Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2020 Nov 1.
Published in final edited form as: Prev Med. 2019 Aug 7;128:105789. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2019.105789

Sensitivity of Hypothetical Purchase Task Indices When Studying Substance Use: A Systematic Literature Review

Ivori Zvorsky 1,2,3, Tyler D Nighbor 1,2, Allison N Kurti 1,2,3, Michael DeSarno 4, Gideon Naudé 5, Derek D Reed 5, Stephen T Higgins 1,2,3
PMCID: PMC6879840  NIHMSID: NIHMS1538604  PMID: 31400376

Abstract

Hypothetical Purchase Tasks (HPTs) simulate demand for a substance as a function of escalating price. HPTs are increasingly used to examine relationships between substance-related correlates and outcomes and demand typically characterized using a common battery of indices (Intensity, Omax, Pmax, Breakpoint, Elasticity). This review examines the relative sensitivity of the HPT indices. Reports were identified using the search term “purchase task” in PubMed and Web of Science. For inclusion, reports had to be original studies in English, examine relationships between HPT indices and substance-related correlates or outcomes, and appear in a peer-reviewed journal through December 2017. Indices were compared using effect sizes (Cohen’s d) and the proportion of studies in which statistically significant relationships were observed. The search identified 1,274 reports with 114 (9%) receiving full-text review and 82 (6%) meeting inclusion criteria. 41 reports examined alcohol, 34 examined cigarettes/nicotine products, and 10 examined other substances. Overall, statistically significant relationships between HPT indices and substance-related correlates and outcomes were most often reported for Intensity (88.61%, 70/79), followed by Omax (81.16%, 56/69), Elasticity (72.15%, 57/59), Breakpoint (62.12%, 41/66), and Pmax (48.08%; 25/52). The largest effect sizes were observed for Intensity (0.75 ± 0.04, CI 0.67–0.84) and Omax (0.64±0.04, CI 0.56–0.71), followed by Elasticity (0.44±0.04, CI 0.37–0.51), Breakpoint (0.30±0.03, CI 0.25–0.36), and Pmax (0.25±0.04, CI 0.18–0.33). Patterns were largely consistent across substances. In conclusion, HPTs can be highly effective in revealing relationships between demand and substance-related correlates and outcomes, with Intensity and Omax exhibiting the greatest sensitivity.

Keywords: hypothetical purchase tasks, purchase task, alcohol purchase task, cigarette purchase task, behavioral economics, substance use, addiction, substance use disorders

INTRODUCTION

Behavioral economics is a discipline that investigates decision-making processes using principles and methods of psychology and microeconomics (Bickel, DeGrandpre, & Higgins, 1993; Hursh, Galuska, Winger, & Woods, 2005; Reed, Galuska, & Kaplan, 2013). According to behavioral economic theory, behavior exists within an economic context in which commodities (i.e., reinforcers) compete for limited consumer resources (e.g., time, money, effort; Hursh & Roma, 2016). Behavioral economic approaches have been applied to the examination of demand for addictive substances. Addiction can be conceptualized as a condition wherein substances become overvalued compared to other reinforcers in a person’s life (Bickel et al., 2014; Higgins, Heil, & Lussier, 2004). This overvaluation is linked to increased consumption, craving, withdrawal, and relapse risk (Cook et al., 2010; Higgins, 1997; Leventhal et al., 2008, 2014; Zvolensky et al., 2009).

Hypothetical purchase tasks (HPT) are commonly used as an ethical and safe method to experimentally investigate the reinforcing value of substances, particularly the overvaluation of substances associated with substance misuse and addiction (Roma et al., 2017). HPTs require individuals to make choices about hypothetical commodities across a range of hypothetical prices in order to examine various dimensions of substance-related reinforcement including addiction potential (Bickel et al., 2014; Higgins et al., 2017a,b; Jacobs & Bickel, 1999; Vuchinich & Heather, 2003). For example, a cigarette purchase task asks smokers to estimate the number of cigarettes they would purchase and smoke in a 24-hour period across a progressively increasing range of monetary prices. Demand curves generated using HPTs have been demonstrated to correspond closely to those obtained when directly measuring actual consumption under controlled laboratory conditions (Amlung et al., 2012; Wilson et al., 2016).

The primary dependent measures obtained using HPTs include an overall demand curve, five indices, and more recently, two latent factors that are derived from combining the individual indices. The overall demand curve depicts consumption within the designated time period across a range of escalating prices (MacKillop et al., 2008). The five indices are: Intensity (consumption at zero or minimal price; maximum consumption); Omax (maximum amount spent for the substance across all price points; maximum expenditure); Pmax (the price point associated with maximum expenditure); Breakpoint (the price point at which consumption is suppressed to zero); and Elasticity (overall sensitivity to increases in price) (Hursh & Roma, 2016). These indices detect related but nuanced aspects of the reinforcing value of substances and provide insights about the overvaluation of substances that contributes to problematic use and addiction. The two latent factors summarizing the five indices are Amplitude and Persistence (Aston et al., 2017; Bidwell et al., 2012; MacKillop et al., 2009). Amplitude typically includes Intensity and Omax and represents maximum consumption of the given substance and maximum expenditure for the substance. Persistence typically encompasses Pmax, Breakpoint, Elasticity, and to a lesser extent, Omax, and represents the sensitivity of consumption to increasing constraint (Bidwell et al., 2012; MacKillop et al., 2009; O’Connor et al., 2016). Although these latent factors appear to have predictive utility (Aston et al., 2017; O’Connor et al., 2016, but see Gonzalez-Roz et al., 2018), they are not yet reported regularly in the literature and thus are not examined as part of this review, including in those studies that otherwise met inclusion criteria for this review (e.g., Aston et al., 2017; Kiselica & Borders, 2013; O’Connor et al., 2016; Teeters et al., 2014).

HPTs are increasingly being used in substance use research to discern individual differences in risk factors for and markers of problematic use (e.g., dependence, comorbid psychiatric symptoms) in addition to response to clinical or regulatory interventions (Roma et al., 2017). For example, HPTs have been shown to be sensitive to individual differences in demand for alcohol among college students (Murphy et al., 2009), to individual differences in demand for cigarettes among those with versus without serious mental illness (MacKillop & Tidey, 2011), and to individual differences in demand for cigarettes varying in nicotine content among the general population of smokers (Smith et al., 2017) and vulnerable populations (Higgins et al., 2017a). Furthermore, HPTs have been shown to independently predict risk factors for continued smoking during pregnancy, above and beyond conventional predictors of quit success (Higgins et al., 2017b). Finally, HPT indices have been shown to be sensitive in assessing response to regulatory policy interventions, such as reducing nicotine content or increasing excise taxes on cigarettes (Smith et al., 2017; Grace et al., 2015b).

As use of HPTs increases, there is emerging recognition of the need to systematically validate and compare the relative sensitivity of the five commonly used HPT demand indices in assessing relationships between demand for substances and substance-related correlates and outcomes. We know of two systematic reviews on this general topic, although both were restricted to the Alcohol Purchase Task (APT) (Kiselica et al., 2016; Kaplan et al., 2018). Kiselica and colleagues separately compared the average effect sizes and 95% confidence intervals (CIs) obtained for each of the five APT indices in relation to four different alcohol-related outcome measures: alcohol consumption, binge/heavy drinking, alcohol problems, and Alcohol Use Disorder symptoms. Results were consistent across the four outcome measures: Intensity produced larger effects sizes than each of the other indices except Omax and only Intensity demonstrated incremental utility in predicting outcomes above conventional measures of alcohol consumption. Kaplan and colleagues (2018) documented considerable variation across studies in how the APT was constructed, administered, and analyzed, underscoring the need to eliminate this methodological heterogeneity to develop best practices in using the APT, but did not examine the relative sensitivity of the indices.

To our knowledge, there have been no systematic reviews examining the relative sensitivity of the five HPT indices beyond the APT. As use of HPTs continues to grow, knowing whether the greater sensitivity of Intensity and Omax relative to other indices reported by Kiselica and colleagues (2016) is specific to the APT or whether this sensitivity extends to HPTs assessing other substances seems especially important and is thus the overarching purpose of this review. Further, Kiselica and colleagues (2016) examined only cross-sectional studies, thus excluding data from longitudinal, observational, and experimental studies. Knowing the relative sensitivity of the HPT indices to changes in substance-related outcomes over time in longitudinal/experimental studies is important information for researchers, policy makers, and clinicians alike. In order to address these gaps in this emerging literature, we examined HPT studies on alcohol, cigarettes, and other substances including cross-sectional and longitudinal/experimental studies in this review. Greater understanding of the relative sensitivity of HPT indices to detect substance-related correlates and outcomes across substances and different types of studies is important as use of this behavioral-economic approach to studying substance use grows and increasingly informs regulatory policy (Roma et al., 2017).

METHODS

This review follows the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) statement guidelines (Liberati et al., 2009). Reports were identified using PubMed, the search engine of the U.S. National Library of Medicine, and Web of Science, a citation indexing service started by the Institute for Scientific Information. The term “purchase task” was entered into each search engine and articles were included if published online through December 31, 2017. All articles were reviewed for inclusion by three of the authors (I.Z., T.N., and A.K.), and discrepancies between reviewers were resolved through discussion. Duplicates across databases were removed.

Criteria for inclusion were that reports had to (a) be in English; (b) appear in a peer-reviewed journal and be published online through December 31, 2017; (c) include an HPT focused on a substance; (d) examine relationships between HPT indices and their association with substance-related correlates and outcomes; and (e) be an original study reporting previously unpublished data.

Included articles were categorized by target substance (e.g., alcohol, cigarettes, other substances). Studies examining multiple substances (Hindocha et al., 2017; Peters et al., 2017; Strickland et al., 2016a, 2016b, 2017) were included in more than one substance category. Cross-sectional and longitudinal/experimental studies were included. Cross-sectional studies included those that examined correlations between demand and substance-related outcomes at one time point in the absence of any experimental manipulation. Longitudinal studies included those measuring demand (a) in observational studies using within-subject designs over time and/or (b) as part of an experimental manipulation. Given the focus of this review, distinctions were not drawn between derived and observed metrics for calculations of HPT indices, although this information is indicated in tables. This review included any index that was reported in the articles regardless of method of calculation.

Two methods were used to assess the relative sensitivity of the HPT indices. First, we compared the proportion of studies in which statistically significant relationships were reported between each of the five commonly used HPT indices (Intensity, Omax, Pmax, Breakpoint, Elasticity) and substance-related correlates and outcomes. If a study did not measure a particular HPT index or if an index was excluded from data analyses, it was not included in calculating the proportion of studies for that particular index.

Second, effect sizes were calculated for each of the HPT indices and substance-related correlates and outcomes collapsing across substances (alcohol, cigarettes, other substances) and then within each of those substance categories. We did not investigate different types of substance-related outcomes given the consistent pattern across outcomes reported by Kiselica and colleagues (2016). Cohen’s d was used as the measure of effect size due to its familiarity and ease of interpretation. Effect sizes were calculated for each study where (a) a statistically significant association (p < 0.05) was reported between one or more HPT index and substance-related correlates and outcomes and (b) enough information was included in the report to calculate an effect size. In those instances where a significant effect involving one or more HPT indices was reported, effect sizes were also calculated for all HPT indices for which sufficient information was reported to calculate an effect size. Whenever possible, effect sizes were computed based on the reported test statistic. In cases where an appropriate test statistic was not provided, effect sizes were computed based on descriptive statistics such as means and standard deviations (or standard errors) presented in tables or figures. Estimated average effect sizes and corresponding 95% confidence intervals were calculated across studies and within study substance groups (alcohol, cigarettes/nicotine products, other substances) and study type groups (cross-sectional, longitudinal/experimental). Omnibus F Tests were conducted in order to test for statistically significant differences between mean effect sizes. The estimates for average effect sizes are based on a random effects meta-analysis model in which each study’s effect was weighted inversely proportional to its variance. A random effects model was used due to the assumption that there is significant heterogeneity of effect sizes across studies. Analyses were conducted using the software package Comprehensive Meta-Analysis version 3 (Biostat Inc., Englewood, NJ, USA).

RESULTS

Search Results

The search identified 1,274 reports, of which 114 (9%, 114/1,274) were included for full-text review and 82 (6%, 82/1,274) met final inclusion criteria (Figure 1). Of the 1,274 reports identified, 1,163 (91%, 1,163/1,274) were excluded when review of title and abstract revealed no mention of an HPT. Of the 114 articles that underwent full-text review, 32 (28%, 32/114) were excluded for the following reasons: 6 (5%, 6/114) did not involve original research (e.g., systematic reviews, commentaries, etc.); 6 (5%, 6/114) examined purchase tasks using actual rather than hypothetical outcomes (the focus of this review is on HPTs); 8 (7%, 8/114) involved HPTs not assessing substance use (e.g., indoor tanning); and 12 (11%, 12/114) were methodological investigations (e.g., identifying criteria for non-systematic responding; testing different statistical models to derive indices).

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses flow diagram.

Of the 82 articles that met final inclusion criteria, 43 (52%) reported cross-sectional studies (Amlung et al., 2013, 2015, 2017a, 2017b; Aston et al., 2015, 2017; Bertholet et al., 2015; Bidwell et al., 2012; Bruner & Johnson, 2014; Dennhardt et al., 2016; Farris et al., 2017a, 2017b; Gray & MacKillop, 2014; Gray et al., 2017; Herschl et al., 2012; Higgins et al., 2017b; Kiselica & Borders, 2013; Lemley et al., 2016; Luehring-Jones et al., 2016; MacKillop et al., 2008; MacKillop & Tidey, 2011; Morris et al., 2017; Murphy & MacKillop, 2006; Murphy et al., 2009, 2011, 2013, 2014; O’Connor et al., 2014, 2016; Pickover et al., 2016; Strickland & Stoops, 2017; Strickland et al., 2016a, 2016b, 2017; Teeters & Murphy, 2015; Teeters et al., 2014; Wahlstrom et al., 2012; Weinstock et al., 2016; Yurasek et al., 2011, 2013; MacKillop et al., 2014; Secades-Villa et al., 2018) and 39 (48%) were longitudinal/experimental studies (Amlung et al., 2012, 2014, 2015; Barnes et al., 2017; Berman & Martinetti, 2017; Bujarski et al., 2012; Bulley & Gullo, 2017; Chase et al., 2013; Collins et al., 2014; Dahne et al., 2017; Dennhardt et al., 2015; Gentile et al., 2012; Grace et al., 2015a, 2015b, 2015c; Higgins et al., 2017a; Johnson et al., 2017; MacKillop et al., 2007, 2012, 2016; Metrik et al., 2016; Murphy et al., 2015, 2016, 2017; Smith et al., 2017; Hindocha et al., 2017; Peters et al., 2017; Owens et al., 2015a, 2015b; Schlienz et al., 2014; Secades-Villa et al., 2016; Skidmore & Murphy, 2011; Smith et al., 2017; Snider et al., 2016, 2017; Tucker et al., 2016a, 2016b; Tucker et al., 2018; Vincent et al., 2017). Effect sizes for each HPT index overall, and separately for cross-sectional and longitudinal/experimental studies are included in Table 1. Among those 82 articles, 41 (50%, 41/82) articles investigated relationships between APT indices and alcohol-related correlates and outcomes (Amlung et al., 2012, 2013, 2015a, 2015b, 2016, 2017; Amlung & MacKillop, 2014; Berman & Martinetti, 2017; Bertholet et al., 2015; Bujarski et al., 2012; Bulley & Gullo, 2017; Dennhardt et al., 2015, 2016; Gentile et al., 2012; Gray & MacKillop, 2014; Herschl et al., 2012; Kiselica & Borders, 2013; Lemley et al., 2016, 2017; Luehring-Jones et al., 2016; MacKillop et al., 2009; Morris et al., 2017; Murphy et al., 2009, 2013, 2014 2015; Murphy & MacKillop, 2006; Owens et al., 2015a, 2015b; Skidmore & Murphy, 2011; Snider et al., 2016; Strickland et al., 2016; Strickland & Stoops, 2017; Teeters & Murphy, 2015; Teeters et al., 2014; Tucker et al., 2016a, 2016b; Wahlstrom et al., 2012; Weinstock et al., 2016; Yurasek et al., 2011, 2013); 34 (41%, 34/82) investigated relationships between Cigarette Purchase Task (CPT) indices and cigarette or other nicotine product-related correlates and outcomes (Barnes et al., 2017; Bidwell et al., 2012; Chase et al., 2013; Dahne et al., 2017; Farris et al., 2017a, 2017b; Grace et al., 2015a, 2015b, 2015c; Gray et al., 2017; Higgins et al., 2017a, 2017b; Hindocha et al., 2017; Johnson et al., 2017; MacKillop et al., 2008, 2012, 2014, 2016; MacKillop & Tidey, 2011; Murphy et al., 2011, 2016, 2017; O’Connor et al., 2012, 2014, 2016; Peters et al., 2017; Schlienz et al., 2014; Secades-Villa et al., 2016, 2018; Smith et al., 2017; Snider et al., 2017; Strickland et al., 2016, 2017; Tucker et al., 2018); and 12 (15%, 12/82) investigated relationships between HPTs for other substances (e.g., cocaine, marijuana) and substance-related correlates and outcomes (Aston et al., 2015, 2017; Bruner & Johnson, 2014; Collins et al., 2014; Hindocha et al., 2017; Metrik et al., 2016; Peters et al., 2017; Pickover et al., 2016; Strickland et al., 2016a, 2016b; Strickland et al., 2017; Vincent et al., 2017). As noted above, significant associations in the five articles examining multiple substances were categorized by each substance examined (Hindocha et al., 2017; Peters et al., 2017; Strickland et al., 2016a, 2016b; 2017).

Table 1.

Effect Sizes for All Studies, Cross-sectional, and Longitudinal/Experimental Studies collapsed across all substances.

Overall Cross-sectional Longitudinal/Experimental
Omnibus F Test Results: F(4, 258) = 30.33 (p < 0.0001) F(1, 159) = 28.81 (p < 0.0001) F(4, 94) = 7.49 (p < 0.0001)

Index Cohen’s d 95% Confidence Intervals Cohen’s d 95% Confidence Intervals Cohen’s d 95% Confidence Intervals
Intensity 0.75 0.67– 0.84 0.72 0.61– 0.84 0.80 0.67– 0.93
Omax 0.64 0.56– 0.71 0.59 0.49– 0.69 0.71 0.59– 0.84
Elasticity 0.44 0.37– 0.51 0.33 0.27– 0.39 0.58 0.45– 0.71
Breakpoint 0.30 0.25– 0.36 0.28 0.22– 0.33 0.37 0.25– 0.49
Pmax 0.25 0.18– 0.33 0.16 0.11– 0.21 0.35 0.17– 0.54

Overall Associations between HPT Indices and Substance-Related Correlates and Outcomes

Across the 82 articles, statistically significant associations were most often reported for Intensity (88.61%, 70/79), followed by Omax (81.16%, 56/69), Elasticity (72.15%, 57/59), Breakpoint (62.12%, 41/66), and Pmax (48.08%; 25/52; Figure 2).

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

Overall percentage of articles reporting significant associations between the 5 HPT indices and substance-related correlates and outcomes.

The largest effect sizes were observed for Intensity (0.75 ± 0.04, CI 0.67–0.84) and Omax (0.64±0.04, CI 0.56–0.71), followed by Elasticity (0.44±0.04, CI 0.37–0.51), Breakpoint (0.30±0.03, CI 0.25–0.36), and Pmax (0.25±0.04, CI 0.18–0.33). Omnibus F tests showed significant differences between overall mean effect sizes, and within cross-sectional and longitudinal/experimental studies (Table 1). For each set of effect sizes, there was a significant difference between means (p < 0.0001 for each). Similar results were observed when comparing average effect sizes and 95% CIs within cross-sectional and longitudinal/experimental studies. Based on the 95% confidence intervals, within the cross-sectional studies, Intensity and Omax had significantly greater mean effect sizes than each of the other indices, but their means did not differ significantly from each other; Elasticity was similar to Breakpoint but greater than Pmax; Breakpoint and Pmax did not differ significantly from each other. Within the longitudinal/experimental studies, Intensity and Omax again differed significantly from Breakpoint and Pmax but not from Elasticity or from each other; Elasticity, Breakpoint, and Pmax did not significantly differ from each other.

Associations between the Alcohol Purchase Task Indices and Alcohol-Related Correlates and Outcomes

Associations between Alcohol Purchase Task (APT) indices and alcohol-related correlates and outcomes were reported in 41 articles (Amlung et al., 2012, 2013, 2015a, 2015b, 2016, 2017; Amlung & MacKillop, 2014; Berman & Martinetti, 2017; Bertholet et al., 2015; Bujarski et al., 2012; Bulley & Gullo, 2017; Dennhardt et al., 2015, 2016; Gentile et al., 2012; Gray & MacKillop, 2014; Herschl et al., 2012; Kiselica & Borders, 2013; Lemley et al., 2016, 2017; Luehring-Jones et al., 2016; MacKillop et al., 2009; Morris et al., 2017; Murphy et al., 2009, 2013, 2014 2015; Murphy & MacKillop, 2006; Owens et al., 2015a, 2015b; Skidmore & Murphy, 2011; Snider et al., 2016; Strickland et al., 2016b; Strickland & Stoops, 2017; Teeters & Murphy, 2015; Teeters et al., 2014; Tucker et al., 2016a, 2016b; Wahlstrom et al., 2012; Weinstock et al., 2016; Yurasek et al., 2011, 2013; Table 2). Broadly speaking, demographic, psychosocial, and psychiatric characteristics associated with risk for problematic alcohol use in addition to symptoms of Alcohol Use Disorder were examined in these articles. Additionally, five of these reports examined individual differences related to response to clinical interventions (Bujarski et al., 2012; Dennhardt et al., 2015; MacKillop & Murphy, 2007; Murphy et al., 2015; Tucker et al., 2016). Statistically significant associations between APT indices and alcohol-related correlates and outcomes were most frequently reported for Intensity (93%, 38/41), followed by Omax (81%, 30/37), Elasticity (69%, 25/36), Breakpoint (60%, 24/40), and Pmax (38%, 9/24; Figure 3, top panel).

Table 2.

Significant associations between PT indices and alcohol-related correlates and outcomes.

Authors Purpose Substance Significant Predictors Results
Amlung et al. (2012) Examine the correspondence between choices for hypothetical and actual outcomes and the correspondence between estimated alcohol consumption and actual drinking behavior among 45 heavy-drinking adults (41 used in final analyses) Alcohol • N/A No significant findings relating to PT indices were reported (Indices assessed: IntensityO, OmaxO, PmaxO, BreakpointO, ElasticityD)
Amlung et al. (2013) Investigate the relationship between caffeine and alcoholic beverages (CAB), alcohol misuse, impulsivity traits, and demand among 316 regularly drinking college students (273 used in final analyses) Alcohol • IntensityO
OmaxO
• BreakpointO
• ElasticityD
Intensity was associated with premeditation and positive urge impulsivity scores; Intensity and Omax were associated with sensation seeking impulsivity scores; Intensity, Omax, Breakpoint, and Elasticity were associated with negative urgency impulsivity scores and CAB use (Indices assessed: IntensityO, OmaxO, BreakpointO, ElasticityD)
Amlung et al. (2015a) Introduce area under the curve as a novel index of demand in PT among 207 college students with at least one heavy drinking episode in the past month (205 used in final analyses) Alcohol • IntensityO
• OmaxO
Intensity and Omax were significant predictors of weekly alcohol consumption and Young Adult Alcohol Consequences Questionnaire (YAACQ) scores (Indices assessed: IntensityO, OmaxO, PmaxO, BreakpointO, ElasticityD)
Amlung et al. (2015b) Examine the effects of alcohol intoxication on alcohol demand and craving among 85 college students (no report on any exclusions from analyses) • IntensityO
OmaxO
• BreakpointO
Increases in Intensity, Omax, and Breakpoint were positively correlated with increases in craving after alcohol consumption (Indices assessed: IntensityO, OmaxO, BreakpointO)
Amlung et al. (2016) Examine whether drinking-and-driving-related cognitions mediate the association between demand and drinking and driving among 147 young adult social drinkers (134 used in final analyses; 132 in Elasticity analyses) Alcohol • IntensityO
OmaxO
• ElasticityD
Intensity was correlated with normative beliefs and perceived danger of drinking and driving; Omax and Elasticity were correlated with perceived personal limit; those who drive while intoxicated had higher Intensity and Omax and lower Elasticity (Indices assessed: IntensityO, OmaxO, BreakpointO, ElasticityD)
Amlung et al. (2017) Examine the relationship between concurrent smoking and overvaluation of alcohol among 121 non-treatment seeking drinkers (111 used in the final analyses) Alcohol • IntensityO
OmaxO
• BreakpointO
• ElasticityD
Intensity, Omax, and Elasticity were correlated with higher alcohol use disorder (AUD) severity and drinks per week; being a smoker predicted higher Omax, higher Breakpoint and lower Elasticity (Indices assessed: IntensityO, OmaxO, PmaxO, BreakpointO, ElasticityD)
Amlung & MacKillop (2014) Investigate the combined effects of acute stress and alcohol cues on craving, arousal, and behavioral economic measures among 90 adult heavy drinkers (84 used in final analyses) Alcohol • IntensityO
OmaxO
• BreakpointO
Intensity, Omax, and Breakpoint were higher after a stress test but decreased after neutral cue exposure; exposure to alcohol cues after a stress test increased Breakpoint; changes in responses to a choice procedure (alcohol now vs. money later) following stress vs. neutral cues were mediated by increases in Omax (Indices assessed: IntensityO, Omax O, BreakpointO, ElasticityD)
Berman & Martinetti (2017) Address the effects of 2 academic variables (next-day course level and class size) on alcohol demand among 65 undergraduate students (59 used in final analyses) Alcohol • IntensityD
OmaxD
PmaxD
• ElasticityD
Higher Intensity, Omax, Pmax, and lower Elasticity were associated with no constraint conditions (no classes); Elasticity was higher for smaller class sizes (Indices assessed: IntensityD, OmaxD, PmaxD, ElasticityD)
Bertholet et al. (2015) Study the impact of drink price on hypothetical consumption and assess whether demand parameters were associated with alcohol use, alcohol use consequences, and problem severity in a general population samples among 5,520 young adult males (4,790 used in final analyses) Alcohol • IntensityO
OmaxO
PmaxO
• BreakpointO
• ElasticityD
All indices were correlated with drinks per week, maximum number of drinks per occasion, monthly binge drinking, AUD criteria, and number of alcohol-related consequences (Indices assessed: IntensityO, OmaxO, PmaxO, BreakpointO, ElasticityD)
Bujarski et al. (2012) Assess medication effects of Naltrexone on the relative value of alcohol both before and after acute alcohol administration among 35 heavy drinking adult Asian Americans (32 used in final analyses) Alcohol • IntensityD
OmaxD
PmaxD
• BreakpointO
Intensity and Omax were negatively correlated with Naltrexone; Naltrexone reduced Intensity, Omax, and Breakpoint; Naltrexone and OPRM1 genotype interaction was significantly associated with Intensity; Higher Pmax and Breakpoint were associated with alcohol exposure (Indices assessed: IntensityD, OmaxD, PmaxD, BreakpointO, ElasticityD)
Bulley & Gullo (2017) Examine the effect of episodic foresight on alcohol-related decision making among 52 undergraduates (48 used in final analyses) Alcohol • IntensityO Intensity was associated with episodic future thinking (thinking about personally relevant future events to attenuate discounting of delayed rewards) and not with the control imagery condition (Indices assessed: IntensityO, OmaxO, PmaxO, BreakpointO, ElasticityD)
Dennhardt et al. (2015) Examine the impact of a brief substance use intervention on indices of substance reward value and whether baseline values and posttreatment change in these variables predict substance use outcomes among 97 heavy drinking college students (no report on any exclusions from analyses) Alcohol • IntensityO
OmaxO
• ElasticityD
Intensity was correlated with alcohol and marijuana problems; Intensity and Omax were reduced after treatment, regardless of additional treatment condition; Intensity, Omax, and Elasticity were correlated with drinks per week and binge drinking; Elasticity increased after treatment, regardless of additional treatment condition (Indices assessed: IntensityO, OmaxO, ElasticityD)
Dennhardt et al. (2016) Examine the factors that contribute to risk for problematic drinking in a high-risk adult sample of 68 heavy drinking military veterans (no report on any exclusions from analyses) Alcohol • IntensityD
OmaxD
Intensity was associated with endorsing more social, coping-anxiety, coping-depression, and enhancement drinking motives; Omax was associated with social and enhancement drinking motives and physical problems relating to alcohol; Intensity and Omax were associated with interpersonal alcohol problems (Indices assessed: IntensityO, OmaxO, ElasticityD)
Gentile et al. (2012) Assess the effects of academic constraints on alcohol demand among 164 college students in one experiment and 59 students in another (no report on any exclusions from analyses) Alcohol • IntensityD
• ElasticityD
Intensity was higher and Elasticity was lower in conditions with no academic constraints; Intensity was lower and Elasticity was higher for early class times and classes with exams compared to later class times (Indices assessed: IntensityD, OmaxD, PmaxD, ElasticityD)
Gray & MacKillop (2014) Examine the relationship between demand and alcohol misuse, sex differences in demand, and the relationship between demand and impulsive personality traits among 787 adult smokers (720 used in final analyses, except for Elasticity, which included 684 participants) Alcohol • IntensityO
OmaxO
• BreakpointO
• ElasticityD
Those with higher negative urgency had higher Intensity and Omax; Intensity, Omax, Breakpoint, and Elasticity were all correlated with Alcohol Use Disorder Identification Test scores; males had higher Intensity and Omax and lower Elasticity than females (Indices assessed: IntensityO, Omax O, BreakpointO, ElasticityD)
Herschl et al. (2012) Examine whether greater implicit (alcohol PT) and explicit drinking motivations would predict alcohol-related risk among 297 college binge drinkers (no report on any exclusions from analyses) Alcohol • IntensityO
OmaxO
• BreakpointO
• ElasticityD
Intensity was correlated with age; males had higher Intensity and Omax; Intensity and Breakpoint were correlated with Rutgers Alcohol Problem Index scores; Intensity, Omax, and Breakpoint correlated with Alcohol Use Disorder Identification Test scores and predicted alcohol-related risk; Intensity, Omax and Elasticity were correlated with age of initiation (Indices assessed: IntensityO, OmaxO, PmaxO, BreakpointO, ElasticityD)
Kiselica & Borders (2013) Determine whether Intensity and Omax mediated the association between four facets of impulsivity and negative drinking outcomes among 247 college drinkers (202 used in final analyses) Alcohol • IntensityO
OmaxO
Intensity was correlated with Urgency, Premeditation, and Sensation Seeking; Omax was correlated with Urgency; males reported higher Intensity and Omax; Intensity and Omax were correlated with more alcohol use and Young Adult Alcohol Consequences Questionnaire scores (Indices assessed: IntensityO, OmaxO)
Lemley et al. (2016) Assess relations between delay discounting, demand, and Young Adult Alcohol Consequences Questionnaire (YAACQ) scores among 115 college drinkers (80 used in final analyses) Alcohol • IntensityD
• BreakpointO
Intensity predicted higher scores on the YAACQ; males had higher Intensity and Breakpoint (Indices assessed: IntensityD, Omax D, PmaxD, BreakpointO, ElasticityD)
Lemley et al. (2017) Examine the relationships between alcohol demand and money, alcohol, and sexual partner delay discounting among 108 college students (85 used in final analyses) Alcohol • IntensityD Intensity was a significant predictor of drinks per week in both typical and heaviest drinking weeks and a predictor of Sexual Risk Survey scores (Indices assessed: IntensityD, ElasticityD)
Luehring-Jones et al. (2016) Extend previous research examining the effects of implicit associations and deficits in self-regulation on alcohol use among 36 college drinkers (no report on any exclusions from analyses) Alcohol • IntensityO, D
OmaxO
• ElasticityD
Intensity was associated with drinking consumption; Intensity was associated with implicit associations between alcohol and approach; Omax predicted drinks per day; interactions between delay discounting and implicit associations predicted Elasticity; alcohol and approach associations predicted Intensity and Elasticity (Indices assessed: IntensityO, D, OmaxO, PmaxO, BreakpointO, ElasticityD)
MacKillop & Murphy (2007) Examine if PT can predict drinking outcomes after a brief intervention among 67 college students (54 used in final analyses) Alcohol OmaxO
PmaxO
• BreakpointO
• ElasticityD
Omax, Pmax, Breakpoint, and Elasticity predicted weekly alcohol use post-intervention; Omax and Breakpoint predicted heavy drinking at 6-month follow-up (Indices assessed: IntensityO, OmaxO, PmaxO, BreakpointO, ElasticityD)
Morris et al. (2017) Validate an alcohol PT and a questionnaire measure of proportionate alcohol reinforcement using an online sample among 976 MTurk users (844 used in final analyses) Alcohol • IntensityO
OmaxO
• BreakpointO
• ElasticityD
Males had higher Intensity; Intensity uniquely predicted Alcohol Use Disorder Identification Test (AUDIT) scores; Intensity, Omax, Breakpoint, and Elasticity were correlated with AUDIT scores; Omax, Breakpoint and Elasticity also predicted AUDIT scores but not uniquely (Indices assessed: IntensityO, Omax O, PmaxO, BreakpointO, ElasticityD)
Murphy et al. (2009) Evaluate the reliability and validity of an alcohol PT among 38 college students (no report on any exclusions from analyses) Alcohol • IntensityO, D
OmaxO, D
• ElasticityD
Intensity, Omax, and Elasticity were correlated with weekly drinking and the Young Adult Alcohol Problems Screening Test (YAAPST) scores; Intensity predicted YAAPST scores after controlling for weekly drinking (Indices assessed: IntensityO, D, OmaxO, D, PmaxO, D, BreakpointO, ElasticityD)
Murphy et al. (2013) Test whether symptoms of depression and PTSD are uniquely associated with elevated alcohol demand among 133 college students (no exclusions except only 93 used in Elasticity analyses) Alcohol • IntensityO
OmaxO
PmaxO
• BreakpointO
• ElasticityD
Intensity and Elasticity were correlated with depressive symptoms; Intensity, Omax, and Elasticity were correlated with drinks per week and binge episodes; all indices were correlated with PTSD symptoms (Indices assessed: IntensityO, OmaxO,PmaxO, BreakpointO, ElasticityD)
Murphy et al. (2014) Examine the association between family history of alcohol misuse and individual differences in both alcohol demand and sensitivity of demand to next-day responsibilities among 207 college drinkers (no report on any exclusions from analyses) Alcohol • IntensityO
OmaxO
• BreakpointO
Intensity, Omax, and change in Intensity were correlated with drinks per week; Drinkers with family history had smaller reductions in Intensity and Breakpoint if they had a next-day responsibility; Intensity, Omax, and Breakpoint were correlated with Young Adult Alcohol Consequences Questionnaire scores (Indices assessed: IntensityO, OmaxO, BreakpointO)
Murphy et al. (2015) Determine whether PT indices, measured before and after a brief alcohol intervention, predict treatment response among 133 heavy drinking college students (no report on any exclusions from analyses) Alcohol • IntensityO
OmaxO
Intensity and Omax were positively correlated with typical weekly drinking and Young Adult Alcohol Consequences Questionnaire (YAACQ) scores; Intensity and Omax were reduced immediately post-intervention; Intensity predicted drinks per week and YAACQ scores at 1-month follow up; reductions in Intensity and Omax post-intervention predicted drinking reductions at 1-month follow-up (Indices assessed: IntensityO, OmaxO)
Murphy & MacKillop (2006) Evaluate the adequacy of the demand equation for describing alcohol consumption, the divergent validity of the indices and their associations to measures of alcohol use, and provide descriptive data on the impact of drink prices on students’ heavy episodic drinking among 267 college students (no report on any exclusions from analyses) Alcohol • IntensityO, D
OmaxO, D
• BreakpointO
Intensity and Omax were correlated with Rutgers Alcohol Problem Index scores; Intensity, Omax, and Breakpoint were correlated with drinks per week and heavy drinking episodes per week; Intensity, Omax, and Breakpoint differed between heavy vs. light drinkers (Indices assessed: IntensityO, D, OmaxO, D, PmaxO, D, BreakpointO, ElasticityD)
Owens et al. (2015a) Determine whether a brief PT would be sensitive in detecting changes in demand after exposure to cues, whether cue-elicited changes in a brief PT were similar to those using a full PT, and distinguish differences between cue- elicited changes in demand and craving among 84 adult heavy drinkers (no report on any exclusions from analyses) Alcohol • IntensityO
OmaxO
• BreakpointO
Intensity, Omax, and Breakpoint were associated with craving and were higher after exposure to alcohol cues compared to neutral cues (Indices assessed: IntensityO, OmaxO, BreakpointO)
Owens et al. (2015b) Use a PT to determine if a personalized stress induction would increase demand compared to a neutral induction among 64 adult non-treatment seeking heavy drinkers (62 used in final analyses) Alcohol • IntensityO
OmaxO
• BreakpointO
• ElasticityD
Intensity was associated with craving after neutral induction; those with higher income had greater Omax after stress induction; Intensity, Omax, and Breakpoint were higher and Elasticity was lower following stress induction compared to neutral induction (Indices assessed: IntensityO, OmaxO, BreakpointO, ElasticityD)
Skidmore & Murphy (2011) Examine next-day academic responsibilities and drink price on reported alcohol consumption among 207 college students who reported heavy drinking (no report on any exclusions from analyses) Alcohol • IntensityO
OmaxO
PmaxO
• BreakpointO
• ElasticityD
Males had higher Intensity, lower Pmax, and lower Breakpoint compared to females; men had higher Intensity overall, although differences were smaller if there were next day responsibilities; Intensity and Omax were higher and Elasticity was lower for high sensation seeking drinkers; Intensity, Omax, Pmax, and Breakpoint were higher and Elasticity was lower when there were no academic responsibilities the following day (Indices assessed: IntensityO, OmaxO, PmaxO, BreakpointO, ElasticityD)
Snider et al. (2016) Examine the effects of engaging alcohol-dependent individuals in an Episode Future Thinking (EFT) group or Episodic Recent Thinking (ERT) group to examine effects on demand using 55 adults with alcohol dependence (37 used in final analyses) Alcohol • IntensityD Intensity was lower in the EFT group compared to the control ERT group (Indices assessed: IntensityD, ElasticityD)
Strickland et al. (2016b) Provide support for the validity and generalizability of an exponentiated model for cocaine, alcohol, and cigarette PT among 40 cocaine using adults (37 used in final analyses) Cocaine, Alcohol, and Cigarettes • IntensityD
• ElasticityD
Alcohol Intensity and Elasticity were correlated with drinks per week, Alcohol Use Disorder Identification Test scores, and AUD symptoms; males had higher alcohol Intensity and lower alcohol Elasticity than females (Indices assessed: IntensityD, ElasticityD)
Strickland & Stoops (2017) Evaluate the stimulus selectivity of drug purchase tasks in 166 MTurk users (139 used in final analyses) Alcohol and Cigarettes • IntensityD
• BreakpointO
• ElasticityD
Alcohol Intensity and Breakpoint were correlated with drinks per weeks, days drinking per week, past-month severity of binge drinking; alcohol Elasticity was negatively associated with the above measures (Indices assessed: IntensityD, BreakpointO, ElasticityD)
Teeters et al. (2014) Examine whether elevated demand was associated with driving after drinking among 207 heavy drinking college students (no reports on any exclusions from analyses) Alcohol • IntensityO
OmaxO
• BreakpointO
• ElasticityD
Intensity and Omax were correlated with drinks per week; Intensity, Omax, and Elasticity were correlated with driving after drinking (Indices assessed: IntensityO, OmaxO, PmaxO, BreakpointO, ElasticityD)
Teeters & Murphy (2015) Examine the association between demand and drinking after driving; determine whether demand decreases in response to a hypothetical driving scenario; determine whether drivers who report drinking after driving show less of a reduction in demand after a driving scenario among 419 college students (no reports on any exclusions from analyses) Alcohol • IntensityO
OmaxO
• BreakpointO
• ElasticityD
Intensity, Omax, Breakpoint, and Elasticity predicted engaging in intoxicated driving; those endorsing intoxicated driving had less of a reduction across Intensity, Omax, and Breakpoint from a standard PT to one involving a driving scenario (Indices assessed: IntensityO, OmaxO, BreakpointO, ElasticityD)
Tucker et al. (2016a) Evaluate which different behavioral economic measures were associated with drinking problem severity and outcomes of natural recovery attempts among 191 problem drinkers trying to quit drinking on their own (152 used in final analyses) Alcohol • IntensityO
• ElasticityD
Higher Intensity predicted higher quantities of alcohol consumed, high scores on the Alcohol Dependence Scale, and fewer days well-functioning; higher Elasticity predicted lower quantities of alcohol consumed and more days well-functioning (Indices assessed: IntensityO, OmaxO, PmaxO, BreakpointO, ElasticityD)
Tucker et al. (2016b) Evaluate the predictive utility of an Alcohol-Savings Discretionary Expenditure index with multiple behavioral economic measures of impulsivity and self-control to predict outcomes of natural recovery attempts among 245 adult problem drinkers (175 used in final analyses) Alcohol • N/A No significant findings relating to PT indices were reported (Indices assessed: IntensityO, Omax O, PmaxO, BreakpointO, ElasticityD)
Wahlstrom et al. (2012) Examine whether a certain genotype would moderate the relationship between alexithymia and a PT among 136 male college binge drinkers (120 used in final analyses) Alcohol • IntensityO
• BreakpointO
• ElasticityD
Intensity was correlated with Alcohol Use Disorder Identification Test scores; Intensity and Elasticity were correlated with alexithymia; those with the DRD2/ANKK1 TaqI A1+ allele type had higher Breakpoint; among those with an A1+ allele type, greater alexithymia predicted less Elasticity (Indices assessed: IntensityO, OmaxO, PmaxO, BreakpointO, ElasticityD)
Weinstock et al. (2016) Examine a gambling PT as a measure gambling using the behavioral economic model employed with the alcohol PT, using the alcohol PT for discriminant validity, for 73 adults across three groups: 28 adults with Gambling Disorder, 24 adults with AUD, and 21 healthy controls (no reports on any exclusions from analyses) Alcohol • IntensityO
OmaxO
PmaxO
• BreakpointO
• ElasticityD
Those with AUD had higher Intensity, Omax, Pmax, and Breakpoint and lower Elasticity on the alcohol PT (Indices assessed: IntensityO, OmaxO, PmaxO, BreakpointO, ElasticityD)
Yurasek et al. (2011) Examine enhancement and coping drinking motives as mediators between demand and alcohol use and problems among 255 college drinkers (215 used in final analyses) Alcohol • IntensityO
OmaxO
PmaxO
• BreakpointO
• ElasticityD
Intensity predicted enhancement and coping motives; Omax predicted enhancement motives; Intensity and Omax were correlated with binge drinking episodes and predicted alcohol use; Intensity and Omax were correlated with coping motives; Intensity, Omax, and Pmax were correlated with peak number of drinks; Intensity, Omax, and Breakpoint were correlated with enhancement motives; Intensity, Omax, Breakpoint, and Elasticity were correlated with drinks per week and Young Adult Alcohol Consequences Questionnaire scores (Indices assessed: IntensityO, Omax O, PmaxO, BreakpointO, ElasticityD)
Yurasek et al. (2013) Investigate whether heavy drinking smokers would have greater demand for alcohol than heavy drinking non-smokers among 207 college drinkers (no reports on any exclusions from analyses) Alcohol • IntensityO
OmaxO
PmaxO
• BreakpointO
• ElasticityD
Alcohol Intensity and Omax were correlated with nicotine dependence scores; smokers had higher Omax, Breakpoint, and Pmax; heavier smokers (7+ cigarettes per day) had higher Omax and Breakpoint; Elasticity was correlated with Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale scores; Intensity, Omax, and Elasticity were correlated with alcohol consumption; Intensity, Omax, Breakpoint, and Elasticity were correlated with Young Adult Alcohol Consequences Questionnaire scores; all indices were correlated with smoking status (Indices assessed: IntensityO, OmaxO, PmaxO, BreakpointO, ElasticityD)

Note.

O

denotes when indices were observed from raw purchase task data

D

denotes when indices were derived from an equation.

Figure 3.

Figure 3.

Percentage of articles reporting significant associations between the 5 HPT indices and substance-related correlates and outcomes within studies examining alcohol (top panel), cigarettes (middle panel), and other substances (bottom panel).

A similar pattern emerged when examining effect sizes. A large effect size was observed with Intensity (0.81 ± 0.05, CI 0.71–0.91). A moderate effect size was observed with Omax (0.62 ± 0.05, CI 0.53–0.71). Smaller effect sizes were observed with Elasticity (0.41 ± 0.05, CI 0.32–0.51), Breakpoint (0.34 ± 0.03, CI 0.29–0.39), and Pmax (0.15 ± 0.03, CI 0.09–0.21). Omnibus F tests showed significant differences between overall mean effect sizes, and within cross-sectional and longitudinal/experimental studies (Supplemental Table 1). For the sets of overall and cross-sectional effect sizes, there were significant differences between means (p < 0.0001 for each), but for the longitudinal effect sizes, the difference across all means was not significant (p = 0.19). Based on 95% confidence intervals for the overall mean effect sizes associated with each of the indices, Intensity and Omax had significantly greater mean effect sizes than each of the other indices but did not differ significantly from each other. Elasticity had significantly greater mean effect size than Pmax but not Breakpoint; Breakpoint produced larger effect sizes than Pmax. Similar patterns were observed across cross-sectional and longitudinal/experimental studies.

Associations between the Cigarette Purchase Task indices and Cigarette/Nicotine-Related Correlates and Outcomes

Associations between Cigarette Purchase Task (CPT) indices and cigarette/nicotine-related correlates and outcomes were reported in 34 articles (Barnes et al., 2017; Bidwell et al., 2012; Chase et al., 2013; Dahne et al., 2017; Farris et al., 2017a, 2017b; Grace et al., 2015a, 2015b, 2015c; Gray et al., 2017; Higgins et al., 2017a, 2017b; Hindocha et al., 2017; Johnson et al., 2017; MacKillop et al., 2008, 2012, 2014, 2016; MacKillop & Tidey, 2011; Murphy et al., 2011, 2016, 2017; O’Connor et al., 2012, 2014, 2016; Peters et al., 2017; Schlienz et al., 2014; Secades-Villa et al., 2016, 2018; Smith et al., 2017; Snider et al., 2017; Strickland et al., 2016b, 2017; Tucker et al., 2018); (Table 3). Demographic, psychosocial, and psychiatric characteristics associated with increased risk for smoking in addition to symptoms of dependence and number of cigarettes smoked were examined in these articles. Additionally, 5 of these reports examined individual differences related to response to clinical interventions (Higgins et al., 2017b; MacKillop et al., 2016; Murphy et al., 2017; Schlienz et al., 2014; Secades-Villa et al., 2016) and 11 examined individual differences related to response to regulatory policy interventions (Grace et al., 2015a, 2015b, 2015c; Higgins, et al., 2017a; Hindocha et al., 2017; Johnson et al., 2017; Murphy et al., 2016; O’Connor et al., 2012; Smith et al., 2017; Snider et al., 2017; Tucker et al., 2018). Statistically significant associations between CPT indices and cigarette/nicotine product-related correlates and outcomes were most often reported for Intensity (84%, 26/31), followed by Omax (80%, 20/25), Elasticity (79%, 26/33), Breakpoint (56%, 15/27), and Pmax (50%, 12/24; Figure 3, middle panel).

Table 3.

Significant associations between PT indices and individual differences relating to cigarettes.

Authors Purpose Substance Significant Predictors Results
Barnes et al. (2017) Examine e-cigarettes’ abuse liability compared to conventional tobacco cigarettes under flavor and message conditions amenable to regulation among 62 adult smokers (36 used in final analyses) Cigarettes and E-cigarettes (E-cigs) • IntensityO
• ElasticityD
Intensity was higher for cigarettes than e-cigs; Elasticity was higher for menthol e-cig flavor with no warning message and unflavored e-cig with reduced exposure message than cigarettes; Elasticity across all e-cig conditions was higher than for cigarettes (Indices assessed: IntensityO, Omax O, PmaxO, BreakpointO, ElasticityD)
Bidwell et al. (2012) Quantify the relationships between demand, cigarette consumption, and nicotine dependence among 138 adolescent smokers (no report on any exclusions from analyses) Cigarettes • IntensityO
OmaxO
PmaxO
• BreakpointO
• ElasticityD
Intensity, Omax, and Breakpoint were negatively correlated with motivation to quit; Omax was correlated with cigarettes per day; Omax, Pmax, and Elasticity were correlated with higher modified Fagerström Tolerance Questionnaire scores; Omax, Pmax, Breakpoint, and Elasticity were correlated with breath carbon monoxide and urinary cotinine levels (Indices assessed: IntensityO, OmaxO, PmaxO, BreakpointO, ElasticityD)
Chase et al. (2013) Reproduce significant associations of a cigarette PT with nicotine dependence and contrast this to a novel chocolate PT among 287 smokers aged 18–25 (237 used in final analyses) Cigarettes • IntensityO
OmaxO
PmaxO
• BreakpointO
• ElasticityD
Intensity, Omax, and Breakpoint predicted dependence scores on the Cigarette Dependence Scale-5; All indices were correlated with dependence scores on Cigarette Dependences Scale-5 (Indices assessed: IntensityO, Omax O, PmaxO, BreakpointO, ElasticityD)s
Dahne et al. (2017) Examine the interaction between depressive symptoms and change in negative affect as a function of induced mood as a predictor of cigarette demand among 104 college smokers between the ages of 18 and 21 (73 used in final analyses) Cigarettes • IntensityO
• BreakpointO
PmaxO
Among those with a large change in negative affect, depressive symptoms were positively associated with Pmax and Breakpoint; among those with small changes in negative affect, depressive symptoms were positively associated with Intensity (Indices assessed: IntensityO, OmaxO, PmaxO, BreakpointO, ElasticityD)
Farris et al. (2017a) Examine the association between psychopathology and tobacco demand in 126 non-treatment seeking adult daily smokers (111 used in final analyses) Cigarettes • IntensityO
OmaxO
• ElasticityD
Smokers with an emotional disorder and smokers with 2 or more disorders had higher Intensity; Intensity and Omax were correlated with cigarettes per day and Fagerström Test for Nicotine Dependence scores; Smokers with any psychopathology had higher Intensity and Omax; smokers with a substance use disorder (SUD) had higher Intensity, Omax, and lower Elasticity (Indices assessed: IntensityO, OmaxO, PmaxO, BreakpointO, ElasticityD)
Farris et al. (2017b) Examine differences in demand and delay discounting and their association with smoking topography among 126 adult smokers with and without past-year psychopathology (107 used in final analyses) Cigarettes • IntensityO
OmaxO
PmaxO
• BreakpointO
• ElasticityD
Intensity and Omax were higher for smokers with psychopathology compared to those without; Omax was associated with number of puffs; Pmax, Breakpoint, and Elasticity were associated with inter-puff interval; for those with psychopathology, Elasticity was associated with time spent smoking (Indices assessed: IntensityO, OmaxO, PmaxO, BreakpointO, ElasticityD)
Grace et al. (2015a) Measure the cross-price elasticity of e-cigarettes and demand for cigarettes both in the presence and absence of e-cigarettes among 226 adults who never tried e-cigs (210 used in final analyses) Cigarettes and E-cigs • IntensityO
OmaxO
• ElasticityD
Intensity, Omax, and Elasticity were correlated with overall e-cig demand (Indices assessed: IntensityO, OmaxO, BreakpointO, ElasticityD)
Grace et al. (2015b) Assess the temporal stability of a PT before and after a tax increase among 226 adult smokers (210 used in final analyses) Cigarettes • IntensityD
OmaxO
• Elasticity D
Intensity and Omax were positively correlated and Elasticity was negatively correlated with cigarettes per day; Fagerström Test for Nicotine Dependence scores; Elasticity increased after tax increase (Indices assessed: IntensityD, OmaxO, BreakpointO, ElasticityD)
Grace et al. (2015c) Examine whether price elasticities for smokers can predict changes in smoking after a tax increase among 357 adult smokers (211 used in final analyses) Cigarettes • IntensityO, D
OmaxO, D
• ElasticityD
Intensity and Omax were positively and Elasticity was negatively correlated with cigarettes per day; Fagerström Test for Nicotine Dependence, Autonomy Over Smoking Scale, and Glover-Nilsson Smoking Behavior Questionnaire scores; Intensity and Omax were positively correlated and Elasticity was negatively correlated with change in cigarettes per day after tax increase (Indices assessed: IntensityO, D, OmaxO, D, PmaxO, BreakpointO, ElasticityD)
Gray et al. (2017) Examine the neural correlates of tobacco demand among 44 non-treatment seeking male smokers (33 used in final analyses) Cigarettes • ElasticityD Elasticity was negatively associated with activity in the left anterior insula (Indices assessed: ElasticityD)
Higgins et al. (2017a) Examine how smokers with psychiatric disorders and other vulnerabilities to tobacco addiction respond to cigarettes with reduced nicotine content among 169 smokers from 3 vulnerable populations (with affective disorders, with opioid dependence, and Socioeconomically disadvantaged women; no report of any exclusions from analyses) Cigarettes • IntensityO
OmaxO
PmaxO
• BreakpointO
Intensity was higher for those with opioid dependence than women with low socioeconomic status; nicotine dose in the reduced content cigarettes was positively associated with Intensity, Omax, Pmax, and Breakpoint (Indices assessed: IntensityO, OmaxO, PmaxO, BreakpointO, ElasticityD)
Higgins et al. (2017b) Examine whether PT can distinguish between women with vs. without risk factors for continued smoking during pregnancy and predict differences in quit attempts among 95 pregnant smokers (93 used in final analyses) Cigarettes • IntensityO, D
OmaxO
• BreakpointO
• ElasticityD
Intensity and Omax were higher for those who smoked 10+ cigarettes per day (CPD) and for those with no pre-pregnancy quit attempts, Intensity and Omax distinguished between making quit attempts for those who smoked less than 10 CPD; Elasticity was lower for those who smoked 10+ CPD; Omax and Breakpoint were independent predictors of antepartum quit attempts (Indices assessed: IntensityO, D, OmaxO, PmaxO, BreakpointO, ElasticityD)
Hindocha et al. (2017) Investigate how cannabis and tobacco, each alone and combined together, affected individuals’ demand for cannabis puffs and cigarettes among 24 adult recreational cannabis and tobacco co-users (no report on any exclusions from analyses) Cigarettes and Marijuana OmaxO
• BreakpointO
Acute administration of active cannabis decreased Omax and Breakpoint for cigarettes compared to placebo cannabis (Indices assessed: IntensityO, Omax O, PmaxO, BreakpointO, ElasticityD)
Johnson et al. (2017) Examine cross-price elasticity of e-cigs and nicotine gum compared to cigarettes and their ability to decrease consumption of cigarettes among 400 adult M-Turk users (326 used in final analyses) Cigarettes, E-cigs, and Nicotine Gum • IntensityO
PmaxD
• ElasticityD
Intensity was higher and Elasticity was lower for e-cigs than cigarettes; Intensity and Pmax were lower and Elasticity was higher for cigarettes when e-cigs were offered concurrently; cross price elasticity was higher for e-cigs than cigarettes suggesting substitutability (Indices assessed: IntensityO, PmaxD, ElasticityD)
MacKillop et al. (2008) Validate a cigarette PT in 33 college smokers (31 used in final analyses) Cigarettes • IntensityO, D
OmaxO, D
Intensity and Omax were associated with Fagerström Test for Nicotine Dependence scores and cigarettes per day (Indices assessed: IntensityO, D, OmaxO, D, PmaxO, D, BreakpointO, ElasticityD)
MacKillop et al. (2012) Examine whether the subjective craving from acute withdrawal and exposure to tobacco cues dynamically increases the relative value of cigarettes among 41 nicotine dependent adults (33 used in final analyses) Cigarettes PmaxO
• BreakpointO
• ElasticityD
Deprivation increased Pmax and Breakpoint; tobacco cues reduced Elasticity (Indices assessed: IntensityO, OmaxO, PmaxO, BreakpointO, ElasticityD)
MacKillop et al. (2013) Apply a behavioral economic approach to the relationship between the price of cigarettes and the probability of attempting smoking cessation among 1124 adult smokers (1070 used in final analyses) Cigarettes • Intensity Intensity was associated with Fagerström Test for Nicotine Dependence scores (Indices assessed: IntensityO, BreakpointO)
MacKillop et al. (2016) Investigate whether PT predicted response to contingent monetary rewards for abstinence among 338 adults with substance use disorders (SUD; no report on any exclusions from analyses) Cigarettes • IntensityO
OmaxO
• ElasticityD
Intensity, Omax, and Elasticity predicted abstinence in the noncontingent voucher group (Indices assessed: IntensityO, OmaxO, BreakpointO, ElasticityD)
MacKillop & Tidey (2011) Compare smokers with schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorders (SS) and control smokers (CS) on demand and delay discounting for 49 adult smokers (35 used in final analyses) Cigarettes • IntensityO
OmaxO
The SS group had higher Intensity than CS group; Intensity and Omax were associated with Fagerström Test for Nicotine Dependence scores (Indices assessed: IntensityO, OmaxO, PmaxO, BreakpointO, ElasticityD)
Murphy et al. (2011) Evaluate the construct validity of a cigarette PT among 138 adolescent smokers (no report on any exclusions from analyses) Cigarettes • IntensityO
OmaxO
PmaxO
• BreakpointO
• ElasticityD
Intensity was associated with motivation to change; Intensity and Omax were associated with Fagerström Test for Nicotine Dependence scores; Intensity, Omax, Breakpoint, and Elasticity were correlated with cigarettes per day and modified Fagerström Tolerance Questionnaire (MFTQ) scores; students with higher MFTQ scores reported higher Omax and Pmax; students with MFTQ scores above 6 had higher Intensity than those with no dependence and had higher Breakpoint than those with moderate dependence (MFTQ scores between 3–5; (Indices assessed: IntensityO, Omax O, PmaxO, BreakpointO, ElasticityD)
Murphy et al. (2016) Quantify the substitutability of food and cigarettes, examine the cross-price elasticity of food and cigarettes, and examine associations between weight-related variables and weight efficacy after quitting smoking among 86 adult smokers (no report on exclusions from any analyses) Cigarettes • ElasticityD For those with higher perceived ability to quit smoking without gaining weight, the cross-commodity elasticity for food was higher when the cost of cigarettes increased (i.e., they were more likely to increase food purchasing) and the cross-commodity elasticity for cigarettes was lower when the price of food increased (less likely to increase cigarette purchasing; (Indices assessed: ElasticityD)
Murphy et al. (2017) Evaluate the effects of varenicline vs. nicotine replacement therapy on demand for cigarettes among smokers with SUD and whether demand predicted abstinence at 1- and 3-month follow-up among 110 adults with SUD (no report on exclusions from any analyses) Cigarettes • IntensityO
OmaxO
• BreakpointO
• ElasticityD
Intensity, Omax, and Elasticity were correlated with baseline cigarettes per day; Intensity, Omax, Breakpoint, and Elasticity were correlated with baseline Fagerström scores; Initial decrease in Intensity and Breakpoint was associated with increased likelihood of abstinence on quit day and predicted longest number of days of continuous abstinence at 1-month follow-up; reduced Intensity predicted greater likelihood of abstinence at 1-month follow-up and longest number of days of continuous abstinence at 3-month follow-up (Indices assessed: IntensityO, OmaxO, BreakpointO, ElasticityD)
O’Connor et al. (2012) Examine how current smokers might respond to a ban on menthol cigarettes using 471 adolescent and adult smokers (453 used in final analyses) Cigarettes • IntensityD
OmaxD
• ElasticityD
Intensity, Omax, and Elasticity were associated with cigarettes per day; Intensity and Omax were higher and Elasticity was lower for preferred cigarette type (menthol vs. non-menthol); Elasticity was lower for those who were likely to use black market cigarettes (Indices assessed: IntensityD, OmaxD, ElasticityD)
O’Connor et al. (2014) Explore how advertising affects demand for cigarettes and potential substitutes (e.g., snus, dissolvable tobacco, and medicinal nicotine) among 1,062 adult smokers (no report on exclusions from any analyses) Cigarettes and alternative nicotine product • IntensityD
• ElasticityD
Intensity was positively correlated with craving; higher Intensity and lower Elasticity were associated with Fagerström Test for Nicotine Dependence and Questionnaire for Smoking Urges scores; Intensity was higher for cigarettes compared to snus, dissolvables and lozenges; Elasticity was higher for snus than dissolvables and lozenges; Elasticity for cigarettes was correlated with cross-price elasticity for alternative products (Indices assessed: IntensityD, ElasticityD)
O’Connor et al. (2016) Examine how PT indices relate to quit intentions and quit attempts among 3,001 adult daily cigarette smokers (1,1194 used in final analyses) Cigarettes • IntensityD
OmaxO
PmaxO
• BreakpointO
• ElasticityD
All indices were correlated with Fagerström Test for Nicotine Dependence scores (Indices assessed: IntensityD, OmaxO, PmaxO, BreakpointO, ElasticityD)
Peters et al. (2017) Examine demand for cigarettes and marijuana independently and concurrently among 105 adult MTurk users who use both cigarettes and marijuana (82 used in final analyses) Cigarettes and Marijuana • ElasticityD Elasticity for cigarettes was lower for males, lower for those with high nicotine dependence, and lower for those who smoked their first cigarette within 30 minutes of waking (Indices assessed: ElasticityD)
Schlienz et al. (2014) Test the reinforcement-reduction hypothesis for varenicline among 60 adult treatment-seeking smokers (52 used in final analyses) Cigarettes • N/A No significant findings relating to PT indices were reported (Indices assessed: IntensityD, BreakpointO, ElasticityD)
Secades-Villa et al. (2016) Explore PT indices as predictors of smoking abstinence among participants
receiving cognitive behavioral treatment (CBT) combined with contingency management (CM) versus CBT alone among 168 adult smokers (159 used in final analyses)
Cigarettes • IntensityO
OmaxO
PmaxO
• BreakpointO
• ElasticityD
Intensity was correlated with years of smoking and breath carbon monoxide levels; Intensity, Omax, Breakpoint, and Elasticity were correlated with cigarettes per day;
Omax, Pmax, Breakpoint, and Elasticity were correlated with SCID7 symptoms of dependence; all indices were correlated with Nicotine Dependence Syndrome Scale scores; Higher Elasticity was a significant predictor for more days of abstinence for those in the CBT & CM condition (Indices assessed: IntensityO, Omax O, PmaxO, BreakpointO, ElasticityD)
Secades-Villa et al. (2018) Compare cigarette demand among 165 smokers with low and elevated depressive symptoms (no report of exclusions for any analyses) Cigarettes • BreakpointO
•ElasticityD
Cigarettes per day and years of regular smoking predicted Breakpoint; depressive symptoms predicted Breakpoint; Elasticity was negatively correlated with nicotine dependence and depressive symptoms (Indices assessed: IntensityO, Omax O, PmaxO, BreakpointO, ElasticityD)
Smith et al. (2017) Assess the impact of a reduction in the nicotine content of cigarettes on PT for reduced content cigarettes and usual brand cigarettes among 839 adult smokers (540–773 used in final analyses) Cigarettes and Reduced Nicotine Content Cigarettes • IntensityO
OmaxO
PmaxO
• BreakpointO
• ElasticityD
Intensity was lower for usual brand at 6-weeks and 24-hour abstinence visits; Omax was lower for usual brand at 6-weeks; Elasticity was lower at 24-hour abstinence visit for lower nicotine content cigarettes; Intensity, Omax, Pmax, and Breakpoint were lower at 6-weeks and 24-hour abstinence visits for lower nicotine content cigarettes (Indices assessed: IntensityO, OmaxO, PmaxO, BreakpointO, ElasticityD)
Snider et al. (2017) Examine how individuals purchase cigarettes and e-cigs alone and in combination as a function of price and the frequency with which they currently use e-cigs among 385 adult M-turk smokers who smoke 10+ cigarettes per day (38–104 used in final analyses) Cigarettes and E-cigs • ElasticityD Elasticity was correlated with more frequent E-cig use in the cigarette only and the concurrent PT in that E-cig users had higher Elasticity for cigarettes alone than those who never used e-cigs (Indices assessed: IntensityD, ElasticityD)
Strickland et al. (2016b) Provide support for the validity and generalizability of an exponentiated model for cocaine, alcohol, and cigarette PT among 40 cocaine using adults (39 used in final analyses) Cocaine, Alcohol, and Cigarettes • IntensityD
• ElasticityD
Cigarette Intensity was higher for those with higher Fagerström Test for Nicotine Dependence scores and those with lifetime cocaine use; cigarette Intensity and Elasticity were correlated with higher Drug Abuse Screening Test scores (Indices assessed: IntensityD, ElasticityD)
Strickland & Stoops (2017) Evaluate the stimulus selectivity of drug purchase tasks among 66 MTurk users (46 used in final analyses) Alcohol and Cigarettes • IntensityD Cigarette Intensity was positively correlated with cigarettes per day and Fagerström Test for Nicotine Dependence scores (Indices assessed: IntensityD, BreakpointO, ElasticityD)
Tucker et al. (2017) Evaluate demand for very low nicotine content cigarettes (VLNC) when available independently or concurrently with regular cigarettes among 40 adult smokers (36 used in final analyses) Cigarettes and VLNC • IntensityO, D
OmaxO, D
PmaxO, D
• BreakpointO
• ElasticityD
Intensity and Elasticity were correlated with higher Fagerström Test for Nicotine Dependence (FTND) scores; Intensity, Omax, and Pmax for regular cigarettes and Intensity for VLNC cigarettes were positively correlated with FTND scores; Intensity, Omax, and Breakpoint were higher for regular cigarettes; Omax and Pmax for VLNC cigarettes was positively associated with cross price elasticity (Indices assessed: IntensityO, D, OmaxO, D, PmaxO, BreakpointO, ElasticityD)

Note.

O

denotes when indices were observed from raw purchase task data

D

denotes when indices were derived from an equation.

A similar pattern emerged when examining effect sizes. Moderate effect sizes were observed with Intensity (0.70 ± 0.07, CI 0.56–0.85), Omax (0.66 ± 0.08, CI 0.50–0.81) and Elasticity (0.52 ± 0.06, CI 0.41–0.63). Smaller effect sizes were observed with Pmax (0.31 ± 0.07, CI 0.16–0.45), and Breakpoint (0.27 ± 0.04, CI 0.18–0.36). Omnibus F tests showed significant differences between overall mean effect sizes, and within cross-sectional and longitudinal/experimental studies (Supplemental Table 2). For each set of effect sizes, there were significant differences between means (p < 0.0001, p = 0.0005, p = 0.0007, respectively). Based on the 95% confidence intervals for the overall mean effect sizes, Intensity, Omax, and Elasticity had significantly greater mean effect sizes than Breakpoint but did not differ significantly from each other. Intensity and Omax but not Elasticity also had significantly greater mean effect sizes than Pmax. Similar patterns were observed across cross-sectional and longitudinal/experimental studies.

Associations between Other Substance Purchase Task indices and Substance-Related Correlates and Outcomes

Associations between Other Substance Purchase Task (OSPT) indices and substance-related correlates and outcomes were reported in 12 articles (Aston et al., 2015, 2017; Bruner & Johnson, 2014; Collins et al., 2014; Hindocha et al., 2017; Metrik et al., 2016; Peters et al., 2017; Pickover et al., 2016; Strickland et al., 2016a, 2016b; Strickland et al., 2017; Vincent et al., 2017; Table 4). Demographic, psychosocial, and psychiatric risk factors for problematic substance use were examined in these articles. One report examined individual differences related to response to a clinical intervention (Strickland et al., 2016b) and two reports examined individual differences related to response to regulatory policy (Hindocha et al., 2017; Peters et al., 2017). Statistically significant associations between OSPT indices and substance-related correlates and outcomes were most often reported for Intensity (75%, 9/12), Omax (75%, 6/8), Elasticity (75%, 9/12), followed by Pmax (57%, 4/7), and Breakpoint (29%, 2/7) (Figure 3, bottom panel).

Table 4.

Significant associations between Purchase Task indices and substance-related correlates and outcomes.

Authors Purpose Substance Significant Predictors Results
Aston et al. (2015) Examine marijuana demand using a marijuana PT among 104 non-treatment seeking frequent using adults (99 used in final analyses) Marijuana • IntensityO
OmaxO
• ElasticityD
Intensity was correlated with age of initiation of regular use;
Intensity and Omax were correlated with frequency of marijuana use; Intensity and Elasticity were associated with more DSM2 dependence symptoms; Intensity, Omax, and Elasticity were correlated with subjective craving (Indices assessed: IntensityO, OmaxO, PmaxO, BreakpointO, ElasticityD)
Aston et al. (2017) Determine whether metrics of reinforcement from a marijuana PT exhibit a latent factor structure that characterized demand among 99 regular (5 days/week) cannabis smokers (no report on any exclusions from analyses) Marijuana • IntensityO Intensity was associated with positive expectancies of use, craving severity, greater frequency of marijuana use, and DSM-IV dependence symptoms (Indices assessed: IntensityO, OmaxO, PmaxO, BreakpointO, ElasticityD)
Bruner & Johnson (2014) Examine the relation between demand metrics from a Cocaine PT and self-reported cocaine use among 86 cocaine-dependent individuals (74 used in final analyses) Cocaine • IntensityO, D
OmaxO, D
PmaxO, D
• ElasticityD
Intensity, Omax, Pmax, and Elasticity were correlated with money spent on cocaine daily and daily cocaine use; Pmax was correlated with cocaine purchased daily (Indices assessed: IntensityO, D, OmaxO, D, PmaxO, D, ElasticityD)
Collins et al. (2014) Utilize the first marijuana PT to examine the relative reinforcing efficacy of marijuana among 61 young adult cannabis users (59 used in final analyses) Marijuana • IntensityO
OmaxO
PmaxO
• ElasticityD
Intensity and Omax were positively associated with marijuana use; Pmax and Elasticity were negatively associated with marijuana use (Indices assessed: IntensityO, D, OmaxO, D, PmaxO, D, BreakpointO, ElasticityD)
Hindocha et al., (2017) Investigate how cannabis and tobacco, each alone and combined together, affected individuals’ demand for cannabis puffs and cigarettes among 24 adult recreational cannabis and tobacco co-users (no report on any exclusions from analyses) Cigarettes and Marijuana • N/A No significant findings relating to Marijuana PT indices were reported (Indices assessed: IntensityO, OmaxO, PmaxO, BreakpointO, ElasticityD)
Metrik et al. (2016) Examine dimensions of marijuana’s incentive salience and compare cue reactivity-induced increases in craving and demand among 93 adult frequent marijuana users (no report on exclusions from any analyses) Marijuana • IntensityO
OmaxO
• ElasticityD
Intensity and Omax were correlated with cue-elicited craving; Intensity and Omax increased while Elasticity decreased after exposure to marijuana cues compared to neutral cues; Intensity significantly predicted attentional bias for marijuana vs. neutral words on a Stroop task (Indices assessed: IntensityO, OmaxO, BreakpointO, ElasticityD)
Peters et al. (2017) Examine demand for cigarettes and marijuana independently and concurrently among 105 adult MTurk users who use both cigarettes and marijuana (82 used in final analyses) Cigarettes and Marijuana • ElasticityD Elasticity for marijuana was lower for those with high nicotine dependence; Elasticity was lower for women compared to men; For women, Elasticity for marijuana was
higher when cigarettes were available compared to when only marijuana was available (Indices assessed: IntensityD, ElasticityD)
Pickover et al. (2016) Introduce nonmedical use of prescription sedative/tranquilizer, stimulant, and opiate pain reliever PT, examine the relationship between demand and SUD symptoms, and examine sex differences among 393 college students (varying n’s used in final analyses depending on substance classification and missing data: 42–258) Prescription Drugs • IntensityO
OmaxO, D
PmaxO, D
• BreakpointO
• ElasticityD
Intensity, Omax, Pmax, and Breakpoint were correlated with sedative and pain reliever use disorder symptoms; Omax and Pmax predicted pain reliever use disorder symptoms in males; Omax, Pmax, and Breakpoint were correlated with past year and life time use of sedatives; all indices were correlated with pain reliever use and stimulant use disorder symptoms, past year & life time use for stimulants and pain
relievers (Indices assessed: IntensityO, OmaxO, PmaxO, BreakpointO, ElasticityD)
Strickland et al. (2016a) Examine the online extension of regulation of craving tasks; a cocaine PT was administered to assess relationship between demand and craving in an Internet sample of 47 cocaine users (44 used in final analyses) Cocaine • N/A No significant findings relating to PT indices were reported (Indices assessed: IntensityD, ElasticityD)
Strickland et al. (2016b) Provide support for the validity and generalizability of an exponentiated model for cocaine, alcohol, and cigarette PT among 40 cocaine using adults (37 used in final analyses) Cocaine, Alcohol, and Cigarettes • IntensityD
• ElasticityD
Cocaine Intensity was correlated with weekly and lifetime cocaine use; cocaine Elasticity was correlated with alcohol and cigarette Elasticity; males had lower cocaine Elasticity than females (Indices assessed: IntensityO, ElasticityD)
Strickland et al. (2017) Evaluate demand and delay-discounting measures for marijuana and alcohol to demonstrate the specificity of marijuana relevant outcomes for predicting marijuana use behaviors among 164 MTurk users (136 used in final analyses) Marijuana and Alcohol • IntensityD
• ElasticityD
Intensity and Elasticity were correlated with weekly marijuana use and cannabis use disorder symptoms; Intensity predicted frequency and amount used (Indices assessed: IntensityD, ElasticityD)
Vincent et al. (2017) Examine whether the reinforcing properties of marijuana varied as a function of perceived quality among 2531 young-adult nonprofit marijuana lobbying group members (683 used in final analyses) Marijuana • IntensityO, D
OmaxO, D
PmaxO, D
• BreakpointO
• ElasticityD
Intensity and Omax were correlated with weekly marijuana use and were associated with heavier weekly use across all grades of marijuana; Intensity, Pmax, and Breakpoint increased as quality improved; lower Breakpoint was associated with heavier use for medium grade marijuana; higher Elasticity was associated with heavier use for high grade marijuana; all indices were correlated with quality (Indices assessed: IntensityO, D, OmaxO, D, PmaxO, D, BreakpointO, ElasticityD)

Note.

O

denotes when indices were observed from raw purchase task data

D

denotes when indices were derived from an equation.

As expected with the relatively small number of studies included in this section, CIs were larger than in the other sections. Due to the relatively small number of studies using OSPTs, a further breakdown by cross-sectional and longitudinal/experimental study type is not reported. Moderate effect sizes were observed for Omax (0.68 ± 0.15, CI 0.40–0.97) and Intensity (0.61 ± 0.06, CI 0.50– 0.72). Smaller effect sizes were observed for Breakpoint (0.41 ± 0.07, CI −0.02–0.56), Pmax (0.34 ± 0.06, CI −0.02– 0.47), and Elasticity (0.29 ± 0.04, CI .08–0.31). Omnibus F tests showed significant differences between overall mean effect sizes, with significant differences observed between means F(4,24) = 6.13 (p = 0.002). Based on the 95% confidence intervals for the overall mean effect sizes, Intensity and Omax did not differ from each other but both produced larger effect sizes than Elasticity. Intensity also differed from Pmax but not Breakpoint; Omax did not differ from Pmax or Breakpoint. There were no other significant differences.

DISCUSSION

This review systematically replicates the findings of Kiselica and colleagues (2016) indicating that Intensity and Omax are the most sensitive of the HPT indices. This review extends these findings by demonstrating the generality of that pattern across multiples substances (e.g., alcohol, cigarettes, marijuana) and study designs (i.e., cross-sectional and longitudinal/experimental studies). This pattern was also evident when examining the proportion of studies reporting statistically significant effects and average effect sizes. Importantly, this pattern was sufficiently robust to be discernible despite the heterogeneity across various substances examined, substance-related outcomes investigated, study designs, and analyses used.

The results of this review illustrate the considerable breadth of HPTs for assessing a wide range of different substances, study types, and populations. HPTs broadly, and the Intensity and Omax indices more specifically, are effective in identifying user subpopulations with problematic use patterns and more complex risk profiles (e.g., exhibiting SUD symptoms, experiencing comorbid psychological symptoms, experiencing negative consequences of their use). Furthermore, they are sensitive to individual differences in risk factors that exacerbate substance use and further complicate an individual’s ability to quit (e.g., increased dependence, craving, and polysubstance use). Overall, HPTs are effective in assessing demand even when examining different substances, outcomes, and study designs. Additionally, Intensity and Omax distinguished individual differences in response to clinical interventions to reduce substance use and were sensitive to differences in response to public policies (e.g., tax increases, nicotine content standards) that can inform future policy decisions.

The results reported above further underscore commonalities in the processes underpinning demand across addictive substances that span very different pharmacological classes. Coupled with the recent documentation of HPTs’ selectivity across substances (Strickland & Stoops, 2017), clinicians and researchers can have confidence that these tasks have broad utility in assessing demand. Given these observed associations between demand and substance use, it appears that HPTs can dynamically capture aspects of the overvaluation of substances in one instrument. These constructs map onto key criteria of Substance Use Disorders (i.e., taking in larger amounts or over a longer period than intended; spending a lot of time, money, or effort obtaining, using, and recovering; continued use despite persistent and/or recurrent problems or costs) and thus provide important information about the relative reinforcing value of substances among diverse substance using populations and under conditions of varying constraint on demand.

Although the results of this review of HPT indices offers abundant evidence regarding their utility in elucidating individual differences, at least three limitations merit mention. First, regarding the substances studied utilizing HPTs, there are disproportionately more studies examining alcohol and cigarette demand compared to other substances (e.g., marijuana; prescription medication). That could limit the generality of the results to those other substances. Second, the relatively orderly patterns observed with HPTs notwithstanding, there is still considerable between-study heterogeneity in HPT methodology and data analytic methods (Kaplan et al., 2018). For example, variability in the number of price points, as well as increases in price units between price points, also referred to as the FR step size (Hursh & Silberberg, 2008) can impact Omax, Pmax, and Breakpoint. More sophisticated and uniform methodology (e.g., controlling for collinearity between indices) in addition to more streamlined and standardized data analytic methods, such as automated spreadsheets (Kaplan & Reed., 2014) or specialized software (Gilroy et al., 2018; Kaplan et al., 2018) may allow for more consistency, increased accessibility, and greater use of HPTs among researchers and clinicians interested in these outcomes. Third, we cannot rule out potential publication bias relating to the varying number of indices for which results were reported in this review. Our assumption is that when results did not include all 5 HPT indices, those that were not reported were not evaluated. However, it is possible that the excluded indices were in fact evaluated but not reported due to lack of significant effects. Failure to report non-significant results could result in an overestimation of the mean effect sizes of the indices that were reported less frequently (e.g., Pmax), underestimation of the magnitude of the difference in effect sizes between those underreported indices and those indices reported more frequently (Intensity and Omax), or both. Only further research will eventually sort out those different possibilities.

In terms of future direction, greater standardization was mentioned above. Another potentially useful future direction is more research examining the latent factors (i.e., Amplitude and Persistence), which may allow researchers to capture the informative facets of demand (e.g., volumetric measures of consumption and sensitivity to price) without artificially separating related constructs and eliminate the complications associated with collinearity. Although Amplitude and Persistence were not included in this review, it appears that the combination of Intensity and Omax, which (typically) define the Amplitude factor, may identify particularly at-risk substance users. Future research, however, is needed to determine whether these indices and latent factors have more predictive utility compared to conventional predictors (e.g., high dependence, heavier use, earlier age of initiation). Further, increased research and continued testing of various methodological approaches to administering HPTs, calculating the five indices, and the relative merits of using individual indices versus factors should continue to improve on the utility of these tasks.

CONCLUSION

HPTs are highly effective at assessing individual differences in substance use at both individual and population levels across different addictive substances and levels of economic constraint. Of the five demand indices typically used with HPTs, Intensity and Omax appear to be the most sensitive in distinguishing substance-related correlates and outcomes.

Supplementary Material

1

Highlights:

  • A systematic literature review of studies using hypothetical purchase tasks (HPTs)

  • The review exclusively focused on studies using HPTs to assess substance use

  • Overarching aim was to assess the relative sensitivity of the five HPT indices

  • Intensity and Omax were more sensitive than Elasticity, Breakpoint, or Pmax

  • That pattern had generality across multiple substances and study types

Acknowledgments

Funding: This project was supported by Tobacco Centers of Regulatory Science award U54DA036114 from the National Institute on Drug Abuse (NIDA) and Food and Drug Administration (FDA), Center of Biomedical Research Excellence award P20GM103644 from the National Institute of General Medical Sciences (NIGMS), and NIDA Institutional Training Grant T32DA007242. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of NIDA, FDA, or NIGMS.

Footnotes

Declaration of competing interests: The authors have no conflict of interests to declare.

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

REFERENCES

  1. Amlung M, Few LR, Howland J, Rohsenow DJ, Metrik J, & MacKillop J (2013). Impulsivity and alcohol demand in relation to combined alcohol and caffeine use. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol, 21(6), 467–474. 10.1037/a0034214 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Amlung M, & MacKillop J (2014). Understanding the effects of stress and alcohol cues on motivation for alcohol via behavioral economics. Alcohol Clin Exp Res, 38(6), 1780–1789. 10.1111/acer.12423 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Amlung M, McCarty KN, Morris DH, Tsai CL, & McCarthy DM (2015b). Increased behavioral economic demand and craving for alcohol following a laboratory alcohol challenge. Addiction, 110(9), 1421–1428. 10.1111/add.12897 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Amlung M, Hatz LE, Teeters JB, Murphy JG, & McCarthy DM (2017). Drinking-and-driving–related cognitions mediate the relationship between alcohol demand and alcohol-impaired driving. J Stud Alcohol Drugs, 77(4), 656–660. 10.15288/jsad.2016.77.656 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Amlung M, Monti PM, & Miranda R (2017). Elevated behavioral economic demand for alcohol in a community sample of heavy drinking smokers. J Stud Alcohol Drugs, 78, 623–628. 10.15288/jsad.2017.78.623 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Amlung M, Yurasek A, McCarty KN, MacKillop J, & Murphy JG (2015a). Area under the curve as a novel metric of behavioral economic demand for alcohol. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol, 23(3), 168–175. 10.1037/pha0000014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Amlung MT, Acker J, Stojek MK, Murphy JG, & MacKillop J (2012). Is talk “cheap”? An initial investigation of the equivalence of alcohol purchase task performance for hypothetical and actual rewards. Alcohol Clin Exp Res, 36(4), 716–724. 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2011.01656.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Aston ER, Farris SG, MacKillop J, & Metrik J (2017). Latent factor structure of a behavioral economic marijuana demand curve. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 234(16), 2421–2429. 10.1007/s00213-017-4633-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Aston ER, Metrik J, & MacKillop J (2015). Further validation of a marijuana purchase task. Drug Alcohol Depend, 152, 32–38. 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2015.04.025 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Barnes AJ, Bono RS, Lester RC, Eissenberg TE, & Cobb CO (2017). Effect of flavors and modified risk messages on e-cigarette abuse liability. Tob Regul Sci, 3(4), 374–387. 10.18001/TRS.3.4.1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Berman HL, & Martinetti MP (2017). The effects of next-day class characteristics on alcohol demand in college students. Psychol Addict Behav, 31(4), 488–496. 10.1037/adb0000275 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Bertholet N, Murphy JG, Daeppen JB, Gmel G, & Gaume J (2015). The alcohol purchase task in young men from the general population. Drug Alcohol Depend, 146, 39–44. 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2014.10.024 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Bickel WK, DeGrandpre RJ, Higgins ST (1993). Behavioral economics: A novel experimental approach to the study of drug dependence. Drug Alcohol Depend, 33, 173–192. 10.1016/0376-8716(93)90059-y [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Bickel WK, Johnson MW, Koffarnus MN, MacKillop J, & Murphy JG (2014). The behavioral economics of substance use disorders: reinforcement pathologies and their repair. Annu Rev Clin Psychol, 10, 641–677. 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032813-153724 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Bidwell LC, MacKillop J, Murphy JG, Tidey JW, & Colby SM (2012). Latent factor structure of a behavioral economic cigarette demand curve in adolescent smokers. Addict Behav, 37(11), 1257–1263. 10.1016/j.addbeh.2012.06.009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Bruner NR, & Johnson MW (2014). Demand curves for hypothetical cocaine in cocaine-dependent individuals. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 231(5), 889–897. 10.1007/s00213-013-3312-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Bujarski S, MacKillop J, & Ray LA (2012). Understanding naltrexone mechanism of action and pharmacogenetics in Asian Americans via behavioral economics: a preliminary study. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol, 20(3), 181–190. 10.1037/a0027379 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Bulley A, & Gullo MJ (2017). The influence of episodic foresight on delay discounting and demand for alcohol. Addict Behav, 66, 1–6. 10.1016/j.addbeh.2016.11.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Chase HW, Mackillop J, & Hogarth L (2013). Isolating behavioural economic indices of demand in relation to nicotine dependence. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 226(2), 371–380. 10.1007/s00213-012-2911-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Collins RL, Vincent PC, Yu J, Liu L, & Epstein LH (2014). A behavioral economic approach to assessing demand for marijuana. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol, 22(3), 211–221. 10.1037/a0035318 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Cook J, Spring B, McChargue D, & Doran N (2010). Effects of anhedonia on days to relapse among smokers with a history of depression: a brief report. Nicotine Tob Res, 12(9), 978–982. 10.1093/ntr/ntq118 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Dahne J, Murphy JG, & MacPherson L (2017). Depressive Symptoms and Cigarette Demand as a Function of Induced Stress. Nicotine Tob Res, 19(1), 49–58. 10.1093/ntr/ntw145 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Dennhardt AA, Murphy JG, McDevitt-Murphy ME, & Williams JL (2016). Drinking motives mediate the relationship between alcohol reward value and alcohol problems in military veterans. Psychol Addict Behav, 30(8), 819–826. 10.1037/adb0000197 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Dennhardt AA, Yurasek AM, & Murphy JG (2015). Change in delay discounting and substance reward value following a brief alcohol and drug use intervention. J Exp Anal Behav, 103(1), 125–140. 10.1002/jeab.121 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Farris SG, Aston ER, Abrantes AM, & Zvolensky MJ (2017a). Tobacco demand, delay discounting, and smoking topography among smokers with and without psychopathology. Drug Alcohol Depend, 179, 247–253. 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2017.06.042 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Farris SG, Aston ER, Zvolensky MJ, Abrantes AM, & Metrik J (2017b). Psychopathology and tobacco demand. Drug Alcohol Depend, 177, 59–66. 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2017.03.020 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Field M, Santarcangelo M, Sumnall H, Goudie A, & Cole J (2006). Delay discounting and the behavioural economics of cigarette purchases in smokers: the effects of nicotine deprivation. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 186(2), 255–263. 10.1007/s00213-006-0385-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Gentile ND, Librizzi EH, & Martinetti MP (2012). Academic constraints on alcohol consumption in college students: a behavioral economic analysis. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol, 20(5), 390–399. 10.1037/a0029665 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Gilroy SP, Kaplan BA, Reed DD, Koffarnus MN, & Hantula DA (2018). The Demand Curve Analyzer: Behavioral economic software for applied research. J Exp Anal Behav, 110(3), 553–568. 10.1002/jeab.479 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Gonzalez-Roz A, Secades-Villa R, Weidberg S, Garcia-Perez A, & Reed DD (2018). Latent structure of the CPT among treatment-seeking smokers with depression and its predictive validity on smoking abstinence. Nicotine Tob Res 10.1093/ntr/nty236 [DOI] [PubMed]
  31. Grace RC, Kivell BM, & Laugesen M (2015a). Assessing the Temporal Stability of a Cigarette Purchase Task After an Excise Tax Increase for Factory-Made and Roll-Your-Own Smokers. Nicotine Tob Res, 17(11), 1393–1396. 10.1093/ntr/ntv025 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Grace RC, Kivell BM, & Laugesen M (2015b). Estimating cross-price elasticity of e-cigarettes using a simulated demand procedure. Nicotine Tob Res, 17(5), 592–598. 10.1093/ntr/ntu268 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Grace RC, Kivell BM, & Laugesen M (2015c). Predicting decreases in smoking with a cigarette purchase task: evidence from an excise tax rise in New Zealand. Tob Control, 24(6), 582–587. 10.1136/tobaccocontrol-2014-051594 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Gray JC, Amlung MT, Owens M, Acker J, Brown CL, Brody GH, … MacKillop J (2017). The neuroeconomics of tobacco demand: An initial investigation of the neural correlates of cigarette cost-benefit decision making in male smokers. Sci Rep, 7, 41930 10.1038/srep41930 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Gray JC, & MacKillop J (2014). Interrelationships among individual differences in alcohol demand, impulsivity, and alcohol misuse. Psychol Addict Behav, 28(1), 282–287. 10.1037/a0032766 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Herschl LC, McChargue DE, MacKillop J, Stoltenberg SF, & Highland KB (2012). Implicit and explicit alcohol-related motivations among college binge drinkers. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 221(4), 685–692. 10.1007/s00213-011-2613-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Higgins ST (1997). The influence of alternative reinforcers on cocaine use and abuse: A brief review. Pharmacol Biochem Behav, 57(3), 419–427. 10.1016/s0091-3057(96)00446-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Higgins ST, Heil SH, Sigmon SC, Tidey JW, Gaalema DE, Hughes JR, … Tursi L (2017). Addiction potential of cigarettes with reduced nicotine content in populations with psychiatric disorders and other vulnerabilities to tobacco addiction JAMA Psychiatry, 74(10), 1056–1064. 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2017.2355 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Higgins ST, Heil S, & Lussier JP. (2004). Clinical implications of reinforcement as a determinant of substance use disorders. Annu Rev Psychol, 55, 431–461. 10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.142033 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Higgins ST, Reed DD, Redner R, Skelly JM, Zvorsky IA, & Kurti AN (2017). Simulating demand for cigarettes among pregnant women: A low-risk method for studying vulnerable populations. J Exp Anal Behav, 107(1), 176–190. 10.1002/jeab.232 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Hindocha C, Lawn W, Freeman TP, & Curran HV (2017). Individual and combined effects of cannabis and tobacco on drug reward processing in non-dependent users. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 234(21), 3153–3163. 10.1007/s00213-017-4698-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Hursh SR, Galuska CM, Winger G, & Woods JH (2005). The economics of drug abuse: A quantitative assessment of drug demand Molecular Interventions, 5(1), 20–28. 10.1124/mi.5.1.6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Hursh SR, & Roma PG (2016). Behavioral Economics and the Analysis of Consumption and Choice. Managerial and Decision Economics, 37(4–5), 224–238. 10.1002/mde.2724 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  44. Hursh SR, & Silberberg A (2008). Economic demand and essential value. Psychol Rev, 115(1), 186–198. 10.1037/0033-295X.115.1.186 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Jacobs EA, & Bickel WK (1999). Modeling drug consumption in the clinic using simulation procedures: Demand for heroin and cigarettes in opioid-dependent outpatients. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol, 7(4), 412–426. 10.1037//1064-1297.7.4.412 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Johnson MW, Johnson PS, Rass O, & Pacek LR (2017). Behavioral economic substitutability of e-cigarettes, tobacco cigarettes, and nicotine gum. J Psychopharmacol, 31(7), 851–860. 10.1177/0269881117711921 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Kaplan BA, Foster RNS, Reed DD, Amlung M, Murphy JG, & MacKillop J (2018). Understanding alcohol motivation using the alcohol purchase task: A methodological systematic review. Drug Alcohol Depend, 191, 117–140. 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2018.06.029 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Kaplan BA, Gilroy SP, Reed DD, Koffarnus MN, & Hursh SR (2018). The R package beezdemand: Behavioral Economic Easy Demand. Perspectives on Behavior Science 10.1007/s40614-018-00187-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  49. Kaplan BA, Reed D D. E. v., Pmax, and Omax automated calculator. Spreadsheet Application http://hdl.handle.net/1808/14934.
  50. Kiselica AM, Borders A (2013). The reinforcing efficacy of alcohol mediates associations between impulsivity and negative drinking outcomes. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 74(3), 490–499. 10.15288/jsad.2013.74.490 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Kiselica AM, Webber TA, & Bornovalova MA (2016). Validity of the alcohol purchase task: a meta-analysis. Addiction, 111(5), 806–816. 10.1111/add.13254 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Lemley SM, Fleming WA, & Jarmolowicz DP (2017). Behavioral economic predictors of alcohol and sexual risk behavior in college drinkers. The Psychological Record, 67(2), 197–211. 10.1007/s40732-017-0239-y [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  53. Lemley SM, Kaplan BA, Reed DD, Darden AC, & Jarmolowicz DP (2016). Reinforcer pathologies: Predicting alcohol related problems in college drinking men and women. Drug Alcohol Depend, 167, 57–66. 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2016.07.025 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Leventhal AM, Ramsey SE, Brown RA, LaChance HR, & Kahler CW (2008). Dimensions of depressive symptoms and smoking cessation. Nicotine Tob Res, 10(3), 507–517. 10.1080/14622200801901971 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Leventhal AM, Trujillo M, Ameringer KJ, Tidey JW, Sussman S, & Kahler CW (2014). Anhedonia and the relative reward value of drug and nondrug reinforcers in cigarette smokers. J Abnorm Psychol, 123(2), 375–386. 10.1037/a0036384 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Liberati A, Altman DG, Tetzlaff J, Mulrow C, Gotzsche PC, Ioannidis JP, … Moher D (2009). The PRISMA statement for reporting systematic reviews and meta-analyses of studies that evaluate health care interventions: explanation and elaboration. PLoS Med, 6(7), e1000100 10.1371/journal.pmed.1000100 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Luehring-Jones P, Dennis-Tiwary TA, Murphy JG, Dennhardt A, Lindgren KP, Yarmush DE, & Erblich J (2016). Favorable associations with alcohol and impaired self-regulation: A behavioral economic analysis. Drug Alcohol Depend, 163, 172–178. 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2016.04.017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. MacKillop J, Amlung MT, Blackburn A, Murphy JG, Carrigan M, Carpenter MJ, & Chaloupka F (2014). Left-digit price effects on smoking cessation motivation. Tob Control, 23(6), 501–506. 10.1136/tobaccocontrol-2012-050943 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. MacKillop J, Brown CL, Stojek MK, Murphy CM, Sweet L, & Niaura RS (2012). Behavioral economic analysis of withdrawal-and cue-elicited craving for tobacco: an initial investigation. Nicotine Tob Res, 14(12), 1426–1434. 10.1093/ntr/nts006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. MacKillop J, Goldenson NI, Kirkpatrick MG, & Leventhal AM (2018). Validation of a behavioral economic purchase task for assessing drug abuse liability. Addict Biol 10.1111/adb.12592 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  61. MacKillop J, Murphy CM, Martin RA, Stojek M, Tidey JW, Colby SM, & Rohsenow DJ (2016). Predictive validity of a cigarette purchase task in a randomized controlled trial of contingent vouchers for smoking in individuals with substance use disorders.. Nicotine Tob Res, 18(5), 531–537. 10.1093/ntr/ntv233 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. MacKillop J, & Murphy JG (2007). A behavioral economic measure of demand for alcohol predicts brief intervention outcomes. Drug Alcohol Depend, 89(2–3), 227–233. 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2007.01.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. MacKillop J, Murphy JG, Ray LA, Eisenberg DT, Lisman SA, Lum JK, & Wilson DS (2008). Further validation of a cigarette purchase task for assessing the relative reinforcing efficacy of nicotine in college smokers. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol, 16(1), 57–65. 10.1037/1064-1297.16.1.57 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Mackillop J, Murphy JG, Tidey JW, Kahler CW, Ray LA, & Bickel WK (2009). Latent structure of facets of alcohol reinforcement from a behavioral economic demand curve. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 203(1), 33–40. 10.1007/s00213-008-1367-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. MacKillop J, & Tidey JW (2011). Cigarette demand and delayed reward discounting in nicotine-dependent individuals with schizophrenia and controls: an initial study. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 216(1), 91–99. 10.1007/s00213-011-2185-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Metrik J, Aston ER, Kahler CW, Rohsenow DJ, McGeary JE, Knopik VS, & MacKillop J (2016). Cue-elicited increases in incentive salience for marijuana: Craving, demand, and attentional bias. Drug Alcohol Depend, 167, 82–88. 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2016.07.027 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Morris V, Amlung M, Kaplan BA, Reed DD, Petker T, & MacKillop J (2017). Using crowdsourcing to examine behavioral economic measures of alcohol value and proportionate alcohol reinforcement. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol, 25(4), 314–321. 10.1037/pha0000130 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Murphy CM, MacKillop J, Martin RA, Tidey JW, Colby SM, & Rohsenow DJ (2017). Effects of varenicline versus transdermal nicotine replacement therapy on cigarette demand on quit day in individuals with substance use disorders. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 234(16), 2443–2452. 10.1007/s00213-017-4635-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Murphy CM, Owens MM, Sweet LH, & MacKillop J (2016). The substitutability of cigarettes and food: A behavioral economic comparison in normal weight and overweight or obese smokers. Psychol Addict Behav, 30(8), 857–867. 10.1037/adb0000223 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Murphy JG, Dennhardt AA, Yurasek AM, Skidmore JR, Martens MP, MacKillop J, & McDevitt-Murphy ME (2015). Behavioral economic predictors of brief alcohol intervention outcomes. J Consult Clin Psychol, 83(6), 1033–1043. 10.1037/ccp0000032 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Murphy JG, & MacKillop J (2006). Relative reinforcing efficacy of alcohol among college student drinkers. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol, 14(2), 219–227. 10.1037/1064-1297.14.2.219 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Murphy JG, MacKillop J, Skidmore JR, & Pederson AA (2009). Reliability and validity of a demand curve measure of alcohol reinforcement. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol, 17(6), 396–404. 10.1037/a0017684 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Murphy JG, MacKillop J, Tidey JW, Brazil LA, & Colby SM (2011). Validity of a demand curve measure of nicotine reinforcement with adolescent smokers. Drug Alcohol Depend, 113(2–3), 207–214. 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2010.08.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Murphy JG, Yurasek AM, Dennhardt AA, Skidmore JR, McDevitt-Murphy ME, MacKillop J, & Martens MP (2013). Symptoms of depression and PTSD are associated with elevated alcohol demand. Drug Alcohol Depend, 127(1–3), 129–136. 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2012.06.022 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Murphy JG., Yurasek AM, Meshesha LZ, Dennhardt AA, MacKillop JM, Skidmore JR, & Martens MP (2014). Family history of problem drinking is associated with less sensitivity of alcohol demand to a next-day responsibility. J. Study. Alcohol Drugs, 75, 653–663. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. O’Connor RJ, Bansal-Travers M, Carter LP, & Cummings KM (2012). What would menthol smokers do if menthol in cigarettes were banned? Behavioral intentions and simulated demand. Addiction, 107(7), 1330–1338. 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2012.03822.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. O’Connor RJ, Heckman BW, Adkison SE, Rees VW, Hatsukami DK, Bickel WK, & Cummings KM (2016). Persistence and amplitude of cigarette demand in relation to quit intentions and attempts. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 233(12), 2365–2371. 10.1007/s00213-016-4286-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. O’Connor RJ, June KM, Bansal-Travers M, Rousu MC, Thrasher JF, Hyland A, & Cummings KM (2014). Estimating demand for alternatives to cigarettes with online purchase tasks. Am J Health Behav, 38(1), 103–113. 10.5993/AJHB.38.1.11 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Owens MM, Murphy CM, & MacKillop J (2015a). Initial development of a brief behavioral economic assessment of alcohol demand. Psychol Conscious (Wash D C), 2(2), 144–152. 10.1037/cns0000056 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Owens MM, Ray LA, & MacKillop J (2015b). Behavioral economic analysis of stress effects on acute motivation for alcohol. J Exp Anal Behav, 103(1), 77–86. 10.1002/jeab.114 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Peters EN, Rosenberry ZR, Schauer GL, O’Grady KE, & Johnson PS (2017). Marijuana and tobacco cigarettes: Estimating their behavioral economic relationship using purchasing tasks. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol, 25(3), 208–215. 10.1037/pha0000122 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Pickover AM, Messina BG, Correia CJ, Garza KB, & Murphy JG (2016). A behavioral economic analysis of the nonmedical use of prescription drugs among young adults. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol, 24(1), 38–47. 10.1037/pha0000052 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Reed DD, Niileksela CR, Kaplan BA (2013). Behavioral economics: A tutorial for behavior analysts in practice. Behav Anal in Practice, 6(1), 34–54. 10.1007/bf03391790 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Roma PG, Reed DD, DiGennaro Reed FD, & Hursh SR (2017). Progress of and Prospects for Hypothetical Purchase Task Questionnaires in Consumer Behavior Analysis and Public Policy. The Behavior Analyst, 40(2), 329–342. 10.1007/s40614-017-0100-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Schlienz NJ, Hawk LW Jr., Tiffany ST, O’Connor RJ, & Mahoney MC (2014). The impact of pre-cessation varenicline on behavioral economic indices of smoking reinforcement. Addict Behav, 39(10), 1484–1490. 10.1016/j.addbeh.2014.05.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Secades-Villa R, Pericot-Valverde I, & Weidberg S (2016). Relative reinforcing efficacy of cigarettes as a predictor of smoking abstinence among treatment-seeking smokers. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 233(17), 3103–3112. 10.1007/s00213-016-4350-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Secades-Villa R, Weidberg S, Gonzalez-Roz A, Reed DD, & Fernandez-Hermida JR (2018). Cigarette demand among smokers with elevated depressive symptoms: an experimental comparison with low depressive symptoms. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 235(3), 719–728. 10.1007/s00213-017-4788-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Skidmore JR, & Murphy JG (2011). The effect of drink price and next-day responsibilities on college student drinking: a behavioral economic analysis. Psychol Addict Behav, 25(1), 57–68. 10.1037/a0021118 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Smith TT, Cassidy RN, Tidey JW, Luo X, Le CT, Hatsukami DK, & Donny EC (2017). Impact of smoking reduced nicotine content cigarettes on sensitivity to cigarette price: further results from a multi-site clinical trial. Addiction, 112(2), 349–359. 10.1111/add.13636 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Snider SE, Cummings KM, & Bickel WK (2017). Behavioral economic substitution between conventional cigarettes and e-cigarettes differs as a function of the frequency of e-cigarette use. Drug Alcohol Depend, 177, 14–22. 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2017.03.017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Snider SE, LaConte SM, & Bickel WK (2016). Episodic Future Thinking: Expansion of the Temporal Window in Individuals with Alcohol Dependence. Alcohol Clin Exp Res, 40(7), 1558–1566. 10.1111/acer.13112 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Strickland JC, Lile JA, Rush CR, & Stoops WW (2016b). Comparing exponential and exponentiated models of drug demand in cocaine users. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol, 24(6), 447–455. 10.1037/pha0000096 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Strickland JC, Lile JA, & Stoops WW (2017). Unique prediction of cannabis use severity and behaviors by delay discounting and behavioral economic demand. Behav Processes, 140, 33–40. 10.1016/j.beproc.2017.03.017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Strickland JC, Reynolds AR, & Stoops WW (2016a). Regulation of cocaine craving by cognitive strategies in an online sample of cocaine users. Psychol Addict Behav, 30(5), 607–612. 10.1037/adb0000180 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Strickland JC, & Stoops WW (2017). Stimulus selectivity of drug purchase tasks: A preliminary study evaluating alcohol and cigarette demand. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol, 25(3), 198–207. 10.1037/pha0000123 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Teeters JB, & Murphy JG (2015). The behavioral economics of driving after drinking among college drinkers. Alcohol Clin Exp Res, 39(5), 896–904. 10.1111/acer.12695 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Teeters JB, Pickover AM, Dennhardt AA, Martens MP, & Murphy JG (2014). Elevated alcohol demand is associated with driving after drinking among college student binge drinkers. Alcohol Clin Exp Res, 38(7), 2066–2072. 10.1111/acer.12448 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Tucker JA, Cheong J, Chandler SD, Lambert BH, Kwok H, & Pietrzak B (2016a). Behavioral economic indicators of drinking problem severity and initial outcomes among problem drinkers attempting natural recovery: a cross-sectional naturalistic study. Addiction, 111(11), 1956–1965. 10.1111/add.13492 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Tucker JA, Cheong J, Chandler SD, Lambert BH, Pietrzak B, Kwok H, & Davies SL (2016b). Prospective Analysis of Behavioral Economic Predictors of Stable Moderation Drinking Among Problem Drinkers Attempting Natural Recovery. Alcohol Clin Exp Res, 40(12), 2676–2684. 10.1111/acer.13245 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Tucker MR, Laugesen M, & Grace RC (2018). Estimating demand and cross-price elasticity for Very Low Nicotine Content (VLNC) Cigarettes using a simulated demand task. Nicotine Tob Res, 20(7), 843–850. 10.1093/ntr/ntx051 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Vincent PC, Collins RL, Liu L, Yu J, De Leo JA, & Earleywine M (2017). The effects of perceived quality on behavioral economic demand for marijuana: A web-based experiment. Drug Alcohol Depend, 170, 174–180. 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2016.11.013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Vuchinich RE, & Heather N (2003). Choice, behavioural economics and addiction Amsterdam: Pergamon/ Elsevier Science. [Google Scholar]
  103. Wahlstrom LC, McChargue DE, & Mackillop J (2012). DRD2/ANKK1 TaqI A genotype moderates the relationship between alexithymia and the relative value of alcohol among male college binge drinkers. Pharmacol Biochem Behav, 102(3), 471–476. 10.1016/j.pbb.2012.06.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Weinstock J, Mulhauser K, Oremus EG, & D’Agostino AR (2016). Demand for gambling: development and assessment of a gambling purchase task. International Gambling Studies, 16(2), 316–327. 10.1080/14459795.2016.1182570 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  105. Wilson AG, Franck CT, Koffarnus MN, & Bickel WK (2016). Behavioral economics of cigarette purchase tasks: within-subject comparison of real, potentially real, and hypothetical cigarettes. Nicotine Tob Res, 18(5), 524–530. 10.1093/ntr/ntv154 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Yurasek AM,MJ, Clawson AH, Denhardt AA, MacKillop J (2013). Smokers report greater demand for alcohol on a behavioral economic purchase task. J. Study. Alcohol Drugs, 74, 626–634. 10.15288/jsad.2013.74.626 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Yurasek AM,MJ, Denhardt AA, Skidmore JR, Buscemi J, McCausland C, Martens MP. (2011). Drinking motives mediate the relationship between reinforcing efficacy and alcohol consumption and problems. J. Study. Alcohol Drugs, 72, 991–999. 10.15288/jsad.2011.72.991 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Zvolensky MJ, Stewart SH, Vujanovic AA, Gavric D, & Steeves D (2009). Anxiety sensitivity and anxiety and depressive symptoms in the prediction of early smoking lapse and relapse during smoking cessation treatment. Nicotine Tob Res, 11(3), 323–331. 10.1093/ntr/ntn037 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

1

RESOURCES