Abstract
Cortical activity has been shown to correlate with different parameters of movement. However, the dynamic properties of cortico‐motor mappings still remain unexplored in humans. Here, we show that during the repetition of simple stereotyped wrist movements both stable and unstable correlates simultaneously emerge in human sensorimotor cortex. Using visual feedback of wrist movement target inferred online from MEG, we assessed the dynamics of the tuning properties of two neuronal signals: the MEG signal below 1.6 Hz and within the 4 to 6 Hz range. We found that both components are modulated by wrist movement allowing for closed‐loop inference of movement targets. Interestingly, while tuning of 4 to 6 Hz signals remained stable over time leading to stable inference of movement target using a static classifier, the tuning of cortical signals below 1.6 Hz significantly changed resulting in steadily decreasing inference accuracy. Our findings demonstrate that non‐invasive neuronal population signals in human sensorimotor cortex can reflect a stable correlate of voluntary movements. Hence, we provide first evidence for a stable control signal in non‐invasive human brain‐machine interface research. However, as not all neuronal signals initially tuned to movement were stable across days, a careful selection of features for real‐life applications seems to be mandatory. Hum Brain Mapp 35:3867–3879, 2014. © 2014 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.
Keywords: brain‐machine interface, classification, magnetoencephalography, movement decoding, stability
INTRODUCTION
Previous studies on voluntary movements showed that cortical activity correlates with muscle activations and movement parameters across different spatial scales from single cells to neuronal populations [Carmena et al., 2003; Cheney and Fetz, 1980; Georgopoulos et al., 1982, 1986; Mehring et al., 2003; Schalk et al., 2008; Waldert et al., 2008]. More recent studies also inferred hand movement parameters such as speed, position, direction, velocity and grasp type in humans [Bradberry et al., 2009; Georgopoulos et al., 2005; Jerbi et al., 2007; Pistohl et al., 2008, 2011; Schalk et al., 2007; Waldert et al., 2008]. Most of these findings were obtained by analyzing recordings from single experimental sessions. The stability of neuronal correlates of movement across sessions or even days has only been investigated in non‐human primate studies, either corroborating [Chao et al., 2010; Chestek et al., 2007; Ganguly and Carmena 2009; Ince et al., 2010; Serruya et al., 2002; Stevenson et al., 2011] or questioning stability [Carmena et al., 2005; Padoa‐Schioppa et al., 2004; Rokni et al., 2007]. Whether the correlation between brain activity and motor behavior in humans shows similar inconsistencies over time has remained unexplored.
To our knowledge, this question has not yet been addressed although its answer is relevant to both basic neuroscience and brain‐machine interface research: first, the stability of a movement‐related neuronal signal may help to clarify whether natural motor control relies on a consistent relation between neuronal activity and behavior. Second, stable components could provide a robust control signal in brain‐machine interfaces where a reliable mapping of neuronal activity to external devices is needed [Nicolelis and Lebedev, 2009; Rossini, 2009]. Therefore, we assessed the temporal properties of neural correlates of wrist movement target in humans. To this end, healthy subjects performed stereotyped movements and received closed‐loop visual feedback of movement target inferred from magnetoencephalographic (MEG) signals. Movement inference over 18 sessions, spanning three days, was based on a static classification algorithm. Hence, the unchanged cortico‐motor coupling in our experiments allowed us to investigate changes in the stability of movement‐related activity across days, as indicated by the classifier's accuracy and the evolution of tuning of population activity to different targets. We demonstrate a stable correlate of motor behavior in human sensorimotor cortex that allows for closed‐loop inference.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Recordings
Brain activity was measured using a 275‐sensor whole‐head MEG system (VSM MedTech Ltd., Vancouver, Canada) that is based on first‐order axial gradiometers (1.8 cm coil diameter; 5 cm baseline; 2.2 cm intersensor spacing) and has a noise level of 10 fT/√Hz. All experiments were carried out in an electromagnetically shielded room (constructed by Vakuum‐Schmelze Hanau, Germany). For obtaining a head position template as well as continuous monitoring of head movements three localization coils were placed on the subject's head: one at the nasion (activated at 1,423 Hz) and two at the preauricular fiducial points (left coil activated at 1,475 Hz; right coil activated at 1,526 Hz).
Electro‐oculograms (EOG) of both eyes were recorded via bipolar electrodes (impedances <5 kΩ) connected to an EEG head box (VSM MedTech) with reference on the clavicle. Horizontal EOG was recorded by placing an electrode to the outer canthi of each eye, vertical EOG by an electrode pair above/below subjects' left eye. All signals were sampled simultaneously at a rate of 1,172 Hz and low‐pass filtered at 391 Hz during acquisition.
Subject's wrist movements were detected using a custom built MEG compatible, nonmagnetic joystick. The position signals of the joystick were transmitted via optical cables and recorded as an external channel by the MEG system.
Experimental Protocol
Fourteen healthy right‐handed subjects (six males, 24 ± 2 years, handedness according to modified Oldfield score 91 ± 11, see http://www.brainmapping.org) participated in this study after giving informed consent. All experimental procedures were approved by the ethics committee of the medical faculty of the University of Tübingen and were in compliance with the Declaration of Helsinki. We randomly assigned subjects to one of two groups: in one group band‐pass (4–6 Hz) filtered signals were used for brain‐control, in the other group (six subjects) the low‐pass (<1.6 Hz) signals were used for brain‐control (for details see subsection MEG processing below). Subjects were instructed to move a non‐magnetic joystick left‐ or rightwards from a centre position by performing fast, stereotyped radial‐ulnar deviations of the wrist. The joystick could only be moved right‐ and leftwards and its maximum deflection was 3.25 cm (6.6 cm arc of a circle, 29°). To minimize movement variability due to postural changes, the forearm was gently pushed into orthopedic memory foam (AT Kunststoffe, Ahaus, Germany) and further fixed in prone position using a flexible hook‐and‐loop tape (Fig. 1A). The head's position inside the MEG device was stabilized with an inflatable air‐pillow to prevent any major head movements during task execution (mean head movement across subjects and runs was 2.9 ± 1.4 mm).
On day 1 we first recorded six calibration sessions (CS) of 50 trials each where the two movement targets were presented in a randomized and balanced way. The data of the CS were used to build a classifier which can infer wrist movement target from MEG sensor activity (see data analysis for details). After subjects took a break outside the MEG chamber for approximately 30 min they performed six brain‐control sessions (BS) of 40 trials each with visual feedback showing the classifier's output. In the BS the classifier built from the CS was held constant. This block of closed‐loop brain‐control sessions was then repeated twice (additional 12 BS) 2 days later with a break of ∼25 min between sessions 7 to 12 and 13 to 18 (Fig. 1B). Foam imprints and head positioning templates obtained during the first CS were used in all subsequent CS and BS to minimize postural sources of variability. Each trial started with the presentation of a small white fixation cross projected against a black background on a screen (42 × 32 cm, 800 × 600 pixels) approximately 60 cm in front of the subject. After 1 s the cross changed color in a randomized and balanced way indicating the upcoming movement target: red equivalent for movements to the right and purple for movements to the left. Following a delay period of 700 ms where the cross color was changed back to white, only the outline of the cross was shown to indicate a “GO” signal. Subjects had to complete the movement within 1 s after which a vertical bar appeared on the screen for providing feedback for another 1.5 s: during the CS the bar was filled with green color in case of correct timing of the movement while white color indicated wrong timing or movement to wrong target. During the BS the visual feedback was coupled to the classifiers' output: while gradual filling of the upper half of the bar showed the classifier's posterior probability of the executed movement when it was above 50% for the correct movement target, the lower half indicated less than 50% for the target. During the BS we additionally presented a monetary reward for the subject that was increased or decreased depending on the classifier's performance. Thereby, we informed subjects about the cumulative performance within a run and introduced a motivational factor. In the following 3 s intertrial interval subjects moved the joystick back to the initial center position marked by the appearance of a white square on the screen. The center position covered a range of ±7% of maximum joystick deflection.
To implement the closed‐loop visual feedback paradigm we utilized the general‐purpose platform BCI2000 [Schalk et al., 2004]. All signals sampled by VSM hardware were buffered in blocks of 82 samples (∼70 ms) and forwarded via Ethernet to a separate machine running the BCI2000 software [for details see Mellinger et al., 2007]. Here, the data blocks were stored and processed in the pipeline shown in Figure 1C.
Data Analysis
Preprocessing
During acquisition all electrophysiological signals were high‐pass filtered (0.4 Hz, first‐order IIR, zero phase shifts) to remove slow drifts. Then synthetic third‐order gradiometer formation, i.e. subtraction of a linear combination of reference MEG sensors, was applied to MEG sensor signals to reduce environmental noise and enhance low‐frequency resolution [Vrba et al., 1999]. To minimize the jitter introduced by reaction time and execution variability, single‐trial epochs were extracted from 1,120 ms before to 1,400 ms after the alignment point which was set at 50% of maximum joystick's deflection. Finally, trials with no movement, movement to the wrong target or a movement with a reaction time above five times the median absolute deviation were excluded. No further artifact rejection was performed for the closed‐loop experiments where this is commonly skipped due to constraints on time, computation and reliability in real‐time applications.
To assess potential influences of artifacts from eye and muscle activity on the stability of brain components, the classification procedure described below was repeated post hoc after excluding noisy trials. To this end, epochs of interest (see MEG processing) for all MEG sensors were band‐pass filtered 1 to 15 Hz (fourth‐order butterworth) to detect eye movements and band‐pass filtered 110 to 140 Hz (eighth‐order butterworth) to reveal muscle artifacts. Then the amplitude of each sensor (calculated using the Hilbert transform) was z‐normalized and subsequently averaged over all MEG sensors. Whenever the resulting single time series reached a threshold of z ≥ 4 and the EOG or respectively EMG signals supported the occurrence of an artifact, the affected trial was rejected. Moreover, all trials were visually inspected for signal jumps and slow drifts.
As the above procedures for artifact removal had only a very small effect on the classification accuracies (see Results section) we performed most of the offline analyses also without the artifact removal to allow for a better comparison with the results from the online, closed‐loop experiments.
MEG processing
Based on Waldert et al. [2008] showing maximum directional tuning in low‐frequency MEG signals, we decided to assess the stability of two different slow cortical components: below 1.6 Hz and 4 to 6 Hz band. The former was obtained using a Savitzky‐Golay smoothing filter (second order, 630 ms window), the latter using a 4 to 6 Hz Butterworth band‐pass filter (third order, zero phase shift). Once filtered, the signals of the first and last 490 ms of each epoch were discarded as filter transients and thus, the final epochs spanned from 630 ms before to 910 ms after the alignment point.
EOG processing
Following the procedure for MEG signals, epochs of electrical activity from EOG were obtained and low‐pass filtered with a cutoff at 30 Hz (third‐order Butterworth, zero phase shifts).
Inference of Wrist Movement Target From Electrophysiological Signals
To infer wrist movement target from both aforementioned slow MEG components we used regularized linear discriminant analysis (RLDA, see classifier description below) as classifier. The features used for classification comprised the filtered signals from the 50 MEG sensors covering bilateral sensorimotor areas (Fig. 2E) at one single time sample. For each subject, two parameters were optimized with respect to classification accuracy (CA): the time sample within the trial and the regularization parameter λ (see classifier description below). We estimated the CA across the parameter space by two means: by leave‐one‐out cross‐validation (LCV) on the first four runs of the CS, and by using the first four runs of the CS as training set and the last two as validation set (intersession validation). We finally selected the parameters that yielded maximum CA for LCV and for which the absolute difference of the CAs between both procedures was below 5%. With these criteria we accounted for both: changes between single runs and biased estimations when using LCV on non‐stationary data.
To assess whether wrist movement target could be inferred from eye movements we applied the same classification procedure to the signals from vertical and horizontal EOG.
Linear Classifier
Regularized linear discriminant analysis (RLDA [Friedman, 1989]) was used to infer movement target from a signal vector of N = 50 MEG sensors. For RLDA the likelihood functions are modeled as multivariate Gaussian distributions according to
(1) |
where s depicts the 50 dimensional signal vector comprising the filtered signals of 50 MEG sensors at a certain point in time. The common, i.e. target independent, covariance matrix C and the movement target specific mean signal vectors µ target m were estimated from the data recorded during CS. To avoid singular covariance matrix estimates and poor generalization due to over‐fitting, we employed a regularization technique and used the following covariance matrix during decoding:
(2) |
where λ is the regularization parameter, C the full covariance matrix estimated from the training data and D represents a diagonal matrix with diagonal elements d = tr(C)/N. λ can take any value between 0 and 1, where 0 represents no regularization and 1 maximal regularization. During decoding the posterior probabilities were computed using Bayes' rule
(3) |
where a uniform prior with p(targetj) = 0.5 was used. The movement target with the highest posterior probability was selected. During brain control sessions the exact value of the posterior probability was also used to generate the visual feedback (see above).
Cortical Sources of Activity
We estimated the neuronal sources of single‐trial MEG surface activity by computing Minimum Norm Estimates (MNE) on a single‐sphere head model using the Brainstorm toolbox [Tadel et al., 2011]. We used constraint dipoles (normal to cortical surface) and standard Tikhonov regularization (λ = 0.1). Localizing electromagnetic brain sources with MNE does not imply any assumptions about the underlying source currents [Hämäläinen and Ilmoniemi, 1994].
Time‐Frequency Analysis
Spectral estimation for frequencies between 0 and 100 Hz was done by time‐resolved Fourier transform using a sliding Hamming window of 400 ms. The window was shifted in steps of 17 ms. Relative power modulations were obtained by dividing the power in each frequency bin by the corresponding mean power during a baseline period (−870 to −630 ms with respect to the time point of 50% maximal joystick deflection).
Temporal Evolution of Tuning Across Sessions
To assess the tuning properties of filtered signals we calculated the signal‐to‐noise ratio (SNR) in the canonical RLDA space at the time point of inference. That is, we first projected the signal vectors s to the eigenvector F of corresponding to the largest eigenvalue, where
(4) |
is the between‐class covariance matrix and
(5) |
is the mean of the class means, yielding projected data y
(6) |
Note that in the two‐class case the rank of B is equal to 1, therefore there is only one eigenvector F with nonzero eigenvalue [Fukunaga, 1990] and the canonical RLDA is one dimensional.
We then defined SNR as the distance between class means µ' in the new space in relation to the fluctuations σ' around this class means, i.e.
(7) |
To analyze tuning strength and tuning profile dynamics we computed SNR across blocks of sessions following two different procedures: first, to assess tuning strength, the projection to canonical space was done independently for each block of sessions (CS 1‐4, BS 1‐6, BS 7‐12, and BS 13‐18), i.e. the eigenvector F was recomputed for each block. Second, to assess changes of tuning with respect to the CS tuning profile, the eigenvector F computed from the CS was applied to the BS. In both procedures the regularization parameter λ from the original classifier of each individual subject was used.
Statistical Analysis
For all the group analyses we report results in the form of mean ± standard error of the mean (SEM). To evaluate significance of classification accuracy we used the cumulative binomial distribution to calculate the confidence level of 99% [see Mehring et al., 2003].
Differences in CA between the two independent subject groups were examined using the Wilcoxon rank‐sum test. Within group differences in CA were assessed with the Wilcoxon signed‐rank test. To test for any trends in performance over trials or over sessions we used a linear fit and assessed the significance of the slope by standard techniques [Belsley, 1980]. A two‐sided Wilcoxon rank‐sum test between pooled absolute values of the raw joystick signals of all movements to the left and to the right was applied to test for differences in movement execution. All data analyses were performed using MATLAB (The MathWorks Inc., Natick, MA).
RESULTS
Fourteen healthy subjects performed delayed radial‐ulnar wrist deviations to move a joystick left‐ or rightwards inside the MEG chamber (Fig. 1A). After a brief practicing period during which subjects could get acquainted with the setup, we recorded six runs, referred to as calibration sessions (CS, see Fig. 1B). The resulting trials of 142 ± 2 (mean ± SEM) for movements to the left and 142 ± 3 for movements to the right across the CS were used to build a linear classifier of movement target based on slow sensorimotor cortical activity: for one group of six subjects MEG signals were low‐pass filtered 1.6 Hz (LP group), while for the other group of eight subjects a 4 to 6 Hz band‐pass component was used (BP group). During subsequent brain control sessions (BS) on 2 different days the classifier provided visual feedback based on the inferred movement target. The classifier was kept constant across all BS and across both days. This closed‐loop paradigm allowed us to assess the stability of movement‐related cortical activity.
Spatiotemporal Characteristics of Cortical Activity
MEG signals exhibited clear movement‐related modulation: in the raw signals of the 50 central sensors used for building the classifiers (Fig. 2A) one can observe an early positive peak at the beginning of the movement that was followed by a negative signal deflection during continued movement execution. After applying a 1.6 Hz low‐pass filter (LP) this general time course was preserved and it became evident that single sensors did vary in peak amplitude and latency (Fig. 2B). Band‐pass filtering (BP) the same raw signals in the range 4 to 6 Hz revealed oscillatory signals with maximum signal modulation during the period of movement execution (Fig. 2C). The time‐resolved spectra are consistent with these findings (Fig. 2E). While the power increased during movement execution in frequencies below 7.5 Hz, in the ranges 37.5 to 45 Hz (low gamma) and 55 to 90 Hz (high gamma), a strong decrease in power was observed for 10 to 15 (mu) and 15 to 30 Hz (beta) within the same period. While power decrease in the mu‐band continued, the beta component in frequencies 25 to 32.5 Hz increased again after movement (beta rebound).
With regard to the topography of magnetic field strength the most salient pattern at the time point of decoding is shown in Figure 3A: a strong, dipolar magnetic activity over central to contralateral sensorimotor areas, which was, however, subject to some amount of interindividual differences. Furthermore, depending on the subject, additional weaker activity was measured at pre‐frontal or posterior parietal sites. Localizing single‐trial source currents revealed activity peaks in precentral (Brodmann area 4, Talairach coordinates x/y/z −30/−30/60 mm), postcentral (Brodmann area 3, −27/−18/−62 mm), and premotor (Brodmann area 6, −30/−10/−60 mm) areas (Fig. 3B).
Inference of Wrist Movement Target
Figure 4 illustrates how well movement target could be inferred during CS using 50 MEG sensors over bilateral sensorimotor areas. For the LP group we correctly classified 76 ± 4% of single trials (mean ± SEM). In comparison to this, the classification accuracy (CA) for the BP group was higher at 85 ± 3% on average (P = 0.059, Wilcoxon rank‐sum test).
We also evaluated the CA in a time‐resolved manner (Fig. 4B): for the LP group this revealed a first significant CA ∼250 ms before the trigger point of 50% maximum joystick deflection. From this point on the subject averaged CA increased monotonically, peaked around movement end and declined after movement end. This general pattern was also evident for the BP group with CA values reaching higher maximum values around 80%. Importantly, the average angular difference between the absolute joystick trajectories of both target movements was 0.8° during the movement period (i.e. from −100 ms to +100 ms relative to the alignment point) as compared to the within standard deviation of 1.1° for both rightwards and leftwards movements (range across sessions and subjects: 0.7–1.7°). Movement displacement was highly stereotyped across sessions with angular differences within each target of 0.7° on average. The time‐resolved CA of electrooculogram (EOG) signals fluctuated around chance level, suggesting that eye movements did not carry information about movement target at any moment within the window of interest (Fig. 4B, upper panel).
Temporal Evolution of Slow Cortical Components
We then assessed the temporal stability of slow cortical signals as indicated by the output of the fixed classifier during all closed‐loop sessions. Hence, Figure 5A shows the CA of wrist movement target for the BP group over 18 BS runs across three days. For all subjects movement target could be inferred with a high mean CA of 81 ± 1% across the BS. The CA during BS was not different from the CA during CS (P = 0.20, Wilcoxon signed‐rank test). Moreover, there was no change in CA between day 1 and 2 and the average CA over all sessions did not reveal a trend (linear regression: slope = −0.08%, P = 0.41). Inference from EOG signals did not yield accuracies above chance level. Altogether this indicates temporally stable correlates of movement target in 4 to 6 Hz brain activity that allowed stable inference across days.
The observed stability of average CA across sessions does not necessarily exclude changes within single BS. To test for such changes, we computed the CA within single BS using a moving average over five trials. We observed high accuracies from the very start of each session, with CA values fluctuating between 78% and 83%, and almost no changes of CA across trials (Fig. 5B, linear regression: slope = 0.04% per trial, P < 0.001). This shows that the signals in the 4 to 6 Hz range could be used for brain control right away from the beginning of each BS.
Applying the same analysis described above to the LP group we again obtained CAs significantly above chance level throughout the BS on both days (Fig. 5C). In contrast to the BP group the average CA across 18 BS (66 ± 1%) was significantly lower than during the CS (P < 0.05, Wilcoxon signed‐rank test). Also the CA decreased across the BS (linear regression: slope = −0.5% per session, P < 0.001). However, intra‐session CA was constant from the start to the end of each session, fluctuating between 60% and 68% with no distinct changes over trials (Fig. 5D, linear regression: slope = −0.04% per trial, P < 0.05). In summary, wrist movement to two targets was successfully inferred during the closed‐loop feedback of the LP group as well. However, in contrast to the BP group the modulation of 1.6 Hz signals between right‐ and leftward movements was not stable across time.
All results presented thus far indicate higher stability of decoding for the BP group when compared to the LP group. Is this difference attributable to the closed‐loop feedback or due to intrinsic properties of the signals? To address this question we post‐hoc built and tested classifiers using swapped pre‐processing filters, i.e. subjects from the LP group were reanalyzed off‐line using 4 to 6 Hz BP filtered signals and subjects from the BP group were reanalyzed offline with 1.6 Hz LP filtered signals. Our results show that the closed‐loop feedback had no influence on the magnitude of the CA and its evolution across sessions (Fig. 6A): the BP component yielded a high and stable CA, not only for the BP but also for the LP group. Likewise, the LP component yielded a lower and unstable CA for both groups. The CAs of each component were not different between the groups (BP component: P = 0.66, Wilcoxon rank‐sum test; LP component: P = 0.85, Wilcoxon rank‐sum test). Overall, these results suggest that the stability and instability of decoding across BS was not due to differences in the closed‐loop coupling but due to different intrinsic signal properties.
What causes the instability of the LP component? One confounding variable may be that eye movements or muscular activity could have influenced this component. To address this issue, we repeated the classification procedure offline after manually removing trials that contained potential artifacts. The resulting weak changes in CA compared to the original data were not significant for either LP or BP component (all P > 0.05, Wilcoxon signed‐rank test; see Table 1). We then asked whether the instability of the LP component is caused by an attenuation of tuning across BS or by a tuning profile, which changes during BS but keeps its strength? To differentiate between these two possible explanations, we separately computed the tuning strength and a measure that quantifies the similarity of the tuning during BS to the tuning during CS (tuning profile, see Methods for details). While the tuning strength of the LP component remained constant across the BS, its tuning profile gradually changed with respect to the initial pattern found during the CS (Fig. 6B,C). This decrease of tuning similarity reached significance on the second experimental day (P < 0.05, Wilcoxon signed‐rank test). In contrast, tuning strength and profile of the BP signals remained unchanged over the BS.
Table 1.
BP component | LP component | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
BP group (%) | LP group (%) | BP group (%) | LP group (%) | |
CS | −1.0 ± 1.2 | −0.8 ± 1.0 | −1.5 ± 2.5 | −1.7 ± 1.5 |
BS | 0.7 ± 3.8 | 0.7 ± 2.1 | −0.4 ± 2.7 | −0.2 ± 2.1 |
Means ± standard deviations across subjects. Note that negative values reflect an increase in classification accuracy after artifact rejection.
BS = brain‐control session. CS = calibration session. BP = band‐pass 4–6 Hz. LP = 1.6 Hz low‐pass.
DISCUSSION
In summary, this study demonstrates stable cortical correlates of stereotyped wrist movement across several days, which can be utilized for stable closed‐loop inference of movement targets in humans.
Concurrent Stable and Unstable Correlates of Wrist Movements
Previous studies have reported slow cortical activations that correlated with different parameters of upper limb movements [Bradberry et al., 2010, 2009; Georgopoulos et al., 2005; Jerbi et al., 2007; Rickert et al., 2005; Schalk et al., 2007; Waldert et al., 2008]. Yet, the stability of the relation between motor behavior and cortical activity remains under debate [Carmena et al., 2005; Chestek et al., 2007; Ganguly and Carmena, 2009; Padoa‐Schioppa et al., 2004; Rokni et al., 2007; Stevenson et al., 2011]. Here, we extend recent reports on stability of neuronal activity during upper limb movements in non‐human primates [Chao et al., 2010; Ince et al., 2010] to humans: by using a constant classifier that provided visual feedback of wrist movement targets inferred online from MEG, we show concurrent stable and unstable cortical signals involved in voluntary movements of humans.
Our results demonstrate that consistent with previous work [Waldert et al., 2008; Wang et al., 2010] slow MEG signals, namely below 1.6 Hz and in the 4 to 6 Hz ranges, are significantly modulated by wrist movement so that movement target could be inferred in a closed‐loop paradigm. However, modulations to the two targets evolved differently over time: while both tuning strength and tuning profile of the 4 to 6 Hz component remained stable, the tuning profile of cortical signals below 1.6 Hz significantly changed, resulting in smoothly decreasing CA. Such change in tuning profile was only noticeable across days and did not degrade the inference of movement target within single BS. Using either signal component we could infer movement targets from the very first trial of each BS suggesting that these activations did not have to be learned. Moreover, the BP component remained stable over BS even if the LP component was used to control the visual feedback. Likewise, the LP component was instable even if the BP component was used for feedback. While the feedback may have helped to assure a high level of attention and involvement in the task and thus a consistent movement execution, we conclude that our experimental design did not entail a direct feedback learning effect on cortical signals. Instead, the observed changes are attributable to the intrinsic properties of the signal components.
Differential Stability of Slow Cortical Activity
What are possible reasons for the instability of the tuning profiles of brain signals below 1.6 Hz? Undesirable influences of artifacts, mostly from eye or muscles, are an omnipresent issue. However, most brain‐machine interface studies do not describe or perform any artifact rejection [Fatourechi et al., 2007], in particular not in a real‐time fashion. As classification based on EOG signals was not informative and the pattern of stability in brain signals did not change after post hoc artifact rejection (see Table 1), an influence of artifacts is therefore very unlikely for our results. The instability could have also been caused by other experimentally uncontrolled factors correlating with cortical activity. For example kinematic variables like arm positioning [Caminiti et al., 1990; Scott and Kalaska 1997], movement speed [Churchland et al., 2006] or altered forces caused by external loads [Kalaska et al., 1989] are known to impact neuronal activity and directional tuning. However, the simplicity of the movements and our experimental setup controlling for head and arm positions as well as for movement timing make this possibility unlikely. In fact, our subjects performed highly stereotypic joystick trajectories with very low movement variability (Fig. 4). Alternatively, the level of fatigue [Tecchio et al., 2006] and fading attention could also affect the stability of cortical signals. We consider it unlikely, however, that this caused the observed instability: (i) while we would expect movement variability to increase with higher levels of fatigue or with reduced attention, the variability remained constant throughout our experiments, speaking in favor of relatively constant levels of fatigue and attention. (ii) Even if the levels of fatigue and attention did change, we would expect fatigue to increase and attention to decrease within sessions and across the first day. The initial level of both factors then should have been restored on the beginning of the second day, after subjects had an extensive break. Consequently, if fatigue or attention affected tuning, the CA should attenuate within sessions but get back to high values on the beginning of the second day, which is not what we observed (Figs. 5 and 6). Furthermore, it has been argued that measurement noise constitutes a potential confounding factor in the assessment of stability of neuronal firing per se [Stevenson et al., 2011]. Notably, the CA measure and its assessment of statistical significance we utilized here, do take measurement noise and other sources of variability into account and are therefore not prone to this problem.
Motor cortical plasticity, most often assessed during sensorimotor learning of complex movement tasks [Ganguly and Carmena, 2009; Karni et al., 1995; Sanes, 2003; Ungerleider et al., 2002], may be more likely to have caused the observed instability. In fact, previous studies demonstrated rapid cortical re‐organization during simple, repeated movements that encoded the kinematic details of the practiced movement [Classen et al., 1998; Halder et al., 2005]. In addition to changes within cortical networks, a shift of main activation from cortical to subcortical areas has been reported when movements become automated during skill learning [Floyer‐Lea and Matthews 2004; Van Der Graaf et al., 2004]. The tuning instability that we observed for the slow 1.6 Hz component may reflect use‐dependent plasticity across sessions.
On the other hand, our results also show that both tuning strength and tuning profile of the 4 to 6 Hz activity remained stable over time. As a consequence, movement target was inferred with high accuracies from this signal throughout all sessions. This component could represent efferent commands, afferent feedback, or computations involving both. Correlates of each of those signals and processes have indeed been described in slow neuromagnetic fields [Cheyne and Weinberg, 1989; Cheyne et al., 2006, 1997; Kristeva et al., 1991] and results from recordings in rat hippocampus [Bland and Oddie, 2001; Bland et al., 2006] and human cortex [Caplan et al., 2003; Cruikshank et al., 2012] generally support the view that theta (4–8 Hz) oscillations might reflect a task‐specific mechanism for sensorimotor integration.
Our choice of frequency bands was based on past studies showing maximum tuning to movement direction for slow movement‐related brain signals [Jerbi et al., 2007; Mehring et al., 2003; Rickert et al., 2005; Schalk et al., 2007; Waldert et al., 2008]. We do not exclude that other high‐frequency bands can also provide valuable movement‐related information (see our Fig. 2E, for example) and this may be investigated in the future.
In addition, it still remains an open question whether source localization can improve inference of movement parameters. A priori knowledge on the cortical regions involved and a better signal‐to‐noise ratio might indeed result in better decoding. First insights have revealed a clear coupling between oscillations localized in motor areas and movement parameters like direction [Wang et al., 2010] or hand speed [Jerbi et al., 2007]. Yet, further experiments will have to verify the feasibility of single‐trial inverse solutions in real‐time applications like brain‐machine interfaces. In particular, issues like the computational costs, biased localizations and mapping of distributed network activity still remain.
CONCLUSIONS
The results of the current study demonstrate two concurrent slow correlates of voluntary movement to targets within overlapping areas of human sensorimotor cortex. Both allowed for closed‐loop inference of movement target but showed different stability over time. We therefore conclude that slow components, although in nearby frequency bands, can reveal a highly specific relation to motor behavior.
In our view, these findings are relevant to the field of brain‐machine interface research. We demonstrate that population signals of non‐invasive recordings in humans allow for closed‐loop inference of motor behavior over days without the need for re‐calibration. As a past offline study of ours showed similar tuning in electroencephalographic signals [Waldert et al., 2008] the transfer of our approach to EEG may be possible in the future. Potential effects of higher variability, both in terms of biological noise like more variable movement behavior in natural settings and in terms of non‐biological noise, need to be evaluated. Taken together our findings might help finding stable control signals for brain‐machine interfaces in humans.
REFERENCES
- Belsley DA, Kuh E, Welsch RE (1980): Regression Diagnostics: Identifying Influential Data and Sources of Collinearity. New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc. [Google Scholar]
- Bland BH, Jackson J, Derrie‐Gillespie D, Azad T, Rickhi A, Abriam J (2006): Amplitude, frequency, and phase analysis of hippocampal theta during sensorimotor processing in a jump avoidance task. Hippocampus 16:673–681. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bland BH, Oddie SD (2001): Theta band oscillation and synchrony in the hippocampal formation and associated structures: The case for its role in sensorimotor integration. Behav Brain Res 127:119–136. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bradberry TJ, Gentili RJ, Contreras‐Vidal JL (2010): Reconstructing three‐dimensional hand movements from noninvasive electroencephalographic signals. J Neurosci 30:3432–3437. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bradberry TJ, Rong F, Contreras‐Vidal JL (2009): Decoding center‐out hand velocity from MEG signals during visuomotor adaptation. Neuroimage 47:1691–1700. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Caminiti R, Johnson PB, Urbano A (1990): Making arm movements within different parts of space: dynamic aspects in the primate motor cortex. J Neurosci 10:2039–2058. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Caplan JB, Madsen JR, Schulze‐Bonhage A, Aschenbrenner‐Scheibe R, Newman EL, Kahana MJ (2003): Human theta oscillations related to sensorimotor integration and spatial learning. J Neurosci 23:4726–4736. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carmena J, Lebedev M, Crist R, O'Doherty J, Santucci D, Dimitrov D, Patil P, Henriquez C, Nicolelis M (2003): Learning to control a brain‐machine interface for reaching and grasping by primates. PLoS Biol 1:e42. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carmena J, Lebedev M, Henriquez C, Nicolelis M (2005): Stable ensemble performance with single‐neuron variability during reaching movements in primates. J Neurosci 25:10712–10716. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chao ZC, Nagasaka Y, Fujii N (2010): Long‐term asynchronous decoding of arm motion using electrocorticographic signals in monkeys. Front Neuroeng 3:3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cheney PD, Fetz EE (1980): Functional classes of primate corticomotoneuronal cells and their relation to active force. J Neurophysiol 44:773–791. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chestek C, Batista A, Santhanam G, Yu B, Afshar A, Cunningham J, Gilja V, Ryu S, Churchland M, Shenoy K (2007): Single‐neuron stability during repeated reaching in macaque premotor cortex. J Neurosci 27:10742–10750. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cheyne D, Bakhtazad L, Gaetz W (2006): Spatiotemporal mapping of cortical activity accompanying voluntary movements using an event‐related beamforming approach. Hum Brain Mapp 27:213–229. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cheyne D, Endo H, Takeda T, Weinberg H (1997): Sensory feedback contributes to early movement‐evoked fields during voluntary finger movements in humans. Brain Res 771:196–202. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cheyne D, Weinberg H (1989): Neuromagnetic fields accompanying unilateral finger movements: Pre‐movement and movement‐evoked fields. Exp Brain Res 78:604–612. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Churchland MM, Afshar A, Shenoy KV (2006): A central source of movement variability. Neuron 52:1085–1096. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Classen J, Liepert J, Wise SP, Hallett M, Cohen LG (1998): Rapid plasticity of human cortical movement representation induced by practice. J Neurophysiol 79:1117–1123. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cruikshank LC, Singhal A, Hueppelsheuser M, Caplan JB (2012): Theta oscillations reflect a putative neural mechanism for human sensorimotor integration. J Neurophysiol 107:65–77. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fatourechi M, Bashashati A, Ward RK, Birch GE (2007): EMG and EOG artifacts in brain computer interface systems: A survey. Clin Neurophysiol 118:480–494. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Floyer‐Lea A, Matthews PM (2004): Changing brain networks for visuomotor control with increased movement automaticity. J Neurophysiol 92:2405–2412. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Friedman JH (1989): Regularized discriminant analysis. J Am Stat Assoc 84:165–175. [Google Scholar]
- Fukunaga K (1990): Introduction to Statistical Pattern Classification. San Diego, CA: Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
- Ganguly K, Carmena J (2009): Emergence of a stable cortical map for neuroprosthetic control. PLoS Biol 7:e1000153. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Georgopoulos AP, Kalaska JF, Caminiti R, Massey JT (1982): On the relations between the direction of two‐dimensional arm movements and cell discharge in primate motor cortex. J Neurosci 2:1527–1537. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Georgopoulos AP, Langheim FJ, Leuthold AC, Merkle AN (2005): Magnetoencephalographic signals predict movement trajectory in space. Exp Brain Res 167:132–135. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Georgopoulos AP, Schwartz AB, Kettner RE (1986): Neuronal population coding of movement direction. Science 233:1416–1419. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Halder P, Sterr A, Brem S, Bucher K, Kollias S, Brandeis D (2005): Electrophysiological evidence for cortical plasticity with movement repetition. Eur J Neurosci 21:2271–2277. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hämäläinen MS, Ilmoniemi RJ (1994): Interpreting magnetic fields of the brain: Minimum norm estimates. Med Biol Eng Comput 32:35–42. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ince NF, Gupta R, Arica S, Tewfik AH, Ashe J, Pellizzer G (2010): High accuracy decoding of movement target direction in non‐human primates based on common spatial patterns of local field potentials. PLoS One 5:e14384. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jerbi K, Lachaux JP, N'Diaye K, Pantazis D, Leahy RM, Garnero L, Baillet S (2007): Coherent neural representation of hand speed in humans revealed by MEG imaging. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 104:7676–7681. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kalaska JF, Cohen DA, Hyde ML, Prud'homme M (1989): A comparison of movement direction‐related versus load direction‐related activity in primate motor cortex, using a two‐dimensional reaching task. J Neurosci 9:2080–2102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Karni A, Meyer G, Jezzard P, Adams MM, Turner R, Ungerleider LG (1995): Functional MRI evidence for adult motor cortex plasticity during motor skill learning. Nature 377:155–158. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kristeva R, Cheyne D, Deecke L (1991): Neuromagnetic fields accompanying unilateral and bilateral voluntary movements: topography and analysis of cortical sources. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 81:284–298. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mehring C, Rickert J, Vaadia E, Cardosa de Oliveira S, Aertsen A, Rotter S (2003): Inference of hand movements from local field potentials in monkey motor cortex. Nat Neurosci 6:1253–1254. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mellinger J, Schalk G, Braun C, Preissl H, Rosenstiel W, Birbaumer N, Kubler A (2007): An MEG‐based brain‐computer interface (BCI). Neuroimage 36:581–593. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nicolelis MA, Lebedev MA (2009): Principles of neural ensemble physiology underlying the operation of brain‐machine interfaces. Nat Rev Neurosci 10:530–540. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Padoa‐Schioppa C, Li CS, Bizzi E (2004): Neuronal activity in the supplementary motor area of monkeys adapting to a new dynamic environment. J Neurophysiol 91:449–473. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pistohl T, Ball T, Schulze‐Bonhage A, Aertsen A, Mehring C (2008): Prediction of arm movement trajectories from ECoG‐recordings in humans. J Neurosci Methods 167:105–114. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pistohl T, Schulze‐Bonhage A, Aertsen A, Mehring C, Ball T (2011): Decoding natural grasp types from human ECoG. Neuroimage 59:248–260. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rickert J, Oliveira SCd, Vaadia E, Aertsen A, Rotter S, Mehring C (2005): Encoding of movement direction in different frequency ranges of motor cortical local field potentials. J Neurosci 25:8815–8824. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rokni U, Richardson AG, Bizzi E, Seung HS (2007): Motor learning with unstable neural representations. Neuron 54:653–666. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rossini PM (2009): Implications of brain plasticity to brain‐machine interfaces operation a potential paradox? Int Rev Neurobiol 86:81–90. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sanes JN (2003): Neocortical mechanisms in motor learning. Curr Opin Neurobiol 13:225–231. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schalk G, Kubánek J, Miller KJ, Anderson NR, Leuthardt EC, Ojemann JG, Limbrick D, Moran D, Gerhardt LA, Wolpaw JR (2007): Decoding two‐dimensional movement trajectories using electrocorticographic signals in humans. J Neural Eng 4:264. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schalk G, McFarland DJ, Hinterberger T, Birbaumer N, Wolpaw JR (2004): BCI2000: A general‐purpose brain‐computer interface (BCI) system. IEEE Trans Biomed Eng 51:1034–1043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schalk G, Miller KJ, Anderson NR, Wilson JA, Smyth MD, Ojemann JG, Moran DW, Wolpaw JR, Leuthardt EC (2008): Two‐dimensional movement control using electrocorticographic signals in humans. J Neural Eng 5:75. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scott SH, Kalaska JF (1997): Reaching movements with similar hand paths but different arm orientations. I. Activity of individual cells in motor cortex. J Neurophysiol 77:826–852. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Serruya M, Hatsopoulos N, Paninski L, Fellows M, Donoghue J (2002): Brain‐machine interface: Instant neural control of a movement signal. Nature 416:141–142. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stevenson IH, Cherian A, London BM, Sachs NA, Lindberg E, Reimer J, Slutzky MW, Hatsopoulos NG, Miller LE, Kording KP (2011): Statistical assessment of the stability of neural movement representations. J Neurophysiol 106:764–774. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tadel F, Baillet S, Mosher JC, Pantazis D, Leahy RM (2011): Brainstorm: A user‐friendly application for MEG/EEG analysis. Comput Intell Neurosci 2011:879716. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tecchio F, Porcaro C, Zappasodi F, Pesenti A, Ercolani M, Rossini PM (2006): Cortical short‐term fatigue effects assessed via rhythmic brain‐muscle coherence. Exp Brain Res 174:144–151. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ungerleider LG, Doyon J, Karni A (2002): Imaging brain plasticity during motor skill learning. Neurobiol Learn Memory 78:553–564. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Der Graaf FH, De Jong BM, Maguire RP, Meiners LC, Leenders KL (2004): Cerebral activation related to skills practice in a double serial reaction time task: striatal involvement in random‐order sequence learning. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 20:120–131. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vrba J, Anderson G, Betts K, Burbank MB, Cheung T, Cheyne D, Fife AA, Govorkov S, Habib F, Haid G, Haid V, Hoang T, Hunter C, Kubik PR, Lee S, McCubbin J, McKay J, McKenzie D, Nonis D, Paz J, Reichl E, Ressl D, Robinson SE, Schroyen C, Sekachev I, Spear P, Taylor B, M. T, Sutherling W (1999): Recent Advancements in Biomagnetism. Sendai, Japan: Tohoku University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Waldert S, Preissl H, Demandt E, Braun C, Birbaumer N, Aertsen A, Mehring C (2008): Hand movement direction decoded from MEG and EEG. J Neurosci 28:1000–1008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang W, Sudre GP, Xu Y, Kass RE, Collinger JL, Degenhart AD, Bagic AI, Weber DJ (2010): Decoding and cortical source localization for intended movement direction with MEG. J Neurophysiol 104:2451–2461. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]