Skip to main content
Human Brain Mapping logoLink to Human Brain Mapping
. 2014 Oct 18;36(2):591–602. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22649

Neural signatures of fairness‐related normative decision making in the ultimatum game: A coordinate‐based meta‐analysis

Chunliang Feng 1, Yue‐Jia Luo 1,2,, Frank Krueger 3,4,
PMCID: PMC6869807  PMID: 25327760

Abstract

The willingness to incur personal costs to enforce prosocial norms represents a hallmark of human civilization. Although recent neuroscience studies have used the ultimatum game to understand the neuropsychological mechanisms that underlie the enforcement of fairness norms; however, a precise characterization of the neural systems underlying fairness‐related norm enforcement remains elusive. In this study, we used a coordinate‐based meta‐analysis on functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies using the ultimatum game with the goal to provide an additional level of evidence for the refinement of the underlying neural architecture of this human puzzling behavior. Our results demonstrated a convergence of reported activation foci in brain networks associated with psychological components of fairness‐related normative decision making, presumably reflecting a reflexive and intuitive system (System 1) and a reflective and deliberate system (System 2). System 1 (anterior insula, ventromedial prefrontal cortex [PFC]) may be associated with the reflexive and intuitive responses to norm violations, representing a motivation to punish norm violators. Those intuitive responses conflict with economic self‐interest, encoded in the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), which may engage cognitive control from a reflective and deliberate System 2 to resolve the conflict by either suppressing (ventrolateral PFC, dorsomedial PFC, left dorsolateral PFC, and rostral ACC) the intuitive responses or over‐riding self‐interest (right dorsolateral PFC). Taken together, we suggest that fairness‐related norm enforcement recruits an intuitive system for rapid evaluation of norm violations and a deliberate system for integrating both social norms and self‐interest to regulate the intuitive system in favor of more flexible decision making. Hum Brain Mapp 36:591–602, 2015. © 2014 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.

Keywords: norm enforcement, fairness, ultimatum game, fMRI, activation likelihood estimation, multiple‐system models

INTRODUCTION

Humans comply with social norms (e.g., equity and fairness pertaining to resource distribution) and they are willing to punish norm violators at the expense of personal costs [Fehr and Fischbacher, 2004; Fehr and Gächter, 2002]. This costly norm enforcement termed altruistic punishment is a hallmark of human culture and plays a key role in promoting widespread cooperation among genetically unrelated strangers in human societies [Fehr et al., 2002; Henrich et al., 2006]. Humans develop preferences for social norms as early as 7–8 year old [Fehr et al., 2008; Güroğlu et al., 2011; Steinmann et al., 2014] and maintain those prosocial preferences across lifespan [Harlé and Sanfey, 2012; Roalf, 2010].

A common, widely used research approach to probe human social norm enforcement is the employment of well‐structured interactive economic games [Camerer, 2003; Fehr and Camerer, 2007; Sanfey, 2007]. For example, during the one‐shot interaction in the ultimatum game (UG) [Güth et al., 1982], two players must assent to split of a sum of money or both get nothing. The first player (proposer) offers a way to divide the sum between the two players and the second player (responder) can either accept (both get paid accordingly) or reject (neither gets paid) the offer. Responders perceive fair offers (i.e., norm compliance) as rewards and simply accept those offers [Tabibnia and Lieberman, 2007; Tabibnia et al., 2008]. When responders are treated fairly, brain regions engaged in reward processing such as ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) or striatum are involved [Baumgartner et al., 2011; Fliessbach et al., 2007; Guo et al., 2014; Tabibnia et al., 2008; Wu et al., in press].

In contrast, unfair offers (i.e., norm violation) induce motivational conflict between economic self‐interest and norm enforcement: responders are tempted to accept those offers for monetary reward, whereas they are also motivated by prosocial preferences to reject those offers [Fehr and Camerer, 2007]. Researchers from various research disciplines are interested in studying the neuropsychological mechanisms underlying the reconciling of different motivations in response to norm violations. For example, previous studies have indicated that responders reject unfair offers at about 50% chance and punish proposers who violate fairness norms by offering less than 20%–30% of the sum [Camerer, 2003; Güth et al., 1982], demonstrating human willingness to incur personal cost to enforce social norms.

Early models addressing human social preferences in fairness‐related norm enforcement have postulated the significant roles of inequality aversion [Fehr and Schmidt, 1999] and intention inference [Blount, 1995; Rabin, 1993]. In recent decades, those initial theoretical models have been extensively elaborated due to interdisciplinary studies in the fields of psychology, economics, and neuroscience [Rilling and Sanfey, 2011; Sanfey et al., 2006]. Recent evidence revealed that fairness‐related norm enforcement in response to norm violations (as measured in UG) consists of multiple psychological components that are implemented by separable neural systems [Buckholtz and Marois, 2012; Sanfey and Chang, 2008], including a reflexive and intuitive system (System 1) and a reflective and deliberate system (System 2) [Lieberman, 2007; Sanfey et al., 2006].

In terms of UG, System 1 is thought to represent psychological components involved in the initial evaluations of norm violations, including brain areas such as the anterior insula (AI) signaling norm violations [Civai, 2013] or emotional processing via representations of (especially aversive) internal states [Chang and Sanfey, 2013; Corradi‐Dell'Acqua et al., 2013; Guo et al., 2013; Sanfey et al., 2003], the amygdala signaling transient negative emotional response to norm violations [Gospic et al., 2011; Haruno and Frith, 2010; Haruno et al., 2014; Yu et al., 2014] and the vmPFC encoding subjective values of perceived offers [Baumgartner et al., 2011; Dawes et al., 2012; Xiang et al., 2013]. Those intuitive responses to norm violations contribute to altruistic punishments of transgressions [Gospic et al., 2011; Sanfey et al., 2003], supporting the notion that reflexive and emotional responses implemented by System 1 lie at the core of human prosocial preferences [Haidt, 2001; Roch et al., 2000; Sanfey and Chang, 2008; Scheele et al., 2012; Zaki and Mitchell, 2011, 2013].

System 2 presumably represents more deliberate psychological components responsible for reappraising and regulating System 1 in favor of either economic self‐interest [Brüne et al., 2012; Sanfey and Chang, 2008] or enforcement of social norms (e.g., fairness) [Fehr and Camerer, 2007]. The unfairness‐evoked aversive responses (norm enforcement) and self‐interest (due to the possibility to gain the monetary reward) contradict each other and result into a motivational conflict that is monitored by the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC) [Fehr and Camerer, 2007; Sanfey et al., 2003]. This conflict can be resolved in two ways: on the one hand, the reflexive reactions of System 1 can be suppressed, probably implemented by brain regions associated with emotion regulation such as ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (vlPFC) and dorsomedial PFC (dmPFC), leading to an increased acceptance of unfair offers [Civai et al., 2012; Grecucci et al., 2013; Tabibnia et al., 2008]. On the other hand, the conflict can be also reconciled by over‐riding economic self‐interest, likely manifested as executive control in the right dorsolateral PFC (dlPFC) [Baumgartner et al., 2011; Knoch et al., 2006, 2008]. Overall, System 2 may contribute to detect and reconcile the motivational conflict between norm enforcement and self‐interest in favor of flexible decision making.

Recent neuroscientific studies have begun examining the neural signatures of isolated components of the fairness‐related norm enforcement and a couple of descriptive reviews have explored the biological basis of human normative decision making from a larger perspective [Buckholtz and Marois, 2012; Rilling and Sanfey, 2011; Ruff and Fehr, 2014]. However, a precise characterization of the neural systems underlying fairness‐related norm enforcement remains elusive. In this study, we used a coordinate‐based meta‐analysis on fMRI studies using the UG with the goal to provide an additional level of evidence for the refinement of the underlying neural architecture of fairness‐related norm enforcement. We expect that enforcement of social norms in the UG recruits multiple brain regions, which have been previously separated into two interacting neural systems [Buckholtz and Marois, 2012; Sanfey and Chang, 2008]: (i) a reflexive and intuitive system (System 1: AI, amygdala, and vmPFC) for recognizing and evaluating norm violations and (ii) a deliberate system (System 2: dACC, dlPFC, vlPFC, and dmPFC) for reappraising and regulating System 1 in favor of either economic self‐interest or norm enforcement.

MATERIAL AND METHODS

Literature Search and Selection

We performed a systematic online database search in July of 2014 on PubMed and ISI Web of Science by entering various combinations of relevant search items (e.g., [“normative decision making” OR “fair” OR “altruistic punishment” OR “ultimatum game”] AND [“fMRI” OR “magnetic resonance imaging” OR “neuroimaging”]) and conducted a follow‐up search by examining the bibliography and citation indices of the preselected articles. The search resulted into 53 potential studies that were further assessed according to the following criteria: (i) subjects were recruited as responders in UG: (ii) fMRI was used as the imaging modality; (iii) whole‐brain general‐linear‐model‐based analyses (rather than region of interest [ROI] analyses) were applied; (iv) statistical models for fairness‐related contrasts (i.e., fair > unfair or unfair > fair) or relevant parametric analyses were reported; and (v) activations were presented in a standardized stereotaxic space (Talairach or MNI). Note that for studies reporting Talairach coordinates a conversion to the MNI coordinates was implemented [Brett, 1999]. Filtering search results according to the inclusion/exclusion criteria yielded a total of 20 published fMRI articles with 17 “unfair > fair” contrasts (180 foci) and 11 “fair > unfair” contrasts (35 foci) reported in a standardized stereotaxic space (Table 1).

Table 1.

Summary of studies included for the meta‐analysis focusing on the responder in UG

Study N Task and contrast No. of foci
Fair Offer
Baumgartner et al. [2011] 32 Responders in UG, fair>unfair 4
Civai et al. [2012] 19 Responders in a modified UG/DG, equal>unequal 3
Harlé and Sanfey.[2012] 38 Responders in UG, fair>unfair 3
Roalf [2010] 27 Responders in UG, fair>unfair 2
Tabibnia et al. [2008] 12 Responders in UG, fair>unfair 1
Tomasino et al. [2013] 17 Responders in UG, fair>unfair 3
White et al. [2013] 20 Responders in UG, parametric analysis, negative correlation with unfairness level 1
White et al. [2014] 21 Responders in UG, parametric analysis, negative correlation with unfairness level 3
Wright et al. [2011] 30 Responders in UG, parametric analysis, negative correlation with inequity 10
Wu et al. [in press] 18 Responders in UG, parametric analysis, positive correlation with subjective utility 4
Zhou et al. [2014] 28 Responders in UG, fair>unfair 1
Unfair Offer
Baumgartner et al. [2011] 32 Responders in UG, unfair>fair 17
Civai et al. [2012] 19 Responders in a modified UG/DG, unequal>equal 12
Haruno et al. [2014] 62 Responders in UG, parametric analysis, positive correlation with inequity. 4
Gospic et al. [2011] 17 Responders in UG, unfair>fair 4
Guo et al. [2014] 18 Responders in UG, unfair>fair 10
Guo et al. [2013] 21 Responders in UG, unfair>fair 13
Güroğlu et al. [2011] 68 Responders in UG, unfair>fair 9
Halko et al. [2009] 23 Responders in UG, unfair>fair 22
Harlé and Sanfey. [2012] 38 Responders in UG, unfair>fair 12
Kirk et al. [2011] 40 Responders in UG, unfair>fair 11
Roalf [2010] 27 Responders in UG, unfair>fair 8
Sanfey et al. [2003] 19 Responders in UG, unfair>fair 17
White et al. [2013] 20 Responders in UG, parametric analysis, positive correlation with unfairness level 8
White et al. [2014] 21 Responders in UG, parametric analysis, positive correlation with unfairness level 7
Wu et al. [in press] 18 Responders in UG, parametric analysis, negative correlation with subjective utility 7
Zheng et al. [in press] 25 Responders in UG, unfair>fair 15
Zhou et al. [2014] 28 Responders in UG, unfair>fair 4

N, number of subjects

Activation Likelihood Estimation Approach

A coordinate‐based meta‐analysis of reported fMRI experiments was conducted using the revised activation likelihood estimation (ALE) algorithm [Eickhoff et al., 2009] implemented in the GingerALE software (version 2.3, http://www.brainmap.org/ale/). Applying the ALE algorithm, the reported coordinates of brain areas associated with fairness‐related norm enforcement were converged across different experiments. A random‐effects analysis was performed to determine statistical significance using a permutation test of randomly generated foci with 5,000 permutations (full‐width at half‐maximum of 10 mm) [Eickhoff et al., 2012; Turkeltaub et al., 2012]. The resulting ALE maps were thresholded using the false discovery rate (FDR, q(FDR) < 0.05) correction for multiple comparisons [Genovese et al., 2002; Laird et al., 2005] and all clusters were set to a minimum volume of 600 mm3 [Lamm et al., 2011]. The meta‐analysis results were overlaid onto an anatomical template (Colin27_T1_seg_MNI.nii, http://www.brainmap.org/ale) and displayed using the Mango software (http://rii.uthscsa.edu/mango/).

RESULTS

Consistent maxima were found in the following brain regions comparing fair with unfair offers in UG: bilateral vmPFC and posterior insula, left posterior cingulate cortex (PCC) and precuneus, and right inferior temporal gyrus (ITG; Table 2 and Fig. 1). Comparing unfair with fair offers in UG, consistent maxima were found in the following brain regions: bilateral dACC/pre‐supplementary motor area (pre‐SMA), AI/vlPFC and dlPFC, left rostral ACC (rACC), posterior superior temporal gyrus (pSTG), STG/anterior temporal lobe (ATL), inferior parietal lobule (IPL) and fusiform gyrus, right dmPFC, precuneus, and lingual gyrus (Table 3 and Fig. 2). The AI clusters revealed in this contrast were primarily located in subregion of the dorso‐AI, according to Kelly et al.'s (2012) templates of insular subregions (Fig. 3).

Table 2.

ALE meta‐analysis results for fair offers (fair > unfair contrast)

Brain regions BA MNI coordinates (mm) ALE (×10−2) Cluster size (mm3)
x y z
vmPFC/vACC 32/10 6 46 −12 0.97 2736
R vACC 32 6 46 −12 0.97
L vmPFC 10 −2 54 −12 0.97
R vmPFC 10 6 52 −16 0.89
L posterior insula/STG 13/22 −54 −12 6 0.92 1688
L STG 22 −54 −12 6 0.92
L posterior insula 13 −40.5 −15 1.5 0.89
L PCC 23 −8 −58 16 1.08 1192
L PCC 23 −8 −58 16 1.08
R posterior insula 13 42 −24 24 0.92 952
R posterior insula 13 42 −24 24 0.92
L precuneus 7 −8 −59 56 0.80 912
L precuneus 7 −8 −59 56 0.80
R ITG 20 60 −48 −12 0.93 648
R ITG 20 60 −48 −12 0.93

BA, Brodmann area; L, left; R, right; ALE, activation likelihood estimation; vmPFC, ventromedial prefrontal cortex; vACC, ventral anterior cingulate cortex; STG, superior temporal gyrus; PCC, posterior cingulate cortex: ITG, inferior temporal gyrus

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Significant clusters from the coordinate‐based activation likelihood estimation (ALE) meta‐analysis (5,000 permutations, q(FDR) < 0.05, min. volume of 600 mm3) for fair offers (fair > unfair contrast). Consistent maxima comparing fair with unfair offers in UG were found in posterior insula, ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC), inferior temporal gyrus (ITG), precunceus, and posterior cingulate cortex (PCC). [Color figure can be viewed in the online issue, which is available at http://wileyonlinelibrary.com.]

Table 3.

ALE meta‐analysis results of unfair offers (unfair > fair contrast)

Brain regions BA MNI coordinates (mm) ALE (×10−2) Cluster size (mm3)
x y z
dACC/pre‐SMA 32/6 −4 16 48 2.59 14168
L dACC 32 −4 16 48 2.59
R dACC 32 8 22 40 2.35
L pre‐SMA 6 −6 2 64 0.97
R pre‐SMA 6 10 24 58 1.46
R AI/putamen/vlPFC 13/47 38 20 0 3.10 9456
R AI 13 38 20 0 3.10
R putamen / 22 12 2 0.96
R vlPFC 47 34 21 −18 0.62
L AI/vlPFC 13/47 −30 24 2 2.79 7376
L AI 13 −30 24 2 2.79
L vlPFC 47 −44 24 −8 1.00
R dlPFC: R middle frontal gyrus/SFG 9/8 40 36 26 1.95 3768
R middle frontal gyrus 9 40 36 26 1.95
R SFG 8 38 22 48 0.82
R dlPFC: R middle frontal gyrus/SFG 10 30 66 2 1.55 1272
R SFG 10 30 66 2 1.55
R middle frontal gyrus 10 30 64 12 1.22
R dlPFC: SFG/middle frontal gyrus 9/10 26 48 22 1.05 664
R SFG 9 26 48 22 1.05
R middle frontal gyrus 10 34 48 18 0.87
L dlPFC: L middle frontal gyrus 9/8 −30 38 30 1.03 1016
L middle frontal gyrus 9 −30 38 30 1.03
L middle frontal gyrus 8 −36 40 38 0.88
L dlFPC: SFG 9 −20 54 30 0.94 648
L SFG 9 −20 54 30 0.94
R dmPFC 9 8 60 16 1.06 1024
R dmPFC 9 8 60 16 1.06
R dmPFC 9 4 54 26 0.97 680
R dmPFC 9 4 54 26 0.97
L rACC/medial PFC 10/32 −12 46 4 0.92 616
L medial PFC 10 −12 46 4 0.92
L rACC 32 −7 47 4 0.31
L IPL 40 −32 −46 42 0.93 1136
L IPL 40 −32 −46 42 0.93
L IPL 40 −44 −46 52 1.24 816
L IPL 40 −44 −46 52 1.24
L pSTG 22 −48 −40 4 0.91 632
L pSTG 22 −48 −40 4 0.91
L STG/ATL 22 −52 −16 −8 1.03 664
L STG/ATL 22 −52 −16 −8 1.03
R precuneus 7 14 −68 40 0.96 664
R precuneus 7 14 −68 40 0.96
L fusiform gyrus 19 −44 −72 −8 1.07 672
L fusiform gyrus 19 −44 −72 −8 1.07
R lingual gyrus 18 10 −80 0 0.98 672
R lingual gyrus 18 10 −80 0 0.98

BA, Brodmann area; L, left; R, right; ALE, activation likelihood estimation; dACC, dorsal anterior cingulate cortex; pre‐SMA, pre‐supplementary motor area; AI, anterior insula; vlPFC, ventrolateral prefrontal cortex; SFG, superior frontal gyrus; dlPFC, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; dmPFC, dorsomedial prefrontal cortex; rACC, rostral anterior cingulate cortex; IPL, inferior parietal lobule; pSTG, posterior superior temporal gyrus; ATL, anterior temporal lobe.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Significant clusters from the coordinate‐based activation likelihood estimation (ALE) meta‐analysis (5,000 permutations, q(FDR) < 0.05, min. volume of 600 mm3) for unfair offers (unfair > fair contrast). Consistent maxima comparing unfair with fair offers in UG were found in dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC), anterior insula (AI), ventrolateral PFC (vlPFC), inferior parietal lobule (IPL), precuneus, dorsal anterior cingulate cortex/presupplementary motor area (dACC/pre‐SMA), rostral ACC (rACC), dorsomedial PFC (dmPFC), and superior temporal gyrus (STG). [Color figure can be viewed in the online issue, which is available at http://wileyonlinelibrary.com.]

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Subregions of insular cortex. (A) Insular subregions (Cluster 1 [green]: dorsoanterior insula, Cluster 2 [cyan]: ventroanterior insula, Cluster 3 [blue]: posterior insula) according to Kelly et al.'s template (K = 3 solutions). (B) Significant clusters from the coordinate‐based ALE (activation likelihood estimation) for unfair offers (red: unfair > fair contrast) and overlaps between those clusters and different insular subregions intersected by Kelly et al.'s template (yellow: intersection of clusters from ALE analysis for unfair offers and dorsoanterior insula; reddish yellow: intersection of clusters from ALE analysis for unfair offers and ventroanterior insula). [Color figure can be viewed in the online issue, which is available at http://wileyonlinelibrary.com.]

DISCUSSION

The willingness to incur personal costs to enforce prosocial norms represents a hallmark of human civilization. Recent neuroscience studies have used UG to understand the neuropsychological mechanisms that underlie enforcement of fairness‐related norms [Fehr and Camerer, 2007; Sanfey, 2007]. The purpose of this coordinate‐based meta‐analysis study was to quantitatively synthesize the results of recent fMRI studies regarding fairness‐related norm enforcement with the aim of identifying consistent activation patterns of the neural signatures underlying this human puzzling behavior. Our results demonstrated a convergence of reported activation foci in brain networks associated with normative decision making [Buckholtz and Marois, 2012; Krueger et al., 2008; Rilling and Sanfey, 2011; Sanfey and Chang, 2008], and these brain regions have been thought to represent two interacting neural systems [Buckholtz and Marois, 2012; Sanfey and Chang, 2008]: a reflexive and intuitive system (System 1) for recognizing and evaluating norm violations and a deliberate system (System 2) for integrating both social norms and economic self‐interest to regulate System 1 in favor of flexible decision makings.

In UG, the responder has to decide whether to accept or to reject fair or unfair offers from proposers. Regarding fair offers, previous findings have indicated that those offers are considered as rewards [Harlé et al., 2012; Tabibnia and Lieberman, 2007; Tabibnia et al., 2008]. Accordingly, the vmPFC, a core region associated with reward processing showed consistent activation across fMRI studies in response to fair offers compared to unfair offers. Presumably, the vmPFC is associated with computing positive values of fairness, owning to its role in representing values of normatively valued goods [Aoki et al., 2014; Moretti et al., 2009; Tricomi et al., 2010]. In accord with this view, the activation of vmPFC to fair offers remains after controlling for momentary payoff [Tabibnia et al., 2008].

The meta‐analysis also revealed the consistent involvement of the posterior insula in response to fair offers, which is consistent with its role in coding fairness or equality [Hsu et al., 2008; Wright et al., 2011]. Moreover, involvement of the PCC was identified, which role in reward processing has also been demonstrated in previous studies [Ballard and Knutson, 2009; Levy and Glimcher, 2011; McClure et al., 2007], but it remains unknown whether this region encodes the positive value of fairness or just the monetary payoff in UG. By contrast, the meta‐analysis did not reveal consistent activation of the ventral striatum in response to fairness, since most of the previous studies reporting a ventral striatum response to fairness applied an ROI analysis approach [Tabibnia et al., 2008; Tricomi et al., 2010; Wu et al., in press].

Responders in UG react to unfair offers with negative emotional feelings, which are thought to play a pivotal role in costly punishment/norm enforcement [Koenigs and Tranel, 2007; Pillutla and Murnighan, 1996; Van't Wout et al., 2006; White et al., 2014; Xiao and Houser, 2005; Yamagishi et al., 2009]. Our meta‐analysis revealed that brain regions of System 1 such as bilateral AI were consistently involved in those intuitive and reflexive responses [Sanfey and Chang, 2008; Sanfey et al., 2003]. The AI may be associated with the visceral experience of negative feelings by signaling aversive interoceptive states due to fairness‐related norm violations [Critchley et al., 2004; Singer et al., 2009]. Previous studies using the UG have shown that emotion infusion [Harlé et al., 2012; Harlé and Sanfey, 2007] and emotion regulation [Grecucci et al., 2013] modulate the unfairness‐evoked AI responses, which predict normative decision making at the behavioral level [Harlé et al., 2012; Kirk et al., 2011; Sanfey et al., 2003; Tabibnia et al., 2008]. However, recent studies have indicated that unfairness‐evoked negative feelings cannot completely account for the activation of AI [Civai, 2013], because neural response to norm violations in the AI is also evident when participants play a role of indifferent third‐party [Civai et al., 2012; Strobel et al., 2011] and when unfair offers are better than expectations [Xiang et al., 2013; Yu et al., 2014]. Furthermore, recent evidence has demonstrated a functional heterogeneity of different AI subregions related to norm violations in UG [Zhou et al., 2014]. In particular, a tripartite subdivision of the insula has been proposed with dorsoanterior, ventroanterior, and posterior portions contributing to cognitive, affective, and sensorimotor processing, respectively [Chang et al., 2013; Kelly et al., 2012]. Recent evidence suggests that the dorsal AI associated with cognition is consistently activated by norm violation in most of previous neuroimaging studies using UG [Zhou et al., 2014]. In lights of these recent findings, it is more likely that the consistent activation of primarily dorsal AI represents a cognitive heuristic to detect norm violations rather than emotional resentment [Civai, 2013; Civai et al., 2010, 2012, 2013].

The emotion processing in response to norm violations may be implemented by other brain regions, including vmPFC and amygdala, both of which are frequently associated with social decision making and affective processing [Buckholtz and Marois, 2012; Buckholtz et al., 2008; Gospic et al., in press]. As discussed above, numerous studies have revealed the role of vmPFC in tracking the positive and negative values/feelings of goods [Grabenhorst and Rolls, 2011; O'Doherty, 2004], including normatively valued goods such as equality [Baumgartner et al., 2011; Tricomi et al., 2010]. Therefore, the lower activation (and often deactivation) of vmPFC in response to unfair offers may reflect negative feelings to norm violations, a viewpoint supported by the correlation between subjective feelings of offers and vmPFC activation [Xiang et al., 2013].

The amygdala is thought to reflect the early and transient emotional response to norm violations, and the failure to detect a consistent activation of this region in the meta‐analysis may be attributed to the coarse onset timing used in most studies [Gospic et al., 2011]. Nevertheless, recent studies have revealed the crucial role of this area in social preferences. For example, amygdala activation to unfair offers predicts individual differences in social preferences for equitable outcomes [Haruno and Frith, 2010; Haruno et al., 2014] and willingness to enforce social norms [Gospic et al., 2011; Yu et al., 2014]. Moreover, Gospic et al. [2011] demonstrated a causal role of amygdala in the norm enforcement by showing that pharmacological attenuation of amygdala response led to decreased costly punishment in UG.

Economic self‐interest constitutes another essential motivation in UG and conflicts with the intuitive response mediated by System 1 [Sanfey and Chang, 2008; Sanfey et al., 2006]. The meta‐analysis identified the dACC—a key region of System 2—in response to norm violations. Based on the role of the dACC in conflict monitoring [Botvinick et al., 2001, 2004], this region presumably monitors the conflict between economic self‐interest and intuitive response to norm violations and signals the need to resolve it [Fehr and Camerer, 2007; Sanfey et al., 2003]. Previous evidence revealed that the neural response to norm violations in dACC is evident only when self‐interest is involved in norm enforcement [Civai et al., 2012] and the response is stronger for those responders who are more prone to self‐interest, and therefore, experience larger conflicts [Güroğlu et al., 2010, 2011; Xiang et al., 2013; Zhou et al., 2014]. However, other potential suggested functions of dACC in the context of UG, such as detection of norm violations [Chang and Sanfey, 2013; Güroğlu et al., 2010; White et al., 2013], emotion appraisal [Etkin et al., 2011; Harlé et al., 2012], and cognitive control [Grecucci et al., 2013; Kerns et al., 2004] have not been well dissociated and are worth elaborating in future studies.

On the one hand, the motivational conflict encoded in the dACC may be resolved by regulating/suppressing the reflexive response of System 1 [Rilling and Sanfey, 2011; Sanfey and Chang, 2008]. Our meta‐analysis identified potential brain regions recruited in favor of the acceptance of unfair offers, including vlPFC [Halko et al., 2009; Tabibnia et al., 2008], rACC [Yu et al., 2014], left dlPFC [Güroğlu et al., 2011; Harlé and Sanfey, 2012], and dmPFC [Grecucci et al., 2013], all of which are generally involved in emotion regulation or cognitive reappraisal [Buhle et al., in press; Etkin et al., 2006; Ochsner et al., 2012; Silvers et al., 2013]. Specifically, the vlPFC may exert top‐down control in regulating AI activity [Tabibnia et al., 2008] by accessing conceptual knowledge represented in the lateral temporal areas as an intermediary to reinterpret the meaning of a situation, which then feeds forward the reinterpreted representation to dorsal AI as a target region of System 1 [Silvers et al., 2013]. The rACC is associated with resolving emotional conflict through top‐down inhibition of amygdala activation [Etkin et al., 2006, 2011]. For example, Yu et al. [2014] observed that rACC suppresses amygdala‐mediated negative emotional response to norm violations, and this coupling between rACC and amygdala predicts attenuated costly punishment. Furthermore, the modulations of left dlPFC on amygdala and vmPFC have been reported in the literature of reappraisal‐relevant emotion regulation [Ochsner et al., 2012], and the left dlPFC may be recruited together with inferior parietal regions to direct attention to reappraisal‐relevant events and maintain reappraisal goals [Ochsner et al., 2012; Silvers et al., 2013]. Finally, given the role of dmPFC in overtly thinking about the internal mental states of others (i.e., mentalizing) [Frith and Frith, 2003; Lieberman, 2007], this region probably supports selective attention to and elaboration of intentions of proposers in UG [Frith and Singer, 2008; Rilling et al., 2004].

On the other hand, the motivational conflict between self‐interest and fairness norm enforcement could also be resolved by over‐riding temptations of self‐interest in favor of the rejection of unfair offers. The right dlPFC as identified in the meta‐analysis has been previously associated with this cognitive mechanism of System 2 [Knoch and Fehr, 2007]. Previous evidence showed that activation of right dlPFC in UG is positively correlated with altruistic punishment behavior [Guo et al., 2014; Roalf, 2010; Zheng et al., in press]. Importantly, disruption of the right dlPFC due to transcranial magnetic stimulation or cathodal transcranial direct current stimulation diminishes the enforcement of fairness norms in UG, providing evidence for a causal role of the right dlPFC in enforcing social norms [Knoch et al., 2006, 2008; Van't Wout et al., 2005].

Besides the role of cognitive control implemented by the prefrontal and cingulate cortex (System 2), an alternative account proposes that these brain areas may contribute to integrating different sources of information (e.g., expectations) and optimizing response selection in specific social contexts [Buckholtz and Marois, 2012; Sanfey et al., 2014]. The “integration‐and‐selection” hypothesis is consistent with recent findings showing that functions served by System 2 are context‐dependent [Grecucci et al., 2013; Ruff et al., 2013]. For instance, brain stimulation of right dlPFC caused opposite effects on norm compliance under different social contexts: it enhances the compliance of social norms in the context of potential sanctions but attenuates voluntary norm compliance [Ruff et al., 2013].

Taken together, normative decision making in the UG may engage two separable but interacting neural systems. System 1 (AI and vmPFC) may be associated with the reflexive and intuitive responses to norm violations, representing a motivation to punish the violators. Those intuitive responses conflict with economic self‐interest, resulting into a conflict signal encoded by the dACC, which may activate cognitive control from a reflective and deliberate System 2 to resolve the conflict by either suppressing (vlPFC, rostral ACC, dmPFC, and left dlPFC) the intuitive responses of System 1 or over‐riding self‐interest (right dlPFC).

Several limitations should be noted as they relate to this meta‐analysis. First, like most neuroimaging meta‐analysis methods, the procedures of the ALE meta‐analysis consider only the reported coordinates and number of subjects from each study, but not other potential mediator variables (e.g., fMRI‐scanning and data‐analysis parameters) that are different between studies and may influence final results. Second, the number of papers used in this meta‐analysis was relatively small (especially for the [fair > unfair] contrast), but statistical power for interpretation of results will increase for future meta‐analyses due to an accumulating number of neuroimaging studies on human normative decision making. In addition, because of the limited number of studies in this domain, it allowed us only to perform a basic contrast between unfair and fair but not moderator analyses (e.g., contextual factors and individual differences), which might have been helpful in differentiating the reflexive and reflective systems. Third, the potential functions of brain regions involved in fairness‐related norm enforcement were evaluated in the context of evidence from neuroscientific studies in the past decades, but the specific functions of many brain areas remain to be clarified. For example, the potential functions of dACC in UG have been rarely dissociated in previous studies, although this brain region constitutes the most consistent reported cluster in this meta‐analysis. Furthermore, to the best of our knowledge, no study has yet provided evidence for a causal role of AI in costly punishment, although many researchers have considered that psychological components mediated by this region play a pivotal role in norm enforcement. Finally, this study identified Systems 1 and 2 involved in the normative decision making; however, the interactions between these neural systems remain to be elucidated. Future functional and effective connectivity fMRI studies applying the UG are necessary to investigate the temporal and causal relationships among regions, which would provide a more comprehensive picture of human norm enforcement.

Conflict of Interest: None of the authors has conflicts of interests to declare.

REFERENCES

  1. Aoki R, Matsumoto M, Yomogida Y, Izuma K, Murayama K, Sugiura A, Camerer CF, Adolphs R, Matsumoto K (2014): Social equality in the number of choice options is represented in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex. J Neurosci 34:6413–6421. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Ballard K, Knutson B (2009): Dissociable neural representations of future reward magnitude and delay during temporal discounting. Neuroimage 45:143–150. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Baumgartner T, Knoch D, Hotz P, Eisenegger C, Fehr E (2011): Dorsolateral and ventromedial prefrontal cortex orchestrate normative choice. Nat Neurosci 14:1468–1474. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Blount S (1995): When social outcomes aren't fair: The effect of causal attributions on preferences. Organ Behav Hum Decis Process 63:131–144. [Google Scholar]
  5. Botvinick MM, Braver TS, Barch DM, Carter CS, Cohen JD (2001): Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychol Rev 108:624–652. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Botvinick MM, Cohen JD, Carter CS (2004): Conflict monitoring and anterior cingulate cortex: An update. Trends Cogn Sci 8:539–546. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Brett M (1999): The MNI brain and the Talairach atlas, Cambridgge Imagers. Available at: http://www.mrc-cbu.cam.ac.uk/Imaging/mnispace.html. Last accessed: 10 October 2014.
  8. Brüne M, Scheele D, Heinisch C, Tas C, Wischniewski J, Güntürkün O (2012): Empathy moderates the effect of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation of the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex on costly punishment. PloS One 7:e44747. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Buckholtz JW, Marois R (2012): The roots of modern justice: Cognitive and neural foundations of social norms and their enforcement. Nat Neurosci 15:655–661. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Buckholtz JW, Asplund CL, Dux PE, Zald DH, Gore JC, Jones OD, Marois R (2008): The neural correlates of third‐party punishment. Neuron 60:930–940. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Buhle JT, Silvers JA, Wager TD, Lopez R, Onyemekwu C, Kober H, Weber J, Ochsner KN: Cognitive reappraisal of emotion: A meta‐analysis of human neuroimaging studies. Cereb Cortex (in press). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Camerer C (2003): Behavioral game theory: Experiments in strategic interaction. Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ. [Google Scholar]
  13. Chang LJ, Sanfey AG (2013): Great expectations: Neural computations underlying the use of social norms in decision‐making. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 8:277–284. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Chang LJ, Yarkoni T, Khaw MW, Sanfey AG (2013): Decoding the role of the insula in human cognition: functional parcellation and large‐scale reverse inference. Cereb Cortex 23:739–749. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Civai C (2013): Rejecting unfairness: Emotion‐driven reaction or cognitive heuristic? Front Hum Neurosci 7:126. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Civai C, Corradi‐Dell'Acqua C, Gamer M, Rumiati RI (2010): Are irrational reactions to unfairness truly emotionally‐driven? Dissociated behavioural and emotional responses in the ultimatum game task. Cognition 114:89–95. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Civai C, Crescentini C, Rustichini A, Rumiati RI (2012): Equality versus self‐interest in the brain: Differential roles of anterior insula and medial prefrontal cortex. Neuroimage 62:102–112. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Civai C, Rumiati RI, Rustichini A (2013): More equal than others: Equity norms as an integration of cognitive heuristics and contextual cues in bargaining games. Acta Psychol 144:12–18. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Corradi‐Dell'Acqua C, Civai C, Rumiati RI, Fink GR (2013): Disentangling self‐and fairness‐related neural mechanisms involved in the ultimatum game: an fMRI study. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 8:424–431. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Critchley HD, Wiens S, Rotshtein P, Öhman A, Dolan RJ (2004): Neural systems supporting interoceptive awareness. Nat Neurosci 7:189–195. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Dawes CT, Loewen PJ, Schreiber D, Simmons AN, Flagan T, McElreath R, Bokemper SE, Fowler JH, Paulus MP (2012): Neural basis of egalitarian behavior. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 109:6479–6483. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Eickhoff SB, Laird AR, Grefkes C, Wang LE, Zilles K, Fox PT (2009): Coordinate‐based activation likelihood estimation meta‐analysis of neuroimaging data: A random‐effects approach based on empirical estimates of spatial uncertainty. Hum Brain Mapp 30:2907–2926. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Eickhoff SB, Bzdok D, Laird AR, Kurth F, Fox PT (2012): Activation likelihood estimation meta‐analysis revisited. Neuroimage 59:2349–2361. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Etkin A, Egner T, Peraza DM, Kandel ER, Hirsch J (2006): Resolving emotional conflict: A role for the rostral anterior cingulate cortex in modulating activity in the amygdala. Neuron 51:871–882. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Etkin A, Egner T, Kalisch R (2011): Emotional processing in anterior cingulate and medial prefrontal cortex. Trends Cogn Sci 15:85–93. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Fehr E, Camerer CF (2007): Social neuroeconomics: The neural circuitry of social preferences. Trends Cogn Sci 11:419–427. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Fehr E, Fischbacher U (2004): Social norms and human cooperation. Trends Cogn Sci 8:185–190. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Fehr E, Gächter S (2002): Altruistic punishment in humans. Nature 415:137–140. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Fehr E, Schmidt KM (1999): A theory of fairness, competition, and cooperation. Q J Econ 114:817–868. [Google Scholar]
  30. Fehr E, Fischbacher U, Gächter S (2002): Strong reciprocity, human cooperation, and the enforcement of social norms. Hum Nat 13:1–25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Fehr E, Bernhard H, Rockenbach B (2008): Egalitarianism in young children. Nature 454:1079–1083. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Fliessbach K, Weber B, Trautner P, Dohmen T, Sunde U, Elger CE, Falk A (2007): Social comparison affects reward‐related brain activity in the human ventral striatum. Science 318:1305–1308. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Frith U, Frith CD (2003): Development and neurophysiology of mentalizing. Philos Trans R Soc Lond Ser B Biol Sci 358:459–473. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Frith CD, Singer T (2008): The role of social cognition in decision making. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci 363:3875–3886. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Genovese CR, Lazar NA, Nichols T (2002): Thresholding of statistical maps in functional neuroimaging using the false discovery rate. Neuroimage 15:870–878. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Gospic K, Mohlin E, Fransson P, Petrovic P, Johannesson M, Ingvar M (2011): Limbic justice—Amygdala involvement in immediate rejection in the ultimatum game. PLoS Biol 9:e1001054. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Gospic K, Sundberg M, Maeder J, Fransson P, Petrovic P, Isacsson G, Karlström A, Ingvar M: Altruism costs—The cheap signal from amygdala. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci (in press). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Grabenhorst F, Rolls ET (2011): Value, pleasure and choice in the ventral prefrontal cortex. Trends Cogn Sci, 15:56–67. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Grecucci A, Giorgetta C, van't Wout M, Bonini N, Sanfey AG (2013): Reappraising the ultimatum: An fMRI study of emotion regulation and decision making. Cereb Cortex 23:399–410. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Guo X, Zheng L, Cheng X, Chen M, Zhu L, Li J, Chen L, Yang Z (2014): Neural responses to unfairness and fairness depend on self‐contribution to the income. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 9:1498–1505. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Guo X, Zheng L, Zhu L, Li J, Wang Q, Dienes Z, Yang Z (2013): Increased neural responses to unfairness in a loss context. Neuroimage 77:246–253. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Güroğlu B, van den Bos W, Rombouts SA, Crone EA (2010): Unfair? It depends: Neural correlates of fairness in social context. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 5:414–423. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Güroğlu B, van den Bos W, van Dijk E, Rombouts SA, Crone EA (2011): Dissociable brain networks involved in development of fairness considerations: Understanding intentionality behind unfairness. Neuroimage 57:634–641. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Güth W, Schmittberger R, Schwarze B (1982): An experimental analysis of ultimatum bargaining. J Econ Behav Organ 3:367–388. [Google Scholar]
  45. Haidt J (2001): The emotional dog and its rational tail: A social intuitionist approach to moral judgment. Psychol Rev 108:814–834. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Halko M.‐L, Hlushchuk Y, Hari R, Schürmann M (2009): Competing with peers: Mentalizing‐related brain activity reflects what is at stake. Neuroimage 46:542–548. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Harlé KM, Sanfey AG (2007): Incidental sadness biases social economic decisions in the ultimatum game. Emotion 7:876–881. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Harlé KM, Sanfey AG (2012): Social economic decision‐making across the lifespan: An fMRI investigation. Neuropsychologia 50:1416–1424. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Harlé KM, Chang LJ, van't Wout M, Sanfey AG (2012): The neural mechanisms of affect infusion in social economic decision‐making: A mediating role of the anterior insula. Neuroimage 61:32–40. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Haruno M, Frith CD (2010): Activity in the amygdala elicited by unfair divisions predicts social value orientation. Nat Neurosci 13:160–161. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Haruno M, Kimura M, Frith CD (2014): Activity in the nucleus accumbens and amygdala underlies individual differences in prosocial and individualistic economic choices. J Cogn Neurosci 26:1861–1870. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Henrich J, McElreath R, Barr A, Ensminger J, Barrett C, Bolyanatz A, Cardenas JC, Gurven M, Gwako E, Henrich N (2006): Costly punishment across human societies. Science 312:1767–1770. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Hsu M, Anen C, Quartz SR (2008): The right and the good: distributive justice and neural encoding of equity and efficiency. Science 320:1092–1095. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Kelly C, Toro R, Di Martino A, Cox CL, Bellec P, Castellanos FX, Milham MP (2012): A convergent functional architecture of the insula emerges across imaging modalities. Neuroimage 61:1129–1142. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Kerns JG, Cohen JD, MacDonald AW, Cho RY, Stenger VA, Carter CS (2004): Anterior cingulate conflict monitoring and adjustments in control. Science 303:1023–1026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Kirk U, Downar J, Montague PR (2011): Interoception drives increased rational decision‐making in meditators playing the ultimatum game. Front Neurosci 5:49. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Knoch D, Fehr E (2007): Resisting the power of temptations. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1104:123–134. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Knoch D, Pascual‐Leone A, Meyer K, Treyer V, Fehr E (2006): Diminishing reciprocal fairness by disrupting the right prefrontal cortex. Science 314:829–832. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Knoch D, Nitsche MA, Fischbacher U, Eisenegger C, Pascual‐Leone A, Fehr E (2008): Studying the neurobiology of social interaction with transcranial direct current stimulation—The example of punishing unfairness. Cereb Cortex 18:1987–1990. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Koenigs M, Tranel D (2007): Irrational economic decision‐making after ventromedial prefrontal damage: Evidence from the ultimatum game. J Neurosci 27:951–956. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Krueger F, Grafman J, McCabe K (2008): Neural correlates of economic game playing. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci 363:3859–3874. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Laird AR, Fox PM, Price CJ, Glahn DC, Uecker AM, Lancaster JL, Turkeltaub PE, Kochunov P, Fox PT (2005): ALE meta‐analysis: Controlling the false discovery rate and performing statistical contrasts. Hum Brain Mapp 25:155–164. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Lamm C, Decety J, Singer T (2011): Meta‐analytic evidence for common and distinct neural networks associated with directly experienced pain and empathy for pain. Neuroimage 54:2492–2502. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Levy DJ, Glimcher PW (2011): Comparing apples and oranges: Using reward‐specific and reward‐general subjective value representation in the brain. J Neurosci 31:14693–14707. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Lieberman MD (2007): Social cognitive neuroscience: a review of core processes. Annu Rev Psychol 58:259–289. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. McClure SM, Ericson KM, Laibson DI, Loewenstein G, Cohen JD (2007): Time discounting for primary rewards. J Neurosci 27:5796–5804. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Moretti L, Dragone D, Di Pellegrino G (2009): Reward and social valuation deficits following ventromedial prefrontal damage. J Cogn Neurosci 21:128–140. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. O'Doherty JP (2004): Reward representations and reward‐related learning in the human brain: Insights from neuroimaging. Curr Opin Neurobiol 14:769–776. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Ochsner KN, Silvers JA, Buhle JT (2012): Functional imaging studies of emotion regulation: A synthetic review and evolving model of the cognitive control of emotion. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1251:E1–E24. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Pillutla MM, Murnighan JK (1996): Unfairness, anger, and spite: Emotional rejections of ultimatum offers. Organ Behav Hum Decis Process 68:208–224. [Google Scholar]
  71. Rabin M (1993): Incorporating fairness into game theory and economics. Am Econ Rev 83:1281–1302. [Google Scholar]
  72. Rilling JK, Sanfey AG (2011): The neuroscience of social decision‐making. Ann Rev Psychol 62:23–48. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Rilling JK, Sanfey AG, Aronson JA, Nystrom LE, Cohen JD (2004): The neural correlates of theory of mind within interpersonal interactions. Neuroimage 22:1694–1703. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Roalf DR (2010): It's not fair!: Behavioral and neural evidence that equity influences social economic decisions in healthy older adults. Student Scholar Archive, Paper 516. [Google Scholar]
  75. Roch SG, Lane JA, Samuelson CD, Allison ST, Dent JL (2000): Cognitive load and the equality heuristic: A two‐stage model of resource overconsumption in small groups. Organ Behav Hum Decis Process 83:185–212. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Ruff CC, Fehr E (2014): The neurobiology of rewards and values in social decision making. Nat Rev Neurosci 15:549–562. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Ruff C, Ugazio G, Fehr E (2013): Changing social norm compliance with noninvasive brain stimulation. Science 342:482–484. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Sanfey AG (2007): Social decision‐making: insights from game theory and neuroscience. Science 318:598–602. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Sanfey AG, Chang LJ (2008): Multiple systems in decision making. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1128:53–62. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Sanfey AG, Rilling JK, Aronson JA, Nystrom LE, Cohen JD (2003): The neural basis of economic decision‐making in the ultimatum game. Science 300:1755–1758. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Sanfey AG, Loewenstein G, McClure SM, Cohen JD (2006): Neuroeconomics: Cross‐currents in research on decision‐making. Trends Cogn Sci 10:108–116. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Sanfey AG, Stallen M, Chang LJ (2014): Norms and expectations in social decision‐making. Trends Cogn Sci 18:172–174. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Scheele D, Mihov Y, Kendrick KM, Feinstein JS, Reich H, Maier W, Hurlemann R (2012): Amygdala lesion profoundly alters altruistic punishment. Biol Psychiatry 72:e5–e7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Silvers JA, Buhle JT, Ochsner KN, Silvers J (2013): The neuroscience of emotion regulation: Basic mechanisms and their role in development, aging and psychopathology. The Oxford Handbook of Cognitive Neuroscience, Vol. 2: The Cutting Edges. p. 52. Oxford University Press, New York, NY.
  85. Singer T, Critchley HD, Preuschoff K (2009): A common role of insula in feelings, empathy and uncertainty. Trends Cogn Sci 13:334–340. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Steinmann E, Schmalor A, Prehn‐Kristensen A, Wolff S, Galka A, Möhring J, Gerber W.‐D, Petermann F, Stephani U, Siniatchkin M (2014): Developmental changes of neuronal networks associated with strategic social decision‐making. Neuropsychologia 56:37–46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Strobel A, Zimmermann J, Schmitz A, Reuter M, Lis S, Windmann S, Kirsch P (2011): Beyond revenge: neural and genetic bases of altruistic punishment. Neuroimage 54:671–680. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Tabibnia G, Lieberman MD (2007): Fairness and cooperation are rewarding. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1118:90–101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Tabibnia G, Satpute AB, Lieberman MD (2008): The sunny side of fairness preference for fairness activates reward circuitry (and disregarding unfairness activates self‐control circuitry). Psychol Sci 19:339–347. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Tomasino B, Lotto L, Sarlo M, Civai C, Rumiati R, Rumiati RI (2013): Framing the ultimatum game: The contribution of simulation. Front Hum Neurosci 7:337. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Tricomi E, Rangel A, Camerer CF, O'Doherty JP (2010): Neural evidence for inequality‐averse social preferences. Nature 463:1089–1091. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Turkeltaub PE, Eickhoff SB, Laird AR, Fox M, Wiener M, Fox P (2012): Minimizing within‐experiment and within‐group effects in activation likelihood estimation meta‐analyses. Hum Brain Mapp 33:1–13. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Van't Wout M, Kahn RS, Sanfey AG, Aleman A (2005): Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation over the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex affects strategic decision‐making. Neuroreport 16:1849–1852. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Van't Wout M, Kahn RS, Sanfey AG, Aleman A (2006): Affective state and decision‐making in the ultimatum game. Exp Brain Res 169:564–568. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. White SF, Brislin SJ, Meffert H, Sinclair S, Blair RJR (2013): Callous‐unemotional traits modulate the neural response associated with punishing another individual during social exchange: A preliminary investigation. J Pers Disord 27:99. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. White SF, Brislin SJ, Sinclair S, Blair JR (2014): Punishing unfairness: Rewarding or the organization of a reactively aggressive response? Hum Brain Mapp 35:2137–2147. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Wright ND, Symmonds M, Fleming SM, Dolan RJ (2011): Neural segregation of objective and contextual aspects of fairness. J Neurosci 31:5244–5252. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Wu Y, Yu H, Shen B, Yu R, Zhou Z, Zhang G, Jiang Y, Zhou X: Neural basis of increased costly norm enforcement under adversity. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci (in press). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Xiang T, Lohrenz T, Montague PR (2013): Computational substrates of norms and their violations during social exchange. J Neurosci 33:1099–1108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Xiao E, Houser D (2005): Emotion expression in human punishment behavior. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 102:7398–7401. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Yamagishi T, Horita Y, Takagishi H, Shinada M, Tanida S, Cook KS (2009): The private rejection of unfair offers and emotional commitment. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA, 106:11520–11523. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Yu R, Calder AJ, Mobbs D (2014): Overlapping and distinct representations of advantageous and disadvantageous inequality. Hum Brain Mapp 35:3290–3301. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Zaki J, Mitchell JP (2011): Equitable decision making is associated with neural markers of intrinsic value. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 108:19761–19766. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Zaki J, Mitchell JP (2013): Intuitive prosociality. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 22:466–470. [Google Scholar]
  105. Zheng L, Guo X, Zhu L, Li J, Chen L, Dienes Z: Whether others were treated equally or not affects neural responses to unfairness in the ultimatum game. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci (in press). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Zhou Y, Wang Y, Rao L.‐L, Yang L, Li S (2014): Money talks: Neural substrate of modulation of fairness by monetary incentives. Front Behav Neurosci 8:150. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Human Brain Mapping are provided here courtesy of Wiley

RESOURCES