Abstract
A systematic review was undertaken to identify intervention characteristics associated with increasing consumption of vegetables in children (2–12 years). PubMed, PsychINFO and CABabstracts were used to identify studies published between 2004–2014 that had measures of vegetable consumption, a minimum of 3‐month follow‐up and were conducted in home and community settings (outside of schools). Twenty‐two studies were included in the review. Details of the study design, population, setting, intervention characteristics, target behaviour, behaviour change techniques used and vegetable intake were extracted. Study quality and intensity were scored. Overall, 12/22 studies were effective short‐term, and 6/10 were effective long‐term (6 + months); mean short‐term change in vegetable intake was 29%, equating to an increase of a quarter to a half of a serving of vegetables. Intervention effectiveness was associated with number of settings targeted and frequency of contact but not length of intervention. Planning for social support, vegetable exposure and provision of staff training were commonly used behaviour change techniques in effective interventions. This review has identified strategies that may optimise effectiveness of future home‐based and community‐based interventions aiming to increase vegetable intake in young children.
Keywords: vegetables, vegetable intake, child, home, community, intervention
Introduction
Vegetables are an important source of dietary fibre, provide a range of nutrients and have a low energy density. Dietary guidelines recommend we consume plenty of vegetables, including a range of different types and colours (U.S. Department of Agriculture and U.S. Department of Health and Human Services 2010; National Health and Medical Research Council 2013). Vegetable intake is protective against chronic diseases including heart disease and some cancers (Boeing et al. 2012) and may help to reduce the risk of obesity (National Health and Medical Research Council 2013). Despite the compelling public health benefits, dietary surveys in large population groups report that children do not meet vegetable intake recommendations (Yngve et al. 2005; Magarey et al. 2006; Kimmons et al. 2009; Australian Bureau of Statistics 2014). Critically, the prevalence of inadequate vegetable consumption increases as children transition from their early years through to adolescence, coinciding with higher intake recommendations. In Australia, the 2011–2013 Australian Health Survey reported that 49% of children aged 2–3 years met the recommended usual intake of vegetables, compared with less than 10% of children aged 4 to 18 years (Australian Bureau of Statistics 2014). This pattern of low vegetable intake in children is particularly concerning given that establishing appropriate healthy eating behaviours early in childhood influences the formation of eating patterns later in life (Maynard et al. 2006).
Numerous studies have aimed to increase children's vegetable consumption; however, systematic reviews of these report limited effectiveness (Knai et al. 2006; Evans et al. 2012), particularly in young (Wolfenden et al. 2012) and overweight children (Bourke et al. 2014). A comprehensive review of primary school‐based initiatives that promoted fruit and vegetable intake reported a small increase in children's self‐reported intake (+0.3 to +0.99 servings); however, the majority of this modest increase was attributed to fruit not vegetables (Knai et al. 2006). A more recent meta‐analysis that focussed on school‐based interventions in children aged 5–12 years reported a small net effect for self‐reported fruit intake (+0.24 servings) but virtually no increase in vegetable consumption (+0.07 servings) (Evans et al. 2012). Given children's low intake of vegetables and the differences in the context for vegetable consumption compared with fruit (Australian Bureau of Statistics 2014), it is important that interventions specifically targeting vegetable consumption are better understood.
In addition to specifically measuring the impact of interventions on vegetable intake, it is important to identify the strategies associated with changing the eating habits in children and describe ‘how’ initiatives were undertaken. Previous reviews of interventions to increase vegetable intake in children have focussed on the magnitude of change and overall effectiveness, with little detail on the components of the intervention per se. Knai et al qualitatively observed that increased exposure to fruit and vegetables was the key intervention component common to effective studies (Knai et al. 2006); however, a systematic critique of intervention components was not conducted. Recently, Diep et al. (2014) conducted a meta‐analysis to determine whether interventions based on a theoretical framework were more effective at increasing children's fruit and vegetable intakes, than interventions with no theoretical underpinning (Diep et al. 2014). They reported that basing an intervention on behavioural theory has a small to moderate enhancement on effectiveness, and after controlling for study quality, this effect was greatest for vegetable consumption (compared with fruit or combined fruit and vegetables) (Diep et al. 2014). Further exploration of how interventions are undertaken (i.e. the strategies associated with successful interventions) and identification of behaviour change techniques (BCT) that may be associated with increased vegetable intake in children is warranted.
Therefore, a key aim of the current review was to identify the intervention characteristics and BCTs associated with increasing children's vegetable consumption. The review was focused upon studies targeting young children (2–12 years) to address the decline in intake that has been observed throughout childhood, to target an age range where children are more malleable and when establishing food acceptance may influence future eating habits (Birch & Ventura 2009; Anzman et al. 2010). The wider age group allowed us to include all children of pre‐school and primary school age (2–12 years in Australia) but excluded adolescents who have greater autonomy over their eating habits and a higher vegetable intake recommendation. Given our target age range and the scope of previous reviews (Knai et al. 2006; Evans et al. 2012), this review focused on interventions delivered in home or community settings (community centres, childcare centres, kindergartens, afterschool care settings and internet based programs); i.e. outside of the school setting.
Key messages.
This review identified 22 prospective studies that reported vegetable intakes among children aged 2 –12 years in the home and community settings.
Overall, 12/22 studies were effective short‐term (3 months follow‐up), and 6/10 were effective longer term (6 months or more).
Mean short‐term change in vegetable intake was ~30%, which based on current intake equates to an average quarter to a half a serve increase.
The design and behavioural change techniques associated with effectiveness were identified, including planning for social support, vegetable exposure and provision of staff training.
Methods
This systematic review was carried out and reported in accordance with the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta‐analyses (PRISMA) 2009 Checklist (Supplementary Appendix).
Search method for identification and screening of studies
PubMed, PsychINFO and CABabstracts were searched for scientific literature published between 2004 and June 2014 to identify intervention studies that targeted vegetable consumption in children aged 2–12 years. Only peer reviewed published scientific papers were included in the search; unpublished work or ‘grey’ literature was excluded. The initial search included the following MeSH (medical subject headings) terms and combinations thereof under the three headings: (1) interventions: research design, controlled clinical trial, randomised controlled trials, random allocation, double‐blind method, single‐blind method, placebos, intervention studies, evaluation studies, comparative study, follow‐up studies, prospective studies, cross‐over studies, clinical trial, latin square, time series, study, studies, trial, random, RCT, matched communities, matched schools, matched populations, control, comparison group, control group, comparative study, matched pairs, outcome study, outcome studies, quasi experimental, pseudo experimental, nonrandomi, non randomi, pseudo randomi, quasi randomi, prospective, volunteer, experimental, intervention, model, models, evaluation, and cross‐over; (2) vegetable intake: vegetables, vegetable, vege; (3) children (2–12 years): child, adolescent, pre‐school, schoolchildren, school children, pediatr, boys, girls, youth, young people, infant, and toddler.
After removal of duplicate records, four investigators (GH, DB, JB & HL) independently analysed the title and abstracts of each paper retrieved in the initial search to identify eligible studies. Studies not meeting the inclusion criteria (below) were excluded. The remaining papers were obtained, and a final full‐text analysis to identify eligible studies was performed. The screening process and article selection is summarised in (Fig. 1).
Figure 1.
Summary of the screening and article selection process.
Inclusion criteria
Studies were eligible for inclusion if they met all of the following criteria: prospective studies evaluating the effectiveness of an intervention, with or without comparison groups; intervention aimed at improving vegetable consumption, either alone or in combination with other healthy eating and lifestyle messages; had a quantitative measure of vegetable consumption separate to other food groups (such as fruit), reported at an individual or group level in either grams, servings, frequency of consumption (times per day), provision (number of vegetables available or served) or purchase habits (shopping receipts); the measure of vegetable consumption was reported as an estimate of ‘usual’ vegetable intake (not intake of a test dish/meal); the intervention was delivered in the home or a community setting including community centres, childcare, kindergarten and afterschool‐care settings or as part of government initiatives; the intervention targeted children aged 2–12 years or had a mean population age of ≥2 years and ≤ 12 years; the outcome was reported at three months post intervention or more (to assess sustained behaviour change and habit formation); and was published in English between 2004 and 2014 in a peer reviewed journal.
Exclusion criteria
Studies targeting specific subgroups of the population such as clinical populations, case studies and children with specific medical conditions were excluded to make the findings relevant to the general population. Similarly, studies reporting intake measures of test vegetable dishes/meals or single vegetables were also excluded as these were not estimates of ‘usual’ consumption and therefore not reflective of the child's usual vegetable intake. Interventions delivered in schools (i.e. with or without a home or community component) were excluded as these have been the focus of another recent review (Evans et al. 2012).
Data extraction and synthesis
Details were extracted for the study design, population, setting, intervention characteristics, target behaviour, behaviour change techniques used and vegetable intake. Two reviewers (DLB and JB) independently extracted the data from papers including study design, intervention duration, setting (i.e. home, community, pre‐school/childcare, afterschool programs) and target (i.e. intervention recipient; children, carers, parents), population (including sample size, age and gender of children), intervention characteristics, contact with intervention and behavioural target of the intervention (i.e. improving vegetable intake only, fruit and vegetables, a healthy diet or diet and lifestyle), data time points and vegetable intake pre‐intervention and post‐intervention. Short‐term (>3 months) and longer term (>6 months; data available for 10 of 22 studies) effectiveness were defined as a significant (P < 0.05) increase in vegetable consumption as reported by the author. Refer to the Supplementary Appendix for the full comprehensive data extraction table. Each study was extracted by a single author. A second author reviewed 20% of studies to ensure accuracy of data extraction and coding. Any inconsistencies were resolved by discussion with a third author (GH).
A number of study design and intervention components were coded using published protocols as detailed in the succeeding sections. An assessment of intervention intensity enabled a comparison of results between all studies despite their varied study design and settings. The intensity rating was based on a Cochrane review of community wide interventions for increasing physical activity (Baker et al. 2011; Francis et al. 2011). Four aspects of the intervention were coded, each scored on a 5‐point scale (1 = low intensity to 5 = high intensity) with a low, medium and high ranking on each component as follows:
Duration of intervention: <6 weeks = 1, 12 weeks to < 6 months = 3 and >12 months = 5.
Contact with intervention: yearly = 1, monthly = 3 and daily = 5.
Type or level of contact: physical, policy or legislative or no face‐to‐face = 1, group level = 3 and individual contact = 5.
Reach (number of settings targeted): 1 setting = 1, 2 settings = 3 and ≥3 settings = 5.
An overall intensity score out of 20 was calculated. Based on the range of scores in this review and our previous use of the tool (Hendrie et al. 2012), scores ≤10 were considered lower intensity, 11–12 were medium intensity and ≥ 13 considered higher intensity.
Study quality was scored using the standard published protocol for the Effective Public Health Practice Project quality assessment tool (Thomas 2003; Thomas et al. 2004a). Eight components were scored (selection bias, study design, confounders, blinding, data collection methods, withdrawals and drop outs, intervention integrity and analyses) from which an overall quality rating was calculated (strong/moderate/weak) (Thomas 2003; Thomas et al. 2004a).
The Behaviour Change Techniques used in the interventions were double coded by a health psychology researcher (HL) using standardised definitions outlined in the CALO‐RE taxonomy of BCTs (Abraham & Michie 2008; Michie et al. 2011). The taxonomy defines 40 BCTs with standard definitions provided in a coding manual. Six additional categories were added to the original taxonomy to account for unique, recurring techniques used in the interventions in this review; tasting and exposure techniques, provision of resources, staff training, community/participant engagement, economic incentives, increasing availability and accessibility within the local environment through, for example, community gardens.
A meta‐analysis of the magnitude of change in vegetable intake was not possible because the methodologies used to measure intake of vegetables were heterogeneous between studies (e.g. number of serves/day, grams consumed) and was only available for 17 out of the 22 studies. However to have some indication of intervention impact, the change in vegetable intake was calculated as the percentage increase/decrease in the unit reported, regardless of significance levels.
Results
Describing the studies overall
The 22 papers included in this review were published between 2004 and 2014 and are summarised in Table 1. Complete data extraction is available in the Supplementary Table. The interventions ranged in length from a single nutrition education newsletter mail out to a 3 year community garden‐based intervention. Seven interventions were home‐based, and the remainder were either set in pre‐school or childcare (n = 6), after‐school programs (n = 4) or community programs (n = 5). They targeted vegetable intake only (n = 4), intake of fruit and vegetables (n = 6), vegetables as a part of a healthy diet (n = 6) and as part of a healthy lifestyle (n = 6). Overall, 12 of 22 studies (54%) were considered effective in increasing children's vegetable intake in the short‐term, and six of ten (60%) were considered effective longer term. Most studies (n = 15) had a medium intensity rating, with five rated low and two rated high intensity. Fourteen studies had a weak quality rating, and seven were rated as moderate quality, with only one paper rated as strong quality (See Table 1 & Supplementary Table for full details extracted from each study).
Table 1.
Description of studies included in the review that assess the impact of an intervention of vegetable intake in the home and community settings
Reference | Study design; duration; setting; target | Population: sample size (N); Age M ± SD (range); % male | Intervention description; contact with intervention | Data time points; Comp; CG | How was vege intake measured? | Intake pre‐intervention | Intake post‐intervention | Intake at Follow‐up | Change in vege consumption |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Annesi et al. 2009 | Cohort; 12 weeks; ASP; children | 43 9 ± 1.3 years (7–12 years) 49% | Youth fit for life program included PA, behavioural skills training, health and nutrition education. Contact: 3 × 45 min sessions/week (20 min Cardio PA/session), 2 × 20 min/week strength training, 20 min × 1/week behavioural skills, health and nutrition info 5–7 min/session) | TP: baseline, post‐I Comp: not stated CG: none | FFQ (2 items): frequency/week | 9.93 (SD ± 5.15) | 12.47 (SD ± 7.21) | — | t 42 = 2.12, P = 0.02, d = .49 |
Bayer et al. 2009 | Cluster RCT; 2 years; Kindy; Children & teachers & parents | 64 kindy's, 1605 children 6 ± 0.4 years (5–6 years) 52–64% | ‘TigerKids’ behavioural intervention targeted at: PA games at kindergarten, F&V consumption and habit formation of ≥2 FV portions/day & drinking water in kindy. Information materials & day‐to‐day activities to teachers, phone hotline for teacher support, information for parents, internet platform with info for teachers & families & shared FV plates offered. Contact: ~5 h/week | TP: baseline, 3–9 m, 12–20 m Comp: not stated CG: usual care | FFQ: portions/day converted to % high V consumer ≥2portions | — |
# of high vege cons umers' sample 1: I: 323, 38.6% (35.3–42.0) C: 155, 33.9% (29.6–38.5) P = 0.0960 sample 2: I: 366, 42.7% (39.4 to 46.1) C: 151, 33.6% (29.2 to 38.1), P = 0.0013 |
—0 | Intervention effects Sample 1: OR 1.26 (0.98–1.61) Sample 2: OR 1.48 (1.08–2.03) |
Castro et al., 2013 | Cohort; 3 years; community; families | 120 (60 families) 6 ± 3.4 years (2–15 years) 49% | Growing healthy kids program: weekly sessions in community garden (staff assisted with garden preparation, planting etc), 7‐week cooking & nutrition workshop (information & resources for healthy food choices), social events for families and garden newsletter. Contact: Weekly | TP: Baseline, 1, 2 & 3 years comp: family gardening participation 45% weekly, 45% 2–3×/month, 7%1/month, 3% no attendance data. CG: none | Survey (parent report): V variety available at home; usual servings consumeday/weekday | Availability: 3.5 ± 2.06 usual intake: 2.1 ± 0.96 | Availability: 7.8 ± 1.80 usual intake: 2.8 ± 1.28 | — | Availability: 4.3 ± 1.82 (123% increase, t = 16.37, df = 47, P < 0.001) Usual intake: 33% increase (t = 3.17, df = 45, P < 0.001) |
Corsini et al. 2011 | RCT; 2 weeks; home; parents, children | 185 children & caregiver (4–6 years) 59% | 3 groups: EO, exposure + reward (E + R) & control. EO group required parent to present & ask child to taste small piece of target vege daily for 2 weeks. E + R group same as EO procedure + 14‐day sticker reward chart for tasting the vege. Contact: short daily activity | TP: Baseline, post‐I (2 & 4 weeks, & 3 months) Comp: 94% returned exposure diary. 86% offered target V on ≥ 10 occasions. 56% children achieved 10 taste exposures. CG: normal feeding behaviour | Target V: g consumed usual intake: FFQ f/day Variety: number V/week | — | — | — | Target V intake: All increased from baseline (X 2(1) = 22.98,P < 0.001) E + R increased post‐I to 3 months (change 6.47, SE 2.26, P = 0.013) & 4 weeks to 4 months (change 7.23, SE 1.49, P < 0.001) C: increased Post‐I to 3 months (change 3.51, SE1.22, P = 0.012) usual V intake: variety increased with time (F(2,126) = 30.23, P < 0.001) frequency, E + R increased from baseline to 4 weeks (change 0.32, SE0.12, P < 0.5) |
Davis, 2011 | Pilot cluster RCT; 12 weeks; ASP; childre and, parents | 104 9.8 ± 0.7 years 52% | Cooking /nutrition education, gardening lessons & market garden visits + 60 min parent nutrition & gardening class Contact: 90 min/week | TP: Baseline, 1‐week post‐I Comp: parent component – 25% participation CG: abbreviated delayed intervention after post‐testing of IG. | FFQ (41 items): servings/day (previous day) | I:1.6 ± 1.0 C:1.9 ± 1.3 | I:1.6 ± 1.0 C:1.3 ± 1.0 | ‐ | I v C, P = 0.11 |
De Bock, 2011 | Cluster RCT; 6 months; pre‐school; children; parents | 377 (18 pre‐schools) 4.26 ± 0.78 years (3–6 years) 53.2% | Nutrition intervention: education on different foods, preparing, cooking, shared meals, parent education (modelling & child nutrition needs), interactive play, active parent participation & peer interaction. FV & water offered every week to increase exposure. Contact: 2 hours/week × 15 nutrition sessions (10 children only, 5 parents only or parent & child) | TP: baseline, 6 & 12 months Comp: not stated CG: waiting list control arm (received the intervention 6 months later). | FFQ: portions/day | 34.6% achieved recommended daily vege intakes | — | — | Intervention effect higher post intervention unadjusted: +0.22 points P < =0.01 Adjusted: +0.15 points, P = 0.027 **a change of 1 point = ~ 1 portion |
Engels et al. 2005 | Pilot cohort; 12 weeks; ASP; children, parents | 56 children (25 parents) 11.1 ± 1.3 years 32% | Program employed social, cognitive, & behavioural strategies. Content included: dance, sport, fitness & nutrition activities, targeted handouts & poster‐board display, & motivational talk by public figure. Participants required to record daily FV intake & step counts. Contact: 60‐75 min sessions 4 days/week | TP: Pre & post‐I Comp: not stated CG: no Control | FFQ: f/day (converted to 0–10 score) | Green salad (+/− other V): 3.42 ± 1.97; fries/fried potato: 3.60 ± 1.54; boiled/mashed potato: 2.79 ± 1.66 V servings (−salad V/potato): 3.96 ± 1.69 | Green salad (+/‐other V): 4.58 ± 2.80; fries/fried potato: 3.92 ± 2.79; boiled/mashed potato: 3.70 ± 2.33; V servings (−salad V/potato): 4.36 ± 2.20 | — | Green salad (+/−other V): +1.16, P = 0.003 Fries/fried potato: +0.32, P = 0.278 Boiled/mashed potato: +0.91, P = 0.005 V servings (−salad V/potato): +0.4, P = 0.276 |
Freedman & Nickell, 2010 | Cohort; 3 weeks; Local library; Children (+1 session for parent & child) | 49 11.1 ± 0.03 years (9–14 years) 36% | ‘Snack Smart’ workshops: nutrition education through videos, food prep, tastings, label‐reading, games, creating recipes, handouts, role modelling, goal setting, barriers, home activities, social support networks. Reinforcements used + recipe book & draw prize post‐I. Contact: 6 h total; 3 × 90 min/week after‐school nutrition workshops, 2 × 45 min weekend workshops | TP: Baseline, post‐I, 3–4 months Comp: Not stated CG: children acted as own control | FFQ: f/d (converted to 0–3 score) | — | — | follow‐up ‐ pretest −1.17, P = 0.24 | posttest – pretest: −2.12, P = 0.03 |
Gholami et al., 2015 | RCT; education leaflet mailed out; Home; Mothers | 155 mothers with daughters aged 6–11 year mothers: 34.13 ± 5.87 years (25–50 year) NA | Theory‐guided instructional leaflet provided to mothers after baseline; included info on consequences of behaviour, WHO healthy eating recommendations, instructions on how to perform behaviour, dietary action planning and coping planning exercises. Contact: Education leaflet mailed out | TP: Baseline, 2 weeks, 3 months Comp: Not stated CG: not stated | Survey: Usual portions/day | I: 2.42 (SD 1.44), C: 2.80 (SD 1.71), (t = 1.54, d = ‐0.24, P = 0.12) | 2 weeks I: 3.09 (SD 1.86), C: 2.53 (SD 1.63), (t = ‐2.07, d = 0.32, P = 0.04) | 3 month I: 3.05 (SD 1.48), C: 2.87 (SD 1.45), t = −0.80, d = 0.12, P = 0.42 | Main effects: time effect P = .03, No treatment effect P = .67. Interaction between time and treatment existed P = 0.02 (I group higher at time 2, not time 3) |
Haire‐Joshu et al., 2008 | Group randomised nested cohort design; 7 months; home; family | 1306 families with preschool aged child (2–5 years) — | High 5 for Kids: tailored newsletter based on pretest interview, home visits & materials for parent & child. Intervention strategies targeted knowledge, parental modelling, feeding practices, FV availability. Contact: 4 × 60 min home visits + newsletters | TP: baseline, 7 months Comp: Program delivered to 78% intervention families, 84% completed posttest CG: standard program | FFQ (27 items): Servings/day | I: 1.55 C: 1.46 | I: −0.02 C: −0.04 | — | Intervention effect, adjusted 0.06, P = 0.10 |
Horne et al., 2011 | Randomised cross over trial; 2 phases 30 days each; childcare; children | 20 34 months (24–52 months) 40% | Modelling & rewards intervention: videos screened during intervention featuring animated characters modelling consumption of target food & being rewarded for eating them. Letters from characters read out to reinforce target food, previous days intake and rewards. Rewards given for eating varying amounts of target foods. Contact: not specified | TP: baseline 1, fruit intervention, baseline 2, christmas break, baseline 3, vege intervention, baseline 4, 6 months follow‐up. Comp: not stated CG: no control | Observational record: % consumed | Baseline 1 snacktime: 24.6% lunchtime: 17.9% Baseline 3 snacktime: 28.8% lunchtime: 30% | Post‐I (cf baseline 3 intake) snacktime: 85.5% lunchtime: 76.9% | 6 months (cf baseline 1 intake) snacktime: 85.1% lunchtime: 84.8% | post‐I snack time: +56.7 (t (19) = 7.09; P < 0.001; d = 1.73) lunchtime: + 46.9 (t (19) = 5.98; P < 0.001; d = 1.42) 6 month FU snack time: +60.5 (t (13) = 8.09; P < 0.001; d = 2.59) lunchtime: +66.9 (t (13) = 8.12; P < 0.001; d = 2.68) |
Horton et al., 2013 | RCT; 4 months; Home; Families | 361 families with ≥ 1 child aged 7–13 years no info | Intervention topics included: family relations/communication/parenting styles, stress & eating, healthy eating & FV, social support. Also included DVD series, goal setting sheets, skill building activities, child‐directed activities & family weekly tasks. Contact: 14 contacts: 11 × home visits, 3 × calls), total 16.5 h. | TP: Baseline, 4 months 6 months Comp: retention rates I: 88%, C:91% CG: delayed treatment intervention | Survey (2Q) Cups/day, Variety in last month (44 items) | — | Adj mean of daily cups of vege consumed: I: 1.19 (SE 0.07), C: 1.05 (SE 0.07) Adj mean of monthly variety of vege: I: 12.0 (SE 0.44), C: 10.8 (SE 0.43) | — | I vs. C post‐I: cups consumed, P = 0.14 variety, P = 0.067 |
Latif et al., 2011 | Cluster RCT; 9 weeks; community group; children | 473 12.8 ± 1.1 years(10–4 years) 100% | 2 x I groups (dietary or PA based). Dietary I: 5 A Day Badge program ‐ functional knowledge & skills, tastings, recipe booklet. PA I: Fit for Life (FFL) Badge program ‐ sporting activities, ‘drills’ booklet. Internet component: BCTs ‐ goal setting/review/modelling and problem solving Contact: 9 × 55 min/week group sessions (30 min in‐troop activities/25 min internet activities) | TP: Baseline, post‐I, 6 months post‐intervention Comp: not stated CG: — | FFQ: servings/day | I: 0.35 (SD ± 0.32) C: — | I: 0.44 (SD ± 0.36) C — | 6 months Post I I: 0.38 (SD ± 0.36) C — | Post I: +0.09 6 months post I: +0.03 (C group NA) |
Martinez‐Andrade et al., 2014 | Pilot cluster RCT; 6 weeks; primary care clinic; families | 306(189 completed 3&6 months FU) 40.6 ± 10 months (2–5 years) 52.6% | Intervention group: obesity awareness & prevention workshops. Topics: portion size, healthy eating, label reading, meal planning, PA & sun exposure. Techniques included motivational interviewing, reflexive listening skills, goal setting/review, barriers, activities (games, cooking). Contact: 2 h/week (90 min workshop, 30 min shared food). | TP: Baseline, 3 months, 6 months Comp: 40% compliance to educational sessions. 35% families did not complete 3 months FU. CG: Usual care | FFQ: Servings/week | I: 20.9 (SD ± 15.0) C:20.6 (SD ± 20.1) | 3 month FU I: 20.0 (SD ± 17.1) C:15.1 (SD ± 13.9) | — | 3 months FU I: ‐0.8 (SD ± 0.2) C: ‐5.5 (SD ± 0.2) adj I‐C dif 6.3 (95%CI 1.8,10.8) 6 months FU I: −3.1 (SD ± 0.2) C: ‐5.0 (SD ± 0.2) adj I‐C dif 2.7 (95%CI −1.3, 6.7) |
Namenek Brouwer & Benjamin Neelon 2013 | Pilot RCT; 4 months; Childcare; Children | 12 (4 childcare centres) 73% aged 3–5 years — | ‘Watch Me Grow’ garden based program: grow a crop/month, weekly class activities (reading, tasting, garden, classroom). Program included a gardener to assist & health educator for menu review. Contact: 4 activities/month (~1/week). Duration not stated. | TP: Baseline, post‐I (5 months) Comp: not stated CG: delayed intervention | Observation record: servings/2 days | Total V: I: 0.8 +/−0.68, C: 0.8+/−00.38 dark V: I: 0.00+/−00.00, C: 0.01+/−00.02 white potatoes: I:0.17+/−00.23, C: 0.22+/−00.39 | Total V: I: 1.05+/−00.67, C: 0.63+/−00.28 dark V: I: 0.07+/−00.13, C: 0.02+/−00.01 white potatoes: I: 0.33+/−00.34, C: 0.00+/−00.00 | — | Total V: I: 0.25 +/−01.1, C: ‐0.18 +/−00.52 dark V: 0.07+/−00.13, C: 0.01+/−00.05 white potatoes: 0.16+/−00.54, C: 0.22+/−00.39 |
Schwinn et al. 2014 | Cohort; 3 weeks; Internet; Mother daughter dyads | 67 mother daughter dyads 11.85 ± 0.88 years (10–12 years) NA | 3 session web delivered program to develop & maintain healthy relationships, bodies & minds. Topics: communication, family meals, knowledge of drugs, setting rules, food shopping skills, preparing healthy dinners, coping skills. Contact: 3 × 25 min sessions | TP: Baseline, post‐test, 5 months FU Comp: 35/36 in intervention completed all sessions CG: no intervention materials | FFQ (21 items): f/week | 2.28+/−00.9 | 1 week postI:2.38+/−00.81 | 5 months postI 2.41+/−00.99 NS | NS |
Slusser et al. 2013 | Evaluation pre/post test; 1 School year; After‐school program; staff | 121 from 8 ASP sites (I:4, Ct:4) 6–11 years 43% | Catch Kids Club: 32‐lesson ASP teaching students nutrition & skills to make healthy dietary & PA choices. Intervention sites received staff training in nutrition, child development & PA routines, curriculum resources, mentoring & assistance visits, nutrition education manual, activity box, & snack prep activities. Contact: ‐ | TP: Baseline (Sep), follow‐up at the end of school yr (June). Comp: Not stated CG: No training or support provided | Survey: f (previous day) | I: 0.37 (SD 0.117) C: 0.36 (SD 0.092) | I: 0.37 (SD 0.284) C: 0.46 (SD 0.140) | — | Change score I: 0.00, C: +0.10, P = 0.084 |
Somerville et al. 2012 | Evaluation with pre/post test; 13 weeks; Community; Children | 40 9 y (6–12 years)‐ | Weekly activities included FV snack preparation, blind tasting, apple stamping, produce Pictionary, & FV bingo. Children encouraged to prepare & consume FV snacks during session & at home. Contact: 1 h/week | TP: Baseline, post‐Is Comp: Not stated CG: No Control | Observation: Servings eaten at snacktime (FV combined) Survey (7 item): usual Servings/day | Usual servings/day 2.17 ± 1.82 | Usual servings/day 3.07 ± 1.87 | — | 0.9, P < 0.05 |
Tabak et al. 2012 | Pilot RCT; 4 months; Phone + print materials; Parents | 43 families 3.6 ± 0.8 years (2–5 years) 16% | Intervention: 4 tailored monthly newsletters & 2 motivational phone calls. Calls addressed V/food issues from baseline surveys, areas for improvement, encouraged parents to describe successes, use problem solving to overcome barriers, receive support & encouragement. Newsletters addressed: V availability, picky eating, family meals, role modelling, individual feedback, goal setting, recipes, tips, resources and goal tracking. Contact: 4 monthly newsletters & 2 calls | TP: baseline, post‐I (~5 months) Comp: not stated CG: 4 children's books (non‐health/nutrition related) (1/month) | FFQ: servings/day | I: 0.8 (SD 0.4) C: 0.6 (SD 0.4) | — | — | change in mean (P = 0.61 adj) I: +0.1 (SD 0.3) C: +0.0 (SD 0.5) |
Witt & Dunn 2012 | RCT; 6 weeks; pre‐school; children | 263 from 17 childcare centres 4–5 years 53% | Colour me Healthy: designed for 4–5 years old pre‐schoolers & delivered by pre‐school teachers, provides interactive learning & teacher toolkit (lesson guides, picture cards, posters, music CD, hand stamps, parent newsletters etc). Activities encourage discussion about FV, tasting experiences. Contact: 3 sessions/week, 15‐30 min each | TP: baseline, post‐I, 3 months post‐I Comp: attendance 14.2 ± 4.0/18 lessons; completed take‐home activities 3.4 ± 2.4/6 CG: no treatment | Weighed record: % of 1 Cup serve consumed | I: 37.9% C: 35.6% | I: 62.1% C: 33.2% | I: 71.0% C: 34.0% | Main effect for time (F [2,240] = 21.67, P <0.01) Interaction effect (F [2,240] = 27.65, P < 0.01) I: higher intake post‐I (F [1,120] = 24.14, P = <0.001; d = 0.90) & FU (F [1,120] = 43.41, P = <0.001; d = 1.20) |
Wolfenden et al. 2014 | RCT; 1 months; phone + materials delivered to homes; Parents & children | 394 parents from 30 pre‐schools 4.3 ± 0.6 years (3–5 years) I:49.0%, C:54.3% | Weekly phone intervention + printed resources relating to healthy eating at home (based on Australian guide to healthy eating – AGHE). Aims: increase FV availability, supportive family routines & parental role modelling & self monitoring & develop contingencies for difficult situations. Contact: 4x30min phone contact + print materials | TP: Baseline, 12 month, 18 month Comp: 87% completed all 4 phone calls, & interviewers covered 97% of key intervention content CG: print material on AGHE only | Survey (CDQ): variety & f on previous d; f/ during previous week; FV combined; servings usually consumed. | serves not measured | V serves: I: 2.95 +/− 0.12 C: 2.47 +/− 0.11 | V serves: I: 2.98 +/− 0.11 C: 2.55 +/− 0.10 | I: sig higher at post‐I & FU, P < 0.01 |
Wright et al. 2012 | RCT; program: 6 weeks, school/community activities: 4 months; ASP; children & teachers & parents | 251 (I:121, C:130) I: 9.0 ± 1.6 years, C:8.3 ± 1.1 year, (–12 years) I:42%, C:38% | Two components: (1) ‘Kids Nutrition & Fitness‘ (KNF): afterschool family lifestyle program & parental group encouraging healthy lifestyle behaviours, food pyramid, cooking patterns, (2) school & community environmental activities to create/deliver school based policy for physical & dietary change, professional development for teachers, home activities via bimonthly newsletters. Contact: KNF: 6 × 90 min sessions School/Community activities: unclear duration | TP: Baseline, post‐I, 12 months Comp: not stated CG: received general education (standard PA program offered by the school) | Survey (6 item): f/day | I: 1.51 +/− 0.5 C: 1.55 +/− 0.69 | — | — | I: +1.51(−2.11,5.50) C: +0.4 (−1.11,1.9), P = 0.03 |
I = intervention group; CG = control group; TP = timepoint; Comp = compliance – if no information provided in manuscript; V/vege = vegetable; F = fruit; PA = physical activity; f = frequency; NS = not significant; SSB = sugar sweetened beverages; Adj = adjusted; EO = exposure only; and ASP = after school program.
Calculated by ((post intervention intake − baseline intake)/baseline intake × 100) or ((post intervention intake of intervention group − post intervention intake of control group)/post intervention intake of control × 100). This is only calculated for intake date, not for frequency data.
Characterising interventions by effectiveness
Short‐term and longer term effectiveness
Table 2 summarises the intervention characteristics for effective and ineffective studies separately for short‐term and longer term outcome measures. The majority (8 out of 12) of interventions that targeted children's vegetable intake in the context of either a healthy diet or healthy lifestyle were effective in the short‐term, whereas one out of four interventions that targeted vegetables alone were effective in the short‐term. None of the interventions that focused solely on vegetable intake had an outcome measure at 6 months or more. Of the six studies that were effective longer term, half focused on vegetables within the context of a healthy diet, the others on increasing fruit and vegetables (n = 2) or as part healthy lifestyle generally (n = 1) (Table 2).
Table 2.
Summary of intervention, effectiveness, intensity and quality of the studies included in this review (n = 22 studies)
Author | Target behaviour | Effectiveness* | Intensity | Study quality | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Short | Longer | Score | Rating | |||
(Annesi et al. 2009) | Lifestyle | Yes | — | 12 | Medium | Strong |
(Bayer et al. 2009) | Lifestyle | Yes | Yes | 16 | High | Weak |
(Castro et al. 2013) | Diet | Yes | Yes | 12 | Medium | Weak |
(Corsini et al. 2011) | Vege | Yes | — | 12 | Medium | Weak |
(Davis et al. 2011) | Diet | No | — | 10 | Low | Moderate |
(De Bock et al. 2011) | Diet | Yes | Yes | 12 | Medium | Weak |
(Engels et al. 2005) | Lifestyle | Yes | — | 11 | Medium | Weak |
(Freedman & Nickell 2010) | Diet | Yes | — | 11 | Medium | Weak |
(Gholami et al. 2015) | Vege | No | — | 4 | Low | Weak |
(Haire‐Joshu et al. 2008) | FV | No | No | 11 | Medium | Moderate |
(Horne et al. 2011) | FV | Yes | Yes | 11 | Medium | Weak |
(Horton et al. 2013) | Vege | No | — | 12 | Medium | Weak |
(Latif et al. 2011) | FV | No | No | 9 | Low | Moderate |
(Martinez‐Andrade et al. 2014) | Lifestyle | No | No | 12 | Medium | Moderate |
(Namenek Brouwer & Benjamin Neelon 2013) | FV | No | — | 11 | Medium | Weak |
(Schwinn et al. 2014) | Diet | No | — | 6 | Low | Weak |
(Slusser et al. 2013) | Lifestyle | No | No | 12 | Medium | Moderate |
(Somerville et al. 2012) | FV | Yes | — | 12 | Medium | Weak |
(Tabak et al. 2012) | Vege | No | — | 12 | Medium | Weak |
(Witt & Dunn 2012) | Lifestyle | Yes | — | 12 | Medium | Moderate |
(Wolfenden et al. 2014) | FV | Yes | Yes | 10 | Low | Moderate |
(Wright et al. 2012) | Diet | Yes | Yes | 13 | High | Weak |
Short‐term effectiveness = significant increase in vegetable consumption at 3 months; longer term effectiveness = significant increase in vegetable consumption at 6 months or more.
Pre‐school included pre‐school and childcare centres.
Aftersch = After‐school; Comm = Community; Vege = vegetables, FV = fruit and vegetables.
Intervention setting, duration, intensity, quality and contact with participants
Table 3 summarises the setting, duration, intensity and quality of effective and ineffective studies, by short and longer term follow‐up. There were seven studies delivered in the home, six in pre‐schools, four in after school programs and five in the community. While the home was a common setting, it was the least likely to be effective in the short‐term (n = 2/7). There were four after‐school programs, three of which were effective in the short‐term. Longer term, interventions within the pre‐school setting were most likely to be effective. The duration of the intervention did appear to be associated with effectiveness. Effective interventions were more likely to be rated as medium to high intensity overall, with only one out of five low intensity intervention considered to be effective. There were two high‐intensity interventions – both effective in the short and longer terms. Only one study was rated as strong quality and this was considered effective in the short‐term; however, it did not include a follow‐up measure at 6 months or more. Effective interventions scored higher in terms of their contact with participants (3.75 vs. 2.7 out of 5) and their reach (2 vs. 1.2 out of 5) (See Table 4).
Table 3.
Summary of intervention target, setting, duration, intensity and quality by short (3 months) and longer term (≥6 months) effectiveness (n = 22 studies)
Short‐term | Longer term | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
Effective (n = 12) | Ineffective (n = 10) | Effective (n = 6) | Ineffective (n = 4) | |
Target | ||||
Vegetables | 1 | 3 | 0 | 0 |
Fruit and vegetables | 3 | 3 | 2 | 2 |
Healthy diet | 4 | 2 | 3 | 0 |
Healthy lifestyle | 4 | 2 | 1 | 2 |
Setting | ||||
Home | 2 | 5 | 1 | 1 |
Pre‐school/childcare | 4 | 2 | 3 | 0 |
After‐school program | 3 | 1 | 1 | 1 |
Community | 3 | 2 | 1 | 2 |
Length of intervention | ||||
0 to 4 weeks (0–1 month) | 3 | 2 | 1 | 0 |
5 to 12 weeks (1–3 months) | 4 | 3 | 1 | 2 |
13 to 24 weeks (3–6 months) | 3 | 3 | 2 | 0 |
25 weeks or more (>6 months) | 2 | 2 | 2 | 2 |
Intensity Rating | ||||
Low (%) | 1 | 4 | 1 | 1 |
Medium (%) | 9 | 6 | 3 | 3 |
High (%) | 2 | 0 | 2 | 0 |
Quality rating | ||||
Weak (%) | 9 | 5 | 5 | 0 |
Moderate (%) | 2 | 5 | 1 | 4 |
Strong (%) | 1 | 0 | 0 | 0 |
Table 4.
Summary of intervention Intensity by short and longer term effectiveness (n = 22 studies)
Short‐term | Longer term | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
Effective (Mean) | Ineffective (Mean) | Effective (Mean) | Ineffective (Mean) | |
Intensity Score* | ||||
Duration of intervention | 2.75 | 2.8 | 3.3 | 3 |
Contact with intervention | 3.75 | 2.7 | 3.7 | 3 |
Level of contact | 3.5 | 3.2 | 3.3 | 3.5 |
Reach (no. of settings) | 2 | 1.2 | 2 | 1.5 |
Overall score (max = 20,range 4–18) | 12 | 9.9 | 12.3 | 11 |
Maximum score for each component = 5.
Behaviour change techniques (BCTs)
Table 5 summarises the BCTs used by effective and ineffective studies and by short and longer term follow‐up. Overall, the most commonly used behaviour change techniques were the provision of resources (82%), information on the consequences of consuming and not consuming vegetables (73%), modelling or demonstrating the behaviour (64%), prompt practice (59%) and providing instruction on how to perform the behaviour (54%). Half of the interventions included planning for social support or change as a tool to increase vegetable intake, and this technique was associated with effective behaviour change in both the short‐term (8/11 studies) and longer term (6/7 studies). The other two techniques that were associated with effective behaviour change were (1) the use of exposure to vegetables, although only associated with short‐term change (6/9 studies) and (2) the provision of staff training, which was associated with both short‐term (6/9 studies) and long term behaviour change (3/4 studies). However, both these techniques were only used in nine of the 22 interventions (41%, Table 5).
Table 5.
Use of behaviour change techniques by short and longer term effectiveness
Behaviour change techniques | Short‐term | Longer term | Total n = 22 (% of all studies) | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Effective | Ineffective | Effective | Ineffective | ||
(N = 12) | (N = 10) | (N = 6) | (N = 16) | ||
1. Provide information on consequences of behaviour in general | 8 | 8 | 5 | 3 | 16 (72.7) |
2. Provide information on consequences of behaviour to the individual | 4 | 5 | 3 | 2 | 9 (40.9) |
3. Provide information about others' approval | 0 | 1 | 0 | 1 | 1 (4.5) |
4. Provide normative information about others' behaviour | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 (0) |
5. Goal setting (behaviour) | 4 | 5 | 2 | 3 | 9 (40.9) |
6. Goal setting (outcome) | 1 | 1 | 0 | 0 | 2 (9.1) |
7. Action planning | 0 | 2 | 0 | 1 | 2 (9.1) |
8. Barrier identification/problem solving | 2 | 5 | 1 | 2 | 7 (31.8) |
9. Set graded tasks | 1 | 1 | 0 | 1 | 2 (9.1) |
10. Prompt review of behavioural goals | 2 | 4 | 1 | 1 | 6 (27.3) |
11. Prompt review of outcome goals | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 (0) |
12. Prompt rewards contingent on effort or progress towards behaviour | 0 | 2 | 0 | 2 | 2 (9.1) |
13. Provide rewards contingent on successful behaviour | 3 | 2 | 1 | 2 | 5 (22.7) |
14. Shaping | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 (0) |
15. Prompting generalisation of a target behaviour | 1 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 1 (4.5) |
16. Prompt self‐monitoring of behaviour | 3 | 4 | 1 | 3 | 7 (31.8) |
17. Prompt self‐monitoring of behavioural outcome | 1 | 1 | 0 | 1 | 2 (9.1) |
18. Prompting focus on past success | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 (0) |
19. Provide feedback on performance | 1 | 2 | 1 | 1 | 3 (13.6) |
20. Provide information on where and when to perform the behaviour | 2 | 1 | 1 | 0 | 3 (13.6) |
21. Provide instruction on how to perform the behaviour | 4 | 8 | 2 | 3 | 12 (54.5) |
22. Model/Demonstrate the behaviour | 8 | 6 | 3 | 3 | 14 (63.6) |
23. Teach to use prompts/cues | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 2 (9.1) |
24. Environmental restructuring | 1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 2 (9.1) |
25. Agree behavioural contract | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 (0) |
26. Prompt practice | 7 | 6 | 2 | 3 | 13 (59.1) |
27. Use of follow‐up prompts | 0 | 1 | 0 | 0 | 1 (4.5) |
28. Facilitate social comparison | 1 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 1 (4.5) |
29. Plan social support/social change * / ** | 8 | 3 | 6 | 1 | 11 (50) |
30. Prompt identification as role model/position advocate | 3 | 4 | 3 | 3 | 7 (31.8) |
31. Prompt anticipated regret | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 (0) |
32. Fear arousal | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 (0) |
33. Prompt self‐talk | 1 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 1 (4.5) |
34. Prompt use of imagery | 1 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 1 (4.5) |
35. Relapse prevention/coping planning | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 (0) |
36. Stress management/emotional control training | 0 | 2 | 0 | 0 | 2 (9.1) |
37. Motivational interviewing | 0 | 3 | 0 | 1 | 3 (13.6) |
38. Time management | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 (0) |
39. General communication skills training | 2 | 5 | 1 | 3 | 7 (31.8) |
40. Stimulate anticipation of future rewards | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 (0) |
41. Exposure * | 6 | 3 | 2 | 2 | 9 (40.9) |
42. Provision of resources | 10 | 8 | 6 | 4 | 18 (81.8) |
43. Staff training ** | 6 | 3 | 3 | 1 | 9 (40.9) |
44. Community/participant engagement | 3 | 3 | 0 | 2 | 6 (27.3) |
45. Economic incentives | 1 | 1 | 1 | 0 | 2 (9.1) |
46. Availability/accessibility | 1 | 1 | 1 | 0 | 2 (9.1) |
Bold: Commonly used techniques (used by greater than 50%)
Bold/italics: Techniques associated with effective intervention;
short‐term effectiveness,
longer term effectiveness.
Change in vegetable intake
Based on the calculated percentage change in vegetable intake (calculated from the results reported in Table 1) interventions reported a mean increase in vegetable intake of 29% (range: −20% to +87%).
Discussion
The present review identified 22 studies that delivered home or community‐based interventions aimed at increasing children's vegetable intake, either in isolation or in combination with other healthy eating and lifestyle messages. Just over half of studies reported a significant increase in children's vegetable intake at 3 months, with six out of ten studies that included a measure at 6 months are also effective. We estimate that the interventions were associated with an increase in vegetable intake of approximately 30% (range −20% to +87%). Based on Australian children's current vegetable intake, this equates to approximately a quarter to half of one vegetable serving. While this increase is small, incremental dietary improvements across the life stages are important and assist in establishing healthy eating patterns that may carry through into later childhood and beyond. Any increase in children's vegetable intake, regardless of the size, is important given the progressive declines usually observed during childhood (Australian Bureau of Statistics 2014).
Internationally, dietary guidelines (U.S. Department of Agriculture and U.S. Department of Health and Human Services 2010; National Health and Medical Research Council 2013) and public health campaigns (Capacci & Mazzocchi 2011; Council 2013) often promote fruits and vegetables together. Our results show that promoting fruits and vegetables together has limited benefit for vegetable intake, confirming previous findings (Evans et al. 2012), while targeting children's vegetable intake in isolation was least likely to increase usual vegetable intake. Our findings suggest that future initiatives should focus on whole meals within the context of a balanced diet and a healthy lifestyle, rather than target vegetables in isolation. Indeed vegetables tend to be consumed as part of a main meal rather than eaten alone. This finding challenges the notion of delivering simple nutrition education messages and is different to a previous review of dairy foods where single target messages were more effective in changing behaviour than targeting multiple food groups (Hendrie et al. 2012).
Effective interventions tended to be delivered in the pre‐school/childcare centre setting, and this setting was particularly effective longer term where all three interventions reported significant increases in children's vegetable intake at 6 months. These findings are supported by the literature that suggests childcare settings are key in influencing children's dietary intakes by promoting positive mealtime environments, role‐modelling positive eating practices, involving children in the meal time and allowing them choice to decide what and how much to eat (Golley et al. 2012; Bell et al. 2015). Additionally, childcare also engages parents about nutrition and assists in transferring positive nutrition practices into the home setting and by reaching out to parents and influencing the nutrition and food environment at home (Golley et al. 2012). It is also possible that such interventions coincided with, or prompted, a change in childcare nutrition policies or food service practices. In contrast, school environments have many competing curriculum priorities, which may dilute the intervention messages. Similarly, schools may have fewer resources (e.g. staff and access to training) to successfully deliver the intervention as intended, again potentially weakening the intervention effects. On the other hand childcare settings may have greater resources (e.g. higher staff to student ratios), some flexibility in their curriculum to implement intervention programs and a stronger parental expectation for the presence and implementation of nutrition policy.
A recent meta‐analysis of school‐based interventions in children aged 5–12 years (Evans et al. 2012) found that intervening in this setting resulted in an average increase in vegetable consumption of +0.07 servings. In comparison, our findings showed that targeting younger (2–12 years) children and intervening outside of the school setting was slightly more effective resulting in an average increase of a quarter to a half serve of vegetables (or ~30% increase in intake). In absolute terms, although a quarter to half a serve may seem small, any increase in vegetable intake at an early age is significant given the downward trajectory of vegetable intakes as young children progress through later childhood and into adolescence. The importance of intervention at an early age is well supported in the literature. For example, Caton et al. found age to be a significant predictor of eating patterns and acceptance, with younger children eating and enjoying novel vegetables more than older children (Caton et al. 2014). Furthermore, Birch & Ventura suggest that intervening before a child reaches school should be a priority and prevention efforts should explore all early childhood contexts such as family homes and childcare settings (Birch & Ventura 2009). Early years are considered the most effective and are characterised by ‘high plasticity’ and ‘rapid transitions’ and are a period of time when parents and caregivers have a high degree of control over their children's food environment and experiences (Anzman et al. 2010). As such, younger children are generally considered easier to influence than older children who have more established food preferences and greater independence in their food choices (Anzman et al. 2010).
However, despite the importance of interventions beginning at an early age, our results and the findings of others (Evans et al. 2012) highlight the difficulty of designing interventions to change children's vegetable intakes effectively. This is particularly demonstrated in our findings where huge disparities between interventions in effect were found, with reports of up to +87% change in vegetable intake and as low as −20%. Indeed, it has been shown that the taste of vegetables is not innately accepted by some children who are ‘hard‐wired’ to reject pure vegetable flavours despite intensive conditioning attempts (Zeinstra et al. 2009). Many vegetables are bitter, have unfamiliar textures and provide little dietary energy (Hetherington et al. 2015); hence, overcoming such strong aversive innate preferences is difficult and may not be effective for all children (Caton et al. 2013). As such, children often require repeated exposure (Zajonc 1968; Caton et al. 2013) and positive reinforcement longer term in order to learn to like such foods (Cooke et al. 2011). In particular, persistence with repeated vegetable exposure in early childhood, prior to starting school, is likely to be important in the formation of taste preferences (Mikkila et al. 2005; Nicklaus & Remy, 2013). Indeed as our results indicate, exposure to vegetables is linked to short‐term behaviour change and is supported by an emerging body of evidence using repeated exposure (and conditioning) of target vegetable stimuli, soups or purees (Hausner et al. 2012; Caton et al. 2013; de Wild et al. 2013; Remy et al. 2013; Ahern et al. 2014; Bouhlal et al. 2014). However, while such studies have shown promising results in increasing intake, liking and acceptance of the test vegetable dishes, there is currently no evidence that responses to test stimuli generalise beyond that particular stimulus to habitual vegetable intake (Corsini et al. 2011). However, in the context of this review, acknowledging our search strategy was directed towards ‘usual intake’ rather than intakes of a specific targeted vegetable or test stimuli, designing interventions that combine exposure and conditioning methodologies with intervention strategies that influence ‘usual’ vegetable intake needs to be addressed by future research.
Greater frequency of participant contact with the intervention was also associated with effective behaviour change. While this review did not specifically consider the economic cost of interventions, it is an important practical consideration. The emergence of digital technologies may allow future initiatives to maintain regular, tailored contact with participants, while minimising cost relative to more traditional forms of face‐to face contact (Cushing & Steele 2010).
Provision of knowledge is necessary for any intervention to be successful, but it is not sufficient to achieve behaviour change when used alone (Klein et al. 2015). This review found that provision of education resources, modelling or demonstrating behaviour and providing instruction were associated with better outcomes when combined with action based techniques such as planning for social support, increased exposure and provision of staff training. Further, planning for social support implies cooperation between parents and caregivers, and across settings such as the home and childcare facilities, to promote a consistent message. Indeed, targeting multiple settings was another important characteristic of effective interventions, suggesting that the full potential of initiatives can be optimised when multiple levels of the environment cooperate synergistically.
The present review synthesised relevant literature and identified key intervention attributes, which can inform the development of future initiatives aiming to increase young children's vegetable intake, primarily in the home and community settings. Because of the vast amount of literature and nature of systematic literature reviews, the scope of this paper needed to be contained, but still make a unique contribution to the area. We included a range of intervention types and outcome measures; however, excluded initiatives in the school setting, which were the focus of a recent review (Evans et al. 2012). However, school‐based initiatives should not be excluded from future efforts to promote good nutrition and improve vegetable intake in children. We also excluded studies that did not report vegetable intake separately to fruit intake as it has been reported that improvements in fruit and vegetables intake, when reported together, is largely due to a change in fruit and not vegetable intake (Evans et al. 2012). Fruits and vegetables are consumed in different contexts, such as ‘where’ it is consumed, ‘with whom’ it is consumed and at what ‘time’ of day it is consumed (Mak et al. 2012) and as such, strategies to increase vegetable consumption are likely to differ from those for fruit.
Previous reviews have focused on ‘how much’ we can increase vegetable consumption (Knai et al. 2006; Evans et al. 2012). In contrast, we were particularly interested in ‘how to’ increase intake. We have critiqued a range of intervention components including the behaviour change techniques used, which has provided some insight to this question. However this critique was limited by the details provided in the published methodology. Details of the intervention messages and exactly how these were delivered were often brief. Therefore, there may have been other strategies that contributed to the success of the intervention that were not described, or instances where we may have not interpreted the description as intended.
The measurement of dietary intake is another commonly reported limitation (Livingstone & Robson 2000; Thompson et al. 2010), particularly in large scale community interventions. We allowed a range of outcome measurements to be included in this review; however, the common use of self‐reported intake poses issues. It is associated with higher variability, meaning that small changes are more difficult to detect. Given our current definition of effectiveness, we may have biased our findings towards studies that were able to measure intake in a more robust way – such as in childcare settings where intake was often supervised, weighed and then the amount consumed reported in grams. Similarly, the heterogeneity in the measurement of intake across the studies prevented us performing meta‐analyses. However, we did attempt to summarise the findings by calculating the average change in vegetable intake as a percentage increase/decrease in the units reported, regardless of the significance levels, and found on average studies reported a 30% increase in children's vegetable intake.
As our findings show, despite the large body of literature describing interventions that target children's dietary intake, there is a need for more high quality, randomised and controlled interventions which target children's vegetable intake with longer term follow‐up. Of the studies included in this review, only one was rated as strong quality, and only half included a measure of intake at 6 months or more. We have made recommendations about characteristics associated with effective intervention based on the authors' description and analysis of results, which varies between studies. Others advocate the need for effective techniques to be described using a systematic reporting process (e.g. adaptations of CONSORT reporting) to improve the quality of how studies are described (Cushing & Steele 2010). We also suggest the interventions themselves can be better described including details of the behaviour change techniques adopted using the CALO‐RE taxonomy (Michie et al. 2011). Such description could compensate for the limitations of post‐intervention surveys or qualitative techniques (Thomas et al. 2004b) that seek to elicit the opinions of parents (not always possible), children (often lacking the cognitive skills to articulate) and other caregivers (who were present during the intervention) on effective techniques and intervention components that contributed most to successful behaviour change. A higher level of robust detail in reporting the methods would allow public health behavioural interventions to be truly reproducible (Cushing & Steele 2010).
Conclusions
Interventions that target children's vegetable intake in the home or community settings are generally effective and may potentially increase intake by ~30%. Based on children's current intakes, which are well below recom‐mendations, this equates to an average of a quarter to a half of a serve. Our review highlights the need for more high‐quality RCT‐based interventions targeting children's vegetable intake, with longer term follow‐up to address the current low levels of consumption effectively and to bring them closer to recommended levels of intake. The findings in the current review identify key strategies and intervention characteristics that may optimise the effectiveness of future programs aiming to increase children's vegetable intake. However, a key challenge still remains in getting these strategies adopted and implemented at a broader community level to reach more children, as increasing children's intake in line with vegetable recommendations remains a significant challenge.
Source of funding
In the past 2 years (2014–2015), author GAH has received funding from the Australian Beverages Council Ltd, BUPA, Probiotec Pty Ltd, SP Health Pty Ltd and Meat & Livestock Australia Ltd; HJL has received funding from Horticulture Australia Limited (VG13090); DLB has received funding from Meat & Livestock Australia Ltd, Probiotec Pty Ltd, SP Health Pty Ltd, Dairy Research Institute (grant 1230), Australian Beverages Council Ltd and Horticulture Australia Limited (VG13090); JB has received funding from Probiotec Pty Ltd, SPC Ardmona Pty Ltd and Horticulture Australia Limited (VG13090); DNC has received funding from Horticulture Australia Limited (VG13090 and VG13089).
Conflicts of interest
This project has been funded by Horticulture Innovation Australia Limited (HIA) using the vegetable levy and funds from the Australian Government with co‐investment from CSIRO.
CSIRO designed and conducted the systematic review, and prepared the manuscript. No data was owned by the sponsor HIA and the sponsor HIA did not play a role in the study design, data collection, analysis, manuscript preparation or revision.
This review did not require ethics approval. The authors declare no competing interest.
Contributions
All authors were involved in at least two of the following three processes: development of the selection criteria, data extraction process and writing of results. All authors contributed to the drafting and revisions of the manuscript and approved the final manuscript for publication.
Supporting information
Additional Supporting information may be found in the online version of this article at the publisher's web site.
Supplementary File 1: Comprehensive data extraction data for review articles (.xls)
Supplementary File 2: PRISMA checklist.
Supporting info item
Supporting info item
Acknowledgements
All authors were funded by Horticulture Innovation Australia Limited (formally Horticulture Australia Limited) using the vegetable levy and funds from the Australian Government with co‐investment from CSIRO.
CSIRO designed and conducted the systematic review, and prepared the manuscript. HIA did not contribute to the preparation of the manuscript, but approved it for publication.
Hendrie G. A., Lease H. J., Bowen J., Baird D. L., and Cox D. N. (2017) Strategies to increase children's vegetable intake in home and community settings: a systematic review of literature, 13, e12276. Maternal & Child Nutrition, doi: 10.1111/mcn.12276.
References
- Abraham C. & Michie S. (2008) A taxonomy of behavior change techniques used in interventions. Health Psychology 27, 379–387. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ahern S.M., Caton S.J., Blundell P. & Hetherington M.M. (2014) The root of the problem: increasing root vegetable intake in preschool children by repeated exposure and flavor flavor learning. Appetite 80, 154–160. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Annesi J.J., Tennant G., Westcott W.L., Faigenbaum A.D. & Smith A.E. (2009) Effects of the Youth Fit for Life protocol on physiological, psychological, and behavioral factors at YMCA Calgary after‐school care sites. Psychological Reports 104, 879–895. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Anzman S.L., Rollins B.Y. & Birch L.L. (2010) Parental influence on children's early eating environments and obesity risk: implications for prevention. International Journal of Obesity 34, 1116–1124. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Australian Bureau of Statistics (2014) Australian Health Survey: Nutrition First Results – Food and Nutrients, 2011‐12. Commonwealth of Australia: Canberra, Australia. [Google Scholar]
- Baker P.R.A., Francis D.P., Soares J., Weightman A.L. & Foster C. (2011) Community wide interventions for increasing physical activity. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, April 13. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Bayer O., von Kries R., Strauss A., Mitschek C., Toschke A.M., Hose A. et al. (2009) Short‐ and mid‐term effects of a setting based prevention program to reduce obesity risk factors in children: a cluster‐randomized trial. Clinical Nutrition 28, 122–128. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bell L.K., Hendrie G.A., Hartley J. & Golley R.K. (2015) Impact of a nutrition award scheme on the food and nutrient intakes of 2‐ to 4‐year olds attending long day care. Public Health Nutrition 18, 2634–2642. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Birch L.L. & Ventura A.K. (2009) Preventing childhood obesity: what works? International Journal of Obesity 33, S74–S81. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boeing H., Bechthold A., Bub A., Ellinger S., Haller D., Kroke A. et al. (2012) Critical review: vegetables and fruit in the prevention of chronic diseases. European Journal of Nutrition 51, 637–663. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bouhlal S., Issanchou S., Chabanet C. & Nicklaus S. (2014) ‘Just a pinch of salt’. An experimental comparison of the effect of repeated exposure and flavor‐flavor learning with salt or spice on vegetable acceptance in toddlers. Appetite 83, 209–217. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bourke M., Whittaker P.J. & Verma A. (2014) Are dietary interventions effective at increasing fruit and vegetable consumption among overweight children? A systematic review. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health 68, 485–490. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Capacci S. & Mazzocchi M. (2011) Five‐a‐day, a price to pay: an evaluation of the UK program impact accounting for market forces. Journal of Health Economics 30, 87–98. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Castro D.C., Samuels M. & Harman A.E. (2013) Growing healthy kids: a community garden‐based obesity prevention program. American Journal of Preventive Medicine 44, S193–S199. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Caton S.J., Ahern S.M., Remy E., Nicklaus S., Blundell P. & Hetherington M.M. (2013) Repetition counts: repeated exposure increases intake of a novel vegetable in UK pre‐school children compared to flavour‐flavour and flavour‐nutrient learning. British Journal of Nutrition 109, 2089–2097. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Caton S.J., Blundell P., Ahern S.M., Nekitsing C., Olsen A., Moller P. et al. (2014) Learning to Eat Vegetables in Early Life: The Role of Timing, Age and Individual Eating Traits. PLoS ONE 9 (5): e97609. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cooke L., Chambers L., Anez E. & Wardle J. (2011) Facilitating or undermining? The effect of reward on food acceptance. A narrative review. Appetite 57, 493–497. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Corsini N., Slater A., Harrison A., Cooke L. & Cox D.N. (2011) Rewards can be used effectively with repeated exposure to increase liking of vegetables in 4‐6‐year‐old children. Public Health Nutrition 16, 942–951. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- COUNCIL, N. H. A. M. R (2013) Fruits & Veggies‐More Matters [Online]. Available: http://www.fruitsandveggiesmorematters.org/ [Accessed 6/2/2014.
- Cushing C.C. & Steele R.G. (2010) A meta‐analytic review of eHealth interventions for pediatric health promoting and maintaining behaviors. Journal of Pediatric Psychology 35, 1–13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davis J.N., Ventura E.E., Cook L.T., Gyllenhammer L.E. & Gatto N.M. (2011) LA Sprouts: a gardening, nutrition, and cooking intervention for Latino youth improves diet and reduces obesity. Journal of the American Dietetic Association 111, 1224–1230. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Bock F., Breitenstein L. & Fischer J.E. (2011) Positive impact of a pre‐school‐based nutritional intervention on children's fruit and vegetable intake: results of a cluster‐randomized trial. Public Health Nutrition 15, 466–475. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Wild V.W.T., de Graaf C. & Jager G. (2013) Effectiveness of flavour nutrient learning and mere exposure as mechanisms to increase toddler's intake and preference for green vegetables. Appetite 64, 89–96. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diep C.S., Chen T.A., Davies V.F., Baranowski J.C. & Baranowski T. (2014) Influence of behavioral theory on fruit and vegetable intervention effectiveness among children: a meta‐analysis. Journal of Nutrition Education and Behavior 46, 506–546. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Engels H.J., Gretebeck R.J., Gretebeck K.A. & Jimenez L. (2005) Promoting healthful diets and exercise: efficacy of a 12‐week after‐school program in urban African Americans. Journal of the American Dietetic Association 105, 455–459. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Evans C.E., Christian M.S., Cleghorn C.L., Greenwood D.C. & Cade J.E. (2012) Systematic review and meta‐analysis of school‐based interventions to improve daily fruit and vegetable intake in children aged 5 to 12 y. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 96, 889–901. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Francis D.P., Baker P.R.A., Doyle J., Hall B.J. & Waters E. (2011) Cochrane Update: reviewing interventions delivered to whole communities: learnings and recommendations for application to policy, practice and evidence devlopment. Journal of Public Health 33, 322–325. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Freedman M.R. & Nickell A. (2010) Impact of after‐school nutrition workshops in a public library setting. Journal of Nutrition Education and Behavior 42, 192–196. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gholami M., Wiedemann A., Knoll N. & Schwarzer R. (2015) Mothers improve their daughters' vegetable intake: a randomized controlled trial. Psychology, Health & Medicine 20, 1–7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Golley R.K., Bell L., Matwiejczyk L. & Hartley J. (2012) South Australian long day care centres engaged with a nutrition incentive award scheme show consistency with mealtime practice guidelines. Nutrition & Dietetics 69, 130–136. [Google Scholar]
- Haire‐Joshu D., Elliott M.B., Caito N.M., Hessler K., Nanney M., Hale N. et al. (2008) High 5 for kids: the impact of a home visiting program on fruit and vegetable intake of parents and their preschool children. Preventive Medicine: An International Journal Devoted to Practice and Theory 47, 77–82. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hausner H., Olsen A. & Moller P. (2012) Mere exposure and flavour‐flavour learning increase 2‐3 year‐old children's acceptance of a novel vegetable. Appetite 58, 1152–1159. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hendrie G.A., Brindal E., Baird D. & Gardner C. (2012) Improving children's dairy food and calcium intake: can intervention work? A systematic review of the literature. Public Health Nutrition 16, 365–376. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hetherington M., Schwartz C., Madrelle J., Croden F., Nekitsing C., Vereijken C. et al. (2015) A step by step introduction to vegetables at the beginning of complementary feeding. The effects of early and repeated exposure. Appetite 84, 280–290. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Horne P.J., Greenhalgh J., Erjavec M., Lowe C.F., Viktor S. & Whitaker C.J. (2011) Increasing pre‐school children's consumption of fruit and vegetables. A modelling and rewards intervention. Appetite 56, 375–385. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Horton L.A., Parada H., Slymen D.J., Arredondo E., Ibarra L. & Ayala G.X. (2013) Targeting children's dietary behaviors in a family intervention: 'Entre familia: reflejos de salud'. Salud Pública de México 55 (Suppl 3), 397–405. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kimmons J., Gillespie C., Seymour J., Serdula M. & Blanck H.M. (2009) Fruit and vegetable intake among adolescents and adults in the United States: percentage meeting individualized recommendations. Medscape Journal of Medicine 11, 26. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klein W., Sheppard J., Suls J., Rothman A. & Croyle R. (2015) Realizing the promise of social psychology in improving public health. Personality and Social Psychology Review 19, 77–92. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Knai C., Pomerleau J., Lock K. & McKee M. (2006) Getting children to eat more fruit and vegetables: a systematic review. Preventive Medicine 42, 85–95. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Latif H., Watson K., Nguyen N., Thompson D., Baranowski J., Jago R. et al. (2011) Effects of goal setting on dietary and physical activity changes in the Boy Scout badge projects. Health Education & Behavior 38, 521–529. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Livingstone M.B.E. & Robson P.J. (2000) Measurement of dietary intake in children. Proceedings of the Nutrition Society 59, 279–293. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Magarey A., Mckean S. & Daniels L. (2006) Evaluation of fruit and vegetable intakes of Australian adults: the National Nutrition Survey 1995. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Public Health 30, 32–37. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mak T.N., Prynne C.J., Cole D., Fitt E., Roberts C., Bates B. et al. (2012) Assessing eating context and fruit and vegetable consumption in children: new methods using food diaries in the UK National Diet and Nutrition Survey Rolling Programme. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity 9, 126–141. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martinez‐Andrade G.O., Cespedes E.M., Rifas‐Shiman S.L., Romero‐Quechol G., Gonzalez‐Unzaga M.A., Benitez‐Trejo M.A. et al. (2014) Feasibility and impact of Creciendo Sanos, a clinic‐based pilot intervention to prevent obesity among preschool children in Mexico City. BMC Pediatrics 14, 77. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maynard M., Gunnell D., Ness A.R., Abraham L., Bates C.J. & Blane D. (2006) What influences diet in early old age? Prospective and cross‐sectional analyses of the Boyd Orr cohort. European Journal of Public Health 16, 316–324. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Michie S., Ashford S., Sniehotta F.F., Dombrowski S.U., Bishop A. & French D.P. (2011) A refined taxonomy of behaviour change techniques to help people change their physical activity and healthy eating behaviours: The CALO‐RE taxonomy. Psychology & Health 26, 1479–1498. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mikkila V., Rasanen L., Raitakari O., Pietinen P. & Viikari J. (2005) Consistent dietary patterns identified from childhood to adulthood: the cardiovascular risk in Young Finns Study. British Journal of Nutrition 93, 923–931. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Namenek Brouwer R.J. & Benjamin Neelon S.E. (2013) Watch me grow: a garden‐based pilot intervention to increase vegetable and fruit intake in preschoolers. BMC Public Health 13, 363. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- National Health and Medical Research Council (2013) Eat for Health: Educators Guide. National Health and Medical Research Council: Canberra, Australia. [Google Scholar]
- Nicklaus S. & Remy E. (2013) Early origins of overeating. Tracking between early food habits and later eating patterns. Current Obesity Report 2, 179–184. [Google Scholar]
- Remy E., Issanchou S., Chabanet C. & Nicklaus S. (2013) Repeated exposure of infants at complementary feeding to a vegetable puree increases acceptance as effectively as flavor‐flavor learning and ore effectively than flavor‐nutirent learning. The Journal of Nutrition 143, 1194–1200. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schwinn T.M., Schinke S., Fang L. & Kandasamy S. (2014) A web‐based, health promotion program for adolescent girls and their mothers who reside in public housing. Addictive Behaviors 39, 757–760. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Slusser W.M., Sharif M.Z., Erausquin J.T., Kinsler J.J., Collin D. & Prelip M.L. (2013) Improving overweight among at‐risk minority youth: results of a pilot intervention in after‐school programs. Journal of Health Care for the Poor and Underserved 24, 12–24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Somerville M., Kessler L.A., Wallace S.P. & Burns‐Whitmore B. (2012) The effect of a garden‐based nutrition education program on the consumption of fruits and vegetables by Hispanic children. Californian Journal of Health Promotion 10, 20–25. [Google Scholar]
- Tabak R.G., Tate D.F., Stevens J., Siega‐Riz A.M. & Ward D.S. (2012) Family ties to health program: a randomized intervention to improve vegetable intake in children. Journal of Nutrition Education and Behavior 44, 166–171. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thomas H. (2003) Quality assessment tool for quantitative studies. Effective Public Health Practice Project. McMaster University: Toronto. [Google Scholar]
- Thomas B., Ciliska D., Dobbins M. & Micucci S. (2004a) A process for systematically reviewing the literature: providing the research evidence for public health nursing interventions. Worldview Evidence Based Nursing 1, 176–184. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thomas J., Harden A., Oakley A., Oliver S., Sutcliffe K., Rees R. et al. (2004b) Integrating qualitative research with trials in systematic reviews. Bristish Medical Journal 328, 1010–1012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thompson F.E., Subar A.F., Loria C.M., Reedy J.L. & Baranowski T. (2010) Need for techological innovation in dietary assessment. Journal of the American Dietetic Association 110, 48–51. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- U.S. Department of Agriculture & U.S. Department of Health and Human Services (2010) Dietary Guidelines for Americans,2010, 7th edn. U.S. Government Printing Office: Washington D.C., U.S.A.. [Google Scholar]
- Witt K.E. & Dunn C. (2012) Increasing fruit and vegetable consumption among preschoolers: evaluation of color me healthy. Journal of Nutrition Education and Behavior 44, 107–113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wolfenden L., Wyse R., Campbell E., Brennan L., Campbell K.J., Fletcher A. et al. (2014) Randomized controlled trial of a telephone‐based intervention for child fruit and vegetable intake: long‐term follow‐up. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 99, 543–550. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wolfenden L., Wyse R.J., Britton B.I., Campbell K.J., Hodder R.K., Stacey F.G., et al. (2012) Interventions for increasing fruit and vegetable consumption in children aged 5 years and under. Cochrane Database Syst Rev, 11, Cd008552. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Wright K., Norris K., Giger J.N. & Suro Z. (2012) Improving healthy dietary behaviors, nutrition knowledge, and self‐efficacy among underserved school children with parent and community involvement. Childhood Obesity 8, 347–356. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yngve A., Wolf A., Poortvliet E., Elmadfa I., Brug J., Ehrenblad B. et al. (2005) Fruit and vegetable intake in a sample of 11‐year‐old children in 9 European countries: the Pro Children Cross‐sectional Survey. Annals of Nutrition and Metabolism 49, 236–245. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zajonc R. (1968) Attitudinal effects of mere exposure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Monograph Supplement 9, 1–27. [Google Scholar]
- Zeinstra G.G., Koelen M.A., Kok F.J. & de Graaf C. (2009) Children's hard‐wired aversion to pure vegetable tastes. A 'failed' flavour‐nutrient learning study. Appetite 52, 528–530. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Additional Supporting information may be found in the online version of this article at the publisher's web site.
Supplementary File 1: Comprehensive data extraction data for review articles (.xls)
Supplementary File 2: PRISMA checklist.
Supporting info item
Supporting info item