Skip to main content
Human Brain Mapping logoLink to Human Brain Mapping
. 2012 Sep 21;35(1):238–247. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22168

Regional specificity of sex effects on subcortical volumes across the lifespan in healthy aging

Wenjing Li 1,, Marie‐José van Tol 2,3,4,, Meng Li 1, Wen Miao 1, Yonghong Jiao 5, Hans‐Jochen Heinze 4,6, Bernhard Bogerts 2, Huiguang He 1,, Martin Walter 1,2,3,4,
PMCID: PMC6869103  PMID: 22996803

Abstract

When conceptualizing age‐specific onsets and sex‐specific characteristics of neuropsychiatric diseases in a neurobiological context, it may be crucially important to consider differential trajectories of aging. Here, we investigated effects of age, sex, and their interactions on absolute and relative volumes of subcortical structures with known involvement in psychiatric disorders, including the basal ganglia, thalamus, hippocampus, and amygdala. Structural MRI data of 76 healthy subjects (38 males, 19–70 years) from the ICBM database were analyzed. Age‐related absolute atrophy was generally found in the basal ganglia and thalamus, while in the hippocampus decline was only observed in males, and was generally absent in the amygdala. Disproportionate degeneration in the basal ganglia and thalamus, exceeding cortical decline was specific for females. When allowing higher‐order models, a quadratic model could better describe the negative relation of absolute volume and age in the basal ganglia in males, and generally in the hippocampus and amygdala. We could show that negative age‐relations are highly specific for certain subcortical structures in either gender. Importantly these findings also emphasize the significant impact of analytical strategies when deciding for correction of subcortical volumes to the whole‐brain decline. Specifically, in the basal ganglia disproportionate shrinkage in females was suggested by the relative analysis while absolute volume analysis rather stressed an accelerating decline in older males. Given strong involvement of the basal ganglia in both cognitive aging and emotional regulation, our findings may be crucial for studies investigating the onset and prevalence of dementia and depressive symptoms in male and female aging. Hum Brain Mapp 35:238–247, 2014. © 2012 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.

Keywords: sex differences, age effects, magnetic resonance imaging, subcortical structure, volume

INTRODUCTION

Previous postmortem and in vivo studies have consistently demonstrated that cerebral volumes of adult human brains systematically shrink with age [Courchesne et al., 2000; Gonoi et al., 2010; Good et al., 2001a; Gur et al., 1991; Jernigan et al., 2001; Pfefferbaum et al., 1994; Raz et al., 2004; Riello et al., 2005; Smith et al., 2007; Taki et al., 2011a; Xu et al., 2000], a process referred to as atrophy that has been most prominently observed in the frontal [Allen et al., 2005; Coffey et al., 1992; Jernigan et al., 2001; Raz et al., 1997] and temporal lobes [Coffey et al., 1992; Raz et al., 2005; Sowell et al., 2003]. Atrophy in these regions has been directly related to cognitive or other deficits of aging [Park and Reuter‐Lorenz, 2009], but the patterns and degree of atrophy may differ between brain regions. Of note, age effects on the volume of specific subcortical brain structures with known function related to cognitive and emotional control might be even more important for understanding the effects of aging in the elderly, for example in Alzheimer's disease [Anderton, 1997; Driscoll et al., 2009] and psychiatric disorders [Bose et al., 2009; Kubota et al., 2011].

Indeed, pathology of subcortical structures such as the basal ganglia (including the caudate [Almeida et al., 2003; Harris et al., 1992; Roth et al., 2000], putamen [Harris et al., 1992; Wellington et al., 2006], and globus pallidus [Richfield and Herkenham, 1994]), thalamus [Braak and Braak, 1991; Lee and Marsden, 1994], hippocampus [Fox et al., 1996; Theodore et al., 1999], and amygdala [Braak et al., 1994; Kalus et al., 2005; Scott et al., 1991] have been related to cognitive and affective dysfunctions in degenerative neurological disorders and psychiatric disorders. However, the degree of atrophy in subcortical structures may differ between males and females [Gonoi et al., 2010; Pruessner et al., 2001; Xu et al., 2000], an observation that may be of great importance for understanding the increased vulnerability for certain psychiatric disorders and Alzheimer's disease in aging females. As of yet, a comprehensive study of the differential age trajectories in males and females of volume of the distinct subcortical structures across the lifespan is, to our knowledge, lacking.

Effects of sex on brain morphometry have been described in old age [Liu et al., 2010], in menopausal females [Goto et al., 2011a, b] and across the lifespan [Gonoi et al., 2010; Good et al., 2001a, b; Lv et al., 2010; Murphy et al., 1996; Takahashi et al., 2011; Taki et al., 2011b; Xu et al., 2000]. Moreover, strong effects of age on subcortical volumes have been reported [Walhovd et al., 2005, 2011; Ziegler et al., 2011], but only a few pioneering studies have incorporated effects of age and sex in models of subcortical atrophy. Among those studies inconsistent results were observed, which might be related to different age ranges or different methodologies. For example, utilizing manual segmentation of brain structures, Pruessner et al. [2001] reported no overall sex differences in the amygdala and hippocampus, while the hippocampus showed a significantly negative relationship between absolute volume and age in males only. However, the age range of 18–42 years may not have been ideally suited for revealing age effects, especially since it would not cover volume loss specifically related to estrogen unavailability after menopause in females [Morrison et al., 2006]. Goto et al. [2011a, b] also investigated hippocampal volume using both a VBM and atlas‐based approach, and report on hippocampal decline in postmenopausal women, and hippocampal decline in males after their 60s, suggesting longer preservation of hippocampal integrity in males. Xu et al. [2000] demonstrated age‐related degeneration in the left basal ganglia (i.e., across caudate nucleus, putamen, pallidum) of males but not females. Takahashi et al. [2011] studied age trajectories across the entire brain and reported on differential effects of sex in older age on relative volume of the thalamus, but divided patients in age groups (<50‐years‐old vs. >50‐years‐old), thereby missing important variance related to continuous age ranges. Importantly, none of these studies specifically tested whether the predictive value of age on volume of each subcortical structure differed between males and females.

A voxel‐based morphometry study in a sufficiently robust sample did test for the formal interaction of sex and age‐trajectories on whole brain gray matter, but could not observe differential age trajectories of subcortical structures in males and females [Good et al., 2001a]. However, this optimized voxel‐based method may not be sensitive enough to detect differential sex effects on absolute and relative volume decreases in volume of several well defined subcortical structures, partially due to the large smoothing kernel (i.e., 12 mm full width at half maximum) necessary to overcome inaccuracies in registration performance. Also, Good et al. [2001a] restricted their analysis to age relations of relative brain volume, and could therefore not infer on the age relations of absolute volume of brain regions relative to whole brain volume. Thus, Good's study could be further amplified by using a subcortical segmentation method, that does not require spatial smoothing and by specifically study relations of age with both absolute and relative brain volume. Additionally, further studies are needed that target the sex‐specific effects on brain atrophy across the whole lifespan in subcortical regions vital for cognitive and emotional adaptation to daily life across the whole lifespan, as well as consider regional‐specificity important to molecular mechanisms, for example region‐specific iron deposition patterns in the basal ganglia [Aquino et al., 2009] that under the influence of hormonal regulation [Crist et al., 2009] may lead to regional specific atrophy patterns [Schuster et al., 2011].

In the current study, we thus tested for the existence of sex‐related differences in age‐dependent subcortical volume changes across the span of adulthood in a well‐characterized sample of healthy individuals. We hypothesized that sex × age interaction effects would be specific for distinct subcortical structures, with steeper age declines in females, reflecting their increased vulnerability for cognitive and emotional disorders. Using the unbiased automated segmentation algorithm implemented in FreeSurfer, volumes of subcortical structures previously reported to show age‐ or sex‐related variations were calculated, including the basal ganglia (caudate, putamen, and pallidum), thalamus, hippocampus, and amygdala, and relations with absolute volumes, as well as relative to global brain decline were assessed. These changes should represent sex‐dependent age effects in the context of maturation, as well as whole‐brain scaled volume differences and sex‐specific periods of atrophy, that could explain behavioral sex‐specific aspects during normal aging as well as to sex‐specific vulnerabilities in degenerative pathologies.

METHODS

Subjects

A total sample of 76 healthy subjects, consisting of 38 females (age: 41.76 ± 17.08 years, range: 19–69 years) and 38 age‐matched males (age: 44.32 ± 14.70 years, range: 19–70 years; P = 0.487) were included in the present study. Participants were selected from 86 healthy individuals (45 females/41 males, with no history of central nervous system (CNS) diseases) that were scanned by the International Consortium for Brain Mapping (ICBM), at the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) and were made publicly available as part of the 1000 Functional Connectomes project (http://www.nitrc.org/projects/fcon_1000) [Biswal et al., 2010]. MNI review boards approved the research protocols, and informed consent was obtained from each subject before participation. To obtain a balanced age range in both sexes, five subjects older than 70 years were excluded [including 4 females (age >78 years) and 1 male (73‐years‐old)], setting the upper limit to 69/70 years in females and males. We further excluded three left‐handed females and one male to avoid potential confounds by different structural lateralization [Good et al., 2001b; Hamilton et al., 2007]. In addition, we discarded one MRI scan from our analyses because it lacked full brain coverage.

MRI Acquisition

All structural T1 images were obtained using a Siemens Sonata 1.5 T MR imaging system (Erlangen, Germany) with an eight‐channel phased‐array head coil. The scanning parameters were as follows: repetition time (TR) = 22 ms, echo time (TE) = 9.2 ms, slice thickness = 1 mm, flip angle = 30°, matrix size = 256 × 256, yielding 176 sagittal slices with in‐plane resolution of 1 × 1 mm2.

Data Preprocessing

Volumetric analysis was conducted using the FreeSurfer software package version 4.5.0 (http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu) [Dale et al., 1999; Fischl et al., 1999]. FreeSurfer is an automated segmentation tool feasible to label brain structures and is unbiased in its segmentation processes and time‐efficient as compared to manual approaches. The accuracy of FreeSurfer has been validated for relevant structures such as the hippocampus, amygdala, caudate, and thalamus [Fischl et al., 2002]. Lehmann et al. [2010] compared FreeSurfer and manual volumetric measurements, and showed that both methods were highly correlated in most regions and showed similar atrophy patterns in patient groups relative to normal controls.

The processing steps involved in the automated segmentation of brain structures have been described in detail elsewhere [Fischl et al., 2002, 2004]. In short, all T1 images first underwent intensity normalization and skull stripping. Segmentation was performed by rigid‐body registration and nonlinear normalization of images to a probabilistic brain atlas, which was constructed from a manually labeled training set. Through the segmentation process, each voxel of the MRI volumes was labeled automatically as a corresponding brain region. The segmented labels were then returned to native space and the segmentation accuracy of each subcortical structure was visually inspected. Finally, volumes of the caudate, putamen, pallidum, thalamus, hippocampus, and amygdala were calculated, and the total brain volume (TBV) was measured as the summation of gray matter volumes (GMV) and white matter volumes throughout the whole brain (i.e., parenchyma), excluding the cerebrospinal fluid (CSF), ventricles, and dura mater. We chose to calculate TBV and not estimated total intracranial volume, because with aging loss of gray and white matter is substituted by CSF, and therefore total intracranial volume may not be the optimal variable to describe volumetric relations with aging. In addition, to correct for the effect of TBV the relative volume of each subcortical structure was calculated as a percentage of brain volume.

Statistical Analysis

All statistical analyses were performed using SPSS 16 (SPSS, Chicago, IL). Two linear multiple regression models were first set up for sex (dummy variable: females = 1; males = 0) and age separately to investigate the main effects of sex and age on all subcortical structures. Then, the interaction effects of age and sex were tested by setting up a linear general model containing an age term, a sex term, and an interaction term of age × sex. We set the significance threshold at alpha = 0.05 for the omnibus tests (multivariate main effects of age, sex, and age × sex on subcortical volumes). Post‐hoc tests per structure were Bonferroni corrected for multiple comparisons. To optimally balance between Type‐I and Type‐II error, we took the correlation between the dependent variables (volumes of the six subcortical structures) into account by using the Simple Interactive Statistical Analysis Bonferroni tool (http://www.quantitativeskills.com/sisa/calculations/bonfer.htm). Using a Bonferroni correction that treats the variables as independent (proper Bonferroni: alpha/number of tests), would lead to a too stringent correction, as the dependent variables are not obtained in independent subgroups. Absolute volumes showed a mean correlation coefficient of r = 0.60, leading to an equivalent corrected alpha of 0.024 (number of tests = 6). Relative volumes showed a mean correlation coefficient of r = 0.26, and therefore a significant level of alpha = 0.013 is equivalent to a corrected P of <0.05. To further investigate age progression of subcortical volumes within sex, we set up a linear model with age as the predictor in females and males separately. Higher‐order, i.e., quadratic models were then employed to demonstrate whether they could improve the fitness. For these follow‐up analyses the above Bonferroni corrected thresholds were divided by two, as we tested for two independent samples (males and females), resulting in corrected thresholds 0.012 for absolute volumes and of 0.007 for relative volumes.

RESULTS

Total Brain and Gray Matter Volumes

Larger volumes of TBV and GMV were found in males as compared with females (Table 1). TBV showed a significantly negative linear correlation with age in males (r = −0.48, P = 0.002) but not in females (r = −0.22, P = 0.19) (Fig. 1A). However, absolute GMV correlated negatively with age in both males (r = −0.55, P < 0.001) and females (r = −0.47, P = 0.003) (Fig. 1B).

Table 1.

Volumes of total brain, gray matter, and subcortical structures in females and males

Females (N = 38) Males (N = 38)
Mean SD Mean SD
Absolute volumes (mm3)
Total brain 1152556.71 80706.49 1294384.24 98808.60
Total GM 675838.79 40687.30 747343.71 62613.52
Caudate 7069.53 970.47 7799.74 961.73
Putamen 10269.61 1098.89 11341.05 1129.72
Pallidum 3218.95 362.41 3632.13 380.28
Thalamus 14730.68 1482.48 16321.29 1596.68
Hippocampus 8371.50 685.98 8770.39 891.50
Amygdala 2975.79 257.07 3415.61 333.82
Relative volumes (%)
Caudate 0.6136 0.0769 0.6036 0.0679
Putamen 0.8916 0.0796 0.8771 0.0691
Pallidum 0.2792 0.0234 0.2809 0.0238
Thalamus 1.2770 0.0772 1.2619 0.0934
Hippocampus 0.7274 0.0511 0.6777 0.0463
Amygdala 0.2588 0.0217 0.2641 0.0188

SD, standard deviation; GM, gray matter; relative volume (%): (absolute volume [structure]/total brain volume) × 100.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

3D view of subcortical segmentations. [Color figure can be viewed in the online issue, which is available at http://wileyonlinelibrary.com.]

Subcortical Volumes

Main effects of sex

An omnibus test showed a main effect of sex on subcortical structures (F 6,69 = 8.57, P < 0.001, η2 = 0.43): all the subcortical structures showed significantly smaller absolute volumes in females than in males at P < 0.05, Bonferroni corrected, with the exception of hippocampus (r = −0.25, P = 0.03 uncorrected) (Statistics per region are listed in Table 2).

Table 2.

Effects of sex, age, and their interaction

Sex Age Age × Sex
r P r P F P
Absolute volumes
Caudate 0.358 0.002 0.278 0.015 1.287 0.260
Putamen 0.438 <0.001 0.430 <0.001 0.009 0.925
Pallidum 0.491 <0.001 0.335 0.003 0.055 0.815
Thalamus 0.464 <0.001 0.339 0.003 1.786 0.186
Hippocampus −0.246 0.032a −0.181 0.118 7.167 0.009
Amygdala 0.599 <0.001 −0.130 0.262 0.382 0.539
Relative volumes
Caudate 0.069 0.551 −0.175 0.130 6.019 0.017a
Putamen 0.098 0.399 0.364 0.001 2.464 0.121
Pallidum −0.037 0.753 −0.271 0.018a 3.613 0.061
Thalamus 0.089 0.444 −0.272 0.018a 0.006 0.937
Hippocampus 0.459 <0.001 0.052 0.655 3.381 0.070
Amygdala −0.132 0.257 0.065 0.578 0.720 0.399

Positive/negative r values for main sex effects represented larger/smaller volumes in females relative to males; in bold font: P < 0.05, Bonferroni corrected [with thresholds of 0.024 for absolute volumes and 0.013 for relative volumes]).

a

Marginal significance (P < 0.05 uncorrected).

When accounting for TBV, a main effect of sex was again observed (F 6,69 = 5.03, P < 0.001, η2 = 0.30), resulting from a significant higher volume ratio of the hippocampus in females as compared to males (Table 2). However, no significant sex effects were detected in the basal ganglia, thalamus, and amygdala.

Main effects of age

A main effect of age on subcortical volumes was observed (F 6,69 = 3.97, P = 0.002, η2 = 0.26), resulting from significant negative linear correlations with age for absolute volumes of the basal ganglia, but not the medial temporal structures (i.e., hippocampus and amygdala) (Table 2).

Effects of age were also observed on relative volumes of subcortical structures (F 6,69 = 2.83, P = 0.02, η2 = 0.20), that resulted from significant negative linear correlations with age in the putamen, and trend‐wise in the pallidum and thalamus (P = 0.018). The effect of age on relative volume of the caudate was not significant (Table 2). Correcting for TBV did not change the age effects on the hippocampus and amygdala.

Interaction effects of age and sex

The multivariate omnibus test indicated significant age × sex interactions on absolute subcortical volumes (F 6,67 = 2.64, P = 0.02, η2 = 0.19), that resulted from a significant interaction of age and sex on the hippocampus, but not on other subcortical structures. Exploration of the age effects in the hippocampus within males and females separately showed a significant negative relation in males (r = −0.45, P = 0.004), but not in females (r = 0.06, P = 0.736).

When considering relative volumes, the interaction effect was also found to be significant over all subcortical structures (F 6,67 = 2.62, P = 0.02, η2 = 0.19), but post‐hoc tests were found to be only moderately significant in the caudate (P = 0.017) and trend‐wise in the pallidum (P = 0.06) and hippocampus (P = 0.07). In these structures age had a significant negative relation with relative volumes in females, while this relation was absent in males. In the hippocampus, females showed a trend‐wise positive relation of relative volume and age, while no relation with age was present in males. Correlations of absolute and relative volume of each structure with age in males and females separately can be found in Table 3 and Figure 2.

Table 3.

Linear regression analyses of subcortical volumes with age in females and males, respectively

Females Males
r square r P r square r P
Absolute volumes
Caudate 0.218 −0.467 0.003 0.029 −0.171 0.305
Putamen 0.309 −0.556 <.001 0.233 −0.483 0.002
Pallidum 0.248 −0.498 0.001 0.132 −0.363 0.025a
Thalamus 0.115 −0.340 0.037a 0.276 −0.525 <0.001
Hippocampus 0.003 0.057 0.736 0.205 −0.453 0.004
Amygdala 0.041 −0.203 0.223 0.064 −0.252 0.127
Relative volume
Caudate 0.169 −0.411 0.010a 0.021 0.145 0.384
Putamen 0.257 −0.507 0.001 0.026 −0.161 0.333
Pallidum 0.243 −0.493 0.002 0.001 −0.029 0.864
Thalamus 0.106 −0.326 0.046a 0.047 −0.217 0.191
Hippocampus 0.083 0.289 0.078 0.020 −0.140 0.402
Amygdala 0.001 −0.031 0.855 0.029 0.169 0.309

In bold font: P < 0.05, Bonferroni corrected [with thresholds of 0.012 for absolute volumes and 0.007 for relative volumes])

a

Marginal significance (P < 0.05 uncorrected).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Linear absolute and relative age trajectories in males and females. Linear age trajectories of subcortical volumes (including both the absolute [left column] and relative [right column] volumes) in the (A) caudate, (B) putamen, (C) pallidum, (D) thalamus, (E) hippocampus, and (F) amygdala in females (red color) and males (blue color). [Color figure can be viewed in the online issue, which is available at http://wileyonlinelibrary.com.]

Higher‐order model

Absolute volumes of the basal ganglia structures and thalamus preferred a high‐order model (i.e., a quadratic model over a linear model) in males. In females, age‐related changes within these structures were best fit with a linear model (i.e., quadratic models did not explain more variance). Of note, a higher‐order model was preferred in the hippocampus and amygdala in both males and females, whereas no significant fits were observed using a linear model for females in the hippocampus and for both sexes in the amygdala. However, the higher‐order model did not better fit the age‐related changes after correction for TBV. Results of model fitting can be found in Table 4 and Figure 3. Also in Supporting Information Figure S1, age‐range (19–34, 35–50, and 51–70 years) dependent sex differences are visualized.

Table 4.

The best‐fitted model fitting the relationships between subcortical volumes and age in females and males, respectively

Females Males
Model r square P Model r square P
Absolute volumes
Caudate Linear 0.218 0.003
Putamen Linear 0.309 <0.001 Quadratic 0.333 0.001
Pallidum Linear 0.248 0.001 Quadratic 0.347 0.009
Thalamus Linear 0.115 0.037a Quadratic 0.373 <0.001
Hippocampus Quadratic 0.148 0.060 Quadratic 0.321 <0.001
Amygdala Quadratic 0.137 0.075 Quadratic 0.235 0.009
Relative volume
Caudate Linear 0.169 0.010a
Putamen Linear 0.257 0.001
Pallidum Linear 0.243 0.002
Thalamus Linear 0.106 0.046a
Hippocampus Linear 0.083 0.078
Amygdala

In bold font: P < 0.05, Bonferroni corrected [with thresholds of 0.012 for absolute volumes and 0.007 for relative volumes]).

a

Marginal significance (P < 0.05 uncorrected).

“–” indicates the relationship could not be fitted by any of the three model (linear, quadratic, and cubic models).

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Improved fitting curves of age trajectories by the quadratic model (right column) compared with the linear model (left column) in (A) putamen, (B) pallidum, (C) thalamus of males, (D) hippocampus, and (E) amygdala of both males (blue color) and females (red color). [Color figure can be viewed in the online issue, which is available at http://wileyonlinelibrary.com.]

DISCUSSION

In this study, we investigated the relations of age on subcortical structures vital for cognitive and emotional adaptation to daily life across the lifespan, while explicitly testing for differential relations in males and females. Using an automated and rater‐unbiased subcortical segmentation procedure on structural MR images, we observed strong contributions of sex on absolute volume of subcortical structures, including the thalamus, caudate nucleus, pallidum, putamen, hippocampus, and amygdala, where females showed smaller volumes. Strong contributions of age where observed on basal ganglia structures and thalamus, but not on the medial temporal lobe structures. Remarkably, observed age relations depended heavily on the sex of individuals and whether volumetric decline was measured in an absolute manner or was considered relative to the whole brain age decline: An age × sex interaction effect was observed on absolute hippocampal volume: males showed a linear negative relation with age, whereas the relation was not significant in females. A second prominent observation was that in females, the basal ganglia showed a negative relation with age in volume across the lifespan, and that the pace of suggested decline exceeded the pace of general brain atrophy (i.e., negative correlations with age after correction for TBV) and the corrected pace of suggested decline in men. In males, absolute volumes of the thalamus, hippocampus, and basal ganglia (except the caudate nuclei) showed a suggested decline, but the pace was synchronized with whole brain atrophy (i.e., no age progression beyond that of TBVs). Volume of the amygdala was shown to be absolutely and relatively preserved with age in both males and females.

Consistent with previous work that was performed on part of the current data set [Pruessner et al., 2001], we observed an absolute volume decline of the hippocampus in males, but not in females. We now extended this finding to the entire adult age range and showed a formal interaction of age and sex on absolute hippocampal volume. Previously, males have been shown to exhibit steeper negative age‐related trends in the hippocampus than females, even after controlling for sex differences in body size [Raz et al., 2004]. In females, however, no decline of absolute hippocampal volumes was observed, but a weak trend towards a positive relation of relative volume and age was observed, indicating that the pace of hippocampal decline is even behind the pace of whole brain decline. Intuitively, higher‐order (i.e., quadratic) models could explain the relation between age and absolute volume of the hippocampus (and amygdala) better than linear models and showed that in males (and trend‐wise in females) decline of volumes was observed in the second half of life, which is consistent with a recent report [Ziegler et al., 2011]. The quadratic decline, with a suggested earlier inflection point for absolute volumes and higher absolute volumes in younger males, may support the notion that sex differences are largest at young ages (also presented in Supporting Information Fig. S1), while volumes become more similar with progressing age, consistent with Jack et al. [1998].

Within the basal ganglia—a group of nuclei (caudate, putamen, and pallidum) involved in cognitive, emotional, and motor behavior [Alexander et al., 1986], an interaction of age and sex was suggested in the caudate nucleus, and trend‐wise in the pallidum, indicative of an age‐progression in females in the basal ganglia beyond that of TBVs. In males age progression was either absent (caudate), or did not exceed that of general brain volumes (putamen and pallidum). Results suggest that males may exhibit more robust whole brain and parallel basal ganglia declines relative to females. The strong effect of aging on basal ganglia volumes in females may reflect common changes in functions mediated by these structures [Herrero et al., 2002], and extents the finding of Xu et al. [2000] indicating that the basal ganglia in females act as a cohesive unit subserving human behavior, when considered in their age‐related changes as compared with declines in the amygdala and hippocampus. Pronounced declines have been reported for females when compared to males in performance of a sustained reaction task and a visuospatial learning and planning task, especially at later ages [Clark et al., 2006; Proust‐Lima et al., 2008]. This sex‐specific effect of aging on females' psycho‐motor performance would represent a suitable morphological correlate of the characteristics of female aging. In contrast, several studies did not report age × sex effects or differential aging effects on cognitive performance in males and females [Finkel et al., 2006; Kavé et al., 2012; Silver et al., 2011], which suggests that the steeper decline of the basal ganglia may be more associated with postmenopausal emotional/somato‐sensory abnormalities, including late‐life depression, anxiety, and sleep problems. However, a direct relation of relative volumes, task performance, and emotional and somato‐sensory disorders needs further investigation.

In the thalamus, our results were consistent with previous studies [Van Der Werf et al., 2001], reporting that total thalamic volume reductions correlate with age in both males and females. However, further investigations of thalamic volumes should consider the numerous sub‐nuclei of this structure with largely varying histo‐architectonics and functions (Metzger et al., 2001). As a result of the specific involvement of individual thalamic substructures either in loops with primary sensorimotor cortices or other subcortical or frontal association areas, it is difficult to determine the contribution of general volumetric variance to a circumscribed set of psychological changes.

These considerations need to be seen in context of the intrinsic limitations of our approach. In the current study, our sample size may have been well suited for the age‐related observation across the lifespan. Although the age distribution was equal over decades, only a moderate number of participants were included in each decade. However, we made optimal use of the age‐variation in our sample by analyzing relations of volume of subcortical structures and age in a continuous manner. Moreover, we could replicate certain previously reported main effects of age and gender. Therefore, we are confident that our results are reliable, despite the somewhat small sample size. A second limitation is that we did not dispose of information concerning social economic status and educational level. Although we acknowledge that these factors are important when considering cognitive reserve, we aimed to only study effects of age and sex, the main predictors of regional brain volume. Thirdly, we used a cross‐sectional design instead of a longitudinal design to study age‐relations, which cannot fully exclude systematic e.g., cohort effects. However, observing age relations across the whole lifespan are not possible for technical reasons. We ran our study with the implicit hypothesis that such an approach would be valid, following a long line of research like many previous studies [e.g., Allen et al., 2005; Bose et al., 2009; Coffey et al., 1992; Courchesne et al., 2000; Gonoi et al., 2010; Good et al., 2001a; Goto et al., 2011a, b; Gur et al., 1991; Jernigan et al., 2001; Kubota et al., 2011; Liu et al., 2010; Pfefferbaum et al., 1994; Pruessner et al., 2001; Raz et al., 2004, 2005; Takahashi et al., 2011; Taki et al., 2011b; Walhovd et al., 2005; Xu et al., 2000; Ziegler et al., 2011]. Therefore, we feel that our approach is within an accepted line of research that is investigating age related volume changes such as decline. Finally, compared to males, females did not have such a steep decline of TBV, although their GMV significantly decreased. This might lead to a second explanation that the observed decline in females does not exceed total brain decline, but a decline in a structure when the whole brain does not show a decline in females. In our sample, whole brain volume correlated strongly to GMV (r = 0.94, P < 0.001), indicating colinearity of the two measures. This in turn suggests that the results would not change if only the GMV was considered: indeed confirmatory tests showed that using only GMV as a regressor did not change the results.

In the present study, we demonstrated that volumetric age relations depended heavily on subcortical structure and sex. Importantly, these findings also emphasize the significant impact of analytical strategies e.g., when deciding for correction of subcortical volumes to the whole‐brain decline. To account for the complexity of age‐related changes across the whole lifespan, we used both linear and quadratic regression models to describe the age progression of subcortical volume. This suggested an accelerating decline in old males of absolute basal ganglia volume analysis. Moreover, both absolute and relative volumes of distinct subcortical structures were studied, an approach that revealed that in females the pace of decline in the basal ganglia and thalamus exceeds the pace of general brain atrophy. This observation seems to parallel the observed neuropsychiatric vulnerability especially in aging females while atrophy in males in these structures seemed to follow rather global declines. The finding of an age × region × sex interaction on individual subcortical structures provides support for the impact of sex on psychopathologies related to reduced cognitive reserve [Perneczky et al., 2007] or depressive comorbidities [Bogerts, 2010], especially in degenerative brain disorders in the elderly. However, we further demonstrated that sex effects are highly depending on data treatment, since basal ganglia age trajectories show a highly sex dependent relative evolution against TBV, which may also explain diverging findings in the existing literature.

Supporting information

Supporting Information

REFERENCES

  1. Alexander GE, DeLong MR, Strick PL (1986): Parallel organization of functionally segregated circuits linking basal ganglia and cortex. Annu Rev Neurosci 9:357–381. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Allen JS, Bruss J, Brown CK, Damasio H (2005): Normal neuroanatomical variation due to age: the major lobes and a parcellation of the temporal region. Neurobiol Aging 26:1245–1260. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Almeida OP, Burton EJ, McKeith I, Gholkar A, Burn D, O'Brien JT (2003): MRI study of caudate nucleus volume in Parkinson's disease with and without dementia with Lewy bodies and Alzheimer's disease. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 16:57–63. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Anderton BH (1997): Changes in the ageing brain in health and disease. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 352:1781–1792. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Aquino D, Bizzi A, Crisoli M, Garavaglia B, Bruzzone MG, Nardocci N, Savoiardo M, Chiapparini L (2009): Age‐related iron deposition in the basal ganglia: Quantitative analysis in healthy subjects. Radiology 252:165–172. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Biswal BB, Mennes M, Zuo XN, Gohel S, Kelly C, Smith SM, Beckmann CF, Adelstein JS, Buckner RL, Colcombe S, Dogonowski AM, Ernst M, Fair D, Hampson M, Hoptman MJ, Hyde JS, Kiviniemi VJ, Kötter R, Li SJ, Lin CP, Lowe MJ, Mackay C, Madden DJ, Madsen KH, Margulies DS, Mayberg HS, McMahon K, Monk CS, Mostofsky SH, Nagel BJ, Pakar JJ, Peltier SJ, Petersen SE, Riedl V, Rombouts SA, Rypma B, Schlaggar BL, Schmidt S, Seidler RD, Siegle GJ, Sorg C, Teng GJ, Veijola J, Villringer A, Walter M, Wang L, Weng XC, Whitfield‐Gabrieli S, Williamson P, Windischberger C, Zang YF, Zhang HY, Castellanos FX, Milham MP (2010): Toward discovery science of human brain function. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 107:4734–4739. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bogerts B (2010): Are there gender‐specific neuropathological correlates of depression? J Neurol Neurochir Psychiatr 11:29–35. [Google Scholar]
  8. Bose SK, Mackinnon T, Mehta MA, Turkheimer FE, Howes OD, Selvaraj S, Kempton MJ, Grasby PM (2009): The effect of ageing on grey and white matter reductions in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 112:7–13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Braak H, Braak E (1991): Alzheimer's disease affects limbic nuclei of the thalamus. Acta Neuropathol 81:261–268. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Braak H, Braak E, Yilmazer D, de Vos RA, Jansen EN, Bohl J, Jellinger K (1994): Amygdala pathology in Parkinson's disease. Acta Neuropathol 88:493–500. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Clark CR, Paul RH, Williams LM, Arns M, Fallahpour K, Handmer C, Gordon E (2006): Standardized assessment of cognitive functioning during development and aging using an automated touchscreen battery. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 21:449–467. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Coffey CE, Wilkinson WE, Parashos IA, Soady SA, Sullivan RJ, Patterson LJ, Figiel GS, Webb MC, Spritzer CE, Djang WT (1992): Quantitative cerebral anatomy of the aging human brain: a cross‐sectional study using magnetic resonance imaging. Neurology 42:527–536. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Courchesne E, Chisum HJ, Townsend J, Cowles A, Covington J, Egaas B, Harwood M, Hinds S, Press GA (2000): Normal brain development and aging: Quantitative analysis at in vivo MR imaging in healthy volunteers. Radiology 216:672–682. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Crist BL, Alekel DL, Ritland LM, Hanson LN, Genschel U, Reddy MB (2009): Association of oxidative stress, iron, and centralized fat mass in healthy postmenopausal women. J Womens Health 18:795–801. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Dale AM, Fischl B, Sereno MI (1999): Cortical surface‐based analysis. I. Segmentation and Surface Reconstruction. Neuroimage 9:179–194. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Driscoll I, Davatzikos C, An Y, Wu X, Shen D, Kraut M, Resnick SM (2009): Longitudinal pattern of regional brain volume change differentiates normal aging from MCI. Neurology 72:1906–1913. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Finkel D, Reynolds CA, Berg S, Pedersen NL (2006): Surprising lack of sex differences in normal cognitive aging in twins. Int J Aging Hum 62:355–357. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Fischl B, Salat DH, Busa E, Albert M, Dieterich M, Haselgrove C, van der Kouwe A, Killiany R, Kennedy D, Klaveness S, Montillo A, Makris N, Rosen B, Dale AM (2002): Whole‐brain segmentation: Automated labeling of neuroanatomical structures in the human brain. Neuron 33:341–355. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Fischl B, Salat DH, van der Kouwe AJ, Makris N, Ségonne F, Quinn BT, Dale AM (2004): Sequence‐independent segmentation of magnetic resonance images. Neuroimage 23:S69–S84. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Fischl B, Sereno MI, Dale AM (1999): Cortical surface‐based analysis. II. Inflation, flattening, and a surface‐based coordinate system. Neuroimage 9:195–207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Fox NC, Warrington EK, Freeborough PA, Hartikainen P, Kennedy AM, Stevens JM, Rossor MN (1996): Presymptomatic hippocampal atrophy in Alzheimer's disease. Brain 119:2001–2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Gonoi W, Abe O, Yamasue H, Yamada H, Masutani Y, Takao H, Kasai K, Aoki S, Ohtomo K (2010): Age‐related changes in regional brain volume evaluated by atlas‐based method. Neuroradiology 52:865–873. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Good CD, Johnsrude IS, Ashburner J, Henson RN, Friston KJ, Frackowiak RS (2001a): A voxel‐based morphometric study of ageing in 465 normal adult human brains. Neuroimage 14:21–36. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Good CD, Johnsrude IS, Ashburner J, Henson RN, Friston KJ, Frackowiak RS (2001b): Cerebral asymmetry and the effects of sex and handedness on brain structure: A voxel‐based morphometric analysis of 465 normal adult human brains. Neuroimage 14:685–700. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Goto M, Abe O, Miyati T, Inano S, Hayashi N, Aoki S, Mori H, Kabasawa H, Ino K, Yano K, Iida K, Mima K, Ohtomo K (2011a): 3 Tesla MRI detects accelerated hippocampal volume reduction in postmenopausal women. J Magn Reson Imaging 33:48–53. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Goto M, Abe O, Miyati T, Inano S, Hayashi N, Aoki S, Mori H, Kunimatsu A, Ino K, Iida K, Yano K, Ohtomo K (2011b): Accelerated hippocampal volume reduction in post‐menopausal women: an additional study with Atlas‐based method. Radiol Phys Technol 4:185–188. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Gur RC, Mozley PD, Resnick SM, Gottlieb GL, Kohn M, Zimmerman R, Herman G, Atlas S, Grossman R, Berretta D, Erwin RJ, Gur RE. (1991): Gender differences in age effect on brain atrophy measured by magnetic resonance imaging. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 88:2845–2849. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Hamilton LS, Narr KL, Luders E, Szeszko PR, Thompson PM, Bilder RM, Toga AW (2007): Asymmetries of cortical thickness: Effects of handedness, sex, and schizophrenia. Neuroreport 18:1427–1431. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Harris GJ, Pearlson GD, Peyser CE, Aylward EH, Roberts J, Barta PE, Chase GA, Folstein SE (1992): Putamen volume reduction on magnetic resonance imaging exceeds caudate changes in mild Huntington's disease. Ann Neurol 31:69–75. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Herrero MT, Barcia C, Navarro JM (2002): Functional anatomy of thalamus and basal ganglia. Childs Nerv Syst 18:386–404. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Jack CR Jr, Petersen RC, Xu Y, O'Brien PC, Smith GE, Ivnik RJ, Tangalos EG, Kokmen E (1998): The rate of medial temporal lobe atrophy in typical aging and Alzheimer's disease. Neurology 51:993–999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Jernigan TL, Archibald SL, Fennema‐Notestine C, Gamst AC, Stout JC, Bonner J, Hesselink JR (2001): Effects of age on tissues and regions of the cerebrum and cerebellum. Neurobiol Aging 22:581–594. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Kalus P, Slotboom J, Gallinat J, Wiest R, Ozdoba C, Federspiel A, Strik WK, Buri C, Schroth G, Kiefer C (2005): The amygdala in schizophrenia: A trimodal magnetic resonance imaging study. Neurosci Lett 375:151–156. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Kavé G, Shrira A, Palgi Y, Spalter T, Ben‐Ezra M, Shmotkin D (2012): Formal education level versus self‐rated literacy as predictors of cognitive aging. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. Epub ahead of print, doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbs031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Kubota M, Miyata J, Yoshida H, Hirao K, Fujiwara H, Kawada R, Fujimoto S, Tanaka Y, Sasamoto A, Sawamoto N, Fukuyama H, Murai T (2011): Age‐related cortical thinning in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 125:21–29. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Lee MS, Marsden CD (1994): Movement disorders following lesions of the thalamus or subthalamic region. Mov Disord 9:493–507. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Lehmann M, Douiri A, Kim LG, Modat M, Chan D, Ourselin S, Barnes J, Fox NC (2010): Atrophy patterns in Alzheimer's disease and semantic dementia: A comparison of FreeSurfer and manual volumetric measurements. Neuroimage 49:2264–2274. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Liu T, Wen W, Zhu W, Trollor J, Reppermund S, Crawford J, Jin JS, Luo S, Brodaty H, Sachdev P (2010): The effects of age and sex on cortical sulci in the elderly. Neuroimage 51:19–27. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Lv B, Li J, He H, Li M, Zhao M, Ai L, Yan F, Xian J, Wang Z (2010): Gender consistency and difference in healthy adults revealed by cortical thickness. Neuroimage 53:373–382. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Metzger CD, Eckert U, Steiner J, Sartorius A, Buchmann JE, Stadler J, Tempelmann C, Speck O, Bogerts B, Abler B, Walter M (2010): High field fMRI reveals thalamocortical integration of segregated cognitive and emotional processing in mediodorsal and intralaminar thalamic nuclei. Front Neuroanat 4:1–17. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Morrison JH, Brinton RD, Schmidt PJ, Gore AC (2006): Estrogen, menopause, and the aging brain: How basic neuroscience can inform therapy in women. J Neurosci 26:10332–10348. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Murphy DG, DeCarli C, Mclntosh AR, Daly E, Mentis MJ, Pietrini P, Szczepanik J, Schapiro MB, Grady CL, Horwitz B, Rapoport SI (1996): Sex differences in human brain morphometry and metabolism: an in vivo quantitative magnetic resonance imaging and positron emission tomography study on the effect of aging. Arch Gen Psychiatry 53:585–594. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Park DC, Reuter‐Lorenz P (2009): The adaptive brain: Aging and neurocognitive scaffolding. Annu Rev Psychol 60:173–196. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Perneczky R, Drzezga A, Diehl‐Schmid J, Li Y, Kurz A (2007): Gender differences in brain reserve: An (18)F‐FDG PET study in Alzheimer's disease. J Neurol 254:1395–1400. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Pfefferbaum A, Mathalon DH, Sullivan EV, Rawles JM, Zipursky RB, Lim KO (1994): A quantitative magnetic resonance imaging study of changes in brain morphology from infancy to late adulthood. Arch Neurol 51:874–887. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Proust‐Lima C, Amieva H, Letenneur L, Orgogozo JM, Jacqmin‐Gadda H, Dartigues JF (2008): Gender and education impact on brain aging: a general cognitive factor approach. Psychol Aging 23:608–620. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Pruessner JC, Collins DL, Pruessner M, Evans AC (2001): Age and gender predict volume decline in the anterior and posterior hippocampus in early adulthood. J Neurosci 21:194–200. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Raz N, Gunning FM, Head D, Dupuis JH, McQuain J, Briggs SD, Loken WJ, Thornton AE, Acker JD (1997): Selective aging of the human cerebral cortex observed in vivo: differential vulnerability of the prefrontal gray matter. Cereb Cortex 7:268–282. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Raz N, Gunning‐Dixon F, Head D, Rodrigue KM, Williamson A, Acker JD (2004): Aging, sexual dimorphism, and hemispheric asymmetry of the cerebral cortex: Replicability of regional differences in volume. Neurobiol Aging 25:377–396. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Raz N, Lindenberger U, Rodrigue KM, Kennedy KM, Head D, Williamson A, Dahle C, Gerstorf D, Acker JD (2005): Regional brain changes in aging healthy adults: General trends, individual differences and modifiers. Cereb Cortex 15:1676–1689. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Richfield EK, Herkenham M (1994): Selective vulnerability in Huntington's disease: Preferential loss of cannabinoid receptors in lateral globus pallidus. Ann Neurol 36:577–584. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Riello R, Sabattoli F, Beltramello A, Bonetti M, Bono G, Falini A, Magnani G, Minonzio G, Piovan E, Alaimo G, Ettori M, Galluzzi S, Locatelli E, Noiszewska M, Testa C, Frisoni GB (2005): Brain volumes in healthy adults aged 40 years and over: A voxel‐based morphometry study. Aging Clin Exp Res 17:329–336. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Roth J, Havrdová E, Růzicka E (2000): Atrophy of caudate nucleus in Huntington's disease measured by computed tomography. J Neurol 247:880–881. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Schuster L, Essig M, Schröder J (2011): Normal aging and imaging correlations. Radiologe 51:266–272. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Scott SA, DeKosky ST, Scheff SW (1991): Volumetric atrophy of the amygdala in Alzheimer's disease: Quantitative serial reconstruction. Neurology 41:351–356. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Silver H, Goodman C, Gur RC, Gur RE, Bilker WB (2011): ‘Executive’ functions and normal aging: selective impairment in conditional exclusion compared to abstraction and inhibition. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 31:53–62. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Smith CD, Chebrolu H, Wekstein DR, Schmitt FA, Markesbery WR (2007): Age and gender effects on human brain anatomy: A voxel‐based morphometric study in healthy elderly. Neurobiol Aging 28:1075–1087. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Sowell ER, Peterson BS, Thompson PM, Welcome SE, Henkenius AL, Toga AW (2003): Mapping cortical change across the human lifespan. Nat Neurosci 6:309–315. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Takahashi R, Ishii K, Kakigi T, Yokoyama K (2011): Gender and age differences in normal adult human brain: Voxel‐based morphometric study. Hum Brain Mapp 32:1050–1058. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Taki Y, Kinomura S, Sato K, Goto R, Kawashima R, Fukuda H (2011a): A longitudinal study of gray matter volume decline with age and modifying factors. Neurobiol Aging 32:907–915. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Taki Y, Thyreau B, Kinomura S, Sato K, Goto R, Kawashima R, Fukuda H (2011b): Correlations among brain gray matter volumes, age, gender, and hemisphere in healthy individuals. PLoS One 6:e22734. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Theodore WH, Bhatia S, Hatta J, Fazilat S, DeCarli C, Bookheimer SY, Gaillard WD (1999): Hippocampal atrophy, epilepsy duration, and febrile seizures in patients with partial seizures. Neurology 52:132–136. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Van Der Werf YD, Tisserand DJ, Visser PJ, Hofman PA, Vuurman E, Uylings HB, Jolles J (2001): Thalamic volume predicts performance on tests of cognitive speed and decreases in healthy aging: A magnetic resonance imaging‐based volumetric analysis. Cogn Brain Res 11:377–385. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Walhovd KB, Fjell AM, Reinvang I, Lundervold A, Dale AM, Eilertsen DE, Quinn BT, Salat D, Makris N, Fischl B (2005): Effects of age on volumes of cortex, white matter and subcortical structures. Neurobiol Aging 26:1261–1270. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Walhovd KB, Westlye LT, Amlien I, Espeseth T, Reinvang I, Raz N, Agartz I, Salat DH, Greve DN, Fischl B, Dale AM, Fjell AM (2011): Consistent neuroanatomical age‐related volume differences across multiple samples. Neurobiol Aging 32:916–932. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Wellington TM, Semrud‐Clikeman M, Gregory AL, Murphy JM, Lancaster JL (2006): Magnetic resonance imaging volumetric analysis of the putamen in children with ADHD: Combined type versus control. J Atten Disord 10:171–180. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Xu J, Kobayashi S, Yamaguchi S, Iijima K, Okada K, Yamashita K (2000): Gender effects on age‐related changes in brain structure. Am J Neuroradiol 21:112–118. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Ziegler G, Dahnke R, Jäncke L, Yotter RA, May A, Gaser C (2011): Brain structural trajectories over the adult lifespan. Hum Brain Mapp. Epub ahead of print, doi: 10.1002/hbm.21374. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supporting Information


Articles from Human Brain Mapping are provided here courtesy of Wiley

RESOURCES