Abstract
To understand temporally extended events, the human brain needs to accumulate information continuously across time. Interruptions that require switching of attention to other event sequences disrupt this process. To reveal neural mechanisms supporting integration of event information, we measured brain activity with functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) from 18 participants while they viewed 6.5‐minute excerpts from three movies (i) consecutively and (ii) as interleaved segments of approximately 50‐s in duration. We measured inter‐subject reliability of brain activity by calculating inter‐subject correlations (ISC) of fMRI signals and analyzed activation timecourses with a general linear model (GLM). Interleaving decreased the ISC in posterior temporal lobes, medial prefrontal cortex, superior precuneus, medial occipital cortex, and cerebellum. In the GLM analyses, posterior temporal lobes were activated more consistently by instances of speech when the movies were viewed consecutively than as interleaved segments. By contrast, low‐level auditory and visual stimulus features and editing boundaries caused similar activity patterns in both conditions. In the medial occipital cortex, decreases in ISC were seen in short bursts throughout the movie clips. By contrast, the other areas showed longer‐lasting differences in ISC during isolated scenes depicting socially‐relevant and suspenseful content, such as deception or inter‐subject conflict. The areas in the posterior temporal lobes also showed sustained activity during continuous actions and were deactivated when actions ended at scene boundaries. Our results suggest that the posterior temporal and dorsomedial prefrontal cortices, as well as the cerebellum and dorsal precuneus, support integration of events into coherent event sequences. Hum Brain Mapp 38:3360–3376, 2017. © 2017 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.
Keywords: temporal integration, interleaved processing, movie, social perception, speech, conflict
INTRODUCTION
Perception of dynamic events in changing environments requires constant accumulation and integration of temporally unfolding information. Interruptions can severely disturb such integration. For example, additional effort is needed to recall what happened in a television series following a commercial break. To integrate temporally unfolding information, the brain builds a predictive model (schema) of the current situation. This facilitates interpretation of future events, detection of event boundaries, and integration of the perceived events with information in memory into a coherent whole [for a review, see Kurby and Zacks, 2008].
Attention modulates processing of both targets and semantically related objects in wide‐spread areas of the cerebral cortex [Çukur et al., 2013; see also Lahnakoski et al., 2014]. The temporoparietal junction and parietal cortical areas participate in detection of perceptual event boundaries [Zacks et al., 2001, 2010, 2011], possibly by monitoring the continuity of action sequences from one event (i.e., independent subset of actions) to another. In particular, temporoparietal regions, complemented by the precuneus and superior occipital lobe, are more sensitive to events where the same action continues across a cut‐point (i.e., edit) between scenes in a video compared with cut‐points where the location or action abruptly changes [Magliano and Zacks, 2011]. These regions may support the integration of ongoing actions across varying viewpoints into a coherent whole. Moreover, differences in gray matter volume in frontal and temporal cortices are associated with both abnormal perception of event boundaries and difficulties in performing action sequences after seeing them in videos [Bailey et al., 2011]. Specifically, reduced gray matter volume in the medial temporal lobe was associated with omitting actions seen in videos whereas, against predictions, increased gray matter volume in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex was correlated with the number of inaccurately performed actions (e.g., packing the correct items but putting them in an inappropriate container) [Bailey et al., 2011].
Temporally scrambled versus intact movies [Hasson et al., 2008a] and audio stories [Lerner et al., 2011] result in lower inter‐subject correlation [ISC; Hasson et al., 2004] in frontal and posterior temporal regions, while ISC in sensory areas remains unaffected. This may reflect different temporal scales of information accumulation in sensory vs. higher‐order areas [Hasson et al., 2015]. Scrambling a movie reduces prior information that the brain can use about preceding events. This compromises the construction of a coherent predictive model of the movie that, in an unscrambled version, would facilitate understanding of events unfolding over long time windows [Hasson et al., 2015]. However, experiments where the chronological order of movie events is simply scrambled cannot distinguish between integration of ongoing, temporally continuous information and integration of information between two parts of the same event sequence when separated in time by intermittent presentation of events from another sequence.
Another line of research has shown that when low‐level visual continuity (rather than temporal order) of a continuous action sequence is disrupted with camera angle changes, ISC in the frontal cortex remains unaffected while ISCs in the occipital and posterior parietal cortices increase [Herbec et al., 2015]. This may be due to the increased visual changes at the cut‐points eliciting synchronized onset‐responses or higher attentional synchrony introduced by, for example, particular camera angles (shot sizes) in the videos [Herbec et al., 2015].
Posterior temporal lobes and midline structures such as precuneus and dorsomedial prefrontal cortex process multiple aspects of social information [see, e.g., Lahnakoski et al., 2012b]. Social perception and particularly speech comprehension relies strongly on contextual prediction of social information and cues [see, e.g., Smirnov et al., 2014]. Usually, such contextual information accumulates over time. Social perception is thus more likely to be affected by manipulation of the temporal structure of incoming stimuli than processing of simple stimulus features or perception of short and contextually isolated event sequences.
The Current Study
Here we aimed at resolving how the human brain integrates temporally extended event sequences by showing subjects excerpts from three movies (i) consecutively and (ii) as interleaved segments with the order of events within each movie retained. We calculated ISCs across subjects in both conditions, and tested whether the strength of the ISC differed across the conditions. We also annotated scene transitions, low‐level visual and auditory features, socially relevant content, and suspense in the movies, to reveal the features whose processing is most sensitive to switching between event sequences. As an additional control analysis, we tested whether continuity disruptions caused deactivations [and continuous cuts activations; Magliano and Zacks, 2011] in the brain regions that presumably integrate event‐sequence information as suggested by the differences between the conditions in the ISC and general linear model (GLM) analyses. Finally, we used an explorative “reverse correlation” approach [extended from Hasson et al., 2004] for revealing the events in the movies that caused the highest ISC differences across conditions.
We hypothesized that viewing the movies as intact versus interleaved segments would trigger stronger ISC in posterior temporal and frontal brain regions [Hasson et al., 2008a; Lerner et al., 2011]. We specifically predicted that the processing of social events, rather than low‐level physical features, would explain the ISC differences because understanding social events requires accumulation of information over longer time periods. By contrast, interleaving is unlikely to affect the processing of low‐level features as suggested by findings showing that the inter‐subject reliability of early sensory‐cortical responses is high even when a movie is played backward [Hasson et al., 2008a].
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Subjects
Eighteen healthy subjects (21–34 years, mean 28; three female; five left‐handed) participated in the study. Subjects reported no history of neurological or psychiatric disorders, and were not taking medications affecting the central nervous system at the time of the study. All subjects provided an informed consent as a part of the protocol approved by the ethics committee of the Hospital District of Helsinki and Uusimaa. The study was carried out in accordance with the guidelines of the declaration of Helsinki.
Stimuli and Procedure
The subjects watched audiovisual excerpts of three feature films: Star Wars: Episode IV—A New Hope (SW; length 6 min 38 s), Indiana Jones: Raiders of the Lost Ark (IJ; length 6 min 22 s), and the James Bond‐movie Golden Eye (GE; length 6 min 24 s). We selected these movies since their storylines are easy to follow and each has a clearly different theme (space opera, classic adventure, and agent adventure, respectively) making it easy for the subjects to know which movie they are currently viewing. The excerpts were taken from the beginning of the films, excluding opening credits and other text. They were subsequently cut into eight short segments (mean duration 49 ± 14 s, 24 segments in total) following the original cut‐points in the movies, so that the abrupt audiovisual changes at the cut‐points were similar in the two presentation conditions (see below).
The clips were presented in two ways (Fig. 1). In the Consecutive‐condition, the excerpts from each movie were presented one after another without breaks. Thus, the participants viewed the 6‐minute excerpts as if they would have been watching the original movies. In the Interleaved‐condition, the clips were also shown without breaks but their order was interleaved in a pseudo‐random manner so that the movie (and thus also the plotline) changed after each segment, while the temporal order of segments within each original movie was retained. In other words, all the storylines unfolded in their original logical order yet the participants were required to switch between the storylines approximately once every 50 s. The presentation order of the conditions was counterbalanced across subjects to remove order effects between the conditions. Subjects were instructed to follow the plot of each of the movies, and to prepare to answer questions about the movie contents after the experiment. However, only ratings of familiarity of the movies and evaluations of the clip transitions were collected after the experiment. To aid the following of the movie events, each movie excerpt depicted only a single sequence of events. Thus, the subjects had to integrate temporally fragmented information only in the interleaved condition.
The beginning of the first video was synchronized to the beginning of fMRI sequence using Presentation software (Neurobehavioral Systems Inc., Albany, California, USA). The stimuli were back‐projected on a semitransparent screen using a 3‐micromirror data projector (Christie X3, Christie Digital Systems Ltd., Mönchengladbach, Germany). The subjects viewed the screen at 34 cm viewing distance via a mirror located above their eyes. Audio was played to the subjects with an UNIDES ADU2a audio system (Unides Design, Helsinki, Finland) via plastic tubes through EAR‐tip (Etymotic Research, ER3, IL, USA) earplugs. The audio track loudness was set to be loud enough to be heard over the scanner noise and was adjusted slightly (±2 dB) if the subject reported the sound to be too loud or too quiet before the experiment.
In a separate behavioral experiment after the scanning, subjects first saw the last 10 s of a movie segment, which was immediately followed by a one‐second‐duration arrow indicating the onset of the first 10 s of the subsequent segment. Then subjects used a 9‐point Likert scale to rate (i) how easy it was to follow the transition from one segment to another and (ii) how natural the transition was. Ratings were given for all possible transitions in the Consecutive and Interleaved movies conditions. In the Consecutive condition these constituted of 20‐s excerpts of the natural flow of events in the movies (apart from the two transitions where the last two movies (IJ and GE) started, which were preceded by a different movie in both conditions). Transitions were presented in a random order. Finally, subjects indicated on a 9‐point Likert scale how familiar they were with each of the movies (1—not at all familiar, 9—extremely familiar). Ratings of one subject were lost due to technical problems.
Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging
Functional brain imaging was carried out in the Advanced Magnetic Imaging (AMI) Centre of Aalto University with a 3.0 T GE Signa VH/i MRI scanner with HDxt upgrade (GE Healthcare Ltd., Chalfont St Giles, UK) using a 16‐channel receiving head coil (MR Instruments Inc., MN, USA). The imaging area consisted of 36 functional gradient‐echo planar oblique slices covering the whole brain (thickness 4 mm, no gap, in‐plane resolution 3.75 mm × 3.75 mm, voxel matrix 64 × 64, TE 30 ms, TR 1800 ms, flip angle 75°, ASSET parallel imaging, R factor 2). ASSET calibration was performed prior to each imaging session. A total of 690 images were acquired continuously during both functional imaging sessions. In addition, a T1‐weighted spoiled gradient echo volume was acquired for anatomical alignment (SPGR pulse sequence, TE 1.9 ms, TR 10 ms, flip angle 15°, ASSET parallel imaging, R factor 2). The T1 image consisted of 182 axial slices, in‐plane resolution was 1 mm × 1 mm, matrix 256 × 256, and slice thickness 1 mm with no gap.
Preprocessing
Data were preprocessed with FSL [Smith et al., 2004; Woolrich et al., 2009]. Motion correction was applied using MCFLIRT [Jenkinson et al., 2002], and non‐brain matter was removed using BET [Smith, 2002]. The intensity distortions of the anatomical images were corrected prior to brain extraction, and values for intensity threshold and threshold gradient in BET were searched manually by changing the parameters and visually inspecting each brain‐extracted volume. Using FLIRT, the datasets were registered to 2 mm MNI152 (Montreal Neurological Institute) standard‐space template using the brain extracted T1 weighted image of each individual subject as an intermediate step [Jenkinson et al., 2002]. Registration both from functional to anatomical images and from anatomical images to standard template volumes was done using 9 degrees of freedom, allowing for translation, rotation, and scaling. Volume data were smoothed using a Gaussian kernel with full width at half maximum (FWHM) of 8.0 mm. High‐pass temporal filtering was applied using Gaussian‐weighted least‐squares straight line fitting, with sigma 200 s, with the first 4 volumes of each dataset discarded to allow the magnetization to stabilize in the tissue (a fixation cross was presented during this time). The last 37 volumes of the data, which were recorded after the end of the stimulus, were removed leaving 649 volumes for the final analyses.
Analysis of Behavioral Data
Mean ratings for easiness to follow and naturalness of the transitions in the Consecutive versus Interleaved conditions were compared across subjects with paired t‐tests. The transitions to the first clips of each movie were excluded from the analysis because they required reorienting to a new storyline in both conditions.
fMRI data analysis
All fMRI results are visualized on the “Conte‐69” cerebral surface atlas and “Colin” cerebellar surface using Caret software [version 5.616; Van Essen et al., 2001].
Inter‐subject correlation analysis
The ISCs for each voxel of the brain over both conditions were calculated using ISC toolbox [https://www.nitrc.org/projects/isc-toolbox/; Kauppi et al., 2014]. To test for significance of ISC, we calculated the ISC between conditions (i.e., across pairs where the two participants were watching different (intact vs. segmented) versions of the stimuli) to build an empirical null distribution of ISC values. Because the first 50 s of stimulation in both conditions was the same, the ISC values in the null distribution were inflated due to the correlated activity during this time window. Thus, we used the 99th percentile of the observed values as a conservative threshold of significance without additional corrections1. Differences in within‐condition ISC strength between Consecutive and Interleaved conditions were tested using Pearson–Filon statistics on Z transformed correlation coefficients, as implemented in the ISC toolbox [Kauppi et al., 2014]. Specifically, this is accomplished by using a non‐parametric permutation test that randomly re‐labels the conditions (switching signs of pairwise differences) 25,000 times with the null hypothesis that relabeling does not change the ISC difference across conditions. The test concurrently controls the family‐wise error rate (FWER) at P < 0.05 by building the null distribution out of the maximal group differences observed in the whole brain. These differences across conditions were also essentially unchanged when the data was analyzed after excluding the first clip.
General linear model analysis
All GLM analyses were performed in SPM12 (http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm). Canonical hemodynamic response function was used as the basis function and high‐pass filter period was set to 128 s. Event‐related analysis of the movie transitions was performed by modeling the movie onsets as events in the design matrix, and a contrast was calculated between the transitions in the Consecutive versus Interleaved conditions. The first clips of each movie were excluded from the event related analysis because an abrupt change in the visual stimulus and event context preceded these clips in both conditions.
To control for activity related to low‐level sensory features, we extracted the visual contrast edges and sound power (root‐mean‐squared power; RMS) from the stimuli [for details, see Lahnakoski et al., 2012a]. Additionally, optic flow was estimated for each frame of the movie using a Matlab toolbox [http://people.csail.mit.edu/celiu/OpticalFlow/, Liu, 2009] and color intensity was estimated as the product of the mean color saturation and color value (brightness) of each frame (i.e., highly saturated bright colors received a high score and subdued and/or dark colors received a low score). Speech within the sound track of each movie segment was annotated using Audacity 1.3.12‐beta (http://audacity.sourceforge.net/). Additionally, prominence of faces, biological motion and non‐biological motion in the movies were annotated manually with 5‐Hz sampling frequency by two independent raters using a web‐based rating system [based on Nummenmaa et al., 2012]. The rating was done by moving a scrollbar beside the video up or down with the mouse while watching the movies. The annotations were loaded to MATLAB, reordered to appropriate order for each condition, rounded to 10‐ms resolution, and down‐sampled to match the TR of the fMRI data prior to analysis.
Stimulus‐feature dependent brain responses were analyzed similarly as those related to the movie clip onsets except that each stimulus feature was modeled with an event and a parametric modulator with one value for each functional volume. Correlation matrix and timecourses of the stimulus features after convolution with a canonical hemodynamic response function are shown in Figure 2. While most features were independent from each other, presence of faces and speech, non‐biological motion and RMS, as well as optic flow and biological motion, were correlated. We performed the analyses both in independent GLMs for each feature and in a combined GLM with all features entered as separate parametric modulators without orthogonalization. Both approaches yielded similar results, suggesting that the collinearity does not confound the results.
As a validation analysis, we also replicated the analyses from a prior study [Magliano and Zacks, 2011] on action‐continuity processing in the brain during movie viewing. To this end, we performed a conjunction analysis of areas showing positive responses to all cut‐points in the movies and negative responses to action discontinuities at a subset of the cut‐points. Each first‐level map was thresholded at FDR corrected q < 0.05 and the binary volumes were multiplied together to produce the conjunction results. To this end, all cut‐points in the movies were semi‐automatically extracted from the videos in Matlab. First, candidate cut‐points were extracted based on root‐mean‐square change of pixel values between subsequent frames. These cut‐points were then verified by viewing through the identified cut‐intervals. Incorrectly identified cut points were removed, and missed cut‐points were added frame by frame by annotating each frame either as a cut (first frame after cut‐point) or a non‐cut. Then, each of the cut‐points was inspected to reveal whether the action presented in the movie was continuous over the cut‐point. The responses to (i) cut‐points depicting discontinuous actions and (ii) all cut‐points were then inserted as event onsets in GLM analyses for estimating their conjunction. The analyses were repeated with both models in a single GLM and in two independent GLMs but the results were nearly identical due to low correlation between the models. Thus, we present only the results where the models were estimated independently.
Finally, we tested whether segmentation of the movies would break down their suspense, thus possibly leading to decreased ISC in brain regions involved in attention and emotion. To that end, we recruited 10 additional participants who rated continuously their experienced suspense while viewing the videos in either the Consecutive or Interleaved condition (five observers per condition). Mean condition‐wise suspense ratings were used to predict BOLD responses in a GLM as described earlier, with the exception that we used a lower high‐pass filter cutoff frequency (512 s period, corresponding to the length of the full movie excerpts) due to the slowly changing suspense ratings. Given the slow and highly autocorrelated signals, we also performed the GLM analysis 20 times with temporally shifted models (shift >±5 samples; independent shift for each subject) to estimate the true null probability. We used these null analyses to build the null distribution of t‐values and find the FDR corrected threshold of statistical significance (q > 0.05).
Exploratory analysis of timepoints contributing to the ISC differences
To localize timepoints that contributed to ISC differences across conditions, we reordered the timepoints in the Interleaved condition so that volumes recorded during the same moment in the videos were aligned in both conditions. We then standardized the subjects' voxelwise timecourses and computed a matrix of pairwise products of these values over subjects for each timepoint and each voxel:
where Xi,x,y,z is the BOLD timecourse of subject i at voxel [x,y,z], μ, and σ are the means and standard deviations of the BOLD timecourses, and ∘ indicates the Hadamard (element‐wise) product. Averaging these products over subject pairs and summing over time and dividing by the number of timepoints minus one directly gives the ISC value for each voxel:
where n is the number of subjects, n * (n − 1)/2 is the total number of subject pairs and T is the number of timepoints. Thus, the timepoints showing the largest differences are the ones that contribute most to the difference between conditions over the entire dataset. For visualization, we performed paired t‐tests over the pairs of subjects for each timepoint and voxel in the brain to find significant differences (P < 0.05, Bonferroni corrected). Further, to highlight the instances that showed the most robust differences across conditions in the regions showing differences over the whole movie, we visualized at which time points the voxels in these regions on average showed significant differences (P < 0.05, Bonferroni corrected by number of clusters) in the mean Z‐score products over subject pairs. For a more stringent hypothesis testing without assumptions regarding the underlying distributions, we also performed a non‐parametric permutation test (1,000 permutations) of difference normalized by standard error of the mean (corresponding to t‐values) across conditions by randomly relabeling the conditions for each subject and by building a null distribution from the observed differences. This test was performed both for individual voxels within the regions of interest (ROIs) and for mean ROI timecourses.
To simplify the exploratory visualization of the timecourses, the clusters showing significant ISC differences were first pruned by 2 mm on all sides to detach weakly connected parts from the main clusters. Clusters smaller than 27 voxels (3 × 3 × 3 voxels) were removed to reduce the number of ROIs in the exploratory analysis. Edge‐voxels that correlated poorly (r < 0.5 in the across‐subjects mean signal) with the mean timecourse of the clusters were pruned removing in total 71 voxels. This left 2,586 voxels for the analysis. Finally, the voxels within the ROIs were grouped with hierarchical clustering (correlation distance, average distance linkage) to find temporally consistent regions that showed distinct synchrony patterns compared with each other. Clustering was repeated with 2–10 clusters to find the optimal number of clusters where within‐cluster correlation distribution was maximally separable from across‐clusters distribution (i.e., there was minimal overlap of correlation values between these two distributions). The optimal number of clusters was 4 with this measure. For visualization, the remaining clusters were inflated by 3 mm on all sides to make them better visible when plotted on the inflated brain surface.
To compare temporal distributions of peaks in the Z‐score products, we calculated the kurtosis of the mean timecourse values of each cluster in both conditions, which describes the heaviness of the tails of the distribution. We also calculated the kurtosis of the across‐conditions difference of the mean timecourses. To characterize how uniformly synchrony peaks were distributed over time we further calculated the burstiness (B) of the timecourses [Goh and Barabási, 2008] that is defined as
where mτ and στ are the mean and standard deviation of the number of events in time windows of a given size. A completely regular signal would thus have a burstiness value of −1 and a maximally bursty signal a value 1. For defining the threshold of an event (peak) we binarized the mean timecourses at Bonferroni corrected (over 4 clusters and 649 timepoints) P < 0.05 from a surrogate distribution of a million values calculated assuming that the underlying signal values for each subject were independently drawn from a standard normal distribution. We repeated the calculation for windows of 20–200 samples in 20‐sample intervals (20, 40, …, 200).
To find the most consistent peaks of synchrony across voxels for reverse correlation exploration, we performed a t‐test over the voxels in the clusters at each timepoint to find occasions where differences across conditions were maximal, thus maximally contributing to the ISC difference. To pinpoint the most robust differences, we also performed a permutation test with 1,000 permutations similar to the one described above for difference in voxel and ROI timecourses over subjects.
RESULTS
Following the movies after clip transitions was significantly more difficult in the Interleaved (M = 8.56) versus Consecutive (M = 6.36) condition (t(16) ≈ 4.1, P ≈ 0.001; Fig. 3a). Transitions within a movie (M = 8.29) were also perceived as significantly more natural than those between movies (M = 1.67; t(16) ≈ 25.6, P ≈ 2 * 10−14; Fig. 3b).
Figure 4 shows brain areas with significant (P < 0.01 in across‐conditions ISC distribution; uncorrected) ISC during both conditions. These areas were very similar including wide bilateral areas in occipital, temporal and parietal cortex, frontal eye fields [Paus, 1996] extending to ventral premotor cortex, and cerebellum.
As shown in Figure 5, Consecutive versus Interleaved movies resulted in stronger ISC in temporal regions (left posterior STS, temporoparietal junction and middle temporal gyrus, and right posterior STS), in bilateral superior and medial occipital cortices, and in the precuneus and cerebellum. The areas that showed different sensitivity to speech across conditions (white outlines in Fig. 5; see also Fig. 7) overlap extensively with the posterior temporal lobe areas showing significantly stronger ISC. Cluster centroid coordinates are listed in Table 1. Interleaved versus Consecutive movies caused higher ISCs in small clusters in bilateral extrastriate visual cortex and right superior temporal gyrus.
Table 1.
MNI coordinates | |||
---|---|---|---|
X | Y | Z | |
Crus II | 34.7 | 21.9 | 19.9 |
MTG L | 18.5 | 46.7 | 28.3 |
MTG/STS R | 75.1 | 43.6 | 37.2 |
mOCC | 45.5 | 21.1 | 39 |
TPJ/pSTS L | 16.6 | 39.4 | 42.1 |
PCu | 43.6 | 32.3 | 62.8 |
dmPFC a | 50.6 | 76.2 | 65.2 |
dmPFC p | 55.2 | 63.8 | 69.4 |
Due to the differences in difficulty of following movie versus scene transitions, we also tested whether responses to the transitions were driving the ISC differences across conditions in our analyses. Figure 6 shows brain regions responding significantly (in a GLM) to scene and movie transitions in the Consecutive and Interleaved conditions. Transitions significantly activated the precuneus, posterior cingulate cortex, lateral parietal cortex, and Crus II of the cerebellum. These areas did not overlap with the clusters showing ISC differences indicated with white outlines. We found no statistically significant differences between the two conditions (q > 0.05 FDR corrected) although, in the thresholded results, movie transitions in the Interleaved condition additionally correlated significantly with activity in anterior middle frontal gyrus (MFG)/frontal pole (FP) and small clusters in the anterior cingulate.
Next we tested whether continuous versus Interleaved presentation of the stimuli selectively influenced the processing of physical stimulus features (Fig. 7). Hot colors indicate brain regions responding to different physical stimulus features over both conditions. Auditory and visual areas responded to low‐level sensory features (RMS energy of soundtrack, high‐frequency contrast edges, color intensity, and optic flow in the video). The loudness of the sound track also correlated with the activity of lateral and ventral visual regions, presumably due to relatively high correlations with visual features and sound RMS (see Fig. 2). Optic flow and biological motion activated similar areas in or near the V5/MT+ and superior occipital cortex. Speech and faces activated relatively similar areas in the temporal lobe, however, speech also activated widespread areas of the superior temporal lobes around the primary auditory cortices whereas faces did not. Conversely, faces but not speech activated occipital regions. The brain responses to low‐level visual and acoustic features were very similar in the Interleaved versus Consecutive conditions. Significant differences were observed only for speech (filled green outline), for which especially posterior temporal responses were stronger in the Consecutive condition. These areas are highly similar to regions showing significant differences in ISC strength across conditions (see Fig. 5).
The areas showing ISC differences were visually very similar to those reported to participate in action continuity processing in a prior study by Magliano and Zacks [2011]. Therefore, we next examined whether the areas showing significant differences in ISC and speech‐related responses would also be selectively active during continuous actions versus action discontinuities in our data. To this end, we replicated a conjunction analysis of Magliano and Zacks [2011] to reveal regions that were selectively activated by cut‐points depicting continuous actions and deactivated by cut‐points with action discontinuities. As shown in Figure 8, areas that were active during action continuity processing (hot colors) are partly overlapping with the areas of the pSTS and MTG that showed differences in ISC (white outlines) and speech responses (turquoise outlines) across conditions. This was, however, observed only in the Consecutive condition. In the Interleaved condition, only small bilateral clusters in the inferior bank of the STS was significantly activated by action continuity processing, yet the difference between Consecutive versus Interleaved conditions was not statistically significant. Other areas showing ISC differences (Pcu, mPFC and Crus II) also showed negative, yet non‐significant, correlations with action discontinuities. The mOC was an exception, as it was activated in all cut‐points, including those where action was discontinuous (see Supporting Information Fig. S1).
To reveal the specific moments that triggered across‐conditions differences in ISC strength, we explored the time‐courses of the clusters showing significantly higher ISC in Consecutive versus Interleaved condition. Figure 9 shows the time‐courses of products of Z‐scores (instantaneous similarity) of subject pairs. The timecourses have been reorganized so that responses to the same timepoints in the stimuli are aligned in both conditions. Panel A shows the hierarchical clusters of voxels, displayed with color‐coding in the correlation matrix of the timecourses. Different clusters are indicated by the same colors on the right and bottom of the matrix, and the same color‐coding indicates the location of the clusters on the brain surface.
Generally, the voxels form two main groups: medial occipital cortex (yellow), and the other brain areas. The latter are further subdivided into ventral temporal cluster (green), PCu‐mPFC cluster (purple) and a cluster consisting of areas of bilateral pSTS/MTG, right mPFC, and left Crus II (orange). Panel B shows mean plots of Z‐score products for both conditions and timepoints of significant group difference for the clusters. Overall, the yellow mOC cluster shows several short synchrony peaks and across‐condition differences that are relatively uniformly distributed over time. By contrast, time courses of other clusters show longer‐duration peaks that are specific to isolated parts of the movies. This is corroborated by lower kurtosis and consistently lower burstiness of timecourses in the mOC compared with the other clusters (kurtosis of timecourses in mOC was 9.6 versus 13.0 to 21.8 in other areas, mean burstiness over time window sizes −0.6 versus −0.3 to −0.1; kurtosis for difference across conditions 6.7 versus 7.4 to 22.0, burstiness −0.6 versus −0.4 to −0.2). Thus, the relatively long periods of significant group differences in the pSTS/MTG, Crus II, PCu, mPFC, and vTC presumably corresponded to events that are particularly differently processed when task switching is required. Panel C shows the consistent between‐condition differences (in t‐tests) in the orange, purple and green clusters that assumedly show moments of content‐specific synchrony. These moments are described in Table 2 and depicted in Supporting Information Video 1. They contain mainly instances of conflict, deception and escape, and also include the opening scenes of the second and third movies when subjects start following a new long narrative. Timepoints that survived a more conservative permutation test are marked with an asterisk. Instances of higher ISC in Consecutive vs. Interleaved conditions that were caused by increased deactivations in Consecutive condition are marked with minus signs in panel C and Table 2 (see red and blue dashed lines in Fig. 9C).
Table 2.
Label | Movie clip at onset | Description |
---|---|---|
a* | GoldenEye 4 | Sneaking in an enemy base, agent 007 is surprised by agent 006 speaking in Russian impersonating an enemy guard; 007 recognizes 006 and the two agents exchange pleasantries and continue the infiltration of the base. |
b | GoldenEye 8 | Agent 006 is held as a hostage at gunpoint to lure out 007; 006 encourages 007 to blow up the chemical containers with explosives they planted rather than surrender. |
c | GoldenEye 8 | Enemy leader counts down from ten to force 007 out and then shoots 006 despite 007 was apparently surrendering. |
d | GoldenEye 8 | Enemy leader orders the enemy soldiers to stop shooting as 007 has dived behind gas tanks that might explode; 007 continues to escape using the tanks as cover. |
e*‐ | Indiana Jones 1 | Indiana studies a map with his companion looking on as a man sneaks behind them and cocks a gun. |
f*‐ | Indiana Jones 1 | Man escapes holding his hand after Indiana disarms him with his whip. Indiana and his companion continue to climb up to a cave. |
g*‐ | Indiana Jones 2 | After a short conversation, Indiana and his companion enter the cave sneaking carefully. |
h‐ | Indiana Jones 2 | Indiana and his companion continue sneaking inside the cave. |
I | Indiana Jones 3 | Indiana orders his companion to stop as he finds a beam of light he suspects is a trap and continues to disarm it. |
j* | Indiana Jones 5 | Indiana disarms another trap and orders his companion to stay behind as he sneaks forward. |
k* | Indiana Jones 7 | Indiana and his companion escape the collapsing cave; Indiana exchanges the idol they found for his whip to get over a chasm but his companion escapes without giving the whip after he receives the idol. |
l*‐ | Star Wars 1 | Star destroyer chases and fires at a rebel ship. |
m‐ | Star Wars 2 | Rebel ship is caught by the Star destroyer as rebel soldiers prepare for an invasion of the imperial soldiers. |
n‐ | Star Wars 4 | Darth Vader enters the ship and looks around at the bodies of dead soldiers. |
o | Star Wars 4 | Surrendered rebel soldiers are being led away by armed storm troopers as a pair of storm troopers finds princess Leia. |
p | Star Wars 4 | After interrogation of princess Leia, a report comes of a jettisoned escape pod; princess Leia is suspected to have lied, and hidden rebel plans in the escape pod. |
Time windows containing timepoints that were significant in the permutation test in Figure 9 are indicated by the asterisks (*). Timepoints of significant difference across conditions caused by deactivations in the Consecutive condition are marked by minus (−) signs.
Finally, because the participants might have experienced higher suspense during the continuous movies we investigated the role of suspense in modulating ISC. Figure 10A shows the areas where brain activity correlated with the suspense ratings across both conditions (positive: parametric in green, non‐parametric in hot colors; negative: parametric in purple, non‐parametric in cold colors). Statistically significant differences across conditions inside the areas showing significant suspense‐related activity are indicated by red (Consecutive > Interleaved) and blue (Interleaved > Consecutive) outlines. The areas showing ISC differences are outlined in white. Suspense ratings correlated positively with nearly all regions showing significant ISCs in the standard parametric t‐test. However, only regions in the vicinity of V5/MT+, FEF, posterior parietal cortex, and opercular parietal cortical regions survived the non‐parametric test. Negative responses were seen in lateral parietal and anterior cingulate cortices (ACC), but only a small cluster in the right ACC was significant in the non‐parametric test. Figure 10B shows the timecourses of the suspense ratings in Consecutive (red) and Interleaved (blue) conditions. The scenes showing highest ISC differences in Figure 9C are indicated by gray bars at the bottom of the plot.
DISCUSSION
Our main finding was that when the natural flow of action sequences was interrupted and frequent switching between parallel event‐sequences was required, the inter‐subject reliability of brain responses was decreased in the bilateral posterior STS region and left MTG, mOC, and smaller clusters in the mPFC and cerebellum (Fig. 5). Particularly, responses associated with speech in the posterior temporal lobe were significantly weaker during interleaved movies (Fig. 7). By contrast, low‐level sensory responses were similar in both conditions. Explorative timepoint‐by‐timepoint reverse correlation comparison revealed that events depicting social conflict and deception contributed strongly to the ISC group difference in temporal lobes, medial prefrontal cortex, and Crus II. By contrast, differences in the occipital visual areas were diffusely distributed in time throughout the movie stimuli (Fig. 9). Our results suggest that (i) temporal interleaving specifically modulates processing of social aspects of stimuli, particularly speech, in the STS/MTG, Crus II, vTC, and mPFC, and that (ii) early sensory processes are either not affected or, in the case of early visual cortex, show general reduction of ISC that is not specific to particular events.
Temporal Integration of Events
The ISC maps in both Consecutive and Interleaved conditions were visually very similar (Fig. 4) and accord with those reported in prior studies [see, e.g., Hasson et al., 2004; Hasson et al., 2008a; Lerner et al., 2011]. However, statistical testing revealed that shifting back and forth between the different movies markedly reduced ISC in occipitotemporal cortices (pSTS/TPJ) occipital cortex and precuneus (Fig. 5). Prior research has established that these regions maintain a temporally consistent model of action sequences depicted in movies across “continuity edits” that depict the same continuous action from different camera angles; such edits are continuous over time, space, and action. This is in contrast with edits with a discontinuity, such as between two consecutive shots that show different locations or people. Discontinuous edits where place, time, or action changed during the transition showed little consistent activity [Magliano and Zacks, 2011]. In line with this, shuffling a movie using discontinuity edits markedly lowers ISC in higher‐order associative and frontal cortices across observers [Hasson et al., 2008a; Lerner et al., 2011]. In contrast, adding continuity edits to depiction of continuous action actually increases ISC in the occipital, ventral temporal, and parietal cortices [Herbec et al., 2015] leaving ISCs in the posterior temporal regions unaffected. Indeed, we did not observe significant differences in the event‐related responses to scene onsets when the clips were preceded by the previous clip of the same movie (Consecutive condition) versus a clip from a different movie (Interleaved condition; Fig. 6) despite the reported decrease of naturalness and ease of following the movie versus scene transitions (Fig. 3). Moreover, most subjects found following the events in the Interleaved condition relatively easy and the event related activity due to movie/scene transitions was markedly different from the foci showing differences across conditions in ISC and speech‐related GLM analyses. This suggests that the circuitry activated by scene transitions between movies is also activated similarly at scene transitions during the natural flow of a movie, which also requires reorientation and updating the contents of working memory [Kurby and Zacks, 2008]. Together with the present results (see Fig. 5), these findings suggests that the pSTS region, precuneus, ventral temporal, and medial prefrontal cortices and part of Crus II are involved in building and maintaining a temporally extended model of observed events.
However, simply following continuity of actions between movie cut‐points cannot explain the observed differences in brain responses between conditions, although areas revealed by the conjunction analysis partially overlapped those showing ISC differences in the pSTS/MTG (no differences across conditions; Fig. 8). Moreover, the regions of the temporal lobe showing the greatest deactivations to action discontinuities extend anteriorly from the regions showing ISC/speech‐sensitivity differences and those reported previously [Magliano and Zacks, 2011]. Furthermore, no significant effects were observed in the precuneus and posterior parietal cortex, which have previously been associated with continuity processing [Magliano and Zacks, 2011]. This may depend on the definition of discontinuity or the complexity of the stimuli. For example, the Magliano and Zacks [2011] study used a children's movie with relatively simple actions and practically no speech, while we found that speech consistently activated the anterior parts of STS/MTG that also showed continuity effects in our data. Altogether these data suggest that not all aspects of action continuity processing are similarly affected by action sequence interleaving in the timescales studied here.
The capacity for prediction and temporal integration may be one of the key properties that allow the pSTS region to serve as a “hub” for social processing [Lahnakoski et al., 2012b]. The pSTS/TPJ region has been proposed to participate in several functions such as integration of “what” and “where” information [Allison et al., 2000], inference of intentionality [Nummenmaa and Calder, 2009] and fine‐grained social evaluation [Pantelis et al., 2015]. These functions could be subserved by a more general function of updating a model of the current situational context used to predict the upcoming events and detect prediction violations. Similar predictive schemes have been proposed to underlie comparison of “efference copies” from the motor/premotor system to current percepts in perception‐action cycles, for example, during speech production [for a review, see e.g. Rauschecker and Scott, 2009]. Updating predictions of the current situation is a continuous process, which may be interrupted by a sudden change in stimulus events. This, in turn, may lead to reduction in the response reliability across observers.
Social and Sensory Feature Driven Brain Responses
We found that speech‐sensitive areas in the pSTS/TPJ showed greatest interleaving effects, and presence of human speech correlated more with the activity in these areas in the Consecutive versus Interleaved condition (Fig. 7). By contrast, the low‐level stimulus features (visual motion, contrast, color, and sound loudness) were associated with activation of the corresponding sensory regions in both conditions, with no differences across conditions in the GLM analysis. This suggests that the observed differences in brain activity may relate specifically to processing of language and possibly other socially relevant movie content. However, our stimulus model is not exhaustive and we cannot rule out that processing of non‐modeled low‐level features could also be affected due to top‐down influences, such as attention [see, e.g., Çukur et al., 2013], although the stimulus material itself was the same in both conditions.
Presence of speech also predicted activity in the Crus II of the cerebellum whose synchrony timecourse was similar to that in pSTS. This is in accordance with the emerging view of cerebellar contribution to social processing [Van Overwalle et al., 2014]. In speech perception, cerebellum appears to participate in temporal, or sequential, prediction [for a review, see Moberget and Ivry, 2016]. Our findings support its role in prediction and following of ongoing social events.
Reverse Correlation Analysis
In the pSTS/MTG, mPFC, vTC, and Crus II, reduced ISC in Interleaved versus Consecutive condition was mainly explained by a few scenes and not by either constantly higher ISC or differences at the transition moments between movie clips. Reverse correlation analysis revealed that the scenes showing most consistent between‐conditions differences in the temporal and frontal regions contained most often deception or conflict (e.g., a man sneaking up and pulling a gun behind Indiana Jones before being disarmed by him; discussion about princess Leia hiding plans in an escape pod after she was questioned).
Half of the scenes showing significantly higher ISCs in Consecutive versus Interleaved condition were driven by stronger deactivations rather than activations in the Consecutive versus Interleaved condition. ISC differences in all of the scenes containing speech were driven by high activation peaks, as suggested by the stronger speech related responses. In contrast, none of the scenes where ISC difference was driven by deactivation contained speech. While some of the peaks of synchrony in the precuneus coincide with those in the speech‐sensitive areas, we did not see speech‐related responses in those parts of precuneus that show ISC differences. The synchrony peaks in precuneus were infrequent compared with those in the speech sensitive regions and thus difficult to interpret alone (see Fig. 9C for timepoints, Table 2 for scene descriptions and Supporting Information Video 1 for excerpts from the marked timepoints and the associated synchrony of brain activity). Importantly, since the scenes with the greatest ISC differences do not consistently coincide with movie transitions in the Interleaved condition, there is no obvious need for integrating information differently during these scenes in the two conditions. This suggests that there is a more general difference in the mode of information processing of these brain regions in the two conditions.
In contrast to the areas discussed above, the mOC showed ISC differences throughout the experiment in activity peaks that were relatively uniformly distributed in time. While most of these peaks were of short duration, some peaks lasted for several seconds. Most prominent peaks correspond to scenes with significant change in lighting (lit torch, golden glow of an idol, and ending the shooting of lasers). However, these are not the only scenes with prominent lighting changes. Additionally, in contrast to the temporal lobes, suspense modulated the activity in the visual cortex more strongly in the Interleaved vs. Consecutive condition. This result was in a direction opposite to the ISC results, and the difference in suspense‐related brain activity was slightly more posterior than the areas showing differences in ISC.
Brain Responses to Suspense During Movie Viewing
Scenes showing highest differences in ISC timecourses also contained moments of high suspense. In the GLM analysis, suspense was associated with activity in corticolimbic areas whose ISC and BOLD‐GLM responses are modulated by affective arousal [Nummenmaa et al., 2012, 2014], which is an important component of suspense experience [Lehne and Koelsch, 2015]. However, due to the slowness of the signals, the parametric test was very liberal. With a stricter FDR corrected non‐parametric threshold, the significant effects were restricted to the dorsal attention network (posterior parietal cortex and areas surrounding the bilateral middle temporal visual areas and frontal eye fields, respectively), according with the well‐established influence of affective significance on attention [Vuilleumier, 2005].
Suspense ratings also differed across the conditions, but of the areas showing ISC differences, only small parts of the left posterior temporal lobe showed higher suspense‐related responses in the Consecutive versus Interleaved condition. Moreover, the differences in ISC did not coincide exclusively with moments of high suspense or with moments where the ratings of suspense differed across conditions. Therefore, we conclude that suspense may have contributed to the differences seen in the ISCs and speech responses between conditions in the temporal lobes, but it is not sufficient to explain all of the differences seen in other analyses.
Time Scales of Integration and Memory
The time windows studied here were relatively long in comparison with time‐scales studied for lower‐level integration effects such as change detection in the auditory cortex at a millisecond scale [Mustovic et al., 2003] or behavioral accounts of event integration in videos in the order of 2–3 s [Fairhall et al., 2014]. Prior fMRI studies employing scrambling the temporal order of video clips or audio stories (Hasson et al., 2008a; Lerner et al., 2011] have found that the correlation of activity in areas of the posterior temporal lobes, parts of mPFC and precuneus is decreased—either across or within subjects—when the stimuli are scrambled in short versus long time windows. Temporal integration windows in fMRI data seems to increase up to timescales of 30–40 s, where the areas of significant ISC appear indistinguishable from those elicited by intact videos. However, by testing the magnitude of ISC statistically, we demonstrated that even at scales of 50 s, neural processing of the stimuli is affected even if the regions in the thresholded ISC maps are visually very similar. Moreover, in contrast to the studies employing temporal scrambling, the subjects of the current study could still follow the storylines in their entirety, as the order of movie parts remained unchanged for each stimulus. Additionally, earlier studies did not test which aspects of the stimuli were integrated over long time periods in these brain areas. Our results demonstrate that even though similar levels of ISC differences may be seen in multiple brain regions, different areas may have different profiles that account for the increased ISC. Thus, in future studies, it is important to consider what particular aspects of stimuli are integrated in each area.
Finally, it is possible that differences in ISC may be related to subsequent memory of the events. Earlier results suggest that improved memory encoding during movie viewing is linked with increased ISC [Hasson et al., 2008b] in STS and TPJ that partly overlap with the areas showing ISC differences in the current study. It is thus possible that some of the differences reported here are related to the encoding to or retrieval from long‐term memory.
CONCLUSIONS
Following multiple interleaved events forces the brain to constantly reorient to a new scene, thus reducing the predictability of the events. We conclude that posterior temporal, medial parietal and lateral frontal cortical areas are the key regions participating in the integration of ongoing events. We propose that these regions build and maintain a temporally extended predictive model of ongoing sequence of events.
Supporting information
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
We thank Juha Salmi for fruitful discussions about the current study, Marita Kattelus and Susanna Vesamo for help in fMRI acquisition, and Enrico Glerean for donating code for the stimulus rating system.
Conflicts of interest: None.
Footnotes
For the sake of comparison, we also calculated the ISCs after removing the first movie clip (which was identical in both conditions) and used the maximum observed ISC value between conditions as a threshold for the within‐condition ISC maps. This procedure yielded essentially identical results to the ones calculated with the full timecourses, thus confirming that the chosen threshold effectively controls for multiple comparisons in the current data.
REFERENCES
- Allison T, Puce A, and Mccarthy G (2000): Social perception from visual cues: role of the STS region. Trends Cogn Sci 4:267–278. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bailey HR, Kurby CA, Giovannetti T, Zacks JM (2011): Action perception predicts action performance. Neuropsychologia 51:2294–2304. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Çukur T, Nishimoto S, Huth AG, Gallant JL (2013): Attention during natural vision warps semantic representation across the human brain. Nat Neurosci 16:763–770. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fairhall SL, Albi A, Melcher D (2014): Temporal integration windows for naturalistic visual sequences. PLoS One 9:e102248. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goh K‐I, Barabási A‐L (2008): Burstiness and memory in complex systems. Europhys Lett 81:48002. [Google Scholar]
- Hasson U, Nir Y, Levy I, Furhmann G, Malach R (2004): Intersubject synchronization of cortical activity during natural vision. Science 303:1634–1640. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hasson U, Yang E, Vallines I, Heeger DJ, Rubin N (2008a): A hierarchy of temporal receptive windows in human cortex. J Neurosci 28:2539–2550. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hasson U, Furman O, Clark D, Dudai Y, Davachi L (2008b): Enhanced intersubject correlations during movie viewing correlate with successful episodic encoding. Neuron 57:452–462. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hasson U, Chen J, Honey CJ (2015): Hierarchical process memory: memory as an integral component of information processing. Trends Cogn Sci 19:304–313. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Herbec A, Kauppi J‐P, Jola C, Tohka J, Pollick FE (2015): Differences in fMRI intersubject correlation while viewing unedited and edited videos of dance performance. Cortex 71:341–348. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jenkinson M, Bannister PR, Brady JM, Smith SM (2002): Improved optimisation for the robust and accurate linear registration and motion correction of brain images. NeuroImage 17:825–841. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kauppi J‐P, Pajula J, Tohka J (2014): A versatile software package for inter‐subject correlation based analyses of fMRI. Front Neuroinform 8:2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kurby CA, Zacks JM (2008): Segmentation in the perception and memory of events. Trends Cogn Sci 12:72–79. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lahnakoski JM, Glerean E, Salmi J, Jääskeläinen IP, Sams M, Hari R, Nummenmaa L (2012b): Naturalistic fMRI mapping reveals superior temporal sulcus as the hub for the distributed brain network for social perception. Front Hum Neurosci 6:233. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lahnakoski JM, Salmi J, Jääskeläinen IP, Lampinen J, Glerean E, Tikka P, Sams M (2012a): Stimulus‐related independent component and voxel‐wise analysis of human brain activity during free viewing of a feature film. PLoS One 7:e35215. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lahnakoski JM, Glerean E, Jääskeläinen IP, Hyönä J, Hari R, Sams M, Nummenmaa L (2014): Synchronous brain activity across individuals underlies shared psychological perspectives. NeuroImage 100:316–324. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lehne M, Koelsch S (2015): Toward a general psychological model of tension and suspense. Front Psychol 6:79. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lerner Y, Honey CJ, Silbert LJ, Hasson U (2011): Topographic mapping of a hierarchy of temporal receptive windows using a narrated story. J Neurosci 31:2906–2915. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liu C (2009). Beyond Pixels: Exploring New Representations and Applications for Motion Analysis. Massachusetts: Massachusetts Institute of Technology. [Google Scholar]
- Magliano JP, Zacks JM (2011): The impact of continuity editing in narrative film on event segmentation. Cogn Sci 35:1489–1517. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moberget T, Ivry RB (2016): Cerebellar contributions to motor control and language comprehension: searching for common computation principles. Ann NY Acad Sci 1369:154–171. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mustovic H, Scheffler K, Di Salle F, Esposito F, Neuhoff JG, Hennig J, Seifritz E (2003): Temporal integration of sequential auditory events: silent period in sound pattern activates human planum temporale. NeuroImage 20:429–434. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nummenmaa L, Calder AJ (2009): Neural mechanisms of social attention. Trends Cogn Sci 13:135–143. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nummenmaa L, Glerean E, Viinikainen M, Jääskeläinen IP, Hari R, Sams M (2012): Emotions promote social interaction by synchronizing brain activity across individuals. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 109:9599–9604. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nummenmaa L, Saarimäki H, Glerean E, Gotsopoulos A, Jääskeläinen IP, Hari R, Sams M (2014): Emotional speech synchronizes brains across listeners and engages large‐scale dynamic brain networks. NeuroImage 102:498–509. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pantelis PC, Byrge L, Tyszka JM, Adolphs R, Kennedy DP (2015): A specific hypoactivation of right temporo‐parietal junction/posterior superior temporal sulcus in response to socially awkward situations in autism. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 10:1348–1356. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paus T (1996): Location and function of the human frontal eye‐field: A selective review. Neuropsychologia 34:475–483. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rauschecker JP, Scott SK (2009): Maps and streams in the auditory cortex: nonhuman primates illuminate human speech processing. Nat Neurosci 12:718–724. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smirnov D, Glerean E, Lahnakoski JM, Salmi J, Jääskeläinen IP, Sams M, Nummenmaa L (2014): Fronto‐parietal network supports contex‐dependent speech comprehension. Neuropsychologia 63:293–303. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith SM (2002): Fast robust automated brain extraction. Hum Brain Mapp 17:143–155. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith SM, Jenkinson M, Woolrich MW, Beckmann CF, Behrens TEJ, Johansen‐Berg H, Bannister PR, De Luca M, Drobnjak I, Flitney DE, Niazy RK, Saunders J, Vickers J, Zhang Y, De Stefano N, Brady JM, Matthews PM (2004): Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL. NeuroImage 23:S208–S219. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Essen DC, Drury HA, Dickson J, Harwell J, Hanlon D, Anderson CH (2001): An integrated software suite for surface‐based analyses of cerebral cortex. J Am Med Inform Assoc 8:443–459. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Overwalle F, Baetens K, Mariën P, Vandekerckhove M (2014): Social cognition and the cerebellum: a meta‐analysis of over 350 fMRI studies. NeuroImage 86:554–572. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vuilleumier P (2005): How brains beware: neural mechanisms of emotional attention. Trends Cogn Sci 9:585–594. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Woolrich MW, Jbabdi S, Patenaude B, Chappell M, Makni S, Behrens T, Beckmann C, Jenkinson M, Smith SM (2009): Bayesian analysis of neuroimaging data in FSL. NeuroImage 45:S173–S186. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zacks JM, Braver TS, Sheridan MA, Donaldson DI, Snyder AZ, Ollinger JM, Buckner RL, Raichle ME (2001): Human brain activity time‐locked to perceptual event boundaries. Nat Neurosci 4:651–655. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zacks JM, Speer NK, Swallow KM, Maley CJ (2010): The brain's cutting‐room floor: segmentation of narrative cinema. Front Hum Neurosci 4:168. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zacks JM, Kurby CA, Eisenberg ML, Haroutunian N (2011): Prediction error associated with the perceptual segmentation of naturalistic events. J Cogn Neurosci 23:4057–4066. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.