Skip to main content
International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research logoLink to International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research
. 2013 Dec 27;23(1):62–68. doi: 10.1002/mpr.1427

Dimensions of psychotic experiences among women in the general population

Sebastian Therman 1, Jaana Suvisaari 1, Christina M Hultman 2
PMCID: PMC6878595  PMID: 24375586

Abstract

Full‐information factor analysis of ordinal data was employed to determine the factorial structure of the responses of 31,822 adult Swedish women to the 20 “positive” psychotic experience items of the Community Assessment of Psychic Experiences (CAPE) questionnaire. Five separable but correlated trait dimensions were found, reflecting Paranoia, Grandiosity, Magical Thinking, Delusions, and Hallucinations. High scores on any dimension were associated with a higher probability of questionnaire‐assessed lifetime major depressive episodes or generalized anxiety disorder, though Grandiosity was so only to a very small degree. Our results closely match previous findings among adolescents and young women, and demonstrate that psychotic experiences cannot be considered a single trait. Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

Keywords: psychotic, CAPE, questionnaire, women, community

Introduction

The focus of psychosis‐risk research has increasingly shifted to clinically significant sub‐psychotic traits and symptoms, as newer questionnaires (e.g. Y‐PARQ by Ord et al., 2004) measure subclinical psychotic experiences (PEs) at the more severe or acute end of the spectrum, largely motivated by the clinical high‐risk paradigm. These experiences are essentially the same as the symptoms of psychotic disorders, though they may be less intense or accompanied by less conviction. There has also been a new wave in theoretical development, and subsequent data analyses suggest that PEs form a continuum from normal variation in behaviour to full psychosis (Johns et al., 2002; Shevlin et al., 2007; van Os et al., 2000), though there have been assertions to the contrary (Lenzenweger and Korfine, 1992). Such conceptualizations focus on the issue of whether there is a single latent dimension or a single latent class explaining the PEs.

However, there is a long history of finding the related concepts of psychosis‐proneness or schizotypy to be multidimensional (Bentall et al., 1989). Traits resembling the features of psychotic disorders have long been of interest as possible predictors of psychosis, and specific questionnaires of postulated psychosis‐proneness or schizotypy have been carefully constructed since the 1970s, with the Wisconsin scales (Chapman et al., 1976) having been the most widely employed. Constructed according to the best practices of classical psychometrics, they consist of items chosen, among other criteria, for their similar and comparatively high response rates to the individual items. The greatest discriminatory power (i.e. precision of measurement) is thus achieved in the average range of the supposed psychosis‐proneness dimensions, though the phenomena of interest tend to be in the upper extreme. Nevertheless, though the severity of item content has been lower than in more recent PE questionnaires, the research in the dimensionality of schizotypy and psychosis‐proneness is likely to be informative to the PE concept.

The primary method for analysing dimensionality in psychosis‐proneness and schizotypy research has been common factor analysis (CFA; e.g. Bentall et al., 1989). However, traditional CFA is ill‐suited for combining items of highly variable severity, as items may aggregate by endorsement rate (i.e. items with a high response rate forming a factor), rather than by underlying trait (Ferguson, 1941). These problems can fortunately be overcome by modern latent‐trait psychometric methods. Rather than being treated as equally difficult, the items are modelled with intercept parameters in addition to factor loadings. When using large samples, latent factors thus allow a new level of precision for questionnaire measurement.

In order to examine the validity of the implicit one‐dimensional assumption of the psychosis continuum, the present study examines whether separable latent dimensions of positive PEs can be identified in a general population sample with the Community Assessment of Psychic Experiences (CAPE; Konings et al., 2006; Stefanis et al., 2002), an instrument explicitly designed to probe clinically relevant PEs. If these PEs turn out to be multi‐faceted and consist of partially independent sub‐dimensions, it may be necessary to assess these subscales separately, as their usefulness in screening for actual psychosis liability may differ and be poorer as an aggregate. For convergent validation of psychopathological significance, identified PE dimensions are contrasted with self‐reports of lifetime depression and generalized anxiety gathered on the same questionnaire.

Method

Instruments

The primary data were self‐reports of PEs assessed with the 20 “positive” CAPE items (Stefanis et al., 2002; questionnaire available at http://www.cape42.homestead.com/index.html) included in a larger questionnaire on women's health. The items were translated from English into Swedish and back‐translated to verify correspondence with the original scale. Two independent professional translators did the back‐translation and the consensus version was tested in a pilot study with 50 subjects. The positive symptoms of the CAPE questionnaire are a modified version of the Peters et al. Delusions Inventory (PDI; Peters et al., 1999), which is based on the ninth edition of the Present State Examination (PSE; Wing et al., 1974). Most of these items are prefaced with “Do you ever feel/hear/see/think …” e.g. “Do you ever hear voices talking to each other when you are alone?”, the exceptions being “Do you believe in the power of witchcraft, voodoo or the occult?” and “Have your thoughts ever been so vivid that you were worried other people would hear them?”: the time span assessed is thus an indeterminate one including the present or the entire lifetime. The CAPE scale has four symptom frequency levels for each item: “almost always,” “often,” “sometimes,” and “never.” The degree of distress associated with the experience was not included in the current study.

Some mental disorders were also addressed on the same questionnaire. Participants responded to a series of questions closely corresponding to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition (DSM‐IV) criteria for a major depressive episode (MDE) and generalized anxiety disorder (GAD). Due to the questionnaire response format, not all criteria were included, and MDE required both the Mood and Anhedonia symptoms to be reported rather than either of them.

Participants

The participants were Swedish women aged 41–61 (mean 51.4 years), who took part in 2003/2004 in the follow‐up phase of the Scandinavian Women's Lifestyle and Health Cohort (Ekman et al., 2006; questionnaires and study information available at http://ki.se/ki/jsp/polopoly.jsp?d=20051&l=en) 47,859 women were invited to complete a web‐based questionnaire, and non‐responders received a paper questionnaire. The overall response rate in the follow‐up phase was 72%, with 34,415 returned questionnaires. At least partial responses to the CAPE items were available for 31,950 individuals, for a section‐specific response rate of 67% of the original sample. Of these, 128 response sets (0.4%) with more than two CAPE items empty were considered unreliable and were removed, leaving 31,822 response sets.

The recruitment methods of the initial study have previously been described in detail (Kumle et al., 2002). Briefly, 96,000 women from the Uppsala region were randomly selected from Central Population Registry and were invited in 1991/1992 to complete a questionnaire on health, contraception, and health‐related behaviours; the total response rate in the initial study was 51.3%. The study protocol has been approved by the Ethical Committee at Karolinska Institutet and the Swedish Data Inspection Board.

Analysis

Response frequencies for the CAPE positive items are reported in Table 1. Endorsement of a particular item at any level varied from 0.5% for Voices Conversing and 1.4% for Capgras to over 50% for False Appearances and Telepathy. Responses to the more severe items were strongly correlated: only 17% of the respondents endorsed, at any level, any of the seven items with overall endorsement rates under 5%.

Table 1.

Response frequencies of CAPE‐42 “positive” items

Item Never Sometimes Often Almost always
Double meaning 56.74 % 41.05 % 1.93 % 0.28 %
Messages from television 86.83 % 12.65 % 0.35 % 0.17 %
False appearance 31.09 % 63.28 % 5.42 % 0.20 %
Being persecuted 95.66 % 3.99 % 0.25 % 0.10 %
Conspiracy 91.94 % 7.61 % 0.35 % 0.10 %
Being important 70.64 % 24.68 % 3.92 % 0.77 %
Being special 60.44 % 34.53 % 4.16 % 0.87 %
Telepathy 47.18 % 45.89 % 5.78 % 1.15 %
Influenced by devices 94.87 % 4.57 % 0.45 % 0.11 %
Voodoo 78.44 % 18.20 % 2.13 % 1.23 %
Odd looks 84.80 % 13.91 % 1.01 % 0.28 %
Thought withdrawal 93.56 % 5.95 % 0.43 % 0.06 %
Thought insertion 94.15 % 5.45 % 0.29 % 0.11 %
Thought broadcasting 95.36 % 4.35 % 0.25 % 0.04 %
Thought echo 94.18 % 5.47 % 0.30 % 0.05 %
External control 95.22 % 4.19 % 0.42 % 0.18 %
Verbal hallucinations 97.70 % 2.15 % 0.12 % 0.03 %
Voices conversing 99.45 % 0.48 % 0.05 % 0.02 %
Capgras syndrome 98.64 % 1.26 % 0.07 % 0.03 %
Visual hallucinations 95.80 % 3.74 % 0.35 % 0.12 %

Full‐information factor analyses of the original ordinal responses to the 20 CAPE items were carried out with the Mplus program version 6.11 (Muthén and Muthén, 2011). The model was computed separately for 1–7 dimensions with the Robust Maximum Likelihood algorithm. This algorithm uses all the available raw data rather than polychoric correlation matrices, and ignores missing responses instead of replacing them or deleting cases. For each item, three threshold parameters were estimated (corresponding to the steps between the four response alternatives) in addition to one loading parameter per factor. Adaptive Monte Carlo integration was used with 2187 integration points, iterating the Expectation‐Maximation phase until the change in slope estimates was less than the default limit of 0.005. For each number of dimensions the analysis was run 20 times, with the best‐fitting result retained, and the resulting factors were rotated with the OBLIMIN method. Models were compared using the Bayes Information Criterion (BIC).

MDE and GAD items were used to classify participants into four diagnostic groups based on lifetime presence of each syndrome. In addition, participants were classified as high‐scorers on each factor separately if they were in the highest 5%.

Results

BIC scores for factorial models improved with increasing dimensions, despite the increasing number of parameters, though improvements were marginal beyond the four‐dimensional solution. The found factorial structures in up to five dimensions were readily interpretable, and are reported in Table 2, with the six‐factor model included for completeness. In the single‐dimensional model, which accounted for 44.4% of the variance, all items had a loading over 0.50. In the two‐dimensional model, the second dimension loaded almost exclusively on the two “Grandiosity” items. In three dimensions, a further “Magical Thinking” factor was separated from the main factor. In the four‐dimensional solution, the main PE dimension split into “Paranoia” and “Delusions & Hallucinations.” In five dimensions “Delusions” and “Hallucinations” were separated from each other, with 62.8 % of the variance explained.

Table 2.

OBLIMIN‐rotated factor loadings of robust maximum likelihood (MLR) solutions, with factor correlations

Item 1‐factor 2‐factor 3‐factor 4‐factor 5‐factor 6‐factor
F1 F1 F2 F1 F2 F3 F1 F2 F3 F4 F1 F2 F3 F4 F5 F1 F2 F3 F4 F5 F6
OBLIMIN‐rotated factor loadings of robust maximum likelihood (MLR) solutions
Double meaning 0.56 0.69 −0.09 0.73 −0.01 −0.08 0.85 −0.02 0.03 −0.10 0.74 −0.03 0.05 0.12 −0.16 0.77 −0.01 0.08 0.04 −0.14 0.09
Messages from television 0.51 0.43 0.15 0.49 0.21 −0.13 0.41 0.23 0.05 −0.03 0.37 0.17 0.11 0.07 −0.07 0.27 0.16 0.04 0.06 −0.03 0.39
False appearance 0.64 0.56 0.16 0.56 0.20 0.01 0.53 0.24 0.07 0.01 0.51 0.20 0.16 0.13 −0.21 0.44 0.14 0.17 0.05 −0.05 0.28
Being persecuted 0.72 0.83 −0.14 0.85 −0.03 −0.09 0.66 −0.02 0.00 0.21 0.73 0.01 −0.02 −0.04 0.25 0.70 0.05 −0.04 0.02 0.24 −0.07
Conspiracy 0.68 0.80 −0.13 0.84 −0.01 −0.14 0.74 0.00 −0.03 0.10 0.79 0.03 −0.04 −0.02 0.13 0.76 0.03 −0.04 −0.01 0.16 −0.02
Being important 0.51 −0.02 0.78 −0.01 0.89 −0.05 −0.04 0.87 −0.05 0.05 −0.04 0.91 −0.07 0.02 0.01 −0.04 0.87 −0.08 −0.02 0.05 0.06
Being special 0.58 0.06 0.80 0.04 0.70 0.14 0.07 0.72 0.14 −0.02 0.07 0.71 0.14 −0.04 0.02 0.07 0.77 0.11 0.04 −0.07 −0.08
Telepathy 0.59 0.35 0.35 0.01 0.17 0.72 0.06 0.15 0.72 0.02 0.02 0.11 0.75 0.06 −0.06 0.00 0.11 0.74 0.06 −0.03 0.08
Influenced by devices 0.63 0.46 0.26 0.29 0.18 0.33 0.03 0.17 0.26 0.37 −0.01 0.13 0.32 0.39 −0.02 −0.05 0.10 0.33 0.23 0.13 0.29
Voodoo 0.53 0.36 0.24 −0.03 −0.02 0.82 0.06 −0.03 0.79 0.02 0.03 −0.06 0.83 −0.04 0.04 0.04 −0.05 0.81 −0.02 0.04 −0.05
Odd looks 0.53 0.66 −0.14 0.64 −0.10 0.01 0.50 −0.09 0.06 0.16 0.48 −0.10 0.08 0.17 0.02 0.50 −0.07 0.07 0.27 −0.08 −0.10
Thought withdrawal 0.71 0.74 −0.02 0.74 0.08 −0.07 0.12 0.07 −0.19 0.77 0.09 0.01 −0.10 0.83 −0.05 0.08 0.00 −0.10 0.64 0.11 0.25
Thought insertion 0.75 0.74 0.02 0.68 0.06 0.08 0.00 0.04 −0.09 0.86 −0.03 −0.02 0.01 0.83 0.05 −0.02 −0.02 −0.01 0.72 0.12 0.13
Thought broadcasting 0.73 0.71 0.05 0.64 0.07 0.10 0.08 0.05 −0.02 0.70 0.06 0.03 0.05 0.62 0.11 0.06 0.04 0.03 0.76 −0.04 −0.07
Thought echo 0.74 0.71 0.06 0.63 0.07 0.13 0.02 0.05 −0.02 0.78 0.01 0.02 0.06 0.67 0.13 0.01 0.04 0.03 0.81 −0.01 −0.09
External control 0.77 0.65 0.19 0.50 0.13 0.29 0.04 0.11 0.17 0.62 0.04 0.10 0.21 0.45 0.20 0.05 0.12 0.19 0.50 0.14 −0.09
Verbal hallucinations 0.77 0.76 0.03 0.59 −0.06 0.31 0.04 −0.07 0.15 0.73 0.09 0.01 0.08 0.14 0.72 0.08 −0.01 0.08 0.09 0.77 0.00
Voices conversing 0.86 0.83 0.04 0.67 −0.07 0.32 −0.01 −0.06 0.11 0.86 0.07 0.06 0.01 0.09 0.87 0.05 0.04 0.00 0.05 0.91 0.01
Capgras syndrome 0.72 0.64 0.12 0.49 0.06 0.28 0.00 0.03 0.15 0.65 0.03 0.07 0.15 0.32 0.38 0.03 0.07 0.13 0.34 0.36 −0.05
Visual hallucinations 0.66 0.49 0.26 0.17 0.03 0.63 −0.04 0.02 0.52 0.36 −0.02 0.04 0.50 0.02 0.40 −0.02 0.04 0.49 0.01 0.42 −0.04
Factor correlations of OBLIMIN‐rotated factors
F1 F2 F1 F2 F3 F1 F2 F3 F4 F1 F2 F3 F4 F5 F1 F2 F3 F4 F5
F1 1 F1 1 F1 1 F1 1 F1 1
F2 0.48 1 F2 0.41 1 F2 0.35 1 F2 0.33 1 F2 0.32 1
F3 0.52 0.35 1 F3 0.31 0.33 1 F3 0.34 0.39 1 F3 0.32 0.41 1
F4 0.66 0.40 0.49 1 F4 0.61 0.42 0.43 1 F4 0.57 0.42 0.44 1

Each diagnostic group's scores on the five factors were standardized using the means and standard– deviations of the group without MDE or GAD, and are reported in Table 3. The p‐values for the differences between the reference group and each disorder group in a two‐way Dunnett's t‐test were all below 0.0001.

Table 3.

Factor score means (M values) and standard deviations (SD values) by lifetime diagnosis group

MDE GAD Both MDE and GAD No MDE or GAD
M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD)
1. Paranoia 0.49 (1.17) 0.72 (1.18) 0.94 (1.24) 0.00 (1.00)
2. Grandiosity 0.08 (0.99) 0.25 (1.04) 0.14 (0.97) 0.00 (1.00)
3. Magical thinking 0.24 (1.08) 0.34 (1.15) 0.39 (1.08) 0.00 (1.00)
4. Delusions 0.40 (1.15) 0.70 (1.31) 0.86 (1.34) 0.00 (1.00)
5. Hallucinations 0.38 (1.23) 0.56 (1.31) 0.75 (1.46) 0.00 (1.00)
N (%) 6343 (19.9%) 375 (1.2%) 929 (2.9%) 24175 (76.0%)

The odds ratios for the top 5% high‐scorers having either disorder were 4.1 [95% confidence interval (CI) 3.7–4.6] for Paranoia, 1.2 (95% CI 1.1–1.3) for Grandiosity, 2.0 (95% CI 1.8–2.2) for Magical Thinking, 2.8 (95% CI 2.5–3.1) for Delusions, and 2.4 (95% CI 2.1–2.6) for Hallucinations.

Discussion

We found the PEs corresponding to positive psychotic symptoms to be readily interpretable as five independent dimensions. Earlier studies of the dimensionality of schizotypy or psychosis‐proneness have consistently found a three‐ or four‐dimensional structure (e.g. Claridge et al., 1996) when an even larger variety of symptoms or traits are included. In these models, the positive and negative symptoms are separable from a third dimension, defined as disorganization, paranoia, or depression, depending on scale content, and sometimes a fourth, such as impulsive non‐conformity. The previously found number of factors might be an artefact of the analysis method and an arbitrary selection of the number of factors. A similar finding of multifactorial subdivision and an associated argument regarding artefactual findings has been forwarded within the literature of schizophrenia symptoms, where a subdivision of the positive symptom dimension into as many as 11 subdimensions may provide a better fit to the data (Stuart et al., 1999). Note that the CAPE lacks items addressing disorganization; these symptoms are usually found to be most closely associated with, but separable from, the other positive symptoms (Reynolds et al., 2000).

Our results were somewhat similar to those of Brenner et al. (2007), who used the full CAPE‐42 and found that unidimensionality could not be confirmed for the original positive and negative symptom scales in a general population sample. They tested exploratory Maximum Likelihood solutions and obtained marginally improved fit indices with a five‐factor solution, which essentially subdivided the positive scale into three factors referred to as “Positive‐bizarre”, “Social Delusions”, and “Popular Psychic Beliefs”. Our study further subdivided their Positive‐bizarre factor into Hallucinations and Delusions, and their Social Delusions factor into Paranoia and Grandiosity. The focus on positive symptoms and the large number of respondents is likely to account for the greater detail in the present results.

Other studies have analysed specifically the positive items of the CAPE. Barragan et al. (2011) reported a four‐dimensional principal component structure of the positive items in a community sample of 777 adolescents. Unfortunately they did not report their criteria for choosing four components, but the structure was very close to our four‐dimensional model. The differences were that their Hallucinatory Experiences included what we labelled Magical Thinking, and they found a component consisting of the items Messages from the television and Influenced by Devices. Note that their analysis treated the frequency categories as a linear scale, did not allow for correlated factors, and did not take measurement error into account, which explains some of the discrepancy. A study of 875 adolescents (Yung et al., 2009) employed linear factor analysis of the frequency categories, with oblique rotation, and the four‐dimensional model was selected based on a Monte‐Carlo simulation. Their model was very similar to ours, but combined our two Grandiosity items in Magical Thinking, had the Capgras item loading most strongly on Paranoia, and associated the Visual Hallucinations item primarily with Hallucinations. Another study using exactly the same methods as Yung et al. (2009) in a school and student sample with 1777 respondents (Armando et al., 2010) presented a model almost identical to our five‐dimensional solution, but it lacked Magical Thinking – of which two items were left out of the analyses, based on the previous results – and Messages from the television was associated with the Delusions dimension.

The methodologically strongest and also largest previous studies of the positive items of the CAPE, however, are those by Wigman et al. (2011, 2012). Using fit indices to determine the optimal number of dimensions, they found a five‐dimensional structure in a general population sample of 7652 adolescents, and verified it in a confirmatory analysis of 2230 adolescents' responses. This model, which was further tested at three time points among 283 young female twin pairs, showed a comparative fit index (CFI) of 0.96, 0.96, and 0.99 over the three measurements at six‐month intervals, indicating an excellent fit. Their model differed from ours in grouping Messages from the television with Delusions, and the Capgras and Visual Hallucinations items with Delusions and Hallucinations, respectively. The latter discrepancies are largely accounted for by the strong cross loadings in our data on these items; Wigman et al. (2011, 2012) do not report cross loadings in their explorative model, but their factor correlations are 0.80 and 0.87 in the relevant dimension pairs. All previous studies have investigated adolescents and young adults, while our study sample consisted of middle‐aged women. Therefore, the consistency of the factor structure of CAPE is very encouraging.

Lifetime presence of MDE and GAD was measured in order to partially assess the clinical significance of the identified dimensions. The GAD lifetime prevalence was similar to that observed in large epidemiological surveys using structured interviews (Lee et al., 2009), but the MDE lifetime prevalence was somewhat small for women of this age group (Bromet et al., 2011). This may be explained by the fact that both depressive mood and anhedonia were required in the diagnostic algorithm. Participants with MDE or GAD had higher scores on the five dimensions, and the top scorers on each dimension had a higher probability of having had one of the syndromes. These associations were not, however, equal across dimensions. In particular, scores on the Grandiosity dimension were only slightly increased among the disorder groups, and those reporting the two primary Grandiosity experiences had only a very slightly increased risk for the assessed mental disorders. These findings parallel those of Yung et al. (2009), who found depression scores among community adolescents to be least associated with their factor corresponding to our Grandiosity and Magical Thinking.

Our study does not support the notion of a single continuum from mild positive PEs to positive psychotic symptoms. Specifically, the present results raise doubts about the usefulness of using PE questionnaires as single summary measures. For instance, the Grandiosity factor in the present study, reflecting two items that could be separated from the others in all multidimensional models, appeared to probe primarily non‐pathological experiences, as 46.8% of the respondents endorsed at least one of them to some degree, and strong endorsement was only negligibly associated with self‐reported MDE or GAD – perhaps reflecting only general response tendency. In addition, the highly varying endorsement rates of the items, reflecting varying psychopathological severity of the content, show that simply summing item scores will lose much relevant information, and increase the relative contribution of measurement noise.

Impact of sample characteristics and questionnaire presentation

Though drawn from the general population, the sample of the present study was unusual, in consisting only of women over the age of 41. The very low endorsement rates of some items are likely to reflect not only the severity of the probed symptoms, but the age of the sample; self‐reported psychotic‐like experiences decline markedly between ages 20 and 40 (Rössler et al., 2007). It is unclear whether this reflects true variation, or understanding of the items increasing with age and therefore less false positives. To our knowledge, gender effects on the structure of psychosis‐proneness have not been reported, and our results can tentatively be generalized to men.

As the current sample was drawn from the general population, it is likely to contain also people with schizophrenia and other psychotic disorders, for which the sum prevalence among women of this age group are about 3% (Perälä et al., 2007). For the items with the lowest endorsement rates, these individuals may have a significant impact on the factor structure. However, as inclusion required responding to two lengthy questionnaires, individuals with psychotic disorders are likely to be under‐represented. Further studies are needed to assess whether the factor structure is affected by the exclusion of groups with current or previous psychotic illness.

Finally, the questionnaire has been constructed with the items in approximately increasing order of a priori severity, but the respondent's understanding of what domain is being probed may also improve as they go along. Specifically, as the probed experiences are infrequent, understanding their general severity level is likely to lead to less false positives in the later parts of the questionnaire. Therefore, item ordering effects may be confounding the item difficulty estimates. The current factor analyses, however, by modelling item difficulty, minimize the effect of item ordering on the factor structure.

Funding sources

This work was supported by the Swedish Research Council, grant number K2012‐69X‐22062‐01‐3, and by the Finnish Graduate School in Psychiatry (personal grant to S.T.).

Declaration of interest statement

The authors have no competing interests.

References

  1. Armando M., Nelson B., Yung A.R., Ross M., Birchwood M., Girardi P. (2010) Psychotic‐like experiences and correlation with distress and depressive symptoms in a community sample of adolescents and young adults. Schizophrenia Research, 119(1–3), 258–265, DOI: 10.1016/j.schres.2010.03.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Barragan M., Laurens K.R., Navarro J.B., Obiols J.E. (2011) Psychotic‐like experiences and depressive symptoms in a community sample of adolescents. European Psychiatry, 26(6), 396–401, DOI: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2010.12.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Bentall R.P., Claridge G.S., Slade P.D. (1989) The multidimensional nature of schizotypal traits: a factor analytic study with normal subjects. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 28(Pt 4), 363–375. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Brenner K., Schmitz N., Pawliuk N., Fathalli F., Joober R., Ciampi A. (2007) Validation of the English and French versions of the Community Assessment of Psychic Experiences (CAPE) with a Montreal community sample. Schizophrenia Research, 95(1–3), 86–95. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bromet E., Andrade L.H, Hwang I., Sampson N.A., Alonso J., de Girolamo G., et al(2011) Cross‐national epidemiology of DSM‐IV major depressive episode. BMC Medicine, 9, 90, DOI: 1741-7015-9-90 [pii] 10.1186/1741-7015-9-90 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Chapman L.J., Chapman J.P., Raulin M.L. (1976) Scales for physical and social anhedonia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 85(4), 374–382. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Claridge G., McCreery C., Mason O., Bentall R., Boyle G., Slade P. (1996) The factor structure of “schizotypal” traits: a large replication study. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 35(Pt 1), 103–115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Ekman A., Dickman P.W., Klint A., Weiderpass E., Litton J.E. (2006) Feasibility of using web‐based questionnaires in large population‐based epidemiological studies. European Journal of Epidemiology, 21(2), 103–111, DOI: 10.1007/s10654-005-6030-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Ferguson G.A. (1941) The factorial interpretation of test difficulty. Psychometrika, 6(5), 323–329. [Google Scholar]
  10. Johns L.C., Nazroo J.Y., Bebbington P., Kuipers E. (2002) Occurrence of hallucinatory experiences in a community sample and ethnic variations. British Journal of Psychiatry, 180, 174–178. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Konings M., Bak M., Hanssen M., van Os J., Krabbendam L. (2006) Validity and reliability of the CAPE: a self‐report instrument for the measurement of psychotic experiences in the general population. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 114(1), 55–61. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Kumle M., Weiderpass E., Braaten T., Persson I., Adami H.O., Lund E. (2002) Use of oral contraceptives and breast cancer risk: the Norwegian–Swedish Women's Lifestyle and Health Cohort Study. Cancer Epidemiology, Biomarkers & Prevention, 11(11), 1375–1381. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Lee S., Tsang A., Ruscio A. M., Haro J.M., Stein D.J., et al(2009) Implications of modifying the duration requirement of generalized anxiety disorder in developed and developing countries. Psychological Medicine, 39(7), 1163–1176, DOI: S0033291708004807 [pii] 10.1017/S0033291708004807 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Lenzenweger M.F., Korfine L. (1992) Confirming the latent structure and base rate of schizotypy: a taxometric analysis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 101(3), 567–571. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Muthén B.O., Muthén L.K. (2011) Mplus (Version 6.11). Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén. [Google Scholar]
  16. Ord L.M., Myles‐Worsley M., Blailes F., Ngiralmau H. (2004) Screening for prodromal adolescents in an isolated high‐risk population. Schizophrenia Research, 71(2–3), 507–508. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. van Os J., Hanssen M., Bijl R.V., Ravelli A. (2000) Strauss (1969) revisited: a psychosis continuum in the general population? Schizophrenia Research, 45(1–2), 11–20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Perälä J., Suvisaari J., Saarni S.I., Kuoppasalmi K., Isometsä E., Pirkola S. (2007) Lifetime prevalence of psychotic and bipolar I disorders in a general population. Archives of General Psychiatry, 64(1), 19–28. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Peters E.R., Joseph S.A., Garety P.A. (1999) Measurement of delusional ideation in the normal population: introducing the PDI (Peters et al. Delusions Inventory). Schizophrenia Bulletin, 25(3), 553–576. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Reynolds C.A., Raine A., Mellingen K., Venables P.H., Mednick S.A. (2000) Three‐factor model of schizotypal personality: invariance across culture, gender, religious affiliation, family adversity, and psychopathology. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 26(3), 603–618. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Rössler W., Riecher‐Rössler A., Angst J., Murray R., Gamma A., Eich D. (2007) Psychotic experiences in the general population: a twenty‐year prospective community study. Schizophrenia Research, 92(1–3), 1–14, DOI: 10.1016/j.schres.2007.01.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Shevlin M., Adamson G., Vollebergh W., de Graaf R., van Os J. (2007) An application of item response mixture modelling to psychosis indicators in two large community samples. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 42(10), 771–779. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Stefanis N.C., Hanssen M., Smirnis N.K., Avramopoulos D.A., Evdokimidis I.K., Stefanis C.N. (2002) Evidence that three dimensions of psychosis have a distribution in the general population. Psychological Medicine, 32(2), 347–358. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Stuart G.W., Pantelis C., Klimidis S., Minas I.H. (1999) The three‐syndrome model of schizophrenia: meta‐analysis of an artefact. Schizophrenia Research, 39(3), 233–242. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Wigman J.T., Vollebergh W.A., Raaijmakers Q.A., Iedema J., van Dorsselaer S., Ormel J. (2011) The structure of the extended psychosis phenotype in early adolescence – a cross‐sample replication. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 37(4), 850–860, DOI: 10.1093/schbul/sbp154 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Wigman J.T., Vollebergh W.A., Jacobs N., Wichers M., Derom C., Thiery E. (2012) Replication of the five‐dimensional structure of positive psychotic experiences in young adulthood. Psychiatry Research, 197(3), 353–355, DOI: 10.1016/j.psychres.2011.09.015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Wing J.K., Cooper J.E., Sartorius N. (1974) Measurement and Classification of Psychiatric Symptoms, first edition Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  28. Yung A.R., Nelson B., Baker K., Buckby J.A., Baksheev G., Cosgrave E.M. (2009) Psychotic‐like experiences in a community sample of adolescents: implications for the continuum model of psychosis and prediction of schizophrenia. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 43(2), 118–128, DOI: 10.1080/00048670802607188 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research are provided here courtesy of Wiley

RESOURCES