Abstract
This meta‐analytic connectivity modeling (MACM) study explores the functional connectivity of the cerebellum with the cerebrum in social cognitive processes. In a recent meta‐analysis, Van Overwalle, Baetens, Mariën, and Vandekerckhove (2014) documented that the cerebellum is implicated in social processes of “body” reading (mirroring; e.g., understanding other persons' intentions from observing their movements) and “mind” reading (mentalizing, e.g., inferring other persons' beliefs, intentions or personality traits, reconstructing persons' past, future, or hypothetical events). In a recent functional connectivity study, Buckner et al. (2011) offered a novel parcellation of cerebellar topography that substantially overlaps with the cerebellar meta‐analytic findings of Van Overwalle et al. (2014). This overlap suggests that the involvement of the cerebellum in social reasoning depends on its functional connectivity with the cerebrum. To test this hypothesis, we explored the meta‐analytic co‐activations as indices of functional connectivity between the cerebellum and the cerebrum during social cognition. The MACM results confirm substantial and distinct connectivity with respect to the functions of (a) action understanding (“body” reading) and (b) mentalizing (“mind” reading). The consistent and strong connectivity findings of this analysis suggest that cerebellar activity during social judgments reflects distinct mirroring and mentalizing functionality, and that these cerebellar functions are connected with corresponding functional networks in the cerebrum. Hum Brain Mapp 36:5137–5154, 2015. © 2015 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.
Keywords: social cognition, cerebellum, functional neuroimaging, meta‐analysis, functional connectivity
Abbreviations
- ALE
activation likelihood estimation;
- MACM
meta‐analytic connectivity modelling;
- mPFC
medial prefrontal cortex;
- MRI
magnetic resonance imaging;
- pSTS
posterior superior temporal sulcus;
- ROI
regions of interest;
- TPJ
temporo‐parietal junction
INTRODUCTION
In 2014, Van Overwalle et al. conducted a large‐scale meta‐analysis on social cognition and the cerebellum that included over 350 functional magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) studies. Social cognition is an important human ability that allows understanding the social origin and purpose of the behaviors of other persons or the self (i.e., “body” reading) and their state of mind (i.e., “mind” reading or mentalizing). Van Overwalle et al. (2014) found robust clusters of activation in the cerebellum that were recruited during these social‐cognitive processes. These clusters seemed to overlap with non‐social functions reported in earlier meta‐analyses on the role of the cerebellum in motor functions, emotions, executive control and language [E et al., 2014; Stoodley and Schmahmann, 2009]. Van Overwalle and colleagues (2014, p. 563) therefore suggested that “the cerebellum does not play a domain‐specific role in social cognition, but most probably provides domain‐general executive and semantic support.”
But is this correct? The same authors recently put forward an alternative interpretation of the cerebellum that focuses on function‐specific social cognitive processes [Van Overwalle, Baetens, Mariën, and Vandekerckhove, 2015]. This novel perspective was based on a connectivity study by Buckner et al. (2011; Buckner, 2013), who explored the organization of circuits between the cerebrum and cerebellum using resting‐state functional connectivity MRI for a total sample of 1000 participants, resulting in a complete topography of the cerebellum in relationship with major networks of the cerebrum (Yeo et al., 2011). The study revealed similar network structures in the cerebellum as in the cerebrum, spanning approximately the same relative volumes (see Fig. 1 for their location and organization). Of interest among the networks in the cerebrum identified by Yeo et al. (2011) were a default network that includes many mentalizing areas recruited during “mind” reading (see meta‐analyses by Schurz et al., 2014; Van Overwalle, 2009), as well as two somatomotor networks that reflect primary somatomotor and somatomotor integration functions which show substantial overlap with mirror areas recruited during “body” reading [see meta‐analysis by Molenberghs et al., 2012; Van Overwalle and Baetens, 2009]. To our knowledge, Buckner et al. (2011) is the only publication to date that provides an empirical parcellation of the cerebellum at such a large scale, and that allows a rough interpretation of the cerebellar networks based on the connectivity with networks of the cerebrum previously described in the literature [see also Habas et al., 2009; O'Reilly et al., 2010]. Importantly, Van Overwalle et al. (2015) detected a remarkable similarity between the results of their meta‐analysis of social mentalizing in the cerebellum and the default/mentalizing network of Buckner et al. (2011), as well as between areas involved in social mirroring or behavior understanding (i.e., “body” reading) in the cerebellum and the somatomotor networks described by Buckner et al. (2011). Van Overwalle et al. argued that their results might therefore be better explained as a reflection of the role of specific social mentalizing and mirroring functions.
Figure 1.
Parcellation of the cerebellum showing three distinct representations, labeled the primary, secondary, and tertiary representations. Each is a mirror‐image ordering of the adjacent map. Networks are color‐coded with their function as proposed by Bruckner et al. (2011, p. 2332). The networks refer to the 7‐network parcellation of the cerebrum shown in the bottom of the figure. The white lines demarcate estimated boundaries between the maps and do not have significance in relation to sulcal boundaries. R* refers to the red network, which can be seen in the x = −12 section. Adapted with permission from Figure 16 of Buckner R, Krienen F, Castellanos A, Diaz JC, Yeo BT, J Neurophysiol, 2011, 106:2322–2345, © Americal Physiological Society and from Figure 11 of Yeo BTT, Krienen FM, Sepulcre J, Sabuncu MR, Lashkari D, Hollinshead M, Buckner RL, J Neurophysiol, 2011, 106, 1125–1165, © Americal Physiological Society.
However, in past research on the cerebellum, most of the networks [Buckner et al., 2011] were identified using clusters of intrinsic functional connectivity during resting state, without independent verification of their assumed functionality during the alleged tasks or processes. Likewise, the novel analysis by Van Overwalle et al. (2015) was based on a close similarity with Buckner's cerebellar networks, without independent verification of the functional connectivity underlying the parcellation by Buckner et al. (2011). Thus, the reinterpretation by Van Overwalle et al. (2015) in terms of these functional networks is post‐hoc, solely based on spatial similarity. In order to provide stronger empirical evidence for the claim by Van Overwalle et al. (2015) that the cerebellar areas involved in mentalizing and mirroring activity are connected to the corresponding functional networks in the cerebrum, the aim of the present study is to conduct meta‐analytic connectivity modeling (MACM), which takes co‐activations across studies in a meta‐analysis as indices of functional connectivity.
Some words on the terminology of the networks (Fig. 1). First, we renamed one of the major networks of Yeo et al. (2011) the mentalizing network, although it was originally labeled the default (red) network. This default network reflects activity during undirected thought and daydreaming at rest. Its function is not entirely clear, although several meta‐analyses have consistently demonstrated that functional activity in this network strongly overlaps with the social mentalizing network in the cerebrum [Mars et al., 2012; Schilbach et al., 2012; Spreng et al., 2009]. Given the consistent and substantial correspondence between the functional activity during rest and mentalizing, Barrett and Satpute (2013) suggested the term “default/mentalizing” for this network and argued that its main function is conceptualization by constructing a “mental model of the past, the present moment, or the future” (p. 365). Second, although actually a subset of the somatosensory integration network (green) defined by Buckner et al. (2011), we use the term mirror network to indicate those areas that correspond most closely to action understanding by the observation of human motion (also labeled mirroring, see next paragraph). Note also that Buckner et al.'s (2011) parcellation results (Fig. 1) are based on the strongest connectivity in each voxel, and hence do not reflect the uncertainty with respect to the assigned network. Thus, areas close to the demarcation between networks may involve populations of neurons of mixed connectivity to different networks.
Four Themes in Social Cognition
The meta‐analysis by Van Overwalle et al. (2014) disclosed four themes or subdomains that coherently recruited the cerebellum, and were related to very similar tasks and stimulus types. These four themes involved studies on (1) mirroring (i.e., related to the mirror network in the cortex) which refers to the understanding of visual or other perceptual input from human movements, (2) mentalizing (without human movements) focusing on specific events, (3) general traits, and (4) abstractions. The first three themes were used in two earlier meta‐analyses on social neuroscience by Van Overwalle (2009) and Van Overwalle and Baetens (2009), while the latter theme was inspired by recent research on abstraction in social mentalizing [cf. Trope and Liberman, 2010]. These four themes were identified based on the similarities and overlap between stimulus input, task requirements, and empirical clusters revealed by an activation likelihood estimation (ALE) analysis. To briefly summarize the findings (see Table 1 for examples):
Table 1.
Themes and task categories, and cerebellar activity in the connectivity meta‐analysis
Theme | Categories (2000–2012)a | Exemplary stimuli | Exemplary instruction | Exemplary target condition | Exemplary control condition | Nb | %c |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mirroring (visible motions of body parts) | |||||||
1. Body part motion | body part motion | passive viewing | motion/manipulating object | no motion | 81 | 26% | |
2. Reflecting on intention | body part motion | passive viewing | intentional/incorrect motion | unintended/correct motion | 33 | 31% | |
Total N of contrasts/Mean % | 114 | 29% | |||||
Event mentalizing (no visible motions of body parts) | |||||||
3. Goal‐directed shape motion | animations with shapes | viewing | goal‐directed reactivity | random motion | 17 | 31% | |
4. Goal‐directed action | action/event stories | choose story ending | intentional action | physical event | 17 | 31% | |
5. ToM belief | action/object stories | several questions | (false/true) belief | physical event | 38 | 8% | |
6. Morality | action stories | appropriateness action | moral | nonmoral | 20 | 10% | |
7. Social Games | games | playing the game | human opponent | computer opponent | 13 | 33% | |
Total N of contrasts/Mean % | 105 | 23% | |||||
Person mentalizing (no visible motions of body parts) | |||||||
8. Traits of distant others | trait words | describe person | person judgment | non trait judgment | 37 | 35% | |
9. Traits of close others | trait words | describe person | close other judgment | non trait judgment | 13 | 38% | |
10. Self‐reference | trait words | describe self | self descriptive | non self descriptive | 79 | 22% | |
Total N of contrasts/Mean % | 129 | 32% | |||||
Abstraction in mentalizing | |||||||
12. General Abstraction | person pictures | describe person | trait category | non trait category (visual) | 4 | 100% | |
13. Social Categories | action stories | action judgment | social category | trait category | 3 | 67% | |
14. Hypothetical | action stories | remember/imagine | hypothetical (counterfactual) | semantic memory | 3 | 67% | |
15. Past and Future | actions/events | remember/imagine | autobiographic past + future | semantic memory | 56 | 75% | |
Total N of contrasts/Mean % | 66 | 77% | |||||
Grand total N of contrasts/Grand mean % | 414 | 40% |
Taken from Van Overwalle et al. (2014), except for the auxiliary task conditions 11, 16 & 17, which were omitted.
Search period of inclusion.
Number of studies included in the search.
% Of studies reporting cerebellar activation. Some studies participated in several contrasts across task categories.
Mirroring involves the observation of human motion (e.g., by hands and fingers, face, and legs) with or without a focus on the intention of the agent, which typically recruits the mirror network in the cortex [Van Overwalle and Baetens, 2009]. Cerebellar activity was found in about 28% of the mirror studies.
Event Mentalizing encompasses mentalizing about the actor's momentary intentions and beliefs, given a behavioral (event) description that does not involve perceptual input from human motion [Van Overwalle and Baetens, 2009]. Approximately 22% of the studies in the meta‐analysis revealed cerebellar activity.
Person Mentalizing involves inferring enduring characteristics of a person or the self, such as traits and preferences [Van Overwalle, 2009]. Again, cerebellar activity was limited to 25% of the studies.
Abstract Mentalizing includes studies that contrast high against low abstraction in social cognition [Trope and Liberman, 2010]. For instance, contrasting person judgments against visual descriptions of the same behaviors (e.g., respectively judging “why” or “how” a person is reading a book), or by contrasting the more distant and abstract past or future, or even contrasting hypothetical events with the momentary present. Abstraction recruited very strong and robust cerebellar activity in 67%–100% of the studies.
Present Meta‐Analytic Connectivity Analysis
In order to identify the functional connectivity (i.e., co‐activations) between the cerebellar activation identified in the meta‐analysis by Van Overwalle et al. (2014), and other brain networks in the cerebrum, our general plan of analysis consisted of the following steps:
As a first step, we selected all the studies (> 350) on social cognition from the meta‐analysis by Van Overwalle et al. (2014) that showed cerebellar activity. This resulted in 133 studies. None of these studies involved any motor activity by the participants (e.g., tasks using imitation and judgment of self‐agency) so that only the effect of pure perception or observation was examined. We used the same overarching division in four themes from this meta‐analysis, as described above. Each theme strikes an optimal balance between a sufficient number of studies on the one hand, and a relatively small number of task categories and functions that are quite homogeneous and consistent with each other. This methodology makes it possible to detect reliable connectivity between the cerebellum and cerebrum.
As a second step, and in addition to the cerebellar coordinates extracted from the previous meta‐analysis [Van Overwalle et al., 2014], we coded the coordinates of significant clusters in the cerebrum that were reported in the selected studies. FVO extracted the coordinates, while TD checked them, and FVO double checked and corrected them.
As a final step, we identified regions of interest (ROI) in the cerebellum based on the meta‐analytic results of Van Overwalle et al. (2014). Next, we selected the studies that contained at least one ROI, and then conducted an activation likelihood estimation (ALE) analysis on the coordinates in the cerebrum in order to identify the clusters of consistent co‐activations with cerebellar activity in that ROI. We then determined whether these clusters were part of a mentalizing or mirror/somatosensory networks in the cerebrum, or of the other major networks identified in the parcellation study by Yeo et al. (2011).
Given the close correspondence between the mentalizing and mirroring networks in the cerebellum and the cerebrum [Buckner et al., 2011], and the close correspondence with the mentalizing and mirroring tasks identified in the meta‐analysis of the cerebellum by Van Overwalle et al. (2014), we hypothesize that the cerebellum is intrinsically involved in function‐specific mentalizing and mirroring processes of social cognition. Consequently, we predict that the areas of the cerebellum involved in these processes are functionally connected (i.e., co‐activated) with the same mentalizing and mirroring/somatomotor networks in the cerebrum.
METHOD
Selection of Studies and Coordinates
The fMRI studies reviewed in the current study were taken from the earlier meta‐analysis on cerebellar activity during social cognitive tasks or judgments by Van Overwalle et al. (2014). Originally, these studies were identified by a search in PubMed using the term “fMRI” along with at least one of the following terms in the title or abstract: “person”, “social”, “self” or “autobiographic[al]” from 2000 to the end of 2012. Additional studies were added from several meta‐analyses on various aspects of social cognition, including mirror studies and mentalizing studies on goal‐directed action, beliefs and theory of mind, morality, traits of others and the self, and several forms of abstraction (i.e., higher construal) including episodic autobiographic studies on the past and future [Bzdok et al., 2012; Carrington and Bailey, 2009; Denny et al., 2012; Lombardo et al., 2011; Mar, 2011; Martinelli et al., 2012; Molenberghs et al., 2012; Qin et al., 2012; Schilbach et al., 2012; Shkurko, 2013; Spreng et al., 2008; Svoboda et al., 2006; van der Meeret al., 2010].
All studies or conditions that involved the participant's own actions, such as imitation of the behavior of others or one's sense of agency during movement, were excluded, because motor activity in itself might activate the cerebellum. As reported in Van Overwalle et al. (2014), studies were only included if they investigated unmedicated healthy adults or adolescents (i.e., between ages 10 and 19 according to the definition of the World Health Organization), used fMRI scanning, involved non‐emotional stimuli or tasks, and reported the coordinates of activations in the space of the MNI template [Collins et al., 1994] or the atlas of Talairach and Tournoux (1988). Clinical studies were included if they reported the results of healthy control participants separately. When activations were reported in MNI space, they were transformed into Talairach and Tournoux coordinates by means of a non‐linear Brett transformation [http://imaging.mrc-cbu.cam.ac.uk/imaging/MniTalairach; Brett et al., 2001] so that all the coordinates had a common stereotaxic framework. As noted earlier, all studies were sorted in the same task categories and four overarching themes from the earlier meta‐analyses by Van Overwalle (2009) and Van Overwalle and Baetens (2009).
Connectivity Meta‐Analysis Using ALE
For each cerebellar cluster reported by Van Overwalle et al. (2014), we identified a ROI based on the (sub)peak coordinates of the cluster by drawing a sphere with a 5 mm radius around it. This relatively small radius was chosen given the smaller size of the cerebellum [and its mentalizing and somatomotor networks; cf. Buckner et al., 2011], so that all ROIs were largely within the boundaries of these cerebellar networks. In their reinterpretation of cerebellar activity, Van Overwalle et al. (2015) identified each cluster as belonging to one of the cerebellar networks from Buckner et al. (2011), most often a mentalizing or a somatomotor (integration) network. Subsequently, the ROIs were categorized along each network and along each of the four task themes (Table 2). The focus here was on the analysis of the mentalizing (red) and somatomotor (blue and green) networks.
Table 2.
Meta‐analytic clusters of Van Overwalle et al. (2014) in function of the networks identified by Buckner et al. (2011) with the peak coordinates serving as ROI seeds for the connectivity analysis
Network | Theme | Cerebellar Label | Volume | x | y | z | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mentalizing network | |||||||
Mirroring | |||||||
Right Posterior ‐ Uvula | Crus I | 192 | 30 | −78 | −24 | ||
Event Mentalizing | |||||||
Left Posterior ‐ Uvula | Crus I | 4128 | −24 | −86 | −24 | ||
Left Posterior ‐ Tuber | −30 | −80 | −28 | ||||
Left Posterior ‐ Pyramis | −14 | −80 | −30 | ||||
Right Posterior ‐ Pyramis | Crus I | 624 | 20 | −80 | −28 | ||
Person Mentalizing | |||||||
Right Posterior ‐ Tuber | Crus I | 2544 | 24 | −82 | −28 | ||
Abstract Mentalizing | |||||||
Right Posterior ‐ Uvula | Crus I | 8112 | 16 | −84 | −26 | ||
Right Posterior ‐ Uvula | & VI | 28 | −80 | −26 | |||
Right Posterior ‐ Tuber | 40 | −72 | −28 | ||||
Right Posterior ‐ Tonsil | 36 | −60 | −42 | ||||
Right Posterior ‐ Tonsil | IX | 4064 | 8 | −48 | −38 | ||
Left Posterior ‐ Tonsil | −4 | −42 | −44 | ||||
Left Posterior ‐ Tonsil | −8 | −48 | −38 | ||||
Left Posterior ‐ Tuber | Crus I | 816 | −22 | −84 | −28 | ||
Left Posterior ‐ Tuber | −36 | −86 | −30 | ||||
Left Posterior ‐ Uvula | −28 | −82 | −24 | ||||
Left Posterior ‐ Uvula | −34 | −74 | −24 | ||||
Somatomotor networks | |||||||
Mirroring (somatomotor integration network) | |||||||
Right Posterior ‐ Uvula | VIIB | 800 | 10 | −78 | −32 | ||
Left Posterior ‐ Uvula | VIIB | 360 | −12 | −78 | −32 | ||
Left Posterior ‐ Inf. Semi‐Lunar | VIIB | 232 | −22 | −70 | −38 | ||
Person Mentalizing (somatomotor network) | |||||||
Right Anterior ‐ Lingual (selfa) | IV | 1016 | 8 | −46 | −18 | ||
Right Anterior ‐ Culmen (selfa) | VI | 576 | 26 | −40 | −16 | ||
Other Networks | |||||||
Mirroring (visual network) | |||||||
Right Anterior ‐ Culmen | VI | 504 | 38 | −54 | −22 | ||
Person Mentalizing (executive network) | |||||||
Left Posterior ‐ Pyramis | VI | 136 | −10 | −70 | −26 |
Anatomical labels given according to the atlas of ALE and Schmahmann et al. (2000). Volume in mm3 for each cluster; All coordinates according to the Talairach & Tournoux (1988) atlas.
Cluster preferentially involved in self‐references.
The cerebellar ROIs served as “seeds” for the connectivity analysis. That is, for each cerebellar network and task theme, we identified the studies that reported activation in at least one of the ROIs. Connectivity was then analyzed by identifying the coactivation in the cerebrum. Thus, for all studies that revealed activation in a cerebellar ROI, the reported peak coordinates in the cerebrum were analyzed using the Activation Likelihood Estimation (ALE) procedure as implemented by GingerALE 2.1 [Eickhoff et al., 2009; Laird et al., 2005]. ALE attempts to reveal clusters of consistent activation across selected studies. More specifically, based on the collection of peak coordinates from each study, ALE estimates the probability that at least one of the peaks lies within a voxel. This computation is repeated for each voxel in the cerebrum and results in an ALE map. A statistical threshold for the ALE map is computed using a nonparametric permutation test. This test identifies real activation if the null hypothesis that the activation foci are spread uniformly throughout the brain (i.e., random clustering) is rejected. We thresholded the nonparametric permutation test for each voxel at a false discovery rate corrected threshold of p < 0.05 and an additional cluster extend threshold of 500 mm3 [Laird et al., 2005].
Next, to isolate the distinct connectivity pattern related to each network, we contrasted the resulting connectivity analysis of each network with one another. To that end, we ran several ALE subtraction analyses. ALE subtraction analyses are processed in a similar manner as simple ALE analyses, although z score images are used to compute the contrast results.
RESULTS
To analyze the connectivity of the cerebellum with the cerebrum, we ran an ALE analysis on the coactivations in the cerebrum of studies that reported cerebral activity. We conducted this analysis for the cerebral mentalizing network (Table 3) and the two somatomotor networks (Table 4) because these were of theoretical interest and involved a sufficient number of studies. All analyses were run for each task theme separately (mirroring, event, person, and abstract mentalizing) as well as combined. Note that MACM has limited selectivity because studies that are selected for, for instance, somatomotor activity in the cerebellum may also contain task elements that trigger mentalizing activity in the cerebrum. Therefore, to isolate the distinct connectivity pattern related to each network, we also conducted contrast analyses on the connectivity results, comparing mentalizing connectivity with somatomotor connectivity and vice‐versa (Table 5).
Table 3.
Clusters and peak coordinates in the cerebrum of the ALE connectivity meta‐analysis given mentalizing seeds in the cerebellum in function of task theme.
Network and anatomical label | All themes | Abstraction | Person | Event | Mirroring | ||||||||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
x | y | z | Volume | Extrema | x | y | z | Volume | x | y | z | Volume | x | y | z | Volume | x | y | z | Volume | |
Mentalizing Areas in Cerebrum | |||||||||||||||||||||
ventral mPFCa | −4 | 32 | −20 | 5584 | 0.036 | −4 | 32 | −20 | 2600 | 0 | 54 | −10 | 160 | ||||||||
−2 | 50 | −12 | 0.030 | −4 | 48 | −12 | |||||||||||||||
4 | 54 | 12 | 0.025 | −4 | 60 | 4 | 4 | 56 | 12 | 440 | |||||||||||
10 | 56 | 20 | 0.023 | −2 | 50 | 22 | 344 | ||||||||||||||
6 | 54 | −2 | 0.017 | 14 | 38 | 14 | 144 | 12 | 62 | 18 | 248 | ||||||||||
4 | 34 | 0 | 704 | 0.027 | 6 | 32 | −2 | 368 | 4 | 34 | 2 | 352 | |||||||||
dorsal mPFCa | −12 | 36 | 50 | 1952 | 0.029 | −18 | 34 | 40 | 576 | −12 | 34 | 50 | 1224 | 4 | 52 | 40 | 608 | ||||
−12 | 26 | 52 | 0.023 | −22 | 26 | 42 | 0 | −52 | 28 | 2168 | |||||||||||
−18 | 34 | 40 | 0.017 | −10 | 40 | 44 | |||||||||||||||
−14 | 50 | 30 | 888 | 0.025 | −14 | 50 | 32 | 688 | −4 | 58 | 30 | 216 | |||||||||
−4 | 58 | 30 | 0.017 | 24 | 30 | 36 | 152 | ||||||||||||||
8 | 48 | 44 | 1272 | 0.028 | 12 | 54 | 22 | 272 | |||||||||||||
TPJa | −44 | −60 | 28 | 6872 | 0.047 | −44 | −60 | 30 | 5088 | −48 | −68 | 32 | 2064 | 58 | −54 | 20 | 848 | ||||
44 | −62 | 24 | 3816 | 0.033 | 44 | −60 | 26 | 2848 | −46 | −54 | 28 | 54 | −48 | 14 | |||||||
52 | −62 | 26 | 0.029 | 50 | −68 | 32 | −56 | −52 | 18 | 576 | |||||||||||
52 | −56 | 36 | 0.022 | 52 | −56 | 38 | −64 | −48 | 16 | ||||||||||||
Posterior Cingulatea | −2 | −54 | 28 | 11912 | 0.056 | −8 | −52 | 16 | 7584 | −6 | −48 | 40 | 1784 | ||||||||
−8 | −52 | 16 | 0.044 | −6 | −50 | 28 | |||||||||||||||
10 | −46 | 6 | 0.027 | 10 | −50 | 30 | |||||||||||||||
— | 8 | −50 | 22 | ||||||||||||||||||
Inferior Temporal Gyrus | −56 | −8 | −18 | 6200 | 0.067 | −58 | −6 | −12 | 992 | −56 | −8 | −16 | 736 | ||||||||
(Temporal Pole) | 58 | −14 | −16 | 336 | |||||||||||||||||
−50 | −4 | −28 | 384 | ||||||||||||||||||
Other Areas in Cerebrum | |||||||||||||||||||||
Parahippocampal Gyrusa | −22 | −20 | −12 | 5936 | 0.039 | −24 | −20 | −12 | 6552 | ||||||||||||
−24 | −36 | −8 | 0.027 | −24 | −36 | −8 | |||||||||||||||
20 | −14 | −14 | 2704 | 0.035 | 20 | −14 | −14 | 2640 | |||||||||||||
24 | −30 | −14 | |||||||||||||||||||
Superior Temporal Gyrus | 44 | 14 | −30 | 904 | 0.025 | ||||||||||||||||
Middle Temporal Gyrus | 46 | 0 | −26 | 0.018 | 58 | −4 | −20 | 344 | |||||||||||||
−58 | −40 | −2 | 872 | 0.025 | −58 | −40 | −4 | 544 | −60 | −38 | 4 | 240 | |||||||||
58 | −4 | −20 | 720 | 0.023 | −54 | −6 | −20 | 4504 | |||||||||||||
46 | 2 | −24 | 112 | ||||||||||||||||||
−56 | −24 | −10 | 104 |
Connectivity in the cerebrum given cerebellar mentalizing ROIs as seeds (see Table 2). Anatomical labels given according to the ALE atlas. Volume in mm3 for each cluster; Value = ALE Extrema value; All x‐y‐z coordinates are according to the Talairach & Tournoux (1988) atlas or transformed from MNI to Talairach & Tournoux (1988) using the Brett transform. All ALE peaks are significant at P < 0.05, FDR corrected and with volume > 500 mm3 for all themes. The number of studies, foci, and participants were respectively for abstraction: 20, 325, 316; person:4, 51, 71; event: 8, 79, 146; and mirroring: 2, 43, 45.
Survives the contrast analysis (see Table 5).
Table 4.
Clusters and peak coordinates in the cerebrum of the ALE connectivity meta‐analysis given somatomotor seeds in the cerebellum in function of task theme
Network and anatomical label | All themes | Person | Mirroring | ||||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
x | y | z | Volume | Extrema | x | y | z | Volume | x | y | z | Volume | |||
Mentalizing Areas in Cerebrum | |||||||||||||||
ventral mPFC | 4 | 36 | 4 | 872 | 0.018 | 4 | 36 | 4 | 1256 | ||||||
Precuneus | 8 | −62 | 24 | 664 | 0.018 | 8 | −62 | 24 | 944 | ||||||
Temporal Pole | −54 | 2 | −24 | 1680 | 0.020 | ||||||||||
−56 | −10 | −14 | 0.021 | −56 | −10 | −16 | 1760 | ||||||||
−54 | −6 | −22 | 0.019 | −54 | −6 | −22 | 1760 | ||||||||
Mirror Areas in Cerebrum | |||||||||||||||
pSTSa | 48 | −54 | 10 | 4248 | 0.020 | 48 | −3 | 0 | 1280 | ||||||
52 | −56 | 14 | 0.021 | 52 | −56 | 16 | 432 | 50 | −56 | 6 | 2672 | ||||
Inferior Frontal Gyrusa | −44 | 8 | 30 | 640 | 0.017 | −46 | 8 | 28 | 240 | ||||||
44 | 12 | 24 | 976 | 0.013 | 44 | 12 | 24 | 1464 | |||||||
Inferior Parietal Lobule | −34 | −40 | 52 | 0.022 | −50 | −42 | 30 | 288 | −34 | −40 | 52 | 1464 | |||
Superior Parietal Lobule | 28 | −48 | 56 | 720 | 0.015 | 28 | −48 | 56 | 1088 | ||||||
−30 | −50 | 42 | 208 | ||||||||||||
Other Somatomotor Areas in Cerebrum | |||||||||||||||
Precentral Gyrusa | 42 | 8 | 32 | 976 | 0.015 | 42 | 8 | 32 | |||||||
Middle Frontal Gyrus | 50 | 20 | 20 | 0.013 | −26 | 31 | 36 | 288 | 50 | 20 | 20 | ||||
−28 | −4 | 52 | 792 | 0.017 | −28 | −6 | 52 | 616 | |||||||
Postcentral Gyrusa | −34 | −30 | 52 | 1592 | 0.015 | −32 | −30 | 54 | 448 | ||||||
−32 | −20 | 54 | |||||||||||||
Insula | 48 | −2 | 0 | 896 | 0.020 | ||||||||||
Other Areas in Cerebrum | |||||||||||||||
Fusiform Gyrus | 42 | −60 | −8 | 4248 | 0.026 | 46 | −62 | −8 | |||||||
−44 | −50 | −14 | 1040 | 0.022 | −20 | −83 | −13 | 288 | −44 | −66 | −8 | 120 | |||
Parahippocampal Gyrus | 12 | −36 | −2 | 1056 | 0.014 | 12 | −36 | −2 | 1584 | ||||||
Lingual Gyrus | 14 | −48 | 2 | 0.020 | 14 | −48 | 2 | 1584 | |||||||
Middle Occipital Gyrus | −46 | −68 | 6 | 712 | 0.018 | −46 | −68 | 6 | 960 | ||||||
Cuneus | −10 | −98 | 8 |
Connectivity in the cerebrum given cerebellar ROIs as seeds (see also Table 2): Somatomotor (person) and somatomotor integration (mirroring). Anatomical labels given according to the ALE atlas. Volume in mm3 for each cluster; Value = ALE Extrema value; All x‐y‐z coordinates are according to the Talairach & Tournoux (1988) atlas or transformed from MNI to Talairach & Tournoux (1988) using the Brett transform. All ALE peaks are significant at P < 0.05, FDR corrected and with volume > 500 mm3 for all themes. The number of studies, foci and participants were respectively for person:6, 69, 118; and mirroring:5, 98, 113.
Survives the contrast analysis (see Table 5).
Table 5.
Clusters and peak coordinates of the ALE connectivity contrast meta‐analysis
Contrasts and Anatomical labels | x | y | z | Volume | Extrema | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mentalizing > Somatomotor seeds in cerebellum | ||||||
Mentalizing areas in Cerebrum | ||||||
ventral mPFC | −1 | 55 | 10 | 472 | 3.353 | |
−4 | 58 | 14 | 3.090 | |||
6 | 52 | 12 | 2.948 | |||
dorsal mPFC | −8 | 51 | 29 | 808 | 3.719 | |
−6 | 56 | 26 | 3.540 | |||
−4 | 57 | 30 | 3.353 | |||
−10 | 42 | 42 | 104 | 3.353 | ||
TPJ | −52 | −62 | 35 | 1592 | 3.540 | |
−40 | −58 | 30 | 2.911 | |||
−48 | −74 | 34 | 2.652 | |||
Posterior cingulate | 0 | −52 | 28 | 1728 | 3.719 | |
−1 | −52 | 29 | 3.540 | |||
4 | −47 | 30 | 3.353 | |||
−10 | −56 | 28 | 3.156 | |||
Other Areas in cerebrum | ||||||
Parahippocampal gyrus | −21 | −14 | −20 | 2264 | 3.540 | |
−25 | −17 | −19 | 3.353 | |||
−21 | −18 | −15 | 3.239 | |||
−28 | −22 | −17 | 3.156 | |||
Somatomotor > mentalizing seeds in cerebellum | ||||||
Mirror Areas in cerebrum | ||||||
pSTS | 54 | −59 | 8 | 720 | 3.239 | |
55 | −59 | 4 | 2.794 | |||
47 | −54 | 2 | 2.848 | |||
50 | −55 | 1 | 2.748 | |||
Inferior frontal gyrus | −37 | 30 | 6 | 120 | 2.948 | |
−38 | 34 | 6 | 2.911 | |||
Other Somatomotor areas in cerebrum | ||||||
Precentral gyrus | −30 | −30 | 52 | 464 | 3.540 | |
Postcentral gyrus | −32 | −34 | 52 | 3.353 | ||
−34 | −29 | 54 | 3.156 |
Anatomical labels given according to the ALE atlas.
Volume in mm3 for each cluster; Value = ALE Extrema value.
Our expectation was that if the reinterpretation of cerebellar activity by Van Overwalle et al. (2014) based on the connectivity analysis by Buckner et al. (2011) is correct, we should find a strong and function‐specific connectivity between the cerebellum and the cerebrum for mentalizing as well as for somatomotor activity. We discuss the results for each network below.
Mentalizing Connectivity
Table 3 shows the connectivity (i.e., coactivation) results for mentalizing. The mentalizing ROIs in the cerebellum show strong connectivity with the mentalizing network in the cerebrum, including the dorsal and ventral parts of the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC), the temporo‐parietal junction (TPJ), posterior cingulate, and temporal pole. These are the largest clusters, spanning volumes of 704 – 11,912 mm3. These coactivations were revealed for all three mentalizing tasks, and for none of the mirror tasks. Apart from this, we also observed substantial connectivity in parahippocampal areas (2704–5936 mm3) and some temporal areas (< 1000 mm3). Figure 2 depicts the mentalizing connectivity clusters and illustrates a substantial similarity with the mentalizing network (red) of the cerebrum (Yeo et al., 2011).
Figure 2.
Functional connectivity in social mentalizing from the cerebellum to the cerebrum. All ALE clusters are significant at P < 0.05, FDR corrected, and with volume > 100 mm3. The bottom of the figure shows the 7‐network parcellation of the cerebrum (Yeo et al., 2011). Adapted with permission from Figure 11 of Yeo BTT, Krienen FM, Sepulcre J, Sabuncu MR, Lashkari D, Hollinshead M, Buckner RL, J Neurophysiol, 2011, 106, 1125–1165, © Americal Physiological Society
Table 5 shows the mentalizing > somatomotor contrast analysis which reveals that, in line with our expectations, only the mentalizing areas in the (ventral and dorsal) mPFC, TPJ, and posterior cingulate (but not the temporal pole) survived the contrast analysis, together with the parahippocampal areas.
Somatomotor Connectivity
Table 4 shows the connectivity (i.e., coactivation) results for the somatomotor networks. The somatomotor ROIs showed strong connectivity with the mirror/somatomotor network in the cerebrum. There was connectivity with all major areas of the mirror network, including the posterior superior temporal sulcus (pSTS), inferior frontal gyrus and parietal lobule, spanning volumes of 640–4248 mm3. In addition, connectivity with other somatomotor areas was found, including the precentral, postcentral and middle frontal gyri (792–1592 mm3), and the insula (896 mm3) which is responsible for somato‐sensory integration. These coactivatons were revealed not only for the mirror tasks as one might expect, but also for person trait tasks. Apart from this, there was also connectivity with mentalizing areas (< 1680 mm3), but only for person trait tasks. Figure 2 depicts the somatomotor connectivity with the somatomotor integration (green) network in the cerebrum predominantly, and also with the primary somatomotor (blue) network (Yeo et al., 2011).
Table 5 shows the somatomotor > mentalizing contrast. In line with our expectations, this analysis revealed that only the mirror areas in the pSTS and inferior frontal gyrus survived the contrast analysis, as well as the primary somatomotor areas in the precentral and postcentral gyrus.
DISCUSSION
This study investigated whether the mentalizing and mirroring clusters identified in a recent large‐scale meta‐analysis of social cognition in the cerebellum [Van Overwalle et al., 2014], are functionally connected with mentalizing and mirror/somatomotor networks in the cerebrum. To do so, we explored robust and systematic cerebellar‐cerebral coactivations across studies using ALE, as signatures of functional connectivity. We found substantial and function‐specific connectivity between the cerebellar clusters reported in the original meta‐analysis of Van Overwalle et al. (2014) and activation in the cerebrum. In line with our hypothesis, the mentalizing clusters in the cerebellum that are recruited mainly during mentalizing tasks about an event, person or more abstract judgments (past, future, or hypothetical events; Van Overwalle et al., 2015), were strongly and distinctly connected to the mentalizing network (red) in the cerebrum, including major areas of the mPFC, TPJ and posterior cingulate (Fig. 2). Likewise, the clusters in the cerebellum involving mainly mirroring and person trait tasks [Van Overwalle et al., 2015] were substantially and distinctly connected to the somatomotor areas in the cerebrum, including the typical mirror network (green) that encompasses the pSTS and inferior frontal gyrus, as well as the primary somatomotor (blue) network that includes the precentral and postcentral gyri (Fig. 3). Although mentalizing activity and connectivity prevailed for all three mentalizing processes involving event, person and abstract tasks (across cerebellum and cerebrum), there was a small exception when judging a person's traits (mainly the self; see Van Overwalle et al., 2014), which also revealed activity and connectivity of somatomotor networks. This finding confirms the original interpretation of Van Overwalle et al. (2014) that bodily experiences and awareness play a role in evaluating the self.
Figure 3.
Functional connectivity in somatomotor/mirroring processes from the cerebellum to the cerebrum. All ALE clusters are significant at P < 0.05, FDR corrected, and with volume > 100 mm3. The bottom of the figure shows the 7‐network parcellation of the cerebrum (Yeo et al., 2011). Adapted with permission from Figure 11 of Yeo BTT, Krienen FM, Sepulcre J, Sabuncu MR, Lashkari D, Hollinshead M, Buckner RL, J Neurophysiol, 2011, 106, 1125–1165, © Americal Physiological Society.
The present connectivity findings confirm the recent interpretation of the meta‐analytic results on distinct networks in the cerebellum by Van Overwalle et al. (2015), and thus shed new light on the function of the cerebellum in social cognition. In the original meta‐analysis by Van Overwalle et al. (2014), cerebellar activity was interpreted in relation to non‐social functions as described in other meta‐analyses of the cerebellum that investigated evolutionary “older” motor and emotional functions as well as “younger” executive and language functions [E et al., 2014; Stoodley and Schmahmann, 2009]. It was concluded that the cerebellum plays a general role in supporting social cognition across different functional tasks. The present analysis suggests a markedly different conclusion. Rather than a general function, the distinct connectivity patterns indicate that social cognition in the cerebellum triggers activity in function‐specific cerebellar networks that are connected to function‐specific networks in the cerebrum serving an identical function.
Moreover, the functional connectivity patterns identified in these analyses, provide further meta‐analytic support for the parcellation of the cerebellum conducted by Buckner et al. (2011; see also Buckner, 2013) founded on the functional connectivity with networks in the cerebral cortex (Yeo et al., 2011). While the connectivity results of this group of researchers were based on activity during idle rest in the scanner, the present study revealed the same connectivity patterns for some of the major networks while participants were actively engaged during various social judgment tasks. The present results are also consistent with earlier anatomical findings reporting topographically ordered reciprocal cerebellar‐cerebral loops via the pons (Schmahmann, 1996). Animal studies indicated that the majority of these connections are characterized by circuits that reflect predominantly contralateral closed loops [Kelly and Strick, 2003] although more open‐ended, ipsilateral loops have also been reported [Suzuki et al., 2012]. Human research exploring structural connectivity using diffusion imaging reported similar cerebellar‐cerebral circuits [Salmi et al., 2010; Sokolov et al., 2014].
An important limitation of the present approach is that functional connectivity was measured through shared activity across studies. Specifically, the analysis identified which brain areas reveal coactivation (i.e., peak coordinates reported together) and how systematic these coactivations are across studies. The unit of analysis was a complete study, not a participant. Hence, the present data do not demonstrate that real functional connectivity exists within individual brains of the participants. Another limitation of this study is that the direction of the connectivity was not assessed, because there was no access to the exact timing of the shared activity.
At a theoretical level, it remains unclear what specific purposes and processes the cerebellum serves in social cognition. Several authors [Andreasen and Pierson, 2008; Bower, 1997; Schmahmann, 1998] suggested that the cerebellum regulates mental operations in much the same way as it regulates movements. According to Schmahmann (1998), its general function is to facilitate “actions harmonious with the goal, appropriate to context, and judged accurately and reliably according to the strategies mapped out prior to and during the behavior. In this view, the cerebellum detects, prevents, and corrects mismatches between intended outcome and perceived outcome of the organism's interaction with the environment” (p. 367). And more specific to social cognition, he also noted that disturbed cerebellar functionality leads to “unpredictability to social and social interaction” (p. 368). Thus, the cerebellum might be a general modulator to prevent and correct errors of actual movement as well as unobserved thought, and it is doing so for distinct functional processes, including social cognitive functions.
The critical role of the cerebellum is perhaps better understood from the perspective of its essential function in constructing internal models of mental processes involving sequencing and planning of action, in order to automate and fine‐tune not only voluntary motor processes, but also cognitive processes where event sequences play a role [Ito, 2008; Pisotta and Molinari, 2014]. This sequencing role is most evident and prominent in mental reconstructions of autobiographic past, future or hypothetical events. Thus, to understand observed events and their underlying goal, or to infer traits from a person's observed behavior, it is imperative that sequences of actions can be imagined or interpreted into a meaningful whole. The cerebellum may play a functional role in this sequencing process during social cognition. One view [Ito, 2008; Pisotta and Molinari, 2014] suggests that such internal models are a copy from the social event implications generated in mentalizing areas in the cerebrum and allows humans to anticipate better action sequences during social interaction in an automatic and intuitive way and to fine‐tune these anticipations. Thus, signals from the cerebellum might continuously check whether an anticipated event sequence based on (abstract) social information fits with current behavior. For example, if we meet a neighbor whom we attribute a friendly trait to based on past observations, we learn to reciprocate with appropriate behaviors (e.g., we approach her with friendly remarks). This behavioral anticipation might be copied into and reconstructed by an internal model in the cerebellum holding predictable patterns of behavior and learned social responses. This allows us to act automatically and socially adept each time we see our neighbor, and to be quickly on alert when mismatches in these predictions occur.
This reasoning suggests that the role of the cerebellum might be relatively automated during the processing of typical events (revealing minimal neural activity), but less so in reconstructing novel past or future events, or in more complex events (revealing more activity). This is in line with the finding that the level of cerebellar recruitment differs between social tasks. The meta‐analysis by Van Overwalle et al. (2014) revealed higher involvement of the cerebellum (77% of the studies) in more abstract and complex social‐cognitive inferences (e.g., projecting oneself into future or hypothetical events, or recalling autobiographical past events), while cerebellar activity was more limited for ordinary social judgments (e.g., mirroring, event, and person mentalizing; 23%–32% of the studies). Interestingly, the present results showed stronger mentalizing connectivity also with parahippocampal gyri under abstract, often autobiographic task conditions. Likewise, recent findings from our lab indicate that the cerebellum is strongly recruited when trait‐implying behavioral descriptions are repeated, that is, when novel behaviors need to be integrated with prior behavioral information [Heleven and Van Overwalle, 2015; Ma et al., 2014a,b]. Hence, it is quite likely that in order to reconstruct or integrate appropriate sequences from past events or to construct de novo future or hypothetical events, extra cerebellar capacity has to be recruited.
Thus, although distinctly connected with function‐specific input and networks, the cerebellum might play a more common basic role in acquiring and predicting motor and cognitive sequences which underlie not only the understanding of planned and observed actions, but also the construction of internal mental models about current events, traits (abstracted out of events), and past, counterfactual or future autobiographic events. This function would be taxed more heavily when imagining novel or complex event sequences. These ideas concerning the role of the cerebellum are admittedly still at an early stage. One obvious avenue for future research is to test the idea that sequencing is an important aspect of the cerebellar function in social cognition.
CONCLUSION
The present connectivity analysis provides support for an alternative interpretation of the meta‐analysis of Van Overwalle et al. (2014). Instead of being involved in general non‐social functions as originally suggested in the 2014 article, the cerebellum seems to plays a crucial role in social cognition by function‐specific mentalizing and somatomotor processes. This conclusion is based on the distinct coactivation between these respective networks in the cerebellum and the cerebrum. Future research is needed to explore the functional connectivity at the level of individual participants, and to resolve theoretical questions with respect to the basic processes underlying cerebellar activity in social cognition. It is suggested that event sequences might be the common underlying functionality.
Correction added on 06 October 2015, after first online publication.
REFERENCES
- Andreasen NC, Pierson R (2008): The role of the cerebellum in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 64:81–88. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.01.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barrett LF, Satpute AB (2013): Large‐scale brain networks in affective and social neuroscience: towards an integrative functional architecture of the brain. Curr Opin Neurobiol 1–12. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2012.12.012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bower J (1997): Control of sensory data acquisition. Int Rev Neurobiol 41:489–513. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brett M, Christoff K, Cusack R, Lancaster J (2001): Using the talairach atlas with the MNI template. NeuroImage 13:85 doi: 10.1016/S1053-8119(01)9 1428‐4 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Buckner R, Krienen F, Castellanos A, Diaz JC, Yeo BT (2011): The organization of the human cerebellum estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. J Neurophysiol 106:2322–2345. doi: 10.1152/jn.00339.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buckner RL (2013): The cerebellum and cognitive function: 25 Years of insight from anatomy and neuroimaging. Neuron 80:807–815. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.10.044 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bzdok D, Schilbach L, Vogeley K, Schneider K, Laird AR, Langner R, Eickhoff SB (2012): Parsing the neural correlates of moral cognition: ALE meta‐analysis on morality, theory of mind, and empathy. Brain Struct Funct, 217:783–796. doi: 10.1007/s00429-012-0380-y [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carrington SJ, Bailey AJ (2009): Are there theory of mind regions in the brain? A review of the neuroimaging literature. Hum Brain Mapp 30:2313–2335. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20671 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Collins DL, Neelin P, Peters TM, Evans AC (1994): Automatic 3D intersubject registration of MR volumetric data in standardized Talairach space. J Comput Assisted Tomogr, 18:192–205. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Denny BT, Kober H, Wager TD, Ochsner KN (2012): A meta‐analysis of functional neuroimaging studies of self‐ and other judgments reveals a spatial gradient for mentalizing in medial prefrontal cortex. J Cogn Neurosci 24:1742–1752. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00233 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- E KH, Chen SHA, Ho MHR, Desmond JE (2014): A meta‐analysis of cerebellar contributions to higher cognition from PET and fMRI studies. Hum Brain Mapp 35:593–615. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22194 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eickhoff S, Laird A, Grefkes C. (2009): Coordinate‐based activation likelihood estimation meta‐analysis of neuroimaging data: A random‐effects approach based on empirical estimates of spatial. Hum Brain 2926: 2907–2926. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20718 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Habas C, Kamdar N, Nguyen D, Keller K, Beckamn CF, Menon V, Greicius MD (2009): Distinct cerebellar contributions to intrinsic connectivity networks. J Neurosci 29:8586–8594. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1868-09.2009.Distinct [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heleven E, Van Overwalle F. The person within: The memory code for agents and traits using fMRI repetition suppression. Social Cogn Affective Neuroscience, in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ito M (2008): Control of mental activities by internal models in the cerebellum. Nat Rev Neurosci 9:304–313. doi: 10.1038/nrn2332 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kelly RM, Strick PL (2003): Cerebellar loops with motor cortex and prefrontal cortex of a nonhuman primate. J Neurosci 23:8432–8444. doi:23/23/8432 [pii] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Laird AR, Fox PM, Price CJ, Glahn DC, Uecker AM, Lancaster JL, Fox PT (2005): ALE meta‐analysis: Controlling the false discovery rate and performing statistical contrasts. Hum Brain Mapp 25:155–164. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20136 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lombardo MV, Chakrabarti B, Bullmore ET, Baron‐Cohen S (2011): Specialization of right temporo‐parietal junction for mentalizing and its relation to social impairments in autism. NeuroImage 56:1832–1838. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.02.067 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ma N, Baetens K, Vandekerckhove M, Kestemont J, Fias W, Van Overwalle F (2014): Traits are represented in the medial prefrontal cortex: an fMRI adaptation study. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 9:1185–1192. doi: 10.1093/scan/nst098 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ma N, Baetens K, Vandekerckhove M, Van der Cruyssen L, Van Overwalle F (2014): Dissociation of a trait and a valence representation in the mPFC. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 9:1506–1514. doi: 10.1093/scan/nst143 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mar RA (2011): The neural bases of social cognition and story comprehension. Annu Rev Psychol, 62:103–134. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-120709-145406 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mars RB, Neubert FX, Noonan MP, Sallet J, Toni I, Rushworth MFS (2012): On the relationship between the “default mode network” and the “social brain”. Front Hum Neurosci 6:189 (June), doi: 10.3389/fnhum.201 2.00189 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martinelli P, Sperduti M, Piolino P. (2013). Neural substrates of the self‐memory system: New insights from a meta‐analysis. Hum Brain Mapp 34:1515–1529 .000. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Molenberghs P, Cunnington R, Mattingley JB (2012): Brain regions with mirror properties: a meta‐analysis of 125 human fMRI studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 36:341–349. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.07.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O'Reilly JX, Beckmann CF, Tomassini V, Ramnani N, Johansen‐Berg H. (2010):. Distinct and overlapping functional zones in the cerebellum defined by resting state functional connectivity. Cereb Cortex 20: 953–965. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp157 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pisotta I, Molinari M (2014):. Cerebellar contribution to feedforward control of locomotion. Front Hum Neurosci 8: 1–5. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00475 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Qin P, Liu Y, Shi J, Wang Y, Duncan N, Gong Q, Northoff G (2012): Dissociation between anterior and posterior cortical regions during self‐specificity and familiarity: A combined fMRI‐meta‐analytic study. Hum Brain Mapp 33:154–164. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21201 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Salmi J, Pallesen KJ, Neuvonen T, Brattico E, Korvenoja A, Salonen O, Carlson S (2010): Cognitive and motor loops of the human cerebro‐cerebellar system. J Cogn Neurosci 22:2663–2676. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21382 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schilbach L, Bzdok D, Timmermans B, Fox PT, Laird AR, Vogeley K, Eickhoff SB (2012): Introspective minds: Using ALE meta‐analyses to study commonalities in the neural correlates of emotional processing, social & unconstrained cognition. PloS One 7:e30920 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0 030920 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmahmann JD (1996): From movement to thought: Anatomic substrates of the cerebellar contribution to cognitive processing. Hum Brain Mapp 4:174–198. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1097‐0193(1996)4:3<174::AID‐HBM3>3.0.CO;2‐0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmahmann, J. D. (1998). Dysmetria of thought: Clinical consequences of cerebellar dysfunction on cognition and affect. Trends Cogn Sci 2:362–371. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21227233 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schurz M, Radua J, Aichhorn M, Richlan F, Perner J (2014): Fractionating theory of mind: a meta‐analysis of functional brain imaging studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 42C:9–34. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.01.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shkurko A (2013): Is social categorization based on relational ingroup/outgroup opposition? A meta‐analysis. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 8:80–87. Retrieved from http://scan.oxfordjournals.org/content/8/8/870.short [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sokolov Aa, Erb M, Grodd W, Pavlova Ma (2014): Structural loop between the cerebellum and the superior temporal sulcus: Evidence from diffusion tensor imaging. Cereb Cortex 24:626–632. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs346 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spreng RN, Mar Ra, Kim ASN (2009): The common neural basis of autobiographical memory, prospection, navigation, theory of mind, and the default mode: A quantitative meta‐analysis. J Cogn Neurosci 21:489–510. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.21029 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spreng RN, Mar RA, Kim ASN (2008): The common neural basis of autobiographical memory, prospection, navigation, theory of mind, and the default mode: A quantitative meta‐analysis. J Cogn Neurosci 21:489–510. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stoodley CJ, Schmahmann JD (2009): Functional topography in the human cerebellum: a meta‐analysis of neuroimaging studies. NeuroImage, 44:489–501. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.08.039 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Suzuki L, Coulon P, Sabel‐Goedknegt EH, Ruigrok TJH (2012): Organization of cerebral projections to identified cerebellar zones in the posterior cerebellum of the rat. J Neurosci 32:10854–10869. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0857-12.2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Svoboda E, McKinnon M, Levine B (2006): The functional neuroanatomy of autobiographical memory: a meta‐analysis. Neuropsychologia 44:2189–2208. Retrieved from http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0028393206002090 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Talairach J, Tournoux P. (1988): Co‐planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain. Stuttgart: Thieme. [Google Scholar]
- Trope Y, Liberman N (2010): Construal‐level theory of psychological distance. Psychol Rev 117:440–463. doi: 10.1037/a0018963 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van der Meer L, Costafreda S, Aleman A, David AS (2010): Self‐reflection and the brain: a theoretical review and meta‐analysis of neuroimaging studies with implications for schizophrenia. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 34:935–946. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.12.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Overwalle F (2009): Social cognition and the brain: a meta‐analysis. Hum Brain Mapp 30:829–858. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20547 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Overwalle F, Baetens K (2009): Understanding others' actions and goals by mirror and mentalizing systems: a meta‐analysis. NeuroImage 48:564–584. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.06.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Overwalle F, Baetens K, Mariën P, Vandekerckhove M. (2014). Social cognition and the cerebellum: A meta‐analysis of over 350 fMRI studies. NeuroImage. Retrieved from http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1053811913009695 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Overwalle F, Baetens K, Mariën P, Vandekerckhove M (2015): Cerebellar areas dedicated to social cognition? A comparison of meta‐analytic and connectivity results. Soc Neurosci 10:337–344 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yeo BTT, Krienen FM, Sepulcre J, Sabuncu MR, Lashkari D, Hollinshead M, Buckner RL (2011): The organization of the human cerebral cortex estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. J Neurophysiol 106:1125–1165. doi: 10.1152/jn.00338.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
REFERENCES FROM THE CONNECTIVITY ANALYSIS
- Addis DR, Knapp K, Roberts RP, Schacter DL (2012): Routes to the past: neural substrates of direct and generative autobiographical memory retrieval. NeuroImage 59:2908–2922. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.066 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Addis DR, Wong AT, Schacter DL (2007): Remembering the past and imagining the future: common and distinct neural substrates during event construction and elaboration. Neuropsychologia 45:1363–1377. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.10.016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Addis DR, Pan L, Vu MA, Laiser N, Schacter DL (2009): Constructive episodic simulation of the future and the past: distinct subsystems of a core brain network mediate imagining and remembering. Neuropsychologia 47:2222–2238. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.10.026 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Akitsuki Y, Decety J (2009): Social context and perceived agency affects empathy for pain: an event‐related fMRI investigation. NeuroImage 47:722–734. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.04.091 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arshamian A, Iannilli E, Gerber JC, Willander J, Persson J, Seo HS, Hummel T, M Larsson. (2013): The functional neuroanatomy of odor evoked autobiographical memories cued by odors and words. Neuropsychologia 51:123–131. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.10.023 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baetens K, Ma N, Steen J, Van Overwalle F. (2014): Involvement of the mentalizing network in social and non‐social high construal. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 9:817–824. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Baetens K, Ma N, Vandekerckhove M, Van Overwalle F. (2013b): The role of the dmPFC in high construal: Constraint or meaning? submitted.
- Baumgaertner A, Buccino G, Lange R, McNamara A, Binkofski F (2007): Polymodal conceptual processing of human biological actions in the left inferior frontal lobe. Eur J Neurosci 25:881–889. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05346.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berthoz S, Armony JL, Blair RJR, Dolan RJ (2002): An fMRI study of intentional and unintentional (embarrassing) violations of social norms. Brain 125:1696–1708. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12135962 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Biagi L, Cioni G, Fogassi L, Guzzetta A, Tosetti M (2010): Anterior intraparietal cortex codes complexity of observed hand movements. Brain Res Bull 81:434–440. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2009.12.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blakemore S‐J, Boyer P, Pachot‐Clouard M, Meltzoff a, Segebarth C, Decety J (2003): The detection of contingency and animacy from simple animations in the human brain. Cereb Cortex 13:837–844. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12853370 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blakemore S‐J, Bristow D, Bird G, Frith C, Ward J. (2005). Somatosensory activations during the observation of touch and a case of vision‐touch synaesthesia. Brain 128(Pt 7): 1571–1583. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh500 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buccino G, Lui F, Canessa N, Patteri I, Lagravinese G, Benuzzi F, Porro Ca, et al. (2004): Neural circuits involved in the recognition of actions performed by nonconspecifics: an FMRI study. J Cogn Neurosci 16:114–126. doi: 10.1162/089892904322755601 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Calvert GA, Campbell R (2003): Reading speech from still and moving faces: the neural substrates of visible speech. J Cogn Neurosci 15:57–70. doi: 10.1162/089892903321107828 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Calvo‐Merino B, Grèzes J, Glaser DE, Passingham RE, Haggard P (2006): Seeing or doing? Influence of visual and motor familiarity in action observation. Curr Biol 16:1905–1910. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.07.065 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chiao JY, Harada T, Komeda H, Li Z, Mano Y, Saito D, Parrish TB, et al. (2009): Neural basis of individualistic and collectivistic views of self. Hum Brain Mapp 30:2813–2820. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20707 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- D'Argembeau A, Stawarczyk D, Majerus S, Collette F, Van der Linden M, Salmon E (2010a): Modulation of medial prefrontal and inferior parietal cortices when thinking about past, present, and future selves. Soc Neurosci 5:187–200. doi: 10.1080/17470910903233562 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- D'Argembeau A, Stawarczyk D, Majerus S, Collette F, Van der Linden M, Feyers D, Maquet P, et al. (2010b): The neural basis of personal goal processing when envisioning future events. J Cogn Neurosci 22:1701–1713. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21314 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Daselaar SM, Rice HJ, Greenberg DL, Cabeza R, LaBar KS, Rubin DC (2008): The spatiotemporal dynamics of autobiographical memory: neural correlates of recall, emotional intensity, and reliving. Cereb Cortex 18:217–229. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm048 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Decety J, Jackson PL, Sommerville Ja, Chaminade T, Meltzoff AN (2004): The neural bases of cooperation and competition: an fMRI investigation. NeuroImage 23:744–751. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.05.025 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Denkova E, Botzung A, Scheiber C, Manning L (2006): Implicit emotion during recollection of past events: a nonverbal fMRI study. Brain Res 1078:143–150. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.01.061 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eich E, Nelson AL, Leghari MA, Handy TC (2009): Neural systems mediating field and observer memories. Neuropsychologia 47:2239–2251. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.02.019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Engel L, Frum C, Puce A, Walker N, Lewis J (2009): Different categories of living and non‐living sound‐sources activate distinct cortical networks. Neuroimage 47:1778–1791. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.05.041.Different [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Farrow TFD, Jones SC, Kaylor‐Hughes CJ, Wilkinson ID, Woodruff PWR, Hunter MD, Spence Sa (2011): Higher or lower? The functional anatomy of perceived allocentric social hierarchies. NeuroImage 57:1552–1560. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.05.069 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fossati P, Hevenor SJ, Lepage M, Graham SJ, Grady C, Keightley ML, Craik F, et al. (2004): Distributed self in episodic memory: neural correlates of successful retrieval of self‐encoded positive and negative personality traits. NeuroImage 22:1596–1604. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.03.034 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gallagher HL, Frith CD (2004): Dissociable neural pathways for the perception and recognition of expressive and instrumental gestures. Neuropsychologia 42:1725–1736. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.05.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gazzola V, Van der Worp H, Mulder T, Wicker B, Rizzolatti G, Keysers C (2007): Aplasics born without hands mirror the goal of hand actions with their feet. Curr Biol 17:1235–1240. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.06.045 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- German TP, Niehaus JL, Roarty MP, Giesbrecht B, Miller MB (2004): Neural correlates of detecting pretense: automatic engagement of the intentional stance under covert conditions. J Cogn Neurosci 16:1805–1817. doi: 10.1162/0898929042947892 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gilead M, Liberman N, Maril A (2014): From mind to matter: neural correlates of abstract and concrete mindsets. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 9:638–645. doi: 10.1093/scan/nst031 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gobbini M, Ida Leibenluft E, Santiago N Haxby JV (2004): Social and emotional attachment in the neural representation of faces. NeuroImage, 22:1628–1635. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.03.049 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gobbini Maria Ida Koralek AC, Bryan RE, Montgomery KJ Haxby JV (2007): Two takes on the social brain: A comparison of theory of mind tasks. J Cogn Neurosci 19:1803–1814. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.11.1803 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grèzes J, Frith C, Passingham R (2004a): Brain mechanisms for inferring deceit in the actions of others. J Neurosci 24:5500 –5505. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0219-04.2004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grèzes J, Frith C, Passingham R (2004b): Inferring false beliefs from the actions of oneself and others: an fMRI study. Neuroimage 21:744 –750. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.10.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grosbras MH, Paus T (2006): Brain networks involved in viewing angry hands or faces. Cereb Cortex 16:1087–1096. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhj050 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hamilton AFDC, Grafton ST (2006): Goal representation in human anterior intraparietal sulcus. J Neurosci 26:1133–1137. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4551-05.2006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harada T, Li Z, Chiao JY (2010): Differential dorsal and ventral medial prefrontal representations of the implicit self modulated by individualism and collectivism: An fMRI study. Soc Neurosci 5:257–271. doi: 10.1080/17470910903374895 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hartwright CE, Apperly IA, Hansen PC (2012): Multiple roles for executive control in belief‐desire reasoning: Distinct neural networks are recruited for self perspective inhibition and complexity of reasoning. NeuroImage 61:921–930. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.03.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hoscheidt SM, Nadel L, Payne J, Ryan L (2010): Hippocampal activation during retrieval of spatial context from episodic and semantic memory. Behav Brain Res 212:121–132. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.04.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Iacoboni M, Lieberman MD, Knowlton BJ, Molnar‐Szakacs I, Moritz M, Throop CJ, Fiske AP (2004): Watching social interactions produces dorsomedial prefrontal and medial parietal BOLD fMRI signal increases compared to a resting baseline. NeuroImage 21:1167–1173. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.11.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Iacoboni M, Molnar‐Szakacs I, Gallese V, Buccino G, Mazziotta JC, Rizzolatti G (2005): Grasping the intentions of others with one's own mirror neuron system. PLoS Biology, 3:e79 doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0 030079 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ino T, Nakai R, Azuma T, Kimura T, Fukuyama H (2011): Brain activation during autobiographical memory retrieval with special reference to default mode network. Open Neuroimag J 5:14–23. doi: 10.2174/1874440001105010014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Izuma K, Saito DN, Sadato N (2010): The roles of the medial prefrontal cortex and striatum in reputation processing. Soc Neurosci 5:133–147. doi: 10.1080/17470910903202559 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jenkins AC, Mitchell JP (2010): Mentalizing under uncertainty: Dissociated neural responses to ambiguous and unambiguous mental state inferences. Cereb Cortex 20:404–410. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp109 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson SC, Baxter LC, Wilder LS, Pipe JG, Heiserman JE, Prigatano GP (2002): Neural correlates of self‐reflection. Brain 125:1808–1814. doi: 10.1093/brain/awf181 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim Y, Lee J, Song H, Kim J, Kwon D, Kim M, Yoo D, et al. (2010): Alterations in cortical activity of male methamphetamine abusers performing an empathy task: fMRI study. 63–70. November 2009), doi: 10.1002/hup [DOI] [PubMed]
- Kircher T, Blümel I, Marjoram D, Lataster T, Krabbendam L, Weber J, Krach S (2009): Online mentalising investigated with functional MRI. Neurosci Lett 454:176–181. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2009.03.026 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kircher TT, Senior C, Phillips ML, Rabe‐Hesketh S, Benson PJ, Bullmore ET, Brammer M, et al. (2001): Recognizing one's own face. Cognition 78:B1–B15. doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(00)00104-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kircher TT, Senior C, Phillips ML, Benson PJ, Bullmore ET, Brammer M, Simmons a, et al. (2000): Towards a functional neuroanatomy of self processing: effects of faces and words. Brain Res 10:133–144. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10978701 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krach S, Blümel I, Marjoram D, Lataster T, Krabbendam L, Weber J, Van Os J, et al. (2009): Are women better mindreaders? Sex differences in neural correlates of mentalizing detected with functional MRI. BMC Neurosci 10:9 doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-10-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kumaran D, Maguire Ea (2005): The human hippocampus: Cognitive maps or relational memory? J Neurosci 25:7254–7259. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1103-05.2005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuzmanovic B, Bente G, Von Cramon DY, Schilbach L, Tittgemeyer M, Vogeley K (2012): Imaging first impressions: Distinct neural processing of verbal and nonverbal social information. NeuroImage 60:179–188. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.12.046 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee SM, Gao T, McCarthy G (2012a): Attributing intentions to random motion engages the posterior superior temporal sulcus. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 1–7. doi: 10.1093/scan/nss110 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee W, Reeve J (2013): Self‐determined, but not non‐self‐determined, motivation predicts activations in the anterior insular cortex: an fMRI study of personal agency. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 8:538–545. doi: 10.1093/scan/nss029 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levine B, Turner GR, Tisserand D, Hevenor SJ, Graham SJ, McIntosh AR (2004): The functional neuroanatomy of episodic and semantic autobiographical remembering: A prospective functional MRI study. J Cogn Neurosci 16:1633–1646. doi: 10.1162/0898929042568587 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lombardo MV, Chakrabarti B, Bullmore ET, Wheelwright SJ, Sadek Sa, Suckling J, Baron‐Cohen S (2010): Shared neural circuits for mentalizing about the self and others. J Cogn Neurosci 22:1623–1635. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21287 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ma N, Vandekerckhove M, Baetens K, Van Overwalle F, Seurinck R, Fias W (2012a): Inconsistencies in spontaneous and intentional trait inferences. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 7:937–950. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsr064 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ma N, Vandekerckhove M, Hoeck N Van, Overwalle F Van. (2012b): Distinct recruitment of temporo‐parietal junction and medial prefrontal cortex in behavior understanding and trait identification. Soc Neurosci 7:591–605. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maguire Ea, Mummery CJ, Büchel C (2000): Patterns of hippocampal‐cortical interaction dissociate temporal lobe memory subsystems. Hippocampus 10:475–482. doi:10.1002/1098‐1063(2000)10:4 < 475::AID‐HIPO14 > 3.0.CO;2‐X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maguire EA, Frith CD (2003): Aging affects the engagement of the hippocampus during autobiographical memory retrieval. Brain 126(Pt 7), 1511–1523. doi: 10.1093/brain/awg157 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Malfait N, Valyear KF, Culham JC, Anton JL, Brown LE, Gribble PL (2010): fMRI activation during observation of others' reach errors. J Cogn Neurosci 22:1493–1503. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21281 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Matura S, Muth K, Magerkurth J, Walter H, Klein J, Haenschel C, Pantel J (2012): Neural correlates of autobiographical memory in amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Psychiatry Res 201:159–167. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2011.06.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McAdams CJ, Krawczyk DC (2012): Who am I? How do I look? Neural differences in self‐identity in anorexia nervosa. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 9:12–21.doi: 10.1093/scan/nss093 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mende‐Siedlecki P, Cai Y, Todorov A (2012): The neural dynamics of updating person impressions. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 8:623–631.doi: 10.1093/scan/nss040 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meyer ML, Spunt RP, Berkman ET, Taylor SE, Lieberman MD (2012): Evidence for social working memory from a parametric functional MRI study. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 109:1883–1888. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1121077109 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Milton F, Butler CR, Benattayallah a, Zeman AZJ (2012): The neural basis of autobiographical memory deficits in transient epileptic amnesia. Neuropsychologia 50:3528–3541. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.09.027 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miyamoto R, Kikuchi Y (2012): Gender differences of brain activity in the conflicts based on implicit self‐esteem. PloS One 7:e37901 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0037901 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moll J, Eslinger PJ, Oliveira‐Souza R (2001): Frontopolar and anterior temporal cortex activation in a moral judgment task: Preliminary functional MRI results in normal subjects. Arquivos De neuro‐Psiquiatria 59:657–664. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11593260 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moriguchi Y, Ohnishi T, Decety J, Hirakata M, Maeda M, Matsuda H, Komaki G (2009): The human mirror neuron system in a population with deficient self‐awareness: an fMRI study in alexithymia. Hum Brain Mapp 30:2063–2076. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20653 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moriguchi Y, Ohnishi T, Lane RD, Maeda M, Mori T, Nemoto K, Matsuda H, et al. (2006): Impaired self‐awareness and theory of mind: An fMRI study of mentalizing in alexithymia. NeuroImage 32:1472–1482. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.04.186 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nedelko V, Hassa T, Hamzei F, Weiller C, Binkofski F, Schoenfeld MA, Tüscher O, et al. (2010): Age‐independent activation in areas of the mirror neuron system during action observation and action imagery. A fMRI study. Restorat Neurol Neurosci 28:737–747. doi: 10.3233/RNN-2010-0542 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oddo S, Lux S, Weiss PH, Schwab A, Welzer H, Markowitsch HJ, Fink GR (2010): Specific role of medial prefrontal cortex in retrieving recent autobiographical memories: an fMRI study of young female subjects. Cortex 46:29–39. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2008.07.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ohnishi T, Moriguchi Y, Matsuda H, Mori T, Hirakata M, Imabayashi E, Hirao K, et al. (2004): The neural network for the mirror system and mentalizing in normally developed children: an fMRI study. Neuroreport 15:1483–1487. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pfeifer JH, Masten CL, Borofsky La, Dapretto M, Fuligni AJ, Lieberman MD (2009): Neural correlates of direct and reflected self‐appraisals in adolescents and adults: when social perspective‐taking informs self‐perception. Child Dev 80:1016–1038. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01314.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Piefke M (2003): Differential remoteness and emotional tone modulate the neural correlates of autobiographical memory. Brain 126:650–668. doi: 10.1093/brain/awg064 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pilgramm S, Lorey B, Stark R, Vaitl JM, Zentgraf D K, (2010): Differential activation of the lateral premotor cortex during action observation. BMC Neuroscience 11:89. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Poettrich K, Weiss PH, Werner A, Lux S, Donix M, Gerber J, Von Kummer R, et al. (2009): Altered neural network supporting declarative long‐term memory in mild cognitive impairment. Neurobiol Aging 30:284–298. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2007.05.027 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Powell LJ, Macrae CN, Cloutier J, Metcalfe J, Mitchell JP (2010): Dissociable neural substrates for agentic versus conceptual representations of self. J Cogn Neurosci 22:2186–2197. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21368 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Quadflieg S, Turk DJ, Waiter GD, Mitchell JP, Jenkins AC, Macrae CN (2009): Exploring the neural correlates of social stereotyping. J Cogn Neurosci 21:1560–1570. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21091 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rabin JS, Rosenbaum RS (2012): Familiarity modulates the functional relationship between theory of mind and autobiographical memory. NeuroImage 62:520–529. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.05.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rameson LT, Satpute AB, Lieberman MD (2010): The neural correlates of implicit and explicit self‐relevant processing. NeuroImage 50:701–708. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.098 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ramnani N, Miall RC (2004): A system in the human brain for predicting the actions of others. Nat Neurosci 7:85–90. doi: 10.1038/nn1168 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rekkas PV, Constable RT (2006): Hemodynamic retrieval intensity in hippocampus is decreased by pre‐exposure to autobiographic test items. Brain Res Bull 70:467–473. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2006.07.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rekkas PV, Constable RT (2005): Evidence that autobiographic memory retrieval does not become independent of the hippocampus: an fMRI study contrasting very recent with remote events. J Cogn Neurosci 17:1950–1961. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16475281 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rekkas PV, Westerveld M, Skudlarski P, Zumer J, Pugh K, Spencer DD, Constable RT (2005): Neural correlates of temporal‐order judgments versus those of spatial‐location: Deactivation of hippocampus may facilitate spatial performance. Brain Cogn 59:103–113. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2005.05.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ryan L, Nadel L, Keil K, Putnam K, Schnyer D, Trouard T, Moscovitch M (2001): Hippocampal complex and retrieval of recent and very remote autobiographical memories: evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging in neurologically intact people. Hippocampus 11:707–714. doi: 10.1002/hipo.1086 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Samson AC, Zysset S, Huber O (2008): Cognitive humor processing: different logical mechanisms in nonverbal cartoons—An fMRI study. Soc Neurosci 3:125–140. doi: 10.1080/17470910701745858 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Santi A, Servos P, Vatikiotis‐Bateson E, Kuratate T, Munhall K (2003): Perceiving biological motion: Dissociating visible speech from walking. J Cogn Neurosci 15:800–809. doi: 10.1162/089892903322370726 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schiller D, Freeman JB, Mitchell JP, Uleman JS, Phelps Ea (2009): A neural mechanism of first impressions. Nat Neurosci 12:508–514. doi: 10.1038/nn.2278 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmitz TW, Johnson SC (2006): Self‐appraisal decisions evoke dissociated dorsal‐ventral aMPFC networks. NeuroImage 30:1050–1058. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.10.030 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shane MS, Stevens M, Harenski CL Kiehl K, a (2008): Neural correlates of the processing of another's mistakes: a possible underpinning for social and observational learning. NeuroImage 42:450–459. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.12.067 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sheldon S, Moscovitch M (2012): The nature and time‐course of medial temporal lobe contributions to semantic retrieval: An fMRI study on verbal fluency. Hippocampus 22:1451–1466. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20985 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spengler S, Von Cramon DY, Brass M (2009): Control of shared representations relies on key processes involved in mental state attribution. Hum Brain Mapp 30:3704–3718. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20800 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spiers HJ, Maguire Ea (2006): Spontaneous mentalizing during an interactive real world task: An fMRI study. Neuropsychologia 44:1674–1682. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.03.028 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spreng RN, Grady CL (2010): Patterns of brain activity supporting autobiographical memory, prospection, and theory of mind, and their relationship to the default mode network. J Cogn Neurosci 22:1112–1123. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21282 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spunt RP, Falk EB, Lieberman MD (2010): Dissociable neural systems support retrieval of how and why action knowledge. Psychol Sci 21:1593–1598. doi: 10.1177/0956797610386618 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spunt, Robert P Lieberman MD (2012a): An integrative model of the neural systems supporting the comprehension of observed emotional behavior. NeuroImage 59:3050–3059. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.10.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spunt, Robert P Lieberman MD (2012b): Dissociating modality‐specific and supramodal neural systems for action understanding. J Neurosci 32:3575–3583. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5715-11.2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- St‐Laurent M, Abdi H, Burianová H, Grady CL (2011): Influence of aging on the neural correlates of autobiographical, episodic, and semantic memory retrieval. J Cogn Neurosci 23:4150–4163. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00079 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Svoboda E, Levine B (2009): The effects of rehearsal on the functional neuroanatomy of episodic autobiographical and semantic remembering: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. The. J Neurosci 29:3073–3082. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3452-08.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Szpunar KK, Watson JM, McDermott KB (2007): Neural substrates of envisioning the future. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 104:642–647. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0610082104 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tamir DI, Mitchell JP (2011): The default network distinguishes construals of proximal versus distal events. J Cogn Neurosci 23:2945–2955. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van der Cruyssen L, Heleven E, Ma N, Vandekerckhove M, Van Overwalle F. (2015): Distinct neural correlates of social categories and personality traits. NeuroImage in press. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Hoeck N, Ma N, Ampe L, Baetens K, Vandekerckhove M, Van Overwalle F. (2013): Counterfactual thinking: an fMRI study on changing the past for a better future. Social cognitive and affective neuroscience. doi: 10.1093/scan/nss031 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vandekerckhove MMP, Markowitsch HJ, Mertens M, Woermann FG (2005): Bi‐hemispheric engagement in the retrieval of autobiographical episodes. Behav Neurol 16:203–210. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16518010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Viard A, Chételat G, Lebreton K, Desgranges B, Landeau B, De La Sayette V, Eustache F, et al. (2011): Mental time travel into the past and the future in healthy aged adults: an fMRI study. Brain Cogn 75:1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2010.10.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Völlm Ba, Taylor ANW, Richardson P, Corcoran R, Stirling J, McKie S, Deakin JFW, et al. (2006): Neuronal correlates of theory of mind and empathy: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study in a nonverbal task. NeuroImage 29:90–98. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.07.022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Walter H, Adenzato M, Ciaramidaro A, Enrici I, Pia L, Bara BG (2004): Understanding intentions in social interaction: the role of the anterior paracingulate cortex. J Cogn Neurosci 16:1854–1863. doi: 10.1162/0898929042947838 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Whitehead C, Marchant JL, Craik D, Frith CD (2009): Neural correlates of observing pretend play in which one object is represented as another. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 4:369–378. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsp021 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson, S. M. , Molnar‐Szakacs, I. , & Iacoboni, M. (2008). Beyond superior temporal cortex: intersubject correlations in narrative speech comprehension. Cereb Cortex 18, 230–242. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm049 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yaoi K, Osaka N, Osaka M (2009): Is the self special in the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex? An fMRI study. Soc Neurosci 4:455–463. doi: 10.1080/17470910903027808 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Young, K. D. , Bellgowan, P. S. F. , Bodurka, J. , & Drevets, W. C. (2012). Functional neuroimaging of sex differences in autobiographical memory recall. Hum Brain Mapp 34:3320–3332. 000(May). doi: 10.1002/hbm.22144 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Young K, Erickson K, Nugent A, Fromm SJ, Mallinger AG, Furey ML, Drevets WC (2011): Functional anatomy of autobiographical memory recall deficits in depression. Psychol Med 42:345–357. Retrieved from http://journals.cambridge.org/production/action/cjoGetFulltext?fulltextid=8468476 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zaki J, Hennigan K, Weber J, Ochsner KN (2010): Social cognitive conflict resolution: contributions of domain‐general and domain‐specific neural systems. J Neurosci 30:8481–8488. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0382-10.2010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]