Skip to main content
Human Brain Mapping logoLink to Human Brain Mapping
. 2005 Apr 15;25(3):328–335. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20110

Anterior cingulate cortex: An fMRI analysis of conflict specificity and functional differentiation

Michael P Milham 1,, Marie T Banich 1,2
PMCID: PMC6871683  PMID: 15834861

INTRODUCTION

Despite more than a decade of neuroimaging studies of human cognition, a comprehensive understanding of the contributions of the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) to cognitive control remains elusive. During this time, increases in ACC activity have been implicated in a variety of processes ranging from word reading to response selection to error processing [Paus, 2001]. In attempting to explain the seemingly widespread involvement of ACC in cognition, several authors have attempted to identify the lowest common denominator. Of these efforts, the conflict monitoring theory has proven to be amongst the most prominent and enduring [Botvinick et al., 2004; Carter et al., 1998]. Proposed by Carter et al. [1998], this theory asserts that ACC works to detect the occurrence of conflict capable of creating a potential for erroneous task performance. In response to the occurrence of such conflict, ACC is posited to send a signal to areas involved in top‐down control (e.g., dorsolateral prefrontal cortex [DLPFC]) to increase their involvement in task performance.

Of note, a recent study by our laboratory suggests that the involvement of the ACC in attentional control may extend beyond conditions of conflict [Milham et al., 2002]. Using the color–word Stroop task, we detected increases in ACC activity for older individuals (age 60–75 years) whenever task‐irrelevant information was related to task demands, not just when it conflicted with task‐relevant information. More specifically, in older subjects we found increases in neural activity for both congruent (e.g., the word “red” in red ink) and incongruent (e.g., the word “blue” in red ink) trial types of the Stroop task when compared to that for neutral trials (e.g., the word “lot” in red ink). In contrast, younger participants (age 20–35 years) only exhibited increased activity in the ACC for incongruent trials relative to the neutral trial baseline. Although these findings may suggest some functional change in ACC associated with aging, we suggest they provide a hint that the role of the ACC in attentional control is not restricted to conflict monitoring.

The primary goal of the present work is to reexamine the validity of the hypothesis that ACC activity is specific to conditions of conflict. In doing so, we will entertain an alternative hypothesis, namely, that ACC is a complex brain region with functionally distinct subregions. As such, it is possible that the conflict monitoring theory may explain the functional contributions of a portion of ACC to cognitive control, but not the ACC as a whole. Several authors have already suggested that ACC contains functionally distinct subregions, typically focusing on rostral regions of ACC, which seem to be involved in error‐related processing [Braver et al., 2001; Kiehl et al., 2000].

To accomplish our goals, we once again made use of the color–word Stroop task, although with an event‐related fMRI design that we expected would increase attentional demands relative to the blocked design employed in our prior work. We expected that such an approach would enhance our ability to detect increases in neural activity within ACC. Our analyses were designed to identify regions of ACC sensitive to: (1) the presence of task‐irrelevant information related to the task concept (i.e., color) regardless of conflict (incongruent and congruent > neutral); and (2) those specifically sensitive to the presence of conflicting task‐irrelevant information (incongruent > congruent = neutral).

Given the frequent usage of Stroop or Stroop‐like stimuli in studies of top‐down attentional control, we also took the opportunity to examine the impact of the spatial characteristics of Stroop stimuli on top‐down control. More specifically, behavioral studies have shown that physically separating the sources of task‐relevant information and task‐irrelevant information produces a marked decrease in Stroop interference and facilitation effects [e.g., Dyer, 1973; Flowers and Stoup, 1977; Glaser and Glaser, 1982]. We thus included a manipulation of whether or not the sources of task‐relevant and task‐irrelevant information were integrated physically. To do so, we compared two conditions, one in which the ink color appeared on the word (integrated stimulus) and one in which the ink color appeared on a rectangle surrounding the word, which was printed in gray (nonintegrated stimulus). Given prior studies of spatial attention and binding [Shafritz et al., 2002], we predicted that posterior regions (i.e., extrastriate and parietal regions) may play a larger role in selection for physically separated stimulus, thereby decreasing the need for the contributions of frontal regions such as DLPFC and ACC.

SUBJECTS AND METHODS

Participants

Eighteen right‐handed, native English speakers recruited from the Champaign‐Urbana community (age range, 18–40 years) participated in the study. All participants were screened to ensure that they did not have a history of neurologic damage or color blindness and gave informed consent before participation.

Stimuli and Design

Similar to the standard color–word Stroop task, we presented a series of trials on which participants are required to identify a color (blue, orange, green, or red) in the presence of a word. We varied whether or not the ink color to be identified was physically separate from the word. For each trial, the stimulus display consisted of a word and a rectangle; such that the rectangle surrounded the word without any overlap, keeping the two items distinct. There were two trial types: (1) integrated trials, in which the word was printed in the to‐be‐identified ink color whereas the rectangle was printed in gray; and (2) nonintegrated trials, in which the rectangle was printed in the to‐be‐identified ink color whereas the word was printed in gray. Of note, for any given trial, the task‐relevant color information had an equal probability of appearing on the word or on the rectangle surrounding the word. Participants thus could not anticipate either the spatial location of the task‐relevant information or the object that would introduce the task‐relevant information. Such a design prevented both space‐based and object‐based attention from modulating selection of task‐relevant information.

To examine the impact of introducing a task‐irrelevant source of color information, we included the following color‐word types: (1) incongruent words that name a color incongruent with the to‐be‐identified ink color (e.g., for integrated stimuli, the word RED printed in green with the rectangle printed in gray; for nonintegrated stimuli, the rectangle printed in green with the word RED printed in gray); (2) congruent words that name the to‐be‐identified ink color (e.g., for integrated stimuli, the word RED printed in red with the rectangle printed in gray; for nonintegrated stimuli, the rectangle printed in red with the word RED printed in gray); (3) neutral words that are unrelated to color (e.g., for integrated stimuli, the word LOT printed in red with the rectangle printed in gray; for nonintegrated stimuli, the rectangle printed in red with the word LOT printed in gray). The neutral words were matched with the color words for word frequency and length.

Our study made use of a rapid‐presentation event‐related design. Functional data for each participant was acquired during a single session consisting of three runs. Each run consisted of 72 trials (12 trials/condition) presented at a rate of 7 s/condition. First‐order counterbalancing was used such that each trial type was preceded by each of the six trial types an equal number of times. Each trial consisted of a 300‐ms fixation cross followed by a 1,200 ms presentation of the stimulus (word and rectangle) and 5,500‐ms intertrial interval. All stimuli were programmed using Mel v2.0 and presented using an IBM‐PC compatible computer.

Data Acquisition

A GE Signa (1.5‐T) MRI system equipped for echo‐planar imaging (EPI) was used for data acquisition. For each run, a total of 261 EPI images was acquired (repetition time [TR] = 2,000 ms; echo time [TE] = 40 ms; flip angle = 90 degrees), each consisting of 13 contiguous slices (thickness = 7 mm, gap = 1 mm, and in‐plane resolution = 3.75 mm), parallel to the anterior commissure–posterior commissure (AC–PC) line. A high‐resolution 3‐D anatomic set was collected for each participant, as well as T1‐weighted images of our functional acquisition slices. The head coil was fitted with a bite bar to minimize head motion during the session. Stimuli were presented on a goggle system designed by Magnetic Resonance Technologies. A four‐button response pad was used to obtain behavioral measures of task‐performance.

Image Processing

Image processing and within‐subject statistical analyses were carried out using FMRIB Easy Analysis Tool (FEAT; http://www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/index.html). The first five volumes of each run's time series were discarded to allow the MR signal to reach steady state. Before statistical tests, images were motion corrected using Motion Correction using FMR1B's Linear Image Registration Tool (MCFLIRT) [Jenkinson and Smith, 2001]. The following prestatistics processing was applied: (1) spatial smoothing using a Gaussian kernel of full‐width half‐maximum (FWHM) = 8 mm; (2) mean‐based intensity normalization of all volumes by the same factor; and (3) nonlinear high‐pass temporal filtering (Gaussian‐weighted LSF straight line fitting, with σ = 35.0 s).

Statistical Analyses

Statistical analysis was carried out for each participant using FMRIB's improved linear model (FILM) with local autocorrelation correction [Woolrich et al., 2001]. Event‐related responses for each condition were modeled using the statistical parametric mapping (SPM99) hemodynamic response. Temporal derivates for each predictor were included in the model as well, to account for possible differences in phase between the stimulus functions and observed data. Trials on which individuals made an error were excluded from our analyses. Our comparisons of interest included the following: competition (when the task‐irrelevant attribute, the word, acts as a source of color information capable of competing with task‐relevant color information; incongruent and congruent > neutral); conflict (incongruent > congruent); stimulus integration (integrated > nonintegrated); Competition × Integration; and Conflict × Integration.

FLIRT was used to transform parameter estimate (PE) maps into a common stereotaxic space [Talairach and Tournoux, 1988]. For each contrast, group random‐effects analyses were used to test if the individual participants' PE values were reliably greater than 0 (P < 0.0025; contiguity threshold = 20 voxels) [Forman et al., 1995].

Confirmatory Overlay Analysis: Regions Active for Both Integrated and Nonintegrated Stimuli

To confirm that the increases in activity during attentionally demanding conditions are present for both integrated and nonintegrated stimuli, we carried out an overlay analysis in addition to the standard approach of testing for possible interactions with stimulus integration. More specifically, at each voxel we tested for the presence of significant increases in activity associated with our comparisons of interest (i.e., competition; conflict) for integrated and nonintegrated stimuli separately, and then overlaid the two (using a threshold of P < 0.05 each). Any voxel not passing the overlay analysis of integrated and nonintegrated stimuli was considered insignificant.

Confirmatory Analysis: Competition and Conflict Specificity

To confirm that the increases in neural activity identified by our contrast testing for regions sensitive to competing color information regardless of conflict (congruent and incongruent > neutral) are present for both congruent and incongruent stimuli, we carried out an overlay (logical AND) analysis. More specifically, for each voxel identified as significant in our primary analysis, we tested for the presence of significant increases in neural activity for congruent and incongruent trial types (relative to neutral) individually and then overlaid the two (i.e., congruent > neutral at P < 0.05 and incongruent > neutral at P < 0.05).

To test for regions that are activated specifically during conditions of conflict, we carried out a disjunction test. First, we excluded any voxels found to be significant (P < 0.0025) when we tested for the impact of competing color information regardless of conflict (congruent and incongruent > neutral). Among the remaining voxels, we next took those found to be sensitive to conflict (incongruent > congruent) at a level of P < 0.0025 and ensured that no increases in activity were noted for the comparison of congruent > neutral at P < 0.05. Finally, to be conservative, we also excluded any voxels that were found to be sensitive to both congruent and incongruent trial types in our prior study [Milham et al., 2002].

RESULTS

Behavioral Findings

Consistent with the findings of prior studies, behavioral measures of performance were influenced by whether or not the sources of task‐relevant and task‐irrelevant information were perceptually conjoined. Because the reaction time for neutral trials differed for integrated (mean ± standard error of the mean [SEM]: neutral, 679 ± 12 ms; congruent, 671 ± 14 ms; incongruent, 775 ± 20 ms) and nonintegrated trials (neutral, 699 ± 15 ms; congruent, 672 ± 15 ms; incongruent, 742 ± 17 ms), we converted reaction time measure of performance for congruent and incongruent trial types into the percentage facilitation and percentage interference, respectively (Fig. 1). We then carried out a repeated‐measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) with the factors of INTEGRATION (integrated; nonintegrated) and STROOP EFFECT TYPE (interference; facilitation). We found a marginally significant main effect of INTEGRATION (F[1,17] = 3.96; P < 0.063), and a significant main effect of STROOP EFFECT TYPE (F[1,17] = 6.338; P < 0.022). More important was a significant interaction between INTEGRATION and STROOP EFFECT TYPE (F[1,17] = 6.338; P < 0.022). The Stroop interference effect noted for integrated stimuli was approximately twice as large as that noted for nonintegrated stimuli (F[1,17] = 7.54; P < 0.014), but facilitation effects noted for nonintegrated and integrated stimuli did not differ significantly (F[1,17] = 1.566; P < 0.228). The greater impact of integration on the interference effect than the facilitation effect is consistent with variability across studies regarding the influence of integration on the facilitation effect [MacLeod, 1991].

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Behavioral findings. Note: For clusters in which two or more peaks were detected, each individual peak is reported listed.

fMRI Findings

Conjunction analysis: Regions sensitive to competing task‐related information regardless of conflict

Regression analyses indicate the presence of ACC subregions sensitive to the presence of competing task‐related information, regardless of conflict. More specifically, our contrast of congruent and incongruent trials with neutral trials detected increases in activity within a portion of anterior cingulate cortex commonly activated in tasks requiring attentional control, referred to by Barch et al. [2001] as the posterior division of the rostral zone. These increases extended into nearby portions of pre‐supplementary motor area (SMA). Confirmatory analyses verified the presence of increases for both congruent and incongruent trial types, indicating that activity within the posterior rostral zone is greater whenever there are two competing source of task‐related information on which one could base a response, a finding that can be viewed as inconsistent with the conflict monitoring theory [Botvinick et al., 2004].

Disjunction analysis: Regions specifically sensitive to competing task‐related information that conflicts

Our disjunction analyses (described in the methods section) identified regions that were active for incongruent trials but not congruent trials relative to the baseline of neutral trials. In contrast to the posterior portion of the rostral zone of the ACC, the anterior portion of the rostral zone (Brodmann area [BA] 24; center of intensity: x = 0; y = 30; z = 22; cluster size = 112) was sensitive solely to the occurrence of conflict (incongruent but not congruent) (Fig. 2). This functional differentiation within anterior cingulate was confirmed by a paired t test, which found that increases in neural activity associated with congruent trial types were greater in the posterior portion of the rostral zone than the anterior portion (n = 18; P < 0.05). These findings are consistent with the assertion by Botvinick et al. [2004] that this region is associated with conflict monitoring. Of note, this same portion of ACC has been implicated in error‐related processing by others [e.g., Braver et al., 2001; Kiehl, 2001]. Consistent with the suggestions of Carter et al. [e.g., Carter et al., 1998], our findings suggest a role for this ACC subregion in handling conflict, of which error processing is a special case.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Functional differentiation within ACC as shown by confirmatory analyses. Our overlay analysis identified regions within the posterior rostral zone of ACC and nearby pre‐SMA sensitive to the presence of competing color information, regardless of whether it conflicted with task‐relevant information (i.e., congruent > neutral and incongruent > neutral). In contrast, our disjunction analysis identified a conflict‐specific region within the anterior rostral zone, only increasing in activity when competing color information conflicted (i.e., incongruent > congruent = neutral). Note: for clusters in which two or more peaks were detected, each individual is reported listed. (Image displayed on radiological orientation.)

Stimulus integration

Consistent with prior studies of the color–word Stroop task, we found increases in neural activity throughout the frontoparietal network when competing or conflicting color information was present (Fig. 3; Table 1 and 2) [Banich et al., 2000; Carter et al., 2000; Milham et al, 2002]. Despite our predictions, neural activity within neither prefrontal nor parietal regions was dependent upon sources of task‐relevant and task‐irrelevant information. Stimulus integration interacted with neither the presence of competing color information (congruent and incongruent > neutral) nor conflicting color information (incongruent > congruent), even at a more liberal threshold of P < 0.01.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Impact of competition and conflict on frontal and parietal regions. Voxel‐wise analyses (P < 0.0025) identified frontal and parietal regions sensitive to (1) the mere presence competing color information, regardless of whether it conflicted with task‐relevant information (congruent and incongruent > neutral) (blue), (2) whether competing color information conflicted with task‐relevant information (orange), and (3) both the presence of competing color information and whether it conflicted (incongruent > congruent neutral) (purple). Note: for clusters in which two or more peaks were detected, each individual peak is reported listed. (Image displayed in radiological orientation.)

Table I.

Regions sensitive to the presence of competing color information

Region BA x y z −Log(prob) Cluster size*
Cerebellum 38 −68 −22 4.14 462
Superior colliculus 12 −28 −6 4.12 825
Recticular nucleus 4 −12 −2 2.95
Thalamus −14 −28 −2 4.99
Thalamus 14 6 −2 4.55 65
Fusiform gyrus 19 −22 −56 −6 3.09 159
Lingual gyrus 18 −6 −86 0 4.31 129
18 −34 14 −14 3.83 34
Inferior occipital gyrus 18 −38 −72 0 5.1 325
Middle occipital gyrus 18/19 −30 −80 8 3.45
Middle occipital gyrus 18/19 38 −82 14 3.91 263
Cuneus 17 10 −78 10 2.85 16
Middle temporal gyrus 21 66 −50 10 6.35 211
Superior temporal gyrus 22 50 −50 22 3.46 79
Precuneus 19 18 −68 28 3.71 353
AIPS 40 28 −62 46 3.54 164
Superior temporal gyrus 22 −54 −48 22 3.43 2,934
Middle temporal gyrus 39 −34 −54 22 3.69
Precuneus 7 −16 −68 28 3.57
AIPS 40 −30 −62 40 4.78
Superior parietal lobule 7 −14 −68 54 2.88
Inferior parietal lobule 40 −46 −30 40 3.78
Insula −32 22 2 4.18 2,650
Middle frontal gyrus 46 −44 46 22 5.15
Inferior frontal gyrus 44 −46 18 26 5.34
Middle frontal gyrus 8 −30 26 54 5.36
Middle frontal gyrus 6 −44 6 52 3.87 49
Middle frontal gyrus 10 −28 42 2 4.04 75
Insula 30 22 2 3.21 318
Middle frontal gyrus 9 34 18 18 3.9
Middle frontal gyrus 10 32 38 20 3.72 959
Middle frontal gyrus 9 36 58 30 3.53
Middle frontal gyrus 8 28 46 44 4.19
Middle frontal gyrus 8 44 24 48 4.11
Anterior cingulate cortex 32 −2 32 36 2.7 590
Anterior cingulate cortex 32 −2 14 38 4.22
Pre‐SMA 6 0 14 54 4.06
Anterior cingulate cortex 32 12 22 36 2.97 24

For clusters in which two or more peaks were detected, each individual peak is reported listed.

*

Cluster size given in number of voxels.

BA, Brodmann area.

Table II.

Regions sensitive to the presence of conflicting color information

Region BA x y z −Log(prob) Cluster size
Cerebellum 34 −32 −24 3.76 43
Thalamus −12 −24 16 3.57 111
Recticular nucleus 8 −16 −4 4.12 237
Fusiform gyrus 37 −40 −50 −12 3.78 188
Fusiform gyrus 19 −26 −78 −12 3.3 75
Lingual gyrus 19 −18 −54 −8 3.58 35
Fusiform gyrus 18 34 −78 −8 4.29 109
Fusiform gyrus 37 50 −50 −16 3.21 42
Middle temporal gyrus 21 56 −38 −4 3.26 33
Lingual gyrus 18 2 −70 2 3.42 197
Lingual gyrus 18 24 −64 6 3.02 37
AIPS −26 −66 34 6.4 3,275
AIPS 39 −32 −48 38 5.9
Precuneus 7 2 −54 36 3.33
Inferior parietal lobule 40 54 −46 26 3.79 945
AIPS 39 34 −50 36 5.13
Superior frontal gyrus 10 −30 56 2 4.08 27
Superior frontal gyrus 10 −36 16 6 4.18 1,817
Middle frontal gyrus 9 −42 6 32 5.86
Inferior frontal gyrus 47 36 28 −6 4 100
Inferior frontal gyrus 44 40 14 26 5.25 675
Anterior cingulate cortex 24 6 20 28 4.86 1,653
Anterior cingulate cortex 32 −4 14 48 3.97

The only portion of the frontal lobes that showed any reliable changes in activity with respect to stimulus integration was located within anterior inferior prefrontal cortex (BA44), with neural activity being greater for integrated than for nonintegrated stimuli, regardless of competition or conflict. Given that this region was not an a priori region of interest, we believe this finding should be taken with some caution.

DISCUSSION

The present work tested a crucial component of the conflict monitoring theory, namely the hypothesis that the involvement of the anterior cingulate cortex in cognitive control is limited to situations involving conflict. Consistent with this hypothesis, the anterior division of the rostral zone exhibited conflict specific increases in neural activity (i.e., incongruent > congruent = neutral). In contrast, the posterior division exhibited increases in neural activity whenever the task‐irrelevant information was color related, regardless of whether it was conflicting (i.e., incongruent and congruent > neutral). Only the anterior division of the rostral zone thus seems to exhibit the conflict specificity posited by the conflict monitoring theory.

The pattern of activity within the posterior zone may be better explained by theories of ACC function that suggest its involvement in response selection, facilitating infrequent or novel responses, and inhibiting pre‐potent responses [Paus et al., 1993; Paus, 2001]. On both incongruent and congruent trials, the response must be based on the stimulus dimension for which processing is less automatic (i.e., ink color) rather than the stimulus dimension for which processing is more automatic (i.e., word reading), thereby increasing the attentional demands including those associated with response selection. As such, increases in neural activity would be predicted for both trial types. The posterior division exhibited such a pattern of activity, suggesting its involvement in response selection. Such a finding is consistent with our prior work indicating that this region of the ACC is only activated when the word names a potential response [Milham et al., 2001]. In that study, we found that this region was activated by response‐eligible trials in which the word names a possible response (e.g., the word “blue” in yellow ink when blue, green, and yellow are possible responses) but not on response‐ineligible trials in which the word does not name a possible response (e.g., the word “purple” in yellow ink when blue, green, and yellow are possible responses). In the present study, both congruent and incongruent trials produced increases in ACC activity (relative to neutral), as both trial types introduce task‐irrelevant information that is associated with a valid response.

Some concern may arise as to whether the more generalized pattern of neural activity noted within the posterior division is truly inconsistent with the conflict monitoring theory. At face value, the conflict monitoring theory would predict no increases in ACC activity for congruent trials, as the color word and ink color are in clear agreement. Only if a more broad definition of conflict is employed can these results be viewed as consistent, namely one in which conflict is construed to be any situation in which there are two or more competing sources of information upon which a response can be based. Even with such a definition, a single hypothesis would be unable to explain the differential patterns of activity noted between the anterior and posterior divisions of the rostral zone.

Although the pattern of neural activity of the anterior division is consistent with the conflict‐monitoring hypothesis, an alternative explanation exists. Activity in this subregion may reflect its involvement with the affective processing associated with the occurrence of conflict. Such an assertion would be consistent with models proposing that the more rostral portion of ACC is involved in emotional rather than cognitive processes. This same subregion has been implicated in error‐related processing by both Braver et al. [2001] and Kiehl et al. [2001]. Similar to conflict, error‐related processing has a clear affective component. Findings of increased neural activity within this subregion for both error‐related and conflict‐related processing are thus consistent with a role in affective processing as well [Gehring and Knight, 2000].

In summary, the present study suggests that no one theory may be sufficient to explain ACC's contributions to control, as it seems to be functionally differentiated. Although the anterior division of the rostral zone exhibited a pattern of activity consistent with the conflict‐monitoring theory, the posterior division exhibited a more generalized pattern of activity more compatible with theories that posit a role for ACC in response facilitation/inhibition. Future studies using higher strength magnets (the present study employed a 1.5‐Tesla GE Signa) may show even further sub‐differentiation than our results indicate. At a minimum, our findings emphasize the need to take into account the specific locations within ACC at which changes in activity are observed in studies of ACC function, rather than treating ACC as a whole.

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by the Beckman Institute for Advanced Science and Technology at the University of Illinois (Urbana‐Champaign, Carle Clinic, Urbana, Illinois), NIMH (MD/PhD pre‐doctoral National Research Service Award MH12415‐01 to the principal investigator, Michael P. Milham). This research was carried out in part to fulfill the requirements for the doctoral degree in neurosciences at the University of Illinois at Urbana‐Champaign. We thank S. Colcombe and K. Erickson.

REFERENCES

  1. Banich MT, Milham MP, Atchley R, Cohen NJ, Webb A, Wszalek T, Kramer AF, Liang ZP, Wright A, Shenker J, Magin R ( 2000): fMRI Studies of Stroop tasks reveal unique roles of anterior and posterior brain systems in attentional selection. J Cogn Neurosci 12: 988–1000. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Barch DM, Braver TS, Akbudak E, Conturo T, Ollinger J, Snyder A ( 2001): Anterior cingulate cortex and response conflict: effects of response modality and processing domain. Cereb Cortex 11: 837–848. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Botvinick MM, Cohen JD, Carter CS ( 2004): Conflict monitoring and anterior angulate cortex: an update. Trends Cogn Sci 8: 539–546. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Braver TS, Barch DM, Gray JR, Molfese DL, Snyder A ( 2001): Anterior cingulate cortex and response conflict: effects of frequency, inhibition and errors. Cereb Cortex 11: 825–836. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Carter CS, Braver TS, Barch DM, Botvinick MM, Noll D, Cohen JD ( 1998): Anterior cingulate cortex, error detection, and the online monitoring of performance. Science 280: 747–749. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Carter CS, Macdonald AM, Botvinick M, Ross LL, Stenger VA, Noll D, Cohen JD ( 2000): Parsing executive processes: strategic vs. evaluative functions of the anterior cingulate cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 97: 1944–1948. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Dyer FN ( 1973): Interference and facilitation for color naming with separate bilateral presentations of the word and color. J Exp Psychol 99: 314–317. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Flowers JH, Stoup CM ( 1977): Selective attention between words, shapes and colors in speeded classification and vocalization tasks. Mem Cognit 5: 299–307. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Forman S, Cohen J, Fitzgerald M, Eddy W, Mintun M, Noll D ( 1995): Improved assessment of significant activation in functional MRI: use of a cluster‐size threshold. Magn Reson Med 33: 636–647. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Gehring WJ, Knight RT ( 2000): Prefrontal‐cingulate interactions in action monitoring. Nat Neurosci 3: 516–520. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Glaser MO, Glaser WR ( 1982): Time course analysis of the Stroop Phenomenon. J Exp Psychol 76: 413–418. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Jenkinson M, Smith SM ( 2001): A global optimisation method for robust affine registration of brain images. Med Image Anal 2: 143–156. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Kiehl KA, Liddle PF, Hopfinger JB ( 2000): Error processing and the rostral anterior cingulate: an event‐related fMRI study. Psychophysiology 37: 216–223. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. MacLeod CM ( 1991): Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review. Psychol Bull 109: 163–203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Milham MP, Banich MT, Webb A, Barad V, Cohen NJ, Wszalek T, Kramer AF ( 2001): The relative involvement of anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortex in attentional control depends on nature of conflict. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 12: 467–473. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Milham MP, Erickson KI, Banich MT, Kramer AF, Webb A, Wszalek T, Cohen NJ ( 2002): Attentional control in the aging brain: insights from an fMRI study of the Stroop task. Brain Cogn 49: 277–296. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Mintun M, Fox P, Raichle M ( 1989): Highly accurate method of localizing regions of neuronal activation in the human brain with positron emission tomography. J Cereb Blood Flow Metabol 1: 96–103. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Paus T ( 2001): Primate anterior cingulate cortex: where motor control, drive and cognition interface. Nat Rev Neurosci 2: 417–424. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Paus T, Petrides M, Evans AC, Meyer E ( 1993): Role of the human anterior cingulate cortex in the control of oculomotor, manual, and speech responses: a positron emission tomography study. J Neurophysiol 70: 453–469. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Shafritz KM, Gore JC, Marois R ( 2002): The role of the parietal cortex in visual feature binding. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 16: 10917–10922. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Talairach J, Tovrnox P ( 1988): Co‐planar stereotaxic atlas of the human brain: 3‐D proportional system: an approach to cerebral imaging. Stuttgart: Thieme. [Google Scholar]
  22. Woolrich MW, Ripley BD, Brady JM, Smith SM ( 2001): Temporal autocorrelation in univariate linear modelling of fMRI Data. Neuroimage 14: 1370. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Human Brain Mapping are provided here courtesy of Wiley

RESOURCES