortical inputs frequently express either Lhx6 or FoxP2. Therefore, we showed first double retrograde labeling cells, which project both STN and the striatum, express highly preferentially Lhx6. It means if we targeted bi-directionally projection cells, they are likely to be Lhx6 neurons. Certainly, as the comment, it is not direct evidence, however, we think at least it is scientifically logical. We modified the corresponding description (subsection “M1 and M2 more frequently innervate GP neurons projecting to the striatum than those projecting to STN”, last two paragraphs), and add the new immunofluorescence experiment to detect FoxP2 in the recorded neurons (Figure 6E; subsection “M1 and M2 more frequently innervate GP neurons projecting to the striatum than those projecting to STN”, last paragraph).

- The experiments shown in Figure 6 also do not belong. They simply replicate abundant prior work showing the relationship between M1/M2 projections and striosome/matrix (Donoghue and Herkenham, 1986; Gerfen, 1989; Ebrahimi et al., 1992; and so on).

- Similarly, Figure 7, which demonstrates the topography of the hyperdirect pathway, is not strongly related to the goals of the paper. And the experiments have been done before (Janssen et al., 2010; Canteras et al., 1990; Kitai and Denieu, 1981; and so on). Instead, the figures showing the LO-pallidal and CG-pallidal projections should be pulled out of the supplement and put in the main text.

- Same with Figures 8 and 9

The above papers did not strictly show the differential axonal distribution between M1 and M2, and quantitative comparison is missing. Anyway, we excluded the corresponding description in the revision.

Reviewer #2:

[…] I found that the only small/moderate limitation of the work is that the origin and cellular identity of this cortico-pallidal inputs could have been better characterized. Indeed, the authors mention (subsection “Motor cortex innervates the GP”, second paragraph) that the GP projections are exclusively ipsilateral implying that the cortical neurons providing these inputs are pyramidal tract (PT) neurons. However, previous work that has investigated the projection sites of PT neurons have not seen any collaterals to GP (Kita and Kita, 2012). What could explain these differences? It would have been nice to clarify this issue in the paper. Interestingly, two population of PT neurons have been identified based on their projection site (i.e. thalamus or medulla projecting, see Economo et al., 2018 DIO: 10.1038/s41586-018-0642-9) and it is possible that this PT neuronal heterogeneity is at the origin of a separate pathway to the GP.

We appreciate the critical comment. This is one of the most interesting points, and our current data are not enough. Thus, we carefully stated the neuronal origin of this innervation (subsection “Potential roles of the cortico-pallidal pathway in the cortex-basal ganglia-thalamus loop”, second paragraph). We have started to label them using retrograde viral vectors for the next publication.

Reviewer #3:

[…] The manuscript in its present form suffers of major organizational problems and need additional data to support more strongly the authors' conclusions. Some of the substantive concerns that must be addressed include:

1) In its present form, the paper is very hard to read and the main message (i.e. existence of the cortico-pallidal inputs to arkypallidal neurons) is lost into a series of other anatomical data that have some value, but not for this paper. I suggest that the authors significantly streamline their paper to make it focused solely on the cortico-pallidal projection.

Thanks the comment. We divided the previous version and focused on cortical projection in the revision.

2) The evidence for a monosynaptic M1/M2 inputs to arkypallidal neurons should be supported by electron microscopic studies of synaptic connections between M1/M2 terminals and arkypallidal neurons. The addition of solid EM data to this study would strengthen significantly the authors' conclusions.

This is an actually important point and we can successfully detected FoxP2 immunoreaction after slice recording with the aid of CUBIC method. Eight of 9 examined GPCPu neurons in which EPSC was observed expressed FoxP2. We added the images as Figure 6E, and corresponding texts (subsection “M1 and M2 more frequently innervate GP neurons projecting to the striatum than those projecting to STN”, last paragraph).

3) The results of optogenetic studies shown in Figure 2 and 4 are hard to interpret because the amplitude of EPSC evoked in the different populations of GP neurons and the number of responding neurons to M1 or M2 terminals is highly dependent on the number of cortical terminals that express ChR2 in each area of light stimulation. Because the number of transfected terminals in the GP is variable between animals due to inherent technical issues related to the characteristics of the injection sites, extent of uptake of the virus by cortical neurons and efficiency of anterograde transport along cortico-pallidal axons, quantitative data that compare M1- and M2-mediated effects on GP neurons are difficult to interpret. A more rigorous analysis that takes into consideration these technical issues must be done to validate these data.

The comment is critical and we agree with it, and our description was too careless. We tried to apply some normalization, but we cannot overcome disadvantages. In the revision, we modified the text to emphasize technical limitation, and intend to interpret the results more carefully (subsection “Cortico-GP terminals elicit monosynaptic EPSCs”, last paragraph; subsection “M1 and M2 more frequently innervate GP neurons projecting to the striatum than those projecting to STN, last paragraph and subsection “Methodological consideration”, last paragraph).

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    Figure 2—source data 1. Source data for Figure 2C.
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.49511.009
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.49511.010
    Figure 4—source data 1. Source data for Figure 4A.
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.49511.014
    Figure 4—source data 2. Source data for Figure 4B and C.
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.49511.015
    Figure 5—source data 1. Source data for Figure 5B.

    The file also contained source data for Table 1.

    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.49511.017
    Figure 5—source data 2. Source data for Figures 5C, D, E and F.
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.49511.018
    Figure 6—source data 1. Source data for Figure 6A and B.
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.49511.021
    Figure 6—source data 2. Source data for Figure 6C and D.
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.49511.022
    Supplementary file 1. Primary and secondary antibodies used in this study.
    elife-49511-supp1.docx (19.6KB, docx)
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.49511.024
    Transparent reporting form
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.49511.025

    Data Availability Statement

    All data generated or analysed during this study are included in the manuscript and supporting files. Source data files in a Microsoft Excel format are provided for Table 2, for Figures 2C, 2D, 2E, 4A2, 4B2, 4B3, 4B4, 4C, 5B, 5C, 5D, 5F, 6A, 6B, 6C, 6D, and also for Figure 1-Figure supplement 2E, and Figure 2-Figure supplement 1D.


    Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

    RESOURCES