Skip to main content
Human Brain Mapping logoLink to Human Brain Mapping
. 2002 Aug 30;17(2):116–130. doi: 10.1002/hbm.10055

Functional neuroanatomy of sustained attention in schizophrenia: Contribution of parietal cortices

Natalia Ojeda 1, Felipe Ortuño 1,, Javier Arbizu 2, Pilar López 1, Josep Maria Martí‐Climent 2, Ivan Peñuelas 2, Salvador Cervera‐Enguix 1
PMCID: PMC6871970  PMID: 12353245

Abstract

Deficits in sustained attention have been frequently described in schizophrenia. The neuroanatomical basis reported previously have included altered levels of activation in cingulate and prefrontal cortex, but the contribution of further regions remains unclear. We explored the full neuroanatomy underlying the sustained attentional deficits observed in naïve schizophrenics compared with controls. Participants included 10 controls and 11 patients. The experimental design included rest, auditory stimulation using clicks, and two counting tasks. Subjects were instructed to mentally count the clicks, and then to count forward at the same frequency they heard previously when listening to the clicks. Relative cerebral blood flow (relCBF) was measured by means of PET 15O‐water. Differences were observed between both groups at superior temporal cortex, superior parietal gyrus, and cerebellum during tasks requiring listening. During all counting conditions, additionally to supplementary motor area (SMA), dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPCF), precentral gyrus, cingulate, cerebellum, and inferior parietal (IP) gyrus, patients engaged other frontal structures including inferior, medial, and superior frontal areas. When counting with no auditory stimulation (C; requires components of working memory and time estimation), significant differences were observed in the level of activation of frontal and IP regions. Our naïve patients presented abnormal activation of auditory associative pathways. They failed to activate prefrontal and parietal regions at a similar level during tasks requiring increased cognitive effort, and they required a higher activation of inferior frontal regions to properly respond to cognitive demands. Hum. Brain Mapping 17:116–130, 2002. © 2002 Wiley‐Liss, Inc.

Keywords: schizophrenia, counting, auditory processing, attention, PET, working memory, time estimation, parietal lobes

INTRODUCTION

Deficits in attention have been considered essential to schizophrenia since Bleuer [1911] and Kraepelin [1913]. Further contributions have been made to consider attentional difficulties as a key symptom and an underlying mechanism to more complex cognitive deficits in schizophrenia [Levin et al., 1989]. Deficient performance in attentional tasks is a stable, long‐lasting characteristic of the disorder identified either during acute or chronic states, and independent of neuroleptic medication. The exact course and biological bases of these deficiencies, however, remains obscure. An increased interest in the relationship between attentional deficits and vulnerability to schizophrenia has been reported [Asarnow et al., 1978; Braff et al., 1982; Nuechterlein et al., 1984]. Current international classification systems for the diagnosis of schizophrenia do not include specific criteria related to these deficiencies. Structured tools and scales utilized for the clinical evaluation of the severity of the symptoms (i.e., SAPS/SANS, BPRS, PANSS) include items to measure attention.

Sustained attention is most often studied in schizophrenia and requires subjects to minimize distractibility, maintain alertness, and focus attention on stimuli within a sensorial modality. The main interest in sustained attention deficits in patients with schizophrenia is its persistence regardless of treatment. Contrary to other symptoms that clearly seem to improve with medication, sustained attention problems seem to be unchanged after treatment [Sax et al., 1998], suggesting that they could be used as trait marker for the illness [Maruff et al., 1999].

Contemporary conceptualization of cognitive deficits in schizophrenia studies tends to place less emphasis on attentional difficulties as being synonymous of prefrontal cortex dysfunction, while maintaining a focus on the frontal‐subcortical circuits believed to subserve many of these functions [Goldman‐Rakic, 1991; Jones, 1997; Palmer et al., 2000]. As pointed out by Weinberger et al. [1996], although some aspects of schizophrenia clearly relate to frontal dysfunction, this disorder does not seem to be explainable exclusively by a simple model of focal frontal vulnerability.

Neuroimaging researches have employed standard neuropsychological tests of combined stimulation, mainly Continuous Performance Test (CPT) [Buchsbaum et al., 1990; Cohen et al., 1988], to study the anatomofunctional bases of sustained attention in both normal subjects and patients with schizophrenia.

Specific prefrontal regions and cingular gyrus seem to be involved in tasks requiring attention, as well as in monitoring cognitive effort during working memory and other mental processes [Grady et al., 1997; Halpern et al., 1999; Petrides et al., 1993; Zatorre et al., 1994]. The activation observed in these regions seems to be connected not to the particular sensory modality of the stimuli, but to underlying aspects of the task requirements.

The contribution of studies employing PET 15O‐water for assessment of cerebral blood flow is relevant in the clarification of the specific role of brain regions during cognitive performance, especially during attentional tasks. Table I shows a summary of published studies using PET 15O‐water during various cognitive demands in schizophrenia, including attention.

Table I.

PET‐H2 15O studies in basal state and cognitive activation tasks in schizophrenia

Condition Study
Basal state Early et al., 1987
Silversweig et al., 1995
Miller et al., 1997
Andreasen et al., 1997
Epstein et al., 1999
Kim et al., 2000
Activation
 Attention O'Leary et al., 1996
Carter et al., 1997
Holcomb et al., 2000
 Verbal fluency Fletcher et al., 1996
Friston et al., 1996
Dye et al., 1999
Artiges et al., 2000
Spence et al., 2000
Josin et al., 2001
 Memory Andreasen et al., 1996
Ganguli et al., 1997
Wiser et al., 1998
Ragland et al., 1998
Carter et al., 1998
Fletcher et al., 1999
Artiges et al., 2000
Bertolino et al., 2000
Crespo‐Facorro et al., 2001
 Executive function Ragland et al., 1998

Further research will help clarify the specific role of the cingulate region during auditory attention and the additional participation of other brain structures connected with the cingulate or engaged during the same cognitive activities. Neuroimaging studies are consistent overall with the historical interest in the potential role of abnormalities in frontal or frontal‐subcortical circuitry in the genesis of schizophrenia [Palmer et al., 2000]. In this framework we are faced with several questions, including (1) how neuroanatomical bases help to explain attentional deficits; (2) how attentional deficits impact the everyday functioning of patients; and (3) how to develop effective interventions, present or potential, for attentional deficits.

We investigated the underlying neural networks of auditory sustained attention with PET‐water in schizophrenic patients and comparison subjects. We have chosen a single paradigm based on counting auditory stimuli and counting mentally without external stimulation, to assess the neuroanatomical structures involved in sustained attention and working memory aspects.

SUBJECTS AND METHODS

Subjects

The study was approved by the University‐Hospital Clinic of Navarra Ethics Committee Review Board. All subjects gave written informed consent after the procedure was explained fully to them. Eleven patients and 10 normal healthy controls participated. All subjects were right‐handed as determined by the Anett Questionnaire [Anett, 1967] and were in good physical health. Normal auditory capacity of the subjects was evaluated as part of their medical screening to confirm perfect hearing. Candidates with a history of substance abuse, neurological conditions, head trauma with loss of consciousness, diabetes, hypertension and asthma were excluded from the study.

Schizophrenics and controls were matched on age (mean age for patients = 27.55, SD = 9.4; controls = 26.10, SD = 6.95), gender (male/female rate for patients = 10/1; controls = 7/3), and years of formal education (mean years for patients = 12.93 ± 1.90; controls = 14.67 ± 2.71). No differences in estimated IQ (as measured by the Barona Index) were observed between groups.

All patients met DSM‐IV criteria for schizophrenia. Eight schizophrenics were fully naïve, and three patients took medication with a required minimum wash‐out period of 1 month before the study (mean wash‐out period = 3 months). Mean age of illness onset was 17.73 years (SD = 4.7) with a mean duration of 10 years of illness (X = 9.82, SD = 7.8) from first diagnosis to date of study. Schizophrenic symptoms were classified as positive or negative using PANNS Scale [Kay et al., 1987]. Seven patients were classified with positive symptoms and four patients were classified with negative symptoms.

Procedures

There were four task conditions used in this study: (1) auditory stimulation (Task A); (2) counting task with auditory stimulation (Task A+C); (3) counting task with no auditory stimulation (Task C); and (4) a rest condition. Auditory stimulation was carried out using an earphone. Stimulation included a series of non‐filtered clicks, applied binaurally, at 90‐dB intensity and 1‐Hz frequency. An auditory click stimulator (S10CTCM; Grass, West Warwick, RI) was used to generate the clicks.

Task A

Subjects were exposed to repeated auditory clicks provided at a predetermined, consistent rate (1/second).

Task A+C

In addition to the previous condition, subjects were asked to mentally count the number of clicks to which they were exposed. A sustained attention and working memory demand was implied. The rate of serial counting is regulated because of the need to attend to an external auditory stimulus.

Task C

Subjects were asked to mentally count at the same rate at which the clicks were presented in previous conditions. This time no auditory stimulation was provided. During the conditions of counting with and without auditory stimulation, subjects were encouraged to avoid finger and oral movements to facilitate the counting tasks. The ability of a subject to count properly was controlled by an external examiner who asked for and registered the total number of clicks recalled by the subject at the end of each counting condition. Table II shows the results reported by controls and patients for both conditions. Differences between groups were not found to be statistically significant for the Task A+C condition (t = 0.42, P = 0.68) or for the Task C condition (t = −0.59, P = 0.56).

Table II.

Subjects performance mentally counting the clicks

Counting while listening Counting without listening
Actual clicks Subject's report Difference Seconds/Clicks Subject's report Difference
Controls 132.7 ± 35.3 133.6 ± 35.3 −0.9 ± 3.5 117.8 ± 7.2 105.4 ± 27.9 12.3 ± 31
Patients 122.8 ± 14.6 119.7 ± 35.8 3.1 ± 8.4 121.6 ± 8.6 107.6 ± 14.1 14 ± 3.4

* Values are expressed as mean ± SD.

Rest Condition

Subjects lay quietly and residual background noise was kept to a minimum. They kept their eyes open and their gaze was fixed on the ceiling of a dimly lit room. No specific instructions about mental activity were provided in this condition.

PET scan

Each subject was scanned four times in a single PET session, measuring relative cerebral blood flow (relCBF) by means of 15O‐water with standard laboratory procedures.

The scans were performed with an ECAT EXAT HR+ (Siemens/CTI, Knoxville, TN) that collects 63 parallel planes over a 15.2‐cm axial field of view. During all conditions patients were tied and an external observer guaranteed no visually significant movements. To minimize the head movement, a head belt was fastened to the head holder. Transmission scanning was done prior to radiopharmaceutical injection using three rotating rod sources of 68Ge.

Measurements of relCBF were made after an i.v. bolus injection of 10 mCi of H2 15O‐water in 5–7 ml of saline. Scans were initiated automatically when the radioactive count rate in the brain reached a threshold value of 100 Kcounts per second, approximately 20 seconds after i.v. injection, and continued for 60 seconds. Data were acquired and reconstructed in full 3D mode (e.g., septa retracted).

The PET images from each subject were centered (left–right), vertically aligned to correct for movement in the transverse and coronal planes, and co‐registered one to another, to correct for slight head movement during the scan (AIR) [Woods et al., 1992]. All other PET scans collected for one subject were then mathematically registered to the first scan by PET–PET alignment (AIR). These processes centered the images and oriented them in the same coordinate system for later processing.

Data analysis

Statistical analysis of the data was carried out using the Statistical Parametric Mapping program (SPM99) [Friston et al., 1991] in MATLAB (Mathworks, Sherborn, MA). The scans were spatially normalized using linear transformation, which removed individual subject variability and transformed each brain into the Talairach and Tournoux atlas reference space [Tailarach et al., 1988]. The scans were then smoothed at 12 mm with a 3D Gaussian filter to suppress noise and minimize effects of normalization errors by increasing the sensitivity of the signal.

Differences in global activity within and between subjects were removed by proportional scaling on a voxel‐by‐voxel basis with global counts as covariate and regional activity across subjects for each task as treatment effect. Proportional scaling was used for the comparison of tasks, with each subject being studied in all conditions. Inter‐subject variability was treated as a random effect to avoid false positives. Comparisons of the means across selected conditions were made on a voxel‐by‐voxel basis using the t test. The resulting values constituted a statistical parametric map (SPM). The critical level of α was set at 0.001 (uncorrected for multiple comparisons).

SPM analysis of subtractions was carried out for all subjects using a standard subtractive methodology utilized previously in studies of functional neuroimaging and cognition [Petersen et al., 1989; Tracy et al., 2000]. In this methodology, experimental conditions are compared directly. This strategy was chosen because comparisons at rest are difficult to interpret, and activation attributed to Task A+C and Task C conditions needed to exceed the activation in the control conditions (including the Task A condition), regardless of the presence of activation in these conditions compared to a resting baseline. In the case of the Task C condition, subjects were required to count mentally at the rate of clicks they had listened to previously, in the Task A and Task A+C conditions. The activation related to this component should present differences compared to the activation observed during Task A and Task A+C.

RESULTS

Cognitive data

All subjects went through a battery of cognitive tests to examine their neuropsychological performance in terms of attention, working memory, verbal fluency, and executive functions. The selection of these cognitive abilities was based on their participation in the experimental conditions. The measures included the Continuous Performance Test (CPT), the Brief Test of Attention (BTA), Digits (WAIS‐III), Trail Making Test Parts A & B (TMT), FAS, and Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST). As shown in Table III, the general performance of the patients was significantly worse than controls for measures of visual attention (TMT A), shifting, divided attention, working memory (measured by TMTB and BTA), verbal fluency (in letters and categories), and in the number of perseverative errors and responses in the WCST.

Table III.

Summary of cognitive performance and differences between normal controls and naïve patients with schizophrenia

Cognitive function Neuropsychological test Mean performance ± SE Significance
Controls (n = 10) Patients (n = 11)
Attention CPT (reaction time) 420.81 ± 65.4 428.75 ± 84.4 0.821
CPT (global index) 3.711 ± 4.3 4.17 ± 5.5 0.841
BTA (total) 18.4 ± 1.26 15.1 ± 3.93 0.021*
Digits (total), WAIS 20.1 ± 5 16.10 ± 4.7 0.082
Trail Making Test A (seconds) 27.8 ± 8.93 48.9 ± 15.7 0.002*
Trail Making Test A (errors) 0.0 ± 0.0 0.0 ± 0.0 1.000
Trail Making Test B (seconds) 52.10 ± 12.25 111.9 ± 35 0.000*
Trail Making Test B (errors) 0.1 ± 0.32 0.7 ± 1.1 0.103
Verbal fluency FAS (total letters) 50.20 ± 9.93 32.10 ± 10.08 0.001*
FAS (animals) 26.9 ± 3.63 17.70 ± 5.62 0.000*
FAS (supermarket) 25 ± 6.13 16.20 ± 3.91 0.001*
Executive functions WCST (categories completed) 5.5 ± 0.76 4.22 ± 2.22 0.144
WCST (perseverative responses) 9.25 ± 6.73 24.11 ± 14.4 0.018*
WCST (perseverative errors) 8.8 ± 6.33 20.33 ± 11.48 0.025*
WCST (conceptual responses) 60.22 ± 14.2 61.9 ± 5.7 0.104
WCST (conceptual responses) 60.22 ± 14.2 61.9 ± 5.7 0.104
*

P < 0.05.

Task performance

To control the counting accuracy of subjects, at the end of the counting conditions they were asked to report the exact number of auditory stimuli that they had listened to. The examiner controlled the time of exposition and number of stimuli presented using a chronometer (mean range, 90–130 stimuli). Healthy controls and patients did not obtain significantly different performances (P = 0.680) in Task A+C compared to actual number of clicks presented (controls: mean = −0.88, SD = 3.5; patients: mean = 3.10, SD = 26.4), or in Task C (controls: mean = 20.29, SD = 19.25; patients: mean = 12.33, SD = 31.01, P = 0.563).

PET data

Auditory stimulation vs. no auditory stimulation

Activation relative to auditory stimulation (Task A) was measured by a comparison between the experimental conditions including auditory stimulation (Tasks A and A+C) and conditions with no auditory stimulation (Task C and rest). This comparison was made for both patient and control groups. Subtraction was intended to capture all the processes involved in listening to controlled external stimuli. Significant activation was observed in the transverse temporal convolutions (higher in the left hemisphere) including both medial temporal auditory cortex (BA 41), and posterior cingulate (BA 23) for both patients and normal controls independently. Normal controls increasingly activated other cortical association areas, such as the superior temporal cortex (BA 22), left superior parietal gyrus, and right cerebellum. In patients, the pattern of significantly increased brain activity included left middle frontal gyrus (BA 8) and left anterior cingulate (BA 32). When explicit comparison between groups was performed, significant differences in activation were found (increased changes for normal controls) at the level of inferior temporal gyrus (Z = 3.26), middle occipital gyrus (Z = 3.18), and parahippocampal gyrus (Z = 3.11). See Table IV for specific Z values and BA areas.

Table IV.

Brain regions associated with significant relCBF changes related to main effect of listening the clicks in controls, patients, and controls minus patients

Region Controls Patients Controls minus patients
Side BA Talairach coordinates Z‐score Side BA Talairach coordinates Z‐score Side BA Talairach coordinates Z‐score
Frontal
 Medial frontal cortex L 8 −32, 10, 26 3.06
 Posterior cingulate R 31 8, −46, 40 3.8 R 23 6, −24, 22 2.88
 Anterior cingulate L 32 −6, 8, 44 3.15
Temporal
 Transversal temporal gyrus L 41 −38, −28, 12 4.26* L 41 −50, −20, 12 3.63
R 41 36, −20, 0 3.58 L 41 −28, −32, 18 3.02
 Superior temporal gyrus L 22 −58, −42, 12 4.0
L 36 −26, 0, −32 3.34
R 22 60, −20, 8 3.47
 Middle temporal gyrus R 21 70, −40, 4 3.58* R 21 50, −8, −14 3.21
 Inferior temporal gyrus R 20 30, −14, −46 2.77 R 20 −34, −10, −42 3.26
R 20 22, −6, −48 2.7 R 20 −44, −10, −22 2.98
 Parahipocampical gyrus R 28 16, −16, −20 3.11
R 28 14, −26, −18 2.97
Parietal
 Superior parietal gyrus R 7 26, −54, 72 3.72
 Cerebelum L −26, −84, −36 3.18
 Medial occipital gyrus L 19 −42, −74, 0 3.18
*

Regions for which the level of activation was also significant at P ≤ 0.001, corrected for multiple comparisons.

Counting vs. no counting

For the analysis of the activation related to counting, the non‐counting tasks (rest and Task A) were used as control condition for the counting tasks (Task A+C and Task C). When this contrast was performed for each group independently, significant increases of relCBF were observed in SMA, precentral gyrus, bilateral DLPF, cerebellum, anterior cingulate, and inferior parietal, for both normal and schizophrenic participants. When intergroup differences were statistically tested, data showed significant increased activation in normal subjects for cerebellum (Z = 4.04), SMA (Z = 3.94), and precentral gyrus (Z = 3.85). Additionally, patients showed significantly greater activation in bilateral inferior and middle part of the superior frontal gyrus, right middle temporal, and left middle frontal. Activation on right putamen and left inferior temporal gyrus at this condition was only seen for controls and not for patients. Specific values (Z scores) for all brain regions with significant changes in relCBF, their coordinates in standard stereotaxic space, and the corresponding Brodmann areas are displayed in Table V.

Table V.

Brain regions associated with significant relCBF changes related to main effect of counting the clicks in controls, patients, and controls minus patients

Region Controls Patients Controls minus patients
Side BA Talairach coordinates Z‐score Side BA Talairach coordinates Z‐score Side BA Talairach coordinates Z‐score
Frontal
 Precentral gyrus R 4 50, −8, 38 5.13* R 4 52, −6, 38 3.85
R 4 60, 6, 18 4.62*
L 6 −62, 2, 20 4.40* L 6 −48, 4, 12 3.44
 Motor supplementary area L 6 −8, 6, 62 4.40* L 6 −2, 16, 60 4.41* L 6 −10, −22, 68 3.94
R 6 6, −2, 68 4.27* R 6 6, −12, 68 3.51
 Inferior frontal gyrus R 47 54, 40, −12 5.31*
R 44–45 54, 14, 8 5.11*
L 45 −26, 20, 0 4.23
 Anterior cingulate L 32 −12, 14, 46 4.06* R 32 6, 32, 30 4.43*
 Dorsolateral prefrontal gyrus R 9 30, 48, 20 4.18 R 10 38, 54, 2 5.29*
L 9 −36, 50, 26 3.74 L 10 −22, 52, −8 4.32
 Medial frontal gyrus L 6 −46, 6, 50 3.65
 Superior frontal medial gyrus R 8 8, 30, 38 4.52*
Temporal
 Inferior temporal gyrus L 20 −78, −34, −18 3.89
 Middle temporal gyrus R 21 68, −44, 0 5.26*
R 21 68, −28, −20 4.42*
 Superior temporal gyrus L 22 −46, −38, 14 3.33
Parietal
 Inferior parietal gyrus R 40 58, −44, 50 4.06 R 40 58, −48, 42 4.46*
R 40 50, −46, 40 3.61
L 40 −50, −52, 46 3.59 L 40 −48, −46, 48 5.03*
 Supramarginal gyrus R 48 50, −60, 46 3.46
 Precuneus L 17 −32, −72, 18 3.17
Occipital
 Medial occipital gyrus L 19 −52, −76, 0 3.62
Cerebelum L −38, −68, −26 4.26 L −28, −68, −24 3.83 L −46, −82, −20 4.04
L −48, −78, −24 3.68 L −42, −64, −40 3.21
Putamen R 22, 14, 6 4.73
*

Regions for which the level of activation was also significant at P ≤ 0.001, corrected for multiple comparisons.

Counting without auditory stimulation vs. rest

To analyze activation in regions of interest during Task C, this condition was first compared to rest. As for controls, activation for patients with schizophrenia engaged the bilateral dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), left cerebellum, left inferior frontal, and right SMA. No activation was observed on the anterior cingulate or in precentral gyrus for patients, but these regions were significantly activated in controls for this condition. The patient group increasingly engaged bilateral middle temporal gyrus and supramarginal gyrus. When intergroup levels of activation were statistically compared, significant differences were observed at the level of right precentral gyrus (Z = 5.28), bilateral SMA (Z = 3.60) and right DLPFC (Z = 3.60; see Table VI).

Table VI.

Brain regions where counting with no auditory stimulation showed relCBF increases when compared to rest in controls (patients and controls minus patients)

Region Controls Patients Controls minus patients
Side BA Talairach coordinates Z‐score Side BA Talairach coordinates Z‐score Side BA Talairach coordinates Z‐score
Frontal
 Precentral gyrus R 6 48, −8, 30 5.16* R 4 52, −6, 32 5.28
R 4 38, −10, 48 2.87
 Inferior frontal gyrus R 44 60, 10, 20 4.75* R 47 54, 40, −12 5.48*
L 44 −50, 8, 6 3.68* R 45 58, 22, 2 4.63*
L 44 −70, 8, 26 3.57* L 47 −42, 42, −4 3.34
L 44 −64, 4, 20 3.52
 Suplementary motor R 6 6, 4, 70 4.62* R 6 0, 16, 56 3.91 R 6 −2, −16, 56 3.60
R 6 12, −8, 68 3.28
 DLPFC R 46 30, 48, 16 4.48 R 10 40, 52, −4 4.99* R 10 12, 68, 4 3.60
R 9 44, 30, 28 3.80 R 10 30, 58, 0 3.56
L 10–46 −48, 56, 4 3.59 L 9 −38, 32, 32 3.78
L 10 −22, 54, −6 3.39
 Cyngulate gyrus L 32 −10, 12, 46 4.34*
L 32 −4, 8, 50 4.28*
 Medial area of superior frontal gyrus, midline R 6 −8, −2, 62 3.27
Temporal
 Middle temporal gyrus R 21 68, −46, 0 4.5
L 20–21 −66, −28, 24 3.19
 Superior temporal gyrus L 22 −46, −36, 16 3.30
Parietal
 Inferior parietal gyrus R 40 58, −42, 48 4.31* L 40 −48, −46, 46 3.77
46, −46, 38 3.93* R 40 60, −42, 46 3.22
L 40 −42, −52, 40 3.37 58, −50, 40 3.20
 Supramarginal gyrus L 39–40 −50, −60, −36 3.42
R 40 50, −60, 46 3.32
Cerebelum L −38, −68, −28 4.35 L −38, −66, −44 3.25
Putamen R 22, 14, 6 4.89* R 28, 0, −2 4.57
*

Regions for which the level of activation was significant at P ≤ 0.001, corrected for multiple comparisons.

Counting without auditory stimulation vs. counting with auditory stimulation

During Task C relative to Task A+C (counting with auditory stimulation) comparison subjects showed greater relCBF than patients in DLPFC, inferior parietal, cingulate, and SMA. Both groups engaged the precentral gyrus (right hemisphere for controls and left hemisphere for patients). Patients, as well as controls, activated right inferior frontal gyrus and right postcentral gyrus. When the differences between groups were analyzed, statistically significant levels of increased activation were obtained for controls on right DLPFC (Z = 3.76), left anterior cingulate (Z = 3.17), and right inferior parietal (Z = 3.15) (Table VII, Fig. 1).

Table VII.

Brain regions where counting with no auditory stimulation showed higher relCBF increases than counting with auditory stimulation in controls, patients, and controls minus patients

Region Controls Patients Controls minus patients
Side BA Talairach coordinates Z‐score Side BA Talairach coordinates Z‐score Side BA Talairach coordinates Z‐score
Frontal
 Precentral gyrus R 6 52, −6, 30 3.85
R 4 36, −4, 42 3.41 L 4 −64, −4, 30 3.08
 DLPFC R 9 34, 14, 34 3.81 R 9 38, 38, 42 3.76
L 10 −44, 56, 4 3.59 R 10 14, 70, 8 3.58
 Inferior frontal gyrus R 47 52, 28, −18 3.45
R 44 54, 10, 20 3.23
R 47–10 56, 38, −14 3.08
R 44 66, 8, 6 3.01
 Suplementary motor R 6 2, 8, 48 3.05
 Posterior cyngulate gyrus R 23 6, −30, 16 3.15
L 24 −8, 16, 24 3.14
 Anterior cyngulate gyrus L 32 −10, 24, 28 3.17
Parietal
 Inferior parietal gyrus R 40 46, −62, 46 3.51 R 39 46, −70, 34 3.15
50, −44, 36 3.02
Figure 1.

Figure 1

Regions exhibiting significant relCBF increases during counting with auditory stimulation relative to counting with no auditory stimulation in controls.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Regions exhibiting significant relCBF increases during counting with auditory stimulation relative to counting with no auditory stimulation in naïve patients with schizophrenia.

DISCUSSION

Listening

Listening conditions demanded subjects pay attention to an external stimulus repeated at a constant rate of 1 click per second. When considering listening conditions, to analyze the main effect of listening we observed, for both groups, the activation of primary auditory cortex (transversal temporal region; with stress in left side) and medial temporal area. This data is consistent with results obtained from previous studies of functional neuroanatomy that link the transversal temporal and medial regions to auditory processing [Fiez et al., 1996; Holcomb et al., 2000]. The absence of significant differences when the levels of activation are compared between groups suggests a similar pattern of auditory processing for both. Differences between normal controls and patients with schizophrenia have been reported by investigators employing PET during auditory processing, but always during more complex dichotic tasks [O'Leary et al., 1996].

During listening tasks we also found the active participation of cingulate regions for both controls and patients. The relation between cingulate area and attention has a vast history of experimental support [Carter et al., 1997; Janer et al., 1991]. The cingular activation could be understood in terms of the attentional component of the tasks of listening where subjects have to keep track of the clicks during a pre‐established time. For schizophrenics, the activation of both anterior and posterior cingulate adds an extra component of participation (overactivation) of cingulate region and medial frontal that has been reported repeatedly to be depressed in schizophrenia [Carter et al., 1997; Crespo‐Farroco et al., 1999; Holcomb et al., 2000]. Additionally, for normal controls we observed the participation of superior parietal and temporal regions on listening conditions and the absence of such activation for patients during the same conditions.

We suggest that auditory processing is similar for both groups because they require the primary sensorial processing regions. Differences are observed in the associative and heteromodal cortex requiring patients to exhibit more activation of the cingulate and medial‐frontal cortex.

Counting while listening

Our findings suggest that mental serial counting (for both samples) requires the participation of specific frontal regions (e.g., SMA, DLPFC, precentral, and cingulate), cerebellum, and inferior parietal region. These results partially replicate the findings of Cowells et al. [2000] for processes of simple number repetition. One unanticipated result was that while counting, patients with schizophrenia seem to engage additional frontal structures, including the inferior, medial, and superior frontal areas.

No specific cognitive effort is required by the subject when counting while listening to a monotonous and repetitive tone, because we seem to transform serial counting into an automatic type of information processing early in life. Authors have already described the relation observed among precentral, DLPFC, SMA, and counting [McCloskey et al., 1987, 1991]. In the specific case of this automatic processing, however, we observed the additional contribution of inferior and superior frontal regions as well as inferior temporal for both experimental and normal subjects. However, patients failed to significantly activate the superior temporal regions for automatic mental processing whereas there is a contribution of the inferior parietal region for the effective performance of this task.

Control processing vs. automatic processing: neuroanatomical basis

Different contributions by several cerebral regions during controlled and automatic processing have been observed in our study. Our findings underline the differential role played by four specific frontal regions (DLPFC, precentral cortex, SMA, anterior cingulate) and the inferior parietal during automatic and control processing tasks. Our naïve patients failed to activate some prefrontal regions and inferior parietal cortex at a similar level during a task that required additional cognitive effort. This fact cannot be explained by different cognitive performances in brief time estimation and working memory during Task C, as calculated differences between group performances were not significant.

DLPFC

The control component in our study is defined by working memory and time estimation factors. The relation between DLPFC and working memory tasks has been reported previously [Miller, 2000]. The direct relation between the DLPFC and “willed action” (when responses have to be selected by the subject without the help of any external clue) was first described by Petrides et al. [1993] and later confirmed by different authors [Frith et al., 1995; Artiges et al., 2000; Bertolino et al., 2000; Jahanshani et al., 2000; Menon et al., 2000].

The idea of the central executive developed by Baddeley [1986] and the SAS [Shallice, 1988] seems to have much in common with these findings. Far from concluding that the DLPC is the seat of willed action, however, our data seems to suggest the participation of these regions together with other areas, such as cingulate and precentral gyrus. During the counting while listening condition (the rate of serial counting is externally provided), an inferior level of activation of the DLPFC region was observed. One possible explanation for this observation could be that when the cognitive task is no longer novel, i.e., learning has occurred, there may be suppression of attention (or diminished attention) to information that is no longer relevant. We confirm the implication of DLFPC when working memory and increased attention are required in tasks relating to auditory stimuli presented previously. Moreover, during the Task C condition, subjects had no need to process information of any sensorial modality. For that reason, we suggest the participation of DLPFC during non‐routine tasks independent from the sensorial modality in normal subjects, and the significantly minor level of activation in DLPFC for the same tasks in patients with schizophrenia [Callicott et al., 1988]. Previous authors have reported deficiencies in quality responses when patients with schizophrenia had to make decisions with a time interval major to 1 second [Tracy et al., 1998, 2000]. We also confirm the altered participation of the frontal structures in patients with schizophrenia when the cognitive task includes a time estimation component equal to 1 second.

Cingulate

Evidence from results obtained in this study indicate general dismissed cingular activation when patients are compared to controls during all counting conditions. Initially, the lack of cingulate activation for both groups, as obtained from the subtraction of Task A from Task A+C, was not expected. The lack of significant levels of activation after the subtraction do not necessarily mean the complete lack of activation, but could be indicative of similar levels of activation for both samples as observed when data is analyzed separately for each group and condition. Posner [1995] has argued persuasively that anterior cingulate cortex is involved in controlled attention (attention that is deliberately and consciously directed rather than simply being captured by salient events). Tzourio et al. [1997] has reported increased activation of cingular region during auditory attention tasks for normal subjects. Jones et al. [1997] described significant increases in relCBF in the thalamus and anterior cingulate, but not DLPFC, during processing of painful stimulus on normal controls. Similar activation has been reported in studies of focused attention to other somatosensory and visual signals [Pardo et al., 1991]. For that reason, the implied participation of the anterior cingulate could be related to the attentional component itself rather than the sensorial modality. In our study, participation of the anterior cingulate (ACC) in normal subjects was not observed equally in all conditions that required focused attention. Moreover, during the listening condition, normal subjects activated the posterior but not anterior cingular region; on the other hand, anterior (and posterior) cingular activation was observed in the group of patients during the same condition. From these data, there is not enough evidence of the participation of the cingulate area in attentional processes independent of the type of attention required. For example, Corbetta et al. [1991] reported ACC activation during tasks of divided attention, but not selective attention conditions, during a visual task; the absence of ACC activation and increases in relCBF in SMA during automatic responses was also reported. Our observation of increases of SMA and cingular activation during counting and listening seems to support the idea of the participation of both structures in some but not all cognitive tasks requiring attentional effort.

DLPFC and ACC

Paus et al. [1993] suggested that ACC and DLPFC were involved equally in processes requiring response selection. Activity at ACC and DLPFC has been related to cognitive control. Both structures seem to participate when subjects perform a task that requires working memory or when more than one task is demanded at the same time. Although both ACC and DLPFC might be part of what Posner and Petersen [1990] called the “executive attentional system,” it seems to be further evidence of the different participation and dissociation of the ACC or DLPFC. MacDonald et al. [2000] argued the major participation of DLPFC during cognitive control tasks (complex and novel tasks), and the increasingly higher participation of ACC in processes of monitoring cognition. Taken together, the data could suggest that cognitive control is a dynamic process implemented in the brain by a distributed network that involves closely interacting but anatomically dissociable components. Within this system, the DLPFC provides top‐down support of task‐appropriate evaluative processes, indicating when cognitive control needs to be more strongly engaged [MacDonald et al., 2000]. From our results, it seems reasonable to conclude that this control system is altered in schizophrenia.

Frontal structures

Parallel to the dismissed activation of ACC and DLPFC for more demanding conditions, our results also show the overactivation of other frontal structures in patients, above the levels observed for normal controls, particularly in the case of the inferior frontal region. From our results we can conclude a similar pattern of activation between controls and schizophrenic participants for frontal regions during simple cognitive tasks, with the participation of DLPFC, precentral gyrus, inferior and superior regions, and supplementary cortex. During more cognitively complex tasks, however, differences were observed for the participation of frontal regions: significant activation was observed in DLPFC, precentral and SMA for normal controls, whereas significantly superior activation at inferior frontal area was observed in patients. Normal volunteers showed increased activation of DLPFC, precentral, AMS and cingulate regions in response to increased demand. Patients failed to sufficiently activate these structures and seemed to need the compensation and support of other frontal regions, such as inferior frontal, to respond (compensation hypothesis). This data could be suggestive of a differential pattern in the way that patients modulate their brain activity according to task demand. A similar differential pattern of response between controls and patients for control tasks has been reported previously by Holcomb et al. [2000].

Parietal lobes

As reported, patients significantly failed to activate inferior parietal (IP) during cognitively demanding tasks. The participation of parietal regions (posterior area) in cognitive tasks requiring effort was described by Posner and Petersen [1990]. Posner proposed a network between the posterior parietal and medial‐lateral frontal cortex, the “posterior attention system,” which participates when the subject needs focused or conscious attention. It would be reasonable to think that the double task of listening and counting in our experimental paradigm would require a mental effort for subjects with schizophrenia, although it would still be an automatic process for normal subjects. Normal subjects activate IP when a cognitive effort is needed, as in the control processing request in C. Other studies have also reported parietal lobe participation and the link to DLPFC during cognitive tasks that require auditory working memory and control processing [Menon et al., 2000].

Laterality

Overall, our results suggest diminished advantage for the left hemisphere (right hemisphere dominance) in patients for all conditions analyzed. This left–right differential ratio is especially significant in auditory processing conditions when compared to controls, for whom the lateralization to left hemisphere is higher at the listening conditions (auditory attention). Laterality effects in audition were suspected previously [Millner, 1962] in normal subjects and later evidenced through neuroimaging studies in monkeys [Pehune et al., 1996]. Our data add support to functional neuroimaging data published previously [Fallgatter et al., 2000; Higashima et al., 2000; Malaspina et al., 2000] indicating altered (higher) left activation in patients during auditory attention tasks. This imbalance during cognitive activities has led some investigators to hypothesize that schizophrenia may be a hemispheric disorder.

We observed differences in the activation of frontal structures, depending on the cognitive demand, for naïve patients with schizophrenia. These data support the idea of overactivation of specific frontal regions (i.e., cingulate, inferior frontal), parallel to observed deficiencies in activated brain areas in normal subjects. Additionally, we suggest a differential pattern of activation in patients, which includes parietal cortices, during cognitively demanding tasks. Taken together, these data could be indicative not only of hypoactivation of specific brain regions or circuits in schizophrenia, but also of differential and compensatory mechanisms integrated by fronto‐parietal structures.

Acknowledgements

This project was supported by a grant from the Research Board of the University of Navarra (PIUNA) to S.C‐E., and a grant from Department of Education, Universities and Research (Departamento de Educación, Universidades e Investigación) of the Basque Government awarded to N.O‐P.

REFERENCES

  1. Andreasen NC, O'Leary DS, Cizadlo T, Arndt S, Rezai K, Watkis L, Ponto LL, Hichwa RD (1997): Schizophrenia and cognitive dysmetria: a positron emission tomography study of dysfunctional prefrontal‐thalamic‐cerebellar circuitry. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 152: 1576–1585. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Andreasen NC, O'Leary DS, Nopoulos P, Watkis L, Boles‐Ponto LL, Hichwa RD (1997): Hypofrontality in schizophrenia: distributed dysfunctional circuits in neuroleptic‐naïve patients. Lancet 349: 1730–1734. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Annett MA (1970): Classification of hand preference by association analysis. Br J Psychol 61: 303–321. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Artiges E, Salamé P, Recasens C, Poline JB, Attar‐Levy D, Raillere A, Paillere‐Maartinot ML, Danion JM, Martinot JL (2000): Working memory control in patients with schizophrenia: a PET study during a random number generation task. Am J Psychiatry 157: 1517–1519. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Asarnow RF, McCrimmon DJ (1978): Residual performance deficit in clinically remitted schizophrenics: a marker for schizophrenia? J Abnorm Psychol 87: 597–608. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Baddeley AD (1986): Working memory. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  7. Barona A, Reynolds CR, Chastain R (1984): A demographically based index of premorbid intelligence for the WAIS‐R. J Consult Clin Psychol 52: 885–887. [Google Scholar]
  8. Bertolino A, Esposito G, Callicot JH, Mattay VS, Van Horn JD, Frank JA, Berman KF, Weinberger DR (2000): Specific relationship between prefrontal neuronal N‐acetyl aspartate and activation of the working memory cortical network in Schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 157: 26–33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bleuer E (1911): Dementia praecox or the group of schizophrenias. New York: International University Press. [Google Scholar]
  10. Braff DL, Saccuzzo DP (1982): The effect of antipsychotic medication on speed of information processing in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 139: 1127–1130. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Buchsbaum MS, Nuechterkein KH, Haier RJ, Wu J, Sicotte N, Asarnow R, Potkin S, Guich S (1990): Glucose metabolic rate in normals and schizophrenics during the continuous performance test assessed by positron emission tomography. Br J Psychiatry 156: 216–227. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Callicott JH, Ramsey NF Tallent K, Bertolino A, Knable MB, Coppola R, Golberg T, Weinberger DR (1988): Response to training, do patients with schizophrenia differ from healthy control groups subjects in their response to auditory discrimination training? Schizophr Res 36: 171. [Google Scholar]
  13. Carter CS, Mintum M, Nichols T, Cohen JD (1997): Anterior cingulate gyrus dysfunction and selective attention deficits in schizophrenia: [15O]H2O PET study during single‐trial Stroop task performance. Am J Psychiatry 154: 1670–1675. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Cohen RM, Semple WE, Gross M, Holcomb HJ, Dowling SM, Nordahl TE (1988): Functional localization of sustained attention. Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol Behav Neurol 1: 3–20. [Google Scholar]
  15. Corbetta M, Miezin FM, Dobmeyer S, Shulman GL, Petersen SE (1991): Selective and divided attention during visual discriminations of shape, color, and speed: functional anatomy by positron emission tomography. J Neurosci 11: 2383–2402. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Cowell SF, Code C (1998): Thinking nuclear medicine—PET activation. J Nucl Med Technol 26: 17–22. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Cowell SF, Egan GF, Code C, Harasty J, Watson JDG (2000): The functional neuroanatomy of simple calculation and number repetition: a parametric PET activation study. Neuroimage 12: 565–573. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Crespo‐Farroco B, Paradiso S, Andreasen NC, O'Leary DS, Watkins GL, Boles Ponto LL, Hichwa RD (2000): Recalling word list reveals “cognitive dysmetria” in schizophrenia: a positron emission tomography study. Am J Psychiatry 157: 662–663. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Crespo‐Farroco B, Wiser AK, Andreasen NC, O'Leary DS, Watkins GL, and Ponto LL, Hichwa RD (2001): Neural basis of novel and well‐learned recognition memory in schizophrenia: a positron emission tomography study. Hum Brain Mapp 12: 219–231. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Dye SM, Spence SA, Bench CJ, Hirsch S, Stefan MD, Sharma T, Grasby PM (1999): No evidence for left superior temporal dysfunction in asymptomatic schizophrenia and bipolar disorder: PET study of verbal fluency. Br J Psychiatry 175: 367–374. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Early TS, Haller JW, Posner MI, Raichle M (1997): The left striato‐pallidal hyperactivity model of schizophrenia In: David AS, Cutting JC, editors. The neuropsychology of schizophrenia. New York: Psychology Press. [Google Scholar]
  22. Epstein J, Stern E, Silbersweig DA (1999): Mesolimbic activity associated with psychosis in schizophrenia. Ann NY Acad Sci 877: 562–575. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Fallgatter AJ, Strik WK (2000): Reduced frontal functional asymmetry in schizophrenia during a cued continuous performance test assessed with near‐infrared spectroscopy. Schizophr Bull 26: 913–919. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Fiez JA, Raichle ME, Balota DA, Tallal P, Petersen SE (1996): ) PET activation of posterior temporal regions during auditory word presentation and verb generation. Cereb Cortex 6: 1–10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Fletcher PC, Frith CD, Grasby PM, Shallice T, Frackowiak RSJ, Dolan RJ (1996): Local and distributed effects of apomorphine on fronto‐temporal function in acute unmedicated schizophrenia. J Neurosci 16: 7055–7062. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Fletcher PC, McKenna P, Frith CD, Grasby PM, Friston KJ, Dolan RJ (1999): Brain activations in schizophrenia during a graded memory task studied with functional neuroimaging. Arch Gen Psychiatry 55: 1001–1008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Friston KJ, Frith CD, Liddle PF, Frackowiak RS (1991): Comparing functional (PET) images: the assessment of significant change. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 11: 690–699. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Frith CD, Grasby PM (1995): rCBF studies of prefrontal function and their relevance to psychosis In: Boller F, Grafman J, editors. Handbook of neuropsychology Vol. 10 New York: Elsevier Science; p 383–403. [Google Scholar]
  29. Ganguli R, Carter CS, Mintun M, Brar J, Becker J, Sarma R, Nichols T, Bennington E (1997): PET brain mapping study of auditory verbal supraspan memory versus visual fixation in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 41: 33–42. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Golden CJ (1994): STROOP: Test de colores y palabras Versión española adaptada por Editorial TEA. Publicaciones de Psicología Aplicada. Madrid: TEA. [Google Scholar]
  31. Goldman‐Rakic PS (1991): Prefrontal cortical dysfunction in schizophrenia: the relevance of working memory In: Carroll BJ, Barrett JE, editors. Psychopathology and the brain. New York: Raven Press; p 1–23. [Google Scholar]
  32. Grady CL, Meter JWV, Maisog JM, Pietrini P, Krasuski J, Rauschecker JP (1997): Attention‐related modulation of activity in primary and secondary auditory cortex. Neuroreport 8: 2511–2516. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Gur RE, Seymour K, Wilson L, Jolley A, Hirsch S (1988): Impairments of neuropsychological tests of temporohippocampal and frontohippocampal functions and word fluency in remitting schizophrenia and affective disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry 44: 119. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Halpern AR, Zatorre RJ (1999): When that tune runs through your head: a PET investigation of auditory imagery for familiar melodies. Cereb Cortex 9: 697–704. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Higashima M, Kawasaki Y, Urata K, Sakai N, Nagasawa T, Koshino Y, Sumiya H, Tonami N, Tsuji S, Matsuda H (2000): Regional cerebral blood flow in male schizophrenic patients performing an auditory discrimination task. Schizophr Res 42: 29–39. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Holcomb HH, Lahti AC, Medoff DR, Weiler M, Dannals RF, Tamminga CA (2000): Brain activation pattern in schizophrenic and comparison volunteers during a matched‐performance auditory recognition task. Am J Psychiatry 157: 1634–1644. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Jahanshahi M, Dirnberger G, Fuller R, Frith CD (2000): The role of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in random number generation: a study with positron emission tomography. Neuroimage 12: 713–725. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Janer KW, Pardo JV (1991): Deficits in selective attention following bilateral anterior cingulotomy. J Cogn Neurosci 3: 231–241. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Jones E.G. (1997): Cortical development and thalamic pathology in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 23: 483–501. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Josin JM, Liddle PF (2001): Neural network analysis of the pattern of functional connectivity between cerebral areas in schizophrenia. Biol Cybern 84: 117–122. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Kay SR, Fiszbein A, Opler LA (1987): The positive and negative syndrome scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 13: 261–276. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Kim JJ, Mohamed S, Andreasen NC, O'Leary DS, Watkins L, Ponto LLB, Hichwa RD (2000): Regional neural dysfunctions in chronic schizophrenia study by positron emission tomography. Am J Psychiatry 157: 542–548. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Kraepelin E (1913): Dementia Praecox and Paraphrenia. Edinburgh: Livingstone. [Google Scholar]
  44. Levin S, Yurgelun‐Todd D, Craft S (1989): Contribution of clinical neuropsychology to the study of schizophrenia. J Abnorm Psychol 8: 341–356. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. MacDonald AW, Cohen JD, Stenger VA, Carter CS (2000): Dissociating the role of dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortex in cognitive control. Science 288: 1835–1838. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Malaspina D, Bruder G, Furman V, Gorman JM, Berman A, Van Heertum R (2000): Schizophrenia subgroups differing in dichotic listening laterality also differ in neurometabolism and symptomatology. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 12: 485–492. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Maruff P, Pantelis C (1999): Attention and neuropsychiatric disorders. Curr Opin Psychiatry 12: 339–344. [Google Scholar]
  48. McCloskey M, Aliminosa D, Sokol SM (1991): Facts, rules, and procedures in normal calculation: evidence from multiple single‐patient studies of impaired arithmetic fact retrieval. Brain Cogn 17: 154–203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. McCloskey M, Caramazza A (1987): Cognitive mechanism in normal and impaired number processing In: Deloche G, Seron X, editors. Mathematical disabilities: a cognitive neuropsychological perspective. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. [Google Scholar]
  50. Menon V, Anagnoson RT, Mathalon DH, Glover GH, Pfefferbaum A (2000): Functional neuroanatomy of auditory working memory in schizophrenia: relation to positive and negative symptoms. Neuroimage 13: 433–446. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Miller D, Andreasen NC, O'Leary DS, Rezai K, Watkins L, Ponto LLB, Hichwa RD (1997): Effect of antipsychotics on regional cerebral blood flow measured with positron emission tomography. Neuropsychopharmacology 17: 323–324. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Miller EK (2000): The prefrontal cortex and cognitive control. Nat Rev Neurosci 1: 59–65. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Millner BA (1962): Laterality effects in audition In: Mountcastle V, editor. Interhemispheric relations and cerebral dominance. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins Press, p 177–195. [Google Scholar]
  54. Nuetchterlein KH (1977): Reaction time and attention in schizophrenia: a critical evaluation of the data and theories. Schizophr Bull 3: 373–428. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Nuetchterlein KH, Dawson ME (1984): Information processing and attentional functioning in the developmental course of schizophrenic disorders. Schizophr Bull 10: 160–203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. O'Leary DS, Andreasen NC, Hurtig RR, Kesler ML, Rogers M, Arndt S, Cizadlo T, Watkins GL, Boles Ponto LL, Kirchner PT, Hichwa RD (1996): Auditory attentional deficits in patients with schizophrenia: a positron emission tomography study. Arch Gen Psychiatry 53: 633–641. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Palmer BW, Heaton RK (2000): Executive dysfunction in schizophrenia In: Sharma T, Harvey P, editors. Cognition in schizophrenia: impairments, importance and treatment strategies. New York: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  58. Pardo JV, Fox PT, Raichle ME (1991): Localization of a human system for sustained attention by positron emission tomography. Nature 349: 61–64. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Paus T, Petrides M, Evans AC, Meyer E (1993): Role of the human anterior cingulate cortex in the control of oculomotor, manual, and speech responses: a positron emission tomography study. J Neurophysiol 70: 453–469. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Penhune VB, Zatorre RJ, MacDonald JD, Evans AC (1996): Interhemispheric anatomical differences in human primary auditory cortex: probabilistic mapping and volume measurement from magnetic resonance scans. Cereb Cortex 6: 661–672. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Petersen SE, Fox PT, Posner MI, Mintu M, Richel ME (1989): Positron emission tomographic Studies of the processing of single words. J Cogn Neurosci 1: 153–170. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Petrides M, Alivisatos B, Evans AC, Meyer E (1993): Dissociation of human mild‐dorsolateral from posterior dorsolateral frontal cortex in memory processing. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 90: 873–877. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Posner M (1995): Attention in cognitive neuroscience: an overview In: Gazzaniga MS, editor. The cognitive neurosciences. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; p 615–623. [Google Scholar]
  64. Posner MI, Petersen SE (1990): The attentional system of the brain. Ann Rev Neurosci 13: 25–42. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Presenti M, Thioux M, Seron X, De Volder A (2000): Neuroanatomical substrates of Arabic number processing, numerical comparison, and simple addition: a PET study. J Cogn Neurosci 12: 461–479. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Raglan JD, Gur RC, Glahn DC, Censits DM, Smith RJ, Lazarew MG, Alavi A, Gur RE (1998): Frontotemporal cerebral blood flow change during executive and declarative memory tasks in schizophrenia: a positron emission tomography study. Neuropsychology 12: 399–413. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Sax KW, Strakowski SM, Keck PE, McElroy SL, West SA, Stanton SP (1998): Symptom correlates of attention improvement following hospitalization for a first episode of affective psychosis. Biol Psychiatry 44: 784–786. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Schretlen D (1997): Brief test of attention. Odessa, FL: PAR Psychological Resources. [Google Scholar]
  69. Shallice T (1988): From neuropsychology to mental structure. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  70. Silbersweig DA, Stern E, Frith CD, Cahill C, Holmes A, Grootoonk S, Seaward J, McKenna P, Chua SE, Schnorr L (1995): A functional neuroanatomy of hallucinations in schizophrenia. Nature 378: 176–179. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Smith JD (1992): The auditory hallucinations of schizophrenia In: Reisberg D, editor. Auditory imagery. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum; p 151–178. [Google Scholar]
  72. Spence SA, Liddle PF, Stefan MD, Hellewell JS, Sharma T, Friston KJ, Hirsch S, Frith CD, Murray RM, Deakin JF (2000): Functional anatomy of verbal fluency in people with schizophrenia and those at genetic risk: focal dysfunction and distributed disconnectivity reappraised. Br J Psychiatry 176: 61–63. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Tailarach J, Tournoux P (1988): Co‐planar stereotaxic atlas of the human brain. New York: Stuttgart; and Georg Thieme Verlag. [Google Scholar]
  74. Tzourio N, Massioui FE, Joliot M, Renault B, Mazoyer B (1997): Functional anatomy of human auditory attention studied with PET. Neuroimage 5: 63–77. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Tracy JI, Faro SH, Feroze BM, Pinsk M, Pinus A (2000): Functional localization of a “Time Keeper” function separate from attentional resources and task strategy. Neuroimage 11: 228–242. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Tracy JI, Monaco C, McMichael H, Tyson K, Chambliss C, Christensen HL, Celenza MA (1998): Information‐processing characteristics of explicit time estimation by patients with schizophrenia and normal controls. Percept Mot Skills 6: 515–526. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Weinberger DR, Berman KF, Zec RF (1986): Physiological dysfunction of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia. I. Regional cerebral blood flow evidence. Arch Gen Psychiatry 43: 114–124. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Wires AK, Andreasen NC, O'Leary DS, Watkins L, Ponto LLB, Hichwa RD (1998): Dysfunctional cortico‐cerebellar circuits cause “cognitive dysmetria” in schizophrenia. Neuroreport 9: 1895–1899. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Woods RP, Cherry SR, Mazziotta JC (1992): Rapid automated algorithm for aligning and reslicing PET images. J Comput Assist Tomogr 16: 620–633. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Zatorre RJ, Evans AC, Meyer E (1994): Neural mechanisms underlying melodic perception and memory for pitch. J Neurosci 14: 1908–1919. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Zatorre RJ, Samson S (1991): Role of the right temporal neocortex in retention of pitch in auditory short‐term memory. Brain 114: 2403–2417. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Human Brain Mapping are provided here courtesy of Wiley

RESOURCES