Abstract
Amyloid‐beta (Aβ) deposition is one of the main hallmarks of Alzheimer's disease. The study assessed the associations between cortical and subcortical 11C‐Pittsburgh Compound B (PiB) retention, namely, in the hippocampus, amygdala, putamen, caudate, pallidum, and thalamus, and subcortical morphology in cognitively normal individuals. We recruited 104 cognitive normal individuals who underwent extensive neuropsychological assessment, PiB–positron emission tomography (PET) scan, and 3‐T magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) acquisition of T1‐weighted images. Global, cortical, and subcortical regional PiB retention values were derived from each scan and subcortical morphology analyses were performed to investigate vertex‐wise local surface and global volumes, including the hippocampal subfields volumes. We found that subcortical regional Aβ was associated with the surface of the hippocampus, thalamus, and pallidum, with changes being due to volume and shape. Hippocampal Aβ was marginally associated with volume of the whole hippocampus as well as with the CA1 subfield, subiculum, and molecular layer. Participants showing higher subcortical Aβ also showed worse cognitive performance and smaller hippocampal volumes. In contrast, global and cortical PiB uptake did not associate with any subcortical metrics. This study shows that subcortical Aβ is associated with subcortical surface morphology in cognitively normal individuals. This study highlights the importance of quantifying subcortical regional PiB retention values in these individuals.
Keywords: Alzheimer's disease, amyloid‐beta, shape, subcortical, volume
1. INTRODUCTION
Amyloid‐beta (Aβ) deposition is one of the main hallmarks of Alzheimer's disease (AD) and may be present for decades when cognitive symptoms are clinically recognized (Braak & Braak, 1997; Villemagne et al., 2013). Aβ binding ligands such as 11C‐Pittsburgh compound B (PiB) allow for in vivo quantification of Aβ in the brain (Klunk et al., 2004; Murray et al., 2015). Several studies have investigated morphological changes in gray matter that are associated with Aβ load, primarily in patients with AD or mild cognitive impairment (MCI) (for a review, see de Flores, La Joie, & Chételat, 2015). However, in order to understand the prodromal mechanisms that may lead to AD, it is important to study these associations in individuals who do not yet present with cognitive impairment; only a few studies have investigated these associations in cognitively normal individuals.
Most studies in this line of research have focused on the volume of the hippocampus and its subfields across disease stages, with the most consistent findings being volume loss in the CA1 subfield and subiculum (de Flores et al., 2015). However, the association between baseline amyloidosis and longitudinal change in hippocampal volume in cognitively normal individuals remains unclear. Several studies have used other morphometric approaches to investigate the surface in the hippocampus (Csernansky et al., 2005; de Flores et al., 2015; Kalin et al., 2017; Tang et al., 2014), which revealed local modifications that did not necessarily translate into changes in the overall size of the structures (Patenaude, Smith, Kennedy, & Jenkinson, 2011). Such approaches have mainly revealed inward surface displacement in regions corresponding to the CA1 subfield and subiculum in AD and MCI patients (de Flores et al., 2015; Kalin et al., 2017) but also surface changes in the amygdala and the lateral ventricles (Tang et al., 2014). In cognitively normal individuals, surface deformation has also been found in association to Aβ burden in the hippocampal head and body and in the thalamus (Achterberg et al., 2014; Carmichael et al., 2012; Csernansky et al., 2005; Schroeder et al., 2016). In comparison to using overall volume values, surface changes were also shown to improve the detection of future cognitive changes (Achterberg et al., 2014; Carmichael et al., 2012; Csernansky et al., 2005).
Studies that investigated the effect of Aβ on subcortical morphology in cognitively healthy individuals differed considerably in their measures of PiB retention values (Villeneuve et al., 2015). Despite some evidence showing that cortical Aβ associates with cognitive decline and hippocampal atrophy in cognitively normal individuals (Andrews et al., 2013; Villemagne et al., 2011), several authors have used approaches based on regional PiB retention by using regions that are more vulnerable to Aβ (Seo et al., 2017; Villeneuve et al., 2015). In this line of thought, regional PiB retention in subcortical structures appears as a promising candidate for improving the prediction of cognitive impairment and cognitive decline (Cho et al., 2018). Moreover, a recent PET staging of amyloidosis showed that high Aβ in the striatum predicted hippocampal atrophy and cognitive impairment better than cortical Aβ (Hanseeuw, Betensky, et al., 2018). In addition, in contrast to cortical PiB retention, only retention in the hippocampus revealed inward and outward surface changes in the CA1 subfield and subiculum regions in cognitively normal individuals (Schroeder et al., 2016). However, the effect of subcortical regional Aβ on subcortical morphology in cognitively normal individuals remains to be understood.
In this study, we investigated the effect of PiB retention in the cortex and in several subcortical structures, namely the hippocampus, amygdala, basal ganglia, and thalamus on the overall volume and the local surface of subcortical structures (including the hippocampal subfields) in cognitively normal individuals. We also investigated whether Aβ‐associated surface changes were primarily due to changes in local volume or shape. We then elaborated a staging model of cerebral Aβ based on cortical and subcortical PiB uptake in order to understand the demographic, cognitive, and morphological characteristics of participants showing higher subcortical Aβ load. We hypothesized that the regional Aβ quantification would reveal more associations with subcortical surface morphology, especially in the hippocampus, than when using cortical PiB retention or global volumes of structures. We also hypothesized that participants with higher subcortical Aβ deposition would show lower performance in episodic memory and smaller hippocampal volumes.
2. MATERIALS AND METHODS
2.1. Subjects
A total of 104 participants aged 65 years and older were recruited through the participant pool of the Centre de recherche de l'Institut universitaire de gériatrie de Montréal (~900 participants) as well as via advertisements and word of mouth. The study protocol included extensive neuropsychological assessment, PiB–PET scan, and MRI data acquisition for every participant. To ensure that all participants included in our study were cognitively normal (i.e., free of dementia and MCI), the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) was used as a screening tool and participants scoring below 23 were excluded; this score is considered the optimal screening threshold for a diagnosis of MCI using the MoCA (Carson, Leach, & Murphy, 2018). Exclusion criteria included untreated diabetes, vascular disease or another health condition that may have had deleterious effects on cognition (including mental health disorders), a history of moderate to severe traumatic brain injury or other neurological disorder as well as general anesthesia in the last 6 months. Participants also had to perform within ±1.5 SD from the mean of age‐matched controls on at least one of two learning tasks: the Logical Memory subtest from the Wechsler Memory Scale‐Third Edition (WMS‐III) and the 2‐minute delay from the DMS‐48. Participants also had to score less than 11 on the Geriatric Depression Scale. Then, a detailed neuropsychological assessment (see below) was performed in every participant to ensure that they were free of dementia and MCI; they also did not express any significant subjective memory complaints at the time of their participation. Research protocols were reviewed and approved by local research ethics boards. All subjects gave their informed consent prior to their participation in the study.
2.2. Cognitive assessment
Every participant underwent an extensive neuropsychological assessment from which seven composite scores were calculated: episodic memory (delayed free recall from the WMS‐III; delayed free recall from the RAVLT), working memory (Digit Span and Arithmetic subtests from the WAIS‐IV), executive functions (number‐letter switching condition of the Trail Making Test—D‐KEFS; sum of perseverative responses from the WCST), language (Vocabulary and Information subtests from the WAIS‐IV; 30‐item BNT; letter and category fluency test), attention (number of omissions from the CPT‐II; concentration performance from the d2 Test of Attention), processing speed (Symbol Search and Coding subtests from the WAIS‐IV; motor speed condition of the Trail Making Test—D‐KEFS), and visuospatial abilities (Block Design and Matrix Reasoning subtests from the WAIS‐IV; Benton Judgment of Line Orientation Test). Raw scores were converted into standardized z scores based on the mean and SD of all participants and averaged together to produce a composite z score for each cognitive domain.
2.3. PET acquisition and processing
PET imaging was conducted at the McConnell Brain Imaging Centre at McGill University on a Siemens/CTI ECAT HR+ scanner in 3D imaging mode (63 parallel planes). The PET scanning session allowed for the acquisition of seven dynamic frames made up of 63 axial slices using a 128 × 128 matrix (voxel dimensions: x = 2.059 mm, y = 2.059 mm, z = 2.425 mm). The participants underwent a 40‐min PET scan (seven frames: 6 × 300 s and 1 × 600 s) 50 min after an intravenous bolus injection of 11C‐PiB.
The postacquisition processing proceeded in two dependent stages: (a) submission of all structural volumes (see next section) to the CIVET pipeline, and (b) subsequent processing of PET volumes via the Beagle pipeline. The CIVET pipeline (version 1.1.11), developed at the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) for the fully automated analysis of structural images (Ad‐Dab'bagh et al., 2006), produced a wide range of products, including gray and white matter masks and transformations from native into standardized space (ICBM152). These products were subsequently injected into the Beagle multimodal analysis pipeline (Nikelski, Chertkow, & Evans, 2012), which performed a wide range of tasks, including alignment of the dynamic volume to the structural, image transformation and resampling into ICBM152 space, and spatial smoothing to increase signal‐to‐noise using a 6‐mm full‐width at half‐maximum kernel. Once dynamic volume preprocessing was complete, the Beagle pipeline quantified PiB load at each voxel by dividing the PiB signal at the voxel by the average signal strength measured within the cerebellar gray matter (the reference tissue). Cerebellar gray matter was used as the reference because it is largely spared from Aβ deposition (Klunk et al., 2004). As such, ratio values >1.0 identified those voxels exhibiting a PiB‐related signal of greater magnitude than that found within the cerebellum. In order to confirm the robustness of our findings, we performed similar analyses with uptake values derived from using subcortical white matter as the reference region. No partial volume correction was performed in this study due to having included participants who were considered cognitively healthy after a thorough neuropsychological assessment and who were therefore not expected to show significant atrophy on brain MRI scans.
Ratio values were then used to produce both global and localized (region of interest (ROI)‐based) metrics. Several standardized uptake value ratio (SUVr) values were computed by creating an average comprised of the values at either all gray matter voxels (global PiB retention value), only those from the cortical surface (cortical PiB retention value), or only those from a set of subcortical structures (subcortical regional PiB retention values). On average, the global PiB retention value was made up of 93% (n = 613,993) cortical voxels and 7% (n = 45,961) subcortical voxels (hippocampus, amygdala, caudate, putamen, pallidum, and thalamus). The ROI‐based values were produced by nonlinearly fitting a modified version of the Automated Anatomical Labeling template (Tzourio‐Mazoyer et al., 2002) to the ratio values volume, thus permitting us to compute average ratio values for the 120 ROIs included in the template. The cortical PiB retention values were the average of the values at all gray matter voxels comprising the cortical surface. Twelve subcortical regional PiB retention values were also derived, namely, from the left and right hippocampus, amygdala, putamen, caudate nucleus, pallidum, and thalamus. Global, cortical, and regional subcortical PiB retention values were used as continuous variables for regression analyses with the other structurally based subcortical metrics.
2.4. MRI acquisition and processing
2.4.1. MRI acquisition
All imaging data were acquired using a 3 T Siemens TrioTIM MR scanner (Siemens, Erlangen, Germany) at the Unité de neuroimagerie fonctionnelle of the Institut universitaire de gériatrie de Montréal. High‐resolution T1‐weighted images were acquired using a magnetization‐prepared rapid acquisition with gradient‐echo sequence, with the following parameters: TR = 2.3 s, TE = 2.94 ms, TI = 900 ms, flip angle = 9°, FOV = 256 × 240, voxel size: 1 × 1 × 1.2 mm3. PiB–PET imaging and MRI scan acquisition both took place within a year, within an average of 89 days.
2.4.2. Processing of the overall volumes of subcortical structures
Processing of the overall volumes of subcortical structures was aimed at generating one volume measurement for every subcortical structure of interest. As a first step, segmentation of subcortical volumes was performed semiautomatically from each individual's T1‐weighted image using the FreeSurfer segmentation pipeline, version 6.0, which has been described in detail elsewhere (Fischl et al., 2002; Fischl et al., 2004). This approach performs registration to standard space, intensity inhomogeneity correction, removal of nonbrain tissue, tissue‐type classification, and probabilistic anatomical labeling. Quality control was conducted at each step and processing that yielded bad results were excluded. This approach generated raw volume measurements for the following deep gray matter nuclei: the left and right amygdala, putamen, caudate nucleus, pallidum, and thalamus. The overall volumes derived from these subcortical nuclei are referred to as “subcortical overall volumes” in this manuscript.
As a second step, we also performed semiautomatic segmentation of hippocampal subfields using the hippocampal subfield module available as part of FreeSurfer, version 6.0 (Iglesias et al., 2015). This approach is based on Bayesian inference using an atlas algorithm of the hippocampal formations built upon ultrahigh resolution (~0.1 mm isotropic) ex vivo MRI data from autopsy brains (Iglesias et al., 2015). It allows for the segmentation of the whole hippocampus (as one of the subcortical overall volumes) and of 24 hippocampal subfields, namely the left and right CA1, CA2/3, CA4, dentate gyrus, hippocampal–amygdalar transition area, fimbria, alveus, molecular layer, parasubiculum, presubiculum, subiculum, hippocampal fissure, and hippocampal tail. This approach was shown to delineate with greater accuracy the boundaries within subfields, thus outperforming previously released FreeSurfer segmentation modules (e.g., version 5.3), which did not translate well to some hippocampal subfields (de Flores et al., 2015; Iglesias et al., 2015). Volumes derived from this approach are referred to as “hippocampal subfields volumes” in this manuscript.
2.4.3. Processing of subcortical local surface
Processing of the local surface of subcortical structures was aimed at generating a surface mesh for every subcortical structure mentioned previously in order to perform vertex‐wise (local) analysis of surface displacement. Surface processing was therefore performed for the left and right hippocampus, amygdala, putamen, caudate nucleus, pallidum, and thalamus using FIRST, version 5.0.11 (Patenaude et al., 2011), part of FSL (Jenkinson, Beckmann, Behrens, Woolrich, & Smith, 2012). Subcortical segmentation was first performed semiautomatically from each subject's T1‐weighted image. Each structure was modeled as a surface mesh based on shape and intensity information from a dataset of 336 manually delineated T1‐weighted images (Patenaude et al., 2011). A ventricle‐specific weighting mask was also applied in all participants to optimize registration to the standard space. Since surfaces resided in native space, vertices were registered into the MNI152 space in order to compare surface between subjects. This allowed for analyzing surface displacement due to local volume or shape and surface displacement due to shape only (Patenaude et al., 2011; Tanner, McFarland, & Price, 2017). The first approach removed between‐subjects changes due to pose (i.e., rotation and translation) by minimizing the sum of squared errors between the corresponding vertices of a participant's surface and the mean target surface; surface information revealed by this approach relates to the effect of both local volume and shape. In contrast, the second approach used a similar processing but with adjustment of the models for scaling; surface information revealed by this approach detects differences in shape even if the overall volume remains similar (i.e., surface changes that were still significant after having removing the effect of size and global scalings when aligning meshes).
2.5. Statistical analysis
2.5.1. Subcortical global volume analysis
Statistical analyses were performed using IBM SPSS Statistics, version 22.0 (IBM Corp., Armonk, NY). All raw overall volume measurements were normalized for brain size before conducting any statistical analyses. Subcortical global volumes were normalized by dividing the raw volume by the subject's estimated total intracranial volume (TIV), calculated as part of the FreeSurfer segmentation pipeline. Hippocampal subfields volumes were also normalized following this procedure. Therefore, all statistical analyses on subcortical global volumes were normalized for head size.
To investigate the associations of subcortical overall volumes and hippocampal subfields volumes with global and cortical regional PiB retention values, partial correlations were performed with age and sex as covariates. Age and sex were used as covariates since they correlated with PiB retention values and subcortical overall volumes and hippocampal subfields volumes. In contrast, education was not used as a covariate since it did not associate significantly with any PiB retention values or subcortical volumes. For correlations with subcortical regional PiB retention, global PiB retention was also added as a covariate in order to investigate the impact of subcortical Aβ load specifically. For each set of partial correlations, associations with global, cortical, and regional PiB retention values were considered statistically significant at p < .05 when corrected for multiple comparisons using a Bonferroni‐corrected threshold. Regarding subcortical regional PiB retention values, only corrections between a structure's normalized volume and the PiB retention value derived from this structure were conducted. Similarly, associations between hippocampal subfields volumes and subcortical regional PiB values were only performed in the left and right hippocampus. Despite significance being considered at p < .05 corrected for multiple comparisons, we also interpreted results as statistical trends when p < .05 without correction for multiple comparisons.
2.5.2. Subcortical surface analysis
To investigate the associations between subcortical surface and global, cortical, and regional Aβ loads, we first performed vertex‐wise “shape considering volume” (i.e., surface changes due to volume and/or shape) and then “shape only” (i.e., surface changes due to shape only) analyses to better understand the likeliest structural contributor to significant associations with surface. Associations with global and cortical PiB retention values were conducted with local surface in all subcortical structures; associations with regional PiB retention values were performed only with the local surface of the structure from which the PiB value was derived (e.g., associations between PiB value in the left hippocampus and the local surface of the left hippocampus). Moreover, associations with hippocampal subfields volumes were only performed with global, cortical, and regional PiB retention values in the left and right hippocampus only.
Local surface displacement represented the vertex projections between each individual's surface and the mean surface mesh, which were stored as scalar values. In order to use the most appropriate statistical model for each analysis, we explored the effects of age, sex, and education on local subcortical surface in every subcortical structure by conducting shape considering volume and shape only analyses, with local surface displacement values as regressands and the covariate as regressor (separately). Age was significantly associated with subcortical surface and sex differences were found in the hippocampus, caudate, putamen, and thalamus. Years of education did not correlate significantly with subcortical surface either when performing analyses with and without taking into account global scalings. Therefore, in all regression models, local surface displacement values were used as regressands, PiB retention values as regressors, and age (and sex when appropriate) as covariates. All analyses involving subcortical PiB retention values also included global PiB uptake as a covariate in order to investigate specifically the effect of subcortical Aβ load on subcortical surface. TIV was not included as a covariate in the models since surface meshes were reconstructed in the MNI152 space, controlling for head size (Patenaude et al., 2011). Vertex‐wise statistical analyses were performed using FSL's randomize tool for nonparametric permutation inference (Winkler, Ridgway, Webster, Smith, & Nichols, 2014), using 10,000 permutations (Nichols & Holmes, 2002) together with a threshold‐free cluster enhancement approach (Smith & Nichols, 2009). Resulting statistical maps were thresholded at p < .05 corrected for multiple comparisons; clusters surviving this threshold were considered significant. Only clusters that were at least 30 vertices in size were considered significant in order to avoid interpretation of marginal results.
2.5.3. Staging of brain amyloidosis
Similarly to previous studies (Cho et al., 2018; Hanseeuw, Betensky, et al., 2018), every participant was classified according to a staging of cerebral Aβ based on the PiB retention values derived from the cortex and the subcortical regions in which Aβ‐associated surface effects were identified. First, we identified cutoff scores in order to classify every participant as Aβ‐positive or Aβ‐negative in terms of their cortical PiB uptake. Based on a previous study (Villeneuve et al., 2015), we used a Gaussian mixture model and the Bayesian information criterion to investigate the distributions underlying the participants' cortical PiB retention values and to find the optimal number of distributions that fit our data. Two distributions were found to estimate best the distribution of cortical PiB uptake, which were considered to reflect participants who were Aβ‐positive (i.e., elevated Aβ retention in the cortex) and participants who were Aβ‐negative (i.e., normal Aβ retention in the cortex). A cutoff was derived that represented the 90% probability of belonging to the Aβ‐negative distribution; every participant whose cortical PiB retention value ≥1.22 was therefore considered Aβ‐positive based on the cortex. This cutoff is very similar to the one reported previously using a similar method (1.21; Villeneuve et al., 2015).
Second, we classified the participants as Aβ‐positive or Aβ‐negative based on subcortical PiB uptake; only participants who had high Aβ retention in the cortex were classified as Aβ‐positive or Aβ‐negative on subcortical PiB retention based on previous studies showing that subcortical Aβ accumulation is likely to happen once the cortex shows Aβ deposition (Beach et al., 2012; Thal, Rub, Orantes, & Braak, 2002). To determine Aβ positivity on subcortical structures, we derived a subcortical PiB index for each participant based on the mean of PiB retention values from the structures in which surface was shown to be influenced by the presence of Aβ, namely, the left and right hippocampus, the left and right thalamus, and the right pallidum (see Section 3). Since only a portion of these structures showed Aβ‐associated effects, the subcortical PiB index was weighted according to the number of vertices comprised in the cluster in which volume/shape was associated with regional Aβ (see Table 3 for the number of vertices in each cluster). Based on a previous study (Cho et al., 2018), the resulting subcortical PiB index (one per participant) was then standardized based on the whole cohort of 103 participants and subcortical Aβ positivity was determined when an already Aβ‐positive participant on the cortex showed a z score of subcortical PiB uptake ≥1.00. Participants with low cortical and subcortical Aβ retention were considered at Stage 0, participants with increased cortical retention but low subcortical Aβ retention at Stage 1, and participants with increased cortical and subcortical retention levels at Stage 2.
In order to investigate the clinical and brain characteristics of the different Aβ stages, we then conducted a series of exploratory analyses of variance between the three groups on the demographic and cognitive variables and on the subcortical overall volumes and hippocampal subfields volumes. Tukey's post hoc tests were used to investigate pairwise differences. We interpreted differences when at p < .05 uncorrected for multiple comparisons.
3. RESULTS
3.1. Participants
A total sample of 104 cognitively normal individuals (mean age: 73.4 years; 75% women; mean education level: 13.7 years; mean MoCA score: 27.3) were recruited, but 1 was removed due to registration failure during imaging processing (total sample = 103 participants). Demographics, clinical variables, and PiB retention values for the total sample are available in Table 1.
Table 1.
Variable | Participants (n = 103) |
---|---|
Demographics | |
Age, years | 73.4 (6.2) |
Men, n (%) | 26 (25%) |
Education, years | 13.7 (3.2) |
GDS, /30 | 3.1 (2.6) |
General cognitive measuresa | |
MoCA score, /30 | 27.3 (2.0) |
WAIS‐IV: Full scale IQ | 104.8 (12.0) |
WMS‐III: Logical memory, immediate recall, /25 | 14.4 (4.0) |
WMS‐III: Logical memory, delayed recall, /25 | 17.6 (3.1) |
PiB retention values | |
Global SUVr | 1.25 (0.17) |
Cortical SUVr | 1.24 (0.18) |
Left hippocampus | 1.43 (0.10) |
Right hippocampus | 1.41 (0.09) |
Left amygdala | 1.45 (0.12) |
Right amygdala | 1.34 (0.11) |
Left caudate | 1.25 (0.16) |
Right caudate | 1.28 (0.14) |
Left putamen | 1.53 (0.18) |
Right putamen | 1.55 (0.19) |
Left pallidum | 1.55 (0.19) |
Right pallidum | 1.68 (0.16) |
Left thalamus | 1.24 (0.12) |
Right thalamus | 1.23 (0.10) |
Note. Data are presented as mean (SD).
Abbreviations: GDS, Geriatric Depression Scale; IQ, intellectual quotient; MoCA, Montreal Cognitive Assessment; PiB, Pittsburgh compound B; SUVr, standardized uptake value ratio; WAIS‐IV, Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale—Fourth Edition; WMS‐III, Wechsler Memory Scale—Third Edition.
A detailed neuropsychological assessment was performed in every participant although only global cognitive measures are reported here.
3.2. Subcortical global volume analyses
A summary of the values of subcortical overall volumes and hippocampal subfields volumes for the whole sample is available in Table 2. Investigation of the associations of the global and cortical PiB retention values with normalized volumes of subcortical structures, namely the left and right hippocampus, amygdala, putamen, caudate nucleus, pallidum, and thalamus, revealed no significant relationships (p > .2) (see Supporting Information for results). In contrast, subcortical regional PiB retention values correlated positively with volume of the left hippocampus (i.e., positive association between Aβ load in the left hippocampus and volume of the left hippocampus, r = .205, p = .040) and negatively with volume of the right putamen (r = −.214, p = .032) (Figure 1a). However, these correlations did not remain significant after correction for multiple comparisons (p < .004).
Table 2.
Hemisphere | Structure | Volume, mm3 |
---|---|---|
Global volume | ||
Estimated total TIV | 1,370,211 (143,572) | |
Subcortical global volumes | ||
Left hemisphere | Hippocampus | 3,637 (450) |
Amygdala | 1,422 (237) | |
Putamen | 4,108 (489) | |
Caudate nucleus | 3,061 (373) | |
Pallidum | 1,816 (254) | |
Thalamus | 5,953 (691) | |
Right hemisphere | Hippocampus | 3,738 (453) |
Amygdala | 1,478 (222) | |
Putamen | 4,155 (517) | |
Caudate nucleus | 3,141 (373) | |
Pallidum | 1,725 (251) | |
Thalamus | 5,984 (675) | |
Hippocampal subfields volumes | ||
Left hemisphere | Subiculum | 396 (53) |
CA1 | 565 (82) | |
Hippocampal fissure | 160 (28) | |
Presubiculum | 290 (39) | |
Parasubiculum | 62 (12) | |
Molecular layer | 508 (70) | |
GC‐ML‐DG | 265 (41) | |
CA2/3 | 188 (34) | |
CA4 | 231 (35) | |
Fimbria | 64 (20) | |
HATA | 54 (10) | |
Hippocampal tail | 506 (70) | |
Right hemisphere | Subiculum | 396 (50) |
CA1 | 590 (82) | |
Hippocampal fissure | 165 (29) | |
Presubiculum | 275 (40) | |
Parasubiculum | 57 (11) | |
Molecular layer | 519 (69) | |
GC‐ML‐DG | 273 (40) | |
CA2/3 | 201 (33) | |
CA4 | 238 (34) | |
Fimbria | 59 (19) | |
HATA | 56 (9) | |
Hippocampal tail | 532 (74) |
Note. Subcortical and hippocampal subfields volumes are presented as raw mean measurements in mm3 (SD).
Abbreviations: CA, cornu ammonis; GC‐ML‐DG, granule cell and molecular layers of the dentate gyrus; HATA, hippocampal–amygdala transition area; SD, standard deviation; TIV, total intracranial volume.
Investigation of the associations between global and cortical PiB retention values and hippocampal subfields volumes revealed no significant relationships (p > .2) (see Supporting Information for results). However, when investigating associations between PiB retention values in the left and right hippocampus (separately) and hippocampal subfields volumes, PiB retention in the left hippocampus correlated positively with volumes of the left CA1 (r = .218, p = .029), subiculum (r = .248, p = .013), and molecular layer (r = .241, p = .016) (Figure 1b,c). However, none of these correlations remained significant after correction for multiple comparisons (p < .004).
When using subcortical white matter as the reference for quantifying ratio values, results were similar, with global and cortical Aβ not being associated with volumes and with Aβ in the left hippocampus being related to increased volume of the whole structure as well as with increase for the CA1 subfield, subiculum, and molecular layer. However, increased Aβ in the left hippocampus was now also related to volume of the CA4 subfield, the granule cell and molecular layers of the dentate gyrus and the hippocampal tail (see Supporting Information). Aβ in the left thalamus was now associated with its volume but the previous correlation between Aβ in the right putamen and its overall volume now became borderline (r = .051).
3.3. Subcortical surface analyses
When investigating Aβ‐related surface changes that are due to local volume or shape (i.e., without removing the effect of size from the analyses), we did not find any significant associations between global and cortical PiB retention values and local surface in any of the subcortical structures. Instead, when using subcortical regional PiB retention values, significant associations with outward surface displacement were found in the bilateral hippocampus and thalamus and in the right pallidum (Table 3 and Figure 2). Specifically, PiB retention in the left hippocampus was associated with extensive surface expansion in regions corresponding to CA1 and the subiculum (r = .442, p < .001) (Figure 2a). Similar associations, although less extensive in size, were found between PiB retention in the right hippocampus and surface expansion in the subiculum and CA1 (anterior cluster: r = .299, p = .002; posterior cluster: r = .376, p < .001) (Figure 2b). PiB retention in the left and right thalamus both correlated significantly with extensive surface expansion on the medial and lateral surfaces of the left (r = .351, p < .001) and right thalamus (r = .331, p = .001) (Figure 2c,d). In addition, PiB retention in the right pallidum was associated with surface expansion on the ventromedial (r = .331, p = .001) and the dorsomedial and lateral surface (r = .255, p = .010) of the right pallidum (Figure 2e). There were no associations between regional PiB retention and inward surface displacement in subcortical structures. When using subcortical white matter as the reference, results remained similar for the bilateral thalamus, similar but less extensive for the right hippocampus, and were no longer significant for the left hippocampus and the right pallidum (see Supporting Information). Global and cortical PiB uptake still did not reveal significant associations with subcortical surface morphology.
Table 3.
Clusters associated with regional PiB retention | Hemisphere | Number of vertices | MNI152 coordinates | F value | r value | p value | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
x | y | z | ||||||
Shape considering volume analyses | ||||||||
Hippocampus | Left | 999 | −33 | −34 | −9 | 22.2 | .442 | <.001 |
Hippocampus | Right | 259 | 24 | −14 | −27 | 10.1 | .376 | <.001 |
187 | 33 | −32 | −6 | 10.6 | .299 | .002 | ||
Thalamus | Left | 2,752 | −3 | −13 | −3 | 17.4 | .351 | <.001 |
Thalamus | Right | 2,225 | 22 | −32 | −4 | 17.0 | .331 | .001 |
Pallidum | Right | 285 | 22 | −1 | 3 | 11.3 | .255 | .010 |
109 | 17 | −7 | −6 | 15.4 | .331 | .001 | ||
Shape only analyses | ||||||||
Thalamus | Left | 162 | −4 | −26 | 4 | 13.9 | −.321 | .001 |
66 | −5 | −9 | 15 | 11.3 | .310 | .002 | ||
Thalamus | Right | 576 | 6 | −27 | 0 | 20.7 | −.389 | <.001 |
199 | 8 | −7 | 15 | 10.6 | .299 | .002 | ||
Pallidum | Right | 44 | 17 | −6 | −6 | 14.7 | .359 | <.001 |
Note. Results are corrected for multiple comparisons using threshold‐free cluster enhancement and clusters were considered significant if below a statistical threshold of p < .05 using TFCE. Only clusters comprising at least 30 vertices are reported.
Abbreviations: MNI, Montreal Neurological Institute; PiB, Pittsburgh compound B; TFCE, threshold‐free cluster enhancement.
When investigating Aβ‐related surface changes that are only due to local shape (i.e., by removing the effect of size from the analyses), surface analyses did not reveal any significant associations between global and cortical PiB retention values and local surface in any of the subcortical structures. However, when investigating associations with subcortical regional PiB retention values, significant associations were found with local surface in the bilateral thalamus and the right pallidum (Table 3 and Figure 3). Specifically, PiB retention in the left and right thalamus correlated with shape contraction on the medial surface (left: r = −.321, p = .001; right: r = −.389, p < .001) and with shape expansion on the dorsal surface (left: r = .310, p = .002; right: r = .299, p = .002) of their respective structure (Figure 3a,b). Qualitatively speaking, the clusters showing surface expansion overlapped with the clusters that were associated with thalamic Aβ load when using shape considering volume analyses. This is in contrast with the clusters showing surface contraction, which were found in regions of the thalamic surface that did not correlate with thalamic Aβ load when using shape considering volume analyses. PiB retention in the right pallidum was also associated with a small cluster of surface expansion on the ventromedial surface of the right pallidum (r = .359, p < .001) (Figure 3c). This cluster qualitatively overlapped with the results found when using shape considering volume analyses. No other associations between subcortical regional PiB retention and shape were found in the remaining subcortical structures, including the bilateral hippocampus. When using subcortical white matter as the reference, results remained similar but were less extensive, with PiB uptake in the left thalamus being associated with outward displacement and with inward displacement in the right thalamus (see Supporting Information). In addition, whereas higher PiB uptake in the right caudate was now associated with inward displacement of the surface, the association that we had in the right pallidum was no longer significant. Global and cortical PiB uptake were still unrelated to subcortical surface morphology.
3.4. Staging of amyloidosis
Every participant was classified into either one of three stages based on our model of Aβ staging of the PiB retention in the cortex and in subcortical regions in which an Aβ‐associated effect on surface was found. Seventy‐one (69%) participants were considered at Stage 0 (i.e., low cortical and subcortical PiB uptake), 26 (25%) participants at Stage 1 (i.e., high cortical but low subcortical PiB uptake), and six (6%) participants at Stage 2 (i.e., high cortical and subcortical PiB uptake). Compared to participants at Stage 1, participants at Stage 2 were older and had increased global and cortical PiB uptake, worse episodic memory performance, and smaller hippocampal subfields volumes, namely, for the subiculum, presubiculum, and fimbria (Table 4). Participants at Stage 2 also had worse performance on tasks assessing attention and smaller overall volume for the right hippocampus compared to participants at Stage 0.
Table 4.
Variables | Stage 0 (n = 71) | Stage 1 (n = 26) | Stage 2 (n = 6) | Difference?a | Post hocb |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Age, years | 72.2 (5.5) | 74.8 (6.4) | 81.2 (6.6) | p < .001 |
2 < 0 (p = .001) 2 < 1 (p = .044) |
Sex (M/F) | 17/54 | 6/20 | 3/3 | p = .340c | |
Education, years | 13.6 (3.2) | 14.3 (3.6) | 11.5 (1.5) | p = .167 | |
Estimated TIV | 1,366,239.8 (156,849.8) | 1,378,901.5 (112,639.4) | 1,379,550.2 (109,188.9) | p = .918 | |
Global SUVr | 1.16 (0.04) | 1.38 (0.18) | 1.67 (0.13) | p < .001 |
2 > 1 (p < .001) 2 > 0 (p < .001) 1 > 0 (p < .001) |
Cortical SUVr | 1.15 (0.04) | 1.38 (0.19) | 1.67 (0.14) | p < .001 |
2 > 1 (p < .001) 2 > 0 (p < .001) 1 > 0 (p < .001) |
Cognition | |||||
MoCA score | 27.4 (1.9) | 26.9 (2.1) | 28.3 (1.5) | p = .219 | |
Episodic memory, z score | 0.18 (0.88) | −0.11 (1.05) | −1.42 (1.01) | p < .001 |
2 < 0 (p < .001) 2 < 1 (p = .007) |
Working memory, z score | 0.22 (0.94) | −0.52 (0.97) | −0.38 (1.08) | p = .003 | 1 < 0 (p = .003) |
Processing speed, z score | 0.14 (0.95) | −0.29 (1.11) | −0.57 (0.76) | p = .061 | |
Executive functions, z score | 0.10 (0.85) | −0.18 (1.26) | −0.54 (1.02) | p = .188 | |
Language, z score | 0.10 (0.96) | −0.24 (1.08) | −0.28 (1.08) | p = 0.263 | |
Attention, z score | 0.18 (0.76) | −0.24 (1.08) | −0.94 (1.00) | p = .003 | 2 < 0 (p = .008) |
Visuospatial abilities, z score | 0.12 (1.02) | −0.30 (1.03) | −0.18 (0.39) | p = .171 | |
Normalized volume of subcortical structures (%) | |||||
Amygdala, left | 0.104 (0.016) | 0.104 (0.014) | 0.100 (0.010) | p = .807 | |
Amygdala, right | 0.108 (0.014) | 0.110 (0.010) | 0.098 (0.013) | p = .119 | |
Thalamus, left | 0.436 (0.031) | 0.437 (0.033) | 0.419 (0.037) | p = .435 | |
Thalamus, right | 0.440 (0.031) | 0.436 (0.038) | 0.417 (0.041) | p = .273 | |
Putamen, left | 0.304 (0.031) | 0.301 (0.051) | 0.279 (0.022) | p = .295 | |
Putamen, right | 0.306 (0.031) | 0.307 (0.060) | 0.283 (0.018) | p = .393 | |
Caudate, left | 0.224 (0.025) | 0.227 (0.031) | 0.214 (0.028) | p = .571 | |
Caudate, right | 0.231 (0.025) | 0.230 (0.030) | 0.224 (0.032) | p = .832 | |
Pallidum, left | 0.133 (0.016) | 0.135 (0.019) | 0.122 (0.005) | p = .199 | |
Pallidum, right | 0.126 (0.016) | 0.128 (0.017) | 0.119 (0.009) | p = .484 | |
Normalized hippocampal volumes (%) | |||||
Whole hippocampus, left | 0.2309 (0.0240) | 0.2264 (0.0260) | 0.2181 (0.0121) | p = .377 | |
Whole hippocampus, right | 0.2365 (0.0245) | 0.2328 (0.0247) | 0.2099 (0.0273) | p = .044 | 2 < 0 (p = .035) |
Hippocampal tail, left | 0.0377 (0.0052) | 0.0362 (0.0053) | 0.0342 (0.0035) | p = .155 | |
Hippocampal tail, right | 0.0396 (0.0052) | 0.0385 (0.0053) | 0.0352 (0.0042) | p = .126 | |
Subiculum, left | 0.0291 (0.0032) | 0.0287 (0.0034) | 0.0283 (0.0015) | p = .711 | |
Subiculum, right | 0.0293 (0.0033) | 0.0291 (0.0032) | 0.0253 (0.0041) | p = .020 |
2 < 0 (p = .014) 2 < 1 (p = .034) |
CA1, left | 0.0415 (0.0048) | 0.0414 (0.0061) | 0.0404 (0.0042) | p = .879 | |
CA1, right | 0.0435 (0.0051) | 0.0429 (0.0051) | 0.0399 (0.0060) | p = .239 | |
Hippocampal fissure, left | 0.0116 (0.0020) | 0.0119 (0.0014) | 0.0117 (0.0021) | p = .855 | |
Hippocampal fissure, right | 0.0122 (0.0022) | 0.0118 (0.0018) | 0.0120 (0.0020) | p = .721 | |
Presubiculum, left | 0.0215 (0.0025) | 0.0210 (0.0033) | 0.0193 (0.0018) | p = .130 | |
Presubiculum, right | 0.0203 (0.0028) | 0.0202 (0.0028) | 0.0171 (0.0018) | p = .024 |
2 < 0 (p = .018) 2 < 1 (p = .036) |
Parasubiculum, left | 0.0045 (0.0008) | 0.0045 (0.0009) | 0.0044 (0.0006) | p = .872 | |
Parasubiculum, right | 0.0042 (0.0008) | 0.0042 (0.0008) | 0.0041 (0.0006) | p = .957 | |
Molecular layer, left | 0.0375 (0.0042) | 0.0367 (0.0048) | 0.0357 (0.0030) | p = .469 | |
Molecular layer, right | 0.0384 (0.0044) | 0.0379 (0.0044) | 0.0341 (0.0056) | p = .075 | |
GC‐ML‐DG, left | 0.0195 (0.0025) | 0.0192 (0.0028) | 0.0183 (0.0019) | p = .496 | |
GC‐ML‐DG, right | 0.0201 (0.0024) | 0.0198 (0.0026) | 0.0183 (0.0031) | p = .215 | |
CA2/3, left | 0.0138 (0.0021) | 0.0136 (0.0023) | 0.0137 (0.0015) | p = .920 | |
CA2/3, right | 0.0149 (0.0020) | 0.0146 (0.0020) | 0.0133 (0.0023) | p = .196 | |
CA4, left | 0.0170 (0.0021) | 0.0167 (0.0023) | 0.0161 (0.0012) | p = .557 | |
CA4, right | 0.0175 (0.0020) | 0.0173 (0.0022) | 0.0161 (0.0025) | p = .295 | |
Fimbria, left | 0.0048 (0.0013) | 0.0046 (0.0015) | 0.0041 (0.0009) | p = .485 | |
Fimbria, right | 0.0044 (0.0011) | 0.0043 (0.0014) | 0.0030 (0.0015) | p = .022 |
2 < 0 (p = .016) 2 < 1 (p = .048) |
HATA, left | 0.0039 (0.0007) | 0.0040 (0.0006) | 0.0037 (0.0005) | p = .658 | |
HATA, right | 0.0042 (0.0006) | 0.0041 (0.0006) | 0.0035 (0.0008) | p = .071 |
Note. Data are presented as mean (SD). Values in bold represent differences that were significant. Note that investigation of group differences was not corrected for multiplicity and therefore remains exploratory.
Abbreviations: CA, cornu ammonis; GC‐ML‐DG, granule cell and molecular layers of the dentate gyrus; HATA, hippocampal–amygdala transition area; MoCA, Montreal Cognitive Assessment; SD, standard deviation; SUVr, standardized uptake value ratio; TIV, total intracranial volume.
Analysis of variance.
Tukey's post hoc tests.
Fisher–Freeman–Halton exact test for contingency.
4. DISCUSSION
In this study, we investigated the subcortical changes in overall volume and local surface associated with global, cortical, and subcortical regional PiB retention values in cognitively normal individuals. We found that subcortical regional Aβ load was associated with surface displacement in the hippocampus, thalamus, and pallidum, as well as with trends for the overall volume of the hippocampus, particularly the CA1 subfield, subiculum, and molecular layer. In contrast, global or cortical PiB retention values did not associate with volume or surface in the hippocampus, amygdala, basal ganglia, and thalamus.
It has been proposed that Aβ pathology propagates hierarchically from the neocortex to the allocortical, diencephalic, and basal ganglia structures, reaching the brainstem and cerebellum in the more advanced pathological stages (Thal et al., 2002). Most studies that investigated morphological changes associated with Aβ have focused mainly on cortical PiB retention measured globally or within a set of predefined regions (Jack et al., 2017; Villeneuve et al., 2015). In this study, we showed that in cognitively normal individuals, subcortical Aβ was associated with morphological changes in the hippocampus, thalamus, and pallidum, which were not found when using global or cortical PiB retention values. This concurs with a recent study in AD, MCI, and cognitively normal individuals that demonstrated the limitations of cortical cutoffs; using various cutoffs based on cortical ligand retention, a high number of individuals were classified as Aβ‐negative despite having abnormally elevated Aβ in at least 50% of wide parts of the frontal, temporal, and parietal association cortices, lower levels of CSF Aβ42, and lower episodic memory performance (Grothe et al., 2017). Also, in contrast with global cortical Aβ values, subcortical Aβ predicted cognitive decline with better accuracy than cortical Aβ (Hanseeuw, Betensky, et al., 2018), suggesting that the use of global or cortical PiB retention values in cognitively normal individuals may overshadow some structural changes associated with accumulation of Aβ in subcortical structures. Therefore, our findings underline the importance of investigating the impact of Aβ accumulation in subcortical structures, especially since subcortical Aβ was shown to be associated with worse clinical outcomes (Beach et al., 2012; Cho et al., 2018; Hanseeuw, Betensky, et al., 2018; Hanseeuw, Jonas, et al., 2018), including steeper cognitive decline and functional and structural abnormalities such as hippocampal atrophy (Cho et al., 2018). It is important to precise that our findings do not propose that Aβ accumulates preferentially in subcortical structures compared to the cortex but that its accumulation in subcortical structures allows revealing morphological changes in individuals still free of cognitive impairment. Indeed, it is thought that Aβ reaches global levels before cognitive impairments become clinically significant (Jack et al., 2010). The staging model of cerebral amyloidosis elaborated for the purpose of the present study supports that Aβ‐associated surface changes have clinical significance in cognitively healthy individuals, namely that elderly subjects with high subcortical Aβ are older, show increased global and cortical PiB uptake, lower episodic memory and attention performance, and smaller hippocampal volumes. However, in relation to future cognitive decline, cognitive follow‐up of these individuals is currently under way and will inform on the importance of subcortical Aβ. Longitudinal data will also help addressing questions about whether subcortical cutoff values may prove useful in the identification of individuals more likely to develop MCI or AD (Edmonds et al., 2016).
In this study, Aβ load in the left hippocampus was marginally associated with the overall volume of the hippocampus, particularly with the CA1 subfield, subiculum, and molecular layer, which are the subregions preferentially affected in AD and MCI patients according to previous neuroimaging and histological studies (de Flores et al., 2015). In cognitively normal individuals, global Aβ burden has also been associated with smaller subiculum but findings also show abnormalities in the hippocampal tail (Hsu et al., 2015). Despite being in line with previous studies, none of our overall volume associations survived correction for multiple comparisons. A vertex‐based surface approach was used to investigate Aβ‐associated local surface changes in subcortical structures and whether these changes related more to local volume or shape. Surface‐based approaches have proven efficient in the detection of subtle surface abnormalities in subcortical structures in AD, MCI, and cognitively normal individuals (Csernansky et al., 2005; de Flores et al., 2015; Kalin et al., 2017; Leh et al., 2016; Schroeder et al., 2016). Unlike our previous analyses using subcortical overall volumes, we found that subcortical regional PiB retention values were associated with the surface of the bilateral hippocampus and thalamus and the right pallidum. Surface displacement associated with hippocampal Aβ owed exclusively to local volume changes; these changes were located bilaterally in regions roughly corresponding to the CA1 subfield and subiculum and were more prominent in the left hippocampus, which is in line with previous shape‐based investigations in AD and MCI patients reporting abnormal surface in the hippocampus (Csernansky et al., 2000; Leh et al., 2016; Lindberg et al., 2012). Lateral deformation of the hippocampus, which we also found in our study, was reported to predict cognitive decline and dementia onset in cognitively normal individuals (Csernansky et al., 2005; Zanchi, Giannakopoulos, Borgwardt, Rodriguez, & Haller, 2017). Similarly, cognitively normal individuals who later converted to MCI or AD showed abnormal surface at baseline in CA1 and subiculum (Apostolova et al., 2010; Csernansky et al., 2005). In AD patients, surface changes in CA1 and subiculum were also related to lower neuronal counts (Blanken et al., 2017), suggesting that our findings in cognitively normal individuals may also relate to ongoing physiological processes in the hippocampus.
Extensive Aβ‐associated surface change was also found in the bilateral thalamus, which was mostly due to changes in local volume and which followed a relatively symmetrical pattern. This concurs with the occurrence of significant amounts of Aβ in the thalamus of AD (Braak & Braak, 1991). Shape abnormalities in the thalamus associated with Aβ have also been reported in cognitively normal individuals (Schroeder et al., 2016) as well as in MCI patients who showed cognitive worsening over a 2‐year period (Kalin et al., 2017). Interestingly, we also found that some of the surface change in the thalamus were also due to changes in shape only (i.e., surface remains abnormal even when analyses were corrected for size and global scalings), particularly in regions which did not show surface changes when investigating the effect of local volume change. This suggests that even the investigation of local volume may not reveal the extent of the changes occurring in the thalamic surface. These changes due to shape only were symmetrical and found in regions corresponding to the medial thalamic nuclei, a region highly connected with the frontal lobes that plays a gating role in the hippocampal‐prefrontal pathway activity and is involved in executive control, strategic retrieval of information in memory, and decision‐making (Mitchell, 2015). It has been proposed that the onset of deficits in executive functions may in part result from the interference with prefrontal cortex functions caused by the pathology within the medial thalamic region (Aggleton, Pralus, Nelson, & Hornberger, 2016). The interplay between volume and shape on subcortical surface remains to be understood; it is possible that shape changes precede volume changes in subcortical structures, and that shape changes would eventually lead to local volume changes as those seen for the rest of the thalamus. As for the right pallidum, the surface was also associated with subcortical regional Aβ, primarily with its volume but also with a smaller cluster of shape. This concurs with shape changes having been reported in the pallidum in MCI patients (Tang et al., 2014; Yi et al., 2016). Of note, Aβ‐associated surface changes in the right hippocampus and in the left and right thalamus were also found when changing the reference region for quantifying SUVr values, suggesting that the effect of subcortical Aβ on these structures is robust.
We generally observed positive relationships between regional subcortical Aβ and morphology. This contrasts with several studies reporting Aβ‐related atrophy (de Flores et al., 2015) but concurs with other studies in cognitively normal individuals that reported increased volume and outward deformation in the thalamus and hippocampus in association with cortical and regional PiB retention (Chetelat et al., 2010; Schroeder et al., 2016). The exact role of Aβ dyshomeostasis in AD pathogenesis remains to be understood (Chételat, 2013), especially in light of other candidates such as tau tangles and other novel proteomics targets associated with cognitive decline but independent from Aβ (Xia et al., 2017; Yu et al., 2018). In addition, a very high level of heterogeneity in neuropathologic comorbidity has been observed at a person‐specific level, with more than 200 distinct combinations of neuropathologies being reported and with 78% of participants showing mixed neuropathology (Boyle et al., 2018). Therefore, the subcortical surface changes found in our study can only be said to be associated with Aβ and cannot yet be linked to other neurodegenerative candidates. Our findings of hypertrophy are, however, congruent with previous evidence of neuroinflammation in AD (Heneka et al., 2015). Increased microglial activation has been reported in MCI and early AD patients (Fan, Brooks, Okello, & Edison, 2017; Hamelin et al., 2016; Parbo et al., 2017) and has been linked to better cognitive performance and increased gray matter volume (Hamelin et al., 2016). Increased microglial activation has also been associated with Aβ deposition more than tau tangles in AD and MCI patients (Parbo et al., 2017; Parbo et al., 2018). In AD, hippocampal shape expansion was associated with anti‐inflammatory parameters and surface contraction with pro‐inflammatory parameters (Cabinio et al., 2018). The increased hippocampal volumes and the outward surface displacements that we found in subcortical structures may be early morphological changes in cognitively normal individuals that hypothetically appear in response to Aβ‐related neuroinflammatory processes. However, this hypothesis is highly speculative and whether neuroinflammation has an explanatory role in our associations remains a matter of investigation.
This study had some limitations. First, in this study, we only reported baseline results from our cohort of cognitively normal individuals. Our cohort is followed longitudinally to assess the development of cognitive impairment, which will allow a clearer understanding of the associations between volume and shape changes and cognitive decline. Second, we did not account for the presence of apolipoprotein‐E ε4, a well‐known risk factor for sporadic AD. Some of our effects may therefore have been influenced by the presence of some ε4‐carriers. However, our cognitive assessment was thorough and all participants were carefully screened for cognitive impairment. Third, we did not apply any partial volume correction in the quantification of SUVr values. However, our elderly participants all had normal cognitive functions as confirmed by a thorough neuropsychological assessment and our pattern of results remained relatively similar when changing the reference region for quantifying SUVr values, particularly the right hippocampus and the left and right thalamus.
5. CONCLUSION
In summary, regional Aβ in the hippocampus, thalamus, and pallidum is associated in cognitively normal individuals with surface in these structures. These associations were not found when using global or cortical PiB retention values or when using global subcortical volumes. Elderly participants with normal cognition but who show higher levels of subcortical PiB uptake were older and had worse episodic memory and attention performance, increased global and cortical PiB uptake, and smaller hippocampal volumes. This highlights the importance, in cognitively normal individuals, of investigating the regional PiB retention in subcortical structures to better understand the neuropathological signature of AD.
CONFLICT OF INTEREST
The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.
6.
DATA AVAILABILITY
The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.
Supporting information
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
This work was supported by a grant from the Canadian Institutes of Health Research (MOP123376) and the Institute of Aging (IA0120269). C.B. declares investment in IMEKA.
Rahayel S, Bocti C, Sévigny Dupont P, et al. Subcortical amyloid load is associated with shape and volume in cognitively normal individuals. Hum Brain Mapp. 2019;40:3951–3965. 10.1002/hbm.24680
Funding information Canadian Institutes of Health Research, Grant/Award Number: MOP123376; Institute of Aging, Grant/Award Number: IA0120269
Data Availability: The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.
REFERENCES
- Achterberg, H. C. , van der Lijn, F. , den Heijer, T. , Vernooij, M. W. , Ikram, M. A. , Niessen, W. J. , & de Bruijne, M. (2014). Hippocampal shape is predictive for the development of dementia in a normal, elderly population. Human Brain Mapping, 35(5), 2359–2371. 10.1002/hbm.22333 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ad‐Dab'bagh, Y. , Lyttelton, O. , Muehlboeck, J. S. , Lepage, C. , Einarson, D. , Mok, K. , … Evans, A. C. (2006). The CIVET Image‐Processing Environment: A Fully Automated Comprehensive Pipeline for Anatomical Neuroimaging Research. Paper presented at the Proceedings of the 12th Annual Meeting of the Organization for Human Brain Mapping.
- Aggleton, J. P. , Pralus, A. , Nelson, A. J. , & Hornberger, M. (2016). Thalamic pathology and memory loss in early Alzheimer's disease: Moving the focus from the medial temporal lobe to Papez circuit. Brain, 139(Pt. 7), 1877–1890. 10.1093/brain/aww083 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andrews, K. A. , Modat, M. , Macdonald, K. E. , Yeatman, T. , Cardoso, M. J. , Leung, K. K. , … Lifestyle Flagship Study of Ageing . (2013). Atrophy rates in asymptomatic amyloidosis: Implications for Alzheimer prevention trials. PLoS One, 8(3), e58816 10.1371/journal.pone.0058816 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Apostolova, L. G. , Mosconi, L. , Thompson, P. M. , Green, A. E. , Hwang, K. S. , Ramirez, A. , … de Leon, M. J. (2010). Subregional hippocampal atrophy predicts Alzheimer's dementia in the cognitively normal. Neurobiology of Aging, 31(7), 1077–1088. 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2008.08.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beach, T. G. , Sue, L. I. , Walker, D. G. , Sabbagh, M. N. , Serrano, G. , Dugger, B. N. , … Souders, L. (2012). Striatal amyloid plaque density predicts Braak neurofibrillary stage and clinicopathological Alzheimer's disease: Implications for amyloid imaging. Journal of Alzheimer's Disease, 28(4), 869–876. 10.3233/JAD-2011-111340 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blanken, A. E. , Hurtz, S. , Zarow, C. , Biado, K. , Honarpisheh, H. , Somme, J. , … Apostolova, L. G. (2017). Associations between hippocampal morphometry and neuropathologic markers of Alzheimer's disease using 7 T MRI. NeuroImage: Clinical, 15, 56–61. 10.1016/j.nicl.2017.04.020 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boyle, P. A. , Yu, L. , Wilson, R. S. , Leurgans, S. E. , Schneider, J. A. , & Bennett, D. A. (2018). Person‐specific contribution of neuropathologies to cognitive loss in old age. Annals of Neurology, 83(1), 74–83. 10.1002/ana.25123 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Braak, H. , & Braak, E. (1991). Alzheimer's disease affects limbic nuclei of the thalamus. Acta Neuropathologica, 81(3), 261–268. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Braak, H. , & Braak, E. (1997). Frequency of stages of Alzheimer‐related lesions in different age categories. Neurobiology of Aging, 18(4), 351–357. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cabinio, M. , Saresella, M. , Piancone, F. , LaRosa, F. , Marventano, I. , Guerini, F. R. , … Clerici, M. (2018). Association between hippocampal shape, neuroinflammation, and cognitive decline in Alzheimer's disease. Journal of Alzheimer's Disease, 66(3), 1131–1144. 10.3233/JAD-180250 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carmichael, O. , Xie, J. , Fletcher, E. , Singh, B. , DeCarli, C. , & Alzheimer's Disease Neuroimaging Initiative . (2012). Localized hippocampus measures are associated with Alzheimer pathology and cognition independent of total hippocampal volume. Neurobiology of Aging, 33(6), 1124), e1131–e1141. 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2011.08.016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carson, N. , Leach, L. , & Murphy, K. J. (2018). A re‐examination of Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) cutoff scores. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 33(2), 379–388. 10.1002/gps.4756 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chételat, G. (2013). Alzheimer disease: Abeta‐independent processes‐rethinking preclinical AD. Nature Reviews. Neurology, 9(3), 123–124. 10.1038/nrneurol.2013.21 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chetelat, G. , Villemagne, V. L. , Pike, K. E. , Baron, J. C. , Bourgeat, P. , Jones, G. , … Lifestyle Study of Ageing Research Group . (2010). Larger temporal volume in elderly with high versus low beta‐amyloid deposition. Brain, 133(11), 3349–3358. 10.1093/brain/awq187 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cho, S. H. , Shin, J. H. , Jang, H. , Park, S. , Kim, H. J. , Kim, S. E. , … Alzheimer's Disease Neuroimaging Initiative . (2018). Amyloid involvement in subcortical regions predicts cognitive decline. European Journal of Nuclear Medicine and Molecular Imaging, 45, 2368–2376. 10.1007/s00259-018-4081-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Csernansky, J. G. , Wang, L. , Joshi, S. , Miller, J. P. , Gado, M. , Kido, D. , … Miller, M. I. (2000). Early DAT is distinguished from aging by high‐dimensional mapping of the hippocampus. Dementia of the Alzheimer type. Neurology, 55(11), 1636–1643. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Csernansky, J. G. , Wang, L. , Swank, J. , Miller, J. P. , Gado, M. , McKeel, D. , … Morris, J. C. (2005). Preclinical detection of Alzheimer's disease: Hippocampal shape and volume predict dementia onset in the elderly. NeuroImage, 25(3), 783–792. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.12.036 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Flores, R. , La Joie, R. , & Chételat, G. (2015). Structural imaging of hippocampal subfields in healthy aging and Alzheimer's disease. Neuroscience, 309, 29–50. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2015.08.033 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Edmonds, E. C. , Bangen, K. J. , Delano‐Wood, L. , Nation, D. A. , Furst, A. J. , Salmon, D. P. , … Alzheimer's Disease Neuroimaging, I. (2016). Patterns of cortical and subcortical amyloid burden across stages of preclinical Alzheimer's disease. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 22(10), 978–990. 10.1017/S1355617716000928 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fan, Z. , Brooks, D. J. , Okello, A. , & Edison, P. (2017). An early and late peak in microglial activation in Alzheimer's disease trajectory. Brain, 140(3), 792–803. 10.1093/brain/aww349 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fischl, B. , Salat, D. H. , Busa, E. , Albert, M. , Dieterich, M. , Haselgrove, C. , … Dale, A. M. (2002). Whole brain segmentation: Automated labeling of neuroanatomical structures in the human brain. Neuron, 33(3), 341–355. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fischl, B. , van der Kouwe, A. , Destrieux, C. , Halgren, E. , Segonne, F. , Salat, D. H. , … Dale, A. M. (2004). Automatically parcellating the human cerebral cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 14(1), 11–22. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grothe, M. J. , Barthel, H. , Sepulcre, J. , Dyrba, M. , Sabri, O. , Teipel, S. J. , & Alzheimer's Disease Neuroimaging, I. (2017). In vivo staging of regional amyloid deposition. Neurology, 89(20), 2031–2038. 10.1212/WNL.0000000000004643 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hamelin, L. , Lagarde, J. , Dorothee, G. , Leroy, C. , Labit, M. , Comley, R. A. , … Clinical, I. t. (2016). Early and protective microglial activation in Alzheimer's disease: A prospective study using 18F‐DPA‐714 PET imaging. Brain, 139(Pt. 4), 1252–1264. 10.1093/brain/aww017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hanseeuw, B. J. , Betensky, R. A. , Mormino, E. C. , Schultz, A. P. , Sepulcre, J. , Becker, J. A. , … Harvard Aging Brain, S. (2018). PET staging of amyloidosis using striatum. Alzheimer's & Dementia, 14, 1281–1292. 10.1016/j.jalz.2018.04.011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hanseeuw, B. J. , Jonas, V. , Jackson, J. , Betensky, R. A. , Rentz, D. M. , Johnson, K. A. , … Donovan, N. J. (2018). Association of anxiety with subcortical amyloidosis in cognitively normal older adults. Molecular Psychiatry. 10.1038/s41380-018-0214-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heneka, M. T. , Carson, M. J. , El Khoury, J. , Landreth, G. E. , Brosseron, F. , Feinstein, D. L. , … Kummer, M. P. (2015). Neuroinflammation in Alzheimer's disease. Lancet Neurology, 14(4), 388–405. 10.1016/S1474-4422(15)70016-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hsu, P. J. , Shou, H. , Benzinger, T. , Marcus, D. , Durbin, T. , Morris, J. C. , & Sheline, Y. I. (2015). Amyloid burden in cognitively normal elderly is associated with preferential hippocampal subfield volume loss. Journal of Alzheimer's Disease, 45(1), 27–33. 10.3233/JAD-141743 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Iglesias, J. E. , Augustinack, J. C. , Nguyen, K. , Player, C. M. , Player, A. , Wright, M. , … Alzheimer's Disease Neuroimaging, I. (2015). A computational atlas of the hippocampal formation using ex vivo, ultra‐high resolution MRI: Application to adaptive segmentation of in vivo MRI. NeuroImage, 115, 117–137. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.04.042 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jack, C. R., Jr. , Knopman, D. S. , Jagust, W. J. , Shaw, L. M. , Aisen, P. S. , Weiner, M. W. , … Trojanowski, J. Q. (2010). Hypothetical model of dynamic biomarkers of the Alzheimer's pathological cascade. Lancet Neurology, 9(1), 119–128. 10.1016/S1474-4422(09)70299-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jack, C. R., Jr. , Wiste, H. J. , Weigand, S. D. , Therneau, T. M. , Lowe, V. J. , Knopman, D. S. , … Petersen, R. C. (2017). Defining imaging biomarker cut points for brain aging and Alzheimer's disease. Alzheimer's & Dementia, 13(3), 205–216. 10.1016/j.jalz.2016.08.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jenkinson, M. , Beckmann, C. F. , Behrens, T. E. , Woolrich, M. W. , & Smith, S. M. (2012). FSL. NeuroImage, 62(2), 782–790. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kalin, A. M. , Park, M. T. , Chakravarty, M. M. , Lerch, J. P. , Michels, L. , Schroeder, C. , … Leh, S. E. (2017). Subcortical shape changes, hippocampal atrophy and cortical thinning in future Alzheimer's disease patients. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 9, 38 10.3389/fnagi.2017.00038 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klunk, W. E. , Engler, H. , Nordberg, A. , Wang, Y. , Blomqvist, G. , Holt, D. P. , … Langstrom, B. (2004). Imaging brain amyloid in Alzheimer's disease with Pittsburgh compound‐B. Annals of Neurology, 55(3), 306–319. 10.1002/ana.20009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leh, S. E. , Kalin, A. M. , Schroeder, C. , Park, M. T. , Chakravarty, M. M. , Freund, P. , … Michels, L. (2016). Volumetric and shape analysis of the thalamus and striatum in amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Journal of Alzheimer's Disease, 49(1), 237–249. 10.3233/JAD-150080 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lindberg, O. , Walterfang, M. , Looi, J. C. , Malykhin, N. , Ostberg, P. , Zandbelt, B. , … Wahlund, L. O. (2012). Hippocampal shape analysis in Alzheimer's disease and frontotemporal lobar degeneration subtypes. Journal of Alzheimer's Disease, 30(2), 355–365. 10.3233/JAD-2012-112210 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mitchell, A. S. (2015). The mediodorsal thalamus as a higher order thalamic relay nucleus important for learning and decision‐making. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 54, 76–88. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.03.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murray, M. E. , Lowe, V. J. , Graff‐Radford, N. R. , Liesinger, A. M. , Cannon, A. , Przybelski, S. A. , … Dickson, D. W. (2015). Clinicopathologic and 11C‐Pittsburgh compound B implications of Thal amyloid phase across the Alzheimer's disease spectrum. Brain, 138(Pt. 5, 1370–1381. 10.1093/brain/awv050 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nichols, T. E. , & Holmes, A. P. (2002). Nonparametric permutation tests for functional neuroimaging: A primer with examples. Human Brain Mapping, 15(1), 1–25. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nikelski, J. , Chertkow, H. , & Evans, A. (2012). Running with the Beagle: A Multi‐Modal, Integrative Imaging Pipeline Specialized for the Processing of Elderly Brains. Paper presented at the Human Amyloid Imaging Conference.
- Parbo, P. , Ismail, R. , Hansen, K. V. , Amidi, A. , Marup, F. H. , Gottrup, H. , … Brooks, D. J. (2017). Brain inflammation accompanies amyloid in the majority of mild cognitive impairment cases due to Alzheimer's disease. Brain, 140(7), 2002–2011. 10.1093/brain/awx120 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Parbo, P. , Ismail, R. , Sommerauer, M. , Stokholm, M. G. , Hansen, A. K. , Hansen, K. V. , … Brooks, D. J. (2018). Does inflammation precede tau aggregation in early Alzheimer's disease? A PET study. Neurobiology of Disease, 117, 211–216. 10.1016/j.nbd.2018.06.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Patenaude, B. , Smith, S. M. , Kennedy, D. N. , & Jenkinson, M. (2011). A Bayesian model of shape and appearance for subcortical brain segmentation. NeuroImage, 56(3), 907–922. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.02.046 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schroeder, C. , Park, M. T. , Germann, J. , Chakravarty, M. M. , Michels, L. , Kollias, S. , … Leh, S. E. (2016). Hippocampal shape alterations are associated with regional Abeta load in cognitively normal elderly individuals. The European Journal of Neuroscience, 45, 1241–1251. 10.1111/ejn.13408 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seo, S. W. , Ayakta, N. , Grinberg, L. T. , Villeneuve, S. , Lehmann, M. , Reed, B. , … Rabinovici, G. D. (2017). Regional correlations between [(11)C]PIB PET and post‐mortem burden of amyloid‐beta pathology in a diverse neuropathological cohort. NeuroImage: Clinical, 13, 130–137. 10.1016/j.nicl.2016.11.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith, S. M. , & Nichols, T. E. (2009). Threshold‐free cluster enhancement: Addressing problems of smoothing, threshold dependence and localisation in cluster inference. NeuroImage, 44(1), 83–98. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.03.061 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tang, X. , Holland, D. , Dale, A. M. , Younes, L. , Miller, M. I. , & Alzheimer's Disease Neuroimaging, I. (2014). Shape abnormalities of subcortical and ventricular structures in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer's disease: Detecting, quantifying, and predicting. Human Brain Mapping, 35(8), 3701–3725. 10.1002/hbm.22431 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tanner, J. J. , McFarland, N. R. , & Price, C. C. (2017). Striatal and hippocampal atrophy in idiopathic Parkinson's disease patients without dementia: A morphometric analysis. Frontiers in Neurology, 8, 139 10.3389/fneur.2017.00139 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thal, D. R. , Rub, U. , Orantes, M. , & Braak, H. (2002). Phases of a beta‐deposition in the human brain and its relevance for the development of AD. Neurology, 58(12), 1791–1800. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tzourio‐Mazoyer, N. , Landeau, B. , Papathanassiou, D. , Crivello, F. , Etard, O. , Delcroix, N. , … Joliot, M. (2002). Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single‐subject brain. NeuroImage, 15(1), 273–289. 10.1006/nimg.2001.0978 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Villemagne, V. L. , Burnham, S. , Bourgeat, P. , Brown, B. , Ellis, K. A. , Salvado, O. , … Lifestyle Research, G. (2013). Amyloid beta deposition, neurodegeneration, and cognitive decline in sporadic Alzheimer's disease: A prospective cohort study. Lancet Neurology, 12(4), 357–367. 10.1016/S1474-4422(13)70044-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Villemagne, V. L. , Pike, K. E. , Chetelat, G. , Ellis, K. A. , Mulligan, R. S. , Bourgeat, P. , … Rowe, C. C. (2011). Longitudinal assessment of Abeta and cognition in aging and Alzheimer disease. Annals of Neurology, 69(1), 181–192. 10.1002/ana.22248 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Villeneuve, S. , Rabinovici, G. D. , Cohn‐Sheehy, B. I. , Madison, C. , Ayakta, N. , Ghosh, P. M. , … Jagust, W. (2015). Existing Pittsburgh compound‐B positron emission tomography thresholds are too high: Statistical and pathological evaluation. Brain, 138(Pt. 7), 2020–2033. 10.1093/brain/awv112 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Winkler, A. M. , Ridgway, G. R. , Webster, M. A. , Smith, S. M. , & Nichols, T. E. (2014). Permutation inference for the general linear model. NeuroImage, 92, 381–397. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.01.060 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Xia, C. , Makaretz, S. J. , Caso, C. , McGinnis, S. , Gomperts, S. N. , Sepulcre, J. , … Dickerson, B. C. (2017). Association of in vivo [18F]AV‐1451 tau PET imaging results with cortical atrophy and symptoms in typical and atypical Alzheimer disease. JAMA Neurology, 74(4), 427–436. 10.1001/jamaneurol.2016.5755 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yi, H. A. , Moller, C. , Dieleman, N. , Bouwman, F. H. , Barkhof, F. , Scheltens, P. , … Vrenken, H. (2016). Relation between subcortical grey matter atrophy and conversion from mild cognitive impairment to Alzheimer's disease. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 87(4), 425–432. 10.1136/jnnp-2014-309105 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yu, L. , Petyuk, V. A. , Gaiteri, C. , Mostafavi, S. , Young‐Pearse, T. , Shah, R. C. , … Bennett, D. A. (2018). Targeted brain proteomics uncover multiple pathways to Alzheimer's dementia. Annals of Neurology, 84(1), 78–88. 10.1002/ana.25266 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zanchi, D. , Giannakopoulos, P. , Borgwardt, S. , Rodriguez, C. , & Haller, S. (2017). Hippocampal and amygdala gray matter loss in elderly controls with subtle cognitive decline. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 9, 50 10.3389/fnagi.2017.00050 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.