Abstract
Binocular stereopsis requires the convergence of visual information from corresponding points in visual space seen by two different lines of sight. This may be achieved by superposition of retinal input from each eye onto the same downstream neurons via ipsi- and contralaterally projecting optic nerve fibers. Zebrafish larvae can perceive binocular cues during prey hunting but have exclusively contralateral retinotectal projections. Here we report brain activity in the tectal neuropil ipsilateral to the visually stimulated eye, despite the absence of ipsilateral retinotectal projections. This activity colocalizes with arbors of commissural neurons, termed intertectal neurons (ITNs), that connect the tectal hemispheres. ITNs are GABAergic, establish tectal synapses bilaterally and respond to small moving stimuli. ITN-ablation impairs capture swim initiation when prey is positioned in the binocular strike zone. We propose an intertectal circuit that controls execution of the prey-capture motor program following binocular localization of prey, without requiring ipsilateral retinotectal projections.
Subject terms: Neural circuits, Visual system
Zebrafish larvae can binocularly detect prey objects in order to strike but lack ipsilateral retinotectal fibers for binocular superposition of visual information. Here the authors describe commissural intertectal neurons and show that they are required for the initiation of capture strikes.
Introduction
In order to feed on moving prey, predatory animals must recognize, pursue, and finally capture their target. Hunting sequences of zebrafish larvae consist of a pursuit phase that sometimes culminates in a capture maneuver1,2. The pursuit phase is characterized by discrete approach swims (AS) and asymmetric bouts (J-turns and high-angle turns) that enable the larva to orient toward and approach its prey1,3–6. Capture maneuvers consist of specific high-acceleration capture swims (also called rams) or suction feedings1,7.
Having two forward facing eyes with a binocular field of view may enable predating animals to evaluate target distance (binocular stereopsis) and thereby increase the likelihood of successfully capturing prey. Zebrafish larvae have laterally positioned eyes resulting in a negligible binocular field of view under most behavioral conditions. However, during hunting sequences, larvae consistently converge their eyes, resulting in a substantial increase in their binocular visual field3. Vergence angle varies with target distance8 and capture swims are initiated once the target is located in a stereotypic strike zone within the binocular visual field, around 500 µm directly in front of a larva1,3,8. Furthermore, prey capture is strongly impaired when retinal input is unilaterally removed9. This evidence suggests that zebrafish larvae use binocular depth cues to locate and capture prey.
A binocular stereopsis mechanism for localizing an object in a three-dimensional environment depends on visual information about corresponding object features, as viewed by each eye, converging on binocular downstream neurons. For example, in mammals, it has been suggested that this convergence is achieved by direct superposition of corresponding retinal input via ipsi- and contralaterally projecting optic nerve fibers from the two eyes10. However, in zebrafish larvae, retinotectal fibers project exclusively contralaterally11. It is thus unclear how a convergence of binocular visual information for localizing prey in the strike zone is accomplished.
The optic tectum (OT) is the main retinorecipient area in the midbrain of teleosts and is homologous to the superior colliculus (SC) in mammals. It is an important sensorimotor integrator that is essential for visually guided behaviors including predator avoidance12 and prey capture9,13. The OT consists of two main areas: a synaptic neuropil and a cellular stratum periventriculare (SPV). The OT neuropil receives input from contralateral retinal ganglion cells (RGCs)11 which innervate four retinorecipient layers, from superficial to deep: the stratum opticum (SO), the stratum fibrosum et griseum superficiale (SFGS), the stratum griseum centrale (SGC), and the stratum album centrale (SAC)14,15.
In this study, we report the surprising discovery of activity in the deep layers of the tectal neuropil in response to visual stimulation of the ipsilateral eye of zebrafish larvae. We identify a population of intertectal interneurons, ITNs, that responds to visual motion and innervates both left and right OT. ITNs are GABAergic and establish synapses in the deep layers of the neuropil. Furthermore, these neurons respond to small moving spots that resemble prey of zebrafish larvae such as Paramecia. ITN-ablation uncovers a specific requirement of these neurons for the initiation of capture swims when prey is positioned in the binocular strike zone directly in front of the larva. We propose that ITNs are a central component of a bilateral neural circuit that integrates binocular visual information to enable localization and capture of prey, in the absence of ipsilateral retinotectal projections (IRPs).
Results
Visually evoked activity in the ipsilateral tectum
First, we wanted to know if visual information from one eye is represented in the ipsilateral tectal hemisphere, despite the lack of ipsilaterally projecting retinotectal axons. To this end, we used 2-photon microscopy to image the OT of 5 days-post-fertilization (dpf) larvae that expressed the calcium indicator GCaMP5G under control of a pan-neuronal promoter. One eye was visually stimulated with moving bars (width: 9°, speed: 20° s−1, 12 angular directions spaced 30° apart) and the second eye was surgically removed prior to functional imaging to ensure monocular visual input (Fig. 1a). The absence of regenerative re-routing of retinal fibers from the remaining eye to the non-innervated tectal hemisphere after monocular enucleation was confirmed by anterograde DiO tracing (Supplementary Fig. 1). After correcting for motion artefacts, image timeseries from each larva were registered to the corresponding z-plane of a 5 dpf Tg(elavl3:GCaMP5G) reference larva. We computed average stimulus-triggered fluorescence responses for 6 anatomical subregions of the OT (SPV, deep and superficial neuropil in the ipsi- or contralateral tectal hemisphere, Fig. 1a). Consistent with fully crossed retinotectal projections in zebrafish, we observed strong calcium transients in all 3 regions of the tectum contralateral to the stimulated eye (Fig. 1b, upper three traces). Surprisingly, we also detected visually evoked responses in the tectal neuropil ipsilateral to the stimulated eye, i.e., contralateral to the enucleated eye (Fig. 1b, lowest two traces). This activity appeared confined to the neuropil as almost no response to moving bars was detected in ipsilateral periventricular neurons (PVNs) (Fig. 1b, ipsilateral SPV trace). To generate anatomical maps of stimulus-correlated activity, we used a regression-based approach16 and, consistent with our previous observations, found highly-correlated voxels in the tectal neuropil and SPV contralateral to the stimulated eye but also within the deep laminae of the ipsilateral tectal neuropil (comprising SGC and SAC) (Fig. 1c, d).
ITNs connect the two hemispheres of the optic tectum
To explain these observations, we hypothesized that a population of commissural neurons might exist that preferentially targets the deep laminae of the tectal neuropil. Moreover, this population would likely be visual motion responsive and directly or indirectly activated by retinal afferents.
Our lab isolated a Gal4-expressing transgenic zebrafish line, Gal4ic3034Tg (referred to as ITNGal4 hereafter), which labels a population of neurons interconnecting the two tectal hemispheres (Fig. 2a, b). Based on this anatomical feature we named the neuronal population intertectal neurons (ITNs). In addition to labeling ITNs, the Gal4ic3034Tg transgene is also expressed in the pineal gland, spinal cord, superficial interneurons in the OT (SINs), and sparsely in PVNs (Fig. 2a). The number of ITNs labeled in ITNGal4, UAS:GFP larvae varied across animals with a minimum of 22 labeled cells (12 on the right, 10 on the left) and a maximum of 48 (27 right, 21 left) (average from 18 randomly chosen larvae: 32 ITNs in total, 16 per side). This variability is likely due to variegation in transgene expression17.
The cell bodies of ITNs are situated in two bilaterally symmetric nuclei below the OT in the mesencephalic tegmentum (Fig. 2a, lower left panel), and send their neurites dorsally, crossing the tectal commissures in ladder-like trajectories (Fig. 2a, upper left panel). ITN neurites develop concomitantly with retinal innervation of the OT: They cross the midline and arborize in the OT starting from 3.5 dpf as revealed by single ITN neurite tracing (Fig. 2c). By 4 dpf, individual ITNs form increasingly complex arborization patterns in both the ipsi- and contralateral OT neuropil (Fig. 2d). Arbors of individual ITNs localize to the deep neuropil layers, between the SGC and the SAC (Supplementary Fig. 2a, b).
2-photon imaging of ITN somata revealed that cells contralateral to the stimulated eye showed visually evoked activity in response to moving bars (Supplementary Fig. 2c, d) indicating that they must receive direct or indirect visual input. ITNs thus match the anatomical and functional profile of the commissural neuronal population whose existence was predicted from ipsilateral activity in the deep neuropil of the OT.
ITNs are GABAergic interneurons and form synapses in the OT
Next, we analyzed the neurotransmitter phenotype and synaptic connections of ITNs. We performed fluorescent whole-mount in situ hybridization in combination with anti-GFP immunostaining in 4 dpf ITNGal4, UAS:GFP zebrafish larvae. Expression of vglut2a/2b did not show any overlap with anti-GFP immunostaining suggesting that ITNs are not glutamatergic (Supplementary Fig. 3a). We furthermore found that ITN nuclei were located in the mesencephalic tegmentum, anterior to the midbrain–hindbrain boundary, and showed no overlap with chata-positive cells of the nucleus isthmus, in rhombomere 1 (Supplementary Fig. 3b)2. However, expression of gad65/67 colocalized with anti-GFP labeling in the ITN nuclei, indicating that ITNs are GABAergic inhibitory interneurons (Fig. 3a).
To find out where ITNs form synapses, we genetically targeted the presynaptic marker synaptophysin, conjugated to GFP (Syp-GFP)18, to ITNs. We discovered that ITNs establish putative presynaptic contacts on their arbors in both tectal hemispheres. Presynaptic puncta were localized to the deep neuropil layers (Fig. 3b, upper right panel and Supplementary Fig. 2a, b), extending along the rostral-caudal axis of the OT (Fig. 3b, lower right panels). Furthermore, we transiently expressed the postsynaptic marker psd95-GFP19 in ITNs and found putative postsynaptic specializations in the deep layers of the neuropil, ipsilateral to ITN cell bodies (Fig. 3c).
In summary, ITNs are a class of inhibitory interneurons that interconnect the two tectal hemispheres.
ITNs respond to prey-like moving target stimuli
In order to explore if ITNs are involved in visual processing during hunting behavior, we first tested if ITNs respond to prey-like stimuli in a virtual hunting assay for tethered zebrafish larvae combined with 2-photon calcium imaging13. We presented 5–6 dpf ITNGal4, UAS:GCaMP3 larvae with small moving spots (speed: 30° s−1, size: 5°) running horizontally across the frontal visual field (Fig. 4a) while simultaneously imaging ITN activity bilaterally. ITNs were strongly responsive to prey-like stimuli (Fig. 4b). Plotting the angular position of the moving spot at the onset of the calcium transient revealed that ITNs responded when the stimulus was in the contralateral visual hemifield. This is consistent with ITNs receiving visual input deriving from the contralateral eye via RGC terminals in the ipsilateral OT, or ipsilateral tectal interneurons (Fig. 4c). Simple light flashes did not evoke ITN responses and we did not observe activity modulations associated with convergent saccades (i.e., initiation of hunting). This suggests that ITNs do not display premotor activity but rather visual sensory responses to prey-like moving target stimuli.
ITNs are required for the binocular localization of prey
Based on their anatomy and physiology, we hypothesized that ITNs might be required for the binocular localization of prey during hunting behavior. We examined this hypothesis using two loss of function approaches:
We generated ITNGal4, UAS: BoTxBLC-GFP larvae to genetically silence vesicle release in Gal4-expressing neurons using zebrafish-optimized Botulinum toxin light chain20, and assessed prey capture performance9. ITNGal4, UAS: BoTxBLC-GFP larvae showed a large reduction in prey consumption compared with sibling controls (Supplementary Fig. 4a). However, further analysis revealed that BoTxBLC-GFP-expressing larvae did not inflate their swim bladder and thus spent significantly less time swimming and covered less distance during prey capture trials (Supplementary Fig. 4b). Therefore, reduced prey consumption is likely secondary to locomotor deficits, perhaps due to BoTxBLC-GFP expression in the spinal cord.
Next we performed 2-photon laser ablation of ITN cell bodies to specifically examine the behavioral requirement of these neurons. We unilaterally ablated ITNs in 4 dpf ITNGal4, UAS:GCaMP3 larvae (12–26 ablated ITNs, Fig. 5a) and, after a recovery period, assessed hunting performance of individual animals. Control larvae, of identical genetic background, underwent the same mounting and imaging procedure but were not ablated. As a further control, sham-ablations were performed by randomly ablating PVNs directly above the ITN nucleus. ITN-ablated larvae were imaged the next day, which confirmed that ITNs were successfully removed (Fig. 5a). At 6 dpf, control, sham-ablated and ITN-ablated larvae were tested in a prey capture performance assay that quantified consumption of paramecia over the course of 4 h.
ITN-ablated larvae consumed fewer paramecia than unablated controls (Fig. 5b) and the strength of this hunting deficit correlated with the number of ablated ITNs (Supplementary Fig. 4c). Sham-ablated larvae did not show a significant difference in paramecia consumption compared with control larvae and were thus pooled with the control group.
ITN-ablated larvae did not show differences in time spent swimming (Fig. 5c), swim bout duration, maximum bout speed, interbout interval or swim bout turn angles (Supplementary Fig. 4d–g), indicating that general motor defects do not account for the impairment of predatory performance. Moreover, prey recognition and hunting-specific motor outputs remained unchanged following ITN ablation: total hunting duration (as estimated from periods of ocular convergence) was unaffected (Fig. 5c) as was average hunting sequence duration and eye vergence angles during hunting (Supplementary Fig. 4h, i). Thus, despite capturing fewer prey than control larvae, ITN-ablated larvae do not display motor defects and can recognize prey and initiate hunting.
We further hypothesized that ITN ablation might result in reduced prey consumption if ITNs are required for larvae to estimate prey location and accordingly initiate capture swims. To uncover at which point during the hunting sequence ITN-ablated larvae deviated from normal hunting behavior, we analyzed a total of 2149 individual hunting sequences from 29 control and 12 ITN-ablated larvae. We subdivided individual hunting routines into a sequence of events enabling us to quantify probabilities of specific outcomes at each stage, including target switching, target fixation, successful capture swims etc. (Fig. 5d). Through this analysis, we detected a specific deficit at the penultimate fixation stage of a hunting sequence, immediately prior to striking at prey (Supplementary Movies 1 and 2). Specifically, when the prey was localized in the binocular strike zone (prey at 0.5 mm distance, ±10° azimuth), ITN-ablated larvae showed a significantly reduced probability of initiating a capture maneuver (Fig. 5g). By contrast, other aspects of hunting behavior of ITN-ablated larvae, including the probability and distance of target fixation, did not appear to differ versus controls (Fig. 5e, f and Supplementary Fig. 5a–f).
In summary, ablating commissural ITNs led to a specific behavioral deficit during binocular localization of prey3, namely a failure to initiate capture strikes. This is consistent with reduced hunting performance in ITN-ablated larvae and supports the idea that ITNs mediate an interhemispheric neural computation that initiates capture swims at the final moment of hunting when prey is located in the binocular strike zone in front of the predator.
Discussion
Ipsilateral retinal projections (IRPs) exist in members of almost all vertebrate classes. Their presence in basal cyclostomes suggests that IRPs are an ancestral trait21 and seem to have been lost at multiple times during evolution such that they are absent or reduced in certain species (for a comprehensive review see ref.22). The existence of IRPs is sometimes attributed to a high degree of overlap of the field of views from both eyes and/or a predatory life style relying on binocular stereoscopic vision. However, this cannot be considered a general rule since there are species that have extensive IRPs but no overlapping field of views (e.g., the hagfish Eptatretus)21 as well as predators such as the chameleon, which lack IRPs entirely23.
Zebrafish larvae are predatory, also lack IRPs to the OT11 but show behavioral responses consistent with processing of binocular cues during prey hunting3,8,9. In this study, we have presented evidence for an intertectal neural circuit that could enable a vertebrate without IRPs to process binocular visual cues.
To our surprise, 2-photon calcium-imaging in zebrafish larvae revealed visual motion-evoked neuronal activity in the deep layers of the tectal neuropil ipsilateral to the stimulated eye, despite the lack of ipsilateral retinotectal projections. We identified a Gal4-expressing transgenic line which labels a previously unknown class of commissural neurons (ITNs) that connect both tectal hemispheres. ITNs respond to visual motion stimuli in the contralateral visual field and extend arbors in the deep neuropil laminae of the OT where ipsilateral visual activity is observed. It is therefore likely that ITNs transfer visual information across the midline, which in turn would enable tectal integration of visual information from the two eyes. Unilateral ablation of ITNs impaired prey hunting performance and produced a specific deficit in the initiation of capture swims. Although ITN-ablated larvae continued to fixate prey at equivalent distances compared to controls, they failed to initiate capture maneuvers when prey was localized to the binocular strike zone. The presence of intertectal connections in zebrafish larvae was previously suggested based on anatomical data from immunohistochemistry24,25 or single cell labeling approaches26,27. However, our study represents the first anatomical, physiological, and behavioral assessment of a genetically accessible class of commissural intertectal neurons.
At the penultimate fixation stage of hunting epochs, eye vergence angles are almost invariant3. Thus, lines of sight to the binocular strike zone will be represented at consistent locations in temporal retinae of the two eyes in accordance with anatomical evidence showing increased photoreceptor density in the temporal retina of larval zebrafish28,29. Coincident activation of these retinal trigger zones could be the neural signal indicating the correct localization of prey that in turn is used to initiate a capture swim3. This simple triangulation mechanism is reminiscent of that proposed for the striking behavior of preying mantids30,31.
In particluar, we suggest that ITNs participate in a circuit that detects the presence of prey in the binocular strike zone by signaling coincident, bilateral activation of specialized trigger zones in the left and right temporal retinae (Fig. 6). In our model, retinal activity is relayed to contralateral tectal neurons as well as contralateral ITNs, consistent with ITN activation by prey-like stimuli in the contralateral visual field (Fig. 4). ITNs in turn project across the midline (Fig. 2) to transfer visual information to the opposite hemisphere (Fig. 1). As ITNs are inhibitory (Fig. 3), we propose a disinhibition mechanism whereby they silence inhibitory interneurons in the contralateral tectum. This enables a circuit motif in which the tectum can detect simultaneous activation of both trigger zones: activation of the contralateral retina is communicated by direct retinal input, and activation of the ipsilateral temporal retina is signaled by ITN-mediated disinhibition of intrinsic interneurons. The combination of excitatory drive and removal of inhibitory block together allow the generation of a pre-motor command that triggers execution of a capture swim.
Binocular neural responses have been previously reported in zebrafish larvae in the context of behaviors driven by whole field motion: the optokinetic reflex (OKR) and the optomotor response (OMR)32–34. Here convergence of monocular visual information is thought to be mediated via the posterior commissure33 and because these behaviors do not depend on retinotectal connections15 it is likely that binocular integration of whole field motion cues is independent of ITNs.
Intertectal connections have been studied in multiple species35,36. In anurans, neurons of the nucleus isthmi indirectly connect the tectal hemispheres and mediate binocular visual responses in the anterior part of the OT37,38. However, bilateral ablation of these nuclei does not impair binocular depth perception during hunting37,39. Direct commissural connections between the superior colliculi, the mammalian homolog of the OT, were also reported in mammals but their function remains rather obscure: Severing the collicular commissure restored visual fixation capabilities of hemianopic cats to the blind hemifield that was originally caused by unilateral ablations of the visual neocortex (Sprague effect)40. While the interpretation of this experiment is not straight forward, it suggests a mostly underappreciated role of intercollicular neurons in visually guided behavior, even in mammals.
Finally, it has been convincingly shown that binocular stereopsis is present in very different evolutionary lineages41–43. However, common and distinct principles of binocular stereopsis have not been addressed yet on the neural circuit level. Our study contributes to defining these principles by providing evidence for a neural circuit that establishes a simple form of binocular stereopsis in the absence of direct superposition of retinal input from both eyes.
Methods
Contact for reagent and resource sharing
Further information and requests for resources and reagents should be directed to and will be fulfilled by Filippo Del Bene (filippo.del-bene@inserm.fr).
Zebrafish embryo maintenance
Zebrafish (Danio rerio) were maintained at 28 °C on a 14 h light/10 h dark cycle. Fish were housed in the animal facility of our laboratories at UCL or Institut Curie, which were built according to the respective local animal welfare standards. All animal procedures were performed in accordance with French, British, and European Union animal welfare guidelines. Animal handling and experimental procedures were approved by the Committee on ethics of animal experimentation—Institut Curie and the UK Home Office under the Animal (Scientific Procedures) Act 1986. No statistical methods were used to predetermine sample size. Where indicated the experiments were randomized and the investigators blinded to allocation during experiments and outcome assessment.
ITN neurite and synapse labeling
Transient single cell labeling to analyze ITN neurite morphology was achieved by injecting 1 nl of a UAS:tagRFP, UAS:GFP plasmid DNA (25 ng μl−1) at 1 cell stage into ITNGal4, UAS:GCaMP3 embryos. To transiently label post-synaptic densities of ITNs, 1 nl of a UAS:psd95-GFP plasmid DNA (25 ng μl−1) were injected at 1 cell stage into ITNGal4, UAS:GCaMP3 embryos. Presynaptic sites were labeled by crossing ITNGal4, UAS:GCaMP3 with Tg(Brn3c:Gal4, UAS:Syp-GFP) larvae and the offspring screened against Brn3c and for Gal4ic3034Tg—and Syp-GFP expression. Larvae were imaged at 3–5 dpf using a Zeiss LSM 780 confocal microscope (Zeiss, Oberkochen, Germany), equipped with a water immersion objective (Zeiss, Plan-Apochromat 40 ×/1.00). Image stacks were analyzed using MATLAB (The MathWorks Inc., Natick, MA, USA) and FIJI44. Skin autofluorescence was removed manually in FIJI to enable an unobstructed view onto ITN synaptic structures and neurites.
Neurite tracing
ITN neurites were traced using the simple neurite tracer plugin45 provided in FIJI. Individual ITNs labeled in ITNGal4, UAS:GCaMP3 larvae injected with UAS:tagRFP, UAS:GFP were followed through the confocal stack based on their color and/or marker intensity. The vector files containing the traced neurites were then exported to MATLAB for further processing using the MatlabIO toolbox included in visualization and analysis software Vaa3D46.
Whole-mount in situ hybridization and immunohistochemistry
Vglut2a/vglut2b, gad67/gad65 probe mixtures were generated from cDNA using the following primers:
vglut2a_fwd: GATTCTCCTCACGTCCACACTGAA
vglut2a_rev: AACACATACTGCCACTCTTCTCGG
vglut2b_fwd: CGTCGACATGGTCAATAACAGCAC
vglut2b_rev: ATAGCACCTACAATCAGAGGGCAG
gad67_fwd: CATCATCCTCACCAGCTGCTGGAG
gad67_rev: AACATTGTAAAGGCACACCCATCATC
gad65_fwd: TCACCTATGAGGTGGCTCCAGTCTTC
gad65_rev: GTCATAATGCTTGTCCTGCTGGAAC
The mRNA anti-sense riboprobes for chata were kindly provided by Marnie Halpern47. For in situ hybridization, larvae were stored in 100% methanol and rehydrated stepwise into 0.1% Tween in PBS. Tissue permeabilization was achieved by proteinase K treatment adapted to larval age at room temperature, followed by post-fixation in 4% PFA in PBS. After prehybridization, hybridization with digoxigenin-UTP-labeled and fluorescein-UTP-labeled riboprobes (Roche Applied Science) was performed overnight at 65/68 °C and larvae were kept in the dark for all subsequent steps. Larvae were washed with TNT (0.1 M Tris, pH 7.5, 0.15 M NaCl, 0.1% Tween 20), incubated in 1% H2O2 in TNT for 20 min, washed several times and blocked in TNB [2% DIG Block (Roche) in TNT] for 1 h. Incubation with anti-digoxigenin-POD (peroxidase) Fab fragments (Roche, 1:50 in TNB) was performed for 24 h at 4 °C. Signals were detected using the Cy3-TSA kit (PerkinElmer) for 60 min (vglut2a/2b mixture) or 30 min (gad65/67 mixture), respectively. Larvae were incubated in DAPI in TNT overnight at 4 °C and washed with TNT prior to imaging. For subsequent immuno-staining, chicken anti-GFP (1:500, Genetex, GTX13970) and mouse anti-ERK (1:500, p44/42 MAPK (Erk1/2), 4696) were used as primary antibodies followed by goat anti-chicken Alexa Fluor 488-conjugated (1:400, Invitrogen, A11039) and anti-mouse Alexa Fluor 594-conjugated (1:500, Invitrogen, A11005) secondary antibodies.
3D Brain registration
Registration of image volumes of 18 ITNGal4, UAS:GCaMP3 at 6 dpf, co-immunolabeled with anti-tErk and anti-GFP, to a tErk reference brain48 was performed using the CMTK toolbox version 3.2.249.
Eye ablations
3 to 4 dpf Tg(elavl3:GCaMP5G) larvae were mounted in 2% low-melting-point agarose and one eye was removed using fine forceps. The larvae were then left to recover in Ringer solution supplemented with Calcium for 1 h. Subsequently, the larvae were transferred into fish medium until 2-photon Calcium imaging was performed on 5 or 6 dpf.
Lipophilic dye tracing
For tracing of retinotectal projections, eye-ablated larvae where fixed in 4% PFA/PBS for 2 h following injection of the lipophilic dye DiO into the remaining eye.
Calcium imaging and visual stimulation
Larvae were mounted in 2.5% low melting-point agarose such that one eye was facing an OLED screen (800 pixel × 600 pixel, eMagin, Bellevue, WA, USA) subtending around 70° × 55° of a larva’s visual field. The screen was covered with a red long-pass filter (Kodak Wratten No. 25) to enable simultaneous imaging and visual stimulation. Larvae were then imaged in vivo using a two-photon microscope (LaVision Biotec, Bielefeld, Germany), equipped with a mode-locked Chameleon Ultra II Ti–Sapphire laser tuned to 920 nm (Coherent Inc., Santa Clara, CA, USA) and a Zeiss Plan-APOCHROMAT 20× water immersion objective (NA 1, Zeiss, Oberkochen, Germany). Emitted fluorescence was detected using a bandpass filter (ET525/50 M, Chroma Technology GmbH, Olching Germany) in front of a GaAsP photo-multiplier tube (H7422-40, Hamamatsu Photonics K.K., Hamamatsu City, Japan). The average laser power at the sample during scanning was 10–20 mW and frames were scanned at 4 Hz resulting in 1.33 μs dwell time per pixel (pixel size: 0.85 μm2). Scanning and image acquisition were controlled with LaVision Biotec’s proprietary ImSpector software.
Visual stimulation was handled by a computer separate from the image acquisition setup using Psychophysics Toolbox50. Stimuli were either moving bars running across the eye’s field of view (bar width: 9°, speed: 20° s−1, direction: pseudo-randomly chosen for each stimulus epoch from 12 angular directions 30° apart, stimulus repetitions: three times per direction, stimulus presentation interval: 10 s) or light flashes instantaneously covering the entire field of view (stimulus repetitions: five times, stimulus presentation interval: 10 s). The image acquisition was synchronized with the visual stimulation by recording the imaging frame number in parallel to stimulus presentation timing via a U3 LabJack DAQ (LabJack, Lakewood, CO, U.S.A.).
Virtual prey-hunting assay
5 to 6 dpf ITNGal4, UAS:GCaMP3 larvae were individually mounted in 2% low-melting-point agarose in a 35 mm petri dish and the eyes and tail subsequently freed from the agarose with a scalpel. Visual stimuli consisted of small moving spots (subtending 5° visual angle) back-projected onto a curved screen in front of the animal, appeared at 76° to the left or right of the midline and then moving 152° right or left across the frontal region of visual field (at an average angular velocity with respect to the fish of 30°/s). At each imaging plane, 12–18 repetitions of each of the visual stimuli (L > R, R > L) were presented in pseudo-random order.
2-photon calcium imaging was simultaneously performed using a custom-built microscope equipped with a 20 × /1.00 NA Olympus objective and a Ti:Sapphire ultra-fast laser (Chameleon Ultra II, Coherent Inc) tuned to 920 nm. Average laser power at the sample was 5–10 mW. Images were acquired by frame scanning at 3.6 Hz with 1 µs dwell time per pixel. For each larva, 1–3 focal planes through the ITN nuclei of the larva were imaged. Image acquisition and visual stimulus presentation were controlled using software written in LabView and MATLAB.
Image analysis for calcium imaging experiments
A voxel-based analysis strategy was chosen for the Calcium-imaging experiments following eye ablations. Acquired timeseries were motion-corrected by calculating the shift between an average image and each timeseries image based on the inverse Fourier-transformed normalized cross-power spectra. Each image timeseries was further registered to the corresponding z-plane of a 5 dpf Tg(elavl3:GCaMP5G) reference larva using MATLAB’s align_nonrigid function. Individual image timeseries were re-shuffled due to the randomized stimulus order during visual stimulation and averaged across repetitions. To identify voxels that were correlated with stimulus presentation we used a regression-based approach16. In short, a binary matrix was generated indicating when a stimulus was present independent of its directionality. For every voxel, the average temporal response profile was estimated using least-squares regression. Voxels with a p value > 0.0001 were discarded from analysis and the maximum value of the regression coefficient from eight larvae was kept for each voxel. Voxels that had a regression coefficient > 0.4 were considered strongly correlated with stimulus presentation and thus active. The intensity values for all voxels in one of six anatomically defined ROIs in the ipsilateral and contralateral tectal hemisphere were averaged over time for each larva. The deltaF/F0 average profiles were calculated by estimating the baseline using MATLAB’s imerode function with a 10 s kernel (i.e., the stimulus presentation interval). DeltaF/F0 was obtained by calculating F<ROI>/F<ROI baseline> −1. DeltaF/F0 average profiles were then averaged across larvae.
The response of ITNs to moving bars and light flashes covering the contralateral field of view was analyzed using a ROI based approach. First, acquired timeseries were registered by calculating the shift between an average image and each timeseries image based on the inverse Fourier-transformed normalized cross-power spectra. ROIs were manually selected from a standard deviation projection of the image timeseries and based on anatomical landmarks. ROI timeseries were re-shuffled to account for the randomized stimulus order during visual stimulation and averaged across repetitions. The deltaF/F0 average profiles were calculated by estimating the baseline using MATLAB’s imerode function with a 10 s kernel and calculating F<ROI>/F<ROI baseline> −1. Finally, the deltaF/F0 averages were normalized to the maximal value.
Analysis of data collected during virtual hunting assays was performed by correcting motion artefacts of timeseries using cross-correlation13. ROIs were selected manually based on an anatomical stack of the brain volume. ROI trials were discarded from analysis if the z-scored motion-error exceeded two standard deviations. The ROI intensity profiles were z-scored and then averaged over trials. Lateral ITN position (i.e., left or right) was recorded from an anatomical stack of the larva and stimulus direction (right to left and vice versa) was remapped to contralateral to ipsilateral and vice versa with respect to the ITN position in the larva. For calculating the onset of the ITN activity with respect to the stimulus position in the visual field, the maximum of the ROI timeseries within the stimulus interval (contra > ipsi and ipsi > contra) was identified. Subsequently, the response onset was defined as timepoint when the response curve was smaller than the maximum and falling below 0.5 standard deviations. Timepoints for contra > ipsi and ipsi > contra were averaged and the corresponding azimuth was calculated based upon the trajectory of the visual cue.
Synaptic silencing
ITNGal4, UAS:RFP larvae were crossed to UAS:BoTxBLC-GFP larvae20, screened at 3 dpf and separated into GFP-positive (ITNGal4, UAS:BoTxBLC-GFP) and GFP-negative (control) siblings. On 5 dpf, all larvae were fed with rotifers to gain feeding experience.
ITN laser ablations
At 4 dpf, ITNGal4, UAS:GCaMP3 larvae, in which more than 12 ITNs were labeled in one nucleus, were randomly assigned to either the control, sham-ablated or ITN-ablated group. Larvae from the two ablation groups were then randomly selected for unilateral cell ablation (i.e., only cells on either the left or right side with respect to the larval rostral–caudal axis to be ablated per larva). Then all larvae independent of the assigned group were anesthetized using 0.02% tricaine (MS-222, Sigma) diluted in fish medium, mounted in 3% low-melting-point agarose and imaged using a custom-made 2-photon microscope13. Single target cells (either ITNs or PVNs) were identified by first taking a full frame scan. Then a mode-locked laser beam (800 nm) was scanned in a spiral pattern around a defined target cell position for ~100 ms. A cell was regarded as successfully ablated when after ablation a small point of saturated intensity was detected at the target position instead of the cell. Then the procedure was repeated and finally the number of successfully ablated cells recorded for each larva. After the procedure, each larva’s brain was inspected under the microscope and only larvae with clearly visible heartbeat and blood flow in both brain hemispheres were kept for further experiments. Control, sham-ablated and ITN-ablated larvae were then removed from the agarose and kept individually. On 5 dpf ITN-ablated larvae were re-imaged to ensure that neurons were successfully removed and were otherwise left to recover. All larvae were subsequently separated into 12-well plates and assigned a code for anonymization. Each larva was fed with Paramecia on 5 dpf to gain feeding experience.
Prey consumption assays
At 6 dpf, all larvae were tested in a prey consumption assay9 and, since the larvae received food more than 12 h earlier, they were at this point considered starved51,52. At the beginning of the assay (t = 0 h) paramecia or rotifers were added to petri dishes containing either a single larva (for ITN-ablations) or 4 larvae (for the BoTxBL-GFP-expression experiments). Short movies of the dishes were then recorded at t = 0 h, t = 1 h, t = 2 h and t = 4 h after the start of the experiment using high-speed infrared sensitive cameras (MC1362 or MC4082, Mikrotron GmbH, Germany) positioned above the dish. Dishes were dark field-illuminated using a custom-made LED-ring (850 nm) placed around the dishes. To obtain temporal prey consumption curves, the number of living prey per dish was counted at each time point during the assay using FIJI’s Cell Counter plugin. These numbers were then normalized to the initial prey number at t = 0 h.
Larva tracking and data analysis
For the behavioral analysis of UAS:BoTxBLC-GFP-expressing larvae, individual larvae were imaged with a Mikrotron MC1362 high-speed camera at 700 Hz. Each recording lasted between 5 to 10 min. The larva’s position in each frame was extracted online using a custom-made tracking software1.
For the analysis of ITN-ablated larvae, individual animals were filmed for around 15 min at 100 frames s−1 and illuminated by a custom-made LED-based diffusive backlight (850 nm) placed below the dish. For each frame, the x/y - position and orientation of the larva’s body centroid and the angles of the larva’s eyes were extracted in real-time based upon image moments computed by OpenCV routines within the open-source visual programming language Bonsai53.
Data Analysis was performed off-line in MATLAB. Hunting sequences were detected by calculating the vergence angles from the difference between the left and right eye angles filtered by a 50 ms boxcar filter. Vergence distributions are mostly bimodal (i.e., representing eyes parallel and convergences) thus thresholds for start (on) and end (off) of hunting sequences were determined by fitting the vergence distributions for each larva with a Two-Term Gaussian Model (using MATLABs fit function). The on-threshold for hunting sequences for each larva was then determined by subtracting 1 standard deviation from the mean of the gaussian distribution with the higher mean (representing convergences). The off-threshold for hunting sequences was the intersection point of two gaussians minus 5°. The start of a hunting sequence was then defined by the point when the vergence angle exceeded the on-threshold and the end was indicated by the vergence angle falling below the off-threshold. From these hunting sequence start and end timepoints, the fraction of time spent hunting could be calculated by taking the number of video frames during hunting sequences normalized by the total number of frames during the experiment. Furthermore, hunting sequence duration and average vergence angle during hunting sequences were determined.
Swim bouts were detected by calculating a larva’s instant velocity in mm s−1 from the x- and y-centroid position difference filtered with a 30 ms (for determining the start of a swim bout) or a 100 ms (for determining the swim bout end) boxcar filter. Putative swim bouts were then detected using MATLABS findpeaks function. Then start and end timepoints of swim bouts were determined based on the on-threshold of 2 mm s−1 in the 30 ms-boxcar-filtered velocity curve and an off-threshold of 1.25 mm s−1 in the 100 ms-boxcar-filtered velocity curve. From the swim bout start and end timepoints, the fraction of time spent swimming could be calculated by taking the number of video frames during swim bouts normalized by the total number of frames during the experiment. Furthermore, swim bout duration, maximum bout speed and interbout intervals were calculated for bouts and interbout intervals occurring during hunting sequences for each larva. To obtain turning angles for each bout during hunting sequences the difference between average orientation changes 30 ms before and 30 ms after a swim bout was calculated. For ITN-ablated larvae turning angles were re-mapped according to the side of the ablation to control for potential unpsecific ablation defects induced through unilateral ablation.
For all analyzed hunting sequence and swim bout parameters, the relative frequency distribution and the cumulative relative frequency distribution per larva were calculated and then averaged for all larvae within the control and ITN-ablated group. To test for equality of the average distributions a Two-Sample Kolmogoroff–Smirnov test was performed.
Single hunting sequence evaluation of ITN-ablated larvae and data analysis
To evaluate the outcome of single hunting sequences, 15 min videos of hunting larvae were taken at 100 frames s−1. Dishes were dark field-illuminated using a custom-made LED-ring (850 nm) placed around the dish to simplify the detection of paramecia. Hunting sequences were identified based on visible eye vergences and assigned to either of the categories shown in Fig. 5d based on their outcome: 1. sequence aborted when prey outside of binocular strike zone (prey at distance smaller than 0.5 mm and smaller than ±10° azimuth between prey and larva), 2. targets were switched or 3. target was fixated i.e., the larva was positioning the target in a stereotypic binocular strike zone around 0.5 mm in front of the larva. If the target was fixated during the hunting sequence it was furthermore evaluated if a capture swim, a suction or sequence abortion when the target was inside the binocular strike zone occurred. Finally, if a capture swim or a suction was performed the success or failure was recorded. Only sequences with an unequivocally identifiable fixated target were included for analysis. In rare cases in which multiple paramecia were fixated by a larva during a single hunting sequence (43/2149 hunting sequences from 29 control and 12 ITN-ablated larvae), the entire hunting sequence was assigned to the target switch category. Data analysis was performed off-line in MATLAB. Category probabilities for a given larva were calculated by counting the total number of events (e.g., successful capture swims) normalized to the number of events of the superordinate node (e.g., capture swims).
Quantification and statistical analysis
All statistical analyses were performed in either MATLAB or FIJI. Statistical tests, p values and sample size are reported in the text or figure legends. All tests were two-tailed. No statistical methods were used to predetermine sample size. Where indicated the experiments were randomized and the investigators blinded to allocation during experiments and outcome assessment.
Reporting summary
Further information on research design is available in the Nature Research Reporting Summary linked to this article.
Supplementary information
Acknowledgements
We would like to thank Kristen Severi and Roshan Jain for comments on the manuscript and all members of the Del Bene and Bianco labs for discussions. The Tg(UAS:BoTxBLC-GFP) zebrafish line was a gift from Claire Wyart. We thank the Developmental Biology Curie imaging facility (PICT-IBiSA@BDD, Paris, France, UMR3215/U934), member of the France-BioImaging national research infrastructure, for their help and advice with everything microscopy related. The Del Bene laboratory Neural Circuits Development is part of the Laboratoire d’Excellence (LABEX) entitled DEEP (ANR -11-LABX-0044), and of the École des Neurosciences de Paris Ile-de-France network. This work has been in part funded by an ATIP/AVENIR program starting grant (F.D.B.), ERC-StG #311159-Zebratectum (F.D.B.), CNRS, INSERM and Institut Curie (F.D.B) core funding. I.H.B. received a UCL Excellence Fellowship and a Sir Henry Dale Fellowship (101195/Z/13/Z) from the Wellcome Trust and Royal Society. C.G. was supported by an EMBO Short-term Fellowship, an Institut Curie Postdoctoral Fellowship and an FRM Postdoctoral Fellowship. T.O.A. was supported by a Boehringer Ingelheim Fonds PhD Fellowship. P.H. was supported by a UCL IMPACT studentship. G.R. received funding from the European Union’s Horizon 2020 Research and Innovation program under the Marie Skłodowska-Curie grant agreement no. 666003.
Source data
Author contributions
C.G., I.H.B., and F.D.B. conceived and designed experiments. C.G. performed all experimental procedures unless otherwise indicated. T.O.A. and C.G. identified the ITNGal4 line. P.H. performed the virtual hunting assay experiments. T.O.A. performed in situ hybridizations, morphological ITN characterization, and the synaptic labeling experiments. K.D. helped with immunostaining experiments and zebrafish line maintenance. G.R. performed the behavioral analysis of ITNGal4 fish expressing BoTxBLC-GFP. C.G. analyzed the data. C.G., I.H.B. and F.D.B. wrote the manuscript with input from all coauthors.
Data availability
The datasets generated and analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding authors on request. The source data underlying Figs. 1b, c, 4b, c, 5b–g and Supplementary Figs. 2d, 4a–i and 5a–f are provided as a Source Data file.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Peer review information Nature Communications thanks Robert Hindges and the other, anonymous, reviewer(s) for their contribution to the peer review of this work.
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
These authors jointly supervised: Isaac H. Bianco, Filippo Del Bene.
Contributor Information
Isaac H. Bianco, Email: i.bianco@ucl.ac.uk
Filippo Del Bene, Email: filippo.del-bene@inserm.fr.
Supplementary information
Supplementary information is available for this paper at 10.1038/s41467-019-13484-9.
References
- 1.Marques, J. C., Lackner, S., Félix, R. & Orger, M. B. Structure of the zebrafish locomotor repertoire revealed with unsupervised behavioral clustering. Curr. Biol. 28, 181–195.e5 (2018). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 2.Henriques PM, Rahman N, Jackson SE, Bianco IH. Nucleus isthmi is required to sustain target pursuit during visually guided prey-catching. Curr. Biol. 2019;29:1771–1786.e5. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2019.04.064. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Bianco IH, Kampff AR, Engert F. Prey capture behavior evoked by simple visual stimuli in larval zebrafish. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 2011;5:101. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2011.00101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Borla MA, Palecek B, Budick S, O’Malley DM. Prey capture by larval zebrafish: evidence for fine axial motor control. Brain. Behav. Evol. 2002;60:207–229. doi: 10.1159/000066699. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.McElligott MB, O’Malley DM. Prey tracking by larval zebrafish: axial kinematics and visual control. Brain. Behav. Evol. 2005;66:177–196. doi: 10.1159/000087158. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Trivedi, C. A. & Bollmann, J. H. Visually driven chaining of elementary swim patterns into a goal-directed motor sequence: a virtual reality study of zebrafish prey capture. Front. Neural Circuits7, 86 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 7.Budick SA, O’Malley DM. Locomotor repertoire of the larval zebrafish: swimming, turning and prey capture. J. Exp. Biol. 2000;203:2565–2579. doi: 10.1242/jeb.203.17.2565. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Patterson BW, Abraham AO, MacIver MA, McLean DL. Visually guided gradation of prey capture movements in larval zebrafish. J. Exp. Biol. 2013;216:3071–3083. doi: 10.1242/jeb.087742. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Gahtan E, Tanger P, Baier H. Visual prey capture in larval zebrafish is controlled by identified reticulospinal neurons downstream of the tectum. J. Neurosci. 2005;25:9294–9303. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2678-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Petros TJ, Rebsam A, Mason CA. Retinal axon growth at the optic chiasm: to cross or not to cross. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2008;31:295–315. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.31.060407.125609. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Burrill JD, Easter SS. Development of the retinofugal projections in the embryonic and larval zebrafish (Brachydanio rerio) J. Comp. Neurol. 1994;346:583–600. doi: 10.1002/cne.903460410. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Dunn TW, et al. Neural circuits underlying visually evoked escapes in larval zebrafish. Neuron. 2016;89:613–628. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.12.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Bianco IH, Engert F. Visuomotor transformations underlying hunting behavior in zebrafish. Curr. Biol. 2015;25:831–846. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2015.01.042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Meek J. Functional anatomy of the tectum mesencephali of the goldfish. An explorative analysis of the functional implications of the laminar structural organization of the tectum. Brain Res. 1983;287:247–297. doi: 10.1016/0165-0173(83)90008-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Roeser T, Baier H. Visuomotor behaviors in larval zebrafish after GFP-guided laser ablation of the optic tectum. J. Neurosci. 2003;23:3726–3734. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-09-03726.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Freeman J, et al. Mapping brain activity at scale with cluster computing. Nat. Methods. 2014;11:941–950. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.3041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Roberts JA, et al. Targeted transgene integration overcomes variability of position effects in zebrafish. Development. 2014;141:715–724. doi: 10.1242/dev.100347. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Meyer MP, Smith SJ. Evidence from in vivo imaging that synaptogenesis guides the growth and branching of axonal arbors by two distinct mechanisms. J. Neurosci. 2006;26:3604–3614. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0223-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Niell CM, Meyer MP, Smith SJ. In vivo imaging of synapse formation on a growing dendritic arbor. Nat. Neurosci. 2004;7:254–260. doi: 10.1038/nn1191. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Sternberg JR, et al. Optimization of a neurotoxin to investigate the contribution of excitatory interneurons to speed modulation in vivo. Curr. Biol. 2016;26:2319–2328. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2016.06.037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Wicht H, Northcutt RG. Retinofugal and retinopetal projections in the pacific hagfish, Eptatretus stouti (Myxinoidea) (Part 1 of 2) Brain. Behav. Evol. 1990;36:315–321. doi: 10.1159/000115317. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Ward R, Repérant J, Hergueta S, Miceli D, Lemire M. Ipsilateral visual projections in non-eutherian species: random variation in the central nervous system? Brain Res. Rev. 1995;20:155–170. doi: 10.1016/0165-0173(94)00009-E. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Bennis M, Repérant J, Rio J-P, Ward R. An experimental re-evaluation of the primary visual system of the European chameleon, Chamaeleo chameleon. Brain. Behav. Evol. 1994;43:173–188. doi: 10.1159/000113633. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Ronneberger O, et al. ViBE-Z: a framework for 3D virtual colocalization analysis in zebrafish larval brains. Nat. Methods. 2012;9:735–742. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.2076. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Wilson SW, Ross LS, Parrett T, Easter SS. The development of a simple scaffold of axon tracts in the brain of the embryonic zebrafish, Brachydanio rerio. Development. 1990;108:121–145. doi: 10.1242/dev.108.1.121. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Helmbrecht, T. O., dal Maschio, M., Donovan, J. C., Koutsouli, S. & Baier, H. Topography of a visuomotor transformation. Neuron100, 1429–1445.e4 (2018). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 27.Sato T, Hamaoka T, Aizawa H, Hosoya T, Okamoto H. Genetic single-cell mosaic analysis implicates ephrinB2 reverse signaling in projections from the posterior tectum to the hindbrain in zebrafish. J. Neurosci. 2007;27:5271–5279. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0883-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Schmitt EA, Dowling JE. Early retinal development in the zebrafish, Danio rerio: light and electron microscopic analyses. J. Comp. Neurol. 1999;404:515–536. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1096-9861(19990222)404:4<515::AID-CNE8>3.0.CO;2-A. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Zimmermann MJY, et al. Zebrafish differentially process color across visual space to match natural scenes. Curr. Biol. 2018;28:2018–2032.e5. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2018.04.075. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Nityananda V, Read JCA. Stereopsis in animals: evolution, function and mechanisms. J. Exp. Biol. 2017;220:2502–2512. doi: 10.1242/jeb.143883. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Prete, F. R., Wells, H., Wells, P. H. & Hurd, L. E. The Praying Mantids. (JHU Press, 1999).
- 32.Kubo F, et al. Functional architecture of an optic flow-responsive area that drives horizontal eye movements in zebrafish. Neuron. 2014;81:1344–1359. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.02.043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Naumann EA, et al. From whole-brain data to functional circuit models: the zebrafish optomotor response. Cell. 2016;167:947–960.e20. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2016.10.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Portugues R, Feierstein CE, Engert F, Orger MB. Whole-brain activity maps reveal stereotyped, distributed networks for visuomotor behavior. Neuron. 2014;81:1328–1343. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.01.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Auen EL. Axonal transport of HRP in descending tectal fibers of the pit viper. Neurosci. Lett. 1978;9:137–140. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(78)90061-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Voneida TJ, Mello NK. Interhemispheric projections of the optic tectum in pigeon. Brain. Behav. Evol. 1975;11:91–108. doi: 10.1159/000123627. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Glasser S, Ingle D. The nucleus isthmus as a relay station in the ipsilateral visual projection to the frog’s optic tectum. Brain Res. 1978;159:214–218. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(78)90122-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Gruberg E, et al. Influencing and interpreting visual input: the role of a visual feedback system. J. Neurosci. 2006;26:10368–10371. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3288-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Collett TS, Udin SB, Finch DJ. A possible mechanism for binocular depth judgements in anurans. Exp. Brain Res. 1987;66:35–40. doi: 10.1007/BF00236199. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Sprague JM. Interaction of cortex and superior colliculus in mediation of visually guided behavior in the cat. Science. 1966;153:1544–1547. doi: 10.1126/science.153.3743.1544. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Collett T. Stereopsis in toads. Nature. 1977;267:349–351. doi: 10.1038/267349a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Ptito M, Lepore F, Guillemot J-P. Stereopsis in the cat: behavioral demonstration and underlying mechanisms. Neuropsychologia. 1991;29:443–464. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(91)90004-R. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Rossel S. Binocular stereopsis in an insect. Nature. 1983;302:821–822. doi: 10.1038/302821a0. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Schindelin J, et al. Fiji: an open-source platform for biological-image analysis. Nat. Methods. 2012;9:676–682. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.2019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Longair MH, Baker DA, Armstrong JD. Simple Neurite Tracer: open source software for reconstruction, visualization and analysis of neuronal processes. Bioinformatics. 2011;27:2453–2454. doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/btr390. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Peng H, Ruan Z, Long F, Simpson JH, Myers EW. V3D enables real-time 3D visualization and quantitative analysis of large-scale biological image data sets. Nat. Biotechnol. 2010;28:348–353. doi: 10.1038/nbt.1612. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Hong E, et al. Cholinergic left-right asymmetry in the habenulo-interpeduncular pathway. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2013;110:21171–21176. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1319566110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Randlett O, et al. Whole-brain activity mapping onto a zebrafish brain atlas. Nat. Methods. 2015;12:1039–1046. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.3581. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Rohlfing T, Maurer CR., Jr. Nonrigid image registration in shared-memory multiprocessor environments with application to brains, breasts, and bees. IEEE Trans. Inform. Technol. Biomed. 2003;7:16–25. doi: 10.1109/TITB.2003.808506. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Brainard DH. The psychophysics toolbox. Spat. Vis. 1997;10:433–436. doi: 10.1163/156856897X00357. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Filosa A, Barker AJ, Dal Maschio M, Baier H. Feeding state modulates behavioral choice and processing of prey stimuli in the zebrafish tectum. Neuron. 2016;90:596–608. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.03.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Jordi J, et al. A high-throughput assay for quantifying appetite and digestive dynamics. Am. J. Physiol. -Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 2015;309:R345–R357. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.00225.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Lopes, G. et al. Bonsai: an event-based framework for processing and controlling data streams. Front. Neuroinform.9, 7 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 54.Preuss SJ, Trivedi CA, vom Berg-Maurer CM, Ryu S, Bollmann JH. Classification of object size in retinotectal microcircuits. Curr. Biol. 2014;24:2376–2385. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2014.09.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The datasets generated and analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding authors on request. The source data underlying Figs. 1b, c, 4b, c, 5b–g and Supplementary Figs. 2d, 4a–i and 5a–f are provided as a Source Data file.