Significance
To examine cognitive and noncognitive predictors of success, we conducted a megaanalysis of prospective, longitudinal data on over 10,000 cadets at the US Military Academy at West Point. Cognitive ability was negatively related to physical ability and grit. While cognitive ability predicted academic and military grades, the noncognitive attributes of physical ability and grit were more prognostic of other achievement outcomes, including successful completion of initiation training and 4-y graduation.
Keywords: cognitive ability, grit, achievement, graduation, military
Abstract
When predicting success, how important are personal attributes other than cognitive ability? To address this question, we capitalized on a full decade of prospective, longitudinal data from n = 11,258 cadets entering training at the US Military Academy at West Point. Prior to training, cognitive ability was negatively correlated with both physical ability and grit. Cognitive ability emerged as the strongest predictor of academic and military grades, but noncognitive attributes were more prognostic of other achievement outcomes, including successful completion of initiation training and 4-y graduation. We conclude that noncognitive aspects of human capital deserve greater attention from both scientists and practitioners interested in predicting real-world success.
Apart from luck and circumstance, what determines the achievement of personally meaningful goals? One hundred and fifty years ago, Galton (1) conjectured that achievement required intelligence as well as “zeal and the capacity for hard labour” (ref. 1, p. 38). In personal correspondence, Darwin (2) agreed that cognitive ability must matter but added, “I have always maintained that, excepting fools, men did not differ much in intellect, only in zeal and hard work; and I still think this is an eminently important difference” (ref. 2, p. 530).
Subsequent theorizing about the psychology of achievement likewise contrasted cognitive ability with noncognitive factors (3–8). Cox (9), for example, suggested that, in addition to intelligence, unusually successful individuals are characterized by the “tendency not to abandon tasks in the face of changeability” and the “tendency not to abandon tasks in the face of obstacles” (ref. 9, p. 174). Likewise, Wechsler (10) distinguished between “intellective” and “non-intellective” factors.
Nevertheless, the past century of empirical research on human accomplishment has disproportionately focused on cognitive ability. In the military, for example, where predicting success holds potentially life or death consequences, the past century of “research was locked in a paradigm that focused on cognitive tests…” (ref. 11, p. 245).* Why? Perhaps because psychologists figured out how to measure cognitive ability earlier, more reliably, and more precisely than most noncognitive attributes (15). It may also be that scholars are drawn to the study of capacities that they themselves exemplify.
The scientific literature is particularly lacking in high-quality, longitudinal studies that assess both cognitive and noncognitive attributes before objectively gauging success outcomes weeks, months, and years later (16). In this investigation, we capitalize on data from more than 10,000 women and men who, on entry to the US Military Academy at West Point, completed measures of grit, cognitive ability, and physical ability and were followed longitudinally through graduation.
Cognitive and Noncognitive Attributes
Cognitive ability is broadly defined as the “ability to reason, plan, solve problems, think abstractly, comprehend complex ideas, learn quickly, and learn from experience” (ref. 17, p. 13). No construct in psychological science has a longer history, and it is now widely accepted that cognitive ability encompasses aptitudes that are hierarchically organized (e.g., math and verbal ability are distinct but also load on a common factor) and prognostic of many forms of academic and professional success (18).
Research on aptitudes other than cognitive ability is comparatively rare (19). Physical ability, for example, is rarely measured in studies of success (20). The scant literature on physical ability suggests that, like cognitive ability, it is hierarchically organized (e.g., comprising distinct but highly correlated components, such as agility, strength, and speed) (21). To date, little is known about the importance of physical ability, even in the military, a profession in which physical challenges are central to both training and active combat (11).
Grit was recently introduced to the scientific literature on human achievement as a facet of Big Five conscientiousness (22). Defined as passion and perseverance for long-term goals of personal significance, grit is a hierarchically organized trait aligned with Galton’s (1) notions of zeal and hard labor and what Cox described as the tendencies not to abandon tasks in the face of either changeability or obstacles (3). Grit seems to be independent of cognitive ability (22, 23) and has demonstrated incremental predictive validity, over and above cognitive ability, for achievement (22, 24, 25), particularly for goals of personal significance (26).
West Point
The oldest military academy in the United States, West Point offers a unique opportunity to study personal attributes that predict success. Each year, about 14,000 young women and men begin an admissions process that includes a standardized test of physical ability and standardized academic achievement tests. On arrival at West Point, each class of ∼1,200 cadets completes a variety of self-report questionnaires designed by researchers within and outside the academy.
What follows is a 6-wk initiation training nicknamed Beast Barracks. This experience has been described in the handbook for new cadets as “the most physically and emotionally demanding part of the 4 years at West Point…designed to help you make the transition from New Cadet to Soldier” (27). As one cadet put it, “You are challenged in a variety of ways in every developmental area—mentally, physically, militarily, and socially. The system will find your weaknesses, but that is the point—West Point toughens you” (27).
Whether cadets excel at or barely survive Beast Barracks, all are given a fresh start come fall, when they begin earning military, academic, and physical grades (each calculated as a separate grade point average [GPA]) during their subsequent 4 y of West Point training. Cadets who complete all requirements during this period graduate from the academy and go on to serve a minimum of 5 y in the US Army.
This Investigation
To address foundational questions about the respective roles of cognitive and noncognitive predictors of success, we conducted a megaanalysis of data from 9 separate cohorts of cadets entering training at West Point over a full decade. Several affordances of these data are worth noting. First, the internal validity of our conclusions is strengthened by the prospective and longitudinal design of the dataset, objectively measured success outcomes from official records, and a rich set of demographic covariates. In contrast, metaanalyses on this topic have by necessity relied on studies that vary in quality and disproportionately, are cross-sectional in design.
Second, another advantage concerns ecological validity. Put simply, the outcomes examined in this investigation are consequential in a way that laboratory measures of success are not (26, 28). We can assume that West Point cadets care a great deal about how they fare at an institution where the admissions process lasts 2 y and graduation leads to a 5-y commitment to active duty in the military. Indeed, prior research confirms that cadets are strongly motivated for both internal and instrumental reasons to succeed at West Point (29). We cannot plausibly do the same with, say, undergraduate volunteers solving puzzles in a research laboratory.
Third, our sample is 50 times larger than the average sample of studies published in high-impact psychology journals (30, 31). The statistical power of this investigation is sufficient to detect both curvilinear and interaction effects—analyses that are notoriously underpowered for the vast majority of datasets collected in social science yet clearly essential for a nuanced understanding of the mechanics of human achievement (32).
The West Point Exempt Determination Officer reviewed this research protocol and determined that it is human subject research according to 32CFR219 and meets the requirements of exempt status under 32CFR219.101(b)(2). While the study was determined to be exempt, all participants were informed and consented to participate in the data collection efforts. All study data are stored securely at West Point and were fully deidentified prior to use or external sharing.
Results
Our analytic approach was as follows. For the 2 binary outcomes of Beast Barracks completion and 4-y graduation, we standardized all predictors prior to fitting binary logistic regression models. For the 3 continuous measures of military, academic, and physical GPAs, we fit ordinary least squares regression models. To adjust for multiple comparisons (i.e., 5 outcome variables) in this investigation, we used a 2-tailed P value of 0.01 as our threshold for statistical significance. Following recommendations by Ganzach (33), we included quadratic terms for curvilinearity as well as 2-way interactions. In cases of statistically significant quadratic terms, we followed Gillespie et al. (34) in determining inflection points (i.e., values of the predictor where the minimum or maximum occurs as well as the value of the predictor where the curvilinear shape indicates concavity).
Following recommendations by Duncan et al. (35), we completed a series of robustness checks, which are summarized in SI Appendix. For example, as shown in SI Appendix, Tables S1–S5, final models using the full sample were generally consistent across individual cohorts. Likewise, as shown in SI Appendix, Tables S7–S11, conclusions were similar whether pretraining predictors were entered separately, whether passion and perseverance subscores were entered as separate predictors, or when grit was operationalized using the 8 items in a short-form version of the Grit Scale (36).
Cognitive and Noncognitive Attributes Are Distinct.
Cognitive ability was negatively related to both physical ability (r = −0.05, P < 0.001) and grit (r = −0.07, P < 0.001), which were, in turn, positively related to each other (r = 0.07, P < 0.001). Differential relations with demographic factors underscored the independence of these 3 predictors. For example, female cadets scored lower than male cadets in both cognitive ability (d = −0.21, P < 0.001) and physical ability (d = −0.25, P < 0.001) but scored higher on grit (d = 0.07, P = 0.006). Additionally, whereas the average cognitive ability of incoming cadets increased slightly (r = 0.05, P < 0.001) with cohort year, their grit (r = 0.02, P = 0.016) and physical ability (r < 0.01, P = 0.99) did not. SI Appendix, Table S6 shows descriptive statistics and bivariate correlations.
Quitting Early: Only Grit Predicts Completing Beast Barracks.
Across cohorts, an average of 3 of every 100 cadets dropped out of West Point during the first summer of training. As shown in Fig. 1 and model 1 of SI Appendix, Table S7, cadets 1 SD higher than their peers in grit had 54% greater odds of completing Beast Barracks training (odds ratio [OR] = 1.54, P < 0.001, 99% confidence interval [99% CI]: 1.31 to 1.81). In contrast, cognitive ability (OR = 1.06, P = 0.33, 99% CI: 0.91 to 1.24) and physical ability (OR = 1.08, P = 0.14, 99% CI: 0.94 to 1.25) did not reliably predict retention during this period.
There was no evidence of curvilinear effects (model 1 of SI Appendix, Table S7), 2-way interactions among pretraining predictors (model 5 of SI Appendix, Table S7), or 2-way interactions between pretraining predictors and demographic factors (models 9 to 11 of SI Appendix, Table S7). SI Appendix, Table S1 shows that, although a higher proportion of cadets completed Beast Barracks in later vs. earlier years in our sample, the predictive validity of pretraining predictors did not differ meaningfully across cohort years.
In summary, grittier—but not necessarily more cognitively or physically able—cadets were more likely to complete Beast Barracks and then continue on to their 4 y of West Point training.
Doing Well: Cognitive and Physical Abilities Best Predict Performance.
After Beast Barracks, West Point uses coursework and standardized assessments to continuously evaluate the military, academic, and physical performance of cadets (Fig. 2 and model 1 of SI Appendix, Tables S8–S10).
Cognitive ability was an especially strong predictor of academic GPA (β = 0.41, P < 0.001, 99% CI: 0.39 to 0.44) and also, the best pretraining predictor of military GPA (β = 0.16, P < 0.001, 99% CI: 0.13 to 0.19). The relationship between cognitive ability and physical GPA was more modest: (β = 0.07, P < 0.001, 99% CI: 0.05 to 0.10).
Likewise, physical ability assessed prior to training was the best measured predictor of physical GPA (β = 0.36, P < 0.001, 99% CI: 0.34 to 0.49) and also predicted military (β = 0.12, P < 0.001, 99% CI: 0.10 to 0.15) and academic (β = 0.07, P < 0.001, 99% CI: 0.05 to 0.09) GPAs.
By comparison, grit predicted a smaller proportion of variance in military (β = 0.10, P < 0.001, 99% CI: 0.08 to 0.13), academic (β = 0.07, P < 0.001, 99% CI: 0.04 to 0.09), and physical (β = 0.06, P < 0.001, 99% CI: 0.04 to 0.08) GPAs.
The handful of reliable quadratic effects observed in these models generally indicated declining marginal returns rather than concave relationships (model 1 of SI Appendix, Tables S8–S10). The benefits of grit and abilities did not depend on each other (model 5 of SI Appendix, Tables S8–S10) or on demographic factors (models 9 to 11 of SI Appendix, Tables S8–S10).
Finishing What You Begin: Grit and Physical Ability Best Predict 4-y Graduation.
Across cohorts, about 81 of every 100 cadets graduated from West Point. As shown in Fig. 3 and model 1 of SI Appendix, Table S11, both grit (OR = 1.18, P < 0.001, 99% CI: 1.10 to 1.26) and physical ability (OR = 1.18, P < 0.001, 99% CI: 1.10 to 1.26) were better than cognitive ability (OR = 1.08, P = 0.011, 99% CI: 0.99 to 1.16) at predicting graduation. Interactions among these pretraining variables were not significant.
Unlike models predicting other success outcomes, models predicting 4-y graduation showed concave curvilinear effects for both cognitive ability (OR = 0.95, P = 0.008, 99% CI: 0.91 to 0.99) and physical ability (OR = 0.89, P < 0.001, 99% CI: 0.85 to 0.93). Specifically, increases in physical ability beyond the 89th percentile predicted a decrease in the odds of graduation. Similarly, increases in cognitive ability beyond the 95th percentile predicted a decrease in the odds of graduation. For grit, although the quadratic term was significant (OR = 0.94, P < 0.001, 99% CI: 0.90 to 0.99), only increases beyond the 99th percentile predicted a decrease in the odds of graduation. Notably, these inverse U relationships were not symmetric—the odds of graduating were clearly lowest for cadets who entered West Point with the lowest levels of grit, cognitive ability, and physical ability.
Discussion
This investigation underscores the independent roles of cognitive and noncognitive determinants of achievement. In data collected on over 10,000 West Point cadets for a full decade, cognitive ability emerged as the best pretraining predictor of academic and military grades, and physical ability proved the best pretraining predictor of physical performance. Grit, in contrast, contributed only modestly to academic, physical, and military performance but was the only reliable predictor of completing Beast Barracks, the initial summer training during which attrition from the academy peaks. Finally, compared with cognitive ability, both grit and physical ability were more prognostic of graduation.
It is worth noting that, by far, the largest effect sizes were observed for the prediction of academic and physical performance by cognitive and physical ability, respectively. While affirming the benefits of an agile mind, this finding also suggests the need to assess a more expansive array of domain-relevant abilities (e.g., military acumen, artistic talent, emotional intelligence) than is typical in research and practice.
Why did noncognitive factors do a better job of predicting surviving Beast Barracks and ultimately, graduating from West Point? A cadet in the Class of 1854 who later achieved the rank of general observed: “For one to succeed here, all that is required is an ordinary mind and application; the latter is by far the most important and desirable of the two. For men of rather obtuse intellect, by indomitable perseverance, have been known to graduate with honor; while some of the greatest geniuses of the country have been found deficient, for want of application” (ref. 37, p. xiv). More recently, one West Point professor observed: “The real reason for most dropouts is the very nature of the selection process. Every new cadet is among the cream of the crop at his high school…Some can cope with this realization and others cannot” (38). These informal observations align with journalistic accounts of the rigors of West Point (39), but the psychological processes underlying attrition require further study.
To our surprise, we identified no reliable interactions among grit, cognitive ability, and physical ability in any of our predictive models. In theory, cognitive and noncognitive attributes could confer a multiplicative advantage (40–42). Logic suggests that cadets who are both especially intelligent and especially gritty, for example, may excel far beyond their peers. Nevertheless, our findings are consistent with null findings in much smaller datasets examining interactions between cognitive ability and either motivation (43, 44) or physical ability (45). Why? It is certainly possible that, in a more heterogeneous sample, evidence of complementarity would have emerged. However, it may be that inherent complementarities are negated by a countervailing dynamic. Perhaps most individuals approach achievement by satisficing (i.e., aiming to reach a threshold level of achievement beyond which additional achievement holds little to no value) (46). If so, following cadets longitudinally to predict who reaches the very highest echelons of military leadership (e.g., the rank of general) would provide a more informative test of dependencies among cognitive and noncognitive attributes.
Another finding is that, at the very highest levels of cognitive and physical ability, cadets were less likely to graduate from West Point. Theoretical arguments have been advanced that all virtues are detrimental at both extremes (47, 48). To date, empirical evidence for these suppositions is relatively sparse, in part because of the requirement of extreme values and hence, large datasets. For example, cognitive ability has been found in some samples to be linearly related to academic achievement (49, 50) but among the very brightest, concavely related to both leadership (51) and mental health (52). Despite the enormous statistical power of the this investigation, we found concave curvilinear effects for only one—although arguably, the most important—measure of success: graduation. Whereas deficits of ability clearly impede success, benefits at the uppermost echelons are less clear, and in fact, it may be that individuals with “everything going for them” get up and go elsewhere. This explanation is consistent with laboratory research showing that having multiple alternatives for achieving one’s goals can diminish commitment to any particular path (53).
Several limitations of this investigation are worth highlighting. First, the generalizability of our findings cannot be assumed. We look forward to conceptual replications outside of the military, in less structured settings, and with less select samples of women and men. Second, the longitudinal data presented here are correlational rather than experimental, thereby limiting inferences to the prediction, rather than causes, of success. Third, another limitation concerns our reliance on available measures, namely those that were administered by West Point repeatedly over a decade. Future research should explore a broader set of cognitive attributes (e.g., spatial ability) and noncognitive attributes (e.g., Big Five personality traits). Fourth, additional longitudinal research—ideally using repeated measures—would help unpack the mechanisms by which cognitive and noncognitive factors lead to success.
The question of what predicts success is among the most basic in social science. The unique affordances of this longitudinal investigation suggest that both cognitive and noncognitive attributes matter in different ways and at different times. Cognitive and physical abilities each enable progress toward goals in their respective domains. In contrast, grit seems to enable individuals to keep going when the going gets tough. Also, finally, the noncognitive factors of grit and physical ability in this case were more prognostic of the ultimate goal of completing a long-term goal of personal consequence.
We note that our models did not account for the preponderance of variance in objectively measured outcomes. For instance, our battery of cognitive and noncognitive attributes as well as a rich set of demographic covariates collectively explained less than 4% of variance in graduation from West Point 4 y after entry. Why? It is likely that a more comprehensive approach to assessing personal attributes (e.g., a multimethod battery of grit measures or a larger battery of cognitive and physical tests) and a more heterogeneous sample would have yielded higher predictive validities than those reported here. Certainly, our models would have been more powerful if we had included a broader set of personal attributes. However, we suspect that the noise in our predictive models—and so many empirical attempts before ours—hints at something deeper.† There may be severe limits to how well any set of personal attributes can forecast an individual’s destiny. People change. Contexts change. And life trajectories are shaped by the whims of chance and path dependency (57, 58).
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
This research was made possible by the generous support of the National Institute on Aging (Grant R24-AG048081-01), Vikki and Michael Price, the Walton Family Foundation, and the John Templeton Foundation. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of funding agencies. We gratefully thank Steve Few and Michelle Shih for invaluable assistance in graphics and editorial support.
Footnotes
The authors declare no competing interest.
This article is a PNAS Direct Submission.
*One notable exception was Project A, which aimed to identify both cognitive and noncognitive correlates of performance among entry-level personnel in the US Army (12). In cross-sectional analyses, cognitive abilities proved especially predictive of “can do” aspects of job performance, including core technical and general soldiering proficiency (13). In contrast, a noncognitive composite of self-reported self-esteem, work orientation, energy level, conscientiousness, nondelinquency, emotional stability, and physical condition proved especially predictive of “will do” aspects of job performance, including ratings of effort and leadership, personal discipline, and physical fitness and military bearing. More recently, a composite of self-reported dominance, sociability, attention seeking, selflessness, cooperation, achievement, order, self-control, nondelinquency, adjustment, even tempered, optimism, intellectual efficiency, tolerance, and physical conditioning predicted attrition of soldiers from entry-level positions 18 mo later (14).
†Consider, for example, the magnitude of predictive validities reported in Moffitt et al. (54), which used a multimethod approach to assess the predictive validity of self-regulation, cognitive ability, and background socioeconomic status on outcomes in a birth cohort. More comprehensive reviews are in Almlund et al. (55) and Smithers et al. (16). The work by Funder and Ozer (56) has an illuminating discussion on credible effect sizes in psychological research.
Data deposition: The preregistration plan of the project can be found on AsPredicted (https://aspredicted.org/wt6vs.pdf). The data and web appendix can be found on Open Science Framework (https://osf.io/yx3vg/).
This article contains supporting information online at www.pnas.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1910510116/-/DCSupplemental.
References
- 1.Galton F., Hereditary Genius: An Inquiry Into Its Laws and Consequences (Macmillan and Co., 1869). [Google Scholar]
- 2.Darwin C., The Correspondence of Charles Darwin (Cambridge University Press, 1869). [Google Scholar]
- 3.James W., The energies of men. Science 25, 321–332 (1907). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Binet A., Simon T., The Development of Intelligence in Children: The Binet-Simon Scale, Kite E. S., Ed. (trans.) (Williams & Wilkins Co., 1916). [Google Scholar]
- 5.Cattell R. B., Description and Measurement of Personality (World Book Company, 1946). [Google Scholar]
- 6.Messick S., Potential uses of noncognitive measurement in education. J. Educ. Psychol. 71, 281–292 (1979). [Google Scholar]
- 7.Jencks C., et al. , Who Gets Ahead? The Determinants of Economic Success in America (Basic Books, 1979). [Google Scholar]
- 8.Heckman J., Humphries J. E., Kautz T., The Myth of Achievement Tests: The GED and the Role of Character in American Life (The University of Chicago Press, 2014). [Google Scholar]
- 9.Cox C. M., Genetic Studies of Genius: The Early Mental Traits of Three Hundred Geniuses (Stanford University Press, 1926). [Google Scholar]
- 10.Wechsler D., Non-intellective factors in general intelligence. J. Abnorm. Soc. Psychol. 38, 101–103 (1943). [Google Scholar]
- 11.Rumsey M. G., Arabian J. M., Military enlistment selection and classification: Moving forward. Mil. Psychol. 26, 221–251 (2014). [Google Scholar]
- 12.Campbell J. P., “Modeling the performance prediction problem in industrial and organizational psychology” in Handbook of Industrial and Organizational Psychology, Dunnette M. D., Hough L. M., Eds. (Consulting Psychologists Press, 1990), vol. 1, pp. 687–732. [Google Scholar]
- 13.McHenry J. J., et al. , Project A validity results: The relationship between predictor and criterion domains. Person. Psychol. 43, 335–354 (1990). [Google Scholar]
- 14.White L. A., Rumsey M. G., Mullins H. M., Nye C. D., LaPort K. A., Toward a new attrition screening paradigm: Latest army advances. Mil. Psychol. 26, 138–152 (2014). [Google Scholar]
- 15.Duckworth A. L., Yeager D. S., Measurement matters: Assessing personal qualities other than cognitive ability for educational purposes. Educ. Res. 44, 237–251 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Smithers L. G., et al. , A systematic review and meta-analysis of effects of early life non-cognitive skills on academic, psychosocial, cognitive and health outcomes. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2, 867–880 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Gottfredson L. S., Mainstream science on intelligence: An editorial with 52 signatories, history, and bibliography. Intelligence 24, 13–23 (1997). [Google Scholar]
- 18.Deary I. J., Intelligence. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 63, 453–482 (2012). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Gardner H., Frames of Mind: The Theory of Multiple Intelligences (Basic Books, 1983). [Google Scholar]
- 20.Persson R. S., Editorial. High. Abil. Stud. 13, 5–6 (2002). [Google Scholar]
- 21.Highmore G., A factorial analysis of athletic ability. Res. Quart. Amer. Assoc. Health Phys. Educ. Recreat. 27, 1–11 (1956). [Google Scholar]
- 22.Duckworth A. L., Peterson C., Matthews M. D., Kelly D. R., Grit: Perseverance and passion for long-term goals. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 92, 1087–1101 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Credé M., Tynan M. C., Harms P. D., Much ado about grit: A meta-analytic synthesis of the grit literature. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 113, 492–511 (2017). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Eskreis-Winkler L., Shulman E. P., Beal S. A., Duckworth A. L., The grit effect: Predicting retention in the military, the workplace, school and marriage. Front. Psychol. 5, 36 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Akos P., Kretchmar J., Investigating grit at a non-cognitive predictor of college success. Rev. Higher Educ. 40, 163–186 (2017). [Google Scholar]
- 26.Jachimowicz J. M., Wihler A., Bailey E. R., Galinsky A. D., Why grit requires perseverance and passion to positively predict performance. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 115, 9980–9985 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.U. S. Military Academy , Information for new cadets and parents class of 2011. http://cdn2.hubspot.net/hub/87149/file-15800743-pdf/docs/westpoint.cadetpay.01-27-12.pdf?t=1525825208709. Accessed 1 June 2019.
- 28.Duckworth A., Gross J. J., Self-control and grit: Related but separable determinants of success. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 23, 319–325 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Wrzesniewski A., et al. , Multiple types of motives don’t multiply the motivation of West Point cadets. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 111, 10990–10995 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Fraley R. C., Vazire S., The N-pact factor: Evaluating the quality of empirical journals with respect to sample size and statistical power. PLoS One 9, e109019 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Marszalek J. M., Barber C., Kohlhart J., Holmes C. B., Sample size in psychological research over the past 30 years. Percept. Mot. Skills 112, 331–348 (2011). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Whisman M. A., McClelland G. H., Designing, testing, and interpreting interactions and moderator effects in family research. J. Fam. Psychol. 19, 111–120 (2005). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Ganzach Y., Misleading interaction and curvilinear terms. Psychol. Methods 2, 235–247 (1997). [Google Scholar]
- 34.Gillespie B. W., Halpern M. T., Warner K. E., “Patterns of lung cancer risk in ex-smokers” in Case Studies in Biometry, Lange N., et al., Eds. (John Wiley & Sons, Inc., 1994). [Google Scholar]
- 35.Duncan G. J., Engel M., Claessens A., Dowsett C. J., Replication and robustness in developmental research. Dev. Psychol. 50, 2417–2425 (2014). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Duckworth A. L., Quinn P. D., Development and validation of the short grit scale (grit-s). J. Pers. Assess. 91, 166–174 (2009). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Robbins J., Last in Their Class: Custer, Pickett and the Goats of West Point (Encounter Books, 2017). [Google Scholar]
- 38.Brown J., What Are Some Reasons Students Drop Out of the US Military Academies? Is It Because the Coursework Is Too Rigorous? Do They Not Like the Structure? (Quora, 2016). [Google Scholar]
- 39.Winerip M., The beauty of Beast Barracks. NY Times Magazine (12 October 1997), pp. 46–53, 52-65. [Google Scholar]
- 40.O’Reilly C., Chatman J. A., Working smarter and harder: A longitudinal study of managerial success. Adm. Sci. Q. 39, 603–627 (1994). [Google Scholar]
- 41.Duckworth A. L., Eichstaedt J. C., Ungar L. H., The mechanics of human achievement. Soc. Personal. Psychol. Compass 9, 359–369 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Vaci N., et al. , The joint influence of intelligence and practice on skill development throughout the life span. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 116, 18363–18369 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Mount M. K., Barrick M. R., Strauss J. P., The joint relationship of conscientiousness and ability with performance: Test of the interaction hypothesis. J. Manage. 25, 707–721 (1999). [Google Scholar]
- 44.Van Iddekinge C. H., Aguinis H., Mackey J. D., DeOrtentiis P. S., A meta-analysis of the interactive, additive, and relative effects of cognitive ability and motivation on performance. J. Manage. 44, 249–279 (2018). [Google Scholar]
- 45.Henderson N. D., Predicting long-term firefighter performance from cognitive and physical ability measures. Person. Psychol. 63, 999–1039 (2010). [Google Scholar]
- 46.Simon H. A., Models of Bounded Rationality: Economic Analysis and Public Policy (The MIT Press, 1982). [Google Scholar]
- 47.Grant A. M., Schwartz B., Too much of a good thing: The challenge and opportunity of the inverted U. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 6, 61–76 (2011). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Aristotle, Nicomachean Ethics, T. Irwin, Trans (Hackett Publishing Company, 2000).
- 49.Guez A., Peyre H., Le Cam M., Gauvrit N., Ramus F., Are high-IQ students more at risk of school failure? Intelligence 71, 32–40 (2018). [Google Scholar]
- 50.Jensen A. R., The G Factor: The Science of Mental Ability (Praeger Publishers/Greenwood Publishing Group, 1998). [Google Scholar]
- 51.Antonakis J., House R. J., Simonton D. K., Can super smart leaders suffer from too much of a good thing? The curvilinear effect of intelligence on perceived leadership behavior. J. Appl. Psychol. 102, 1003–1021 (2017). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Karpinski R. I., Kolb A. M. K., Tetreault N. A., Borowski T. B., High intelligence: A risk factor for psychological and physiological overexcitabilities. Intelligence 66, 8–23 (2018). [Google Scholar]
- 53.Shah J. Y., Kruglanski A. W., “Aspects of goal networks: Implications for self-regulation” in Handbook of Self-Regulation, Boekaerts M., Pintrich P. R., Zeidner M., Eds. (Academic Press, 2000), pp. 85–110. [Google Scholar]
- 54.Moffitt T. E., et al. , A gradient of childhood self-control predicts health, wealth, and public safety. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 2693–2698 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Almlund M., Duckworth A. L., Heckman J., Kautz T., “Personality psychology and economics” in Handbook of the Economics of Education, Hanushek E. A., Machin S., Woessmann L., Eds. (Elsevier, 2011), pp. 1–182. [Google Scholar]
- 56.Funder D. C., Ozer D. J., Evaluating effect size in psychological research: Sense and nonsense. Adv. Meth. Pract. Psychol. Sci. 2, 156–168 (2019). [Google Scholar]
- 57.Pluchino A., Biondo A. E., Rapisarda A., Talent vs luck: The role of randomness in success and failure. Adv. Complex Syst. 21, (2018). [Google Scholar]
- 58.Bandura A., The psychology of chance encounters and life paths. Am. Psychol. 37, 747–755 (1982). [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.