Abstract
Influential theories of brain‐viscera interactions propose a central role for interoception in basic motivational and affective feeling states. Recent neuroimaging studies have underlined the insula, anterior cingulate, and ventral prefrontal cortices as the neural correlates of interoception. However, the relationships between these distributed brain regions remain unclear. In this study, we used spatial independent component analysis (ICA) and functional network connectivity (FNC) approaches to investigate time course correlations across the brain regions during visceral interoception. Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) was performed in thirteen healthy females who underwent viscerosensory stimulation of bladder as a representative internal organ at different prefill levels, i.e., no prefill, low prefill (100 ml saline), and high prefill (individually adapted to the sensations of persistent strong desire to void), and with different infusion temperatures, i.e., body warm (∼37°C) or ice cold (4–8°C) saline solution. During Increased distention pressure on the viscera, the insula, striatum, anterior cingulate, ventromedial prefrontal cortex, amygdalo‐hippocampus, thalamus, brainstem, and cerebellar components showed increased activation. A second group of components encompassing the insula and anterior cingulate, dorsolateral prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices and temporal‐parietal junction showed increased activity with innocuous temperature stimulation of bladder mucosa. Significant differences in the FNC were found between the insula and amygdalo‐hippocampus, the insula and ventromedial prefrontal cortex, and the ventromedial prefrontal cortex and temporal‐parietal junction as the distention pressure on the viscera increased. These results provide new insight into the supraspinal processing of visceral interoception originating from an internal organ. Hum Brain Mapp 36:4438–4468, 2015. © 2015 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.
Keywords: fMRI, insula, prefrontal, salience, self‐referential, limbic, homeostasis, bladder, visceral sensation, alliesthesia
Abbreviations
- ACC
Anterior cingulate cortex
- ANOVA
Analysis of variance
- BA
Brodmann's area
- BOLD
Blood oxygenation level–dependent
- EPI
Echo‐planar‐imaging
- FDR
False discovery rate
- fMRI
Functional magnetic resonance imaging
- FNC
Functional network connectivity
- GI
Gastrointestinal
- HRF
Hemodynamic response function
- IC
Independent component
- ICA
Independent component analysis
- IFG
Inferior frontal gyrus
- Iq
Quality index
- MDL
Minimum description length
- MNI
Montreal Neurological Institute
- PAG
Periaqueductal gray
- PFC
Prefrontal cortex
- TPJ
Temporal‐parietal junction
- UB
Urinary bladder
INTRODUCTION
Interoception collectively refers to the sensing, representation and perception of the physiological condition of the body arising from the afferent information anywhere and everywhere within the body (Cameron, 2001; Craig, 2003a). Interoception is closely associated with the endogenous homeostatic mechanisms necessary for survival and prosperity, and underlies affective states and fundamental cognitive processes in human (Craig, 2008; Critchley, 2005; Damasio, 2000; Denton et al., 2009). It can be contrasted with the exteroceptive systems that keep the individual organism aware of the external environment as opposed to the bodily or self‐referential (introspectively‐oriented) processes (Craig, 2003a). However, in contrast to the rich history of studying the exteroceptive senses and their well‐recognized sensory cortical systems, less is known about the neural substrates that provide the brain with a dynamic representation of the body.
Neuroanatomical evidence suggests that information concerning the internal state of the body—including mechanical, thermal, chemical, osmotic, and hormonal aspects—is not only relayed to the spinal cord for autonomic reflexes, but the information is also projected to supraspinal regions (Craig, 2003a; Critchley, 2005; Saper, 2002). In particular, the Aδ‐ and C‐fiber projection to a specific lamina I spinothalamocortical pathway as well as the vagal and glossopharyngeal afferents to the parabrachial nucleus in brainstem via the nucleus of the solitary tract in medulla, have been proposed to carry sympathetic and parasympathetic afferents related to homeostatic and autonomic activities to the brain (Craig, 2003a; Critchley, 2005). For example, in the case of a visceral organ such as the urinary bladder (UB), the Aδ‐fibers are mechanosensors (more specifically tension receptors) responsible for the physiologic sensation of fullness (Häbler et al., 1993; Jänig and Morrison, 1986). The C‐fibers are generally insensitive to intravesical pressure but given their proximity to the intravascular milieu (Kanai and Andersson, 2010), they respond to cooling (Fall et al., 1990) or chemical irritation of the mucosa (Häbler et al., 1990). Modality‐specific lamina I neurons primarily project to autonomic cell columns in the spinal cord, where sympathetic preganglionic motoneurons are located, and to the integration homeostatic sites in the brainstem (Craig, 2003b). In primates, lamina I neurons also project topographically to two relay nuclei in thalamus: the posterior part of the ventral medial nucleus (Blomqvist et al., 2000; Craig et al., 1994), and the ventral caudal part of the medial dorsal nucleus (Craig, 2003b). The posterior part of the ventral medial nucleus is contiguous rostrally with the basal part of the ventral medial thalamic nucleus, which receives a direct input from the nucleus of the solitary tract (i.e. parasympathetic afferent input) in addition to the lamina I inputs (i.e. sympathetic afferent input). The posterior part of the ventral medial nucleus eventually projects to the interoceptive cortex in the posterior insula (Craig, 2014). The ventral caudal part of the medial dorsal nucleus, which receives projection from the parabrachial nucleus in the brainstem in addition to the lamina I inputs, projects to the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) (Craig, 2003b). According to the “homeostatic emotion” proposition of interoception (Craig, 2003a, 2003b; Craig, 2002), in humans the integration of afferent physiological signals with higher order contextual information, motivational state, and previously learned associations, is postulated to occur upon further propagation of interoceptive (homeostatic sensory) inputs from the interoceptive cortex in the posterior insula to the anterior insula (Evrard and Logothetis, 2014), the ACC, and the ventral prefrontal cortices (PFC) (Craig, 2002; Kurth et al., 2010; Wager and Barrett, 2004). In line with this proposition, neuroimaging studies of interoception including those involving somesthesis (e.g. nociception; Brooks et al., 2005), autonomic arousal (Cameron and Minoshima, 2002; Critchley et al., 2000, 2003), viscerosensory stimulation (Fowler and Griffiths, 2010; Kavia et al., 2005; Mayer, 2011), somatic control (including the regulation of cardioregulatory, vasomotor and visceromotor activity; e.g., Lerner et al., 2009), temperature (Craig et al., 2000), dyspnea (Liotti et al., 2001), thirst (Denton et al., 1999; Egan et al., 2003), hypoglycemia (Teves et al., 2004), satiety and craving (Naqvi and Bechara, 2009) have substantiated activation in the insula, the ACC, and the PFC among other paradigm‐specific cortical and subcortical regions when bodily states significantly deviated from the steady state.
Few studies have used multivariate approaches such as the independent component analysis (ICA) (Beckmann and Smith, 2004; Calhoun et al., 2002a; McKeown et al., 1998) and multi‐voxel pattern analysis (Norman et al., 2006) to investigate brain activation during interoception. For example, using a pattern recognition analytical technique, Björnsdotter et al. (2009) reported an antero‐posterior somatotopic organization in dorsal posterior insula caused by gentle touch (i.e., slow brush stimulation of C‐fiber tactile afferents). Liu et al. (2013) applied support vector machine technique, a multivariate pattern analysis method, to investigate the differences of resting‐state function mapped by regional homogeneity in patients with functional dyspepsia. The results showed highly discriminative brain regions mainly included the PFC involving the orbitofrontal cortex, supplementary motor area, insula, anterior/middle cingulate, thalamus, hippocampus/parahippocampus, and cerebellum. Hojo et al. (2012) used ICA approach to analyze brain activity during intraesophageal infusion of acid and saline in six healthy males and observed the common cerebral regions activated with saline and HCl infusion were the thalamus, insula, cingulate, and portions of the frontal, temporal, and parietal cortices. Similarly, Hall et al. (2010) applied ICA as a multivariate analysis technique to assess brain networks during a single slow ramp‐tonic rectal distension in seven females with irritable bowel syndrome and six healthy females and demonstrated that patients with irritable bowel syndrome had increased activation of the insula, ACC and ventromedial PFC while healthy controls showed increased activation in the thalamus, striatum and dorsolateral PFC.
Despite recent advances in understanding the neural correlates of interoception, still it is not clear how these distributed neural systems in the brain are functionally connected during the interoceptive processing. The study of functional connectivity is concerned with the temporal coherence among spatially remote brain regions (Friston, 2002). In the traditional functional connectivity analysis, time courses of individually selected seed voxels from predefined brain regions of interest are cross correlated with other voxels' time courses to identify voxels showing significant connectivity with the seed voxels (Cordes et al., 2002; Fox et al., 2005). An alternative approach is the ICA‐based functional network connectivity (FNC), which is a data‐driven method that measures the statistical dependencies among maximally spatially independent configurations of voxel weightings (referred to as independent components) (Calhoun et al., 2001b; Jafri et al., 2008). Networks (Erhardt et al., 2011a) obtained in this way are characterized by a single time course (called the mixing coefficients) (Calhoun et al., 2001b), and have been shown to track closely with previously identified functional domains (Calhoun et al., 2008; Smith et al., 2009).
Studying task‐induced modulations and dynamics of brain networks during interoception can provide more details on how brain process afferent visceral signals originating within the body. Furthermore, understanding the neural regulation of visceral function and sensation provides essential insights into the biological basis of emotion and feeling (Craig, 2002; Damasio, 2004, 2008, 2000; Damasio et al., 2000; LeDoux, 2002). The main aim of this study, therefore, was to extend previous research by
Exploring the modulation of brain networks identified with ICA during the viscerosensory stimulation.
Examining the time course correlations across these networks (FNC) during different visceral interoceptive conditions.
To study network modulation and dynamics during interoception, spatiotemporal control of the viscerosensory stimulation is necessary. The UB was selected as a designated visceral organ to deliver direct viscerosensory stimulations since it differs from other visceral organs in its pattern of activity and in the organization of its neural control mechanisms (Fowler et al., 2008). Specifically, the neural circuits that control the UB function (i.e., urine storage and elimination) have phasic (switch‐like) patterns of activity, which contrasts the tonic patterns of the autonomic pathways that regulate the cardiovascular organs. Furthermore, micturition is a learned behavior hence under voluntary control whereas many other visceral functions such as digestion are regulated involuntarily (Fowler et al., 2008). Previous functional brain imaging studies of continence have identified several cortical and subcortical regions involved in the normal sensation of the UB fullness. These areas include the insula, predominately on the right (Blok et al., 1998; Griffiths et al., 2005, 2007; Kuhtz‐Buschbeck et al., 2005, 2009, 2009; Matsuura et al., 2002; Mehnert et al., 2008; Michels et al., in press; Nour et al., 2000; Tadic et al., 2008, 2012), the ACC (Athwal et al., 2001; Blok et al., 1997, 1998 Griffiths et al., 2005, 2007; Komesu et al., 2011; Matsuura et al., 2002; Mehnert et al., 2008; Michels et al., in press; Seseke et al., 2006; Tadic et al., 2008, 2012, 2012), right inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) (Athwal et al., 2001; Blok et al., 1998; Griffiths et al., 2007; Mehnert et al., 2008), PFC including orbitofrontal cortex (Mehnert et al., 2008), thalamus (Griffiths et al., 2005; Kuhtz‐Buschbeck et al., 2005, 2009; Matsuura et al., 2002; Michels et al., in press; Seseke et al., 2006), right middle temporal gyrus (Mehnert et al., 2008; Tadic et al., 2008), inferior parietal cortex (Mehnert et al., 2008), basal ganglia (Matsuura et al., 2002; Nour et al., 2000; Seseke et al., 2006; Zhang et al., 2005), midbrain periaqueductal gray (PAG) (Athwal et al., 2001; Blok et al. 1997, 1998 Griffiths et al., 2005; Kuhtz‐Buschbeck et al., 2005, 2009; Matsuura et al., 2002; Michels et al., in press; Seseke et al., 2006), brainstem pons (Athwal et al., 2001; Blok et al., 1997, 1998Griffiths et al., 2005; Michels et al., in press; Seseke et al., 2006) and the cerebellum (Griffiths et al., 2005, 2007; Herms et al., 2006; Kuhtz‐Buschbeck et al., 2005; Mehnert et al., 2008; Seseke et al., 2006; Zhang et al., 2005). Additionally, neuroimaging of innocuous temperature stimulation of the UB mucosa by the intravesical ice water instillation showed enhanced activations in the frontal and parietal lobes including the bilateral inferior parietal lobe, right insula, right middle temporal gyrus and the right cerebellum (Mehnert et al., 2011).
The pattern of regional brain activations during the distention pressure on the viscera due to the UB fullness shows similarity to the matrix of brain regions implicated in control of the gastrointestinal (GI) tract (Mayer, 2011; Mayer et al., 2006), which indicates some overlap in neural regulations of visceral function. A systematic review of neuroimaging data of GI visceral stimulation identified key regions of activation to include the insula (both anterior and posterior sections) and the ACC, followed (in order of reported frequency) by primary sensory cortex, PFC especially the orbitofrontal cortex, posterior parietal cortex, and thalamus (Derbyshire, 2003; Mayer et al., 2009) . Furthermore, psychological factors including the cognitive influences such as learning, anticipation, attention/distraction, and emotional features like positive or negative affect, unpleasantness, and emotional context or state have shown to modulate the brain processing of visceral sensation in the ACC, insula, and sensory cortices (Gregory et al., 2003; Van Oudenhove et al., 2011; Yágüez et al., 2005). However, compared to the GI viscerosensory stimulations studies, only few neuroimaging studies of the UB have attempted to identify the connectivity of brain regions with some indications of their functions and interactions among the viscerosensory, affective and cognitive components of the continence/micturition circuits. Tadic et al. (2008) examined the connectivity of continence‐related brain regions in women with urge incontinence using physiophysiological interaction method. The results showed that in normal subjects frontotemporal and sensorimotor cortex, forebrain, midbrain and pontine region were effectively connected with the right insula and ACC (two seeds regions chosen a priori) but in the urge‐incontinent subjects, the effective connectivity was shifted to a parietotemporal complex. Kuhtz‐Buschbeck et al. (2009) used similar approach to explore the effective connectivity of cortical regions during attempted micturition and found that mid‐cingulate cortex had stronger connectivity with the PAG and medial motor regions during attempted micturition compared to the baseline while both the left and right insula showed decreased connectivity. In a more recent study, Michels et al. (in press) performed seed‐based correlation analysis of activated brain areas during the switch from storage to micturition and demonstrated that initiation of micturition induced major activity in the PAG, pons, insula, thalamus, PFC, parietal operculum and cingulate cortex with significant functional connectivity between the forebrain and parietal operculum. Still further research is required to understand the supraspinal networks related to the sensory‐perception of the visceral functions to delineate how brain perceives visceral sensations originating from within the body, detects deviation of physiological parameters, brings them into consciousness when a deviation exceeds a limit, and motivates behavior as well as appropriate motor output (e.g., micturition) to maintain homeostasis (e.g., continence). Based on previous multivariate analysis of visceral interoception (Hall et al., 2010; Hojo et al., 2012) as well as previous conventional (univariate) fMRI analyses of the UB viscerosensory stimulations that we had performed (i.e., Mehnert et al., 2008, 2011; Michels et al., in press; Zempleni et al., 2010; Zhang et al., 2005), our hypothesis was that visceral interoception of the UB (i.e., the mechanical distention and temperature stimulation of the mucosa) would modulate the brain networks specifically related to the stimulus‐driven detection of salient stimuli including the insula and ACC. We further expected that during the gradual increase of distention pressure on the viscera (from the first sensation of filling to strong desire to void), FNC between the interoceptive cortex and subcortical structures involved in affective and motivational dimensions, such as amygdala and ventral striatum (Cardinal et al., 2002), as well as higher order areas related to executive control of the micturition inhibition such the medial PFC (Griffiths, 2004) would be altered.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Participants
Fifteen healthy, right‐handed female participants (age range 21 – 48) with no history of neurological, major medical, or psychiatric disorder participated in the study. Since there are some evidences in the literature indicating gender differences in brain response to viscerosensory stimulation (Kuhtz‐Buschbeck et al., 2009; Labus et al., 2008; Naliboff et al., 2003; Seseke et al., 2008), only female participants were recruited. All participants underwent comprehensive clinical evaluation including (i) detailed medical history, (ii) completion of a three‐day voiding diary providing information on voiding frequency and volumes, amount of fluid intake, and degree of desire to void, (iii) completion of validated German version of the questionnaires on lower urinary tract function and symptoms (used with the permission of the International Consultation on Incontinence Modular Questionnaire, Bristol Urological Institute, Southmead Hospital Bristol, UK) (Jackson et al., 1996), and (iv) urodynamic measurements (Abrams et al., 2006) to ensure the health of the participants. The urodynamic test included the filling cystometry and pressure flow studies of voiding involving the precise measurements of the Intravesical and abdominal pressures. UB sensations were evaluated in relation to the UB filling volume during filling cystometry and comprised assessments of the first sensation (when a participant first becomes aware of the UB filling), first desire to void (UB volume that leads a participant to pass urine at the next convenient moment, but voiding can be delayed if necessary), and strong desire to void (a persistent desire to void without the fear of leakage) (Abrams et al., 2002). [TQ1]Written, informed, IRB approved consent were collected from every participant, and monetary compensation was provided. Two of the participants did not complete one of the functional tasks, and their data were subsequently discarded. The data from the remaining thirteen participants (31.7 ± 8.4 years, mean ± SD) were analyzed further.
Task Design
The study design is derived from previous investigations of viscerosensory stimulation task that consists of controlled filling and emptying of the UB alternating with rest and subjective ratings (Griffiths et al., 2005; Komesu et al., 2011; Mehnert et al., 2008, 2011). To deferentially stimulate this visceral organ, intravesical infusion was performed at different prefill levels, i.e., no prefill, low prefill (100 ml saline), and high prefill (individually adapted to the sensations of persistent strong desire to void (Abrams et al., 2002) measured from maximum cytometric capacity during the pre‐scan urodynamic test), and with different infusion temperatures, i.e., body warm (∼37°C) or ice cold (4–8°C) saline solution (0.9% NaCl, B Braun Melsungen AG, Germany) (Fig. 1A–C). The intravesical instillation of cold saline (ice water test) has been routinely used as a supplement to routine cystometry especially in conditions such as spinal cord injury or multiple sclerosis for evaluation of the cold‐sensitive receptor via the afferent C‐fiber (Chancellor et al., 1998; Geirsson et al., 1993). Although opinion varies with respect to thermal sensitivity of the UB mucosa (Bors and Parker, 1956; Nathan, 1952; Tammela et al., 1992), intravesical instillation of 4–8°C saline is considered innocuous and any pain during ice water test distinguishes clinical UB hypersensitivity (Mukerji et al., 2006) induced by pathological conditions including inflammation and hypertrophy (Burnstock, 2007). To avoid potential influences of the menstrual cycle on viscerosensory sensitivity (Powell‐Boone et al., 2005), or possible interference of menstrual pain with the experiment, participants were scanned during days 14–28, i.e., the luteal phase, of their menstrual cycle.
Upon entering the scanner, participants were catheterized transurethrally and a high‐resolution T1 image was acquired. During a break, each participant underwent training to become familiar with a rating apparatus and the procedure of the fMRI scan. They were instructed to focus on their pain and desire to void sensations during stimulus administration, wait for the appearance of the visual analogue scale on the screen, and rate their sensations by taking the self‐perspective from 0 (no desire to void/no pain) to 10 (strongest desire to void/most intense pain imaginable) using a custom‐built fMRI‐compatible handheld response system (Jarrahi and Wanek, 2014; Jarrahi et al., 2013). They were asked to use their dominant right hand to rate and do it as quickly and as accurately as possible. The timing and sequencing of all stimulus events were controlled with the operational control software of an automated fMRI‐compatible visceral infusion and drainage system that we developed for fMRI studies of visceral interoception to ensure precise temporal execution of visceral and visual stimuli during fMRI.
The NO‐PREFILL WARM and NO‐PREFILL COLD conditions each started with an empty UB. The task paradigm consisted of eight blocks of viscerosensory stimulation (15 min) with additional rest periods at the beginning (1 min) and end (1 min) of the experiment (17 min total measuring time) (Fig. 1B). Each viscerosensory block consisted of intravesical infusion (24 s), ratings (15 s), immediately followed by an intravesical drainage lasting 60 s, and rest (jittered between 7 and 9 s). The operation of the automated infusion and drainage system was time‐locked to each functional volume that was acquired via a trigger signal generated by the MRI scanner at the beginning of each volume acquisition. This allowed the precise matching of functional images with a corresponding visceral infusion and drainage epoch. Each infusion epoch consisted of a15‐s intravesical infusion of 100 ml warm (for NO‐PREFILL WARM condition) or cold (for NO‐PREFILL COLD condition) saline, which was performed by our fMRI‐compatible device at the rate of 400 ml/min followed by a 9‐s pause to ensure adequate time for the slow visceral sensation to be registered before participants rated their sensations. During the intravesical drainage epoch, the UB was drained passively by opening the valve of a 3‐way stop‐cock that connected participants’ catheter to a separate plastic container instead of an active drainage by the automated device to avoid possible suction of the UB mucosa at this zero prefill level.
The LOW‐PREFILL WARM and HIGH‐PREFILL WARM conditions started with 100 ml, and strong‐desire‐to‐void prefilled level, respectively. Each condition consisted of eight blocks of viscerosensory stimulation (13 min) with 1‐min rest periods at the beginning and end (15 min total measuring time) (Fig. 1C). The viscerosensory block comprised an intravesical infusion (24 s), followed by ratings (15 s), and rest (jittered between 7 and 9 s), then an intravesical drainage (24 s), ratings after drainage (15 s), and rest (again jittered between 7 and 9 s). Visceral infusion and drainage were again carried out automatically by the computerized system with a 15‐s intravesical infusion or withdrawal of 100 ml warm saline at the rate of 400 ml/min and subsequent 9‐s pause for a total of 24‐s infusion or drainage epoch. Since the UB cooling is an not a natural physiological event and it is considered a more intense stimuli than warm filling, and similarly the HIGH‐PREFILL WARM condition is a more intense stimuli than the LOW‐ and NO‐PREFILL WARM conditions, we defined the following sequence of conditions to avoid interferences from the previous condition: all participants started with the NO‐PREFILL WARM condition, followed by the LOW‐PREFILL WARM condition, then the HIGH‐PREFILL WARM condition, and finished with the NO‐PREFILL COLD condition. Scanning of the participants was performed in a darkened room, with either a fixation crosshair (while subjects were not rating) or the visual analogue scale (during rating) projected via an LCD projector onto a translucent screen that stood at the foot of the gantry of the MRI scanner. The participants viewed the screen from inside the scanner through a mounted mirror attached to the head coil positioned above their eyes at a 45° angle. Participants lay in supine position on the scanner table with their legs slightly abducted and comfortably resting on custom cushions.
Image Acquisition
All images were acquired at the MRI Center of the University Hospital of Zurich on a Philips Ingenia 3T system (Best, Netherlands) using a 15‐channel receive head coil. The blood oxygenation level–dependent (BOLD)‐sensitive functional images were acquired by the gradient‐echo echo‐planar‐imaging (EPI) sequence (TR/TE = 2000/30 ms, field of view = 24 cm, image matrix = 96 × 96, flip angle = 80°, slice thickness = 3 mm, inter‐slice gap = 1 mm, 34 slices in ascending order). Four dummy scans were acquired at the beginning of each functional condition to ensure the signal had achieved a steady state, and were subsequently discarded. A total of 500 volumes of the entire brain were collected for each NO‐PREFILL WARM and NO‐PREFILL COLD condition, and 445 volumes were obtained for each LOW‐PREFILL WARM and HIGH‐PREFILL WARM condition. A high‐resolution T1‐weighted anatomical scan (3D T1‐FFE, TE/TR = 3.1/6.9 ms, field of view = 22 cm, matrix = 256 × 256, slices = 180, slice thickness = 0.75 mm, no gap) was also acquired for each participant for registration with the functional data and visualization purposes. An independent neuroradiologist evaluated all T1‐weighted images and no gross morphological or focal abnormalities were reported for any of the participants.
Statistical Analysis of Online Sensation Ratings
The Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS, version 21.0, SPSS, Chicago, IL) was used for rating analysis. The visual analogue scale ratings and rating times for desire to void and pain induced by viscerosensory stimulation were analyzed separately by means of one‐way repeated measure analysis of variance (ANOVA) with the prefill volumes (no‐, low‐ and high‐load) as the within‐subjects factor. Two‐tailed post‐hoc pairwise comparison tests were performed to examine which pairs differed among different viscerosensory testing conditions. A Greenhouse‐Geisser correction was used for sphericity when Mauchly's test indicated sphericity assumption was violated. Paired Student's t‐test was used for pairwise comparison of different interoceptive task conditions and differences were considered significant with P < 0.05.
Data Preprocessing
Following the completion of each scan, acquired imaging data were exported to an offline workstation and preprocessed using Statistical Parametric Mapping (SPM) software (Wellcome Department of Cognitive Neurology, London, UK, version 8) running under the MATLAB environment (Mathworks, Natick, MA, release 13a). Data from each participant were first realigned (motion‐corrected) using rigid body transformations (Friston et al., 1995), followed by slice‐timing correction by temporally aligning all slices to the same reference time point (first slice). We chose to perform realignment first to minimize the effect of inter‐slice movement (Sladky et al., 2011). This step was followed by co‐registration of the functional images to the T1‐weighted images, and spatial normalization to a standard stereotactic space (Montreal Neurological Institute, MNI, Quebec, Canada) (Friston et al., 1995). The normalized functional images were smoothed with an isotropic Gaussian kernel of 8 mm at full width at half maximum. Finally, the data were intensity‐normalized by dividing the time series of each voxel by its average intensity, and converting data to percent signal change units to improve the accuracy and test–retest reliability of ICA (Allen et al., 2011).
Group Independent Component Analysis
Data were decomposed into independent components (ICs) using spatial ICA. Spatial ICA applied to fMRI data aims to separate spatially independent patterns (referred to as spatial maps) from their linearly mixed BOLD signals (referred to as time courses) via maximization of mutual independence among components (Calhoun et al., 2001a). We used ICA to identify the networks because this multivariate, data‐driven method requires no a priori hypothesis or model of brain activity. It simultaneously takes into account the relationships between all voxels, instead of, e.g., considering only the single region‐dominant networks produced by seed‐based correlations (Zuo et al., 2010). Additionally, ICA is capable of extracting scanner or physiological noise and motion artifacts from the dataset (Thomas et al., 2002), and is not influenced by temporal sampling rates (De Luca et al., 2006). Finally, compared to seed‐based approaches, ICA may offer better sensitivity to detect subtle differences among subjects (Koch et al., 2010), which is important for our study since interoceptive sensation varies among individuals (Herbert et al., 2012; Pollatos et al., 2005; Schandry, 1981; Wiens et al., 2000).
Preprocessed time series were analyzed using the Group ICA of fMRI toolbox (GIFT) software package (Medical Image Analysis Lab, University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, New Mexico; version1.3 h). Data from all functional conditions were analyzed in one group ICA instead of four separate ICAs so that a tighter comparison between different task conditions could be performed without the added variability from separate ICA analyses (Arbabshirani et al., 2013). The number of independent sources (i.e., optimal dimensionality of the ICs) was estimated using the GIFT dimensionality estimation tool based on the aggregated data using the minimum description length (MDL) criterion, modified to account for spatial correlation (Li et al., 2007). Note that there's no standard for selecting the number of ICA components; too few means that essentially we are removing too much single subject variance while too many might segregate networks. MDL algorithm offers an estimate of the optimal number of ICs by making a decision based upon the complexity or information content of the data. Briefly, this algorithm employs a subsampling scheme to obtain a set of effectively independent and identically distributed samples from the data and then applies the information‐theoretic criteria to the sample set to estimate the number of informative components (Li et al., 2006, 2007). MDL algorithm estimated the optimal dimensionality of the ICs in our dataset to be about 40 averaged across all participants (39.4 ± 6.15, mean ± SD). A two‐step data reduction process was subsequently conducted (Calhoun et al., 2001a). First, fMRI data from individual participants were reduced to a lower dimensionality through subject‐specific data reduction by principal component analysis with a standard economy‐size decomposition. Next, reduced data from all participants were concatenated and reduced to one group and the independent group components were retained using the expectation‐maximization algorithm. Statistical reliability of the IC decomposition (i.e., whether an IC has the tendency to split or merge with another IC) was determined by running the information maximization (infomax) algorithm (Bell and Sejnowski, 1995) 100 times in the ICASSO software, a tool that is specialized for studying the reliability of ICA estimates by clustering and visualization (Himberg et al., 2004). Using this software, which is incorporated as part of the GIFT software package, the unmixing matrix was randomly initialized 100 times, then the infomax algorithm ran with each initialized unmixing matrix to extract specified number of ICs. The similarity between ICs from each condition was calculated by the absolute correlation. The agglomerative hierarchical clustering with average‐linkage criterion was used for the clustering of the estimated ICs (Himberg et al., 2004). Finally, to identify subject‐specific spatial maps and time courses, back‐reconstruction was performed on the group components using a dual regression analysis [group ICA back‐reconstruction approach number 3 (GICA‐3) algorithm in the GIFT software package (Erhardt et al., 2011b)].
Following previous studies, we used a methodical approach to identify ICs that were brain signals as opposed to noise or physiological artifacts (Allen et al., 2011; Arbabshirani et al., 2013; Calhoun et al., 2002b, 2004c; Ma et al., 2011; Meda et al., 2009; Zhang and Li, 2012). First, the cluster quality index (Iq) from the ICASSO software was examined as the criterion to validate the reliability and stability of IC decomposition. The Iq ranges from 0 to 1 with values close to unity signifying that the corresponding component is reliably extracted, i.e., similar components are estimated at each condition of the ICA algorithm (Ma et al., 2011). On the other hand, Iq values approaching zero implies that the likelihood that the component is randomly produced is higher (Li et al., 2007). ICs with the Iq value below 0.8 from 100 ICASSO repetitions were excluded from further analysis. Next, the spatial correlation of each IC's spatial map with a priori probabilistic maps of cerebrospinal fluid, white matter, and gray matter in MNI space as provided with SPM8 was assessed using GIFT spatial sorting. Components that showed high correlations with cerebrospinal fluid or white matter, or low correlations with gray matter indicating they might be artifacts or noise rather than hemodynamic change were identified and excluded from further analysis. Visual inspection of spatial patterns and frequency analysis of each IC spectra (i.e., time courses dominated by low‐frequency fluctuations, see (Cordes et al., 2000)) allowed additional components related to artifacts to be discarded. Identification of remaining ICs was performed through cross‐correlation with reference standard network templates as well as knowledge from established brain networks described in previous studies (Allen et al., 2011; Beckmann et al., 2009; D'Argembeau et al., 2005; Damoiseaux et al., 2006; Kiviniemi et al., 2009; Laird et al., 2011; Mantini et al., 2007, 2013Seeley et al., 2007; Smith et al., 2009). Specifically, we performed a multiple spatial regression with a previously established and publicly available baseline set of reference components (Allen et al., 2011) as regressors of interest to identify equivalent ICs in our dataset. We chose reference networks from (Allen et al., 2011) to identify equivalent ICs in our dataset because for both datasets ICA was conducted using the same software package (i.e., GIFT) following similar preprocessing procedure in SPM, while reference ICs from others studies such as (Laird et al., 2011; Smith et al., 2009) used multivariate exploratory linear optimized decomposition into independent components (MELODIC) toolbox (Beckmann et al., 2005) in the FMRIB Software Library (FSL) (Smith et al., 2004; Woolrich et al., 2009). Although unlike our task‐derived ICs, reference networks from (Allen et al., 2011) are derived from the resting‐state data, ICA networks have been consistently and reliably identified in both the resting state (Damoiseaux et al., 2006; Zuo et al., 2010) as well as task activation fMRI studies (Fukunaga et al., 2006; Laird et al., 2011; Smith et al., 2009; Vincent et al., 2007) affirming that similar networks, with strong spatial correspondence to the resting state, could also be elicited using task paradigms (Calhoun et al., 2008).To display voxels relevant to a particular IC, we normalized back‐reconstructed spatial maps of each IC into z‐scores (Beckmann et al., 2005; Calhoun et al., 2001a). The normalized spatial maps of z‐scores of individual subject were averaged together across the four functional conditions, and the averaged maps of z‐scores were plotted as t‐statistics, thresholded at t > μ + 4σ (following the justification described in appendix B in (Allen et al., 2011)). Component anatomical locations and Brodmann's areas (BA) were determined using the cytoarchitectonic probability maps as implemented in the SPM Anatomy Toolbox (Eickhoff et al., 2005).
Task‐Related Modulations of Networks
In order to assess the degree of task relevance of each ICA component, regression analysis of component time courses was conducted. First a design matrix of task regressors for each viscerosensory condition was constructed using SPM8. The design matrices represented the onset of each task condition, convolved with a box‐car hemodynamic response function (HRF; Friston et al., 1994). For the NO‐PREFILL WARM and NO‐PREFILL COLD conditions, a design matrix of four regressors (i.e., infusion, filled state, rating after filled, and drainage), and for the LOW‐PREFILL WARM and HIGH‐PREFILL WARM conditions, a design matrix of six regressors (i.e., infusion, filled state, rating after filled, drainage, emptied state, and rating after emptied) were created. For each viscerosensory task, the component time courses were temporally sorted according to the strength of correlation between the component time courses and the time course of each of these events as embodied in the design matrices. This procedure led to a set of regression coefficients (termed β‐weights) for each regressor associated with each participant. These β‐weights, which represented the degree of synchrony between IC time courses and reference time courses, were used to assess engagement of each IC during different task conditions (Meda et al., 2009; Zhang and Li, 2012). Positive and negative β‐weights at one task condition relative to another indicate positive and negative correlation with task‐related activity in the IC. Furthermore, a greater absolute value of the β‐weight usually indicates a greater engagement (task‐related up‐modulation if the sign is positive) or disengagement (task‐related down‐modulation for negative βs) of an IC (Zhang and Li, 2012). Two‐tailed one‐sample Student's t‐test against zero was performed on β‐weights separately for each condition (P < 0.05). These P‐values were not corrected for the total number of comparisons because these β‐weights were not all independent between trials (e.g., infusion vs. drainage) in the same component or between components for the same condition (Zhang and Li, 2012). In addition to the β‐weights statistics, we computed the event‐related averages for each task‐related ICs over a default window of 30 s as implemented in the GIFT software package. These event‐related averages represent their associated IC activation level over the course of a typical HRF during a task.
Functional Network Connectivity
Temporal correlations among the time series of the task‐related ICs were assessed using the FNC toolbox (Medical Image Analysis Lab, University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, New Mexico; version 2.3). The ICA algorithm assumes synchronicity between the time courses of the responses in cortical areas within one IC (Calhoun et al., 2004b). Though the ICs are maximally spatially independent, significant temporal correlations can exist between them (Calhoun et al., 2001a). To examine this, a constrained maximal time‐lagged correlation method was used. Pearson's correlation between the time course of each task‐related IC pairs was computed as in (Jafri et al., 2008) with maximal lag set to 4 s (2*TR, TR = 2 s). Allowing lag between time courses is important to account for HRF shape variations among brain regions as well as among different subjects, although the source of this lag is still debatable (Handwerker et al., 2004). Prior to computing correlations, networks’ time courses were filtered using a band pass Butterworth filter with cut‐off frequencies at 0.017 and 0.24 Hz to suppress the very low and very high frequencies, respectively (Arbabshirani et al., 2013). The maximum correlation and the corresponding lag values were subsequently calculated for every subject and separately for each task condition. Correlations were normalized using Fisher's transformation [z = arctanh (r)] and the robustness of maximum lagged correlation between time course pairs was tested. To determine the significant differences between task conditions, paired Student's t‐test was performed. For all FNC analyses, the cut‐off P‐value was set at 0.05, false discovery rate (FDR)‐corrected for multiple comparisons (Genovese et al., 2002).
RESULTS
Rating Sensations During the Scan
Statistical analysis of the rating sensations during the scan (Table 1) by means of ANOVA revealed a significant effect of adding 100 ml warm saline to the prefill volume on desire to void sensation [F(2, 24) = 128.11, P <0.001, ηp 2 = 0.914]. Post‐hoc pairwise comparisons indicated that the mean desire to void rating after infusion was lower for NO‐PREFILL WARM condition than for LOW‐PREFILL WARM [t(12) = 2.67, P = 0.02], and HIGH‐PREFILL WARM [t(12) = 18.50, P < 0.001]. There was a significant difference in the mean desire to void rating between LOW‐PREFILL WARM and HIGH‐PREFILL WARM [t(12) = 11.594, P < 0.001] (Fig. 2). Despite having base level of zero (i.e., empty UB) for NO‐PREFILL WARM and NO‐PREFILL COLD conditions, a paired Student's t‐test indicated that subjective desire to void sensation after infusion of 100 ml cold saline was significantly greater than desire to void sensation after infusion of 100 ml warm saline [t(12) = 5.60, P < 0.001]. There was no significant effect of prefill volume on pain after infusion of 100 ml warm saline in any prefill conditions [Greenhouse‐Geisser corrected, F(1, 15) = 4.49, P = 0.056, ηp 2 = 0.272]. Similarly, no significant effect of prefill volume was found on either desire to void or pain rating times [F(2, 24) = 0.801, P = 0.460, ηp 2 = 0.063, and F(2, 24) = 0.832, P = 0.447, ηp 2 = 0.065, respectively].
Table 1.
Visceraosensory condition | Rating value | Rating time | |
---|---|---|---|
NO‐PREFILL WARM | Desire to void after infusion | 1.47 ± 1.07 | 5.68 ± 0.64 s |
Pain after infusion | 0.55 ± 0.31 | 4.83 ± 0.78 s | |
LOW‐PREFILL WARM | Desire to void after infusion | 2.79 ± 1.87 | 5.92 ± 0.67 s |
Pain after infusion | 0.53 ± 0.27 | 5.27 ± 0.87 s | |
Desire to void after Drainage | 1.72 ± 1.39 | 5.62 ± 0.60 s | |
Pain after Drainage | 0.55 ± 0.34 | 5.33 ± 0.71 s | |
HIGH‐PREFILL WARM | Desire to void after infusion | 8.17 ± 1.01 | 5.90 ± 0.68 s |
Pain after infusion | 0.99 ± 0.75 | 5.12 ± 1.02 s | |
Desire to void after Drainage | 5.41 ± 1.06 | 6.07 ± 0.62 s | |
Pain after Drainage | 0.59 ± 0.31 | 5.57 ± 0.75 s | |
NO‐PREFILL COLD | Desire to void after infusion | 5.46 ± 2.44 | 5.69 ± 0.95 s |
Pain after infusion | 1.56 ± 1.63 | 5.45 ± 0.81 s |
Paired Student's t‐test showed that the mean desire to void rating after drainage was significantly higher for HIGH‐PREFILL WARM compared to LOW‐PREFILL WARM condition [t(12) = 10.191, P < 0.001]. The desire to void ratings after drainage were also significantly different from desire to void rating after infusion for the LOW‐PREFILL WARM [t(12) = 2.66, P = 0.021], and HIGH‐PREFILL WARM [t(12) = 6.695, P < 0.001]. There was no significant difference in the mean rating time for desire to void after infusion versus after drainage for the LOW‐PREFILL WARM [t(12) = 1.592, P = 0.137], or the HIGH‐PREFILL WARM [t(12) = 0.725, P = 0.482]. Similarly, there was no statistically significant difference in pain rating value or rating time between any pairs across the four task conditions.
In post‐scan interviews, all participants reported following the suggested strategy of focusing on the sensations during stimulus administration and waiting for the appearance of the visual analogue scale on the screen to perform the rating. There was no report of discomfort or painful sensations associated with the supine‐lying position, length of the scanning period, or the use of the stabilization foam pads around their head and neck areas. A potential reason that pain was not a true zero here might be due to the fact that we used an analogue visual scale with two‐digit decimal accuracy. We also used a custom‐made response box (Jarrahi et al., 2013) with a manipulandum instead of a commercial button box. Thus getting a true zero was difficult using this analogue scale and the said custom‐made rating device.
Furthermore, prior to fMRI scans participants were evaluated clinically (voiding diary) and urodynamically (maximum cystometric capacity, maximum detrusor pressure, desire to void, etc.). The maximum cystometric capacity, which reflect the maximum functional UB volume, and the prefilled UB volume for strong desire to void, which was used for profiling prior to the HIGH‐PREFILL WARM condition, were on average 658.7 ± 138.3 ml, and 446.7 ± 110.6 ml (mean ± SD), respectively (Supporting Information Table I).
Network Modulations During Visceral Interoception
Out of 40 ICs (Supporting Information Fig. 1), we identified 25 components to be brain networks and discarded the rest as physiological artifacts, head movements, scanner noise, or mixture of brain signals and artifacts. Specifically, to identify equivalent ICs in our dataset to the standard references, we performed a multiple spatial regression with a previously established baseline set of ICA components as regressors of interest (Allen et al., 2011). Subsequently, we tested the significance level of β‐weights for the 25 networks (i.e., non‐artifactual ICA components). For ICs surviving the uncorrected P < 0.05, we selected those that were among the top five rank ordered components for “infusion,” “filled,” “drainage” or “emptied” trials separately for each viscerosensory stimulation condition and attained task‐relevant networks for the corresponding condition. In Figures 3, 4, 5, 6, (a) the task‐related components' spatial maps, (b) associated β‐weights, and (c) related event averages of task trials as computed from the component time courses are presented for the NO‐PREFILL WARM, NO‐PREFILL COLD, LOW‐PREFILL WARM, and HIGH‐PREFILL WARM conditions, respectively. Detailed information of component spatial map including the main regions of positive activation, BA, number of voxels, peak activation t‐value, and MNI coordinates are listed in the Table 2. The group mean and standard deviation of β‐weights, and corresponding uncorrected P‐values from one‐sample t‐tests for task‐related ICs are presented in Tables 3 and 6, separately for each condition.
Table 2.
IC | Brain region | Cluster size (voxels) | t max | MNI coordinates | Cytoarchitectonic Brodmann area (Probability, if available) | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
x | y | z | |||||
IC 2 (0.98) | Right anterior cingulate cortex | 1944 | 44.32 | 6 | 32 | 0 | |
Right inferior frontal gyrus | same cluster | 33.45 | 27 | 29 | −15 | ||
Left caudate nucleus | same cluster | 22.47 | −6 | 17 | 0 | ||
Left middle orbital gyrus | same cluster | 16.94 | −21 | 32 | −21 | ||
Right Parahippocampal gyrus | 57 | 13.65 | 21 | −1 | −27 | Hipp (EC) (90%) | |
IC 3 (0.97) | Right parahippocampa gyrus | 1068 | 21.43 | 9 | 2 | −18 | Hipp (EC) (20%) |
Right olfactory cortex | same cluster | 28.42 | 3 | 11 | −9 | ||
Left caudate nucleus | same cluster | 27.44 | −6 | 8 | 6 | ||
Right amygdala | same cluster | 15.66 | 24 | −1 | −12 | Amyg (SF) (80%) | |
IC 9 (0.97) | Right cerebellum | 2140 | 31.32 | 12 | −46 | −36 | |
Left cerebellum | same cluster | 30.33 | −21 | −46 | −48 | Lobule VIIIb (Hem) (52%) | |
IC 11 (0.98) | Right insula lobe | 3979 | 50.75 | 39 | −7 | −6 | Insula (Id1) (20%) |
Right thalamus | same cluster | 33.50 | 12 | −16 | 3 | Th‐Prefrontal (84%) | |
Left putamen | same cluster | 31.22 | −30 | −1 | 9 | ||
IC 12 (0.97) | Left cerebellum | 2741 | 32.09 | −18 | −70 | −30 | Lobule VI (Hem) (98%) |
Right cerebellum | same cluster | 30.57 | 12 | −49 | −24 | Lobule V (69%) | |
IC 16 (0.96) | Left insula lobe | 1549 | 26.19 | −39 | −5 | 5 | |
Left inferior frontal gyrus | same cluster | 22.62 | −51 | 23 | 0 | Area 45 (40%) | |
Left rolandic operculum | same cluster | 20.98 | −54 | 5 | 6 | OP 4 (40%) | |
Right insula lobe | 1026 | 31.90 | 45 | −1 | 0 | OP 3 (10%) | |
Right inferior frontal gyrus | same cluster | 18.43 | 48 | 20 | −6 | Area 45 (10%) | |
IC 22 (0.97) | Right fusiform gyrus | 3575 | 34.33 | 33 | −37 | −21 | |
Right Parahippocampal gyrus | same cluster | 32.66 | 24 | −4 | −33 | Hipp (EC) (100%) | |
Left inferior frontal gyrus | same cluster | 26.22 | −48 | 26 | −9 | Area 45 (20%) | |
Left caudate nucleus | 70 | 13.74 | −12 | 19 | 5 | ||
IC 24 (0.97) | Left thalamus | 2314 | 36.22 | −15 | −31 | 3 | |
Right thalamus | same cluster | 24.69 | 6 | −16 | 12 | ||
Pons | 86 | 13.54 | −9 | −25 | −33 | ||
IC 26 (0.95) | Right insula lobe | 2568 | 44.03 | 39 | −19 | 12 | Insula (Ig2) (80%) |
Right rolandic operculum | same cluster | 31.82 | 51 | −1 | 6 | OP 4 (30%) | |
Right putamen | same cluster | 23.58 | 30 | −7 | 12 | OP 3 (20%) | |
Left superior temporal gyrus | 2052 | 24.91 | −60 | −28 | 6 | ||
Left rolandic operculum | same cluster | 24.04 | −48 | −16 | 12 | OP 1 (10%) | |
Left insula lobe | same cluster | 22.91 | −36 | −13 | 6 | Insula (Ig2) (50%) | |
Right middle cingulate cortex | 216 | 22.90 | 6 | 29 | 30 | ||
Right anterior cingulate cortex | same cluster | 8.83 | 15 | 38 | 21 | ||
IC 27 (0.95) | Left middle temporal gyrus | 1820 | 28.07 | −45 | −52 | 12 | IPC (PGa) (30%) |
Left inferior parietal lobule | same cluster | 26.29 | −51 | −25 | 36 | IPC (PFt) (80%) | |
Right middle temporal gyrus | 1661 | 44.67 | 48 | −55 | 12 | IPC (PGp) (20%) | |
Right Supramarginal gyrus | same cluster | 29.67 | 63 | −37 | 24 | IPC (PF) (100%) | |
Left precuneus | 307 | 22.47 | −9 | −52 | 48 | SPL (5M) (20%) | |
IC 30 (0.92) | Left precentral gyrus | 4155 | 38.75 | 8 | −57 | 18 | Area 44 (50%) |
Left inferior frontal gyrus | same cluster | 27.39 | −42 | 35 | 15 | Area 45 (60%) | |
Left middle orbital gyrus | same cluster | 25.87 | −42 | 50 | −3 | ||
Right precentral gyrus | 454 | 14.45 | −4 | 45 | −9 | Area 44 (40%) | |
Right inferior frontal gyrus | same cluster | 18.22 | 57 | 20 | 9 | Area 45 (90%) | |
Right rolandic operculum | same cluster | 11.37 | 57 | −13 | 18 | OP 4 (70%) | |
Right cerebellum | 338 | 19.51 | 36 | −70 | −36 | Lobule VIIa (Hem) (60%) | |
Left inferior temporal gyrus | 121 | 19.83 | −54 | −40 | −15 | ||
Left superior medial gyrus | 100 | 13.41 | −9 | 44 | 42 | ||
IC 31 (0.92) | Right middle orbital gyrus | 1967 | 29.04 | 36 | 50 | −6 | |
Right superior medial gyrus | same cluster | 25.03 | 12 | 32 | 45 | ||
Right middle frontal gyrus | same cluster | 24.63 | 36 | 53 | 3 | ||
Left cerebellum | 1179 | 25.37 | −39 | −76 | −30 | Lobule VIIa (Hem) (98%) | |
Right inferior parietal lobule | 1149 | 31.99 | 48 | −55 | 45 | IPC (PGa) (50%) | |
Right postcentral gyrus | same cluster | 13.23 | 39 | −31 | 51 | Area 3b (100%) | |
Left inferior parietal lobule | 408 | 18.99 | −42 | −58 | 48 | hIP1(50%) | |
IC 33 (0.93) | Left inferior parietal lobule | 2157 | 21.37 | −51 | −52 | 45 | IPC (PFm) (60%) |
Right superior temporal gyrus | 420 | 19.15 | 60 | −31 | 3 | ||
Right precuneus | 361 | 23.68 | 3 | −55 | 33 | SPL (7A) (10%) | |
Left posterior cingulate cortex | same cluster | 20.81 | −6 | −52 | 30 | ||
Right angular gyrus | 174 | 17.49 | 45 | −55 | 36 | IPC (PGa)(60%) | |
Right middle frontal gyrus | same cluster | 9.75 | 36 | 35 | 36 | IPC (PFm) (70%) | |
IC 35 (0.90) | Left mid orbital gyrus | 1889 | 42.95 | −6 | 65 | −6 | |
Left superior medial gyrus | same cluster | 30.18 | −3 | 71 | 3 | ||
Left anterior cingulate cortex | same cluster | 17.89 | −3 | 47 | 6 | ||
Right superior frontal gyrus | same cluster | 15.49 | 24 | 38 | 51 | Area 6 (10%) | |
Left precuneus | 962 | 29.50 | 0 | −58 | 30 | SPL (7A) (10%) | |
Right cuneus | same cluster | 26.77 | 12 | −61 | 21 | ||
Left cerebellum | 679 | 19.90 | −12 | −64 | −42 | Lobule VIIIb (Hem) (30%) | |
Right superior parietal lobule | 482 | 16.18 | 18 | −49 | 63 | SPL (5L) (80%) | |
Right Parahippocampal gyrus | 417 | 26.54 | 30 | −25 | −21 | Hipp (SUB) (80%) | |
Right angular gyrus | 357 | 31.10 | 54 | −64 | 30 | IPC (PGp) (90%) | |
Left Superior parietal lobule | 355 | 16.47 | −27 | −76 | 45 | IPC (PGp) (20%) |
The MNI (Montreal Neurological Institute) coordinates show the coordinates of the peak voxels with negative x coordinates refer to left hemisphere activations.
Brodmann's area as indicated by the cytoarchitectonic maximum probability map using the SPM‐Anatomy toolbox developed by Eickhoff et al. (2005). The quality index (Iq) associated with each component is listed in parentheses next to the component number.
Table 3.
IC | β‐weights: Mean ± SD | One‐sample t‐test: P‐valuea | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
I | F | RF | D | I | F | RF | D | |
IC 30 | 0.46 ± 0.31 | 0.43 ± 0.33 | 0.69 ± 0.54 | 0.27 ± 0.34 | 0.000b | 0.000b | 0.001b | 0.014b |
IC 26 | 0.36 ± 0.27 | 0.34 ± 0.27 | 0.38 ± 0.43 | 0.46 ± 0.33 | 0.000b | 0.001b | 0.008b | 0.000b |
IC 31 | 0.25 ± 0.35 | 0.21 ± 0.37 | 0.67 ± 0.41 | 0.03 ± 0.32 | 0.026b | 0.060 | 0.000b | 0.784 |
IC 27 | 0.19 ± 0.30 | 0.27 ± 0.41 | 1.19 ± 0.57 | 0.07 ± 0.35 | 0.042b | 0.037b | 0.000b | 0.454 |
IC 35 | 0.05 ± 0.41 | 0.19 ± 0.33 | −1.08 ± 0.51 | 0.30 ± 0.30 | 0.693 | 0.061 | 0.000b | 0.004b |
IC 22 | 0.13 ± 0.25 | 0.20 ± 0.36 | 0.05 ± 0.36 | 0.20 ± 0.17 | 0.083 | 0.066 | 0.655 | 0.001b |
One‐sample t‐test was performed to determine whether the β‐weights are statistically different from zero. All P‐values were uncorrected because these β‐weights were not independent between trials for the same IC or between ICs for the same trial.
P < 0.05.
Table 6.
IC | β‐weights: Mean ± SD | One‐sample t‐test: P‐valuea | ||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
I | F | RF | D | E | RE | I | F | RF | D | E | RE | |
IC 11 | 0.19 ± 0.25 | 0.21 ± 0.24 | 0.13 ± 0.33 | 0.14 ± 0.15 | 0.11 ± 0.18 | 0.10 ± 0.31 | 0.019b | 0.008b | 0.182 | 0.005b | 0.057 | 0.285 |
IC 2 | 0.09 ± 0.16 | 0.17 ± 0.31 | −0.24 ± 0.29 | −0.01 ± 0.21 | 0.22 ± 0.26 | −0.13 ± 0.28 | 0.056 | 0.063 | 0.013b | 0.873 | 0.010b | 0.117 |
IC 3 | 0.12 ± 0.09 | 0.19 ± 0.15 | 0.03 ± 0.16 | 0.06 ± 0.12 | 0.15 ± 0.13 | 0.06 ± 0.16 | 0.000b | 0.001b | 0.514 | 0.086 | 0.002b | 0.216 |
IC 33 | 0.20 ± 0.41 | 0.15 ± 0.54 | −1.06 ± 0.39 | 0.29 ± 0.30 | 0.21 ± 0.46 | −1.28 ± 0.41 | 0.104 | 0.326 | 0.000b | 0.005b | 0.131 | 0.000b |
IC 9 | 0.08 ± 0.14 | 0.05 ± 0.16 | 0.09 ± 0.21 | 0.13 ± 0.17 | 0.04 ± 0.17 | 0.08 ± 0.17 | 0.052 | 0.304 | 0.124 | 0.018b | 0.403 | 0.110 |
IC 12 | 0.12 ± 0.19 | 0.08 ± 0.41 | 0.10 ± 0.25 | 0.05 ± 0.24 | −0.04 ± 0.32 | 0.00 ± 0.21 | 0.045b | 0.482 | 0.165 | 0.497 | 0.682 | 0.936 |
IC 24 | 0.00 ± 0.18 | 0.18 ± 0.25 | 0.89 ± 0.30 | 0.03 ± 0.20 | 0.03 ± 0.28 | 0.66 ± 0.32 | 0.966 | 0.024b | 0.000b | 0.629 | 0.683 | 0.000b |
One‐sample t‐test was performed to determine whether the β‐weights are statistically different from zero. All P‐values were uncorrected because these β‐weights were not independent between trials for the same IC or between ICs for the same trial.
P < 0.05
ICs 30 and 31 involve the dorsolateral PFC and posterior parietal cortices (similar to the reference components 34 and 60 in (Allen et al., 2011), with multiple regression values of 0.34 and 0.61, respectively). These reference components were identified in (Allen et al., 2011) as central executive networks (Bressler and Menon, 2010; Seeley et al., 2007). IC 35 matched the so‐called the default mode brain network reference components from (Allen et al., 2011) (reference component 53, multiple regression value: 0.46, reference component 25, multiple regression value: 0.29, and reference component 68, multiple regression value: 0.17) with clusters in the medial PFC, ACC, the precuneus/posterior cingulate cortex, bilateral parietal lobules and the inferior temporal gyri (Greicius and Menon, 2004; Raichle et al., 2001). IC 33 showed the strongest overlap with reference component 50 from (Allen et al., 2011) (multiple regression value: 0.16), which comprises more posterior parts of the default mode network involving the posterior midline structures of the precuneus/posterior cingulate cortex and inferior parietal lobules (Raichle et al., 2001; Shulman et al., 1997). IC 16 involves the anterior insula/frontal opercula extending to the bilateral IFG (Table 2). Reference component 55 from (Allen et al., 2011), which is classified as an ACC/insular network in that publication, showed the strongest overlap with this network (multiple regression value: 0.28). This reference component for the ACC/insular network also partially matched a second component (IC 26) in our dataset covering bilateral posterior insula infringing toward putamen, and IFG in addition to the ACC (multiple regression value: 0.08). IC 11 encompassing the bilateral striatum, insula, and thalamus, showed strong overlap with the reference component 21 from (Allen et al., 2011) (multiple regression value: 0.71). Reference component 21 from (Allen et al., 2011) is termed basal ganglia network in that publication with activation focused in the putamen and pallidum (Laird et al., 2011; Robinson et al., 2009). IC 27 involving the bilateral temporal‐parietal junction (TPJ) including the supramarginal and superior temporal cortex and precuneus, showed strong similarity to the reference component 71 focused at the TPJ (Allen et al., 2011; multiple regression value: 0.55), as well as the reference component 72 centered in the central and anterior precuneus (Allen et al., 2011; multiple regression value: 0.14). IC 2 includes the ACC (small dorsal, more ventral, i.e., pregenual and subgenual ACC) and the adjacent ventromedial PFC, IFG, as well as the ventral sections of the bilateral caudate (Table 2). This IC showed strong overlap with the anterior default mode network reference component from (Allen et al., 2011) (i.e., component 25; multiple regression value: 0.38). It resembles a network termed self‐referential network in (D'Argembeau et al., 2005, Mantini et al., 2007). IC 3 is composed of the bilateral amygdala, parahippocampal gyri and midbrain. IC 22 also involves the bilateral hippocampi and parahippocampal gyri as well as the thalamus, midbrain and brainstem. IC 24 contains the bilateral thalamus, and brainstem. ICs 9 and 12 cover the bilateral cerebellum. The latter ICs did not strongly match any of the reference component from (Allen et al., 2011) since no cerebellar/brainstem components were included in the list of 28 reference components.
Network modulations during the NO‐PREFILL WARM condition
Accounting for the largest positive β‐weight during both “infusion” and “filled” phases of the NO‐PREFILL WARM condition was IC 30 (Fig. 3, Table 3). IC 26 was the second most up‐regulated network during both “infusion” and “filled” after IC 30. During the “drainage,” IC 30 still showed large positive up‐regulation, but its correlation with the drainage task was weaker than IC 26. Additionally, ICs 35 and 22 showed significant positive correlation during the “drainage.” The other highly‐rank networks include ICs 31 and 27. These networks both displayed positive up‐regulation during “infusion” but neither correlates with the “drainage.”
Network modulations during the NO‐PREFILL COLD condition
Similar to the NO‐PREFILL WARM condition, the highest positively task‐modulated network during both “infusion” and “filled” trials was IC 30 (Fig. 4, Table 4). It was closely followed by IC 31, as well as IC 27. IC 26 was the next highly‐correlated network during the “infusion” but it was not significantly modulated during the “filled” phase. In its place, IC 16 showed significant task‐correlation during “infusion” as well as “filled” phases. The other network that was highly active during the “filled” phase is IC 24. During the “drainage,” ICs 26 and 24 showed significant task‐related modulation.
Table 4.
IC | β‐weights: Mean ± SD | One‐sample t‐test: P‐valuea | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
I | F | RF | D | I | F | RF | D | |
IC 30 | 0.57 ± 0.22 | 0.48 ± 0.51 | 0.49 ± 0.46 | 0.12 ± 0.29 | 0.000b | 0.006b | 0.002b | 0.143 |
IC 31 | 0.34 ± 0.27 | 0.43 ± 0.38 | 0.42 ± 0.45 | 0.10 ± 0.25 | 0.001b | 0.002b | 0.006b | 0.191 |
IC 26 | 0.27 ± 0.35 | 0.15 ± 0.57 | 0.38 ± 0.56 | 0.24 ± 0.33 | 0.016b | 0.357 | 0.032b | 0.021b |
IC 27 | 0.23 ± 0.28 | 0.37 ± 0.35 | 1.04 ± 0.45 | 0.01 ± 0.33 | 0.013b | 0.002b | 0.000b | 0.921 |
IC 16 | 0.16 ± 0.21 | 0.18 ± 0.22 | 0.14 ± 0.30 | 0.06 ± 0.11 | 0.015b | 0.012b | 0.125 | 0.101 |
IC 24 | −0.07 ± 0.25 | 0.24 ± 0.26 | 0.65 ± 0.32 | −0.10 ± 0.16 | 0.345 | 0.006b | 0.000b | 0.048b |
One‐sample t‐test was performed to determine whether the β‐weights are statistically different from zero. All P‐values were uncorrected because these β‐weights were not independent between trials for the same IC or between ICs for the same trial.
P < 0.05.
Network modulations during the LOW‐PREFILL WARM condition
Four networks show significant task‐related modulation during different phases of the LOW‐PREFILL WARM condition (Fig. 5, Table 5): ICs 11, 2, 3, and 33. Specifically, IC 11 showed significant up‐regulation during the “infusion,” “drainage” and “emptied” trials. ICs 2 and 3 were both highly correlated with the “filled” and “emptied” trials. IC 33 was active only during the “drainage.”
Table 5.
IC | β‐weights: Mean ± SD | One‐sample t‐test: P‐valuea | ||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
I | F | RF | D | E | RE | I | F | RF | D | E | RE | |
IC 11 | 0.12 ± 0.15 | 0.08 ± 0.30 | 0.06 ± 0.29 | 0.12 ± 0.11 | 0.13 ± 0.14 | 0.09 ± 0.25 | 0.014b | 0.361 | 0.446 | 0.002b | 0.007b | 0.238 |
IC 2 | 0.08 ± 0.14 | 0.20 ± 0.26 | −0.38 ± 0.20 | −0.02 ± 0.20 | 0.17 ± 0.26 | −0.25 ± 0.30 | 0.068 | 0.015b | 0.000b | 0.683 | 0.034b | 0.013b |
IC 3 | 0.05 ± 0.09 | 0.17 ± 0.20 | 0.06 ± 0.22 | 0.05 ± 0.16 | 0.15 ± 0.18 | 0.01 ± 0.14 | 0.057 | 0.009b | 0.345 | 0.297 | 0.013b | 0.722 |
IC 33 | 0.15 ± 0.38 | 0.10 ± 0.53 | −1.28 ± 0.45 | 0.25 ± 0.20 | 0.18 ± 0.56 | −1.24 ± 0.27 | 0.170 | 0.489 | 0.000b | 0.001b | 0.279 | 0.000b |
One‐sample t‐test was performed to determine whether the β‐weights are statistically different from zero.
All P‐values were uncorrected because these β‐weights were not independent between trials for the same IC or between ICs for the same trial.
P < 0.05.
Network modulations during the HIGH‐PREFILL WARM condition
In addition to the four networks that showed significant task‐related modulation during different trials of the LOW‐PREFILL WARM condition, i.e., ICs 11, 2, 3, and 33, three additional networks were significantly up‐regulated during the HIGH‐PREFILL WARM condition (Fig. 6, Table 6). These extra components included ICs 9, 12, and 24. During the “infusion,” ICs 11, 3, and 12 were highly modulated. During the “filled” phase, IC 24 becomes significantly active in addition to ICs 11 and 3. During the “drainage,” ICs 11 and 33 were up‐regulated as well as another cerebellar component, IC 9. ICs 2 and 3 were only up‐regulated network during the “emptied” trials.
Functional Network Connectivity During Visceral Interoception
For each viscerosensory stimulation condition, the top three networks that were highly modulated by its task condition were selected for FNC analysis. Seven task‐relevant networks were thus retained for this purpose: ICs 2, 3, 11, 26, 27, 30, and 33. The maximum lagged correlation between these task‐related networks was computed for each subject and task condition following the procedures described in (Jafri et al., 2008). The correlation was then examined between all pair‐wise combinations where the number of combinations of seven components, taken two at a time resulted in 21 possible combinations. For the correlation pairs, Student's t‐test was conducted at P = 0.05, FDR‐corrected for multiple comparison. In Figure 7, the matrices of mean FNC correlation, and the corresponding t‐test results for each condition are shown in the lower and upper half of matrices, respectively. The black asterisk in each correlation matrix identifies the correlation pairs that survived the t‐test. The time lag pairs are also shown in Figure 8 with arrows representing significant FNC correlation between components (P < 0.05, FDR‐corrected). For demonstration purpose, the lag time between the connected networks is shown by the direction of the arrow. The averaged time lag values for the four viscerosensory stimulation conditions are provided in the Supporting Information Figure 2.
To determine which FNC between network pairs are significantly different among four interoceptive conditions, paired t‐tests were conducted at FDR‐corrected threshold level of P = 0.05. The results are shown in Figure 9A,B, corresponding to significant differences that were found between the NO‐PREFILL WARM and LOW‐PREFILL WARM condition pair, and the NO‐PREFILL WARM and HIGH‐PREFILL WARM condition pair, respectively. Matrices of mean correlation difference and t‐value resulting from paired t‐tests for network pairs are shown in Figure 10. The asterisk indicates those surviving the paired t‐test. The corresponding values of the time lag differences for the network pairs are supplied in the Supporting Information Figure 3.
DISCUSSION
In this study, we used a multivariate approach, namely ICA, to study brain networks (Erhardt et al., 2011a) (i.e., ICA components) modulated by visceral interoception from the UB as a representative internal organ. Results show that the innocuous (non‐painful) temperature stimulation of the UB mucosa activated ICA components encompassing the bilateral insula, ACC, dorsolateral PFC and posterior parietal cortex, thalamus, and TPJ. The distention pressure on the viscera (i.e., UB fullness), on the other hand, engaged the bilateral striatum, insula, ACC, ventromedial PFC, the amygdalo‐hippocampi and parahippocampal gyri, thalamus, the posterior midline structures of the precuneus/posterior cingulate cortex and the bilateral inferior parietal lobules, and cerebellar components. Significant differences in FNC (P < 0.05, FDR‐corrected) were found between the insula, ACC, and amygdalo‐hippocampus, between the insula, ACC, and ventromedial PFC, and between the ventromedial PFC and TPJ as the distention pressure on the viscera increased confirming that functional interactions between components changed as a function of viscerosensory condition. In the following we discuss the significance of these new findings.
Network Modulation During Innocuous Temperature Stimulation of Mucosa
In the NO‐PREFILL WARM condition, ICs 26, 30 and 31 and 27 demonstrated increased activity during infusion. In the NO‐PREFILL COLD condition, almost identical components showed increased activity during infusion. COLD filling also significantly activated ICs 16 and 24. The majority of the activated components during the NO‐PREFILL conditions including ICs 26, 16, 30 and 31 involving the insula, ACC, and dorsolateral PFC are proposed to be responsible for detection of salient targets and task‐appropriate attentional focus (Menon, 2011). ICs 16 and 26 anchored by the bilateral insula and ACC, have been shown by several neuroimaging studies to play a crucial role in (a) attending to homeostatically relevant stimuli (Brooks and Tracey, 2005; Craig, 2003b; Critchley et al., 2000), (b) facilitating interactions of other large‐scale intrinsic networks involved in externally oriented attention and internally oriented cognition (Menon and Uddin, 2010; Mutschler et al., 2009), (c) integrating interoceptive and exteroceptive signals (Damasio et al., 2000; Seth et al., 2011), and (d) engendering subjective feeling states (Craig, 2009; Critchley et al., 2004; Harrison et al., 2010; Paulus and Stein, 2006). A recent meta‐analytic clustering of data obtained from the BrainMap database revealed that insula can be parcellated to the anterior, and the posterior clusters (Cauda et al., 2012). The anterior cluster, which is characterized by an attentional pattern of connectivity with frontal, cingulate, parietal, cerebellar areas, is more homogeneous in comparison to the posterior cluster that is characterized by a more local connectivity pattern with connections to sensorimotor, temporal and posterior cingulate regions (Cauda et al., 2012). This finding supports the view that the network to which the anterior insula belongs is related to saliency detection. Similar meta‐analytic clustering of cingulate cortex showed extensive co‐activations within the cingulate cortex and the insula further supporting the view that the cingulate cortex and the insula form a saliency network devoted to the integration of internal (e.g., homeostatic) and the external (e.g., sensory) information (Torta et al., 2013). According to the interoceptive model (Craig, 2003a, 2009, 2002, 1994), different sections of the insula are involved in distinctive and sequential neural processing of interoception from sensing the homeostatic condition of the body to the integration of motivational, hedonic, and social conditions (Herbert and Pollatos, 2012). It is proposed that the raw interoceptive signals such as those coming from viscerosensory stimulations and pain, are first mapped into a first‐order mapping structure in posterior insula (i.e., primary interoceptive cortex) with additional regions—the so‐called second‐order regions such as the anterior insula, the ACC and the PFC become progressively involved (Craig, 2003a, 2009, 2002, 1994) as subjective awareness of feeling (e.g., desire to void), hedonic information, contextual motivational, and volitional control of related actions (e.g., voiding) emerges (Bechara and Naqvi, 2004; Damasio, 2000). The present ICA results dovetail with this model. In the NO‐PREFILL WARM condition, the visceral afferents mapped the UB filling state to the posterior insula (the primary interoceptive cortex in IC 26) but did not ascend further to the anterior section of insula (i.e., no activation of IC 16) as the accumulated volume in the UB did not surpass the threshold of the so‐called “first desire to void” (Abrams et al., 2002). This corresponded well with the fact that subjects could not discern the UB fullness after the NO‐PREFILL WARM filling. Accordingly, the interoceptive awareness measured by online desire to void rating was insignificant. In contrast, in the NO‐PREFILL COLD condition, the visceral afferent signals ascended to the posterior insula in IC 26 then further to the anterior insula in IC 16 (the second‐order region) resulting in subjective awareness of visceral sensation implied by enhanced desire to void ratings. The information about the interoceptive state also projects to the ACC (Craig, 2003a, 2009, 2002, 1994). Although it is plausible to consider co‐activation of the ACC in ICs 26 and 16 for conflict monitoring and focusing attention (Botvinick et al., 2004; Carter et al., 1999), in the context of our paradigm ACC activity is more consistent with the involvement of the ACC in arousal and initiating autonomic responses related to motivation and goal‐directed behaviors (Craig, 2003a; Critchley et al., 2003). The ACC forms part of the limbic system and has also been shown to play a role in generating an affective response to a stimulus. In addition, the ACC has connections with the motor cortex thus can be involved in selecting appropriate behavioral responses to a stimulus (Devinsky et al., 1995). In previous UB studies, the ACC was implicated in the micturition control including pelvic floor contractions (Blok et al., 1997; Griffiths et al., 2007; Herms et al., 2006; Mehnert et al., 2008; Seseke et al., 2006), and the PFC was considered to be responsible for planning complex social behaviors, such as whether or not micturition should take place (Kavia et al., 2005). Taken together, the posterior insular activation in IC 26 indicates unconscious monitoring of afferent signals from the viscera, while upregulation of the anterior insula in IC 16 signifies interoceptive awareness of the visceral sensation with autonomic arousal. Such awareness affords detection of changes in viscera when significant deviations from the homeostatic norm occur and accompanies an incentive motivation (urge/desire) to initiate an action (e.g., voiding) to preserve the body.
Furthermore, changes in the viscera tend to draw attention involuntarily (i.e., bottom‐up) as the majority of interoceptive, visceral signals are subliminal. This explains activation of IC 27 covering the bilateral TPJ in the NO‐PREFILL conditions as TPJ often shows up‐regulation during stimulus‐triggered bottom‐up attentional orientation (Asplund et al., 2010; Corbetta and Shulman, 2002; Fox et al., 2006). Previous neuroimaging studies showed that TPJ is activated in respond to salient stimuli regardless of sensory modality (Downar et al., 2000; Downar et al., 2002). Furthermore, stimulus‐triggered activations in the TPJ usually occur in tandem with the insula (Corbetta et al., 2008), which was also observed in our study. Although the conventional temporal‐parietal component is right‐lateralized, our ICA revealed a bilateral temporal‐parietal activation with slightly more pronounced activation on the left. Recent studies indicate that the left temporal‐parietal activation may well accompany the right temporal‐parietal during stimulus‐driven attention (Asplund et al., 2010). It is also suggested that left‐lateralization of the temporal‐parietal network may be due to contextual relevance of sensory stimuli (DiQuattro and Geng, 2011). Similarly, the fronto‐parietal cortical networks (ICs 30 and 31) are implicated in processing the salient stimuli to facilitate efficient performance of tasks on hand (Seeley et al., 2007). More specifically, the dorsolateral PFC in ICs 30 and 31 is considered to be responsible for cognitive functions such as evaluation and attention (Dias et al., 1996; Frith and Dolan, 1996) involved in working memory and complex attention tasks (Frith and Dolan, 1996) as well as anticipation of and attention to visceral sensations and pain (Aziz et al., 2000; Peyron et al., 2000). The fact that the left fronto‐parietal component (IC 30) was more active than the right fronto‐parietal component (IC 31) in our experiment is intriguing but consistent with left‐lateralized activation of the dorsolateral PFC observed during the visceral esophageal sensation indicating possible functional differences between the left and right dorsolateral PFC (Aziz et al., 2000; Duncan and Owen, 2000).
The transient relay of visceral afferents in thalamus on their way to the insula was also captured by significant activation of the IC 24 during the COLD filling. Activation of the posterior insula during “drainage” phase in both NO‐PREFILL WARM and COLD conditions implies the afferent fiber response to the UB emptying and consequent (re‐)mapping of current visceral state. In the NO‐PREFILL WARM, ICs 35 and 22 also showed up‐regulation during “drainage.” The activity of IC 35, which involves the default‐mode network, can be related to previous findings that show stimulus‐induced activity during tasks with low cognitive load (Fox et al., 2005; Gilbert et al., 2006). Alternatively, IC 35 may be recruited for introspective interrogation such as autobiographical memory, and self‐monitoring (Spreng et al., 2009; Uddin et al., 2009). Up‐regulation of IC 22 during NO‐PREFILL WARM drainage can be interpreted as activation of the IFG and limbic association areas including the bilateral hippocampi and parahippocampal gyri as well as the thalamus, midbrain and brainstem. Previous studies showed such network of brain regions in particular involving the limbic association areas is strongly associated with interoceptive processing elicited during air‐hunger, olfactory and gustatory responses (Laird et al., 2011). The midbrain/brainstem activation observed as part of this component can be related to the midbrain PAG (the mesencephalic homeostatic motor center), which relays information to the pons in brainstem and back to the sacral parasympathetic nerves during the UB emptying (Athwal et al., 2001; Blok and Holstege, 1994; Michels et al., in press). This is also consistent with previous neuroimaging studies of the UB stimulation where activation of the right IFG (Athwal et al., 2001; Blok et al., 1998; Griffiths et al., 2007; Mehnert et al., 2008), thalamus (Griffiths et al., 2005; Kuhtz‐Buschbeck et al., 2005, 2009, 2009; Matsuura et al., 2002; Seseke et al., 2006), PAG (Athwal et al., 2001; Blok et al., 1997, 1998; Griffiths et al., 2005; Kuhtz‐Buschbeck et al., 2005, 2009; Matsuura et al., 2002; Michels et al., in press; Seseke et al., 2006), and pons (Athwal et al., 2001; Blok et al., 1997, 1998 Griffiths et al., 2005; Michels et al., in press; Seseke et al., 2006) were observed.
Network Modulation During Distention Pressure on the Viscera
During the LOW‐ and HIGH‐PREFILL WARM conditions, ICs 2, 3, 11, and 33 showed activity increases. In addition to the said components, during the HIGH‐PREFILL WARM condition, modulation of ICs 9, 12, and 24 were also observed. At the LOW‐ to HIGH‐PREFILL relative to NO‐PREFILL WARM condition, participants were already aware of their UB fullness, thus eradicating a need for bottom‐up deployment of attentional resources (i.e., no activation of IC 27 involving TPJ). Instead, the task condition imposed modulations of component involved in top‐down control of the visceral sensation (e.g., withholding of the urine) as well as the viscerosensory alliesthesia. Alliesthesia (from allios, changed, and esthesia, sensation) refers to the modulation of hedonic aspects of a stimulus by the homeostatic (internal) state of the body (Cabanac, 1971). In the case of the UB, the viscerosensory alliesthesia is the feeling of unpleasantness a subject experiences as distention pressure on the viscera increases. This affective appraisal of viscerosensory alliesthesia recruits IC 2 involving the ventral ACC and the adjacent ventromedial PFC and IFG, IC 3 comprising bilateral amygdala, parahippocampa gyri and midbrain, and IC 11 with bilateral striatum, insula, and thalamus. Elevated activation in limbic regions, including the striatum, amygdala, ACC and medial PFC can be also associated with increased unpleasantness with increased desire to void in response to increased distention pressure on the viscera. Cognitive inhibition, however, can postpone voiding despite increased urge.
The PFC is defined as that part of the frontal lobe that lies anteriorly to the premotor areas (BA 9, 10, 46) that are involved in the planning of complex motor actions, and the primary motor regions, which mediate conscious movement. Anatomically, the ventral PFC can be divided into orbital and medial sections. The more lateral parts of the PFC are involved in aspects of cognition, especially working memory (Bechara et al., 2000), while the ventromedial regions seem to be involved in decision‐making especially in an emotional and social context (Damasio et al., 1996b). The role of the frontal lobes in control of micturition has been previously investigated (for a review of the micturition switch and its forebrain influences see (Griffiths and Fowler, 2013)). In our earlier study in healthy subjects performing pelvic floor contractions with empty and full UB, we showed that medial PFC activity is involved in the control of micturition, while limbic system was likely involved in inhibition of the voiding control mechanism (Zhang et al., 2005). In animal lesion as well as neuroimaging studies, forebrain structures including the PFC and the ACC were found to play a modulatory role in the firing of the voiding reflex (Andrew and Nathan, 1964; Athwal et al., 2001; Blok et al., 1998; Kuhtz‐Buschbeck et al., 2005; Maurice‐Williams, 1974; Michels et al., in press). Similar observations have been reported in patients with spinal cord lesions (Zempleni et al., 2010), PFC underperfusion (Griffiths et al., 1994), and stroke (Sakakibara et al., 1996). In fact, the significance of the PFC in the continence and healthy UB function was initially established in clinical studies by Ueki (1960), and Andrew and Nathan (1964). The latter researchers showed that lesions in the white matter tracts located in the medial PFC caused lasting urinary tract dysfunction while lesions in the gray‐matters in medial PFC led to transient dysfunction (relatively short‐term incontinence) (de Groat et al., 2015; Fowler and Griffiths, 2010). These data indicate that the medial PFC has a pivotal role in the top‐down control of the visceral sensation although the foci of PFC activation are not consistent among studies.
Furthermore, meta‐analysis of neuroimaging studies implies involvement of brain regions encompassed in IC 2 (in particular the ventromedial PFC) in the top‐down modulation between sensory, self‐referential, and higher‐order processing (Northoff et al., 2006). Neuroanatomical studies have revealed reciprocal connections not only between the ventromedial PFC and PAG, which mediate the visceral autonomic activity, but also between the ventromedial PFC and limbic regions, including the ventral striatum and amygdala (Saper, 2002). During the storage and micturition phases, IFG, part of the PFC, has been seen to be active (Athwal et al., 2001; Blok et al., 1998; Griffiths et al., 2007; Mehnert et al., 2008), as well as the orbitofrontal cortex (Mehnert et al., 2008). Previous studies showed activation in the orbitofrontal cortex is related to affective decision‐making, and reward valuation in hedonic experiences (Kringelbach and Rolls, 2004). Dopamine is crucially involved in reward valuation as well as the reinforcement learning and habit development (Baldo and Kelley, 2007; Wise, 2004). The brain's reward system synthesizes both the bottom‐up and top‐down inputs from the ventral striatum (IC 11), the ventromedial PFC and the ACC (IC 2) to create an integrated view of the subjective value of a stimulus. As such, IC 2 activation observed in our study may reflect not only the cortical control of the visceral autonomic system (via hypothalamus and the PAG), but more importantly the affective appraisal of increased visceral pressure (via striatum, IC 11, and amygdala, IC 3). An intermediate step in this operation is the production of “somatic markers,” which signals the intensity (salience) of the valence (negative value) of the stimulus (visceral pressure) experienced by subjects (Damasio, 2000). Together with ICs 2 and 11, this “fronto‐insula‐limbic‐striatal” system helps to identify the emotional salience of interoceptive stimuli, regulate the viscera‐affective state, and motivate action to retain psychophysical homeostasis. Activation of this system in our study is in line with the James–Lange theory of emotion (James, 1994; Lange, 1922) which underlies the role of physiologic and cognitive responses in the formation of emotion (Bechara, 2004; Damasio et al., 1996a). Critical to our survival as a species is the acquisition of the social cognition, a complex phenomenon that entails awareness of one's self as well as the perception of others [i.e., linking “I do and I feel” with “she does and she feels” (Gallese et al., 2004)]. For example, loss of the UB control and thus voiding or leakage is socially undesirable and highly embarrassing for both the sufferer and those who witness it; thus planning and assessment of the appropriateness of when to void are deeply linked not only to our ability to sense and perceive the physiological condition of our body, but also to the social appropriateness of action (Griffiths and Fowler, 2013). Such unique human faculty (i.e., social cognition) is instantiated in the medial PFC (Amodio and Frith, 2006).
The absence of direct activation in the posterior insula in IC 26, and the anterior insula in IC 16, key areas in salience detection was perhaps surprising. However, absence of insular activation during the UB stimulation has also been noted in other studies with high‐level artificial UB filling (Komesu et al., 2011; Krhut et al., 2014). The insula is most activated during changing salience (such as rapid change in distention pressure) and its activity is reported in the “early” evaluation of the significance of sensory and affective stimuli presented (Dutta et al., 2014). However, in the LOW‐ and HIGH‐PREFILL WARM conditions compared to the NO‐PREFILL condition, the prefilled UB levels might have prevented detection of significant changes in salience. We also find Komesu et al. (2011) argument for the lack of insular activation due to habituation caused by repetitive stimulation of the UB plausible. IC 11 showed activity increases during almost all phases of LOW‐ and HIGH‐PREFILL WARM tasks. A closer look at ICs 11, 16, and 26 reveals considerable overlap of these components at insula (Supporting Information Figure 4). Note that the spatial ICA algorithm attempts to identify maximally independent sets of spatial maps (which can overlap) each of which are represented by a strongly coherent or correlated time course (Cetin et al., 2014). Component overlap has been observed in previous ICA studies and has been interpreted as evidence of multiple concurrent central processes associated with common brain regions (Beldzik et al., 2013; Xu et al., 2013).
In our previous fMRI investigation of the supraspinal central representation of the UB stimulation (Mehnert et al., 2008; Mehnert et al., 2011), similar to the current study, one of the most prominent activated brain areas during the UB cooling was the insula but with prominent right‐lateralized activation in the right‐handed subjects. In this study, we also observed slightly prominent right‐sided activity in insula (both ICs 16 and 26) as well as in IC 3. Previous neuroanatomical studies demonstrated that small‐diameter sympathetic afferent fibers provide input to lamina I while small‐diameter parasympathetic afferents project to the nucleus of the solitary tract in medulla (Craig, 2002). However, the parasympathetic activity is postulated to be re‐represented in the left (dominant) hemisphere, whereas the sympathetic activity is assumed to be re‐represented in the right (non‐dominant) hemisphere (Craig, 2002). The right‐lateralization of the insular components that we observed in relation to the visceral stimulation of the UB might be related to the greater activation of the sympathetic afferent fibers compared to their parasympathetic counterparts. Considering the autonomic regulation of the UB, the sympathetic activity causes the internal urethral sphincter to close allowing the UB to fill. At the same time, moderate UB distension inhibits parasympathetic activity, which would otherwise contract the UB to start the voiding process (de Groat and Steers, 1990). Therefore, during visceral (thermal and mechanical) stimulation of the UB, the sympathetic activity seems to outweigh the parasympathetic activity in order to allow urine accumulation (UB muscle relaxation), and prevent voiding (and UB contraction). These re‐representations of both sympathetic and parasympathetic afferents are postulated to provide the foundation for a subjective evaluation of interoceptive state, which is forwarded to the PFC especially the ventromedial and orbitofrontal cortices where hedonic valence is represented (Craig, 2002). The lateralization of the component activations can also be related to the hemispheric dominance in the supraspinal processing of the UB sensations. Such hemispheric asymmetry has been previously reported not only for UB sensations (Blok et al., 1997; Mehnert et al., 2008, 2011) but also other visceral functions such as the ano‐rectal motor function (Hamdy et al., 1999). The significance of the lateralized cortical representations of visceral sensations is still unknown.
The HIGH‐PREFILL WARM paradigm in our study also revealed significant cerebellar activation, namely in ICs 9 and 12. Previous studies showed cerebellar activation following increasing UB filling (Athwal et al., 2001; Matsuura et al., 2002; Nour et al., 2000). Furthermore, cerebellar lesions can disrupt both storage and expulsion (Nishizawa et al., 1989). Thus, the cerebellum seems to be involved in processing sensory information of UB filling and motor control of micturition.
Functional Network Connectivity During Visceral Interoception
We performed FNC using maximal correlation on back‐reconstructed subject‐specific time courses of seven task‐modulated networks. FNC, a measure of inter‐network connectivity, is weaker than intranetwork connectivity (Arbabshirani et al., 2013). The average ± standard deviation of FNC values were 0.154 ± 0.111, 0.141 ± 0.103, 0.161 ± 0.120, and 0.161 ± 0.112 for the NO‐PREFILL WARM, NO‐PREFILL COLD, LOW‐PREFILL WARM, and HIGH‐PREFILL WARM conditions, respectively. As shown in Figure 7, most of the correlations are positive. Across different conditions, the FNC correlation between networks was the weakest between IC 33 involving posterior parts of the default mode network, namely the posterior midline structures of the precuneus/posterior cingulate cortex and the bilateral inferior parietal lobules (Raichle et al., 2001; Shulman et al., 1997) and IC 27 centered on the bilateral TPJ. The correlations value for this network pair was negative at all times, i.e., −0.05, −0.04, −0.12 and −0.16 for the NO‐PREFILL WARM, NO‐PREFILL COLD, LOW‐PREFILL WARM, and HIGH‐PREFILL WARM conditions, respectively. The anticorrelation between the TPJ in IC 27 and the regions comprising the posterior midline structures in IC 33 is consistent with previous finding (Kucyi et al., 2012). Across all conditions, the strongest FNC correlation was always between IC 2 encompassing the ventromedial PFC, and IC 3 involving the bilateral amygdala, and parahippocampa gyri, i.e., 0.33, 0.28, 0.32, and 0.33 for the NO‐PREFILL WARM, NO‐PREFILL COLD, LOW‐PREFILL WARM, and HIGH‐PREFILL WARM conditions, respectively.
The present FNC demonstrated that the connectivity of IC 33 covering the more posterior default mode component with other ICA components significantly (P < 0.05, FDR‐corrected) differed during the visceral stimulation. In the NO‐PREFILL conditions, there was no FNC between IC 33 and IC 26 involving the posterior insula, or between IC 33 and IC 27 focused at the TPJ. In the HIGH‐PREFILL WARM, the FNC between ICs 33 and IC 30, the left fronto‐parietal network, was also not significant. Much of the regions in ICs 26, 30, and 27 that showed non‐significant FNC with IC 33, overlap with the so‐called task‐positive network including the dorsolateral and ventral PFC, insula, and attention networks (Fox et al., 2005). Uddin et al. (2009) also showed that posterior midline structures of the precuneus/posterior cingulate cortex has an antagonistic relationship with prefrontal‐based motor planning and control circuits. In addition to the above network pairs, the FNC between ICs 2 and 27 was not significant for all conditions (P < 0.05, FDR‐corrected). The main node in IC 2 is the ventromedial PFC, which resembles the anterior node of the default mode network. The main node of IC 27 is the TPJ (more left‐lateralized here) which is suggested to be involved in integrating contextual knowledge with incoming sensory information through interactions with the insula and the frontopariental networks (DiQuattro and Geng, 2011). Kucyi et al. (2012) showed previously that both the right and left TPJ exhibit negative functional connectivity with the default mode network (e.g., medial PFC, precuneus/posterior cingulate cortex). Consequently, the lack of FNC between ICs 2 and 27 can also be interpreted as the lack of correlation between the task‐positive and task‐negative networks.
An intriguing finding of our study is that compared to the NO‐PREFILL WARM condition, both LOW‐ and HIGH‐PREFILL WARM conditions showed significantly increased FNC between ICs 11 and 3 (P < 0.05, FDR‐corrected); i.e., 0.14 ± 0.07 for the NO‐PREFILL WARM condition compared to 0.22 ± 0.09, 0.25 ± 0.08 for the LOW‐ and HIGH‐PREFILL WARM conditions, respectively. The enhanced FNC between these two ICA components in the LOW‐ and HIGH‐PREFILL WARM conditions may represent an increased crosstalk between these two networks once the awareness of the visceral stimulation is achieved. Moreover, FNC comparison between the NO‐PREFILL WARM and LOW‐PREFILL WARM conditions revealed there are significant difference in FNC between (i) ICs 2 and 27, (ii) ICs 2 and 11, and (iii) ICs 33 and 26 (P < 0.05, FDR‐corrected). A major behavioral distinction between the NO‐PREIFLL WARM and LOW‐PREFILLE WARM is a shift from non‐conscious visceral sensation to conscious feeling (interoceptive awareness). As discussed earlier, this shift in awareness activates the fronto‐insula‐limbic‐striatal system, which starts a cascade of events including regulation of the autonomic and arousal states as well as motivating action in response to increased viscerosensory alliesthesia with the aim of retaining homeostasis. This reallocation of activity from the salience detection circuitry to affective‐arousal‐motivation networks with the onset of subjective conscious experience echoes well with a large‐scale reorganization of FNC in which the FNC between ICs 2 and 27 significantly decreased (i.e., from 0.12 ± 0.17 to −0.03 ± 0.15, P < 0.05, FDR‐corrected), while the FNC between ICs 2 and 11 were significantly increased (i.e., from 0.14 ± 0.09 to 0.27 ± 0.12, P < 0.05, FDR‐corrected). The significant increase between ICs 33 and 26 (i.e., from 0.03 ± 0.14 to 0.17 ± 0.11, P < 0.05, FDR‐corrected) can also be related to increased crosstalk between the primary interoceptive cortex involved in representing bodily state and the autobiographical activity of the default mode network.
Caveats and Future Direction
There are some important caveats that must be considered in the interpretation of our results. First, we included only female subjects to control for the confounding factor of sex‐related differences in visceral interoception, and better ability to perform non‐painful catheterization. Second, the long acquisition time of the datasets (15–17 min per task condition) has benefited the precision of estimation of FNC correlations. Conversely, longer fMRI time series are more prone to subjects’ distraction, thus less likely to represent a stable brain functional state. Third, although our analysis was based on adequate sampling of imaging data from each subject, there was a modest number of subjects in the final sample. Thus, the findings certainly deserve replication with a larger sample size of both genders using more time‐optimized viscerosensory paradigm. Another potential limitation is the possible contamination of the BOLD signals related to the visceral stimulation with the brain activates related to the rating. Although we explicitly instructed the subjects not to mentally perform the subjective estimation of the rating during the event periods prior to the motor task of moving the manipulandum on the response box (and none of the subjects reported doing so in the post‐scan questionnaires), it is difficult to be sure when subjects start to think about what. On the other hand, this thinking process cannot be completely excluded; after all this is probably an integral part of interoceptive feeling. There is also a possibility of false negatives due to thresholding and smearing of activity from individual participants. Lastly, our experiment was performed with the UB as a representative internal organ, in which the translation of the UB volume and fluid temperature into sensory afferent activity lies at the heart of the interoceptive process. Despite this constraint, we believe our observations can potentially be compared to interoception from other visceral organs such as GI tract as neuroimaging studies of especially lower GI tract showed activations in similar brain areas to the UB (Derbyshire, 2003; Fowler and Griffiths, 2010; Mayer, 2011; Mayer et al., 2009). However, as a strong point of this study, we emphasize that we used ICA as a model free model with no bias in selecting the activation regions. To add to fidelity of our results, we particularly followed the ICA procedure that is suggested to yield more robust and reliable components in the literature [e.g., the GICA‐3 back‐reconstruction algorithm (Erhardt et al., 2011b), intensity normalization in preprocessing (Allen et al., 2011), 100 runs with random initiation in ICASSO (Himberg et al., 2004)].
In conclusion, ICA and FNC analysis revealed that visceral interoception is a dynamic process that involves closely interacting, yet dissociable components. Within this system, the posterior and anterior insula, ACC, dorsolateral PFC and posterior parietal cortex, thalamus, and TPJ provide monitoring of visceral state and salience detection, whereas the ventromedial PFC, IFG, ventral striatum, insula, ACC, amygdala, parahippocampal gyri, midbrain are more likely to be involved in the regulation of the arousal, motivational and affective states and action initiation. Future studies are expected to further detail the role of these intrinsic networks in interoception and its effect on human conscious experience, and emotional states.
Supporting information
ACKNOWLEDGMENT
The authors thank anonymous reviewers for their critical and helpful comments on the earlier draft of this manuscript.
REFERENCES
- Abrams P, Cardozo L, Fall M, Griffiths D, Rosier P, Ulmsten U, van Kerrebroeck P, Victor A, Wein A (2002): The standardisation of terminology of lower urinary tract function: Report from the Standardisation Sub‐committee of the International Continence Society. Am J Obstet Gynecol 187:116–126. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Abrams P, Feneley R, Torrens M (2006): Urodynamics. London: Springer. [Google Scholar]
- Allen EA, Erhardt EB, Damaraju E, Gruner W, Segall JM, Silva RF, Havlicek M, Rachakonda S, Fries J, Kalyanam R (2011): A baseline for the multivariate comparison of resting‐state networks. Front Syst Neurosci 5:1–23. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Amodio DM, Frith CD (2006): Meeting of minds: The medial frontal cortex and social cognition. Nat Rev Neurosci 7:268–277. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andrew J, Nathan P (1964): Lesions of the anterior frontal lobes and disturbances of micturition and defaecation. Brain 87:233–262. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arbabshirani MR, Havlicek M, Kiehl KA, Pearlson GD, Calhoun VD (2013): Functional network connectivity during rest and task conditions: A comparative study. Human Brain Mapping 34:2959–2971. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Asplund CL, Todd JJ, Snyder AP, Marois R (2010): A central role for the lateral prefrontal cortex in goal‐directed and stimulus‐driven attention. Nat Neurosci 13:507–512. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Athwal BS, Berkley KJ, Hussain I, Brennan A, Craggs M, Sakakibara R, Frackowiak RS, Fowler CJ (2001): Brain responses to changes in bladder volume and urge to void in healthy men. Brain 124:369–377. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aziz Q, Thompson D, Ng V, Hamdy S, Sarkar S, Brammer M, Bullmore E, Hobson A, Tracey I, Gregory L (2000): Cortical processing of human somatic and visceral sensation. J Neurosci 20:2657–2663. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baldo BA, Kelley AE (2007): Discrete neurochemical coding of distinguishable motivational processes: Insights from nucleus accumbens control of feeding. Psychopharmacology 191:439–459. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bechara A (2004): The role of emotion in decision‐making: Evidence from neurological patients with orbitofrontal damage. Brain Cognit 55:30–40. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bechara A, Naqvi N (2004): Listening to your heart: Interoceptive awareness as a gateway to feeling. Nat Neurosci 7:102–103. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bechara A, Damasio H, Damasio AR (2000): Emotion, decision making and the orbitofrontal cortex. Cereb Cortex 10:295–307. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beckmann CF, Smith SM (2004): Probabilistic independent component analysis for functional magnetic resonance imaging. IEEE Trans Med Imaging 23:137–152. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beckmann CF, DeLuca M, Devlin JT, Smith SM (2005): Investigations into resting‐state connectivity using independent component analysis. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci 360:1001–1013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beckmann CF, Mackay CE, Filippini N, Smith SM (2009): Group comparison of resting‐state FMRI data using multi‐subject ICA and dual regression. Neuroimage 47:S148 [Google Scholar]
- Beldzik E, Domagalik A, Daselaar S, Fafrowicz M, Froncisz W, Oginska H, Marek T (2013): Contributive sources analysis: A measure of neural networks' contribution to brain activations. Neuroimage 76:304–312. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bell AJ, Sejnowski TJ (1995): An information‐maximization approach to blind separation and blind deconvolution. Neural Comput 7:1129–1159. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Björnsdotter M, Löken L, Olausson H, Vallbo Å, Wessberg J (2009): Somatotopic organization of gentle touch processing in the posterior insular cortex. J Neurosci 29:9314–9320. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blok BF, Holstege G (1994): Direct projections from the periaqueductal gray to the pontine micturition center (M‐region). An anterograde and retrograde tracing study in the cat. Neurosci Lett 166:93–96. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blok B, Willemsen A, Holstege G (1997): A PET study on brain control of micturition in humans. Brain 120:111–121. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blok B, Sturms LM, Holstege G (1998): Brain activation during micturition in women. Brain 121:2033–2042. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blomqvist A, Zhang ET, Craig A (2000): Cytoarchitectonic and immunohistochemical characterization of a specific pain and temperature relay, the posterior portion of the ventral medial nucleus, in the human thalamus. Brain 123:601–619. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bors E, Parker R (1956): Observations on some modalities of bladder sensation. J Urol 76:566–575. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Botvinick MM, Cohen JD, Carter CS (2004): Conflict monitoring and anterior cingulate cortex: An update. Trend Cognit Sci 8:539–546. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bressler SL, Menon V (2010): Large‐scale brain networks in cognition: Emerging methods and principles. Trend Cognit Sci 14:277–290. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brooks J, Tracey I (2005): Review: From nociception to pain perception: Imaging the spinal and supraspinal pathways. J Anat 207:19–33. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brooks J, Zambreanu L, Godinez A, Craig A, Tracey I (2005): Somatotopic organisation of the human insula to painful heat studied with high resolution functional imaging. Neuroimage 27:201–209. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burnstock G (2007): Physiology and pathophysiology of purinergic neurotransmission. Physiol Rev 87:659–797. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cabanac M (1971): Physiological role of pleasure. Science 173:1103–1107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Calhoun V, Adali T, Pearlson G, Pekar J (2001a): A method for making group inferences from functional MRI data using independent component analysis. Hum Brain Mapp 14:140–151. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Calhoun V, Adali T, Pearlson G, Pekar J (2001b): Spatial and temporal independent component analysis of functional MRI data containing a pair of task‐related waveforms. Human Brain Mapp 13:43–53. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Calhoun V, Adalı T, Pearlson G, Van Zijl P, Pekar J (2002a): Independent component analysis of fMRI data in the complex domain. Magn Reson Med 48:180–192. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Calhoun VD, Pekar JJ, McGinty VB, Adali T, Watson TD, Pearlson GD (2002b): Different activation dynamics in multiple neural systems during simulated driving. Hum Brain Mapp 16:158–167. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Calhoun VD, Adalı T, Pekar JJ (2004a): A method for comparing group fMRI data using independent component analysis: Application to visual, motor and visuomotor tasks. Magn Reson Imaging 22:1181–1191. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Calhoun VD, Kiehl KA, Liddle PF, Pearlson GD (2004b): Aberrant localization of synchronous hemodynamic activity in auditory cortex reliably characterizes schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 55:842–849. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Calhoun VD, Pekar JJ, Pearlson GD (2004c): Alcohol intoxication effects on simulated driving: Exploring alcohol‐dose effects on brain activation using functional MRI. Neuropsychopharmacology 29:2097–2107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Calhoun VD, Kiehl KA, Pearlson GD (2008): Modulation of temporally coherent brain networks estimated using ICA at rest and during cognitive tasks. Hum Brain Mapp 29:828–838. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cameron OG (2001): Interoception: The inside story—a model for psychosomatic processes. Psychosom Med 63:697–710. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cameron OG, Minoshima S (2002): Regional brain activation due to pharmacologically induced adrenergic interoceptive stimulation in humans. Psychosom Med 64:851–861. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cardinal RN, Parkinson JA, Hall J, Everitt BJ (2002): Emotion and motivation: The role of the amygdala, ventral striatum, and prefrontal cortex. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 26:321–352. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carter CS, Botvinick MM, Cohen JD (1999): The contribution of the anterior cingulate cortex to executive processes in cognition. Rev Neurosci 10:49–58. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cauda F, Costa T, Torta DM, Sacco K, D'Agata F, Duca S, Geminiani G, Fox PT, Vercelli A (2012): Meta‐analytic clustering of the insular cortex: Characterizing the meta‐analytic connectivity of the insula when involved in active tasks. Neuroimage 62:343–355. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cetin MS, Christensen F, Abbott CC, Stephen JM, Mayer AR, Cañive JM, Bustillo JR, Pearlson GD, Calhoun VD (2014): Thalamus and posterior temporal lobe show greater inter‐network connectivity at rest and across sensory paradigms in schizophrenia. Neuroimage 97:117–126. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chancellor M, Lavelle J, Ozawa H, Jung S, Watanabe T, Kumon H (1998): Ice‐water test in the urodynamic evaluation of spinal cord injured patients. Tech Urol 4:87–91. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Corbetta M, Shulman GL (2002): Control of goal‐directed and stimulus‐driven attention in the brain. Nat Rev Neurosci 3:201–215. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Corbetta M, Patel G, Shulman GL (2008): The reorienting system of the human brain: From environment to theory of mind. Neuron 58:306–324. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cordes D, Haughton VM, Arfanakis K, Wendt GJ, Turski PA, Moritz CH, Quigley MA, Meyerand ME (2000): Mapping functionally related regions of brain with functional connectivity MR imaging. Am J Neuroradiol 21:1636–1644. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cordes D, Haughton V, Carew JD, Arfanakis K, Maravilla K (2002): Hierarchical clustering to measure connectivity in fMRI resting‐state data. Magn Reson Imaging 20:305–317. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Craig A (2003a): Interoception: The sense of the physiological condition of the body. Curr Opin Neurobiol 13:500–505. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Craig A (2003b): A new view of pain as a homeostatic emotion. Trend Neurosci 26:303–307. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Craig A (2008): Interoception and emotion: A neuroanatomical perspective In: Lewis M, Haviland‐Jones JM, Feldman Barrett L, editors. Handbook of Emotions. New York: Guilford Press; pp 2722–2788. [Google Scholar]
- Craig A (2009): How do you feel—Now? the anterior insula and human awareness. Nat Rev Neurosci 10:59–70. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Craig A (2014): Topographically organized projection to posterior insular cortex from the posterior portion of the ventral medial nucleus in the long‐tailed macaque monkey. J Comp Neurol 522:36–63. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Craig AD (2002): How do you feel? Interoception: The sense of the physiological condition of the body. Nat Rev Neurosci 3:655–666. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Craig A, Bushnell M, Zhang ET, Blomqvist A (1994): A thalamic nucleus specific for pain and temperature sensation. Nature 372:770–773. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Craig AD, Chen K, Bandy D, Reiman EM (2000): Thermosensory activation of insular cortex. Nat Neurosci 3:184–190. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Critchley HD (2005): Neural mechanisms of autonomic, affective, and cognitive integration. J Comp Neurol 493:154–166. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Critchley H, Corfield D, Chandler M, Mathias C, Dolan R (2000): Cerebral correlates of autonomic cardiovascular arousal: A functional neuroimaging investigation in humans. J Physiol 523:259–270. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Critchley HD, Mathias CJ, Josephs O, O'Doherty J, Zanini S, Dewar BK, Cipolotti L, Shallice T, Dolan RJ (2003): Human cingulate cortex and autonomic control: Converging neuroimaging and clinical evidence. Brain 126:2139–2152. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Critchley HD, Wiens S, Rotshtein P, Öhman A, Dolan RJ (2004): Neural systems supporting interoceptive awareness. Nat Neurosci 7:189–195. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- D'Argembeau A, Collette F, Van der Linden M, Laureys S, Del Fiore G, Degueldre C, Luxen A, Salmon E (2005): Self‐referential reflective activity and its relationship with rest: A PET study. Neuroimage 25:616–624. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Damasio AR (2000): The Feeling of What Happens: Body, Emotion and the Making of Consciousness. New York: Harcourt Brace & Company. [Google Scholar]
- Damasio AR (2004): Looking for Spinoza: Joy, sorrow, and the feeling brain. New York, NY: Harvest. [Google Scholar]
- Damasio A (2008): Descartes' Error: Emotion, Reason and the Human Brain. New York, NY: Random House. [Google Scholar]
- Damasio AR, Everitt B, Bishop D (1996a): The somatic marker hypothesis and the possible functions of the prefrontal cortex [and discussion]. Philos Trans R Soc Lond Ser B: Biol Sci 351:1413–1420. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Damasio AR, Everitt B, Bishop D (1996b): The somatic marker hypothesis and the possible functions of the prefrontal cortex [and discussion]. Philos Trans R Soc B: Biol Sci 351:1413–1420. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Damasio AR, Grabowski TJ, Bechara A, Damasio H, Ponto LL, Parvizi J, Hichwa RD (2000): Subcortical and cortical brain activity during the feeling of self‐generated emotions. Nat Neurosci 3:1049–1056. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Damoiseaux J, Rombouts S, Barkhof F, Scheltens P, Stam C, Smith SM, Beckmann C (2006): Consistent resting‐state networks across healthy subjects. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 103:13848–13853. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Groat W, Steers W (1990): Autonomic regulation of the urinary bladder and sexual organs In: Loewy AD, Spyer KM, editor. Central Regulation of Autonomic Functions. New York: Oxford University Press; pp 310–333. [Google Scholar]
- de Groat WC, Griffiths D, Yoshimura N (2015): Neural control of the lower urinary tract. Comprehensive Physiol 5:327–396. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Luca M, Beckmann C, De Stefano N, Matthews P, Smith SM (2006): fMRI resting state networks define distinct modes of long‐distance interactions in the human brain. Neuroimage 29:1359–1367. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Denton D, Shade R, Zamarippa F, Egan G, Blair‐West J, McKinley M, Lancaster J, Fox P (1999): Neuroimaging of genesis and satiation of thirst and an interoceptor‐driven theory of origins of primary consciousness. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 96:5304–5309. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Denton D, McKinley M, Farrell M, Egan G (2009): The role of primordial emotions in the evolutionary origin of consciousness. Conscious Cogn 18:500–514. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Derbyshire SW (2003): A systematic review of neuroimaging data during visceral stimulation. Am J Gastroenterol 98:12–20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Devinsky O, Morrell MJ, Vogt BA (1995): Contributions of anterior cingulate cortex to behaviour. Brain 118:279–306. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dias R, Robbins T, Roberts A (1996): Dissociation in prefrontal cortex of affective and attentional shifts. Nature 380:69–72. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- DiQuattro NE, Geng JJ (2011): Contextual knowledge configures attentional control networks. J Neurosci 31:18026–18035. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Downar J, Crawley AP, Mikulis DJ, Davis KD (2000): A multimodal cortical network for the detection of changes in the sensory environment. Nat Neurosci 3:277–283. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Downar J, Crawley AP, Mikulis DJ, Davis KD (2002): A cortical network sensitive to stimulus salience in a neutral behavioral context across multiple sensory modalities. J Neurophysiol 87:615–620. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Duncan J, Owen AM (2000): Common regions of the human frontal lobe recruited by diverse cognitive demands. Trend Neurosci 23:475–483. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dutta A, McKie S, Deakin JW (2014): Resting state networks in major depressive disorder. Psychiatry Res 224:139–151. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Egan G, Silk T, Zamarripa F, Williams J, Federico P, Cunnington R, Carabott L, Blair‐West J, Shade R, McKinley M (2003): Neural correlates of the emergence of consciousness of thirst. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 100:15241–15246. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eickhoff SB, Stephan KE, Mohlberg H, Grefkes C, Fink GR, Amunts K, Zilles K (2005): A new SPM toolbox for combining probabilistic cytoarchitectonic maps and functional imaging data. Neuroimage 25:1325–1335. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Erhardt EB, Allen EA, Damaraju E, Calhoun VD (2011a): On network derivation, classification, and visualization: A response to Habeck and Moeller. Brain Connect 1:105–110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Erhardt EB, Rachakonda S, Bedrick EJ, Allen EA, Adali T, Calhoun VD (2011b): Comparison of multi‐subject ICA methods for analysis of fMRI data. Human Brain Mapp 32:2075–2095. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Evrard HC, Logothetis NK (2014): Modular architectonic organization of the insula in the macaque monkey. J Comp Neurol 522:64–97. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fall M, Lindström S, Mazières L (1990): A bladder‐to‐bladder cooling reflex in the cat. J Physiol 427:281–300. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fowler CJ, Griffiths D, de Groat WC (2008): The neural control of micturition. Nat Rev Neurosci 9:453–466. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fowler CJ, Griffiths DJ (2010): A decade of functional brain imaging applied to bladder control. Neurourol Urodyn 29:49–55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fox MD, Snyder AZ, Vincent JL, Corbetta M, Van Essen DC, Raichle ME (2005): The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 102:9673–9678. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fox MD, Corbetta M, Snyder AZ, Vincent JL, Raichle ME (2006): Spontaneous neuronal activity distinguishes human dorsal and ventral attention systems. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 103:10046–10051. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Friston K (2002): Beyond phrenology: What can neuroimaging tell us about distributed circuitry? Ann Rev Neurosci 25:221–250. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Friston KJ, Holmes AP, Worsley KJ, Poline JP, Frith CD, Frackowiak RS (1994): Statistical parametric maps in functional imaging: A general linear approach. Human Brain Mapping 2:189–210. [Google Scholar]
- Friston K, Ashburner J, Frith CD, Poline JB, Heather JD, Frackowiak RS (1995): Spatial registration and normalization of images. Human Brain Mapp 3:165–189. [Google Scholar]
- Frith C, Dolan R (1996): The role of the prefrontal cortex in higher cognitive functions. Cognit Brain Res 5:175–181. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fukunaga M, Horovitz SG, van Gelderen P, de Zwart JA, Jansma JM, Ikonomidou VN, Chu R, Deckers RH, Leopold DA, Duyn JH (2006): Large‐amplitude, spatially correlated fluctuations in BOLD fMRI signals during extended rest and early sleep stages. Magn Reson Imaging 24:979–992. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gallese V, Keysers C, Rizzolatti G (2004): A unifying view of the basis of social cognition. Trend Cognit Sci 8:396–403. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Geirsson G, Fall M, Lindstrom S (1993): The Ice‐water Test—A Simple and Valuable Supplement to Routine Cystometry. Br J Urol 71:681–685. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Genovese CR, Lazar NA, Nichols T (2002): Thresholding of statistical maps in functional neuroimaging using the false discovery rate. Neuroimage 15:870–878. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gilbert SJ, Simons JS, Frith CD, Burgess PW (2006): Performance‐related activity in medial rostral prefrontal cortex (area 10) during low‐demand tasks. J Exp Psychol: Hum Percept Perform 32:45–58. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gregory L, Yágüez L, Williams S, Altmann C, Coen S, Ng V, Brammer M, Thompson D, Aziz Q (2003): Cognitive modulation of the cerebral processing of human oesophageal sensation using functional magnetic resonance imaging. Gut 52:1671–1677. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Greicius MD, Menon V (2004): Default‐mode activity during a passive sensory task: Uncoupled from deactivation but impacting activation. J Cognit Neurosci 16:1484–1492. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Griffiths DJ (2004): Cerebral control of bladder function. Curr Urol Rep 5:348–352. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Griffiths D, Fowler C (2013): The micturition switch and its forebrain influences. Acta Physiol 207:93–109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Griffiths DJ, McCracken PN, Harrison GM, Gormley E, Moore K, Hooper R, McEwan A, Triscott J (1994): Cerebral aetiology of urinary urge incontinence in elderly people. Age Ageing 23:246–250. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Griffiths D, Derbyshire S, Stenger A, Resnick N (2005): Brain control of normal and overactive bladder. J Urol 174:1862–1867. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Griffiths D, Tadic SD, Schaefer W, Resnick NM (2007): Cerebral control of the bladder in normal and urge‐incontinent women. Neuroimage 37:1–7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Häbler H, Jänig W, Koltzenburg M (1990): Activation of unmyelinated afferent fibres by mechanical stimuli and inflammation of the urinary bladder in the cat. J Physiol 425:545–562. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Häbler H, Jänig W, Koltzenburg M (1993): Myelinated primary afferents of the sacral spinal cord responding to slow filling and distension of the cat urinary bladder. J Physiol 463:449–460. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hall G, Kamath M, Collins S, Ganguli S, Spaziani R, Miranda K, Bayati A, Bienenstock J (2010): Heightened central affective response to visceral sensations of pain and discomfort in IBS. Neurogastroenterol Motil 22:276–e80. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hamdy S, Enck P, Aziz Q, Uengoergil S, Hobson A, Thompson D (1999): Laterality effects of human pudendal nerve stimulation on corticoanal pathways: Evidence for functional asymmetry. Gut 45:58–63. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Handwerker DA, Ollinger JM, D'Esposito M (2004): Variation of BOLD hemodynamic responses across subjects and brain regions and their effects on statistical analyses. Neuroimage 21:1639–1651. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harrison NA, Gray MA, Gianaros PJ, Critchley HD (2010): The embodiment of emotional feelings in the brain. J Neurosci 30:12878–12884. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Herbert BM, Pollatos O (2012): The body in the mind: On the relationship between interoception and embodiment. Topics Cognit Sci 4:692–704. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Herbert BM, Muth ER, Pollatos O, Herbert C (2012): Interoception across modalities: On the relationship between cardiac awareness and the sensitivity for gastric functions. PloS One 7:e36646 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Herms ADG, Veit R, Reisenauer C, Herms A, Grodd W, Enck P, Stenzl A, Birbaumer N (2006): Functional imaging of stress urinary incontinence. Neuroimage 29:267–275. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Himberg J, Hyvärinen A, Esposito F (2004): Validating the independent components of neuroimaging time series via clustering and visualization. Neuroimage 22:1214–1222. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hojo M, Takahashi T, Nagahara A, Sasaki H, Oguro M, Asaoka D, Watanabe S (2012): Analysis of brain activity during visceral stimulation. J Gastroenterol Hepatol 27:49–52. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jackson S, Donovan J, Brookes S, Eckford S, Swithinbank L, Abrams P (1996): The Bristol female lower urinary tract symptoms questionnaire: Development and psychometric testing. Br J Urol 77:805–812. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jafri MJ, Pearlson GD, Stevens M, Calhoun VD (2008): A method for functional network connectivity among spatially independent resting‐state components in schizophrenia. Neuroimage 39:1666–1681. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- James W (1994): The physical basis of emotion. Psychol Rev 101:205–210. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jänig W, Morrison J (1986): Functional properties of spinal visceral afferents supplying abdominal and pelvic organs, with special emphasis on visceral nociception. Prog Brain Res 67:87–114. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jarrahi B, Wanek J (2014): Design of an fMRI‐compatible optical touch stripe based on frustrated total internal reflection. Conf Proc IEEE Eng Med Biol Soc 2014:4952–4955. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jarrahi B, Wanek J, Mehnert U, Kollias S (2013): An fMRI‐compatible multi‐configurable handheld response system using an intensity‐modulated fiber‐optic sensor. Conf Proc IEEE Eng Med Biol Soc 2013:6349–6352. p [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kanai A, Andersson KE (2010): Bladder afferent signaling: Recent findings. J Urol 183:1288–1295. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kavia RBC, Dasgupta R, Fowler CJ (2005): Functional imaging and the central control of the bladder. J Comp Neurol 493:27–32. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kiviniemi V, Starck T, Remes J, Long X, Nikkinen J, Haapea M, Veijola J, Moilanen I, Isohanni M, Zang YF (2009): Functional segmentation of the brain cortex using high model order group PICA. Hum Brain Mapp 30:3865–3886. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koch W, Teipel S, Mueller S, Buerger K, Bokde AL, Hampel H, Coates U, Reiser M, Meindl T (2010): Effects of aging on default mode network activity in resting state fMRI: Does the method of analysis matter? Neuroimage, 51:280–287. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Komesu YM, Ketai LH, Mayer AR, Teshiba TM, Rogers RG (2011): Functional MRI of the brain in women with overactive bladder: Brain activation during urinary urgency. Female Pelvic Med Reconstr Surg 17:50–54. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krhut J, Holy P, Tintera J, Zachoval R, Zvara P (2014): Brain activity during bladder filling and pelvic floor muscle contractions: A study using functional magnetic resonance imaging and synchronous urodynamics. Int J Urol 21:169–174. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kringelbach ML, Rolls ET (2004): The functional neuroanatomy of the human orbitofrontal cortex: Evidence from neuroimaging and neuropsychology. Prog Neurobiol 72:341–372. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kucyi A, Hodaie M, Davis KD (2012): Lateralization in intrinsic functional connectivity of the temporoparietal junction with salience‐and attention‐related brain networks. J Neurophysiol 108:3382–3392. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuhtz‐Buschbeck JP, van der HORST C, Pott C, Wolff S, Nabavi A, Jansen O, JÜNemann KP (2005): Cortical representation of the urge to void: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J Urol 174:1477–1481. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuhtz‐Buschbeck J, Gilster R, Van der Horst C, Hamann M, Wolff S, Jansen O (2009): Control of bladder sensations: An fMRI study of brain activity and effective connectivity. Neuroimage 47:18–27. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kurth F, Zilles K, Fox PT, Laird AR, Eickhoff SB (2010): A link between the systems: Functional differentiation and integration within the human insula revealed by meta‐analysis. Brain Struct Funct 214:519–534. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Labus JS, Naliboff BN, Fallon J, Berman SM, Suyenobu B, Bueller JA, Mandelkern M, Mayer EA (2008): Sex differences in brain activity during aversive visceral stimulation and its expectation in patients with chronic abdominal pain: A network analysis. Neuroimage 41:1032–1043. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Laird AR, Fox PM, Eickhoff SB, Turner JA, Ray KL, McKay DR, Glahn DC, Beckmann CF, Smith SM, Fox PT (2011): Behavioral interpretations of intrinsic connectivity networks. J Cognit Neurosci 23:4022–4037. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lange CG (1922): The Emotions. Baltimore, ML: Williams & Wilkins. [Google Scholar]
- LeDoux JE (2002): Synaptic Self: How Our Brains Become Who We Are. New York: Viking. [Google Scholar]
- Lerner A, Bagic A, Hanakawa T, Boudreau EA, Pagan F, Mari Z, Bara‐Jimenez W, Aksu M, Sato S, Murphy DL (2009): Involvement of insula and cingulate cortices in control and suppression of natural urges. Cereb Cortex 19:218–223. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li YO, Adali T, Calhoun VD (2006): Sample dependence correction for order selection in fMRI analysis. Conf Proc IEEE ISBI 2006:1072–1075. [Google Scholar]
- Li YO, Adalı T, Calhoun VD (2007): Estimating the number of independent components for functional magnetic resonance imaging data. Human Brain Mapp 28:1251–1266. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liotti M, Brannan S, Egan G, Shade R, Madden L, Abplanalp B, Robillard R, Lancaster J, Zamarripa FE, Fox PT (2001): Brain responses associated with consciousness of breathlessness (air hunger). Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 98:2035–2040. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liu P, Qin W, Wang J, Zeng F, Zhou G, Wen H, von Deneen KM, Liang F, Gong Q, Tian J (2013): Identifying neural patterns of functional dyspepsia using multivariate pattern analysis: A resting‐state fMRI study. PloS One 8:e68205 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ma S, Correa NM, Li XL, Eichele T, Calhoun VD, Adali T (2011): Automatic identification of functional clusters in FMRI data using spatial dependence. IEEE Trans Biomed Eng 58:3406–3417. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mantini D, Perrucci MG, Del Gratta C, Romani GL, Corbetta M (2007): Electrophysiological signatures of resting state networks in the human brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 104:13170–13175. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mantini D, Corbetta M, Romani GL, Orban GA, Vanduffel W (2013): Evolutionarily novel functional networks in the human brain? J Neurosci 33:3259–3275. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Matsuura S, Kakizaki H, Mitsui T, Shiga T, Tamaki N, Koyanagi T (2002): Human brain region response to distention or cold stimulation of the bladder: A positron emission tomography study. J Urol 168:2035–2039. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maurice‐Williams R (1974): Micturition symptoms in frontal tumours. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 37:431–436. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mayer EA (2011): Gut feelings: The emerging biology of gut–brain communication. Nat Rev Neurosci 12:453–466. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mayer EA, Naliboff BD, Craig A (2006): Neuroimaging of the brain‐gut axis: From basic understanding to treatment of functional GI disorders. Gastroenterology 131:1925–1942. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mayer EA, Aziz Q, Coen S, Kern M, Labus J, Lane R, Kuo B, Naliboff B, Tracey I (2009): Brain imaging approaches to the study of functional GI disorders: A Rome working team report. Neurogastroenterol Motil 21:579–596. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McKeown MJ, Makeig S, Brown GG, Jung TP, Kindermann SS, Bell AJ, Sejnowski TJ (1998): Analysis of fMRI data by blind separation into independent spatial components. Human Brain Mapp 6:160–188. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meda SA, Stevens MC, Folley BS, Calhoun VD, Pearlson GD (2009): Evidence for anomalous network connectivity during working memory encoding in schizophrenia: An ICA based analysis. PLoS One 4:e7911 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mehnert U, Boy S, Svensson J, Michels L, Reitz A, Candia V, Kleiser R, Kollias S, Schurch B (2008): Brain activation in response to bladder filling and simultaneous stimulation of the dorsal clitoral nerve—An fMRI study in healthy women. Neuroimage 41:682–689. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mehnert U, Michels L, Zempleni MZ, Schurch B, Kollias S (2011): The supraspinal neural correlate of bladder cold sensation—An fMRI study. Hum Brain Mapp 32:835–845. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Menon V (2011): Large‐scale brain networks and psychopathology: A unifying triple network model. Trend Cognit Sci 15:483–506. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Menon V, Uddin LQ (2010): Saliency, switching, attention and control: A network model of insula function. Brain Struct Funct 214:655–667. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Michels L, Blok BF, Gregorini F, Kurz M, Schurch B, Kessler TM, Kollias S, Mehnert U (2014): Supraspinal control of urine storage and micturition in men—An fMRI study. Cereb Cortex http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1093/cercor/bhu140 published online June 26. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mukerji G, Waters J, Chessell IP, Bountra C, Agarwal SK, Anand P (2006): Pain during ice water test distinguishes clinical bladder hypersensitivity from overactivity disorders. BMC Urol 6:31 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mutschler I, Wieckhorst B, Kowalevski S, Derix J, Wentlandt J, Schulze‐Bonhage A, Ball T (2009): Functional organization of the human anterior insular cortex. Neurosci Lett 457:66–70. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Naliboff BD, Berman S, Chang L, Derbyshire SW, Suyenobu B, Vogt BA, Mandelkern M, Mayer EA (2003): Sex‐related differences in IBS patients: Central processing of visceral stimuli. Gastroenterology 124:1738–1747. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Naqvi NH, Bechara A (2009): The hidden island of addiction: The insula. Trend Neurosci 32:56–67. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nathan P (1952): Thermal sensation in the bladder. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 15:150 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nishizawa O, Ebina K, Sugaya K, Noto H, Satoh K, Kohama T, Harada T, Tsuchida S (1989): Effect of cerebellectomy on reflex micturition in the decerebrate dog as determined by urodynamic evaluation. Urol Int 44:152–156. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Norman KA, Polyn SM, Detre GJ, Haxby JV (2006): Beyond mind‐reading: Multi‐voxel pattern analysis of fMRI data. Trend Cognit Sci 10:424–430. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Northoff G, Heinzel A, de Greck M, Bermpohl F, Dobrowolny H, Panksepp J (2006): Self‐referential processing in our brain—a meta‐analysis of imaging studies on the self. Neuroimage 31:440–457. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nour S, Svarer C, Kristensen JK, Paulson OB, Law I (2000): Cerebral activation during micturition in normal men. Brain 123:781–789. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paulus MP, Stein MB (2006): An insular view of anxiety. Biol Psychiatry 60:383–387. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peyron R, Laurent B, Garcia‐Larrea L (2000): Functional imaging of brain responses to pain. A review and meta‐analysis (2000). Neurophysiol Clin 30:263–288. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pollatos O, Kirsch W, Schandry R (2005): Brain structures involved in interoceptive awareness and cardioafferent signal processing: A dipole source localization study. Hum Brain Mapp 26:54–64. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Powell‐Boone T, Ness TJ, Cannon R, Lloyd LK, Weigent DA, Fillingim RB (2005): Menstrual cycle affects bladder pain sensation in subjects with interstitial cystitis. J Urol 174:1832–1836. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Raichle ME, MacLeod AM, Snyder AZ, Powers WJ, Gusnard DA, Shulman GL (2001): A default mode of brain function. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 98:676–682. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robinson S, Basso G, Soldati N, Sailer U, Jovicich J, Bruzzone L, Kryspin‐Exner I, Bauer H, Moser E (2009): A resting state network in the motor control circuit of the basal ganglia. BMC Neurosci 10:137 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sakakibara R, Hattori T, Yasuda K, Yamanishi T (1996): Micturitional disturbance after acute hemispheric stroke: Analysis of the lesion site by CT and MRI. J Neurol Sci 137:47–56. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saper CB (2002): The central autonomic nervous system: Conscious visceral perception and autonomic pattern generation. Ann Rev Neurosci 25:433–469. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schandry R (1981): Heart beat perception and emotional experience. Psychophysiology 18:483–488. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seeley WW, Menon V, Schatzberg AF, Keller J, Glover GH, Kenna H, Reiss AL, Greicius MD (2007): Dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks for salience processing and executive control. J Neurosci 27:2349–2356. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seseke S, Baudewig J, Kallenberg K, Ringert RH, Seseke F, Dechent P (2006): Voluntary pelvic floor muscle control—An fMRI study. Neuroimage 31:1399–1407. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seseke S, Baudewig J, Kallenberg K, Ringert RH, Seseke F, Dechent P (2008): Gender differences in voluntary micturition control—An fMRI study. NeuroImage 43:183–191. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seth AK, Suzuki K, Critchley HD (2011): An interoceptive predictive coding model of conscious presence. Front Psychol 2:395 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shulman GL, Fiez JA, Corbetta M, Buckner RL, Miezin FM, Raichle ME, Petersen SE (1997): Common blood flow changes across visual tasks: II. Decreases in cerebral cortex. J Cognit Neurosci 9:648–663. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sladky R, Friston KJ, Tröstl J, Cunnington R, Moser E, Windischberger C (2011): Slice‐timing effects and their correction in functional MRI. Neuroimage 58:588–594. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith SM, Jenkinson M, Woolrich MW, Beckmann CF, Behrens TE, Johansen‐Berg H, Bannister PR, De Luca M, Drobnjak I, Flitney DE (2004): Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL. Neuroimage 23:S208–S219. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith SM, Fox PT, Miller KL, Glahn DC, Fox PM, Mackay CE, Filippini N, Watkins KE, Toro R, Laird AR (2009): Correspondence of the brain's functional architecture during activation and rest. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 106:13040–13045. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spreng RN, Mar RA, Kim AS (2009): The common neural basis of autobiographical memory, prospection, navigation, theory of mind, and the default mode: A quantitative meta‐analysis. J Cognit Neurosci 21:489–510. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tadic SD, Griffiths D, Schaefer W, Resnick NM (2008): Abnormal connections in the supraspinal bladder control network in women with urge urinary incontinence. Neuroimage 39:1647–1653. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tadic SD, Griffiths D, Schaefer W, Murrin A, Clarkson B, Resnick NM (2012): Brain activity underlying impaired continence control in older women with overactive bladder. Neurourol Urodyn 31:652–658. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tammela T, Hellström P, Kontturi M (1992): Cold sensation and bladder instability in patients with outflow obstruction due to benign prostatic hyperplasia. Br J Urol 70:404–407. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Teves D, Videen TO, Cryer PE, Powers WJ (2004): Activation of human medial prefrontal cortex during autonomic responses to hypoglycemia. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 101:6217–6221. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thomas CG, Harshman RA, Menon RS (2002): Noise reduction in BOLD‐based fMRI using component analysis. Neuroimage 17:1521–1537. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Torta DM, Costa T, Duca S, Fox PT, Cauda F (2013): Parcellation of the cingulate cortex at rest and during tasks: A meta‐analytic clustering and experimental study. Front Hum Neurosci 7: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Uddin LQ, Clare Kelly A, Biswal BB, Xavier Castellanos F, Milham MP (2009): Functional connectivity of default mode network components: Correlation, anticorrelation, and causality. Hum Brain Mapp 30:625–637. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ueki K (1960): Disturbances of micturition observed in some patients with brain tumor. Neurol Medico‐Chirurgica 2:25–33. [Google Scholar]
- Van Oudenhove L, McKie S, Lassman D, Uddin B, Paine P, Coen S, Gregory L, Tack J, Aziz Q (2011): Fatty acid–induced gut‐brain signaling attenuates neural and behavioral effects of sad emotion in humans. J Clin Invest 121:3094–3099. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vincent J, Patel G, Fox M, Snyder A, Baker J, Van Essen D, Zempel J, Snyder L, Corbetta M, Raichle M (2007): Intrinsic functional architecture in the anaesthetized monkey brain. Nature 447:83–86. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wager TD, Barrett LF (2004) From affect to control: Functional specialization of the insula in motivation and regulation. Published online at PsycExtra.
- Wiens S, Mezzacappa ES, Katkin ES (2000): Heartbeat detection and the experience of emotions. Cognit Emotion 14:417–427. [Google Scholar]
- Wise RA (2004): Dopamine, learning and motivation. Nat Rev Neurosci 5:483–494. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Woolrich MW, Jbabdi S, Patenaude B, Chappell M, Makni S, Behrens T, Beckmann C, Jenkinson M, Smith SM (2009): Bayesian analysis of neuroimaging data in FSL. Neuroimage 45:S173–S186. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Xu J, Zhang S, Calhoun VD, Monterosso J, Li CSR, Worhunsky PD, Stevens M, Pearlson GD, Potenza MN (2013): Task‐related concurrent but opposite modulations of overlapping functional networks as revealed by spatial ICA. Neuroimage 79:62–71. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yágüez L, Coen S, Gregory LJ, Amaro E, Altman C, Brammer MJ, Bullmore ET, Williams SC, Aziz Q (2005): Brain response to visceral aversive conditioning: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Gastroenterology 128:1819–1829. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zempleni MZ, Michels L, Mehnert U, Schurch B, Kollias S (2010): Cortical substrate of bladder control in SCI and the effect of peripheral pudendal stimulation. Neuroimage 49:2983–2994. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhang S, Li CSR (2012): Functional networks for cognitive control in a stop signal task: Independent component analysis. Hum Brain Mapp 33:89–104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhang H, Reitz A, Kollias S, Summers P, Curt A, Schurch B (2005): An fMRI study of the role of suprapontine brain structures in the voluntary voiding control induced by pelvic floor contraction. Neuroimage 24:174–180. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zuo XN, Kelly C, Adelstein JS, Klein DF, Castellanos FX, Milham MP (2010): Reliable intrinsic connectivity networks: Test–retest evaluation using ICA and dual regression approach. Neuroimage 49:2163–2177. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.