Abstract
The human brain shows remarkable development of functional brain activity from childhood to adolescence. Here, we investigated whether electroencephalogram (EEG) recordings are suitable for predicting the age of children and adolescents. Moreover, we investigated whether overestimation or underestimation of age was stable over longer time periods, as stable prediction error can be interpreted as reflecting individual brain maturational level. Finally, we established whether the age‐prediction error was genetically determined. Then, 3 min eyes‐closed resting‐state EEG data from the longitudinal EEG studies of Netherlands Twin Register (NTR; n = 836) and Washington University in St. Louis (n = 702) were used at ages 5, 7, 12, 14, 16, and 18. Longitudinal data were available within childhood (5–7 years) and adolescence (16–18 years). We calculated power in 1 Hz wide bins (1–24 Hz). Random forest (RF) regression and relevance vector machine with sixfold cross‐validation were applied. The best mean absolute prediction error was obtained with RF (1.22 years). Classification of childhood versus puberty/adolescence reached over 94% accuracy. Prediction errors were moderately to highly stable over periods of 1.5–2.1 years (0.53 < r < 0.74) and signifcantly affected by genetic factors (heritability between 42 and 79%). Our results show that age prediction from low‐cost EEG recordings is comparable in accuracy to those obtained with magnetic resonance imaging. Children and adolescents showed stable overestimation or underestimation of their age, which means that some participants have stable brain activity patterns that reflect those of an older or younger age, and could therefore reflect individual brain maturational level. This prediction error is heritable, suggesting that genes underlie maturational level of functional brain activity. We propose that age prediction based on EEG recordings can be used for tracking neurodevelopment in typically developing children, in preterm children, and in children with neurodevelopmental disorders.
Keywords: age prediction, brain age, brain maturation, development, electroencephalography (EEG), machine learning
1. INTRODUCTION
The neural tissue of the brain shows remarkable development from childhood to adolescence, which includes changes in dendritic arborization, synaptogenesis, and myelination, and synaptic pruning (Anderson, Northam, Hendy, & Wrennall, 2001; Huttenlocher, 1979). These neuronal‐level processes result in brain volume increases and gray matter changes (Giedd et al., 2009; Hedman, van Haren, Schnack, Kahn, & Hulshoff Pol, 2012; Lenroot & Giedd, 2006; Mills & Tamnes, 2014). These anatomical changes are accompanied by changes in brain function as reflected in electrophysiological brain activity. One of the most striking features is the change in oscillatory patterns in the electroencephalogram (EEG) (Niedermeyer & Lopes Da Silva, 2005; Smit et al., 2012). During childhood, temporal and posterior theta rhythm (4–7 Hz) dominates (Benniger, Matthis, & Scheffner, 1984; Puligheddu et al., 2005), but strongly decreases over the years. The alpha rhythm increases in frequency from 8 Hz in childhood to 10 Hz in adolescence (Smit et al., 2012). Maturation of alpha rhythms begins in posterior regions and ends in anterior regions while the beta frequency (12–30 Hz) matures from central to lateral and finally to frontal regions (Barriga‐Paulino, Flores, & Gomez, 2011; Benniger et al., 1984; Bresnahan, Anderson, & Barry, 1999; Gasser, Jennen‐Steinmetz, Sroka, Verleger, & Mocks, 1988; Gasser, Verleger, Bächer, & Sroka, 1988; Matousek & Petersen, 1973a, 1973b; Niedermeyer & Lopes Da Silva, 2005).
During maturation, children and adolescents show marked development of behavioral skills and cognition (Mills & Tamnes, 2014; Walhovd, Tamnes, & Fjell, 2014). Interestingly, they also show large differences in developmental speed. (Fischer & Silvern, 1985; Philip Shaw et al., 2006). One of the challenges of neurodevelopmental research is to investigate how these differences in behavioral development can be explained by underlying changes in neural function (Durston & Casey, 2006). Several studies have attempted to create measures of brain maturation by using brain imaging data for predicting calendar age—often using machine learning. These studies have largely focused on brain anatomy derived from magnetic resonance imaging (MRI). For example, Franke, Luders, May, Wilke, and Gaser (2012) computed the so‐called brain age in children and adolescents and obtained an average prediction error of 1.1 years (defined as the average absolute difference between the subjects' ages and the ages estimated by the machine learning model. Brown et al. (2012) predicted calendar age using the multidimensional nature of brain anatomy. At age 3, they obtained an average prediction error of 0.66 years. The prediction error increased with age until an average of 1.42 years at age 20.
These studies show that anatomical brain maturation in childhood and adolescence can be estimated using expensive MRI scanning, which may result in limited availability. By contrast, EEG is inexpensive, and EEG units can be flexibly deployed in the field including educational and medical institutions. Moreover, the brain activity measured with EEG is known to show large developmental changes within the critical developmental periods of school‐aged children (e.g., Smit et al., 2012). Our aim is to investigate whether EEG can be used to estimate the participants' age with the same level of accuracy as obtained with MRI. To this end, we applied machine learning with cross‐validation to predict age from resting‐state EEG in a large sample of children and adolescents (5–18 years).
The previous studies also have shown that predicting calendar age is not perfect, and always results in (sometimes substantial) residual error. It has been suggested that this error is a biomarker of brain maturation (or brain age, Franke et al., 2012), since it indicates that some participants have brain patterns that are more appropriate for a different age than their own. Moreover, it has been suggested that the estimated brain maturation reflects the behavioral changes observed at an individual level, or correlates with neurodevelopmental disorders. However, this is arguably only the case if the prediction error is stable, and is not the result of model misspecification or measurement noise. Our second aim was therefore to use longitudinal data present in our large EEG databases to establish the relatively longer term (>1 year) stability of the prediction error.
As a final aim, we investigated the genetic etiology of the EEG‐based predicted age. If the prediction error is stable (i.e., some subjects show systematic overestimation or underestimation of calendar age), and if this is to be predictive of behavioral outcomes that are known to be heritable—such as the reaching of cognitive milestones or neurodevelopmental disorders like Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD)—then the age‐prediction error should also be heritable (Derks et al., 2008; de Geus, 2010; Philip Shaw et al., 2006; Smit, De Geus, Boersma, Boomsma, & Stam, 2016). Our final aim was therefore to assess the heritability of age‐prediction error by using longitudinal twin EEG data sets of children and adolescents (Boomsma, Busjahn, & Peltonen, 2002).
2. METHODS
2.1. Participants and procedure
In this study, two large developmental twin‐family data sets with EEG recordings were used (see Table S2, Supporting Information for details). The first data set was from the NTR (N = 836) collected as part of a study into the genetics of brain development and cognition (Boomsma et al., 2002). The EEG recordings were obtained in four waves divided into two groups. The first group of participants was measured at ages 5 and 7 and a different group of participants at ages 16 and 18 (van Beijsterveldt et al., 2013). The second data set consisted of participants taking part in a longitudinal study of Genetics, Neurocognition and Adolescence Substance Abuse (GNASA) of Washington University in St. Louis (WUSTL), and contained measurement waves at 12, 14, and 16 years (N = 621).
Ethical permission for the NTR study was obtained from Medisch Ethische Toetsingscommissie of the VUmc. All participants (and parents/guardians for participants under 18) were informed about the nature of the study and were invited by letter to participate. Agreement to participate was obtained in writing. The GNASA study was approved by the human studies committee at the Washington University School of Medicine. A written informed assent was obtained from all participants, and a written informed consent was obtained from their parents.
2.2. EEG acquisition
A detailed procedure of NTR EEG data recording is described elsewhere (Smit, Posthuma, Boomsma, & Geus, 2005; Van Baal, de Geus, & Boomsma, 1996). NTR EEG data were recorded with tin electrodes placed according to 14 channels of the 10–20 system and connected to a Nihon Kohden PV‐441A polygraph with time constant 5 s (corresponding to a 0.03 Hz high‐pass filter) and low‐pass of 35 Hz, digitized at 250 Hz using an in‐house built 12‐bit A/D converter board and stored for offline analysis. Leads were Fp1, Fp2, F7, F3, F4, F8, C3, C4, T5, P3, P4, T6, O1, O2, and bipolar horizontal and vertical electrooculographic (EOG) derivations. Electrode impedance was kept below 10 kΩ. All EEG signals were measured against physically connected earlobe electrodes with high impedance preamplifiers following Pivik et al. (1993). Participants were seated in a dimly lit and sound attenuated booth for recording. They were instructed to close their eyes. Acquisition lasted for three periods of 1 min. Between recording epochs, the door was opened and participants were checked not to have fallen asleep. Acquisition was extended when data was observed to have excessive artifact or sleep EEG.
The GNASA sample EEG data were recorded using Compumedics‐Neuroscan SynAmps2 system from 30 scalp locations according to the extended 10–20 system using an elastic cap with Ag/AgCl electrodes and a ground electrode on the forehead, with high‐ and low‐pass filters set at 0.05 and 100 Hz respectively on a Neuroscan SynAmps recording system. The left mastoid served as a reference during recording, and the right mastoid was recorded as a separate channel. Averaged mastoid reference was computed offline. Participants were recorded for 1 min periods with alternating eyes closed and eyes open for a total of 4 min. The data for eyes‐closed were extracted from the continuous recordings.
2.3. EEG processing
In the present study, a set of 12 channels overlapping in both NTR and WUSTL data sets was used: F3/4/7/8, C3/4, P3/4/7/8, and O1/2. All EEG signals were filtered between 1 and 30 Hz, individually inspected, and periods with artifacts were removed. Channels were excluded if artifact removal reduced the length of the channel signal to below the minimum total length of 90 s. EOG artifacts were removed using independent component analysis (ICA) (Delorme & Makeig, 2004), and the cleaned EEG data were partitioned into 2s epochs. Next, the signals were converted from the time domain into the frequency domain using fast Fourier transformation. The resulting power spectrum was divided into bins of 1 Hz, ranging from 1–24 Hz (24 bins).
GNASA and NTR samples used different apparatus and acquisition parameters (specifically, the use of different time constants and hardware low‐pass filters during recording), which could lead to age‐correlated differences in power scores which could be capitalized by the machine learning algorithms for classification. We expected these spurious effects to be minimal, as the largest age range recordings (NTR childhood and adolescents, age range 5–18 years) are the most informative for the machine learning algorithms, and these recordings were made on the same apparatus. Nevertheless, we removed any remaining apparatus effects by removing power differences between the GNASA and NTR. Since apparatus was also confounded with age, we decided to find age‐matched pairs of individuals in the GNASA and WUSTL data sets (N = 30 each, mean age = 16.3). PSDs were obtained for each channel, and the difference obtained between GNASA and WUSTL. These differences were then averaged across channels, since effects in apparatus were not expected to change between channels with the same settings. The averaged difference was used to correct the WUSTL power values for each 1 Hz power bin. The results were not critically affected by the removal of the apparatus/cohort effect; however, MAE increased by approximately 0.10 years. Figure S1 shows the average power spectra for these participants (NTR vs. GNASA). The corrected power spectral densities for representative leads (F3 and O2) and for each wave are provided in Figure S2, Supporting Information. All subsequent analyses used the corrected power spectra.
2.4. Machine learning analyses
To estimate brain maturational level, the three most common machine learning algorithms in previous studies were applied (see Table S1); random forest (RF), support vector machine (SVM), and relevance vector machine (RVM) using power in 1 Hz wide bins from 1 to 24 Hz, as input features to the machine learning models. For both cohorts, each bin was corrected for sex difference by subtracting the difference from the female group to match the males for each power bin.
2.4.1. Random forest
RF is one of the most popular machine learning methods for classification and regression (Breiman, 2001). RF creates a large number of decision trees using various bootstrapped subsamples of the data and features, a so‐called RF. To classify a new data vector based on attributes, each tree gives a classification and the tree “votes” for that class. The forest chooses the classification having the most votes.
The regression extension of RF works similarly, but additionally assigns a value for the outcome variable whenever a decision falls below or under a certain threshold. RF improves the predictive accuracy over standard regression in cross‐validation, controls for overfitting, and naturally allows for interactions between the features. In this study, the number of trees was fixed at 500 trees. The number of predictors was set to 40. None of these numbers were critical from 500+ trees and ~15+ predictors. Below these values, variability in prediction started to appear.
2.4.2. Support vector machine
In classification SVM, the feature data points are projected on a high‐dimensional space. The classification groups are then separated using a hyperplane with shared hyperparameters; variables such as α, which control the distribution of model parameters (Vapnik, 1998). The hyperplane is formed by the so‐called support vectors, which are data points that are highly informative on the separation between classes, and close to the decision boundary. This supervised method aims to find the hyperplane that provides the largest margin between data points in the support vectors that fall into different classes. SVM has become an important classifier over recent years.
2.4.3. Relevance vector machine
RVM is an extension of SVM (Tipping, 2000; Vapnik, 1998). RVM utilizes a Bayesian approach to increase sparseness in the prediction. The approach aids machine learning with highly correlated predictors (as is likely to be the case in EEG data.) RVM uses a probabilistic measure to define the separation space; it imposes an explicit zero‐mean Gaussian prior. The relevance vectors are formed by samples appearing to be more representative of the classes, which are located away from the decision boundary of the classifier, whereas SVM typically uses the samples close to the decision boundary as so‐called support vectors. The key difference in RVM compared to SVM is that a separate hyperparameter is introduced for each of the parameters, instead of a single shared hyperparameter. When the evidence concerning these hyperparameters is maximized, a significant proportion of them go to infinity and play no role in the prediction of the model (Bishop, 2006). Therefore, the RVM is a sparse classifier as the decision function depends on fewer input data that a comparable SVM. This sparsity may lead to a faster performance on training data and results of more generalizable results by decreasing the overfitting, which can reduce error during cross‐validation.
2.4.4. Validation, classification, and accuracy measures
The performance of the methods was assessed in different ways. All prediction performance measures were estimated using sixfold cross‐validation. In cross‐validation, all data are iteratively split into a training and a testing set. Because of the complex familial (twin) dependence in the input data sets, we first reduced the data set by selecting a single person from each family to avoid family relations between participants in the training and testing data sets. Next, sixfold cross‐validation was applied on the reduced set with each time the regression RVM and RF performed on the test set. Note that we did not use the out‐of‐bag option for RF to maximize comparability with the RVM approach. We then repeated this procedure 12 times for a different set of family members, again selecting only a single person from each family. Finally, all available prediction values were averaged.
Prediction accuracy was determined in several ways. First, we used mean absolute error, defined simply as the sum of the prediction errors divided by the number of recordings/measurements. Next, we assessed the wave‐by‐wave prediction accuracy, that is, we compared the median age to median predicted age per wave (four waves for NTR; three waves for GNASA). This method allows individual age prediction to systematically deviate from actual age without penalty, only assessing the prediction error of the wave medians compared to median actual age. Finally, we assessed longitudinal stability (correlation between waves) of the predicted age. Stability was assessed as the correlation between the prediction errors (estimated minus actual age) of subjects at baseline with their own prediction error at follow‐up. Finally, classification accuracy was assessed for childhood versus puberty/adolescence waves of the NTR data set (cutoff age of 10 years).
To assess the heritability of the age‐prediction errors, we used structural equation modeling (SEM) implemented in R package OpenMx (Boker et al., 2011). The relative contribution of genetic and environmental effects to the total trait variance can be estimated by weighing the contribution of known levels of resemblance to the correlational structure between family members (D. Boomsma, Busjahn, & Peltonen, 2002; Neale and Cardon, 1992). Specifically, additive genetic effects (A) are correlated 1 between monozygotic twin pairs (mz), and on average 0.5 between dizygotic (dz) twins and siblings. Large contributions of additive genetic effects (A), therefore, result in observed twin/sibling correlations close to these levels (r MZ = 1, r DZ = 0.5). Nonadditive genetic effects (D)—effects of genetic dominance or epigenetic effects—are correlated 1 for MZ twins and 0.25 for (r MZ = 1, r DZ = 0.25). Common environmental effects (C)—such as the effects of rearing environment—are shared among all family members (r MZ = 1, r DZ = 1). Large contributions of common environmental effects (C) to the trait variance will result in high correlations equal for MZ and DZ/sibling correlations. Unique environmental effects (E) are the residual variance that cannot be explained by the familial resemblance (A or C). For twin data, unique environmental effects (E) largely reflect the variance not explained by the MZ twin correlation (1‐r MZ). Admixtures of these effects will result in specific correlation patterns based on summing of each effect's theoretical twin/sibling correlations weighted by the contribution of that effect. SEM iteratively searches through these relative contributions comparing the estimated and actual correlations, finishing at the maximally likely solution.
Note that in the current twin design, the contributions of C and D effects are collinear and cannot be estimated simultaneously. The Akaike information criterion was used to decide which variance component (D or C) was used. We fitted ADE or ACE models to the data to estimate the relative proportions of A, C or D, and E effects on the age‐prediction error of the participants. The significance of the effects was determined by fixing the estimated contribution of the effect to zero. Comparing the models with and without the effect results in a difference in likelihood of the models. Twice the difference in likelihood is approximately chi‐square distributed with the reduction in free parameters as the degrees of freedom. From this p‐values are obtained with the number of parameters dropped as degrees of freedom (1 df). We first tested the significance of the D or C effect. If this effect could be dropped, we established significance of heritability (A) by comparing the fit of a model with AE variance components against a model with only E.
3. RESULTS
3.1. Machine learning prediction performance
SVM regression did not perform well in comparison to both RF and RVM on all criteria and was not pursued further. In the full age range from childhood into adolescence, application of RF resulted in the smaller MAE of 1.22 years compared to RVM yielding an MAE of 1.46 years. Both these methods were able to classify childhood versus adolescence of NTR participants well (93.9% for RF, 95.2% for RVM) (see Table 1). Predicted‐age wave medians were plotted against the actual age medians for all seven waves (wave ages 5, 7, 16, and 18 of NTR data and ages 12, 14, and 16 of WUSTL data) (see Figure 1, red [NTR] and green [WUSTL] dots indicates the median predicted against median actual age). The figures reveal that RF is not able to extrapolate beyond the minimum (4.9 years) and maximum actual age (18.5 years), resulting in bounded prediction estimates as evidenced from the very small lower error bar at age 5 and upper error bar at age 18. This phenomenon may have reduced the prediction error for RF compared to RVM, therefore, the lower MAE for RF may not reflect real prediction accuracy,
Table 1.
Type of classification | Child | Adolescence | % of correct classification | Chi‐square | p | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
RF | ||||||
Correct | 736 | 1,768 | 93.9 | >100 | <<.001 | |
Incorrect | 115 | 48 | ||||
RVM | ||||||
Correct | 748 | 1,792 | 95.2 | >100 | <<.001 | |
Incorrect | 91 | 36 |
RF = random forest; RVM = relevance vector machine.
A better criterion for prediction accuracy may be to look at wave centroids (i.e., average prediction age compared to average age within a wave of subjects). These wave centroids showed more error in RF (average absolute error = 0.503) than in RVM (average absolute error = 0.363). The latter showed almost perfect overlap with the perfect prediction line.
3.2. Stability of age prediction error
For each participant, we calculated the age‐prediction error as the deviation between RVM or RF estimation and actual age (see Table 2). We then correlated these between consecutive time‐points across longitudinal measurements. Prediction error stability across time was moderate to high (RF: 0.54 < r < 0.74; RVM: 0.53 < r < 0.67).
Table 2.
NTR (95% CI) | GNASA (95% CI) | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Age range | 5–7 | 16–18 | 12–14 | 14–16 | |
RF | 0.54 (0.47, 0.62) | 0.57 (0.50, 0.65) | 0.73 (0.68, 0.80) | 0.74 (0.61, 0.75) | |
RVM | 0.57 (0.48, 0.63) | 0.53 (0.47, 0.62) | 0.66 (0.60, 0.74) | 0.67 (0.54, 0.70) |
CI = confidence interval; RF = random forest; RVM = relevance vector machine.
3.3. Heritability of age prediction error
Age prediction errors from the RVM predictions were entered into univariate SEMs with age and sex covariates. Common environmental (C) and nonadditive/dominant genetic effects (D) were not significant for any of the models (p > .080), and were subsequently dropped. The best fitting models fitted only additive genetic (A) and unique environmental (E) effects. Heritability (h 2) is then defined as the proportion of variance explained by A to the total variance (A + E). For most waves, h 2 was substantial (h 2 > 50%) (see Table 3). For NTR age 18, the heritability was moderate (h 2 = 42.7%). All heritabilities were highly significant (p < .001).
Table 3.
Age in years | h 2 (%) | (95% CI) | p |
---|---|---|---|
NTR | |||
5 | 50.8 | (37.7, 66.6) | <.001 |
7 | 62.8 | (53.3, 74.5) | <.001 |
16 | 52.8 | (39.8, 65.2) | <.001 |
18 | 42.7 | (28.9, 60.4) | <.001 |
GNASA | |||
12 | 78.9 | (68.7, 85.6) | <.001 |
14 | 78.8 | (70.8, 84.6) | <.001 |
16 | 77.5 | (64.5, 84.0) | <.001 |
CI = confidence interval; GNASA = Genetics, Neurocognition and Adolescence Substance Abuse; SEMs = structural equation models.
Note. Heritability was estimated in with additive genetic (A) and unique environmental effects (E). Age and sex were used as covariates. Significance was determined by likelihood ratio test with 1 df.
3.4. Genetic correlations across waves
We calculated the genetic correlations across time points for consecutive waves with sample overlap. These results are shown in Table 4. The results show that the genetic overlap of the heritable variance of the prediction error is very substantial (r G > 0.77), but significantly different from unity.
Table 4.
r G (95% CI) | |
---|---|
RF | |
Age 5–7 | 0.91 (0.75, 1.00) |
Age 16–18 | 0.92 (0.70, 1.00) |
Age 12–14 | 0.83 (0.75, 0.90) |
Age 14–16 | 0.81 (0.72, 0.88) |
RVM | |
Age 5–7 | 0.86 (0.75, 1.00) |
Age 16–18 | 0.87 (0.69, 1.00) |
Age 12–14 | 0.77 (0.65, 0.87) |
Age 14–16 | 0.80 (0.68, 0.91) |
CI = confidence interval; RF = random forest; RVM = relevance vector machine.
3.5. Contributing features in RF
In order to investigate the contribution of each feature to the predictive model, we performed analysis of feature importance using the RF regression only, because the random feature selection during each regression tree allows each feature to obtain a feature importance score. The lack of randomizing feature in RVM and the high collinearity will obscure the importance of some features in RVM, even if they are nearly as good in predicting age as the highly collinear power values at nearby electrodes and/or nearby frequencies.
Figure 2 shows the log‐transformed feature importance averaged across different brain regions. The most contributing frequencies were identified at lower frequencies (delta). Other contributing frequencies were lower alpha, which changed in power but also reflected increased alpha peak frequency (see Figure S2). In addition, beta frequency power contributed to age predictions. For these frequencies, topographic plots of the feature importance from RF models for these frequencies are shown in Figure 3. These indicate that the best regression was obtained with central delta, frontal lower alpha, parietal alpha, frontal lower beta, and occipital upper beta. These observations are largely consistent with the known developmental patterns in these regions (Bresnahan et al., 1999; Matousek & Petersen, 1973a, 1973b).
4. DISCUSSION
The results showed that brain maturational level can be estimated based on 3 min resting‐state EEG recordings with high accuracy. Comparison of our outcomes based on EEG with age estimation from MRI revealed comparable results. Childhood versus puberty/adolescent classification accuracy in MRI studies ranged from 75 to 95% (Franke et al., 2012). With EEG, we obtained classification accuracy of childhood versus puberty/adolescence that was even higher (>95% for RVM). The mean prediction error of 1.22 for RF and 1.46 for RVM are only slightly higher than the lowest prediction errors estimates obtained in MRI studies (1.1 years) in comparable age groups. The analysis of feature importance in RF showed that classification depended mostly on low‐frequency power; further notable contributors to the prediction were temporal theta, frontal lower alpha waves.
The RVM and RF machine learning methods were comparable in their results. One limitation of RF is that it does not give fully continuous predicted ages, and cannot extrapolate outside the input age range. RF prediction performance may have been artificially decreased by this capping of the output values. RVM, on the other hand, does extrapolate, which resulted in higher mean prediction error, but also yielded illogical age estimates (even below zero for two participants), suggesting that the remaining age‐prediction error has variability that cannot logically be attributed to brain maturation. On the other hand, RVM did provide the best fit of age‐group median centroids, outperforming RF. In our view, these results suggest that sparser models such as the RVM are superior for age prediction using EEG power, even though remaining error variance is likely to exist.
The underestimation or overestimation relative to actual age indicates that some children and adolescents exhibit EEG brain activity patterns more like those of younger or older ages. This study is the first to show that this prediction error was systematic, and moderately to highly stable over a period of 1.5 years (0.54 < r < 0.74 for RF and 0.53 < r < 0.67 for RVM). In addition, age‐prediction error was to a large extent heritable, ranging from 43 to 79%. No significant effects of shared environment or nonadditive genetic effects—that is, the interactions among alleles both within and across gene loci—were found, suggesting that the systematic deviation is largely genetically determined, but with substantial unique environmental influences that include nonstable error estimates. By and large, the same genetic influences determined age‐prediction error in consecutive age groups; genetic correlations ranged from 0.77 to 0.89. This further adds to the stable character of the age‐prediction error.
The stability and heritability scores of the GNASA sample were significantly higher than those of the NTR sample as evidenced from the 95% confidence intervals. This we explain by the fact that the NTR sample has the largest age range and were from an identical population as well as identically measured. The RF and RVM models will largely be shaped by this cohort's extensive age range. Any population differences or subtle apparatus or measurement effects between the NTR and GNASA cohorts will cause the model to be slightly suboptimal for the GNASA sample with a more restricted age range. Applying such a suboptimal model may result in several effects, thus including additional signal to the predicted age that depends in part on heritable traits, viz., power in specific frequencies. However, GNASA should in this case also show larger prediction error. This was, however, not the case. These differences may therefore reflect true population or sample differences between the cohorts.
Although it has yet to be determined whether these stable and heritable deviations are predictive of behavioral traits and/or neurodevelopmental disorders, the fact that they do not fully reflect unstable variance due to measurement noise or model misspecification indicates that prediction error may be a good candidate for predicting stable individual differences in neurodevelopment, cognition, and neurodevelopmental disorders. Previous research has suggested that scoring high or low on selected behavioral phenotypes (viz., IQ and ADHD) are associated with slower or faster brain‐maturation trajectories (P. Shaw et al., 2006, Shaw et al., 2007, Shaw et al., 2012; Sowell, 2004). These studies were the first to suggest that brain maturational level estimated from brain parameters is a property correlated with behavioral outcomes. These neurodevelopmental phenotypes are therefore prime candidates to link to the EEG‐based predicted age. Moreover, attention problems and IQ are to a large extent genetically determined (Derks et al., 2008; Posthuma, Mulder, Boomsma, & De Geus, 2002).
Overall, our results show that age predictions from low‐cost EEG recordings can be performed with a precision comparable to predictions obtained from MRI in an age range from childhood to adolescence. In addition, we have shown that the prediction error is not random noise, but moderately stable over a period of about 1.5 years, and to a large extent influenced by the genetic background of the subject. These findings clear the way for EEG‐based age prediction as a marker of brain maturation and investigation of its relation with (genetically mediated) neurodevelopmental phenotypes, such as cognitive performance and ADHD. In clinical practice, age prediction—and especially the systematic deviation from actual age—may prove to become a valuable biomarker for neuropsychiatric disorders related to abnormal brain development, or normal behavioral outcomes. In parallel with the epigenetic clock (Horvath, 2013; Jones, Goodman, & Kobor, 2015; Jylhävä, Pedersen, & Hägg, 2017), EEG‐estimated brain maturation might become a tool that address questions concerning developmental trajectories. Some studies have shown a relation between brain maturational level and behavioral phenotypes (ADHD, impulsivity, and IQ; Shaw et al., 2007, 2012; Yang et al., 2015). These studies showed that ADHD and superior IQ both were related to a delay in maturation. Future studies may show that this relation can also be captured by EEG‐estimated brain maturation, and could therefore be an important additional predictor of scholastic achievement and attention problems.
Supporting information
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
This research was supported by grants BBR Foundation (NARSAD) Young Investigator grant 21668 and NWO/MagWVENI‐451‐08‐026 to D.S.; National Institutes of Health DA018899 and DA027096 to A.P.A.
Vandenbosch MMLJZ, van 't Ent D, Boomsma DI, Anokhin AP, Smit DJA. EEG‐based age‐prediction models as stable and heritable indicators of brain maturational level in children and adolescents. Hum Brain Mapp. 2019;40:1919–1926. 10.1002/hbm.24501
Funding information BBR Foundation (NARSAG), Grant/Award Number: Young Investigator grant 21668; NWO/MagWVENI, Grant/Award Number: 541‐08‐026; National Institutes of Health, Grant/Award Number: DA018899, DA027096
REFERENCES
- Anderson, V. , Northam, E. , Hendy, J. , & Wrennall, J. (2001). Developmental neuropsychology: A clinical approach. [Google Scholar]
- Barriga‐Paulino, C. , Flores, A. , & Gomez, C. (2011). Developmental changes in the EEG rhythms of developmental changes in the EEG rhythms of children and young adults analyzed by means of correlational, brain topography and principal component analysis. Journal of Psychophysiology, 25(3), 143–158. 10.1027/0269-8803/a000052 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Benniger, C. , Matthis, P. , & Scheffner, D. (1984). EEG development of healthy boys and girls: Results of a longitudinal study. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 57(1), 1–12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bishop, C. M. (2006). Pattern recognition and machine learning. Journal of Chemical Information and Modeling, 53, 049901 10.1117/1.2819119 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Boker, S. , Neale, M. , Maes, H. , Wilde, M. , Spiegel, M. , Brick, T. , … Fox, J. (2011). OpenMx: An open source extended structural equation modeling framework. Psychometrika, 76(2), 306–317. 10.1007/s11336-010-9200-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boomsma, D. , Busjahn, A. , & Peltonen, L. (2002). Classical twin studies and beyond. Nature Reviews Genetics, 3(11), 872–882. 10.1038/nrg932 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boomsma, D. I. , Vink, J. M. , van Beijsterveldt, T. C. , de Geus, E. J. , Beem, A. L. , Mulder, E. J. , … van Baal, G. C. (2002). Netherlands twin register: A focus on longitudinal research. Twin Research, 5(5), 401–406. 10.1375/136905202320906174 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Breiman, L. (2001). Random forests. Machine Learning, 45(1), 5–32. 10.1023/A:1010933404324 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Bresnahan, S. M. , Anderson, J. W. , & Barry, R. J. (1999). Age‐related changes in quantitative EEG in attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 46(12), 1690–1697. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brown, T. T. , Kuperman, J. M. , Chung, Y. , Erhart, M. , McCabe, C. , Hagler, D. J. , … Dale, A. M. (2012). Neuroanatomical assessment of biological maturity. Current Biology, 22(18), 1693–1698. 10.1016/j.cub.2012.07.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Geus, E. J. C. (2010). From genotype to EEG endophenotype: A route for post‐genomic understanding of complex psychiatric disease? Genome Medicine, 2, 63 10.1186/gm184 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Delorme, A. , & Makeig, S. (2004). EEGLAB: An open source toolbox for analysis of single‐trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. Journal of Neurscience Methods, 134, 9–21. 10.1016/j.virol.2015.08.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Derks, E. M. , Hudziak, J. J. , Dolan, C. V. , Van Beijsterveldt, T. C. E. M. , Verhulst, F. C. , & Boomsma, D. I. (2008). Genetic and environmental influences on the relation between attention problems and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Behavior Genetics, 38(1), 11–23. 10.1007/s10519-007-9178-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Durston, S. , & Casey, B. J. (2006). What have we learned about cognitive development from neuroimaging? Neuropsychologia, 44(11), 2149–2157. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.10.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fischer, K. W. , & Silvern, L. , (1985). Stages and individual differences in cognitive development. Annual Review of Psychology, 36(1), 613–648. 10.1146/annurev.ps.36.020185.003145 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Franke, K. , Luders, E. , May, A. , Wilke, M. , & Gaser, C. (2012). Brain maturation: Predicting individual BrainAGE in children and adolescents using structural MRI. NeuroImage, 63(3), 1305–1312. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.08.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gasser, T. , Jennen‐Steinmetz, C. , Sroka, L. , Verleger, R. , & Mocks, J. (1988). Development of the EEG of school‐age children and adolescents. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 69(2), 100–109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gasser, T. , Verleger, R. , Bächer, P. , & Sroka, L. (1988). Development of the EEG of school‐age children and adolescents. I. Analysis of band power. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 69(2), 91–99. 10.1016/0013-4694(88)90204-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Giedd, J. N. , Lalonde, F. M. , Celano, M. J. , White, S. L. , Wallace, G. L. , Lee, N. R. , & Lenroot, R. K. (2009). Anatomical brain magnetic resonance imaging of typically developing children and adolescents. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 48(5), 465–470. 10.1097/CHI.0b013e31819f2715 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hedman, A. M. , van Haren, N. E. M. , Schnack, H. G. , Kahn, R. S. , & Hulshoff Pol, H. E. (2012). Human brain changes across the life span: A review of 56 longitudinal magnetic resonance imaging studies. Human Brain Mapping, 33(8), 1987–2002. 10.1002/hbm.21334 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Horvath, S. (2013). DNA methylation age of human tissues and cell types. Genome Biology, 14(10), R115 10.1186/gb-2013-14-10-r115 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huttenlocher, P. R. (1979). Synaptic density in human frontal cortex ‐ developmental changes and effects of aging. Brain Research, 163(2), 195–205. 10.1016/0006-8993(79)90349-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jones, M. J. , Goodman, S. J. , & Kobor, M. S. (2015). DNA methylation and healthy human aging. Aging Cell, 14(6), 924–932. 10.1111/acel.12349 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jylhävä, J. , Pedersen, N. L. , & Hägg, S. (2017). Biological age predictors. eBioMedicine, 21, 29–36. 10.1016/j.ebiom.2017.03.046 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lenroot, R. K. , & Giedd, J. N. (2006). Brain development in children and adolescents: Insights from anatomical magnetic resonance imaging. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 30, 718–729. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2006.06.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Matousek, M. , & Petersen, I. (1973a). Automatic evaluation of EEG background activity by means of age‐dependent EEG quotients. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 35, 603–612. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Matousek, M. , & Petersen, I. (1973b). Frequency analysis of the EEG in normal children and in normal adolescents. In: P. Kellaway and I. Petersen (Eds.), Automation of clinical electroencephalography (pp. 75–102). New York: Raven Press.
- Mills, K. L. , & Tamnes, C. K. (2014). Developmental cognitive neuroscience methods and considerations for longitudinal structural brain imaging analysis across development. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 9, 172–190. 10.1016/j.dcn.2014.04.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Neale, M. C. , & Cardon, L. R. (1992). Methodology for genetic studies of twins and families. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publishers. [Google Scholar]
- Niedermeyer, E. , & Da Silva, F. L. (2005). Maturation of the EEG: Development of waking and sleep patterns. Electroencephalography: Basic principles, clinical applications, and related fields. Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. [Google Scholar]
- Pivik, R. T. , Broughton, R. J. , Coppola, R. , Davidson, R. J. , Fox, N. , & Nuwer, M. R. (1993). Guidelines for the recording and quantitative analysis of electroencephalographic activity in research contexts. Psychophysiology, 30, 547–558. 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1993.tb02081.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Posthuma, D. , Mulder, E. J. C. M. , Boomsma, D. I. , & De Geus, E. J. C. (2002). Genetic analysis of IQ, processing speed and stimulus‐response incongruency effects. Biological Psychology, 61(1–2), 157–182. 10.1016/S0301-0511(02)00057-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Puligheddu, M. , De Munck, J. C. , Stam, C. J. , Verbunt, J. , De Jongh, A. , Van Dijk, B. W. , & Marrosu, F. (2005). Age distribution of MEG spontaneous theta activity in healthy subjects. Brain Topography, 17(3), 165–175. 10.1007/s10548-005-4449-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shaw, P. , Eckstrand, K. , Sharp, W. , Blumenthal, J. , Lerch, J. P. , Greenstein, D. , … Rapoport, J. L. (2007). Attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder is characterized by a delay in cortical maturation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104(49), 19649–19654. 10.1073/pnas.0707741104 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shaw, P. , Greenstein, D. , Lerch, J. , Clasen, L. , Lenroot, R. , Gogtay, N. , … Giedd, J. (2006). Intellectual ability and cortical development in children and adolescents. Nature, 440(7084), 676–679. 10.1038/nature04513 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shaw, P. , Lerch, J. , Greenstein, D. , Sharp, W. , Clasen, L. , Evans, A. , … Rapoport, J. (2006). Longitudinal mapping of cortical thickness and clinical outcome in children and adolescents with attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 63(May 2006), 540–549. 10.1001/archpsyc.63.5.540.ABSTRACT [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shaw, P. , Malek, M. , Watson, B. , Sharp, W. , Evans, A. , & Greenstein, D. (2012). Development of cortical surface area and gyrification in attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 72(3), 191–197. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.01.031 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smit, D. J. A. , Boersma, M. , Schnack, H. G. , Micheloyannis, S. , Boomsma, D. I. , Hulshoff Pol, H. E. , … de Geus, E. J. C. (2012). The brain matures with stronger functional connectivity and decreased randomness of its network. PLoS ONE, 7(5), e36896 10.1371/journal.pone.0036896 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smit, D. J. A. , De Geus, E. J. C. , Boersma, M. , Boomsma, D. I. , & Stam, C. J. (2016). Life‐span development of brain network integration assessed with phase lag index connectivity and minimum spanning tree graphs. Brain Connectivity, 6(4), 312–325. 10.1089/brain.2015.0359 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smit, D. J. A. , Posthuma, D. , Boomsma, D. I. , & Geus, E. J. C. D. E. (2005). Heritability of background EEG across the power spectrum. Psychophysiology, 42, 691–697. 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2005.00352.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sowell, E. R. (2004). Longitudinal mapping of cortical thickness and brain growth in Normal children. Journal of Neuroscience, 24(38), 8223–8231. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1798-04.2004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tipping, M. E. (2000). The relevance vector machine. In S. A. Solla, T. K. Leen, & K. R. Müller (Eds.), Advances in neural information processing system (Vol. 12, pp. 652–658). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
- Van Baal, G. C. M. , de Geus, E. J. C. , & Boomsma, D. I. (1996). Genetic architecture of EEG power spectra in early life. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 98, 502–514. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Beijsterveldt, C. E. M. , Groen‐Blokhuis, M. , Hottenga, J. J. , Franić, S. , Hudziak, J. J. , Lamb, D. , … Boomsma, D. I. (2013). The young Netherlands twin register (YNTR): Longitudinal twin and family studies in over 70,000 children. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 16(1), 252–267. 10.1017/thg.2012.118 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vapnik, V. N. (1998). Statistical learning theory. New York, NY: Wiley; 10.2307/1271368 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Walhovd, K. B. , Tamnes, C. K. , & Fjell, A. M. (2014). Brain structural maturation and the foundations of cognitive behavioral development. Current Opinion in Neurology, 27(2), 176–184. 10.1097/WCO.0000000000000074 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yang, Y. , Wang, P. , Baker, L. A. , Narr, K. L. , Joshi, S. H. , Hafzalla, G. , … Thompson, P. M. (2015). Thicker temporal cortex associates with a developmental trajectory for psychopathic traits in adolescents. PLoS One, 10(5), 1–15. 10.1371/journal.pone.0127025 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.