Skip to main content
Human Brain Mapping logoLink to Human Brain Mapping
. 2011 Apr 4;33(7):1553–1581. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21307

Enhanced visual functioning in autism: An ALE meta‐analysis

Fabienne Samson 1, Laurent Mottron 1, Isabelle Soulières 1,2, Thomas A Zeffiro 2,
PMCID: PMC6870295  PMID: 21465627

Abstract

Autistics often exhibit enhanced perceptual abilities when engaged in visual search, visual discrimination, and embedded figure detection. In similar fashion, while performing a range of perceptual or cognitive tasks, autistics display stronger physiological engagement of the visual system than do non‐autistics. To account for these findings, the Enhanced Perceptual Functioning Model proposes that enhanced autistic performance in basic perceptual tasks results from stronger engagement of sensory processing mechanisms, a situation that may facilitate an atypically prominent role for perceptual mechanisms in supporting cognition. Using quantitative meta‐analysis of published functional imaging studies from which Activation Likelihood Estimation maps were computed, we asked whether autism is associated with enhanced task‐related activity for a broad range of visual tasks. To determine whether atypical engagement of visual processing is a general or domain‐specific phenomenon, we examined three different visual processing domains: faces, objects, and words. Overall, we observed more activity in autistics compared to non‐autistics in temporal, occipital, and parietal regions. In contrast, autistics exhibited less activity in frontal cortex. The spatial distribution of the observed differential between‐group patterns varied across processing domains. Autism may be characterized by enhanced functional resource allocation in regions associated with visual processing and expertise. Atypical adult organizational patterns may reflect underlying differences in developmental neural plasticity that can result in aspects of the autistic phenotype, including enhanced visual skills, atypical face processing, and hyperlexia. Hum Brain Mapp, 2011 © 2011 Wiley‐Liss, Inc.

Keywords: hyperlexia, reading, fMRI, vision, perception, enhanced perceptual functioning model, expertise, plasticity

INTRODUCTION

Atypical perceptual processing, often manifested as enhanced perceptual performance [Dakin and Frith,2005], is now included as an associated feature of the autistic phenotype [Belmonte et al.,2004]. Autistic visual strengths are consistently reported for the Block Design subtest of the Wechsler Intelligence Scales [Caron et al.,2006; Shah and Frith,1993], the Embedded Figures Task [Jolliffe and Baron‐Cohen,1997], visual search tasks [Joseph et al.,2009; Kemner et al.,2008; O'Riordan,2004; O'Riordan et al.,2001], and visual discrimination tasks [Bertone et al.,2005; Plaisted et al.,1998]. In addition, an increasing number of studies have demonstrated autistic early sensory processing advantages or atypicalities in stimulus dimension extraction, with examples including crowding [Baldassi et al.,2009; Keita et al.,2010], contour and texture processing [Pei et al.,2009; Vandenbroucke et al.,2008] and spatial frequency processing [Jemel et al.,2010; Milne et al.,2009]. These behavioral characteristics, along with other aspects of the autistic perceptual phenotype, have been summarized in the Enhanced Perceptual Functioning Model (EPF) [Mottron et al.,2006]. Assuming generally stronger physiological engagement of the visual system in autism, this model predicts generally superior perceptual performance and a wider role for perceptual processes in autistic cognition. It also incorporates the observation that autistics1 display better access to information typically masked by top‐down influences [Wang et al.,2007], as well as relative autonomy of perceptual processes from top‐down influences [Caron et al.,2006; Soulières et al.,2009].

Several neuroimaging studies have revealed stronger task‐related activity in visual cortex in autism, evidenced as either higher levels of activity associated with visual information processing, or as serendipitous findings in studies employing memory or language tasks. In association with the Embedded Figures Test, autistic brain activity is higher in right occipital cortex, left posterior parietal cortex, bilateral occipital cortex, and bilateral cerebellar cortex, and lower in frontal cortex [Lee et al.,2007; Manjaly et al.,2007; Ring et al.,1999]. Higher occipital cortex activity in autistics is seen in relation to faster and more accurate visual search [Keehn et al.,2008]. These results suggest that the autistics' behavioral advantages might arise from stronger and more pervasive engagement of visual processing mechanisms. Stronger occipital activity has also been reported in association with reduced frontal activity in autism for tasks incorporating a broad range of cognitive and perceptual components, including embedded figure detection [Ring et al.,1999], attention shifting [Belmonte and Yurgelun‐Todd2003], word learning [Hazlett et al.,2004], saccades to visual targets [Luna et al.,2002], working memory [Koshino et al.,2005], visuomotor learning [Muller et al.,2003], face processing [Hubl et al.,2003], and social attribution [Castelli et al.,2002]. The wide variety of tasks associated with higher activity in autistics' visual cortical areas suggests that the atypical physiological processing mechanisms may be related to task performance in a less straightforward way than initially posited by the EPF model.

Quantitative meta‐analysis of functional neuroimaging studies is one means to characterize the role of perceptual processes in autism. Neuroimaging meta‐analysis combines results from independent experiments to achieve a quantitative summary of the state of research in a specific domain [Turkeltaub et al.,2002]. It assesses the reliability of results across imaging techniques, tasks, and laboratories by revealing consistently modulated voxel activity in a collection of studies. In addition, meta‐analysis can establish the specificity of the relationship between a region or network of regions and a particular task type [Wager et al.,2009]. Voxel‐wise meta‐analysis of neuroimaging studies, called Activation Likelihood Estimation (ALE; Turkeltaub et al.,2002] has recently been used in autism to document between‐group differences in activity related to social compared to non‐social tasks [Di Martino et al.,2009].

We used ALE meta‐analysis to summarize patterns of activity related to visual processing by merging activity maxima reported in experiments including both autistic and non‐autistic groups, a process that resulted in group maps assessing the regions of common task‐related modulation across studies. Maps revealing regions differently engaged between groups were then generated by contrasting the within‐group ALE maps [Laird et al.,2005]. We included the coordinates of activity increases for each group instead of using the reported coordinates of differential activity between autistics and non‐autistics, an approach used in a recent autism meta‐analysis [Di Martino et al.,2009]. Our method allowed identification of processing activity without any a priori bias that might result from including only studies reporting higher or lower activity in autistics compared to non‐autistics. For instance, some reports do not include tables listing coordinates related to higher activity in autistics, even when such findings are described in the body of the paper. To minimize regional selection bias, we also limited our meta‐analysis to studies that reported coordinates resulting from whole‐brain analysis, as contrasted with region‐of‐interest (ROI) analysis. Because the resource allocation proposition, stated as Principle 4 of the EPF Model [Mottron et al.,2006], was primarily based on a review of neuroimaging studies of visual perception in autistics, and because there are only a limited number of neuroimaging studies of auditory processing, we limited the current analysis to studies employing visual stimuli.

In this meta‐analysis our aim was to quantitatively summarize the neuroimaging findings concerning visual processing in autism in order to test the prediction that autistics will exhibit generally stronger engagement of the visual system. Additionally, we explored the relative domain specificity of atypical visual processes in autism, by examining whether any differences between autistics and non‐autistics showed specificity for face, object, or word stimulus classes.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Literature Review and Contrast Selection

We performed a PubMed literature search (http://www.pubmed.org) to identify functional neuroimaging studies published from 1995 to July 2009 in which visual stimuli were presented to both autistic and non‐autistic groups. For this analysis, what we call the autistic group included participants with diagnostic assignments falling within what are generally referred to as autism spectrum conditions. We used the following search terms: «(Autism OR Asperger OR PDD) AND (fMRI OR PET OR Neuroimaging)» and retrieved 787 articles. Among those, 692 were excluded through an initial review of the abstracts. Studies excluded were 217 reviews, 207 reports without an autistic group, 255 reports not using PET or fMRI, and 19 reports including no visual stimuli. Of the remaining 89 studies, 22 were rejected because of small sample size (n < 10), 21 because of partial brain coverage or analysis, 11 because results were not reported in a standard anatomical space, and 9 because only between‐group contrasts were presented. The remaining 26 peer‐reviewed fMRI articles reporting within‐group results using whole brain acquisition techniques in a standardized stereotaxic space were included in the meta‐analysis. Coordinates reported in MNI space were converted to Talairach anatomical space using the “Convert Foci” tool of the GingerALE 1.1 program [Laird et al.,2005]. This tool uses the icbm2tal Lancaster transform [Lancaster et al.,2007]. The total number of participants included 370 typical controls and 357 individuals with an autism spectrum condition determination. Most studies were conducted on adults and all included participants with Full Scale IQ in the normal range. Seven out of the 26 studies included only autistics, while the others included autistics, individuals with Asperger syndrome and Pervasive Developmental Disorder Not Otherwise Specified (Table I).

Table I.

Participant characteristics for the studies included in the meta‐analysis

Reference N(nAUT) Age M (SD) (nAUT) N(AUT) Age M (SD) (AUT) AUT AS PDD
Bird et al.,2006 16 33.3 (11.5) 16 35.3 (12.1) 1 15
Bookheimer et al.,2008 12 11.9 (2.4) 12 11.3 (4.0) Unspecified
Dapretto et al.,2006 10 12.38 (2.2) 10 12.05 (2.5) Unspecified
Ditcher and Belger, 2007 15 23.2 (5.7) 14 22.9 (5.2) 11 3
Gaffey et al., 2007 10 25.3 (9.8) 10 26.1 (10.5) 8 2
Greimel et al., 2010 15 15 (1.4) 15 14.9 (1.6) 3 12
Harris et al.,2006 22 31 (9) 14 36 (12) 7 5 2
Hubl et al.,2003 10 25.3 (6.9) 10 27.7 (7.8) 10
Just et al.,2004 17 N/A 17 N/A 17
Just et al.,2007 18 24.5 (9.9) 18 27.1 (11.9) 18
Kana et al.,2009 12 24.4 (3.7) 12 24.6 (6.9) 12
Kennedy et al.,2006 14 26.07 (7.95) 12 25.49 (9.61) 8 3 1
Kennedy et al., 2008 12 27.5 (10.9) 13 26.9 (12.3) 6 6 1
Kleinhans et al., 2008a 14 22.41 (8.67) 14 23.79 (9.58) 8 3 3
Kleinhans et al., 2008b 21 25.1 (7.6) 19 23.5 (7.8) 8 9 2
Knaus et al.,2008 12 14.94 (2.71) 12 15.45 (2.48) Unspecified
Koshino et al., 2007 11 28.7 (10.9) 11 24.5 (10.2) 11
Lee et al.,2007 14 10.85 (1.47) 17 10.37 (1.85) 8 9
Manjaly et al.,2007 12 14.3 (2.7) 12 14.4 (2.8) 3 9
Mason et al.,2008 18 27.4 (N/A) 18 26.5 (N/A) 18
Schmitz et al.,2006 12 39 (6) 10 38 (9) 2 8
Schmitz et al.,2008 10 20‐50 (N/A) 10 20‐50 (N/A) 3 7
Silani et al.,2008 15 33.7 (10.3) 15 36.6 (11.7) Unspecified
Solomon et al.,2009 23 15.9 (2.1) 22 15.2 (1.7) 10 12
Soulières et al.,2009 13 20.15 (3.02) 12 22.08 (4.91) 12
Uddin et al., 2008 12 12.23 (2.10) 12 13.19 (2.61) Unspecified

A total of 48 contrasts (504 foci) for the non‐autistic and 44 (415 foci) for the autistic group were identified in the 26 included studies. These contrasts were categorized according to domain specificity into face, object, and word categories. The contrasts for one study [Silani et al.,2008] could not be classified, as the stimuli contained both faces and animal pictures. These contrasts were eliminated from the domain specific analysis. The face processing domain included face viewing, discrimination, matching, recognition, imitation, and identification tasks as well as one task involving facial emotional state inference and one task involving gaze direction identification. Fourteen contrasts (134 foci) for the autistic group and 14 contrasts (175 foci) for the non‐autistic group were included in this domain. For the object processing domain, stimuli included pictures of houses, arrows, geometric shapes, complex figures, letters, patterns, in addition to more complex stimuli, including problems from the Tower of London task and Raven's Progressive Matrices. The tasks required matching, response inhibition, interference, identification, mental state attribution to shapes, and simple viewing. A total of 14 contrasts (123 foci) were assigned to the object processing domain for the autistic group and 15 contrasts (166 foci) for the non‐autistic group. Finally, the word processing domain included visually presented words or sentences, with participants identifying word category, making a semantic judgment, answering reading comprehension questions, counting words, or generating words in a given category (verbal fluency). The word processing domain included 14 contrasts (137 foci) for the autistic group and 17 contrasts (136 foci) for the non‐autistic group. In addition, we investigated the effect of contrasting high to low level baselines across all tasks, by computing separate ALE maps using either low level baselines, such as fixation or rest, or high level baselines such as complex figure matching. Both types of maps yielded superimposable patterns for both the autistic and non‐autistic groups. Therefore, in an effort to increase statistical power, contrasts with both high and low level baselines were pooled for all subsequent analyses.

ALE Meta‐Analysis

ALE maps were computed using GingerALE (version 1.1 http://www.brainmap.org/ale) software [Laird et al.,2005], based on methods introduced by Turkeltaub et al., [2002]. The ALE technique models the uncertainty in location of task‐related activity foci as Gaussian probability distributions, yielding statistical maps in which each voxel value represents an estimate of the likelihood that activity occurred in the studies included in the meta‐analysis. The critical threshold for the ALE map is set using a Monte Carlo permutation analysis of sets of randomly distributed foci. A FWHM of 8 mm was selected for the Gaussian probability distributions to reflect the average smoothness of the fMRI data from which the foci were derived. The critical threshold was set using a 5,000 permutations test, corrected for multiple comparisons (False Discovery Rate (FDR); Laird et al.,2005]. The model is of the fixed‐effects class and permits inferences over the studies included in the meta‐analysis.

Maps reflecting regions of convergence across all reported coordinates both within‐ and between‐groups were computed, using maxima drawn from all three processing domains. As there was an imbalance between the total number of foci included for the non‐autistic (48 experiments, 504 foci) and autistic (44 experiments, 415 foci) samples, it was necessary to randomly remove experiments from the non‐autistic group to equalize the number of foci between groups (44 experiments, 438 foci), increasing the possibility that the difference maps would reflect activity differences between groups rather than an imbalance in coordinate numbers between categories [Laird et al.,2005]. Second, domain specific within‐group ALE maps for face, object, and word processing were computed. For each domain, the number of experiments and foci were similar enough for direct comparison. To compare activity patterns between autistics and non‐autistics, the within‐group ALE maps were subtracted from one another and randomization testing with 5,000 permutations was performed. This procedure tests for the presence of differences between the groups under the null hypothesis that both sets of foci are uniformly distributed [Laird et al.,2005]. The critical threshold was set at p FDR (<0.05 (k = 250 voxels). To present results in the anatomical space most commonly used in the current literature, the ALE coordinate results were transformed into the MNI anatomical space using the Lancaster transform [Lancaster et al.,2007].

RESULTS

Behavior

Table II summarizes the behavioral findings for all studies included in the meta‐analysis. In the majority of studies, autistics and non‐autistics exhibited similar accuracies or response times. There were no significant between‐group differences in performance in 69% of the studies (18/26), whereas autistics showed better performance in 7.6% of the studies (2/26) and poorer performance in 23% of the studies (6/26). Across domains, no between‐group behavioral differences were observed in 64% of the face tasks (9/14 contrasts), 93% of the object tasks (14/15), and 71% of the word tasks (12/17 contrasts).

Table II.

A listing of: (1) studies included in the meta‐analysis, (2) tasks, (3) stimuli, (4) observed performance, (5) task contrasts, (6) processing domain, and (7) number of maxima for the autistic (AUT) and non‐autistic (nAUT) groups

Reference Task Stimuli Performance Contrast Domain nAUT AUT
Bird et al.,2006 Look at a fixation cross in the center of each face or house picture Photographs of houses and faces No task Faces vs. Houses Faces 5 5
Houses vs. Faces Objects 7 7
Indicate if two faces or two houses were the same or different (four pictures presented at same time, with attention directed to the houses or faces) No between‐group differences in RT or ACC Attended faces vs. Unattended faces Faces 3 2
Attended houses vs. Unattended houses Objects 2 0
Bookheimer et al.,2008 Select one of two choices to match a target face/target shape Oval forms and pictures of faces (upright or inverted) Less accurate responses in autistics for upright faces but no between‐group differences in RT Matching upright face to target vs. Form matching Faces 10 6
Matching inverted face to target vs. Form matching Faces 9 8
Dapretto et al.,2006 Observe or imitate faces Pictures of emotional faces No between‐group differences in RT or ACC Imitation of emotional faces vs. Fixation Faces 36 16
Observation of emotional faces vs. Fixation Faces 14 10
Ditcher and Belger, 2007 Reaction time flanker task: Indicate by button press whether a central stimulus (flanked by same or different direction stimuli) point to the left of to the right Arrow flanked by arrows No between‐group differences in RT or ACC Incongruent arrow vs. Congruent arrow Objects 8 10
Gaze pictures flanked by gaze pictures Incongruent gaze vs. Congruent gaze Faces 8 2
Gaffrey et al.,2007 Semantic: Indicate category (Tool, Color, Feeling) membership of words; Perceptual: Indicate if a target letter is present in an consonant string Words or letters No between‐group differences in RT, but the control group was more accurate for Colors and Feeling categories Semantic vs. perceptual Words 14 13
Greimel et al., 2010 Empathize with the person whose face is presented and infer the emotional state (Other) or judge their own response (Self); Baseline: Judge the width of neutral faces Happy, sad, neutral faces No between‐group differences in RT, but autistics made more errors when judging emotional state from weak expressions. Other vs. Face width judgment Faces 19 23
Self vs. Face width judgment Faces 19 14
Harris et al.,2006 Indicate if a word is positive/negative (semantic)or in upper/lower case (perceptual) Words No between‐group differences in RT or ACC Concrete vs. Abstract Words 8 4
Semantic vs. Perceptual Words 7 3
Hubl et al.,2003 Button press to happy face or face of a woman (for the real faces blocks); No task for the scrambled faces blocks Emotional and scrambled face pictures No between‐group differences in ACC, however RT were longer in autistics Real vs. Scrambled Faces 12 12
Just et al.,2004 Read a passive or active sentence and respond to a probe Sentences and probe No between‐group differences in ACC, but autistics responded faster than controls Sentence comprehension vs. Fixation Words 8 10
Just et al.,2007 Tower of London task : rearrange the position of 3 balls until they match a goal configuration Initial and goal configuration No between‐group differences in RT or ACC Tower of London (number of steps to goal) vs. Fixation Objects 13 19
Kana et al.,2009 Theory of mind: attributing mental state to the movement of geometrical figures Geometrical figures No between‐group differences in RT or ACC Theory of Mind vs. Random animations Objects 12 5
Kennedy et al.,2006 Count the number of presented words (emotional, neutral or number words) and select response (3, 4, 5 words). Words No between‐group differences in RT or ACC Count number of words vs. Fixation Objects 8 8
Count emotional vs. neutral words Objects 3 0
Kennedy et al., 2008 Statement: Make true/false judgments for statements about themselves (self) or a close other person (other) describing psychological personality traits (internal) or observable characteristics (external); Equation: Indicate if a math equation was true or false Statements or math equations No between‐group differences in RT or ACC All statements vs. Equation Words 11 8
Internal vs.External Words 4 0
External vs. Internal Words 7 12
Other vs. Self Words 6 4
Kleinhans et al., 2008a Verbal fluency: Generate as many words as possible beginning with a given letter or items in given category Letters or categories Autistics generated less words than control group for both conditions, but no between group differences in number of errors (word repetition, non‐target item, neologism) Generate words starting with a given letter vs. repeat “nothing” Words 1 1
Generate words in a given category vs. repeat “nothing” Words 3 3
Generate words in a category vs. starting with a given letter Words 5 0
Kleinhans et al., 2008b Press a button whenever identical stimuli appear in succession (1‐back) Pictures of neutral faces and houses No between‐group differences in RT or ACC Faces vs. Houses Faces 1 3
Houses vs. Faces Objects 2 2
Knaus et al.,2008 Reading: select a word that best match a three‐word phrase description; Letter judgment: Indicate whether letter strings were in upper or lower case Sentences or letter strings Autistics have better and faster responses than controls. Reading and responding vs. Letter judgment Words 7 8
Koshino et al., 2007 Face recognition (0‐back, 1‐back, 2‐back): Identify a remembered target face Grayscale pictures of faces No between‐group differences in RT or ACC Face recognition vs. Fixation Faces 15 9
Lee et al.,2007 Embedded Figure Task: Select one of two probe figure that contained the target shape; Matching Task: Select one of two probe figure that is identical to the target shape Pairs of complex figures and target shapes No between‐group differences in RT or ACC Embedded Figure Task vs. Matching Task Objects 11 3
Manjaly et al.,2007 Embedded Figure Task: Decide if a target figure matched a subpart of a complex figure; Matching Task: Indicate if a highlighted part of a complex figure matched a target shape Complex and target figure No between‐group differences in RT or ACC Embedded Figure Task vs. Matching task Objects 2 4
Mason et al.,2008 Read three‐sentence stories and respond to a simple yes/no comprehension question based on a physical (direct consequence), intentional (character's goal) or emotional (character's emotion) inference Sentence and question N/A Intentional inference vs. Fixation Words 12 20
Emotional inference vs. Fixation Words 15 17
Physical inference vs. Fixation Words 17 26 26
Schmitz et al.,2006 Motor response inhibited or executed depending on GO /no Go signal Arrow pointing left or right No between‐group differences in RT or ACC Go vs. No Go Objects 11 6
Stoop: Press a button if an arrow indicating left (or right) is displayed on left (or right) Arrow on left or right side No between‐group differences in RT or ACC Correct stroop inhibition vs. Congruent Objects 5 9
Shift attention and switch response to new association patterns Red dot and four squares No between‐group differences in RT or ACC Switch vs. Repeat set trials Objects 9 9
Schmitz et al.,2008 Press a button to two target letters, one of which was linked to monetary reward Letter No between‐group differences in RT or ACC Successful reward vs. Successful unrewarded Objects 5 4
Silani et al.,2008 Rate (visual analogue scale) the emotional value or the ratio of black/white pixels in pleasant, unpleasant or neutral pictures Affective pictures No between‐group differences in RT or ACC Emotion vs. Color rating * 24 8
Unpleasant vs. Neutral * 3 13
Solomon et al.,2009 Preparing to overcome prepotency (POP) task (response inhibition): Press key on same or different side as target Squares and arrows No differences in RT, but autistics made more errors on trials requiring response inhibition Inhibition vs. No Inhibition Objects 16 4
Soulières et al.,2009 Pattern matching: Select one of 8 response that best match a pattern; Raven's standard progressive matrices (RSPM): Select one of 8 response to matrices from which the final entry is missing Target pattern or RSPM plus 8 response choices No between‐group differences in RT or ACC Pattern matching vs. Fixation Objects 33 23
Raven vs. Fixation Objects 30 18
Uddin et al., 2008 Press a button if the face presented looks like self and another button if it looks like another or scrambled face Pictures of participant and another person No between‐group differences in RT or ACC Own face vs. Fixation Faces 12 18
Other face vs. Fixation Faces 12 6

Five studies included information about eye movement characteristics, reporting the number or duration of fixations or saccades or the eye movement related fluctuations in the orbital BOLD‐contrast signal. None of these studies found any significant between‐group differences in eye movement measures acquired either during the scanning sessions [Greimel et al.,2009; Soulières et al.,2009] or in separate experimental sessions [Bird et al.,2006; Dapretto et al.,2006; Kleinhans et al.,2008b].

Combined Face, Object, and Word Processing

Within‐group maps

We first analyzed the task‐related activity across all processing domains within each group. ALE maxima values for the autistic and non‐autistic groups are presented in Table III. Figures 1 and S1 show a broadly overlapping pattern of activity in the two groups, with large clusters in bilateral striate and extrastriate areas (BA 17, 18, 19); fusiform gyrus (BA 37); precuneus (BA 7); inferior (BA 44, 45, 47), middle (BA 46), and superior (BA 8, 9) frontal gyri; precentral (BA 6) gyrus; and the insula (BA 13).

Table III.

ALE maxima of regions showing within‐group effects for combined «FACES, OBJECTS and WORDS» processing domains (p FDR< 0.05, k = 250vx)

Left Right
Region BA x y z ALE (x10−3) x y z ALE (x10−3)
Non‐autistic
Occipital
Inferior occipital gyrus 18 −18 −95 −4 39.10 14 −95 −7 30.83
Fusiform gyrus 19 −31 −87 −9 36.88 42 −74 −9 33.00
19 −42 −81 −12 34.62
Lingual gyrus 17 −18 −94 7 22.00
18 −18 −78 −10 27.02
Temporal
Fusiform gyrus 37 −42 −54 −19 53.51 41 −59 −13 28.54
42 −47 −21 40.38
Middle temporal gyrus 21 −53 −35 −7 30.23 59 −37 −2 29.67
21 −55 −32 2 21.62
21 −61 −47 −4 20.19
Parietal
Precuneus 7 −1 −59 35 30.21 32 −65 42 32.15
Superior parietal lobule 7 26 −62 50 26.13
Angular gyrus 39 −30 −58 44 26.23
Frontal
Precentral gyrus 6 −46 1 34 37.45 47 7 28 63.17
Middle frontal gyrus 46 42 33 12 40.01
9 −46 15 30 31.66 42 26 21 24.04
9 −1 57 17 29.53
9 −46 23 25 20.22
Superior frontal gyrus 6 0 15 52 39.52
6 −5 9 57 38.34
8 −11 55 37 29.90
8 −3 29 42 28.23
9 −25 51 27 27.14
Inferior frontal gyrus 47 −47 27 −4 35.11
45 −53 22 10 27.29
44 −55 14 −1 25.92 53 9 9 21.71
Insula 13 −31 23 0 24.50 31 26 −1 52.71
13 44 14 9 24.94
Subcortical
Cingulate gyrus 31 −1 −47 31 43.46
Parahippocampal gyrus 37 −27 −46 −11 27.81 29 −46 −12 37.16
27 −25 −31 −8 29.53
Thalamus 27 −26 −3 34.68
Caudate 21 −24 18 34.23
Putamen −23 0 3 25.61
Autistic
Occipital
Fusiform gyrus 19 −48 −72 −5 24.78 38 −74 −9 46.87
19 −40 −66 −18 48.79 29 −83 −15 23.81
19 −20 −81 −10 24.48
Middle occipital gyrus 18 −31 −85 −7 42.92 34 −87 10 27.23
18 −22 −93 18 26.71 23 −98 −9 25.31
19 32 −80 23 21.51
Cuneus 17 −12 −99 3 32.31
Lingual gyrus 19 23 −71 −2 28.53
18 8 −77 3 22.37
Temporal
Fusiform gyrus 37 −44 −51 −17 34.13 31 −47 −16 51.51
37 −33 −63 −7 29.81 46 −49 −16 36.56
37 −33 −48 −22 26.59
Superior temporal gyrus 39 60 −60 25 27.09
22 −50 −55 20 23.97
Parietal
Precuneus 7 −26 −67 41 39.29 4 −55 37 27.34
7 30 −65 42 27.23
Superior parietal lobule 7 −22 −64 51 15.04
Frontal
Inferior frontal gyrus 47 −49 22 −10 44.14
Middle frontal gyrus 9 −48 19 26 30.82 56 23 35 32.84
9 47 6 39 16.45
Superior frontal gyrus 6 −7 10 60 39.76
6 −3 15 43 19.38
9 3 59 18 23.42
Precentral gyrus 6 51 7 27 35.58
6 40 11 32 26.44
Insula 13 −31 24 5 36.79 36 24 1 45.55
Subcortical
Putamen −25 6 4 32.22
Globus pallidus −25 −10 −10 29.41
Cingulate gyrus 31 2 −51 32 27.35
24 3 43 0 26.35
Parahippocampal gyrus 28 −25 −27 −8 25.76
19 27 −54 −6 23.02
37 −27 −46 −11 21.69
Thalamus −12 −19 9 24.20 27 −30 0 25.57
25 −24 −5 22.32
Figure 1.

Figure 1

Within‐ and between‐group distribution of task‐related activity in inferior occipital and inferotemporal cortex. A: Regions showing increases in autistics (RED), non‐autistics (GREEN), and their overlap (YELLOW) for «FACES, OBJECTS and WORDS» tasks combined. B: Regions showing more task‐related activity in autistics vs. non‐autistics (RED‐YELLOW) and less task‐related activity in autistics vs. non‐autistics (BLUE‐GREEN) for the combined «FACES, OBJECTS, and WORDS» tasks. C: Regions showing increases in autistics (RED), non‐autistics (GREEN), and their overlap (YELLOW) for the «FACES» tasks. D: Regions showing more task‐related activity in autistics vs. non‐autistics (RED‐YELLOW) and less task‐related activity in autistics vs. non‐autistics (BLUE‐GREEN) for the «FACES» tasks. ALE maps (p FDR < 0.05) are superimposed on axial slices from a gray matter template in MNI space. Anatomical left is image left.

Between‐group maps

Direct comparisons between autistic and non‐autistic group maps revealed differing ALE values in occipito‐temporal and frontal regions (Table IV; Fig. 1 and S1). Overall higher ALE values in striate (BA 17) and extrastriate (BA 18, 19) cortex were found in autistics. Small bilateral clusters in posterior extrastriate cortex (BA 18) exhibit lower ALE values in autistics. While both groups showed strong activity in BA 37, lower ALE values were found in autistics bilaterally in the anterior fusiform gyrus and in the medial part of the left fusiform gyrus. Additionally, autistics had lower ALE values in left middle temporal gyrus (BA 21) and higher ALE values in the left precuneus and intraparietal sulcus (BA 7).

Table IV.

ALE maxima of regions showing between‐group differences for combined «FACES, OBJECTS and WORDS» processing domains (p FDR< 0.05, k = 250vx)

Left Right
Region BA x y z ALE (x10−3) x y z ALE (x10−3)
Non‐autistic > Autistic
Occipital
Fusiform gyrus 19 29 −66 −17 27.25
Inferior occipital gyrus 18 −18 −95 −4 31.23
Temporal
Fusiform gyrus 37 −40 −56 −19 34.50
37 −44 −63 −25 18.35
Middle temporal gyrus 21 −51 −35 −7 29.58
21 −55 −33 2 21.12
Frontal
Precentral gyrus 4 −44 1 34 25.73 47 7 30 38.91
Superior frontal gyrus 6 4 13 52 35.91
9 −25 51 27 27.14
8 −3 29 42 27.51
Inferior frontal gyrus 45 44 31 12 33.29
47 −44 29 −5 26.21
Insula 13 31 25 −3 37.00
Subcortical
Cerebellum 18 −74 −35 31.99
Autistic > Non‐autistic
Occipital
Fusiform gyrus 19 −29 −86 −2 25.97 38 −74 −9 35.29
38 −68 −12 31.87
Middle occipital gyrus 18 −22 −93 18 26.27 34 −85 10 22.62
19 32 −82 19 19.26
19 32 −80 23 19.14
Parietal
Precuneus 7 −28 −67 41 32.61
Temporal
Fusiform gyrus 37 −38 −67 −18 43.26 32 −49 −14 30.20
37 −33 −63 −5 27.91 36 −49 −14 37.16
37 −34 −48 −22 24.89
Frontal
Inferior frontal gyrus 47 −49 22 −10 37.33
Middle frontal gyrus 8 55 23 35 29.17

In the frontal cortex, lower ALE values were observed in autistics in bilateral precentral (BA 4, 6), superior frontal (BA 6, 8, 9) and inferior frontal (BA 45, 47) gyri. Higher ALE values in autistics were limited to small regions in the posterior part of the left inferior frontal gyrus (BA 47) and in right medial frontal gyrus (BA 8). Clusters of lower activity in the autistics were also observed in bilateral insula (BA 13) and in cingulate cortex (BA 24) (Fig. S1).

To better visualize the spatial pattern of the differential visual activity in both groups, we computed the number of voxels for which ALE values differed between autistics and non‐autistics in the left and right hemispheres for the frontal, parietal, occipital, temporal and subcortical regions (Table V). Combining counts across all tasks, 6368 voxels had higher ALE values, and 2016 voxels had lower ALE values in the temporal, occipital and parietal lobes of the autistics compared to the non‐autistics. In contrast, the frontal lobes of the autistics exhibited a reversed pattern, with higher ALE values in 1360 voxels and lower ALE values in 4808 voxels (see Fig. 2). The associated analysis of variance revealed a significant effect of Region, F (4, 10) = 6.4, p = 0.008 and a Region x Group interaction F (4, 10) = 6.2, p = 0.009. These patterns reveal an atypical spatial distribution of visual processing in autism, seen as a posterior to anterior gradient of group activity differences, with the autistics exhibiting generally higher ALE values in posterior regions and lower ALE values in frontal regions.

Table V.

Autistics show a rightward shift of temporal and parietal lobe visual activity when compared with non‐autistics

Autistics > Non‐autistics Non‐autistics > Autistics
Left Right Left Right
Temporal lobe 1,384 2,960 1,216 200
Occipital lobe 968 904 384 96
Parietal lobe 96 296 552 80
Frontal lobe 872 488 2,104 2,704
Subcortical 448 392 264 617

The differential between‐group voxel counts for the left and right hemisphere lobes are shown for the combined «FACES, OBJECTS, and WORDS» domains (p FDR< 0.05).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

In both hemispheres, autistics exhibit more activity in temporal and occipital cortex. Between‐group differences in task‐related effects related to the combined «FACES, OBJECTS and WORDS» processing domains are shown with individual bars representing the number of suprathreshold voxels for autistics vs. non‐autistics (BLACK) and non‐autistics vs. autistics (WHITE) (p FDR < 0.05). Voxel counts are presented separately for the left and right temporal, occipital, parietal and frontal lobes.

Face Processing

Within‐group maps

We then restricted the analysis to the face processing domain (Table VI). Figures 1 and S2 show partially overlapping clusters of group activity. While both groups had high ALE values bilaterally along the fusiform gyrus (BA 19, 37), the largest overlap was observed in the anterior and middle fusiform gyrus, involving more posterior and lateral regions on the left than on the right. Additionally, both groups had high ALE values in right superior temporal gyrus (BA 22) and medial parietal cortex (BA 7). Moreover, both groups displayed activity in the posterior cingulate, the globus pallidus and at the temporo‐occipital junction (BA 21, 39). Significant ALE values in frontal cortex were more numerous in non‐autistics (BA 4, 6, 9, 10, 44, 45, 46) and overlap between the groups was limited to ALE values in precentral gyrus (BA 6) and insula (BA 13).

Table VI.

ALE maxima of regions showing within‐group effects for the «FACES» processing domain (p FDR< 0.05, k = 250vx)

Region Left Right
BA x y z ALE (x10−3) x y z ALE (x10−3)
Non‐autistic
Occipital
Fusiform gyrus 19 −42 −81 −12 26.88 42 −74 −9 32.95
19 −29 −86 −16 22.67 29 −64 −17 22.24
19 31 −59 −15 20.76
Middle occipital gyrus 18 −31 −84 4 16.47
18 −33 −87 −7 14.62
Lingual gyrus 18 −18 −79 −10 22.79
18 −12 −76 −1 13.52
Temporal
Fusiform gyrus 37 −42 −56 −19 38.18 42 −47 −21 30.45
42 −59 −13 28.37
Superior temporal gyrus 22 −61 −45 30 22.40 53 −45 19 17.40
Middle temporal gyrus 21 −62 −42 −4 19.84 59 −39 −2 20.53
57 −44 8 14.18
Parietal
Precuneus 19 34 −65 42 18.74
Frontal
Middle frontal gyrus 46 42 33 12 35.95
10 40 48 10 22.27
Precentral gyrus 6 −44 −2 34 13.81 47 5 28 33.36
4 −44 −7 42 17.49
Inferior frontal gyrus 44 −55 14 −1 25.53 53 9 9 21.70
45 55 30 −2 23.20
Insula 13 44 14 9 24.89
13 33 28 5 24.36
Subcortical
Cingulate gyrus 31 −1 −47 31 31.16
Globus Pallidus −18 −10 −10 24.02
Autistic
Occipital
Fusiform gyrus 19 −31 −85 −9 25.30 40 −74 −9 32.96
19 −36 −61 −9 25.12 29 −83 −15 23.02
19 −38 −62 −16 23.30
19 −42 −79 −14 19.35
Lingual gyrus 18 −25 −74 −6 14.38 23 −71 −2 28.33
17 8 −77 3 18.32
Middle occipital gyrus 19 −51 −76 −3 19.38
Temporal
Fusiform gyrus 37 −44 −51 −17 29.61 36 −49 −14 47.93
−34 −48 −22 24.63
Superior temporal gyrus 39 62 −60 23 21.50
22 −48 −54 20 20.01 59 −39 −2 21.21
Middle temporal gyrus 21 −36 0 −42 21.04
38 35 2 −26 22.85
Parietal
Precuneus 19 −28 −67 43 22.15
Frontal
Precentral gyrus 6 42 10 34 17.78
Insula 13 36 24 1 20.12
Subcortical
Cerebellum −5 −67 2 15.25 3 −67 −3 13.23
Cingulate gyrus 31 2 −51 32 22.74 12 −51 32 12.66
31 −9 −49 30 12.77
Globus Pallidus −25 −10 −10 16.25

Between‐group maps

Between‐group comparisons of face processing revealed areas of differential activity in occipital, temporal and frontal cortex (Table VII; Figs. 1, 3, and S2). First, higher ALE values in autistics were found in the fusiform gyrus (BA 37) bilaterally, while regions immediately posterior showed lower ALE values. Autistics also had higher ALE values in the middle portion of the left fusiform gyrus, the right lingual gyrus (BA 18, 19) and primary visual cortex (BA 17), with below threshold clusters at −20, −95, +3; vx = 48 and −14, −99, +1; vx = 32. Maxima were also seen in left middle temporal gyrus (BA 21), with greater ALE values for autistics in the extreme anterior portion and lower values in autistics in the posterior part of the gyrus. The autistics had lower ALE values in left superior temporal gyrus (BA 39), while the corresponding region on the right had higher ALE values. The between‐group differences in frontal cortex all involved lower ALE values in the autism group. For instance, differences were observed in right dorsolateral cortex (BA 9, 46), right anterior prefrontal cortex (BA 10), bilateral inferior frontal cortex (BA 44), bilateral premotor cortex (BA 6) and left primary motor cortex (BA 4). The autistics also exhibited lower ALE values in right anterior insula (BA 13).

Table VII.

ALE maxima of regions showing between‐group differences for the «FACES» processing domain (p FDR < 0.05, k = 250vx)

Region Left Right
BA x y z ALE (x10−3) x y z ALE (x10−3)
Non‐autistic > Autistic
Occipital
Fusiform gyrus 19 38 −69 −3 13.75
19 34 −69 −5 13.50
19 29 −64 −17 21.13
Temporal
Fusiform gyrus 37 −40 −54 −19 25.06
37 −42 −63 −25 19.96
37 −42 −55 −7 13.25
Superior temporal gyrus 39 −61 −45 30 22.40
Middle temporal gyrus 21 −62 −42 −4 19.85
Frontal
Middle frontal gyrus 46 42 33 9 29.05
10 40 48 10 22.22
9 51 13 29 13.10
Inferior frontal gyrus 44 −55 14 −1 25.50 53 9 9 21.63
Precentral gyrus 6 −44 −2 34 13.80 47 5 28 24.78
4 −44 −7 42 17.45
Insula 13 44 14 9 24.70
13 31 28 5 20.41
Autistic > Non‐autistic
Occipital
Lingual gyrus 19 23 −71 −2 27.89
Fusiform gyrus 19 −33 −61 −7 22.15
−36 −66 −15 13.21
Temporal
Fusiform gyrus 37 −34 −47 −23 23.30 33 −46 −14 41.65
Middle temporal gyrus 21 −36 0 −42 21.04
Superior temporal gyrus 39 62 −60 22 21.41
Figure 3.

Figure 3

Spatial distribution of regions showing more task‐related activity in autistics than non‐autistics for the three processing domains: «FACES» in RED, «OBJECTS» in GREEN, and «WORDS» in BLUE. ALE maps (p FDR < 0.05) are superimposed on slices from a gray matter template in MNI space. LEFT, a right hemisphere sagittal slice at x = +35; RIGHT, an axial slice at z = −18.

Voxel count in the fusiform gyrus for faces

To visualize the differential activity related to visual processing, we computed the number of voxels in the fusiform gyrus for which ALE values differed between autistics and non‐autistics for the face, object and word processing domains in both hemispheres (Table VIII). The associated analysis of variance revealed higher ALE values for the autistics F (1,6) = 9.12, p = 0.023, such that more voxels had ALE values that were greater in the autistic vs. non‐autistic groups and a trend for the largest between group differences to be associated with face processing tasks, F (2, 6) = 4.64, P = 0.060 (see Fig. 4).

Table VIII.

Autistics exhibited relatively stronger engagement of the fusiform gyrus for face processing compared with objects and words

Domain Autistics > Non‐autistics Non‐autistics > autistics
LEFT FG RIGHT FG LEFT FG RIGHT FG
Faces 1440 2688 592 168
Objects 1136 1616 1232 952
Words 624 520 496 8

The differential between‐group voxel counts for the «FACES», «OBJECTS», and «WORDS» processing domains are shown for the left and right hemispheres (p FDR< 0.05, k = 250vx).

Figure 4.

Figure 4

In the fusiform gyrus, more suprathreshold voxels are found for the autistic vs. non‐autistic than the non‐autistic vs. autistic contrasts. Between‐group differences in effects related to the «FACES», «OBJECTS», and «WORDS» processing domains are shown with bars representing the number of suprathreshold voxels for autistics vs. non‐autistics (BLACK) and non‐autistics vs. autistics (WHITE) (p FDR < 0.05). The voxel counts are presented separately for the left and right hemispheres.

Object Processing

Within‐group maps

The within‐group analysis for contrasts involving object processing revealed a roughly overlapping pattern of activity in both groups (Table IX; Figs. 5 and S3), including bilateral clusters in the anterior fusiform gyrus (BA 37) and posterior extrastriate cortex (BA 18, 19). In contrast, most of the activity in the occipital gyri did not exhibit overlap between groups. In the parietal cortex, overlapping ALE values were seen in medial parietal cortex (BA 7), while activity was observed in slightly different portions of the inferior parietal lobule (BA 40) in each group. Overlapping activity was also seen bilaterally in anterior insula (BA 13), and precentral and middle frontal (BA 6) gyri.

Table IX.

ALE maxima of regions showing within‐group effects for the «OBJECTS» processing domain (p FDR < 0.05, k = 250vx)

Region Left Right
BA x y z ALE (x10−3) x y z ALE (x10−3)
Non‐autistic
Occipital
Inferior occipital gyrus 18 −16 −93 −2 27.07
Cuneus 17 −18 −94 9 21.79
Middle occipital gyrus 18 −29 −92 3 13.08
Fusiform gyrus 19 −27 −68 −13 20.46
19 −29 −87 −9 12.49
Parietal
Precuneus 7 −18 −66 52 24.86 28 −63 39 26.33
7 −24 −61 44 15.97
7 −13 −72 57 13.05
Precuneus 31 −26 −75 30 22.48
Superior parietal lobule 7 26 −62 50 26.09
Inferior parietal lobule 40 −35 −41 47 21.36
40 −50 −48 44 14.12
40 −44 −54 46 13.67
Frontal
Precentral gyrus 6 47 7 30 30.85
60 9 23 14.31
Superior frontal gyrus 6 2 13 52 30.01 4 19 45 13.63
6 −24 −2 54 26.45 28 0 53 21.70
Middle frontal gyrus 9 42 23 24 19.53
Insula 13 −31 25 0 20.93 31 25 −3 36.33
Subcortical
Parahippocampal gyrus 36 −27 −46 −11 26.33 29 −44 −12 35.78
Putamen −25 −1 −2 17.34
Brainstem 3 −36 −41 23.45
Autistic
Occipital
Fusiform gyrus 19 −36 −81 −9 19.43 36 −76 −9 21.79
Middle occipital gyrus 19 32 −84 17 18.92
19 38 −81 9 13.38
18 −22 −93 16 24.11 32 −87 10 16.55
18 −27 −88 −2 15.57 34 −88 1 15.91
18 −27 −90 2 15.55
18 −31 −87 11 13.23
Inferior occipital gyrus 18 38 −84 −4 15.21
18 24 −93 −3 14.87
Cuneus 17 25 −97 0 14.31
Lingual gyrus 17 −11 −97 1 20.34
Parietal
Superior parietal lobule 7 −22 −64 51 14.52 32 −51 49 23.13
Inferior parietal lobule 40 −44 −26 −48 18.64 36 −39 43 19.40
40 36 −33 56 13.64
40 56 −26 46 14.65
Precuneus 7 −24 −63 42 15.51 23 −59 55 22.32
7 −15 −66 51 13.99
31 30 −75 25 12.65
Frontal
Precentral gyrus 6 −48 9 31 13.64 53 7 30 25.69
6 −46 3 34 14.25
6 −55 2 31 13.00
Middle frontal gyrus 6 −24 −2 54 19.13
Insula 13 −31 23 3 22.89 38 24 1 16.74
Subcortical
Cingulate gyrus 24 −3 −6 30 23.85
24 −3 5 31 13.34
32 8 25 40 15.49
Parahippocampal gyrus 36 −27 −46 −11 20.42
Thalamus −14 −19 9 19.83
Figure 5.

Figure 5

Within‐ and between‐group distribution of task‐related activity in inferior occipital and inferotemporal cortex. A: Regions showing increases in autistics (RED), non‐autistics (GREEN), and their overlap (YELLOW) for the «OBJECTS» tasks. B: Regions showing more task‐related activity in autistics vs. non‐autistics (RED‐YELLOW) and less task‐related activity in autistics vs. non‐autistics (BLUE‐GREEN) for the «OBJECTS» tasks. C: Regions showing increases in autistics (RED), non‐autistics (GREEN), and their spatial overlap (YELLOW) for the «WORDS» tasks. D: Regions showing more task‐related activity in autistics vs. non‐autistics (RED‐YELLOW) and less task‐related activity in autistics vs. non‐autistics (BLUE‐GREEN) for the «WORDS» tasks. ALE maps (p FDR < 0.05) are superimposed on axial slices from a gray matter template in MNI space. Anatomical left is image left.

Between‐group maps

Widespread between‐group differences in visual object processing were seen in occipital, temporal, parietal and frontal cortex (Table X; Figs. 5, 3, and S3). In occipital regions, the autistic group had greater ALE values bilaterally in the posterior fusiform gyrus (BA 19) and the middle occipital gyrus (BA 19). Conversely, autistics had lower ALE values in left lingual gyrus (BA 18) and the right anterior fusiform gyrus (BA 37). Additionally, autistics had lower values in the left mid‐fusiform gyrus (BA 19; +29, −68, −14), in an area anterior and medial to the area in which autistics had higher values. Both groups had ALE value maxima in the medial (precuneus) and lateral parietal cortex. Higher ALE values in autistics were more medial than those of non‐autistics in right inferior parietal cortex (BA 40) and anterior to those of controls in right lateral and medial superior parietal cortex (BA 7). As for frontal cortex, lower ALE values were observed in superior frontal gyrus (BA 6) in the autistics. Additionally, the autistic group exhibited lower ALE values in the right anterior insular cortex (BA 13) and higher ALE values in the cingulate gyrus (BA 24).

Table X.

ALE maxima of regions showing between‐group differences for the «OBJECTS» processing domain (p FDR < 0.05, k = 250vx)

Region Left Right
BA x y z ALE (x10−3) x y z ALE (x10−3)
Non‐autistic > Autistic
Occipital
Lingual gyrus 18 −18 −93 −4 25.07
18 −18 −94 7 17.91
Temporal
Fusiform gyrus 37 29 −44 −10 24.82
Parietal
Precuneus 7 28 −61 39 26.12
Superior parietal lobule 7 28 −62 48 20.65
Inferior parietal lobule 40 −35 −41 47 20.89
40 −50 −48 −43 14.10
40 −43 −54 46 13.67
Frontal
Superior frontal gyrus 6 2 13 52 30.02
Insula 13 31 25 −3 33.33
Subcortical
Putamen −25 0 0 16.75
Autistic > Non‐autistic
Occipital
Middle occipital gyrus 19 −24 −91 16 22.78
Fusiform gyrus 19 −36 −79 −10 19.16 36 −76 −9 21.24
Parietal
Superior parietal lobule 7 32 −51 49 22.99
Inferior parietal lobule 40 34 −39 43 17.32
40 36 −32 47 14.72
Subcortical
Cingulate gyrus 24 −3 −6 30 23.39

Word Processing

Within‐group maps

ALE maps were computed for contrasts involving word processing (Table XI; Fig. 5 and S4). In both groups, activity was observed in striate (BA 17) and extrastriate cortex (BA 18), overlapping mostly in the right hemisphere, while left hemisphere activity was slightly more anterior in autistics. In parietal cortex, both groups showed overlapping activity in the medial parietal cortex (BA 7), while activity in left middle temporal gyrus (BA 21) was observed in a more posterior location in autistics. In frontal cortex, both groups had significant ALE values in inferior (left BA 45, 47), middle (BA 6, 46), and superior frontal (BA 6, 8, 9) gyri, with overlapping activity in the left inferior and superior frontal gyri. We observed group overlap in subcortical activity in the thalami, right cingulate gyrus (BA 31), and left parahippocampal gyrus (BA 36).

Table XI.

ALE maxima of regions showing within‐group effects for the «WORDS» processing domain (p FDR< 0.05, k = 250vx)

Left Right
Region BA x y z ALE (x10−3) x y z ALE (x10−3)
Non‐autistic
Occipital
Inferior occipital gyrus 17 −18 −97 −4 20.85 14 −96 −7 30.74
Lingual gyrus 18 −10 −96 −11 19.55
Temporal
Middle temporal gyrus 21 −51 −35 −5 24.30
21 −55 −33 2 21.16
39 56 −66 26 17.94
Parietal
Precuneus 7 2 −59 37 18.08
Frontal
Superior frontal gyrus 8 −18 28 47 25.57
8 −12 55 37 21.17
8 −7 47 45 16.68
8 −13 33 53 13.59
9 −1 60 19 20.53
8 −1 34 44 18.88
6 −5 9 57 26.20
Inferior frontal gyrus 45 −49 24 12 20.48
47 −47 27 −6 23.50
47 −40 29 −5 19.21
47 −32 33 −12 13.18
Middle frontal gyrus 46 −46 21 23 13.18
6 −44 8 49 13.95
Precentral gyrus 4 −55 −3 46 24.87
6 −50 6 45 14.62
6 −44 0 50 13.59
Subcortical
Parahippocampal gyrus 27 −25 −31 −8 29.49
36 −21 −42 −9 13.71
Caudate 21 −24 18 33.27
Thalamus −18 −47 5 21.50 27 −27 7 31.97
Cingulate 29 −5 −51 10 17.58
30 −3 −62 8 13.10
Autistic
Occipital
Fusiform gyrus 19 −40 −69 −17 37.16 38 −68 −12 25.65
Inferior occipital gyrus 17 23 −98 −9 23.84
Lingual gyrus 18 −14 −87 −7 16.75
Temporal
Middle temporal gyrus 21 −57 −42 −2 17.79
21 −61 −47 8 13.87
Fusiform gyrus 37 46 −49 −14 23.50
Parietal
Precuneus 7 −5 −59 37 13.96 4 −57 37 20.54
7 −9 −54 41 13.62 30 −65 42 22.02
Frontal
Inferior frontal gyrus 47 −49 22 −10 42.79
45 −49 24 5 13.28
Superior frontal gyrus 6 −7 10 60 36.64
8 −9 50 38 20.25 12 45 49 17.32
9 1 58 26 18.25
Middle frontal gyrus 46 −44 21 21 26.65
9 56 23 35 20.67
8 28 28 44 19.59
Subcortical
Putamen −25 6 4 32.21
−25 −8 −8 13.34
Thalamus −5 −12 12 15.07 27 −30 0 25.37
−7 −16 13 14.99
Parahippocampal gyrus 35 −25 −27 −10 24.76 12 −51 5 13.20
Insula 13 −31 24 5 13.82
13 −40 23 3 13.81
13 −31 27 0 13.05
Amygdala −23 −10 −12 14.30
−29 −8 −14 13.41
Cingulate 30 4 −53 7 15.25

Between‐group maps

Between‐group ALE maps revealed differences in word processing activity (Table XII; Figs. 3, 5 and S4). First, ALE values differed between groups in occipitotemporal areas, with lower activity in bilateral striate cortex in autistics, just under the critical threshold on the right (+16, −95, −7), and higher activity in autistics in extrastriate cortex (BA 18; −14, −87, −5 and +25, −98, −9). Autistics also had higher activity in both the right fusiform gyrus (BA 19, 37) and, more ventrally, in the left ventral fusiform gyrus (BA 19). In parietal cortex, the autistics had higher ALE values in bilateral medial parietal cortex (BA 7), although the values were subthreshold on the left (−28, −68, +38; vx = 144). Between‐group differences were also seen in the middle temporal gyrus, with higher ALE values found posteriorly in autistics and anteriorly in non‐autistics on the left.

Table XII.

ALE maxima of regions showing between‐group differences for the «WORDS» processing domain (p FDR < 0.05, k = 250vx)

Region Left Right
BA x y z ALE (x10−3) x y z ALE (x10−3)
Non‐autistic > Autistic
Occipital
Inferior occipital gyrus 17 −18 −97 −4 20.28
Lingual gyrus 18 −10 −96 −11 19.32
Temporal
Middle temporal gyrus 21 −51 −36 −7 23.83
21 −55 −33 2 20.66
39 56 −66 26 17.94
Frontal
Precentral gyrus 4 −55 −3 46 24.85
Superior frontal gyrus 8 −18 26 47 22.24
8 −1 34 44 18.88
Inferior frontal gyrus 47 −40 31 −5 18.21
Subcortical
Caudate 21 −24 18 24.97
Thalamus −27 −33 −5 19.14
Parahippocampal gyrus 36 −21 −42 −9 13.67
36 −18 −36 −14 13.61
Autistic > Non−autistic
Occipital
Fusiform 19 −40 −69 −17 36.82
Temporal
Middle temporal gyrus 21 −59 −44 −2 17.03
Fusiform gyrus 37 46 −49 −14 23.50
Parietal
Precuneus 7 30 −65 42 22.02
Frontal
Inferior frontal gyrus 44 −49 22 −10 36.82
Superior frontal gyrus 6 −7 81 55 23.15
Middle frontal gyrus 46 −42 29 18 13.09
8 56 23 35 20.66
9 −51 17 26 19.91
9 −42 21 21 17.23
Subcortical
Putamen −25 6 4 30.20

A more complex pattern of effects was observed in frontal and subcortical regions. For example, while autistics generally had more areas exhibiting lower ALE values in frontal cortex compared to non‐autistics, the lower ALE values were seen primarily in left inferior, superior frontal, and precentral gyri (BA 4, 8, 47) and higher ALE values were found bilaterally in left posterior inferior frontal gyrus (BA 47), left superior frontal gyrus (BA 6), as well as left and right middle frontal gyri (BA 8, 9, 46). At the subcortical level, the right caudate nucleus, and bilateral thalami (sub‐threshold cluster on the right; +29, −26, −2; vx = 120) exhibited lower ALE values in autistics, while the left putamen had higher ALE values in autistics.

DISCUSSION

Summary of Findings

On the basis of the behavioral, cognitive and physiological phenomena previously summarized in the enhanced perception function model, we predicted that autistics would exhibit stronger engagement of the visual system across a range of tasks. In addition, we were interested in whether any observed atypical visual activity patterns in autism were specific to particular processing domains. We compared the magnitude and spatial distribution of brain activity associated with visual processing in autistics and non‐autistics using ALE meta‐analysis, including data drawn from 26 neuroimaging studies using visual stimuli. The analysis provided information about between‐group differences with respect to location and amplitude of task‐related activity. Combining all visual tasks, we observed widespread effects in both groups in regions spanning temporal, occipital, parietal, and frontal cortex. However, compared to non‐autistics, autistics displayed generally higher task‐related activity in posterior regions, and lower task‐related activity in frontal cortex. In addition, for each processing domain, we observed spatial overlap in activity in autistics and non‐autistics, accompanied by an atypical functional spatial distribution of domain‐specific responses in autism.

Domain‐Independent Similarities and Differences

As visual stimuli were used in all studies, large clusters of activity were found in both groups in the cortical areas involved in the first visual processing stages, namely striate (BA 17) and extrastriate (BA 18, 19) cortex. Both groups had responses in inferotemporal cortex, a region involved in recognition and identification of visually presented animate or inanimate objects [Op de Beeck et al.,2008]. Both groups also displayed posterior parietal cortex activity mainly in the medial parietal cortex (BA 7), an associative region involved in visuospatial information processing [Cavanna and Trimble2006]. In addition, both groups exhibited activity in the dorsal (BA 6, 8, 9, 46) and ventral (BA 44‐47) prefrontal cortex, regions involved in multiple aspects of sensorimotor and cognitive control [D'Esposito et al.,2000; Duncan and Owen2000; Petrides1996; Petrides2005]. The high ALE values seen in both groups across a broad network comprising temporal, occipital, parietal, and frontal regions were consistent with the wide range of visual processing tasks included in the study.

Between‐group comparisons using the combined face, object, and word processing tasks revealed an atypical pattern of resource allocation in autistics, with relatively higher activity in posterior visual processing regions and lower activity in frontal regions, as demonstrated by voxel count lobar distributions (see Fig. 2). In inferotemporal, occipital, and inferior parietal regions, more voxels showed higher ALE values in autistics than in non‐autistics in areas subserving integration of local visual features, manipulation of visual features, object recognition and object identification [Wandell et al.,2007]. Moreover, autistics displayed higher activity bilaterally in the precuneus (BA 7), a region subserving visual imagery [Suchan et al.,2002], visual search and detection [Brown et al.,2006; Hufner et al.,2008; Patel and Sathian,2000], and the maintenance of visual information in working memory [Owen,2004; Suchan et al.,2006; Yeh et al.,2007].

Conversely, ALE values in more anterior frontal regions (BA 4, 6, 8, 9, 45, and 47) were mostly lower in autistics. These areas include a range of regions with specialization for movement execution, movement planning, and cognitive control. The most posterior frontal region (BA 4) in the precentral gyrus is involved in fine motor control and sensorimotor transformations [He et al.,1993; Rizzolatti and Luppino2001]. The posterior part of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC; BA 6, 8) is responsible for response selection, attention shifting between alternative stimuli or responses in visuomotor tasks [Petrides,1994,2005]. The mid‐DLPFC (BA 9) is involved in planning and monitoring of behavior in accordance with internal goals [Petrides,1991,2000]. The adjacent mid‐ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (VLPFC; BA 45, 47) plays an important role in decision making [Petrides,2002], response comparison, selection and inhibition based on stored stimulus representations [Badre and Wagner,2007; Petrides,2005]. Finally, BA 6 and 9 are believed to be involved in cognitive control, mainly through the activation of task representations to adjust behavior to changing contexts [Brass et al.,2005].

Our principal finding resulting from the examination of results from the pooled face, object and word domains is that, in performing predominantly visual tasks, autistics exhibit a consistent pattern of stronger engagement of posterior cortical regions known to support visual processes of varying complexity. In addition, autistics exhibit lower activity in frontal regions subserving motor and cognitive control functions across a wide range of stimulus and task types.

Domain‐Specific Similarities and Differences

Although our results are largely consistent across the three visual processing categories, examining the domain‐specific patterns of differential activity informs the understanding of specific atypical functional resource allocation patterns in autism. The decision to classify the included tasks broadly by stimulus type rather than by specific cognitive operation was dictated by our difficulty in identifying sufficient numbers of studies utilizing tasks employing comparable cognitive operations. As the number of papers using functional neuroimaging to explore the neural mechanisms of perception and cognition in autism is expanding rapidly, it may soon be possible to attempt meta‐analysis of particular cognitive processes in autism.

Face processing

Much effort has been directed towards identifying the nature of face processing in autism. Our meta‐analysis of face processing tasks revealed strong, and partially overlapping, occipital and temporal activity in both groups. Face processing involves occipital and temporal cortical areas that show selectivity for face versus nonface stimuli in typical groups [Haxby et al.,2000; Kanwisher et al.,1997]. Consistent identification of preferential activity for face stimuli have been observed in the middle and lateral fusiform gyri, sometimes referred to as the Fusiform Face Area (FFA). This region generally shows stronger responses to faces compared to objects. Activity in the FFA correlates with successful face detection [Andrews and Schluppeck2004; Grill‐Spector et al.,2004]. A region in the lateral inferior occipital gyrus, referred to as the occipital face area (OFA), also shows selectivity for faces [Gauthier et al.,2000]. While the OFA is mostly sensitive to the individual physical features of faces, the FFA shows strong responses to both face parts and configurations [Liu et al.,2010; Rotshtein et al.,2005]. The third face‐selective region is found in the posterior superior temporal sulcus and is called fSTS, showing stronger responses to more complex aspects of face processing, such as eye‐gaze direction [Hoffman and Haxby2000] and emotional expression [Haxby et al.,2000].

With regards to face processing, spatial overlap in activity for autistics and non‐autistics was observed in the FFA [Kanwisher et al.,1997; Lehmann et al.,2004; Rhodes et al.,2009; Scherf et al.,2010]. Activity was also seen in the OFA [Rhodes et al.,2009; Rotshtein et al.,2005]. In addition, activity in fSTS was seen in both groups on the right, but only in non‐autistics on the left. Therefore, the results of our meta‐analysis do not support the notion that autism is characterized by an overall hypoactivation in face‐selective areas. We believe that the reported reduction of FFA activity in autism in response to face images [Dalton et al.,2005; Pierce et al.,2001; Schultz et al.,2000] could be dependent on specific task properties, rather than related to a generalized dysfunction of fusiform gyrus mechanisms or stemming from a lack of face expertise [Hadjikhani et al.,2004; Hadjikhani et al.,2007; Pierce et al.,2004].

However, our results do suggest that face stimuli are processed in an atypical fashion in autism, such that stronger, but less category‐specific, occipital and temporal activity may underlie face processing in this population. Specifically, compared with non‐autistics, autistics showed bilateral clusters of higher ALE values in the anterior fusiform gyrus, extending into the posterior part of the parahippocampal gyrus (see Fig. 3). In typical individuals, these areas are thought to be important for face recognition [Hudson and Grace,2000], perceptual expertise [Gauthier et al.,1999], and object processing [Grill‐Spector,2003]. Moreover, previous studies have identified functional response selectivity for places and spatial layout in the posterior parahippocampal cortex [Epstein and Kanwisher,1998]. In our results, autistics showed greater activity bilaterally in extrastriate (BA 18, 19) and striate (BA 17) cortex compared to non‐autistics. Therefore, face processing in autistics seems to rely on a large network of occipital and temporal areas specifically responsive to other visual categories in non‐autistics. Interestingly, the more anterior inferotemporal areas were more responsive to nonface objects in non‐autistics. A recent fMRI study looking at response specificity to faces, objects, and places in autism reported a similar atypical distribution of activity, in the form of bilateral displacement of the face‐specific response to the postero‐ventral fusiform gyrus in autistics, while non‐autistics showed greater object‐related responses in the same region [Scherf et al.,2010]. These findings are consistent with the results of our meta‐analysis, indicating a general pattern of atypical facial response selectivity in autism, with a corresponding atypical spatial distribution of place‐ and object‐specific responses.

The differential activity we observed in autistics could reflect an atypical processing strategy for facial stimuli. Langdell [1978] first reported superior performance in judging face identity based on the presentation of elementary facial features such as the eye or mouth in autistic children compared with non‐autistics. More recent studies confirmed that autistics rely to a greater extent on individual features to process faces [Deruelle et al.,2004; Lahaie et al.,2006; Pelphrey et al.,2002]. However, these atypical processing strategies are not necessarily detrimental to performance, as autistics and non‐autistics exhibited similar performance in 9 out of 14 contrasts included in the meta‐analysis.

We observed generally lower activity in prefrontal cortex in autistics during face processing, consistent with previous reports [Di Martino et al.,2009; Scherf et al.,2010]. It is known that frontal top‐down mechanisms may modulate extrastriate and inferotemporal activity during “deep” processing of faces, facilitating facial feature recognition [Haxby et al.,2000; Johnson et al.,2007; Li et al.,2009; Mechelli et al.,2004] and visual category determination [Jiang et al.,2007; Jiang et al.,2006]. Our findings suggest that, although frontal processes are consistently engaged for face processing in non‐autistics, the perceptual mechanisms in temporal, occipital, and parietal regions may be sufficient to allow for successful face processing in autistics. Although it is possible that the lack of task‐related frontal activity in autistics could result from localized dysfunction of the frontal cortex, suggested by some current models [e.g. Courchesne and Pierce2005], an alternative account is that utilization of frontal processing mechanisms may not be mandatory under some circumstances in autistics due to the existence of more efficient perceptual processing resources available in posterior cortical structures [Soulières et al.,2009]. Finally, the reduced engagement of frontal regions may reflect atypical connectivity between anterior and posterior regions, resulting in reduced functional coupling and regional interaction during visual processing. As our results are consistent with all of these hypothetical mechanisms, further studies are warranted to better delineate the physiological basis of the generalized frontal cortical hypoactivity commonly seen in autism.

Object processing

Autistics often exhibit unexpectedly strong and atypical abilities in visual tasks involving object detection or manipulation. For object processing, we observed activity in both groups in occipital (BA 17, 18, 19), temporal (BA 37), medial and lateral superior parietal (BA 7), inferior parietal (BA 40), and dorsal and ventral lateral prefrontal cortex (BA 6, 9 46, 47). Object processing is typically associated with activity in occipital and temporal cortex, with previous studies identifying responses in lateral occipital cortex to pictures of common objects [Malach et al.,1995], line drawings of objects [Kanwisher et al.,1996] and shapes [Hayworth and Biederman2006]. We observed activity in both groups that was located more medially than previously reported, possibly due to the heterogeneity of stimuli and tasks combined in the current analysis. While the lateral occipital region plays a specific role in object recognition [Grill‐Spector et al.,2001], object recognition as such was not a prominent component of all the tasks included in the meta‐analysis. Activity common to both groups was also observed in the anterior fusiform gyrus, another area involved in object processing [Grill‐Spector,2003] and spatial relations [Epstein and Kanwisher,1998]. Overall, both groups showed occipital and temporal activity in brain regions typically recruited by material‐independent visual information processing, such as integration of local visual features and manipulation of visual properties [Wandell et al.,2007].

Both groups also showed responses in prefrontal cortical regions, consistent with cognitive control requirements of the object processing tasks. For instance, lateral prefrontal cortex activity has been reported in relation to set shifting [Rogers et al.,2000], inhibitory control [Konishi et al.,1999], and category discrimination [Jiang et al.,2007; Jiang et al.,2006], processes common in object processing tasks [Dichter and Belger,2007; Schmitz et al.,2006; Schmitz et al.,2008; Solomon et al.,2009]. The observed prefrontal activity could also be related to planning and categorization [Petrides2005], processes critical to tasks such as the Embedded Figure Test [Lee et al.,2007; Manjaly et al.,2007], the Tower of London task [Just et al.,2007], spatial reasoning, and pattern matching [Soulières et al.,2009].

Both groups showed activity in superior parietal cortical areas involved in visuospatial attention [Corbetta et al.,1993; Nobre et al.,1997] and manipulation of information in working memory [Cabeza,2008; Cabeza et al.,2008]. Despite the variability of the tasks and stimuli combined within the object category, we observed a pattern concordant with the previous literature.

Regarding between‐group differences in activity related to object processing, autistics had higher ALE values in occipital (BA 19) and parietal (BA 7, 40) areas and lower values in the fusiform gyri (BA 37). The clusters of between‐group differential activity were smaller for the object than the face domains, which may be explained by greater task and stimulus variability for the object vs. face domain. Greater task variability within each domain might be expected to lead to a greater degree of spatial variability and consequently weaker constructive interference among the local maxima. As with face processing, autistics performed similarly to non‐autistics while displaying lower ALE values in the superior frontal gyrus (BA 6). Enhanced autistic performance has been reported in a broad range of visual perceptual tasks based on pattern detection, matching, and manipulation of objects, aspects encompassed here in the very general object processing domain. Therefore, we tentatively relate the atypical functional allocation of activity in visual perceptive regions in autism to enhanced performance in object processing.

Word processing

Some autistics acquire reading skills at an unexpectedly early age, a phenomenon known as hyperlexia. It is possible that these atypical reading skills result from differential organization in the visual areas responsible for processing letters or words. In our meta‐analysis results, group activity distributions related to word processing corresponded well to the known functional neuroanatomy of reading systems. A first level of word analysis in the occipito‐temporal junction supports word identification; a second level at the parieto‐temporal junction supports phonological processing; and a third level in the inferior frontal cortex supports semantics, phonology, and articulation [Shaywitz and Shaywitz,2008]. Both groups displayed bilateral posterior fusiform and lingual activity, presumably associated with word form analysis [Fiez and Petersen,1998; Price,2000]. Also consistent with this finding are the previous studies that have reported occipitotemporal and lateral occipital sensitivity to letter strings [Puce et al.,1996] and written words [Baker et al.,2007]. In addition, both groups displayed activity in regions typically associated with semantic processing [Howard et al.,1992; Martin and Chao,2001; Petersen et al.,1988; Poldrack et al.,1999], verbal fluency [Abrahams et al.,2003; Gaillard et al.,2000], and sentence comprehension [Just et al.,1996; Roder et al.,2002], including the left middle temporal gyrus, the left superior temporal gyrus, the left inferior frontal gyrus and multiple lateral prefrontal regions. The word processing tasks included semantic decision [Gaffrey et al.,2007; Harris et al.,2006], sentence judgment and comprehension [Just et al.,2004; Kennedy and Courchesne,2008; Mason et al.,2008], word counting [Kennedy et al.,2006], and verbal fluency [Kleinhans et al.,2008a], for which we observed the expected activity in a number of left hemisphere language regions.

We observed group differences for the word processing tasks, with higher task‐related ALE values in autistics in the fusiform gyrus (mostly on the right; BA 19, 37), medial parietal cortex (BA 7), middle posterior temporal gyrus (BA 21), left inferior frontal gyrus (BA 44), and bilateral lateral prefrontal cortex (BA 6, 8, 9, 46). Many of these areas are also part of the reading network seen in non‐autistics. However, predominantly left lateralization, expected based on previous studies of language in typical samples, was not seen here in autism, in line with reports of reduced leftward hemispheric response lateralization for speech processing in autism [Boddaert et al.,2003; Boddaert et al.,2004; Lepisto et al.,2005]. Higher activity for words in the fusiform gyrus and medial parietal cortex supports the hypothesis that autistics more strongly engage mental imagery and visualization to process written sentences [Just et al.,2004] and words [Gaffrey et al.,2007; Toichi and Kamio,2001]. In addition, we observed lower activity in the autistic group in many reading regions, including occipital (BA 17, 18), left parieto‐temporal (BA 21, 39) and left inferior frontal (BA 47) cortex. In summary, the regional functional allocation of word related activity is clearly atypical in autism, as we observed more right‐lateralized activity in autistics related to reading as well as stronger involvement of regions typically involved in broader aspects of perceptual expertise (BA 19, 37).

This atypical activity pattern could explain the emergence of hyperlexic abilities in some autistics. Hyperlexia is defined as reading skills exceeding those predicted by an individual's general intelligence or language comprehension capacities [Grigorenko et al.,2003]. Hyperlexia occurs in about 5–10% of autistic children [Burd et al.,1985]. While it has been suggested that enhanced visual pattern recognition may underlie hyperlexia [Cobrinik,1982], heightened phonological and orthographic abilities may also contribute to precocious reading skills [Goldberg and Rothermel,1984]. Although hyperlexic children could engage typical reading strategies to attain superior word recognition abilities, word recognition mechanisms could operate more autonomously from more abstract word comprehension mechanisms in this group [Newman et al.,2007]. The atypical pattern of occipital and temporal word processing activity seen in our meta‐analysis might underlie this autonomy, a phenomenon that we called functional independence in a different cognitive context [Soulières et al.,2009].

Alternative Interpretations of the Between‐Group Differences

Could atypical saccades cause the observed atypical occipital and parietal activity?

Differences in brain activity apparently associated with visual processing might trivially result from differences in eye movements used to explore the stimuli, rather than from differences in perceptual processing per se. We argue that this is not the case for the following reasons. First, all studies included in the meta‐analysis that reported eye movement data found no differences between the autistic and non‐autistic groups [Bird et al.,2006; Dapretto et al.,2006; Greimel et al.,2009; Kleinhans et al.,2008b; Soulières et al.,2009], in line with other studies reporting no differences in visual saccade or fixation properties between autistics and non‐autistics [Dalton et al.,2005; Kemner et al.,2004; Luna et al.,2007; Luna et al.,2002; Takarae et al.,2004; Takarae et al.,2007]. Second, the spatial pattern of activity across tasks for the between‐group differences reported here does not overlap with the network thought to control visual search and saccades. For instance, both groups exhibited activity in lateral prefrontal cortex in the frontal eye fields [FEF; Amiez and Petrides2009; Grosbras et al.,2005]. This area is consistently involved in controlling saccade and pursuit eye movements [Astafiev et al.,2003; Ettinger et al.,2008; Grosbras et al.,2005]. It is also active in tasks requiring changes in visuospatial attention, even in the absence of saccades [Armstrong et al.,2009]. Nevertheless, no significant between‐group differences were observed in this region. Similarly, regions previously reported as less active in autism in association with visually‐guided saccades [Takarae et al.,2007] do not correspond to the areas of lower activity reported here in autistics across all visual tasks. Lower ALE values in non‐autistics were observed in the dorsal part of the medial frontal gyrus, anterior to the supplementary eye fields [Grosbras et al.,1999]. However, this region is known to be less active in autism well beyond the context of saccade generation, specifically during executive and working memory tasks [Gilbert et al.,2008; Silk et al.,2006]. Therefore, the pattern of between‐group differences reported here is unlikely to be related to oculomotor effects.

Are the observed activity patterns explained by differences in task complexity?

Another interpretation of the differential engagement of cortical regions in autistics across a range of visual tasks could be that these differences are driven mainly by tasks incorporating more substantial perceptual complexity. However, the autistic pattern of relative posterior hyperactivity was consistently found for a range of tasks involving visual information ranging from simple to complex, and cognitive complexity ranging from low to high. For instance, our meta‐analysis included stimuli varying from simple shapes (i.e. letters in Keehn et al.,2008] to more complex visual patterns (i.e. facial stimuli in Hall et al.,2003; Raven's Progressive Matrices in Soulières et al.,2009]. Tasks of varying complexity were included as well, ranging from passive viewing of faces (e.g. Bird et al.,2006] and stimulus matching (e.g. Bookheimer et al.,2008; Lee et al.,2007] to sentence comprehension (e.g. Mason et al.,2008], mental state inference (e.g. Kana et al.,2009] and abstract reasoning [Soulières et al.,2009]. In sum, more strongly engaged perceptual processing regions engaged across a disparate collection of tasks indicates a greater role for perceptual processes in autism for tasks not necessarily incorporating complex perceptual or cognitive components.

Does differential between‐group performance explain the observed activity patterns?

It is possible that performance differences could be responsible for atypical neural activity patterns in autistics. However, autistics and non‐autistics exhibited similar performance levels in 18 of the 26 included studies, compared to two studies with enhanced and six studies with diminished performance in autistics. While enhanced autistic performance was seen in the form of faster responses for sentence comprehension tasks [Just et al.,2004; Knaus et al.,2008], diminished performance was mainly observed in the form of reduced accuracy. Even in studies where accuracy was significantly reduced, the autistics still performed fairly well. While one study reported 93% correct responses in autistics compared with 100% in non‐autistics [Bookheimer et al.,2008], another observed reduced but significantly greater than chance (81.9% and 73.8%) accuracy for a semantic decision task in autistics [Gaffrey et al.,2007]. Kleinhans et al. [2008a] reported that autistics generated fewer words than non‐autistics in a verbal fluency task, while no group differences in error number were seen. One study [Hubl et al.,2003] reported longer response times for autistics detecting the sex of real and scrambled faces, but the task instructions did not explicitly require participants to respond as quickly as possible. Other studies reported more errors when autistics were asked to judge emotional states from weak facial expressions [Greimel et al.,2009] or when they had to overcome an automatic response tendency [Solomon et al.,2009]. Given that atypical visual processing is observed in association with mostly typical performance levels in autism, we suggest that autistics make more use of perceptual processes than do non‐autistics in executing cognitive tasks involving complex operations.

Is Hemispheric Asymmetry for Visual Processing Atypical in Autism?

Face processing was associated with generally similar hemispheric effects in autistics and non‐autistics, with both groups showing bilateral activity in the FFA and the OFA. However, while activity increases were seen in posterior fSTS in both groups on the right, it was observed only in non‐autistics on the left. For face processing in autistics, some have hypothesized atypical regional allocation of activity, not necessarily reflecting reduced lateralization of the face‐specific activity compared to non‐autistics [Pierce et al.,2001]. In addition, recent studies have demonstrated displacement of the face‐specific response in autism to regions typically responsive to non‐face visual stimuli in non‐autistics in both hemispheres [Humphreys et al.,2008; Scherf et al.,2010]. One other meta‐analysis of functional neuroimaging studies looking at social vs. non social tasks did not report activity lateralization differences between autistics and non‐autistics [Di Martino et al.,2009]. For object processing, hemispheric activity was similarly distributed in both groups. The laterality of word processing is atypical in autism, as evidenced by more symmetric activity in autistics related to reading. Predominantly left lateralization, expected based on previous studies of language in typical samples, does not appear to characterize autism. With respect to language tasks in general, some studies have suggested that atypical hemispheric specialization might be related to the communication difficulties observed in autistics. Atypical leftward lateralization in autism has been most consistently observed at the structural level in frontal language areas [Herbert et al.,2005] and in temporal regions such as planum temporale, middle and inferior temporal gyri [Herbert et al.,2005; Rojas et al.,2002]. Some functional imaging studies have reported reduced left frontal activity associated with language tasks [Gaffrey et al.,2007; Just et al.,2004; Kana et al.,2006] and others have reported reduced leftward temporal response lateralization for auditory language tasks in autism [Boddaert et al.,2003; Boddaert et al.,2004; Lepisto et al.,2005]. It is possible that the lateralization effects related to language might be task‐dependent, as the hemispheric differences between autistics and non‐autistics were not the same for two language tasks examined in a study in which autistics showed reduced leftward asymmetry for one task (fluency) and typical lateralization for the other (categorization) [Kleinhans et al.,2008a].

In summary, while we observed a trend for decreased hemispheric asymmetry in autism for word processing, the left/right differences in associated ALE values were more subtle than the more consistent finding of higher ALE values across all three task domains in posterior compared to frontal cortical regions.

Are the Results Consistent With the Predictions of the EPF Model?

Our ALE meta‐analysis results both confirm and extend the original EPF Model, demonstrating that: (1) perceptual processing in autistic individuals plays an enhanced role across a wide range of visual tasks and (2) that the neural organization of perceptual processing is atypically organized, extending to areas involved in the development of perceptual expertise. The first major finding of this study consists of evidence for generally stronger engagement of visual processing regions in autism across a range of tasks, consistent with our previous non‐quantitative review of brain imaging results [Mottron et al.,2006]. In addition, the observed stronger engagement of visual areas emerges despite multiple sources of noise introduced by variations in matching strategies, participant age and general intelligence, and whether group assignment was defined using a specific diagnosis of autism versus the broader classification of autism spectrum condition. Our findings are consistent with the hypothesis that autistics rely more heavily on visual processing mechanisms regardless of the stimulus domain, particularly for language functions [Gaffrey et al.,2007; Just et al.,2004; Lambert et al., 2004]. Enhanced activity in brain regions related to visual processing may therefore represent a core atypicality in autistic neural organization.

However, while behavioral evidence for visuospatial strengths in autism is now strong, it is not possible to simply associate higher levels of neural activity with superior behavioral performance, a relationship that has been clearly demonstrated in only a limited number of studies. For instance, we recently reported increased extrastriate (BA 18) combined with reduced prefrontal (BA 9) and parietal (BA 7) activity during performance of a matrix reasoning task in a group of autistics who had been matched with a non‐autistic group on both accuracy and response time [Soulières et al.,2009]. In this study, an autistic behavioral advantage, as evidenced by faster performance, and enhanced occipital activity both increased as task complexity increased. The relative independence of the observed occipital findings with respect to task performance in the present study indicates that higher levels of neural activity may be associated with more efficient task performance in only some circumstances.

The second main finding of this meta‐analysis is that atypical regional functional resource allocation, involving both primary and associative cortical areas across a range of visual processing tasks, engages mechanisms responsible for the development of perceptual expertise in areas such as the fusiform gyrus. This important finding allows an extension of the original EPF Model that suggests that the overall process of perceptual expertise development, as well as the specific nature of related category‐specific responses, may be atypical in autism. Material‐independent variations in the acquisition of autistic perceptual expertise, their reciprocal interactions with low‐level perceptual processes, and their involvement in a broad range of both social and non‐social atypical behaviors characteristic of autism, may all represent promising fields for future investigation.

Lastly, considering that atypical spatial allocation of brain resources may be an indication of developmental functional plasticity, our results indicate that enhanced cortical plasticity may be beneficial to visual perception in autism, in the light of preliminary findings of greater cortical plasticity, including enhanced long‐term potentiation of synaptic strength in an animal model of autism [Rinaldi et al.,2008], and more lasting changes in cortical excitability following in vivo theta burst stimulation in a few autistics [Oberman et al.,2010].

Supporting information

Additional Supporting Information may be found in the online version of this article.

Supporting Information Figure 1.

Supporting Information Figure 2.

Supporting Information Figure 3.

Supporting Information Figure 4.

Acknowledgements

The authors thank Michelle Dawson for editing and commenting on the paper and Marouane Nassim for research assistance.

Footnotes

1

Throughout the report we respectfully use the term autistics, following Sinclair, J. (1999). Why I dislike “person first” language. http://www.jimsinclair.org/person_first.htm

REFERENCES

  1. Abrahams S, Goldstein LH, Simmons A, Brammer MJ, Williams SC, Giampietro VP, Andrew CM, Leigh PN ( 2003): Functional magnetic resonance imaging of verbal fluency and confrontation naming using compressed image acquisition to permit overt responses. Hum Brain Mapp 20: 29–40. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Amiez C, Petrides M ( 2009): Anatomical organization of the eye fields in the human and non‐human primate frontal cortex. Prog Neurobiol 89: 220–230. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Andrews TJ, Schluppeck D ( 2004): Neural responses to Mooney images reveal a modular representation of faces in human visual cortex. Neuroimage 21: 91–98. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Armstrong KM, Chang MH, Moore T ( 2009): Selection and maintenance of spatial information by frontal eye field neurons. J Neurosci 29: 15621–15629. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Astafiev SV, Shulman GL, Stanley CM, Snyder AZ, Van Essen DC, Corbetta M ( 2003): Functional organization of human intraparietal and frontal cortex for attending, looking, and pointing. J Neurosci 23: 4689–4699. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Badre D, Wagner AD ( 2007): Left ventrolateral prefrontal cortex and the cognitive control of memory. Neuropsychologia 45: 2883–2901. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Baker CI, Liu J, Wald LL, Kwong KK, Benner T, Kanwisher N ( 2007): Visual word processing and experiential origins of functional selectivity in human extrastriate cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 104: 9087–9092. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Baldassi S, Pei F, Megna N, Recupero G, Viespoli M, Igliozzi R, Tancredi R, Muratori F, Cioni G ( 2009): Search superiority in autism within, but not outside the crowding regime. Vision Res 49: 2151–2156. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Belmonte MK, Cook EH, Jr. , Anderson GM, Rubenstein JL, Greenough WT, Beckel‐Mitchener A, Courchesne E, Boulanger LM, Powell SB, Levitt PR, et al. ( 2004): Autism as a disorder of neural information processing: Directions for research and targets for therapy. Mol Psychiatry 9: 646–663. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Belmonte MK, Yurgelun‐Todd DA ( 2003): Functional anatomy of impaired selective attention and compensatory processing in autism. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 17: 651–664. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Bertone A, Mottron L, Jelenic P, Faubert J ( 2005): Enhanced and diminished visuo‐spatial information processing in autism depends on stimulus complexity. Brain 128( Part 10): 2430–2441. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Bird G, Catmur C, Silani G, Frith C, Frith U ( 2006): Attention does not modulate neural responses to social stimuli in autism spectrum disorders. Neuroimage 31: 1614–1624. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Boddaert N, Belin P, Chabane N, Poline JB, Barthelemy C, Mouren‐Simeoni MC, Brunelle F, Samson Y, Zilbovicius M ( 2003): 3erception of complex sounds: abnormal pattern of cortical activation in autism. Am J Psychiatry 160: 2057–2060. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Boddaert N, Chabane N, Belin P, Bourgeois M, Royer V, Barthelemy C, Mouren‐Simeoni MC, Philippe A, Brunelle F, Samson Y, et al. ( 2004): Perception of complex sounds in autism: Abnormal auditory cortical processing in children. Am J Psychiatry 161: 2117–2120. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Bookheimer SY, Wang AT, Scott A, Sigman M, Dapretto M ( 2008): Frontal contributions to face processing differences in autism: Evidence from fMRI of inverted face processing. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 14: 922–932. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Brass M, Derrfuss J, Forstmann B, von Cramon DY ( 2005): The role of the inferior frontal junction area in cognitive control. Trends Cogn Sci 9: 314–316. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Brown MR, Goltz HC, Vilis T, Ford KA, Everling S ( 2006): Inhibition and generation of saccades: Rapid event‐related fMRI of prosaccades, antisaccades, and nogo trials. Neuroimage 33: 644–659. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Burd L, Kerbeshian J, Fisher W ( 1985): Inquiry into the incidence of hyperlexia in a statewide population of children with pervasive developmental disorder. Psychol Rep 57: 236–238. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Cabeza R ( 2008): Role of parietal regions in episodic memory retrieval: The dual attentional processes hypothesis. Neuropsychologia 46: 1813–1827. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Cabeza R, Ciaramelli E, Olson IR, Moscovitch M ( 2008): The parietal cortex and episodic memory: An attentional account. Nat Rev Neurosci 9: 613–625. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Caron MJ, Mottron L, Berthiaume C, Dawson M ( 2006): Cognitive mechanisms, specificity and neural underpinnings of visuospatial peaks in autism. Brain 129( Part 7): 1789–1802. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Castelli F, Frith C, Happe F, Frith U ( 2002): Autism, Asperger syndrome and brain mechanisms for the attribution of mental states to animated shapes. Brain 125( Part 8): 1839–1849. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Cavanna AE, Trimble MR ( 2006): The precuneus: A review of its functional anatomy and behavioural correlates. Brain 129( Part 3): 564–583. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Cobrinik L ( 1982): The performance of hyperlexic children on an “incomplete words” task. Neuropsychologia 20: 569–577. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Corbetta M, Miezin FM, Shulman GL, Petersen SE ( 1993): 1 PET study of visuospatial attention. J Neurosci 13: 1202–1226. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Courchesne E, Pierce K ( 2005): Why the frontal cortex in autism might be talking only to itself: Local over‐connectivity but long‐distance disconnection. Curr Opin Neurobiol 15: 225–230. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. D'Esposito M, Postle BR, Rypma B ( 2000): Prefrontal cortical contributions to working memory: Evidence from event‐related fMRI studies. Exp Brain Res 133: 3–11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Dakin S, Frith U ( 2005): Vagaries of visual perception in autism. Neuron 48: 497–507. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Dalton KM, Nacewicz BM, Johnstone T, Schaefer HS, Gernsbacher MA, Goldsmith HH, Alexander AL, Davidson RJ ( 2005): Gaze fixation and the neural circuitry of face processing in autism. Nat Neurosci 8: 519–526. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Dapretto M, Davies MS, Pfeifer JH, Scott AA, Sigman M, Bookheimer SY, Iacoboni M ( 2006): Understanding emotions in others: Mirror neuron dysfunction in children with autism spectrum disorders. Nat Neurosci 9: 28–30. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Deruelle C, Rondan C, Gepner B, Tardif C ( 2004): Spatial frequency and face processing in children with autism and Asperger syndrome. J Autism Dev Disord 34: 199–210. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Di Martino A, Ross K, Uddin LQ, Sklar AB, Castellanos FX, Milham MP ( 2009): Functional brain correlates of social and nonsocial processes in autism spectrum disorders: An activation likelihood estimation meta‐analysis. Biol Psychiatry 65: 63–74. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Dichter GS, Belger A ( 2007): Social stimuli interfere with cognitive control in autism. Neuroimage 35: 1219–1230. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Duncan J, Owen AM ( 2000): Common regions of the human frontal lobe recruited by diverse cognitive demands. Trends Neurosci 23: 475–483. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Epstein R, Kanwisher N ( 1998): A cortical representation of the local visual environment. Nature 392: 598–601. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Ettinger U, Ffytche DH, Kumari V, Kathmann N, Reuter B, Zelaya F, Williams SC ( 2008): Decomposing the neural correlates of antisaccade eye movements using event‐related FMRI. Cereb Cortex 18: 1148–1159. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Fiez JA, Petersen SE ( 1998): Neuroimaging studies of word reading. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 95: 914–921. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Gaffrey MS, Kleinhans NM, Haist F, Akshoomoff N, Campbell A, Courchesne E, Muller RA ( 2007): Atypical [corrected] participation of visual cortex during word processing in autism: An fMRI study of semantic decision. Neuropsychologia 45: 1672–1684. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Gaillard WD, Hertz‐Pannier L, Mott SH, Barnett AS, LeBihan D, Theodore WH ( 2000): Functional anatomy of cognitive development: fMRI of verbal fluency in children and adults. Neurology 54: 180–185. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Gauthier I, Tarr MJ, Anderson AW, Skudlarski P, Gore JC ( 1999): 3ctivation of the middle fusiform ‘face area’ increases with expertise in recognizing novel objects. Nat Neurosci 2: 568–573. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Gauthier I, Tarr MJ, Moylan J, Skudlarski P, Gore JC, Anderson AW ( 2000): The fusiform “face area” is part of a network that processes faces at the individual level. J Cogn Neurosci 12: 495–504. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Gilbert SJ, Bird G, Brindley R, Frith CD, Burgess PW ( 2008): Atypical recruitment of medial prefrontal cortex in autism spectrum disorders: An fMRI study of two executive function tasks. Neuropsychologia 46: 2281–2291. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Goldberg TE, Rothermel RD,Jr . 1984. Hyperlexic children reading. Brain 107 ( Part 3): 759–785. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Greimel E, Schulte‐Ruther M, Kircher T, Kamp‐Becker I, Remschmidt H, Fink GR, Herpertz‐Dahlmann B, Konrad K ( 2009): Neural mechanisms of empathy in adolescents with autism spectrum disorder and their fathers. Neuroimage 49: 1055–1065. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Grigorenko EL, Klin A, Volkmar F ( 2003): Annotation: Hyperlexia: disability or superability? J Child Psychol Psychiatry 44: 1079–1091. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Grill‐Spector K ( 2003): The neural basis of object perception. Curr Opin Neurobiol 13: 159–166. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Grill‐Spector K, Knouf N, Kanwisher N ( 2004): The fusiform face area subserves face perception, not generic within‐category identification. Nat Neurosci 7: 555–562. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Grill‐Spector K, Kourtzi Z, Kanwisher N ( 2001): The lateral occipital complex and its role in object recognition. Vision Res 41( 10–11): 1409–1422. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Grosbras MH, Laird AR, Paus T ( 2005): Cortical regions involved in eye movements, shifts of attention, and gaze perception. Hum Brain Mapp 25: 140–154. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Grosbras MH, Lobel E, Van de Moortele PF, LeBihan D, Berthoz A ( 1999): An anatomical landmark for the supplementary eye fields in human revealed with functional magnetic resonance imaging. Cereb Cortex 9: 705–711. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Hadjikhani N, Joseph RM, Snyder J, Chabris CF, Clark J, Steele S, McGrath L, Vangel M, Aharon I, Feczko E, et al. ( 2004): Activation of the fusiform gyrus when individuals with autism spectrum disorder view faces. Neuroimage 22: 1141–1150. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Hadjikhani N, Joseph RM, Snyder J, Tager‐Flusberg H ( 2007): Abnormal activation of the social brain during face perception in autism. Hum Brain Mapp 28: 441–449. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Hall GB, Szechtman H, Nahmias C ( 2003): Enhanced salience and emotion recognition in Autism: A PET study. Am J Psychiatry 160: 1439–1441. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Harris GJ, Chabris CF, Clark J, Urban T, Aharon I, Steele S, McGrath L, Condouris K, Tager‐Flusberg H ( 2006): Brain activation during semantic processing in autism spectrum disorders via functional magnetic resonance imaging. Brain Cogn 61: 54–68. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Haxby JV, Hoffman EA, Gobbini MI ( 2000): The distributed human neural system for face perception. Trends Cogn Sci 4: 223–233. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Hayworth KJ, Biederman I ( 2006): Neural evidence for intermediate representations in object recognition. Vision Res 46: 4024–4031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Hazlett EA, Buchsbaum MS, Hsieh P, Haznedar MM, Platholi J, LiCalzi EM, Cartwright C, Hollander E ( 2004): Regional glucose metabolism within cortical Brodmann areas in healthy individuals and autistic patients. Neuropsychobiology 49: 115–125. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. He SQ, Dum RP, Strick PL ( 1993): Topographic organization of corticospinal projections from the frontal lobe: Motor areas on the lateral surface of the hemisphere. J Neurosci 13: 952–980. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Herbert MR, Ziegler DA, Deutsch CK, O'Brien LM, Kennedy DN, Filipek PA, Bakardjiev AI, Hodgson J, Takeoka M, Makris N, et al. ( 2005): Brain asymmetries in autism and developmental language disorder: A nested whole‐brain analysis. Brain 128( Part 1): 213–226. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Hoffman EA, Haxby JV ( 2000): Distinct representations of eye gaze and identity in the distributed human neural system for face perception. Nat Neurosci 3: 80–84. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Howard D, Patterson K, Wise R, Brown WD, Friston K, Weiller C, Frackowiak R ( 1992): The cortical localization of the lexicons. Positron emission tomography evidence. Brain 115 ( Part 6): 1769–1782. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Hubl D, Bolte S, Feineis‐Matthews S, Lanfermann H, Federspiel A, Strik W, Poustka F, Dierks T ( 2003): Functional imbalance of visual pathways indicates alternative face processing strategies in autism. Neurology 61: 1232–1237. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Hudson AJ, Grace GM ( 2000): Misidentification syndromes related to face specific area in the fusiform gyrus. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 69: 645–648. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Hufner K, Stephan T, Glasauer S, Kalla R, Riedel E, Deutschlander A, Dera T, Wiesmann M, Strupp M, Brandt T ( 2008): Differences in saccade‐evoked brain activation patterns with eyes open or eyes closed in complete darkness. Exp Brain Res 186: 419–430. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Humphreys K, Hasson U, Avidan G, Minshew N, Behrmann M ( 2008): Cortical patterns of category‐selective activation for faces, places and objects in adults with autism. Autism Res 1: 52–63. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Jemel B, Mimeault D, Saint‐Amour D, Hosein A, Mottron L ( 2010): 3EP contrast sensitivity responses reveal reduced functional segregation of mid and high filters of visual channels in Autism. J Vis 10: 13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Jiang X, Bradley E, Rini RA, Zeffiro T, Vanmeter J, Riesenhuber M ( 2007): Categorization training results in shape‐ and category‐selective human neural plasticity. Neuron 53: 891–903. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Jiang X, Rosen E, Zeffiro T, Vanmeter J, Blanz V, Riesenhuber M ( 2006): Evaluation of a shape‐based model of human face discrimination using FMRI and behavioral techniques. Neuron 50: 159–172. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Johnson MR, Mitchell KJ, Raye CL, D'Esposito M, Johnson MK ( 2007): A brief thought can modulate activity in extrastriate visual areas: Top‐down effects of refreshing just‐seen visual stimuli. Neuroimage 37: 290–299. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Jolliffe T, Baron‐Cohen S ( 1997): Are people with autism and Asperger syndrome faster than normal on the Embedded Figures Test? J Child Psychol Psychiatry 38: 527–534. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Joseph RM, Keehn B, Connolly C, Wolfe JM, Horowitz TS ( 2009): Why is visual search superior in autism spectrum disorder? Dev Sci 12: 1083–1096. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Just MA, Carpenter PA, Keller TA, Eddy WF, Thulborn KR ( 1996): Brain activation modulated by sentence comprehension. Science 274: 114–116. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Just MA, Cherkassky VL, Keller TA, Kana RK, Minshew NJ ( 2007): Functional and anatomical cortical underconnectivity in autism: Evidence from an FMRI study of an executive function task and corpus callosum morphometry. Cereb Cortex 17: 951–961. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Just MA, Cherkassky VL, Keller TA, Minshew NJ ( 2004): Cortical activation and synchronization during sentence comprehension in high‐functioning autism: Evidence of underconnectivity. Brain 127( Part 8): 1811–1821. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Kana RK, Keller TA, Cherkassky VL, Minshew NJ, Just MA ( 2006): Sentence comprehension in autism: Thinking in pictures with decreased functional connectivity. Brain 129( Part 9): 2484–2493. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Kana RK, Keller TA, Cherkassky VL, Minshew NJ, Just MA ( 2009): Atypical frontal‐posterior synchronization of Theory of Mind regions in autism during mental state attribution. Soc Neurosci 4: 135–152. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Kanwisher N, Chun MM, McDermott J, Ledden PJ ( 1996): Functional imaging of human visual recognition. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 5( 1–2): 55–67. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Kanwisher N, McDermott J, Chun MM ( 1997): The fusiform face area: A module in human extrastriate cortex specialized for face perception. J Neurosci 17: 4302–4311. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Keehn B, Brenner L, Palmer E, Lincoln AJ, Muller RA ( 2008): Functional brain organization for visual search in ASD. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 14: 990–1003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Keita L, Mottron L, Bertone A ( 2010): Far visual acuity is unremarkable in autism: Do we need to focus on crowding? Autism Res 3; 333–341. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Kemner C, van der Geest JN, Verbaten MN, van Engeland H ( 2004): In search of neurophysiological markers of pervasive developmental disorders: Smooth pursuit eye movements? J Neural Transm 111: 1617–1626. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Kemner C, van Ewijk L, van Engeland H, Hooge I ( 2008): Brief report: Eye movements during visual search tasks indicate enhanced stimulus discriminability in subjects with PDD. J Autism Dev Disord 38: 553–557. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Kennedy DP, Courchesne E ( 2008): Functional abnormalities of the default network during self‐ and other‐reflection in autism. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 3: 177–190. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Kennedy DP, Redcay E, Courchesne E ( 2006): Failing to deactivate: Resting functional abnormalities in autism. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 103: 8275–8280. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Kleinhans NM, Muller RA, Cohen DN, Courchesne E ( 2008a) Atypical functional lateralization of language in autism spectrum disorders. Brain Res 1221: 115–125. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Kleinhans NM, Richards T, Sterling L, Stegbauer KC, Mahurin R, Johnson LC, Greenson J, Dawson G, Aylward E ( 2008b): Abnormal functional connectivity in autism spectrum disorders during face processing. Brain 131( Part 4): 1000–1012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Knaus TA, Silver AM, Lindgren KA, Hadjikhani N, Tager‐Flusberg H ( 2008): fMRI activation during a language task in adolescents with ASD. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 14: 967–979. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Konishi S, Nakajima K, Uchida I, Kikyo H, Kameyama M, Miyashita Y ( 1999): Common inhibitory mechanism in human inferior prefrontal cortex revealed by event‐related functional MRI. Brain 122 ( Part 5): 981–991. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Koshino H, Carpenter PA, Minshew NJ, Cherkassky VL, Keller TA, Just MA ( 2005): Functional connectivity in an fMRI working memory task in high‐functioning autism. Neuroimage 24: 810–821. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Lahaie A, Mottron L, Arguin M, Berthiaume C, Jemel B, Saumier D ( 2006): Face perception in high‐functioning autistic adults: Evidence for superior processing of face parts, not for a configural face‐processing deficit. Neuropsychology 20: 30–41. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Laird AR, Fox PM, Price CJ, Glahn DC, Uecker AM, Lancaster JL, Turkeltaub PE, Kochunov P, Fox PT ( 2005): ALE meta‐analysis: Controlling the false discovery rate and performing statistical contrasts. Hum Brain Mapp 25: 155–164. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Lancaster JL, Tordesillas‐Gutierrez D, Martinez M, Salinas F, Evans A, Zilles K, Mazziotta JC, Fox PT ( 2007): Bias between MNI and Talairach coordinates analyzed using the ICBM‐152 brain template. Hum Brain Mapp 28: 1194–1205. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Langdell T ( 1978): Recognition of faces: An approach to the study of autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 19: 255–268. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Lee PS, Foss‐Feig J, Henderson JG, Kenworthy LE, Gilotty L, Gaillard WD, Vaidya CJ ( 2007): Atypical neural substrates of Embedded Figures Task performance in children with Autism Spectrum Disorder. Neuroimage 38: 184–193. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Lehmann C, Mueller T, Federspiel A, Hubl D, Schroth G, Huber O, Strik W, Dierks T ( 2004): Dissociation between overt and unconscious face processing in fusiform face area. Neuroimage 21: 75–83. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Lepisto T, Kujala T, Vanhala R, Alku P, Huotilainen M, Naatanen R ( 2005): The discrimination of and orienting to speech and non‐speech sounds in children with autism. Brain Res 1066( 1–2): 147–157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Li J, Liu J, Liang J, Zhang H, Zhao J, Huber DE, Rieth CA, Lee K, Tian J, Shi G ( 2009): A distributed neural system for top‐down face processing. Neurosci Lett 451: 6–10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Liu J, Harris A, Kanwisher N ( 2010): Perception of face parts and face configurations: An FMRI study. J Cogn Neurosci 22: 203–211. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Luna B, Doll SK, Hegedus SJ, Minshew NJ, Sweeney JA ( 2007): Saturation of executive function in autism. Biol Psychiatry 61: 474–481. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Luna B, Minshew NJ, Garver KE, Lazar NA, Thulborn KR, Eddy WF, Sweeney JA ( 2002): Neocortical system abnormalities in autism: An fMRI study of spatial working memory. Neurology 59: 834–840. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Malach R, Reppas JB, Benson RR, Kwong KK, Jiang H, Kennedy WA, Ledden PJ, Brady TJ, Rosen BR, Tootell RB ( 1995): Object‐related activity revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging in human occipital cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 92: 8135–8139. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Manjaly ZM, Bruning N, Neufang S, Stephan KE, Brieber S, Marshall JC, Kamp‐Becker I, Remschmidt H, Herpertz‐Dahlmann B, Konrad K, et al. ( 2007): Neurophysiological correlates of relatively enhanced local visual search in autistic adolescents. Neuroimage 35: 283–291. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Martin A, Chao LL ( 2001): Semantic memory and the brain: Structure and processes. Curr Opin Neurobiol 11: 194–201. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Mason RA, Williams DL, Kana RK, Minshew N, Just MA ( 2008): Theory of Mind disruption and recruitment of the right hemisphere during narrative comprehension in autism. Neuropsychologia 46: 269–280. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Mechelli A, Price CJ, Friston KJ, Ishai A ( 2004): There bottom‐up meets top‐down: Neuronal interactions during perception and imagery. Cereb Cortex 14: 1256–1265. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Milne E, Griffiths H, Buckley D, Scope A ( 2009): Vision in children and adolescents with autistic spectrum disorder: Evidence for reduced convergence. J Autism Dev Disord 39: 965–975. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Mottron L, Dawson M, Soulieres I, Hubert B, Burack J ( 2006): 3nhanced perceptual functioning in autism: An update, and eight principles of autistic perception. J Autism Dev Disord 36: 27–43. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Muller RA, Kleinhans N, Kemmotsu N, Pierce K, Courchesne E ( 2003): Abnormal variability and distribution of functional maps in autism: An FMRI study of visuomotor learning. Am J Psychiatry 160: 1847–1862. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. Newman TM, Macomber D, Naples AJ, Babitz T, Volkmar F, Grigorenko EL ( 2007): Hyperlexia in children with autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord 37: 760–774. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. Nobre AC, Sebestyen GN, Gitelman DR, Mesulam MM, Frackowiak RS, Frith CD ( 1997): Functional localization of the system for visuospatial attention using positron emission tomography. Brain 120 ( Part 3): 515–533. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. O'Riordan MA ( 2004): Superior visual search in adults with autism. Autism 8: 229–248. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. O'Riordan MA, Plaisted KC, Driver J, Baron‐Cohen S ( 2001): Superior visual search in autism. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 27: 719–730. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  113. Oberman L, Ifert‐Miller F, Najib UU, Bashir SS, Woollacott I, Gonzalez‐Heydrich J, Picker J, Rotenberg A, Pascual‐Leone A ( 2010): Transcranial magnetic stimulation provides means to assess cortical plasticity and excitability in humans with fragile X syndrome and autism spectrum disorder. Frontiers Synaptic Neurosci 2: 1–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. Op de Beeck HP, Dicarlo JJ, Goense JB, Grill‐Spector K, Papanastassiou A, Tanifuji M, Tsao DY ( 2008): Line‐scale spatial organization of face and object selectivity in the temporal lobe: Do functional magnetic resonance imaging, optical imaging, and electrophysiology agree? J Neurosci 28: 11796–11801. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Owen AM ( 2004): Working memory: Imaging the magic number four. Curr Biol 14: R573–R574. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Patel GA, Sathian K ( 2000): Visual search: Bottom‐up or top‐down? Front Biosci 5: D169–D193. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  117. Pei F, Baldassi S, Procida G, Igliozzi R, Tancredi R, Muratori F, Cioni G ( 2009): Neural correlates of texture and contour integration in children with autism spectrum disorders. Vision Res 49: 2140–2150. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  118. Pelphrey KA, Sasson NJ, Reznick JS, Paul G, Goldman BD, Piven J ( 2002): Visual scanning of faces in autism. J Autism Dev Disord 32: 249–261. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  119. Petersen SE, Fox PT, Posner MI, Mintun M, Raichle ME ( 1988): Positron emission tomographic studies of the cortical anatomy of single‐word processing. Nature 331: 585–589. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  120. Petrides M ( 1991): Monitoring of selections of visual stimuli and the primate frontal cortex. Proc Biol Sci 246: 293–298. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  121. Petrides M ( 1994): Frontal lobes and behaviour. Curr Opin Neurobiol 4: 207–211. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  122. Petrides M ( 1996): Specialized systems for the processing of mnemonic information within the primate frontal cortex. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 351: 1455–1461; discussion 1461–1462. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  123. Petrides M ( 2000): Dissociable roles of mid‐dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior inferotemporal cortex in visual working memory. J Neurosci 20: 7496–7503. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  124. Petrides M ( 2002): The mid‐ventrolateral prefrontal cortex and active mnemonic retrieval. Neurobiol Learn Mem 78: 528–538. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  125. Petrides M ( 2005): Lateral prefrontal cortex: Architectonic and functional organization. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 360: 781–795. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  126. Pierce K, Haist F, Sedaghat F, Courchesne E ( 2004): The brain response to personally familiar faces in autism: Findings of fusiform activity and beyond. Brain 127( Part 12): 2703–2716. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  127. Pierce K, Muller RA, Ambrose J, Allen G, Courchesne E ( 2001): Face processing occurs outside the fusiform ‘face area’ in autism: Evidence from functional MRI. Brain 124( Part 10): 2059–2073. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  128. Plaisted K, O'Riordan M, Baron‐Cohen S ( 1998): Enhanced discrimination of novel, highly similar stimuli by adults with autism during a perceptual learning task. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 39: 765–775. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  129. Poldrack RA, Wagner AD, Prull MW, Desmond JE, Glover GH, Gabrieli JD ( 1999): Functional specialization for semantic and phonological processing in the left inferior prefrontal cortex. Neuroimage 10: 15–35. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  130. Price CJ ( 2000): The anatomy of language: Contributions from functional neuroimaging. J Anat 197 ( Part 3): 335–359. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  131. Puce A, Allison T, Asgari M, Gore JC, McCarthy G ( 1996): Differential sensitivity of human visual cortex to faces, letterstrings, and textures: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J Neurosci 16: 5205–5215. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  132. Rhodes G, Michie PT, Hughes ME, Byatt G ( 2009): The fusiform face area and occipital face area show sensitivity to spatial relations in faces. Eur J Neurosci 30: 721–733. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  133. Rinaldi T, Silberberg G, Markram H ( 2008): Hyperconnectivity of local neocortical microcircuitry induced by prenatal exposure to valproic acid. Cereb Cortex 18: 763–770. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  134. Ring HA, Baron‐Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Williams SC, Brammer M, Andrew C, Bullmore ET ( 1999): Cerebral correlates of preserved cognitive skills in autism: A functional MRI study of embedded figures task performance. Brain 122 ( Part 7): 1305–1315. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  135. Rizzolatti G, Luppino G ( 2001): The cortical motor system. Neuron 31: 889–901. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  136. Roder B, Stock O, Neville H, Bien S, Rosler F ( 2002): Brain activation modulated by the comprehension of normal and pseudo‐word sentences of different processing demands: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Neuroimage 15: 1003–1014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  137. Rogers RD, Andrews TC, Grasby PM, Brooks DJ, Robbins TW ( 2000): Contrasting cortical and subcortical activations produced by attentional‐set shifting and reversal learning in humans. J Cogn Neurosci 12: 142–162. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  138. Rojas DC, Bawn SD, Benkers TL, Reite ML, Rogers SJ ( 2002): Smaller left hemisphere planum temporale in adults with autistic disorder. Neurosci Lett 328: 237–240. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  139. Rotshtein P, Henson RN, Treves A, Driver J, Dolan RJ ( 2005): Morphing Marilyn into Maggie dissociates physical and identity face representations in the brain. Nat Neurosci 8: 107–113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  140. Scherf SK, Luna B, Minshew N, Behrmann M ( 2010): Location, location, location: Alterations in the functional topography of face‐ but not object‐ or place‐related cortex in adolescent with autism. Frontiers Hum Neurosci 4: 1–16. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  141. Schmitz N, Rubia K, Daly E, Smith A, Williams S, Murphy DG ( 2006): Neural correlates of executive function in autistic spectrum disorders. Biol Psychiatry 59: 7–16. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  142. Schmitz N, Rubia K, van Amelsvoort T, Daly E, Smith A, Murphy DG ( 2008): Neural correlates of reward in autism. Br J Psychiatry 192: 19–24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  143. Schultz RT, Gauthier I, Klin A, Fulbright RK, Anderson AW, Volkmar F, Skudlarski P, Lacadie C, Cohen DJ, Gore JC ( 2000): Abnormal ventral temporal cortical activity during face discrimination among individuals with autism and Asperger syndrome. Arch Gen Psychiatry 57: 331–340. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  144. Shah A, Frith U ( 1993): Why do autistic individuals show superior performance on the block design task? J Child Psychol Psychiatry 34: 1351–1364. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  145. Shaywitz SE, Shaywitz BA ( 2008): Paying attention to reading: The neurobiology of reading and dyslexia. Dev Psychopathol 20: 1329–1349. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  146. Silani G, Bird G, Brindley R, Singer T, Frith C, Frith U ( 2008): Levels of emotional awareness and autism: An fMRI study. Soc Neurosci 3: 97–112. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  147. Silk TJ, Rinehart N, Bradshaw JL, Tonge B, Egan G, O'Boyle MW, Cunnington R ( 2006): Visuospatial processing and the function of prefrontal‐parietal networks in autism spectrum disorders: A functional MRI study. Am J Psychiatry 163: 1440–1443. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  148. Solomon M, Ozonoff SJ, Ursu S, Ravizza S, Cummings N, Ly S, Carter CS ( 2009): The neural substrates of cognitive control deficits in autism spectrum disorders. Neuropsychologia 47: 2515–2526. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  149. Soulières I, Dawson M, Samson F, Barbeau EB, Sahyoun CP, Strangman GE, Zeffiro TA, Mottron L ( 2009): Enhanced visual processing contributes to matrix reasoning in autism. Hum Brain Mapp 30: 4082–4107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  150. Suchan B, Botko R, Gizewski E, Forsting M, Daum I ( 2006): Neural substrates of manipulation in visuospatial working memory. Neuroscience 139: 351–357. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  151. Suchan B, Yaguez L, Wunderlich G, Canavan AG, Herzog H, Tellmann L, Homberg V, Seitz RJ ( 2002): Neural correlates of visuospatial imagery. Behav Brain Res 131( 1–2): 163–168. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  152. Takarae Y, Minshew NJ, Luna B, Krisky CM, Sweeney JA ( 2004): Pursuit eye movement deficits in autism. Brain 127( Part 12): 2584–2594. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  153. Takarae Y, Minshew NJ, Luna B, Sweeney JA ( 2007): Atypical involvement of frontostriatal systems during sensorimotor control in autism. Psychiatry Res 156: 117–1127. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  154. Toichi M, Kamio Y ( 2001): Verbal association for simple common words in high‐functioning autism. J Autism Dev Disord 31: 483–490. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  155. Turkeltaub PE, Eden GF, Jones KM, Zeffiro TA ( 2002): Meta‐analysis of the functional neuroanatomy of single‐word reading: Method and validation. Neuroimage 16( Part 1): 765–780. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  156. Vandenbroucke MW, Scholte HS, van Engeland H, Lamme VA, Kemner C ( 2008): A neural substrate for atypical low‐level visual processing in autism spectrum disorder. Brain 131( Part 4): 1013–1024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  157. Wager TD, Lindquist MA, Nichols TE, Kober H, Van Snellenberg JX ( 2009): Evaluating the consistency and specificity of neuroimaging data using meta‐analysis. Neuroimage 45( 1 Suppl): S210–S221. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  158. Wandell BA, Dumoulin SO, Brewer AA ( 2007): Visual field maps in human cortex. Neuron 56: 366–383. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  159. Wang L, Mottron L, Peng D, Berthiaume C, Dawson M ( 2007): Local bias and local‐to‐global interference without global deficit: A robust finding in autism under various conditions of attention, exposure time, and visual angle. Cogn Neuropsychol 24: 550–574. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  160. Yeh YY, Kuo BC, Liu HL ( 2007): The neural correlates of attention orienting in visuospatial working memory for detecting feature and conjunction changes. Brain Res 1130: 146–157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Additional Supporting Information may be found in the online version of this article.

Supporting Information Figure 1.

Supporting Information Figure 2.

Supporting Information Figure 3.

Supporting Information Figure 4.


Articles from Human Brain Mapping are provided here courtesy of Wiley

RESOURCES