Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2019 Nov 27;8:e49915. doi: 10.7554/eLife.49915

Kv1.1 contributes to a rapid homeostatic plasticity of intrinsic excitability in CA1 pyramidal neurons in vivo

Peter James Morgan 1,, Romain Bourboulou 1, Caroline Filippi 1, Julie Koenig-Gambini 1,2, Jérôme Epsztein 1,
Editors: Inna Slutsky3, Eve Marder4
PMCID: PMC6881145  PMID: 31774395

Abstract

In area CA1 of the hippocampus, the selection of place cells to represent a new environment is biased towards neurons with higher excitability. However, different environments are represented by orthogonal cell ensembles, suggesting that regulatory mechanisms exist. Activity-dependent plasticity of intrinsic excitability, as observed in vitro, is an attractive candidate. Here, using whole-cell patch-clamp recordings of CA1 pyramidal neurons in anesthetized rats, we have examined how inducing theta-bursts of action potentials affects their intrinsic excitability over time. We observed a long-lasting, homeostatic depression of intrinsic excitability which commenced within minutes, and, in contrast to in vitro observations, was not mediated by dendritic Ih. Instead, it was attenuated by the Kv1.1 channel blocker dendrotoxin K, suggesting an axonal origin. Analysis of place cells’ out-of-field firing in mice navigating in virtual reality further revealed an experience-dependent reduction consistent with decreased excitability. We propose that this mechanism could reduce memory interference.

Research organism: Rat

Introduction

The contribution of intrinsic excitability to learning has often been overlooked in favor of synaptic mechanisms (Daoudal and Debanne, 2003; Zhang and Linden, 2003), but recent evidence indicates that it plays an important role in memory allocation processes and the creation of engrams (Josselyn and Frankland, 2018; Titley et al., 2017; Lisman et al., 2018). In the amygdala and hippocampus it has been shown that the recruitment of cells to represent any new association or environment is biased towards those cells with high levels of intrinsic excitability (Yiu et al., 2014; Pignatelli et al., 2019; Grosmark and Buzsáki, 2016; Alme et al., 2014; Rich et al., 2014; Buzsáki and Mizuseki, 2014; Han et al., 2007; Zhou et al., 2009; Epsztein et al., 2011). In particular, intracellular recordings from individual CA1 pyramidal neurons have shown that higher excitability correlates with neurons becoming place cells in a novel environment (Epsztein et al., 2011), and that enhancing excitability with depolarizations of just a few millivolts is sufficient to reveal place specific activity in previously silent cells (Lee et al., 2012).

Intrinsic excitability can therefore influence memory allocation processes. However, given that the populations representing different environments are largely orthogonal (Leutgeb et al., 2004; Leutgeb et al., 2005), a regulatory mechanism is needed to diversify recruitment and prevent the same cells from always being activated.

One attractive mechanism is homeostatic regulation of intrinsic excitability based on firing rate history, as it is known that neurons can adapt their excitability to maintain their activity within set bounds (Turrigiano et al., 1994; Desai et al., 1999). However, these changes occur slowly in response to sustained changes in activity levels rather than activity patterns associated with coding. Faster changes in intrinsic excitability that accompany synaptic plasticity have also been described, though these are typically synergistic (going in the same direction than the observed synaptic changes) and restricted to the dendritic branches containing the potentiated synapses (Daoudal and Debanne, 2003; Zhang and Linden, 2003). Some studies have shown, however, that high frequency bursts of action potentials alone can induce a homeostatic decrease in the global excitability of CA1 pyramidal neurons (Fan et al., 2005; Narayanan and Johnston, 2007). Such global change in excitability could be particularly relevant to regulate the allocation process as it allows cells to tune their excitability based on their involvement in coding, without disrupting information contained within the synapses. However, this plasticity was mediated by an upregulation of the hyperpolarization-activated cation current (Ih), and it is thus questionable whether it would serve this function under natural conditions, as Ih is primarily expressed in the dendrites where it strongly influences synaptic integration (Magee, 1998; Tsay et al., 2007) and plasticity (Maroso et al., 2016; Nolan et al., 2004). Stimulations inducing weaker, more physiological, levels of synaptic potentiation can also have the opposite effect, locally reducing Ih and enhancing dendritic excitability around potentiated synapses (Campanac et al., 2008). A central question, therefore, is whether an activity-dependent plasticity of global excitability is present in CA1 pyramidal cells in vivo.

To date, only a few studies have described intrinsic plasticity in vivo (Mahon and Charpier, 2012; Paz et al., 2009; Aou et al., 1992; Belmeguenai et al., 2010), and the underlying mechanisms were not identified. Here we have examined the effect of a theta-burst firing pattern induced directly through the patch pipette on the intrinsic excitability of CA1 pyramidal neurons in vivo. We found a homeostatic decrease in excitability that was superficially similar to an Ih-mediated plasticity that has been described in vitro (Fan et al., 2005; Narayanan and Johnston, 2007). However, mechanistically our effect was independent of Ih, and instead our findings implicate an increase in ID carried by Kv1.1 channels in the axon initial segment (AIS). This indicates that in vivo, action potential firing can regulate global intrinsic excitability independently of dendritic excitability, which may support the co-existence of synaptic learning and memory allocation processes based on plasticity of intrinsic excitability.

Results

Theta burst firing induces a homeostatic decrease in intrinsic excitability of CA1 pyramidal neurons in vivo

We obtained whole-cell patch-clamp recordings from CA1 pyramidal neurons (n = 56) in anesthetized rats using the blind technique (Lee et al., 2009; Margrie et al., 2002). To determine if the burst (100 Hz) firing of CA1 pyramidal cells at theta (5 Hz) frequency, as observed during place field traversals (Epsztein et al., 2011; Harvey et al., 2009), could induce long-term changes in intrinsic excitability we applied a theta-burst stimulation (TBS) protocol directly through the patch pipette (Figure 1A; see methods). We used an EPSP-like waveform for the current pulses as this reliably entrained the cells to fire at 100 Hz, and where failures occurred they were at the beginnings and ends of the bursts. The identity of the cell was verified in 45 out of 56 cases by biocytin staining (Figure 1B). Neuronal excitability was assessed by measuring the firing rate of CA1 pyramidal cells in response to a series of depolarizing current steps from a holding potential of −60 mV (with liquid junction potential correction this corresponds to a membrane potential of around −75 mV). The TBS was applied immediately after the first testing protocol, and the testing protocol was then repeated every 10 min.

Figure 1. Theta-burst stimulation reduces intrinsic excitability.

(A) Left In vivo whole-cell recordings were made from CA1 pyramidal neurons. The TBS was applied through the patch pipette. Right An example of TBS. EPSP-like waveforms were applied in bursts of 10 (at 100Hz, inset), these bursts were repeated 10 times at theta frequency (5Hz) and the process was repeated three times with an interval of 15 seconds. (B) Morphology of a recorded cell. SR stratum radiatum, SP stratum pyramidale, SO stratum oriens. (C-D) Examples of a TBS and a control cell. The plots show the response to each of the maximum current injections at different times during the testing protocol (grey circles). The red circles mark the mean response at each timepoint. The traces shown in (D) are marked by dark grey borders at their respective timepoints. (E) Left Mean number of spikes evoked in response to the maximal current injection prior to and 20 minutes post-TBS for each cell. Right Distribution of changes in spike count (F) Comparison of change in firing rate at 20 minutes between TBS and control groups. (G) Correlation between the number of spikes evoked during TBS and the change in firing rate. (H) Firing rate vs injected current for the populations of TBS and control cells. The current range is normalized to correct for differences in the range of current amplitudes used. In the TBS group there was a significant reduction (p < 0.05) in the intercept at 10 min (green) and 20 min (red) post-TBS but no change in the gain of the fits. Error bars show mean ± SEM. TBS n = 16 (30 min n = 14), Control n = 11 (30 min n = 6).

Figure 1—source data 1. Spike count in TBS and control conditions.

Figure 1.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1. Change in excitability is not related to inital state of the cells.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1.

(A) Correlation with the input resistance prior to TBS. (B) Correlation with membrane potential immediately after breaking in. (C) Correlation with initial AP threshold. (D) Correlation with burstiness. n = 16

Following TBS we saw a long-lasting reduction in intrinsic excitability that began within minutes of the stimulation (Figure 1C,D). Average firing rates were significantly reduced 20 min post-TBS (Change in firing rate: TBS −29.2 ± 7.1%, p=7.3×10−4, n = 16, Figure 1E), which was not seen in control cells (−5.3 ± 4.8%, p=0.20, n = 11, Figure 1F). The magnitude of the reduction correlated with the number of spikes evoked during the TBS (R2 = 0.40, p=0.0083; Figure 1G) in line with a homeostatic mechanism. To see if the gain of the input-output relationship was altered by the change in excitability we first corrected for variations between cells by normalizing the current-firing responses of each cell and then fitted the population with linear regression. We found that the reduction in firing rate after TBS was uniform across all current step amplitudes, resulting in a rightward shift in the input/output curve with no change in the slope of the fits (Slope: Pre vs 10 min p=0.78, 20 min p=0.34 and 30 min p=0.43, Figure 1H). As a result the current threshold was significantly increased by the TBS protocol (Intercept: Pre vs 10 min p=0.044, n = 16, 20 min p=0.024, n = 16, 30 min p=0.28 n=14). No effect on slope or intercept was seen in control cells (Pre vs 10 min p=0.26, n = 11, 20 min p=0.49, n = 11, 30 min p=0.27, n = 6, Figure 1H). The effect of TBS did not appear to relate to the initial excitability state of the cell, as the change in firing rate did not show any relationship with the initial input resistance (R2 = 0.11, p=0.21, F Figure 1—figure supplement 1A), membrane potential (R2 = 0.015, p=0.66, Figure 1—figure supplement 1B), action potential threshold (R2 = 0.0045, p=0.80, Figure 1—figure supplement 1C) or burstiness (R2 = 0.02, p=0.60, Figure 1—figure supplement 1D). Our results therefore show that a short episode of theta-burst firing is sufficient to induce a fast and long-lasting decrease in intrinsic excitability of CA1 pyramidal cells in vivo.

Plasticity of intrinsic excitability is associated with changes in Rin and first spike latency

We next looked for changes in cellular properties that could indicate what mechanisms might explain the reduction in excitability. We found a reduction in the input resistance following TBS that correlated well with the reduction in excitability (pre vs 20 min p=0.0087, n = 16; R2 = 0.38, p=0.012, Figure 2A–C), and that was significant compared to control cells (Change in Rin: TBS −9.7 ± 2.6%, n = 16, Control −1.8 ± 2.5%, n = 11, p=0.043, Figure 2D). We similarly found an increase in the latency to first spike that correlated with the change in excitability (pre vs 20 min p=0.005, n = 16; R2 = 0.69, p=1.6×10−4, Figure 2E–G), and that was significant compared to control (Change in latency: TBS 103.4 ± 29.5%, n = 16, Control 19.6 ± 12.8%, n = 11, p=0.034, Figure 2H). These observations suggest that sub-threshold conductances might be modified by the TBS protocol.

Figure 2. Reduction in excitability is associated with decreased Rin and increased latency to first spike.

(A) Example traces showing a reduction in input resistance. Each trace is the mean of 10 traces. Rin was calculated on hyperpolarizing steps only and the voltage was taken as the minimum within the first 150ms. (B) Input resistance prior to and 20 minutes post-TBS. (C) Correlation between the change in input resistance and the change in firing rate. (D) Comparison of change in input resistance at 20 minutes between TBS and control groups. (E) Example trace showing increase in latency after TBS. (F) Latency to first spike prior to and 20 minutes post-TBS. (G) Correlation between the change in latency and the change in firing rate. (H) Comparison of change in latency at 20 minutes between TBS and control groups. Error bars show mean ± SEM. TBS n = 16, Control n = 11.

Figure 2—source data 1. Input resistance and first spike latency in TBS and control conditions.

Figure 2.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1. TBS did not change background synaptic activity.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1.

(A) Examples of synaptic activity prior to testing excitability before and 20 minutes post-TBS. (B) Membrane potential variance prior to and 20 minutes post-TBS. (C) Scatter plot showing no correlation between the change in membrane potential variance and the change in firing rate. (D) Comparison of change in variance of the membrane potential at 20 minutes between TBS and control groups.
Figure 2—figure supplement 2. Action potential threshold and medium AHP.

Figure 2—figure supplement 2.

(A) Action potential threshold prior to and 20 minutes post-TBS. (B) Comparison of change in threshold between TBS and control cells. (C) AHP amplitude prior to and 20 minutes post-TBS. (D) Comparison of change in AHP between TBS and control cells. Error bars show mean ± SEM. TBS n = 16, Control n = 11.

To verify that the synaptic drive on to the recorded neurons remained constant for the duration of the recording we compared Vm variance and amplitude before and after application of the TBS protocol. There was no difference in variance before versus 20 min post-TBS (pre vs 20 min p=0.84, n = 16, Figure 2—figure supplement 1A–B), no correlation with the change in excitability (R2 = 0.03 p=0.53, Figure 2—figure supplement 1C) or difference compared to control cells (∆Variance: TBS −0.03 ± 0.16 mV2 n=16, Control 0.39 ± 0.22 mV2, n = 11, p=0.12, Figure 2—figure supplement 1D).

We also looked for changes in the action potential threshold and spike afterhyperpolarization (AHP), properties which have been associated with changes in excitability in vivo and ex vivo, respectively (Mahon and Charpier, 2012; Oh et al., 2003). Although after TBS there was an increase in the action potential threshold (pre vs 20 min p<0.019, n = 16, Figure 2—figure supplement 2A) the difference between control and TBS cells was not significant (∆Threshold: TBS 1.16 ± 0.44 mV, n = 16, Control 0.44 ± 0.54 mV, n = 10, p=0.32, Figure 2—figure supplement 2B). We did not find any effect on the AHP (pre vs 20 min p=0.92, ∆AHP: TBS 0.4 ± 0.48 mV, n = 16, Control −0.48 ± 0.37 mV, p=0.19, n = 11, Figure 2—figure supplement 2C,D). These observations indicate that changes in supra-threshold conductances are unlikely to explain the decrease in excitability we observed.

Plasticity induction depends on rise in intracellular calcium but is independent of NMDARs

If the plasticity we observed indeed depends on action potential firing, we would expect it to be dependent on increases in intracellular calcium. To verify that this was the case we repeated the experiments with the addition of the calcium chelator BAPTA (0.5 mM) to the intracellular solution. As expected, in the presence of BAPTA the depression of intrinsic excitability was prevented (BAPTA n = 7, change in firing rate: 23.0 ± 13.7%, p=0.0012 vs TBS; change in Rin: 2.4% ± 5.16, p=0.029 vs TBS; change in latency: 8.7 ± 7.4%, p=0.05 vs TBS, Figure 3).

Figure 3. Calcium chelation prevents the TBS-induced decrease in excitability.

Figure 3.

(A-B) Example cell showing that the decrease in excitability is blocked with 0.5mM BAPTA inside the patch pipette. Scatter plots show the response to each of the maximum current injections during the testing protocol (grey circles). The red circles mark the mean response at each timepoint. Dark grey borders mark the current steps depicted in (B). (C-E) Comparisons of changes in firing rate (C), input resistance (D) and latency (E) between TBS alone (n = 16) and TBS with BAPTA (n = 7). Error bars show mean ± SEM.

Figure 3—source data 1. Spike count, input resistance and first spike latency in TBS condition with BAPTA.

We next blocked calcium influx through NMDARs, as this has been described to block various mechanisms for changes in intrinsic excitability in vitro (Fan et al., 2005; Wang et al., 2003; Frick et al., 2004; Losonczy et al., 2008), even in the absence of synaptic stimulation (Fan et al., 2005). In the latter case it is assumed that there is either background NMDAR activity or that the backpropagating action potentials depolarize the dendrite sufficiently to remove the Mg2+ block for ongoing activity to activate NMDARs. We found, however, that blocking NMDARs by adding 1 mM MK-801 to intracellular solution did not affect the TBS-induced reduction in excitability (MK-801 n = 7, change in firing rate: −42.1 ± 12.2%, p=0.35 vs TBS; change in Rin: −13.2 ± 2.5%, p=0.43 vs TBS; change in latency: 156.4 ± 70.3%, p=0.42 vs TBS, Figure 4). This is perhaps unsurprising given the lack of potentiating synaptic stimulation, and we can infer that influx through voltage-gated calcium channels is the major, or at least initial, source of calcium for the induction of the plasticity we observed.

Figure 4. NMDAR block does not prevent the TBS-induced decrease in excitability.

Figure 4.

(A-B) Example cell showing that 1mM MK-801 inside the pipette does not prevent the decrease in excitability. Scatter plot shows the response to each of the maximum current injections during the testing protocol (grey circles). The red circles mark the mean response at each timepoint. Dark grey borders mark the current steps depicted in (B). (C-E) Comparisons of changes in firing rate (C), input resistance (D) and latency (E) between TBS alone (n = 16) and TBS with MK-801 (n = 7). Error bars show mean ± SEM.

Figure 4—source data 1. Spike count, input resistance and first spike latency in TBS condition with MK801.

Decreased excitability cannot be explained by an increase in Ih

Our observations to this point are consistent with the Ih-mediated reduction in intrinsic excitability seen in vitro (Fan et al., 2005; Campanac et al., 2008). To see if Ih could similarly explain the reduction in excitability, we looked to see if TBS affected the sag ratio (a measure of the voltage change resulting from Ih activation, Figure 5A). However, we found that the sag ratio actually increased following TBS (pre vs 20 min p=0.0088, n = 16, Figure 5B), which indicates a decrease in Ih that would be expected to make the cells more excitable. This decrease was not detected in the presence of BAPTA and MK-801 (BAPTA p=0.39, n = 7, MK-801 p=0.97, n = 7, Figure 5C), conditions which blocked the in vitro effects of TBS on Ih (Fan et al., 2005). The change in sag ratio was inversely correlated with the strength of stimulation (R2 = 0.42, p=0.0065, Figure 5D), which is consistent with previous observations following weak LTP induction (Campanac et al., 2008), but did not significantly correlate with the overall change in excitability (R2 = 0.08, p=0.29, Figure 5E). To more directly assess the role of Ih in the plasticity we observed, we repeated the experiments with the addition of 100 µM ZD7288 (a potent and specific blocker of Ih channel with an intracellular site of action) to the intracellular solution. Under this condition the membrane sag was abolished and Rin was increased (Pre-TBS Rin: TBS 48.7 ± 2.5 MΩ, ZD 68.6 ± 5.9 MΩ), indicating Ih was blocked (Figure 6A–B). However, intracellular ZD7288 did not prevent TBS-induced decrease in firing rate (ZD n = 6, change in firing rate: −22.9 ± 8.6%, p=0.63 vs TBS, Figure 6C–E), decrease in input resistance (ZD change in Rin: −14.3 ± 3.8%, p=0.36 vs TBS, Figure 6F) and increase in first spike latency (ZD change in latency: 78.7 ± 13.1%, p=0.62 vs TBS, Figure 6G).

Figure 5. Changes in Ih do not account for decrease in excitability.

Figure 5.

(A) Example traces showing the voltage sag caused by Ih pre and 20 min post-TBS. Each trace is the mean of 10 trials. (B) Sag ratio prior to and 20 minutes post-TBS (n = 16). (C) Sag ratio prior to and 20 minutes post-TBS with 0.5mM BAPTA (n = 7) or 1mM MK-801 (n = 7) in the patch pipette. (D) Correlation between the change in sag ratio and the number of spikes evoked during TBS. (E) Scatter plot showing that there was no strong relationship between the change in sag ratio and the decrease in firing rate.

Figure 5—source data 1. Sag ratio in TBS condition without drugs and with BAPTA and MK801.

Figure 6. Block of Ih does not prevent reduction in excitability.

Figure 6.

(A-B) Inclusion of ZD7288 in the patch pipette blocked Ih, abolishing the voltage sag. Each trace shown is the mean of 10 responses. (C-D) Example cell showing the effect of TBS in the presence of ZD7288 inside the patch pipette. Scatter plot shows the response to each of the maximum current injections during the testing protocol (grey circles). The red circles mark the mean response at each timepoint. Dark grey borders mark the current steps depicted in (D). (E-F) Comparisons of changes in firing rate (E), input resistance (F) and latency (G) between TBS alone (n = 16) and TBS with ZD7288 (n = 6). Error bars show mean ± SEM.

Figure 6—source data 1. Spike count, input resistance and first spike latency in TBS condition with ZD7288.

These results indicate that while plasticity of Ih might have been induced by our stimulation protocol, it would tend to increase rather than decrease intrinsic excitability. Therefore plasticity in another, opposing, mechanism determined the overall change in excitability.

ID contributes to the decrease in intrinsic excitability

Failing a role for Ih, one possible source for the decreased excitability would be potassium conductances. The large increases in first spike latency are suggestive of ID, a slowly inactivating potassium current activated at subthreshold membrane potentials (Storm, 1988), which in CA1 pyramidal neurons is carried by Kv1.1 channels expressed at the AIS (Kirizs et al., 2014). To determine if ID contributed to the reduction in excitability we applied dendrotoxin K, a Kv1.1 specific channel blocker (Robertson et al., 1996). Prior to patching, we pressure injected 200 nM DTX-K for 5 min approximately 100 µm above the pyramidal cell layer, into the region containing the AIS. We then patched cells with an additional 10 nM DTX-K in the pipette solution. In the presence of DTX-K the effect of TBS on excitability appeared to be reduced (Figure 7A–B). The TBS protocol induced more spikes in the presence of DTX-K (>250 spikes in 2/3 of the cells in DTX-K but only 1/3 of the cells in control condition). As the effect on excitability depended on the number of spikes induced during TBS (see Figure 1G), we focused on those cells where TBS evoked at least 250 spikes in both control and DTX-K conditions. We found that the reduction in firing rate was significantly smaller with DTX-K (change in firing rate: TBS −51.8 ± 8.7%, n = 6, DTX-K −25.5 ± 5.3%, n = 6, p=0.037, Figure 7C), the reduction in input resistance was mostly abolished (change in Rin: TBS −17.4 ± 1.7%, n = 6, DTX-K −5.5 ± 1.7%, n = 6, p=9.7×10−4, Figure 7D), as was the increase in latency (change in latency: TBS 192.0 ± 54.2%, n = 6, DTX-K 51.0 ± 10.8%, n = 6, p=0.04, Figure 7E). These results suggest that the reduction in excitability following TBS is mediated, at least in part, by an increase in ID.

Figure 7. Dendrotoxin-K reduces the effect of TBS on excitability.

Figure 7.

(A-B) Example cell recorded after the application of DTX-K. Scatter plot shows the response to each of the maximum current injections during the testing protocol (grey circles). The red circles mark the mean response at each timepoint. Dark grey borders mark the current steps depicted in (B). (C-E) Comparisons of changes in firing rate (C), input resistance (D) and latency (E) between TBS alone (n = 6) and with DTX-K (n = 6) for cells where TBS evoked at least 250 spikes. Error bars show mean ± SEM.

Figure 7—source data 1. Spike count, input resistance and first spike latency in TBS condition with DTX-K.

Bias towards reduced excitability of place cells during virtual spatial navigation

Our results demonstrate that action potential firing can induce a decrease in the intrinsic excitability of CA1 pyramidal neurons, but the question remains as to whether this plasticity affects coding in awake animals. To look for evidence of its expression we reanalyzed data from a previous study, where we made extracellular recordings from CA1 in mice while they navigated virtual reality tracks in both novel and familiar conditions (Bourboulou et al., 2019). The novel and familiar conditions differed by the availability of local visual cues (virtual 3D objects) and population analysis showed that place cell identity and firing locations between the two were uncorrelated, indicating there was a global remapping. To examine how firing rate changed during the session we used two complementary measures: the firing rate during the start, middle and end time periods of the session (which shows the size of the effect), and the lap-by-lap correlation of firing rate over the entire session (which shows the consistency of the effect). We hypothesized that a change in intrinsic excitability should be visible in the out-of-field firing rate, as unlike in-field firing it should not be affected by synaptic plasticity.

We found that in the novel condition the out-of-field firing rate decreased over time (Start 1.58 ± 0.11 Hz, Middle 1.47 ± 0.1 Hz, End 1.37 ± 0.1 Hz, n = 171, F = 5.17 p=0.0065, Figure 8A–C), and that the distribution of the lap-by-lap correlation was shifted towards a decrease in activity (n = 171, p=0.0093, Figure 8D). This effect is compatible with decreased intrinsic excitability. The in-field firing rate, in contrast, increased across the session (Start 3.63 ± 0.19 Hz, Middle: 3.92 ± 0.21 Hz, End 4.19 ± 0.22 Hz, n = 171, F = 5.02, p=0.0071, Figure 8E) and there was a positive shift in the correlation distribution (p=0.048, n = 171, Figure 8F). These observations are consistent with synaptic potentiation within the place field (Bittner et al., 2015; Diamantaki et al., 2018; Mehta et al., 1997; Cohen et al., 2017). In the familiar condition we did not detect any change in the out-of-field firing rate (Start 1.71 ± 0.1 Hz, Middle: 1.62 ± 0.1 Hz, End 1.74 ± 0.11 Hz, n = 268, F = 1.53, p=0.22, Figure 8—figure supplement 1A), though there was a negative shift in the center of the correlation (p=0.012, n = 268, Figure 8—figure supplement 1B). The in-field firing rate also did not change over time (Start 5.3 ± 0.23 Hz, Middle: 5.34 ± 0.22 Hz, End 5.15 ± 0.22 Hz, n = 268, F = 0.87, p=0.42, Figure 8—figure supplement 1C) and there was no change in the firing rate correlation distribution (p=0.25, n = 268, Figure 8—figure supplement 1D). Overall, these results are consistent with the presence of an activity-dependent reduction in intrinsic excitability during exploration of a novel environment.

Figure 8. Changes in excitability during exploration of a novel virtual reality track.

(A) Firing rate map of one cell. Lighter colors indicate higher rate. (B) In-field and out-of-field firing rate by lap for the example cell. (C) Mean firing rates outside the place field across the session. (D) Distribution of the out-of-field firing rate correlations. (E) Mean in-field firing rates across the session. (F) Distribution of the in-field firing rate correlations. Box and whisker plots show median, 25-75th and 5-95th percentiles, n = 171

Figure 8—source data 1. In-field, out-of-field firing rates and correlations across laps for place cells in new condition.

Figure 8.

Figure 8—figure supplement 1. Changes in excitability during exploration of a familiar environment.

Figure 8—figure supplement 1.

(A) Mean firing rates outside the place field across the session. (B) Distribution of the out-of-field firing rate correlations. (C) Mean in-field firing rates across the session. (D) Distribution of the in-field firing rate correlations. Box and whisker plots show median, 25-75th and 5-95th percentiles, n = 268.
Figure 8—figure supplement 1—source data 1. In-field, out-of-field firing rates and correlations across laps for place cells in familiar condition.

Discussion

Our results show that short periods of theta-burst firing, mimicking the firing of place cells within their place fields (Epsztein et al., 2011; Harvey et al., 2009), can induce a global and long-lasting depression of intrinsic excitability in CA1 pyramidal neurons in vivo. This plasticity was homeostatic as its magnitude correlated with the number of action potentials used during the induction protocol. Its induction required intracellular calcium and its expression was partially blocked by the Kv1.1 blocker DTX-K, implicating ID in the AIS. These observations demonstrate an additional form of plasticity of intrinsic excitability not previously described in CA1 pyramidal neurons in vitro. Furthermore, analysis of place cells in behaving mice revealed a bias towards reduced excitability that is consistent with the expression of this plasticity during spatial navigation.

Previous report of plasticity of global intrinsic excitability of CA1 pyramidal cells in vitro have implicated an upregulation of the dendritic conductance Ih (Fan et al., 2005; Campanac et al., 2008). However, subsequent work revealed that the direction and spatial extend of Ih modulation depends on the strength of the stimulation used to induce the plasticity. Thus Ih could be locally decreased (thus enhancing intrinsic excitability) in response to more moderate stimulations associated with lower levels of synaptic potentiation (Campanac et al., 2008). The change in Ih we observed was small and went in the opposite direction to the overall decrease in excitability, consistent with in vitro observations following moderate stimulation (Campanac et al., 2008), and indicates action potential firing only weakly engages Ih plasticity in vivo. One reason for this may be the lower input resistance of dendrites in vivo reducing action potential backpropagation (Kamondi et al., 1998; but see Waters and Helmchen, 2004) and concomitant calcium entry (Campanac et al., 2008). This lower dendritic input resistance in vivo (Destexhe et al., 2003), however, also limits the ability of dendritic currents to propagate to the soma, and therefore changes may have occurred in the distal dendrites but had little to no impact on somatic excitability. In either case our results suggest that Ih regulates excitability locally rather than globally under our conditions.

Our results instead suggest that an increase in ID was contributing to the decrease in excitability, through increasing the delay to the onset of firing. Perhaps surprisingly, we also saw a reduction in the change in input resistance by DTX-K, which suggests that some Kv1 channels are active at the holding membrane potential. This could be explained by the overlap of the activation and de-inactivation voltage dependencies of ID at around −75 mV (Storm, 1988), approximately the true holding potential in our recordings, which could allow for a small, continuous ID. This observation also favors a shift in the activation properties of the channels over membrane insertion as the mechanism of regulation, as most of those newly inserted channels would remain closed. In CA1 pyramidal neurons ID is primarily carried by Kv1.1 and its expression is highly localized to the AIS (Kirizs et al., 2014). As this is the site of action potential generation, changes in AIS excitability can have a significant influence on the excitability of a neuron without affecting synaptic integration (Kole and Stuart, 2012; Rama et al., 2017). Functionally, this distinguishes it from plasticity in dendritic conductances such as Ih and Ia, which primarily regulate the input rather than output of the neuron. That DTX-K did not completely prevent the reduction in excitability might be explained by incomplete block of Kv1.1 channels or the involvement of other Kv1 channel subtypes (Monaghan et al., 2001), or by plasticity in other conductances such as inwardly rectifying potassium channels (Kim and Johnston, 2015) which are active around the resting membrane potential. Another possibility is an increase in the tonic GABA current, for example through a calcium-dependent increase in GABA-A receptors exocytosis such as has been described in cultured hippocampal neurons (Saliba et al., 2012). This increase could contribute to the shift in the input-output curve, though it should also speed up the membrane time constant and therefore decrease the latency to firing (Wlodarczyk et al., 2013), opposite to what we observed. Homeostatic plasticity of Kv1 has typically been associated with structural changes over hours or days in response to sustained input (Kuba et al., 2015; Cudmore et al., 2010), and in this sense the plasticity we observed is unusual, as it occurs rapidly and in response to a relatively small stimulus. Such a sensitive mechanism for homeostasis may be necessary in CA1 as pyramidal neurons are characterized by their near silence outside of their active fields, and therefore homeostatic regulation based on sustained changes in firing rates, as seen in the neocortex, is less relevant under normal conditions.

Extrinsic changes might also be a factor. CA1 pyramidal neurons are embedded in a complex local feed-back inhibitory network (Pouille and Scanziani, 2004) and it is possible that the TBS-induced burst firing was sufficient to induce plasticity in recurrently connected interneurons. Indeed, theta-burst firing of CA1 pyramidal cell has been shown to induce plasticity at inhibitory synapses in vitro via retrograde endocannabinoid signaling (Younts et al., 2013). This plasticity, however, led to a long-term depression at both somatic and dendritic inhibitory synapses and is thus unlikely to account for the decrease in excitability we observed. On the other hand, feed-forward synaptic stimulations (of the Schaffer collaterals) were shown to induce plasticity of intrinsic excitability (in addition to synaptic plasticity) in parvalbumin-expressing basket cells through a mGluR-dependent downregulation of Kv1 channels (Campanac et al., 2013). It is unclear, however, if this plasticity could be engaged via feedback activation. Opposite to this prediction, our effect was prevented by intracellular BAPTA and therefore calcium-dependent, unlike the mGluR-mediated plasticity, and the initial input resistance of CA1 pyramidal cells was increased in the presence of DTX-K, opposite of what would be expected from increased synaptic inhibition. Nonetheless, a contribution of inhibitory plasticity via other mechanisms is difficult to fully rule out in vivo given the paucity of efficient intracellular blockers of GABA-A receptors (Atherton et al., 2016).

The differences between our findings and those in vitro may relate to the nature of the different preparations. The in vitro slice preparation offers many advantages to study synaptic and intrinsic plasticity such as the ability to perform long-lasting high-quality recordings, good spatial and temporal control of stimulated fibers and the ease to deliver antagonists of ligand- and voltage-gated channels. Studies using this preparation have led to a wealth of data on plasticity of intrinsic excitability that constitute a solid framework to interpret results obtained in vivo. Nevertheless, the in vitro slice preparation remains artificial, the neuronal activity is strongly reduced and the neuromodulatory system is inoperant. The slicing procedure itself is also traumatic for the neurons, invoking stress responses alongside inevitable damage to the axons and dendrites, which results in cellular changes that could affect the expression of plasticity (Taubenfeld et al., 2002; Ho et al., 2004). Therefore, an important question is to decipher whether and how plasticity of intrinsic neuronal excitability operate in more naturalistic conditions in vivo. Ideally, these recordings should be performed in awake drug free animals. However these are often short lasting (Epsztein et al., 2011; Lee et al., 2009; Harvey et al., 2009; Epsztein et al., 2010; Lee et al., 2006) and obtaining long-lasting, stable recordings, necessary to measure long-term plasticity, remains difficult. To balance these problems we recorded in anesthetized rats, which affords more stability than awake preparations at the expense of being in an unnatural brain state. Because of this, some considerations should be made in interpreting our results. The anesthetics used will have directly and indirectly affected neuronal excitability, including via an allosteric potentiation of GABA-A receptors and activation of inwardly rectifying potassium channels (Möhler et al., 2002; Nagi and Pineyro, 2014; Hein, 2006), and under anesthesia the levels of neuromodulators that regulate synaptic plasticity (Palacios-Filardo and Mellor, 2019; Dias et al., 2013) differ compared to awake states. These factors may have influenced the plasticity we observed, and therefore its significance needs to be confirmed in behaving animals.

We did, however, see changes in out-of-field firing during exploration of virtual reality environments that are consistent with an activity-dependent decrease in intrinsic excitability. These effects were present in novel environments, and although we did not detect any changes in the out-of-field firing rate in the familiar environment, the same tendency towards a decrease was still visible in the firing rate correlations. This leaves open the possibility that the same processes underlying the decrease in excitability were present but masked by other changes. One caveat to consider, however, is that it is not known how synaptic strength outside of the place field is affected by exploration and plasticity within the place field itself (Diamantaki et al., 2018). And as discussed above, plasticity of interneuronal activity may have also been involved. Nonetheless, despite these open questions, our results show that in a novel environment the excitability of place cells tends to decrease on a timescale that is consistent with the plasticity we have described.

Fast, sensitive homeostatic plasticity of intrinsic excitability could potentially be involved in the regulation of coding in CA1. Indeed, the population of neurons that encode an environment or experience is only a subset of the population that could potentially do so (Thompson and Best, 1989), and those cells that are active tend to have higher levels of intrinsic excitability (Pignatelli et al., 2019; Grosmark and Buzsáki, 2016; Alme et al., 2014; Rich et al., 2014; Buzsáki and Mizuseki, 2014; Epsztein et al., 2011). Furthermore, studies that have enhanced excitability in a subset of cells by overexpressing CREB have shown that this biases cells to participate in the engram (Kim et al., 2014; Sekeres et al., 2010). As the population of active cells is roughly constant in both CA1 and the amygdala it appears that selection is determined, in part, competitively through inhibition, a process which the more excitable cells are most likely to win (Trouche et al., 2016). To prevent the network from becoming dominated by the same sets of neurons this requires mechanisms to modulate excitability, without interfering with learning already contained within the synapses, for which global forms of plasticity of intrinsic excitability appear ideally suited. The activity-driven decrease in excitability we have described might therefore contribute to such a process. In line with this interpretation, it was recently shown that cFos-labelled engram cells show reduced likelihood to encode a novel environment (Tanaka et al., 2018). It remains to be determined, though, how these changes relate to the CREB-associated increases in excitability that have been associated with learning (Lisman et al., 2018). Enhanced excitability by CREB is associated with a decrease in the AHP (Lopez de Armentia et al., 2007; Yu et al., 2017), in which we saw no change. This is unsurprising though, as CREB is not activated by action potential firing, but rather requires calcium entry at the synapses (Deisseroth et al., 1996). This points towards the wider question of how action potential-driven plasticity interacts with synaptically-driven changes in intrinsic excitability, as the balance between them may determine how the cell contributes to coding future experiences. In particular, it remains to be determined how intrinsic excitability is modulated following place field activity in behaving animals (Schmidt-Hieber and Nolan, 2017Debanne et al., 2019), and if it is affected by the plateau potentials that have been shown to induce place field formation (Bittner et al., 2015; Diamantaki et al., 2018; Bittner et al., 2017).

Materials and methods

Key resources table.

Reagent type
(species) or
resource
Designation Source or reference Identifiers Additional information
Chemical compound, drug ZD7288 Tocris Bioscience Cat. No. 1000
Chemical compound, drug Dendrotoxin K Sigma Aldrich D4183
Chemical compound, drug MK-801 Tocris Bioscience Cat. No. 0924

Ethics statement

All experiments were approved by the Institut National de la Santé. et de la Recherche Médicale (INSERM) animal care and use committee and authorized by the Ministère de l’Education Nationale de l’Enseignement Superieur et de la Recherche (agreement number 692 02048.02), in accordance with the European community council directives (2010/63/UE).

Surgery and anesthesia

Male Wistar rats (P25-P35, Charles River) were anesthetized with a combination of fentanyl (7.5 µg/Kg), medetomidine (225 µg/Kg), midazolam (6 mg/Kg). Maintenance doses of one third the initial dose were given every 45 min. The surface of the skull was cleaned and covered with a thin layer of cyanoacrylate glue, and a headplate attached with dental cement. The craniotomies were drilled at −3.5 mm dorsoventral and 2.5 mm lateral from bregma, and were approximately 0.5 mm in diameter. To allow access with the patch pipettes, the dura was dried and a small incision was made using a 26G needle. The ground electrode was placed underneath the skin at the back of the neck and sealed with Kwik-Cast (World Precision Instruments).

In vivo electrophysiology

Whole-cell patch-clamp recordings were obtained from CA1 pyramidal cells. Before patching we located CA1 using a low-resistance glass electrode filled with ringer solution (consisting of, in mM: 135 NaCl, 5.4 KCl, 1 MgCl2, 1.8 CaCl2 and 5 HEPES, pH 7.3 with NaOH). This was lowered through the cortex until ripples and bursting activity characteristic of CA1 could be detected in the LFP. Typically this was around 2100 µm deep. Patch electrodes (5-10MOhm) were pulled from borosilicate glass (Harvard Apparatus) and backfilled with an intracellular solution consisting of (in mM): 135 K-Gluconate, 4 KCl, 10 HEPES, 2 Mg-ATP, 0.4 GTP, plus 0.2% biocytin. The pH was adjusted to 7.2 with KOH. To prevent the pipette from clogging high pressure (around 800mbar) was applied while it was slowly lowered to the target depth, about 50 µm above the pyramidal cell layer. Once there the pressure was immediately lowered to between 25 and 35mbar. Before searching for cells the craniotomy was filled with 2% agar solution and the pipette capacitance was compensated. After breaking in we allowed 10 min for the cells to stabilize before starting the protocol.

For the pressure injection of DTX-K, a broken patch pipette (tip diameter approximately 10–20 µm) containing 200 nM DTX-K dissolved in ringer solution was lowered to 100 µm above the pyramidal cell layer. Once positioned, DTX-K was injected with gentle pressure (around 15mbar) for 5 min.

Each TBS series consisted of 10 EPSC-like waveforms delivered at 100 Hz, repeated 10x at 5 Hz. This protocol was repeated 3x at 15 s intervals. The testing protocol consisted of 3 hyperpolarizing current injections followed by between 3–6 depolarizing steps, repeated 10 times. Each step was 300 ms long, with a 1.5 s interval. The hyperpolarizing step amplitudes were −300 pA, −250 pA and −200 pA, while the range of depolarizing step amplitudes was adjusted for each cell, depending on their initial excitability. For the TBS and testing protocols the cell was held at −60 mV. The liquid junction potential was calculated at 17 mV between the intracellular and ringer solutions, and was not corrected.

Data were acquired using a NPI ELC-3XS amplifier (NPI Electronics) and digitized with an LIH-1600 (HEKA Electronik). Acquisition protocols were implemented in Patch Master software (HEKA Electronik). Output signals were passed through a digidata 1440A (Molecular Devices) and recorded with axoscope (Molecular Devices). The current signal was filtered with a 50 Hz notch filter (Quest Scientific).

Histology

After recording the rats were transcardially perfused with 4% PFA. The brains were then left in 4% PFA overnight at 4°C, before washing and storing at 4°C in PBS. To stain for biocytin 50–100 µm thick slices were incubated in PBS containing 1:1000 streptavidin-alexa 594 (Invitrogen) or streptavidin-cyanine 3 (Jackson Immunoresearch), 0.3% triton X-100% and 2% normal goat serum for 48–72 hr at 4°C, and agitated throughout. Slices were then washed in PBS three times for 10 min each at 4°C and under agitation. Images were acquired under a confocal microscope (SP5X, Leica) with a 40x objective and cells were reconstructed in Neurolucida (MBF Bioscience). In total we recovered 45/56 cells (TBS: 12/16, Control: 4/11, ZD: 6/6, MK: 7/7, BAPTA: 7/7, DTX-K: 9/9).

Reagents and drugs

All reagents and DTX-K were obtained from Sigma-Aldrich. The drugs ZD7288 and MK-801 were obtained from Tocris Biosciences. Fentanyl (Fentadon, Dechra) and medetomidine (Domitor, Orion Pharma) were obtained from Centravet, and midazolam was obtained from Accord Healthcare France.

Data analysis

Data were analyzed using clampfit (Molecular devices), Matlab (Mathworks), Statistica (Tibco) and Excel (Microsoft). Only cells with an initial Vm < −55 mV, Rs <100 MOhms and spike amplitude >40 mV, and which were stable up to completion of the 20 min post-TBS test protocol were included in the analysis.

Current-firing rate curves were normalized using the equation:

(X – minPre)/ (maxPre – minPre)

where X is the spike count, minPre is the mean spike count in response to the smallest current step prior to TBS and maxPre is the mean spike count for the largest current step prior to TBS.

To calculate the series resistance an exponential between 5 and 15 ms on each current step and extrapolating back to 0 ms to estimate the voltage drop, and then the voltage drop was plotted against the injected current and fitted with linear regression. The series resistance was subtracted offline. The input resistance was measured on the hyperpolarizing current injections only, using the voltage minimum within the first 150 ms. The median of Rin values across all steps was taken as the input resistance of the cell. The AP threshold was taken as the point where dV/dT reached 10 V/s, and was measured as the median value of the 1st AP across all depolarizing steps. The AHP was measured as the minimum voltage reached during a 50 ms window after the time of the spike threshold. Only spikes in which the following spike or end of the current step fell outside of this window were included in the analysis. The latency to 1st spike was measured on the maximal current steps. The burst index represents the mean fraction of spikes occurring in bursts, which we defined as two or more spikes with inter-spike intervals of less than 10 ms, and was calculated for each current step amplitude. We then took the highest value from the step amplitudes that induced an average of 5 or more spikes. The Vm variance was calculated in 5 s segments of spontaneous activity, the value for each time point was taken as the median of all the segments in the period before commencing the testing protocol. The sag ratio was calculated as the steady state Vm (median of the final 50 ms) divided by the minimum Vm during the first 150 ms. The value for each cell was taken as the median across all hyperpolarizing current steps.

To examine the evolution of place cell firing rate we re-analyzed data from a previous study, where we made silicon probe recordings from CA1 in mice as they ran back and forth along a virtual linear track (Bourboulou et al., 2019). In that study each experiment consisted of up to four sessions, two in a familiar condition followed by one or two in a novel condition. Each session lasted 15–20 min, with 3 min in the dark between sessions. For our analysis we took only data from the second familiar (14 sessions from five mice) and first novel sessions (10 sessions from five mice). The conditions used were the ‘object track’, ‘pattern no object track’ and ‘pattern object track’ (the latter was not described in Bourboulou et al., 2019, but consisted of the ‘pattern no object track’ with identical objects to the ‘object track’). Each direction along the track was analyzed independently. To ensure we did not use unstable cells we took only those that had stability and spatial information values above 0.1. The stability was the mean of the distribution of all the pairwise correlations of all the laps. The lap-by-lap firing rate correlation across each session was a Pearson correlation. To calculate the mean firing rates each session was divided into thirds based on the duration of the session, corresponding to the start, middle and end of the session.

Within groups statistical comparisons were made using paired t-tests, and between group comparisons using two-tailed t-tests. Linear regression was used for comparisons between time points of the current-spiking curves. The extracellular firing rates were compared using one-way repeated measures ANOVA. The center of the in-field and out-of-field correlation distributions was tested using either a sign-test or a one-tailed t-test, depending on the normality of the distribution. The threshold for significance was p=0.05. Values are given as mean ± SEM unless otherwise stated.

Acknowledgements

This study was funded by INSERM, Aix-Marseille University, a rising star grant from of the A*MIDEX project (n° ANR-11-IDEX-0001–02) funded by the « Investissements d’Avenir» French Government program (to JE), by the European Research Council under the European Community's Seventh Framework Program (ERC-2013-StG-338141_Intraspace to JE), by the ANR (FLAG-ERA-HBP-partnering project ANR-17-HBPR-0004 HIPPOPLAST) and region PACA. The authors would like to thank Laurent Aniksztejn, Dominique Debanne and Matthew Nolan for comments on the manuscript, Geoffrey Marti for carefully reading and correcting the manuscript, members of the Epsztein lab for discussions and the animal facility, administrative and imaging platforms of INMED for support.

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Contributor Information

Peter James Morgan, Email: peter.morgan@inserm.fr.

Jérôme Epsztein, Email: jerome.epsztein@inserm.fr.

Inna Slutsky, Tel Aviv University, Israel.

Eve Marder, Brandeis University, United States.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grants:

  • Seventh Framework Programme ERC-2013-StG 338141 - IntraSpace to Jérôme Epsztein.

  • Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale to Jérôme Epsztein.

  • Agence Nationale de la Recherche FLAG-ERA-HBP-partnering project ANR-17-HBPR-0004 HIPPOPLAST to Jérôme Epsztein.

  • A*MIDEX ANR-11-IDEX-0001–02 to Jérôme Epsztein.

  • Région Provence-Alpes-Côte d'Azur to Jérôme Epsztein.

Additional information

Competing interests

No competing interests declared.

Author contributions

Conceptualization, Data curation, Software, Formal analysis, Investigation, Visualization, Methodology, Writing—original draft.

Formal analysis, Investigation.

Formal analysis, Investigation.

Formal analysis, Investigation.

Conceptualization, Resources, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Project administration, Writing—review and editing.

Ethics

Animal experimentation: All experiments were approved by the Institut National de la Santé. et de la Recherche Médicale (INSERM) animal care and use committee and authorized by the Ministère de l'Education Nationale de l'Enseignement Superieur et de la Recherche (agreement number 692 02048.02), in accordance with the European community council directives (2010/63/UE).

Additional files

Transparent reporting form

Data availability

We provide the source data files used to generate all figures.

References

  1. Alme CB, Miao C, Jezek K, Treves A, Moser EI, Moser MB. Place cells in the Hippocampus: eleven maps for eleven rooms. PNAS. 2014;111:18428–18435. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1421056111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Aou S, Woody CD, Birt D. Increases in excitability of neurons of the motor cortex of cats after rapid acquisition of eye blink conditioning. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1992;12:560–569. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.12-02-00560.1992. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Atherton LA, Burnell ES, Mellor JR. Assessment of methods for the intracellular blockade of GABAA receptors. PLOS ONE. 2016;11:e0160900. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0160900. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Belmeguenai A, Hosy E, Bengtsson F, Pedroarena CM, Piochon C, Teuling E, He Q, Ohtsuki G, De Jeu MT, Elgersma Y, De Zeeuw CI, Jörntell H, Hansel C. Intrinsic plasticity complements long-term potentiation in parallel fiber input gain control in cerebellar purkinje cells. Journal of Neuroscience. 2010;30:13630–13643. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3226-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bittner KC, Grienberger C, Vaidya SP, Milstein AD, Macklin JJ, Suh J, Tonegawa S, Magee JC. Conjunctive input processing drives feature selectivity in hippocampal CA1 neurons. Nature Neuroscience. 2015;18:1133–1142. doi: 10.1038/nn.4062. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bittner KC, Milstein AD, Grienberger C, Romani S, Magee JC. Behavioral time scale synaptic plasticity underlies CA1 place fields. Science. 2017;357:1033–1036. doi: 10.1126/science.aan3846. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bourboulou R, Marti G, Michon FX, El Feghaly E, Nouguier M, Robbe D, Koenig J, Epsztein J. Dynamic control of hippocampal spatial coding resolution by local visual cues. eLife. 2019;8:e44487. doi: 10.7554/eLife.44487. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Buzsáki G, Mizuseki K. The log-dynamic brain: how skewed distributions affect network operations. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2014;15:264–278. doi: 10.1038/nrn3687. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Campanac E, Daoudal G, Ankri N, Debanne D. Downregulation of dendritic I(h) in CA1 pyramidal neurons after LTP. Journal of Neuroscience. 2008;28:8635–8643. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1411-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Campanac E, Gasselin C, Baude A, Rama S, Ankri N, Debanne D. Enhanced intrinsic excitability in basket cells maintains excitatory-inhibitory balance in hippocampal circuits. Neuron. 2013;77:712–722. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.12.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Cohen JD, Bolstad M, Lee AK. Experience-dependent shaping of hippocampal CA1 intracellular activity in novel and familiar environments. eLife. 2017;6:e23040. doi: 10.7554/eLife.23040. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Cudmore RH, Fronzaroli-Molinieres L, Giraud P, Debanne D. Spike-time precision and network synchrony are controlled by the homeostatic regulation of the D-type potassium current. Journal of Neuroscience. 2010;30:12885–12895. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0740-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Daoudal G, Debanne D. Long-term plasticity of intrinsic excitability: learning rules and mechanisms. Learning & Memory. 2003;10:456–465. doi: 10.1101/lm.64103. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Debanne D, Inglebert Y, Russier M. Plasticity of intrinsic neuronal excitability. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2019;54:73–82. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2018.09.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Deisseroth K, Bito H, Tsien RW. Signaling from synapse to nucleus: postsynaptic CREB phosphorylation during multiple forms of hippocampal synaptic plasticity. Neuron. 1996;16:89–101. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80026-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Desai NS, Rutherford LC, Turrigiano GG. Plasticity in the intrinsic excitability of cortical pyramidal neurons. Nature Neuroscience. 1999;2:515–520. doi: 10.1038/9165. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Destexhe A, Rudolph M, Paré D. The high-conductance state of neocortical neurons in vivo. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2003;4:739–751. doi: 10.1038/nrn1198. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Diamantaki M, Coletta S, Nasr K, Zeraati R, Laturnus S, Berens P, Preston-Ferrer P, Burgalossi A. Manipulating hippocampal place cell activity by Single-Cell stimulation in freely moving mice. Cell Reports. 2018;23:32–38. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2018.03.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Dias RB, Rombo DM, Ribeiro JA, Henley JM, Sebastião AM. Adenosine: setting the stage for plasticity. Trends in Neurosciences. 2013;36:248–257. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2012.12.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Epsztein J, Lee AK, Chorev E, Brecht M. Impact of spikelets on hippocampal CA1 pyramidal cell activity during spatial exploration. Science. 2010;327:474–477. doi: 10.1126/science.1182773. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Epsztein J, Brecht M, Lee AK. Intracellular determinants of hippocampal CA1 place and silent cell activity in a novel environment. Neuron. 2011;70:109–120. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.03.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Fan Y, Fricker D, Brager DH, Chen X, Lu HC, Chitwood RA, Johnston D. Activity-dependent decrease of excitability in rat hippocampal neurons through increases in I(h) Nature Neuroscience. 2005;8:1542–1551. doi: 10.1038/nn1568. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Frick A, Magee J, Johnston D. LTP is accompanied by an enhanced local excitability of pyramidal neuron dendrites. Nature Neuroscience. 2004;7:126–135. doi: 10.1038/nn1178. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Grosmark AD, Buzsáki G. Diversity in neural firing dynamics supports both rigid and learned hippocampal sequences. Science. 2016;351:1440–1443. doi: 10.1126/science.aad1935. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Han JH, Kushner SA, Yiu AP, Cole CJ, Matynia A, Brown RA, Neve RL, Guzowski JF, Silva AJ, Josselyn SA. Neuronal competition and selection during memory formation. Science. 2007;316:457–460. doi: 10.1126/science.1139438. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Harvey CD, Collman F, Dombeck DA, Tank DW. Intracellular dynamics of hippocampal place cells during virtual navigation. Nature. 2009;461:941–946. doi: 10.1038/nature08499. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Hein L. Adrenoceptors and signal transduction in neurons. Cell and Tissue Research. 2006;326:541–551. doi: 10.1007/s00441-006-0285-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Ho OH, Delgado JY, O'Dell TJ. Phosphorylation of proteins involved in activity-dependent forms of synaptic plasticity is altered in hippocampal slices maintained in vitro. Journal of Neurochemistry. 2004;91:1344–1357. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.2004.02815.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Josselyn SA, Frankland PW. Memory allocation: mechanisms and function. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 2018;41:389–413. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-080317-061956. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Kamondi A, Acsády L, Buzsáki G. Dendritic spikes are enhanced by cooperative network activity in the intact Hippocampus. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1998;18:3919–3928. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-10-03919.1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Kim J, Kwon JT, Kim HS, Josselyn SA, Han JH. Memory recall and modifications by activating neurons with elevated CREB. Nature Neuroscience. 2014;17:65–72. doi: 10.1038/nn.3592. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Kim CS, Johnston D. A1 adenosine receptor-mediated GIRK channels contribute to the resting conductance of CA1 neurons in the dorsal Hippocampus. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2015;113:2511–2523. doi: 10.1152/jn.00951.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Kirizs T, Kerti-Szigeti K, Lorincz A, Nusser Z. Distinct axo-somato-dendritic distributions of three potassium channels in CA1 hippocampal pyramidal cells. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2014;39:1771–1783. doi: 10.1111/ejn.12526. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Kole MH, Stuart GJ. Signal processing in the axon initial segment. Neuron. 2012;73:235–247. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.01.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Kuba H, Yamada R, Ishiguro G, Adachi R. Redistribution of Kv1 and Kv7 enhances neuronal excitability during structural axon initial segment plasticity. Nature Communications. 2015;6:8815. doi: 10.1038/ncomms9815. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Lee AK, Manns ID, Sakmann B, Brecht M. Whole-cell recordings in freely moving rats. Neuron. 2006;51:399–407. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.07.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Lee AK, Epsztein J, Brecht M. Head-anchored whole-cell recordings in freely moving rats. Nature Protocols. 2009;4:385–392. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2009.5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Lee D, Lin BJ, Lee AK. Hippocampal place fields emerge upon single-cell manipulation of excitability during behavior. Science. 2012;337:849–853. doi: 10.1126/science.1221489. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Leutgeb S, Leutgeb JK, Treves A, Moser MB, Moser EI. Distinct ensemble codes in hippocampal Areas CA3 and CA1. Science. 2004;305:1295–1298. doi: 10.1126/science.1100265. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Leutgeb S, Leutgeb JK, Barnes CA, Moser EI, McNaughton BL, Moser MB. Independent codes for spatial and episodic memory in hippocampal neuronal ensembles. Science. 2005;309:619–623. doi: 10.1126/science.1114037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Lisman J, Cooper K, Sehgal M, Silva AJ. Memory formation depends on both synapse-specific modifications of synaptic strength and cell-specific increases in excitability. Nature Neuroscience. 2018;21:309–314. doi: 10.1038/s41593-018-0076-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Lopez de Armentia M, Jancic D, Olivares R, Alarcon JM, Kandel ER, Barco A. cAMP response element-binding protein-mediated gene expression increases the intrinsic excitability of CA1 pyramidal neurons. Journal of Neuroscience. 2007;27:13909–13918. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3850-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Losonczy A, Makara JK, Magee JC. Compartmentalized dendritic plasticity and input feature storage in neurons. Nature. 2008;452:436–441. doi: 10.1038/nature06725. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Magee JC. Dendritic hyperpolarization-activated currents modify the integrative properties of hippocampal CA1 pyramidal neurons. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1998;18:7613–7624. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-19-07613.1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Mahon S, Charpier S. Bidirectional plasticity of intrinsic excitability controls sensory inputs efficiency in layer 5 barrel cortex neurons in vivo. Journal of Neuroscience. 2012;32:11377–11389. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0415-12.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Margrie T, Brecht M, Sakmann B. In vivo, low-resistance, whole-cell recordings from neurons in the anaesthetized and awake mammalian brain. Pfluegers Archiv - European Journal of Physiology. 2002;444:491–498. doi: 10.1007/s00424-002-0831-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Maroso M, Szabo GG, Kim HK, Alexander A, Bui AD, Lee SH, Lutz B, Soltesz I. Cannabinoid control of learning and memory through HCN channels. Neuron. 2016;89:1059–1073. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.01.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Mehta MR, Barnes CA, McNaughton BL. Experience-dependent, asymmetric expansion of hippocampal place fields. PNAS. 1997;94:8918–8921. doi: 10.1073/pnas.94.16.8918. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Möhler H, Fritschy JM, Rudolph U. A new benzodiazepine pharmacology. Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics. 2002;300:2–8. doi: 10.1124/jpet.300.1.2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Monaghan MM, Trimmer JS, Rhodes KJ. Experimental localization of Kv1 family voltage-gated K+ channel alpha and beta subunits in rat hippocampal formation. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2001;21:5973–5983. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-16-05973.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Nagi K, Pineyro G. Kir3 channel signaling complexes: focus on opioid receptor signaling. Frontiers in Cellular Neuroscience. 2014;8:186. doi: 10.3389/fncel.2014.00186. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Narayanan R, Johnston D. Long-term potentiation in rat hippocampal neurons is accompanied by spatially widespread changes in intrinsic oscillatory dynamics and excitability. Neuron. 2007;56:1061–1075. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.10.033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Nolan MF, Malleret G, Dudman JT, Buhl DL, Santoro B, Gibbs E, Vronskaya S, Buzsáki G, Siegelbaum SA, Kandel ER, Morozov A. A behavioral role for dendritic integration: hcn1 channels constrain spatial memory and plasticity at inputs to distal dendrites of CA1 pyramidal neurons. Cell. 2004;119:719–732. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2004.11.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Oh MM, Kuo AG, Wu WW, Sametsky EA, Disterhoft JF. Watermaze learning enhances excitability of CA1 pyramidal neurons. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2003;90:2171–2179. doi: 10.1152/jn.01177.2002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Palacios-Filardo J, Mellor JR. Neuromodulation of hippocampal long-term synaptic plasticity. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2019;54:37–43. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2018.08.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Paz JT, Mahon S, Tiret P, Genet S, Delord B, Charpier S. Multiple forms of activity-dependent intrinsic plasticity in layer V cortical neurones in vivo. The Journal of Physiology. 2009;587:3189–3205. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2009.169334. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Pignatelli M, Ryan TJ, Roy DS, Lovett C, Smith LM, Muralidhar S, Tonegawa S. Engram cell excitability state determines the efficacy of memory retrieval. Neuron. 2019;101:274–284. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.11.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Pouille F, Scanziani M. Routing of spike series by dynamic circuits in the Hippocampus. Nature. 2004;429:717–723. doi: 10.1038/nature02615. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Rama S, Zbili M, Fékété A, Tapia M, Benitez MJ, Boumedine N, Garrido JJ, Debanne D. The role of axonal Kv1 channels in CA3 pyramidal cell excitability. Scientific Reports. 2017;7:315. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-00388-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Rich PD, Liaw HP, Lee AK. Place cells. large environments reveal the statistical structure governing hippocampal representations. Science. 2014;345:814–817. doi: 10.1126/science.1255635. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Robertson B, Owen D, Stow J, Butler C, Newland C. Novel effects of dendrotoxin homologues on subtypes of mammalian Kv1 potassium channels expressed in xenopus oocytes. FEBS Letters. 1996;383:26–30. doi: 10.1016/0014-5793(96)00211-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Saliba RS, Kretschmannova K, Moss SJ. Activity-dependent phosphorylation of GABAA receptors regulates receptor insertion and tonic current. The EMBO Journal. 2012;31:2937–2951. doi: 10.1038/emboj.2012.109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Schmidt-Hieber C, Nolan MF. Synaptic integrative mechanisms for spatial cognition. Nature Neuroscience. 2017;20:1483–1492. doi: 10.1038/nn.4652. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Sekeres MJ, Neve RL, Frankland PW, Josselyn SA. Dorsal hippocampal CREB is both necessary and sufficient for spatial memory. Learning & Memory. 2010;17:280–283. doi: 10.1101/lm.1785510. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Storm JF. Temporal integration by a slowly inactivating K+ current in hippocampal neurons. Nature. 1988;336:379–381. doi: 10.1038/336379a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Tanaka KZ, He H, Tomar A, Niisato K, Huang AJY, McHugh TJ. The hippocampal engram maps experience but not place. Science. 2018;361:392–397. doi: 10.1126/science.aat5397. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Taubenfeld SM, Stevens KA, Pollonini G, Ruggiero J, Alberini CM. Profound molecular changes following hippocampal slice preparation: loss of AMPA receptor subunits and uncoupled mRNA/protein expression. Journal of Neurochemistry. 2002;81:1348–1360. doi: 10.1046/j.1471-4159.2002.00936.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Thompson LT, Best PJ. Place cells and silent cells in the hippocampus of freely-behaving rats. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1989;9:2382–2390. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.09-07-02382.1989. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Titley HK, Brunel N, Hansel C. Toward a Neurocentric View of Learning. Neuron. 2017;95:19–32. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.05.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Trouche S, Perestenko PV, van de Ven GM, Bratley CT, McNamara CG, Campo-Urriza N, Black SL, Reijmers LG, Dupret D. Recoding a cocaine-place memory Engram to a neutral Engram in the Hippocampus. Nature Neuroscience. 2016;19:564–567. doi: 10.1038/nn.4250. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Tsay D, Dudman JT, Siegelbaum SA. HCN1 channels constrain synaptically evoked Ca2+ spikes in distal dendrites of CA1 pyramidal neurons. Neuron. 2007;56:1076–1089. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.11.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Turrigiano G, Abbott LF, Marder E. Activity-dependent changes in the intrinsic properties of cultured neurons. Science. 1994;264:974–977. doi: 10.1126/science.8178157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Wang Z, Xu NL, Wu CP, Duan S, Poo MM. Bidirectional changes in spatial dendritic integration accompanying long-term synaptic modifications. Neuron. 2003;37:463–472. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)01189-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Waters J, Helmchen F. Boosting of action potential backpropagation by neocortical network activity in vivo. Journal of Neuroscience. 2004;24:11127–11136. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2933-04.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Wlodarczyk AI, Xu C, Song I, Doronin M, Wu YW, Walker MC, Semyanov A. Tonic GABAA conductance decreases membrane time constant and increases EPSP-spike precision in hippocampal pyramidal neurons. Frontiers in Neural Circuits. 2013;7:205. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2013.00205. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Yiu AP, Mercaldo V, Yan C, Richards B, Rashid AJ, Hsiang HL, Pressey J, Mahadevan V, Tran MM, Kushner SA, Woodin MA, Frankland PW, Josselyn SA. Neurons are recruited to a memory trace based on relative neuronal excitability immediately before training. Neuron. 2014;83:722–735. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.07.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Younts TJ, Chevaleyre V, Castillo PE. CA1 pyramidal cell theta-burst firing triggers endocannabinoid-mediated long-term depression at both somatic and dendritic inhibitory synapses. Journal of Neuroscience. 2013;33:13743–13757. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0817-13.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Yu XW, Curlik DM, Oh MM, Yin JC, Disterhoft JF. CREB overexpression in dorsal CA1 ameliorates long-term memory deficits in aged rats. eLife. 2017;6:e19358. doi: 10.7554/eLife.19358. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Zhang W, Linden DJ. The other side of the Engram: experience-driven changes in neuronal intrinsic excitability. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2003;4:885–900. doi: 10.1038/nrn1248. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Zhou Y, Won J, Karlsson MG, Zhou M, Rogerson T, Balaji J, Neve R, Poirazi P, Silva AJ. CREB regulates excitability and the allocation of memory to subsets of neurons in the amygdala. Nature Neuroscience. 2009;12:1438–1443. doi: 10.1038/nn.2405. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision letter

Editor: Inna Slutsky1
Reviewed by: Stefan Remy2

In the interests of transparency, eLife publishes the most substantive revision requests and the accompanying author responses.

Acceptance summary:

This work presents a pioneering study addressing whether theta-burst firing, mimicking the firing of place cells within their place fields, induces homeostatic plasticity of intrinsic excitability in vivo in CA1 pyramidal cells. The authors' results in anesthetized rats suggest that theta-bursts induce a rapid homeostatic decrease in excitability. This reduction in excitability is regulated by an increase Kv1.1 conductance in the axon initial segment. This study indicates that action potential firing can regulate global intrinsic excitability of a neuron independently of dendritic excitability, which may have profound implications for computations performed by CA1 neurons.

Decision letter after peer review:

Thank you for submitting your article "Kv1.1 contributes to a rapid homeostatic plasticity of intrinsic excitability in CA1 pyramidal neurons in vivo" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by two peer reviewers, and the evaluation has been overseen by a Reviewing Editor and Eve Marder as the Senior Editor. The following individual involved in review of your submission has agreed to reveal their identity: Stefan Remy (Reviewer #2).

The reviewers have discussed the reviews with one another and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this decision to help you prepare a revised submission.

Summary:

The study by Morgan et al. presents an interesting and technically challenging set of experiments with potential implications for the regulation of place cell firing by intrinsic plasticity. The authors use current injections via whole-cell patch clamp electrodes in anesthetized rats to induce plasticity of intrinsic excitability in CA1 pyramidal cells. Intrinsic plasticity mechanisms have been reported in acute brain slices and they could be involved in longer lasting regulation of cellular excitability without synaptic input specificity. The presence and potential functional implications of such mechanisms have only rarely been investigated in vivo and thus it is currently unknown whether they are relevant for the regulation of firing rates during behavior. Therefore, this study addresses a novel, potentially relevant topic. A demonstration that intrinsic plasticity mechanisms regulate the firing rates of CA1 place cells during spatial navigation would indeed almost represent a paradigm shift. The authors propose activity-dependent plasticity as a candidate mechanism for preventing the activation of identical cell ensembles within different environments, thereby preventing memory interference.

While the reviewers are convinced by the data that TBS can be evoked in vivo (albeit during anesthesia) and that D-type conductances play a role, they are not yet convinced that such a mechanism would be of major relevance for the regulation of place cell activity and memory mechanisms in response to natural stimuli (and firing patterns) observed during behavior. The additional analyses (as specified below) should provide at least some evidence for or be consistent with the presence of intrinsic plasticity in unperturbed in vivo conditions (place cell firing). Moreover, the limitations of the study should be addressed in the Discussion and the interpretation of the results, together with the expectations generated by Abstract and Introduction, should be toned down.

Essential revisions:

1) The manuscript investigates potential mechanisms of place cell ensemble regulation. We know how place cells fire during behavior from decades of extracellular recordings. Whole-cell work of Albert Lee's group involving the senior author of this study and from the Magee lab even make it possible to simulate the subthreshold and suprathreshold membrane potential changes very precisely. Even if we would assume that a theta-burst pattern very roughly resembles place related firing, multiple repetitions of the same firing within an interval of 15 seconds are extremely unlikely during natural behavior. So it remains unclear to us, how ensemble suppression in different environments could be evoked, if it would exist. If intrinsic plasticity would be evoked by a single TBS or a long constant current injection (1-2s), which would be somewhat more naturalistic, I would be more convinced of its relevance, but it would still remain unclear to me how ensemble suppression could work.

We have one suggestion that does not require additional experimentation, but some analyses, which would increase our confidence in the relevance of such an intrinsic plasticity mechanism for place related firing. It is based on the observation, that CA1 neurons of rodents either on linear tracks or on treadmills quite accurately show repeated activation of place cells in natural firing patterns with repetition intervals that are within the time frame of the induction paradigm. The authors could request such data from a lab that has published such results in the past (they may even have these data available from their own work). In my view, two predictions derive from the current manuscript: a) Repeated activation should result in a gradual decrease of the place cell firing rate and b) The time to first spike should be gradually prolonged.

For the theoretically possible scenario that synaptic and intrinsic plasticity mechanisms counterbalance each other, out of place field firing rates could be analysed, which may also represent input from different cell assemblies and should be gradually depressed. If these predictions are confirmed, it will significantly strengthen the manuscript.

2) A cocktail of anesthetic compounds is used (opiods, benzodiazepines, medetomidine) that generates quite artificial conditions, that are impossible to control for (effects on GABAergic transmission and tonic inhibition, deprivation of excitatory input, unclear effects on neuromodulatory afferents and many unknown effects of the drugs alone and in combination). In these conditions, it is not very surprising that the mechanism observed here (D-type dependent) differs from the mechanism described earlier in slice work (HCN). We agree, that the unperturbed morphology of the neurons in vivo could be a possible explanation, but the authors do not provide any experimental evidence for a role of axonal damage, it is a pure speculation. Not only do the authors fail to reproduce an Ih dependent mechanism (see Campanac et al., 2005; Fan et al., 2005; Narayan and Johnston, 2007), they now present an alternative mechanism. This whole issue should be discussed in a comprehensive and fair way, including a discussion of how well distal dendritic currents (and membrane potential changes) can be resolved in somatic in vivo recordings and the pros and cons of slice vs. in vivo work.

3) Role of inhibition. Although briefly discussed, plasticity of inhibitory transmission and tonic inhibition have not been convincingly ruled out as a potential alternative or additional mechanism. This is important as the use of benzodiazipenes and other drugs acting on GABAergic transmission might have been an important co-factor in the data presented here. The discussion of relevant work is generally a bit too superficial.

eLife. 2019 Nov 27;8:e49915. doi: 10.7554/eLife.49915.sa2

Author response


Essential revisions:

1) The manuscripts investigates potential mechanisms of place cell ensemble regulation. We know how place cells fire during behavior from decades of extracellular recordings. Whole-cell work of Albert Lee's group involving the senior author of this study and from the Magee lab even make it possible to simulate the subthreshold and suprathreshold membrane potential changes very precisely. Even if we would assume that a theta burst pattern very roughly resembles place related firing, multiple repetitions of the same firing within an interval of 15 seconds are extremely unlikely during natural behavior. So it remains unclear to us, how ensemble suppression in different environments could be evoked, if it would exist. If intrinsic plasticity would be evoked by a single TBS or a long constant current injection (1-2s), which would be somewhat more naturalistic, I would be more convinced of its relevance, but it would still remain unclear to me how ensemble suppression could work.

We have one suggestion that does not require additional experimentation, but some analyses, which would increase our confidence in the relevance of such an intrinsic plasticity mechanism for place related firing. It is based on the observation, that CA1 neurons of rodents either on linear tracks or on treadmills quite accurately show repeated activation of place cells in natural firing patterns with repetition intervals that are within the time frame of the induction paradigm. The authors could request such data from a lab that has published such results in the past (they may even have these data available from their own work). In my view, two predictions derive from the current manuscript: a) Repeated activation should result in a gradual decrease of the place cell firing rate and b) The time to first spike should be gradually prolonged.

For the theoretically possible scenario that synaptic and intrinsic plasticity mechanisms counterbalance each other, out of place field firing rates could be analysed, which may also represent input from different cell assemblies and should be gradually depressed. If these predictions are confirmed, it will significantly strengthen the manuscript.

We thank the reviewer for suggesting a possible way to test the relevance of our results obtained in anesthetized animals to awake behaving conditions. Following the reviewer’s suggestion, we analyzed data previously obtained in our lab using extracellular silicon probe recordings of CA1 place cells’ activity in head-fixed mice navigating virtual linear tracks in familiar and new conditions. Familiar and new conditions differed by the availability of local visual cues (virtual objects). The pure linear behaviour of mice in these tracks allowed multiple passes through the place field and analysis of the evolution of place cells firing rate over time in both new and familiar conditions. As suggested by the reviewer to search for changes in intrinsic excitability we decided to focus on the out-of-field firing since synaptic plasticity mechanisms are likely to occur inside the place field which might compensate for or even overcome any overall decrease in excitability. We observed a clear and significant tendency for out-of-field firing to decrease over time in the new condition (Start: 1.58 ± 0.11Hz, Middle: 1.47 ± 0.1Hz, End: 1.37 ± 0.1Hz, n = 171, F = 5.17 p = 0.0065, Figure 8A-C), which is consistent with decreased intrinsic excitability over time. And consistent with the presence of synaptic potentiation within the place field we saw an increase in in-field firing rate over time (Start: 3.63 ± 0.19Hz, Middle: 3.92 ± 0.21Hz, End: 4.19 ± 0.22Hz, n = 171, F = 5.02, p = 0.0071, Figure 8E). The changes in in-field firing rate are compatible with synaptic plasticity occurring within the place field (Bittner et al., 2017; Diamantaki et al., 2018) as previously suggested (Mehta et al., 1997). The observed changes in in-field and out-of-field firing rates were experience dependent because they were strongly attenuated or absent in the familiar condition (Figure 8—figure supplement 1). Overall these results are consistent with the presence of an activity-dependent reduction in intrinsic excitability during exploration of a novel environment.

2) A cocktail of anesthetic compounds is used (opiods, benzodiazepines, medetomidine) that generates quite artificial conditions, that are impossible to control for (effects on GABAergic transmission and tonic inhibition, deprivation of excitatory input, unclear effects on neuromodulatory afferents and many unknown effects of the drugs alone and in combination). In these conditions, it is not very surprising that the mechanism observed here (D-type dependent) differs from the mechanism described earlier in slice work (HCN). We agree, that the unperturbed morphology of the neurons in vivo could be a possible explanation, but the authors do not provide any experimental evidence for a role of axonal damage, it is a pure speculation. Not only do the authors fail to reproduce an Ih dependent mechanism (see Campanac et al., 2008; Fan et al., 2005; Narayan and Johnston, 2007), they now present an alternative mechanism. This whole issue should be discussed in a comprehensive and fair way, including a discussion of how well distal dendritic currents (and membrane potential changes) can be resolved in somatic in vivo recordings and the pros and cons of slice vs. in vivo work.

We agree that the anesthetic compounds used in our recordings generate artificial conditions that we cannot ignore when interpreting our data. In the revised version of the manuscript these effects and the fact that they must be considered when interpreting our data is more clearly stated in the Discussion (fifth paragraph). Concerning slice versus in vivo work we would like to highlight that our goal was not to oppose these different preparations which have their advantages and limitations. We are fully aware of the value of previous work using in vitro recordings that directly inspired our own work. In the revised version of the manuscript we tried to account for these different advantages and limitations (see the aforementioned paragraph) and explain our choice of anesthetized in vivo recordings which afford more stability than fully awake preparations at the expense of being in an unnatural brain state. Using this preparation, although we failed to reproduce some forms of Ih plasticity observed in vitro such as a global increase following theta burst firing of CA1 pyramidal cells (Fan et al., 2005; Campanac et al., 2008) we were able to reproduce other forms of Ih plasticity observed in vitro (Figure 5) such as a Ca2+- and NMDAR-dependent decrease in Ih following weaker stimulations inducing a lower level of potentiation (Campanac et al., 2008). The complexity of Ih plasticity in vitro and the need to further assess it in vivo in now more clearly stated in the revised Discussion along with possible explanations (Discussion, second paragraph). The fact that assessing the impact of dendritic conductances on somatic excitability via somatic patch-clamp recordings is more limited in vivo is also acknowledged.

3) Role of inhibition. Although briefly discussed, plasticity of inhibitory transmission and tonic inhibition have not been convincingly ruled out as a potential alternative or additional mechanism. This is important as the use of benzodiazipenes and other drugs acting on GABAergic transmission might have been an important co-factor in the data presented here. The discussion of relevant work is generally a bit too superficial.

We agree with the reviewer that inhibition could be involved in the plasticity we describe and have expanded the Discussion to further examine the possibility of plasticity of both tonic (third paragraph) and phasic (fourth paragraph) inhibition. Notably we highlight that burst firing of CA1 pyramidal cells in vitro induces a long-term depression of both somatic and dendritic feedback inhibition (Younts, Chevaleyre and Castillo, 2013) which is unlikely to account for the reduced excitability we observe.

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    Figure 1—source data 1. Spike count in TBS and control conditions.
    Figure 2—source data 1. Input resistance and first spike latency in TBS and control conditions.
    Figure 3—source data 1. Spike count, input resistance and first spike latency in TBS condition with BAPTA.
    Figure 4—source data 1. Spike count, input resistance and first spike latency in TBS condition with MK801.
    Figure 5—source data 1. Sag ratio in TBS condition without drugs and with BAPTA and MK801.
    Figure 6—source data 1. Spike count, input resistance and first spike latency in TBS condition with ZD7288.
    Figure 7—source data 1. Spike count, input resistance and first spike latency in TBS condition with DTX-K.
    Figure 8—source data 1. In-field, out-of-field firing rates and correlations across laps for place cells in new condition.
    Figure 8—figure supplement 1—source data 1. In-field, out-of-field firing rates and correlations across laps for place cells in familiar condition.
    Transparent reporting form

    Data Availability Statement

    We provide the source data files used to generate all figures.


    Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

    RESOURCES