Abstract
Psychological factors have been implicated in STEM persistence but remain poorly understood. In particular, the role of self-regulation--the cognitive, behavioral, and emotional skills that allow individuals to work efficiently toward their desired goals, especially when under stress--has received minimal attention. Psychological factors may be particularly important for persistence by underrepresented minority (URM) students, many of whom face significant barriers to success in STEM. We examined the extent to which self-regulation predicts STEM persistence in 732 STEM students and whether minority status moderated self-regulation’s associations with STEM persistence. We found minimal differences in self-regulation styles between URM and nonunderrepresented minority students. Baseline cognitive-emotional self-regulation predicted intentions to persist in a science career, using alcohol and drugs to cope with stress predicted less persistence in STEM major across the year, and only URM status predicted end-of-year GPA. Minority status did not moderate these associations. Future research is needed on self-regulation skills and students’ trajectories of STEM success.
Keywords: Self-regulation, STEM, Persistence, Student success, Minorities
1. Introduction
Attrition throughout the Science-Technology-Engineering-Mathematics (STEM) pipeline remains a national concern (PCAST, 2012), driving considerable research to identify factors that predict and/or facilitate student success and persistence in STEM fields. Despite early intentions to pursue a STEM career, the percentage of college students who start off in a STEM field and go on to earn a STEM degree within five years ranges from 33% to 46% (Hurtado, Eagan, and Chang, 2010; Huang et al., 2000). Some loss in STEM degree attainment is understandable as there is likely some disconnect between the appeal of a STEM career (e.g., job opportunities, career status) and the challenges associated with many STEM majors (e.g., academic preparation, additional requirements) as well as the potentially negative experiences associated with STEM curricula (e.g., weed out/gateway courses). However, even high-achieving STEM students sustain significant losses in STEM degree attainment (Chen and Soldner, 2014).
1.1. STEM Persistence in Underrepresented Minority Groups
Understanding the lack of student persistence in STEM is important for all students, but particularly so for underrepresented minority (URM) groups, who comprise a growing percentage of the U.S. population but remain woefully underrepresented among STEM professionals (Estrada et al., 2016). These discrepancies in STEM degree attainment between URM and nonunderrepresented minority (NURM) groups are not due to a lack of interest in STEM. In fact, interest and intent to pursue a STEM career are comparable for college bound URMs and NURMs, yet NURMs are twice as likely to graduate with a bachelor’s degree in STEM than are URMs (Hurtado, Eagan, and Hughes, 2012).
1.2. Predicting STEM Persistence
To date, factors shown to predict STEM persistence among college students include prior academic achievement (e.g., high school grades and standardized test scores; Hurtado et al., 2012; Swail, Redd, and Perna, 2003), science-related experiences (e.g., research experience and science identification; Hurtado et al., 2008; Schultz et al., 2011), goal-directed behaviors and mastery orientation (Harackiewicz, Barron, Tauer, and Elliot, 2002; Hernandez, Schultz, Woodcock, and Chance, 2013), and institutional supports (Chang, Cerna, Han, and Saenz, 2008; Estrada et al., 2016; Hurtado et al., 2012). Many of these same factors also predict URM success in STEM, but the relative strength of their predictive associations may differ for URMs compared to NURMs. For instance, research experiences benefit students in general, but the benefits for URMs may be much broader, including not only STEM persistence but lower overall attrition (Barlow and Villarejo, 2004) and higher graduation rates (Maton, Hrabowski, and Schmitt, 2000); further, research experiences may have greater impact on less academically prepared URM students than other groups (Kinkead, 2003).
In more recent years, greater emphasis has been placed on psychological variables such as identity threat (Cohen, Garcia, Apfel, and Master, 2006; Walton and Cohen, 2007), academic and science self-efficacy (Larson et al, 2015; MacPhee, Farro, and Canetto, 2013), values orientation (Hrackiewicz, Canning, Tibbetts, Priniski, and Hyde, 2015), and motivation (Eccles, 2004; Estrada, Woodcock, Hernandez, and Schultz, 2011), and their impact on STEM persistence. Some of these psychological variables have been shown to be especially impactful on URM persistence (Espinoza, 2011; Estrada et al., 2016). One potentially important individual- level variable that is receiving increasing attention in psychology—self-regulatory processes— has not received wide attention in the STEM literature.
1.3. Self-Regulation and STEM Persistence
Self-regulation refers to processes by which individuals purposefully expend effort monitoring, managing, controlling or otherwise altering their responses and impulses in order to pursue focal or valued objectives (Carver and Scheier, 1998, 2013; Luszczynska, Diehl, Gutiérrez-Doña, Kuusinen, and Schwarzer, 2004; Zell and Baumeister, 2013). Self-regulatory processes can take the form of more cognitive skills such as attention, concentration, goal awareness, self-monitoring vis-à-vis goal progress, and problem-solving to better attain goals (for reviews, see Carver and Scheier, 1998; Sitzmann and Ely, 2011). Self-regulation also involves managing positive and negative emotions, controlling impulses, effectively channeling emotional energy, and expressing and reading emotions appropriately (see Gross and Thompson, 2007; Koole, 2009; Web, Miles, and Sheeran, 2012). Other self-regulation skills involve using adaptive behaviors (e.g., seeking information, talking about one’s problems, minimizing distractions, improving study skills, and appropriate self-care) to effectively reach one’s desired goals rather than engaging in more avoidant or less effective strategies (e.g., playing video games, using alcohol) (see Carver and Connor-Smith, 2010; Skinner and Zimmer-Gembeck, 2007).
Although self-regulatory processes include cognitive, emotional, and behavioral skill sets, there is currently no consensus on how specific self-regulation skills should be categorized (Augustine and Hemenover, 2009). Self-regulation can take place through cognitive, behavioral and emotional strategies, but any one of these strategies can also target the regulation of cognitions, behaviors, or emotions, and often more than one type simultaneously (Gard, Noggle, Park, Vago, and Wilson, 2014; Kelley, Wagner, and Heatherton, 2015). For instance, positive reframing is often categorized as a cognitive strategy (i.e., reappraising a negative event in a more positive light) but can also function as an emotional strategy (i.e., doing so reduces negative affect), and/or behavioral strategy (i.e., talking about the event differently). Thus, the “self-regulation” umbrella includes multiple and overlapping constructs.
Despite difficulties in conceptualization, self-regulation has been related to success in many domains (Carver and Scheier, 1998; Heatherton, 2011), especially in stressful situations (Eisenberg, Smith, and Spinrad, 2016; Skinner and Zimmer-Gembeck, 2007; Wood, 2016) and in contexts of performance and ability challenge (Davis, DiStefano, and Schultz, 2008). Such stresses and challenges are commonly experienced in and around classrooms and other educational settings (Vermetten, Vermunt, and Lodewijks, 1999; Zimmerman and Bandura, 1994) including academic success in general undergraduate samples (e.g., Tangney, Baumeister, and Boone, 2004). For example, one meta-analysis found self-regulation predicted academic performance and retention over and above traditional and other psychosocial indicators (e.g., Robbins et al., 2004). Moreover, self-regulatory processes reliably predict or account for several key behavioral factors (e.g., beneficial habit formation, organizing material, note-taking, choosing study-conducive environments) that, in turn, explain long-term and sustained academic achievement (e.g., Galla and Duckworth, 2015; Robbins et al., 2004; Robbins, Le, and Oh, 2009). To date, none of this work has examined its specific role in minority student success or in STEM persistence or success.
We assert that students majoring in STEM fields deal with many academic challenges and stressors above and beyond those faced by all college students. Self-regulation processes may be particularly important for persisting in and achieving success in biomedical science/STEM fields, especially for URM college students who intend to pursue STEM careers. . To our knowledge, self-regulation has not yet been examined in terms of URM success in STEM or biomedical research careers. If self-regulation is found to promote STEM persistence and success, it will provide a useful target for interventions beyond existing curricular and institutional interventions.
1.4. Current Study: Self-Regulation Predicting Persistence and Success in STEM
We examined three separate indicators of success in the first year of college for STEM students. A few studies have examined academic performance (e.g., GPA) and persistence (e.g., retention in STEM major), reporting different patterns of predictors for each (e.g., Hurtado et al., 2008: Espinoza, 2011; Robbins et al., 2004); however, very few studies have compared these institutional outcomes in the context of self-regulatory processes. In the present study, we examined both (a) institution-reported outcomes of students’ GPAs and majors and (b) self- reported intentions to pursue a science career as our indicators of academic success in the first year. We asked three research questions: (1) Do URM and NURM students bound toward STEM degrees and careers possess different levels of self-regulation skills at entry into college? (2) Do self-regulation skills predict success for URM and NURM students in STEM majors, over and above background characteristics at the end of the first year of college? and (3) Is self-regulation particularly important for URM success in STEM (i.e., are associations with self-regulation moderated by URM status)?
2. Method
2.1. Participants
A sample of 755 first year students in STEM majors was drawn from a larger study of student success – The UConn Success Study, an ongoing longitudinal panel study primarily comprising first year students enrolled at the University of Connecticut (UConn) in the fall of 2015. This study was designed to examine the role of self-regulation skills as potential determinants of academic success, with a particular focus on URM and non-URM students in STEM majors. The study began in the spring/summer of 2015 (baseline) and surveyed 1,839 students accepted for fall admission who participated in orientation prior to fall enrollment. Of that number, 11% (n = 210) were nonfreshmen (i.e., transfer students) and less than 1% (n = 11) ultimately did not enroll despite participating in the summer orientation. Here we report STEM success and persistence outcomes from the first year of data collection. The study sample was restricted to freshmen who enrolled at the university (i.e., n = 1,618) and the current analytic sample consists of those students who declared a STEM major at any point within the first year of college (n = 755).
At baseline, the analytic sample was approximately evenly split by gender (51.9% male). Regarding the sample’s race/ethnicity, 63.1% self-identified as White, 19.9% were Asian, 6.9% were Hispanic/Latino, 3.9% were Black/African American, 2.9% were 2-or-more races, and 3.5% did not self-report race/ethnicity. Most (87.3%) identified English as their first/native language, 9.3% reported English and another language as their first language, and 3.5% reported a language other than English as their first language. Finally, 87.7% of the analytic sample were admitted to the university in a STEM major.
There were few demographic differences between freshmen who participated in the study (n = 1618) and those who did not (n = 2193). Similar to students who participated, less than 1% (n = 20) of nonparticipants did not enroll despite participating in orientation and paying an enrollment fee. In addition, gender was comparable between groups, with males constituting 53.6% of nonparticipants. Self-reported race/ethnicity via institutional records indicated that nonparticipant freshmen were 57% White, 6.3% Black, 3.0% Hispanic/Latino, 21.3% Asian, .6% two or more races, and 11.6% unknown. While there were some differences in the representation of race/ethnicity between participating and nonparticipating groups, these differences may be explained in part by the higher number of nonparticipants who did not report their race/ethnicity on their admissions forms as well as differences in how race versus ethnicity were assessed on the survey measures versus admissions records. The overall racial/ethnic composition of the Fall 2015 cohort at UConn was comparable to previous years at the university.
At baseline (i.e., prior to fall enrollment), 670 students were STEM majors and 85 were non-STEM majors. In the fall semester, 672 students were STEM majors and 43 were non- STEM majors. In the fall semester, 672 students were STEM majors and 43 were non-STEM majors. By the spring semester, 637 students were STEM majors and 78 were non-STEM majors. Cross-tabulation analysis of fall and spring STEM majors showed that 43 (6%) non- STEM students switched into a STEM major from fall to spring, 78 (11%) STEM students switched to a non-STEM major from fall to spring, and 594 (83%) students started and remained STEM majors from fall to spring.
2.2. Procedure
The study protocol consisted of (a) an online survey administered to students in the summer prior to starting at the university and (b) administrative data provided by the institution (e.g., declared major, grade point average) gathered after the end of the first academic year. All study procedures were approved by the university Institutional Review Board and participants compensated $20 following each wave of data collection. All participants provided documented informed consent. No authors have a conflict of interest to declare.
2.3. Measures
2.3.1. Self-Regulation Skills.
Because self-regulation skills have been variously defined and there is no agreed-upon measurement strategy, we elected to cast a wide net by employing a range of self-regulation measures tapping cognitive, emotional and behavioral facets and testing whether an underlying factor structure emerged: (a) Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (ERQ: Gross and John, 2003) consists of two scales: the Cognitive Reappraisal scale comprises six items that measure the tendency to redirect or reinterpret negative emotional stimuli, and the Expressive Suppression scale comprises four items that measure the degree to which individuals manage their emotions by restricting emotional reactions and responses. Response options for each item range from 1 (Strongly disagree) to 7 (Strongly agree). (b) Brief COPE (Carver, 1997). The Brief COPE assesses 14 coping strategies employed over the past year in response to difficult or stressful events. We used nine scales from the Brief COPE that reflect self-regulating coping strategies ranging from cognitive approaches (Positive Reframing) to emotional approaches (Venting) to behavioral approaches (Active Coping). The remaining six COPE scales included were Instrumental Support Seeking, Emotional Support Seeking, Humor, Religious coping, Substance Use, and Planning. Each strategy consisted of two items and response options for each item range from 1 (not at all) to 4 (a lot). Scores are derived as a mean of the two items.
2.3.2. Control Variables/Demographics.
2.3.2.1. Demographics.
Students self-reported their gender (Female, Male, or Other [Please specify]), race/ethnicity (American Indian/Alaskan Native, Asian, Black/African American, Hispanic/Latino, Native Hawaiian/Pacific Islander, Two or more races, White, or Prefer not to respond), and first language spoken (English, English and non-English, or non- English). Demographic variables were transformed into dummy-coded indicators or demographic background for all analyses. We dummy-coded gender (0 = male, 1 = female), Race/ethnicity (0 = NURM (White or Asian), 1 = URM (American Indian/Alaskan Native, Black/African American, Hispanic/Latino, Native Hawaiian/Pacific Islander, or Two or more races), and first language spoken (0 = English, 1 = English and non-English, or non-English).
2.3.2.2. Precollege Enrichment Experiences.
Precollege enrichment experiences were measured using items developed from previous research (Chang et al., 2008; Hurtado, et al., 2006). Hurtado et al. (2006) found that specific precollege activities that reflect more enriched academic experiences (e.g., “participated in summer research programs”, “took advanced placement courses in science, math, or technology”), along with traditional academic success indicators (e.g., high school GPA, standardized test scores and background characteristics), predicted both aspirations and persistence in science-based careers and degree attainment. Additional enrichment activities unique to UConn students were also included (e.g., a summer bridge program for prospective engineering students and a program that allows students in Connecticut to earn UConn course credits while in high school). Students indicated whether they had participated in or experienced 15 enrichment experiences prior to enrolling in college. Response options were binary, scaled 1 (Yes) or 0 (No). Scale scores were derived as the sum of the 15 items. Prior research with similar scales has indicated that the scores were reliable with undergraduates (Hurtado et. al., 2006).
2.3.2.3. Ever a STEM Major.
Student administrative records were obtained to determine major at three time points during the first year of college: at time of admission, beginning of fall semester, and beginning of spring semester. Majors were categorized as STEM (1) or non-STEM (0) using the National Science Foundation STEM Classification (2016), which includes 137 STEM majors. Major was coded as STEM or non-STEM at each time point.
2.3.3. Outcomes.
2.3.3.1. Intentions to pursue a scientific research career.
Our proximal outcome measure was students’ intention to pursue a scientific career at baseline (i.e., prior to entering college). Students were asked to respond to two questions: “To what extent do you plan to pursue a science-related research career?” and “What is the likelihood of you obtaining a science-related degree?” on a scale from 0 (Definitely will not) to 10 (Definitely will). Scale scores were derived as the mean of the two items.
2.3.3.2. Cumulative GPA (Spring 2016).
Student administrative records were obtained to determine cumulative grade point average (GPA) at the end of the spring semester of the first academic year.
2.3.3.3. Persistence in a STEM major.
Student administrative records were used to derive a measure of persistence. Students admitted as a STEM major and still a declared STEM major in the spring semester of their first year were categorized as having persisted (1). Students admitted as a non-STEM major, but who switched to a STEM major by the spring semester were also categorized as having persisted (1). Finally, students who were admitted as a STEM major or declared a STEM major in the fall semester, but switched to a non-STEM major by the spring semester were categorized as not having persisted (0).
3. Results
3.1. Statistical Assumptions and Analysis Details
Prior to conducting substantive analyses, we evaluated the distributional characteristics of the data (Table 1) and intercorrelations among the variables (see Table 2) in SPSS software version 23.). Because self-regulation skills were measured on different metrics, we transformed all self-regulation scale scores to put them on a common normalized metric (Blom, 1958) and used these scores in all subsequent analyses. Prior to conducting inferential tests with transformed variables, we examined the data for outliers and inspected the tenability of statistical assumptions. We found no evidence of extreme outliers (i.e., leverage and Cook’s D values were all acceptably small; Judd, McClelland, and Ryan, 2009) and determined that the statistical assumptions were met (i.e., Box’s M = 66.67, F[66, 49681.90] = 0.95, p = .59; the ratio of largest to smallest variance between URM and NURM groups did not exceed 3.0 for any of the self- regulation variables; and Q-Q plots showed only minor deviations from normality for most self- regulation variables). The only exception concerned the normality of the Religious Coping and Substance Use scale scores, which Q-Q plots showed to be positively skewed in both groups.
Table 1.
Variable | N | Standardized | Unstandardized | Skew | Kurtosis |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
M (SD) | M (SD) | ||||
Female | 755 | 0.49 (0.5) | 0.49 (0.50) | 0.06 | −2.00 |
Precollege Enrichment Experiences | 755 | 0.00 (1.94) | 2.54 (1.94) | 0.28 | −0.61 |
URM Status | 755 | 0.15 (0.35) | 0.15 (0.36) | 1.98 | 1.92 |
First Language Status | 755 | 0.14 (0.35) | 0.18 (0.47) | 2.09 | 2.35 |
Religious Coping | 532 | 0.00 (0.83) | 1.73 (0.94) | 0.69 | −0.79 |
Humor Coping | 532 | 0.00 (0.85) | 2.11 (0.92) | 0.24 | −0.82 |
Substance Use Coping | 532 | 0.00 (0.54) | 1.10 (0.38) | 3.13 | 8.61 |
Emotional Suppression | 525 | 0.00 (1.01) | 3.72 (1.25) | 0.04 | −0.2 |
Cognitive Reappraisal | 525 | 0.00 (0.99) | 5.00 (1.03) | −0.07 | −0.23 |
Positive Reframing | 531 | 0.00 (0.91) | 2.96 (0.80) | −0.29 | −0.55 |
Planning Coping | 531 | 0.00 (0.89) | 3.01 (0.76) | −0.23 | −0.61 |
Active Coping | 622 | 0.00 (0.92) | 3.10 (0.74) | −0.1 | −0.13 |
Instrumental Support Seeking | 624 | 0.00 (0.96) | 2.69 (0.93) | 0.09 | −0.45 |
Emotional Support Seeking | 531 | 0.00 (0.93) | 2.56 (0.91) | −0.01 | −0.79 |
Venting | 531 | 0.00 (0.91) | 1.98 (0.73) | 0.28 | −0.45 |
Scientific Career Persistence Intentions | 518 | 8.67 (2.51) | 8.67 (2.52) | −1.33 | 1.08 |
First Year Cumulative GPA | 683 | 3.17 (0.66) | 3.17 (0.67) | −1.18 | 1.72 |
Persistence in a STEM Major | 691 | 0.89 (0.31) | 0.89 (0.31) | −2.49 | 4.22 |
Notes: Standardized variables derived using the Blom transformation.
Table 2.
Variable | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 | 9 | 10 | 11 | 12 | 13 | 14 | 15 | 16 | 17 | 18 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. Gender | 1.00 | |||||||||||||||||
2. Precollege Enrichment Experiences | −.02 | 1.00 | ||||||||||||||||
3. URM | .02 | −.12** | 1.00 | |||||||||||||||
4. First Language Status | .05 | −.12** | .04 | 1.00 | ||||||||||||||
5. Religious Coping | .17*** | .11* | .14** | .13** | 1.00 | |||||||||||||
6. Humor Coping | −.09* | .02 | −.05 | −.01 | −.06 | 1.00 | ||||||||||||
7. Substance Use Coping | −.01 | −.10* | .06 | −.01 | .03 | .15*** | 1.00 | |||||||||||
8. Expressive Suppression | −.19*** | .07 | −.03 | .06 | −.08 | .05 | −.01 | 1.00 | ||||||||||
9. Cognitive Reappraisal | .17*** | −.04 | .01 | .04 | .13** | −.02 | −.06 | −.16*** | 1.00 | |||||||||
10. Positive Reframing | .11* | −.12* | −.01 | .07 | .24*** | .21*** | .12** | −.22*** | .42*** | 1.00 | ||||||||
11. Planning Coping | .00 | .00 | .02 | .08 | .21*** | .16*** | .00 | −.07 | .26*** | .47*** | 1.00 | |||||||
12. Active Coping | .06 | −.07 | .00 | .09* | .18*** | .15** | .02 | −.10* | .35*** | .55*** | .68*** | 1.00 | ||||||
13. Instrumental Support Seeking | .20*** | −.09* | −.01 | .08* | .22*** | .18*** | .04 | −.28*** | .24*** | .47*** | .49*** | .49*** | 1.00 | |||||
14. Emotional Support Seeking | .23*** | −.08 | .05 | .04 | .18*** | .14** | .04 | −.33*** | .25*** | .44*** | .40*** | .41*** | .75*** | 1.00 | ||||
15. Venting | .17*** | −.03 | .02 | .02 | .16*** | .23*** | .07 | −.20*** | .07 | .22*** | .27*** | .18*** | .35*** | .37*** | 1.00 | |||
16. Intentions to Pursue a Scientific Career | .05 | .19*** | −.02 | .03 | .05 | −.01 | −.10* | −.05 | .17*** | .02 | .06 | .07 | .04 | .06 | −.01 | 1.00 | ||
17. First Year Cumulative GPA | .06 | .26*** | −.26*** | −.07 | .08 | .02 | −.08 | .02 | .04 | −.01 | −.00 | .02 | −.03 | −.02 | .04 | .03 | 1.00 | |
18. Persistence in a STEM Major | .00 | .06 | −.06 | .00 | −.04 | −.00 | −.14** | −.05 | −.02 | −.02 | −.02 | .00 | .02 | .02 | .05 | .09* | .05 | 1.00 |
p<.05
p<.01
p<.001
Structural equation modeling (SEM) was used to simultaneously predict the three outcomes from the self-regulation skills and control variables. Control variables included gender, first language status, precollege enrichment score, and URM status given that these variables have been shown to affect success and persistence in STEM (e.g., Han, Capraro, and Capraro, 2015; Wolniak, 2016). Because the STEM persistence outcome was binary, we used Weighted Least Squares (WLSMV) to estimate structural equation models in Mplus version 8.01. Finally, consistent with recommendations for controlling Type-I error rate in complex SEMs, we adopted a Bonferroni adjusted alpha level of .017 (i.e., alpha level = .05/#DVs = .05/3 = .017) when evaluating parameter estimates (Green and Babyak, 1997).
3.2. Differences in Levels of Self-Regulation Skills for URMs Versus NURMs
To address our first research question (i.e., degree to which URM and NURM students possess different levels of self-regulation skills), we conducted a one-way MANOVA to compare URMs and NURMs on the 11 measures of cognitive, emotional, and behavioral self- regulation skills. The resulting multivariate F test was nonsignificant, Wilks’ λ = .97, F(11, 512) = 1.67, p = .08, η2partial = .04, indicating no differences in initial self-regulation skills were evident between URM and NURM groups, Table 1.
3.3. Self-Regulation Skills Predict Persistence and Academic Outcomes
3.3.1. Measuring self-regulation.
Prior to addressing our second and third research questions, we sought to discover if the 11 self-regulation skills could be more parsimoniously represented in terms of the expected cognitive, behavioral, and emotional self-regulation constructs. Therefore, we used exploratory factor analysis (EFA) with principal axis factoring to reveal the underlying number of latent factors. Pre-EFA diagnostics revealed that three of the self-regulation measures, Religious coping, Substance Use coping, and Expressive Suppression, had extremely low communalities (i.e., <.20) and thus were not retained in the EFA (Thompson, 2004). Robust factoring techniques such as parallel analysis (Horn, 1965; Hayton, Allen, Scarpello, 2004) and Velicer’s minimum average partial revised test (Velicer, 1976; Velicer et al., 2000) indicated two factors explained the pattern of associations among the remaining eight measures of self-regulation skills.
We examined the pattern and structure matrices to determine the nature of the two underlying factors. The first factor, which we label Cognitive-Emotional self-regulation skills, had strong positive loadings with Active coping, Planning coping, Positive Reframing coping, and Cognitive Reappraisal (supplemental materials, Table S1), while the second factor, which we label Support-Seeking self-regulation skills, had strong positive loadings with Emotional Support Seeking coping, Instrumental Social Support Seeking coping, and Venting coping. Humor coping did not load strongly onto either factor and was therefore removed prior to reliability analyses. Reliability analyses indicated that the Cognitive-Emotional and Support- Seeking self-regulation factors exhibited acceptable reliability (α = .75 and .73, respectively).
Based on the EFA information, scale scores were created for Cognitive-Emotional and Support Seeking self-regulations skills for use as predictors in the following outcomes models. Self-regulation skills that did not load onto either factor, Humor coping, Religious coping, Substance Use coping, and Expressive Suppression coping, were included as individual predictors in the outcomes models.
3.3.2. Predicting persistence and academic outcomes and testing moderation.
To directly address our second and third research questions (i.e., predicting outcomes from self- regulation skills and moderating effect of URM status on self-regulation skills, respectively), we conducted a series of three nested structural equation models predicting the outcomes. Specifically, the first SEM predicted the outcomes of persistence intentions, end of first year cumulative GPA, and persistence in a STEM major from: a) control variables (i.e., gender, first language status, precollege enrichment score, and URM status), b) all self-regulation skills (i.e., Cognitive-Emotional, Support Seeking, Religious coping, Substance Use coping, Expressive Suppression coping, and Humor coping), and c) six multiplicative terms representing two-way interactions between URM status and each self-regulation skill. The first SEM provided adequate fit to the data, χ2(28) = 113.81, p < .001, CFI = .96, RMSEA = .06 with 90% CI [.05, .08]. The second SEM directly tested research question two (i.e., degree to which self-regulation skills predict success for students in STEM majors) by constraining the paths from self-regulation skills and multiplicative terms to the outcomes to zero. A comparison of fit in Model 1 versus Model 2 revealed that Model 2 significantly worsened fit, Δχ2(48) = 68.65, p = .03. The third SEM tested research question three (i.e., degree to which self-regulation skills are particularly important for URM success) by constraining the paths from multiplicative terms to the outcomes to zero. Again, a comparison of fit in Model 1 versus Model 3 revealed that Model 3 significantly worsened fit, Δχ2(24) = 37.35, p = .04. The model comparisons tests indicated that both self-regulation skills and differential effects of self-regulations skills across URM and NURM groups significantly contributed to the prediction of the outcomes.
We inspected the parameter estimates in order to determine the precise nature of the relationship between the self-regulation skills and outcomes (Table 3). Concerning scientific persistence intentions, the parameter estimates indicated a statistically significant URM by Cognitive-Emotional self-regulations skills moderation effect, as well as a significant positive effect of precollege enrichment experiences. A simple slopes analysis, depicted in Figure 1, revealed that Cognitive-Emotional self-regulation skills was a stronger predictor of scientific persistence intentions in the URM group (dotted black line) compared to the NURM group (solid black line).
Table 3.
Scientific Career Persistence Intentions | First Year Cumulative GPA | Persistence in a STEM Major | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Predictor | b (SE) | β | b (SE) | β | b (SE) | β |
Intercept | 8.54 (.21)*** | 3.19 (.16)*** | 1.26 (.12)*** | |||
Female status | 0.22 (.23) | .04 | 0.11 (.05) | .08 | 0.00 (.13) | .00 |
English first language status | 0.22 (.36) | .03 | −0.12 (.09) | −.06 | −0.02 (.19) | −.01 |
Precollege Enrichment Experiences | 0.25 (.06)*** | .19 | 0.07 (.02)*** | .21 | 0.06 (.04) | .11 |
URM Status (U) | 0.13 (.30) | .02 | −0.48 (.08)*** | −.26 | −0.23 (.18) | −.08 |
Cognitive−Emotional SRS (CE) | 0.36 (.20) | .11 | 0.04 (.04) | .04 | −0.09 (.12) | −.07 |
Support-Seeking SRS (SS) | −0.04 (.21) | −.01 | −0.04 (.04) | −.05 | 0.10 (.14) | .08 |
Religious Coping (RC) | −0.09 (.16) | −.03 | 0.07 (.05) | .09 | −0.10 (.12) | −.08 |
Humor Coping (HC) | −0.08 (.15) | −.03 | 0.06 (.04) | .08 | 0.04 (.10) | .03 |
Substance Use Coping (SU) | −0.34 (.21) | −.07 | −0.1 (.06) | −.08 | −0.34 (.12)** | −.18 |
Emotional Suppression (ES) | −0.13 (.13) | −.05 | −0.01 (.04) | −.02 | −0.13 (.11) | −.13 |
U×CE | 1.61 (.61)** | .18 | 0.00 (.13) | .00 | 0.18 (.44) | .05 |
U×SS | −0.49 (.58) | −.06 | 0.00 (.11) | .00 | 0.02 (.42) | .01 |
U×RC | −0.06 (.37) | −.01 | −0.01 (.12) | .00 | 0.06 (.28) | .02 |
U×HC | 0.39 (.36) | .05 | −0.31 (.08)*** | −.14 | −0.32 (.21) | −.10 |
U×SU | −0.68 (.40) | −.06 | 0.14 (.14) | .05 | 0.13 (.29) | .03 |
U×ES | 0.23 (.32) | .04 | 0.13 (.07) | .09 | 0.21 (.29) | .09 |
Notes: Female status and first language status (control variables) were nonsignificant predictors and thus were constrained to zero in all models; therefore female status and first language status variables do not appear in the table above. All continuous self-regulation skills were grand-mean centered for analysis and all covariances were estimated among the continuous predictors (including multiplicative terms).
p<.017
p<.01
p<.001
Concerning first year cumulative GPA, the parameter estimates revealed a URM by Humor coping moderation effect, as well as effects of precollege enrichment experiences and URM status. A simple slopes analysis, depicted in Figure 2, revealed that Humor coping was a stronger negative predictor of first year cumulative GPA in the URM group (dotted black line) compared to the NURM group (solid black line). Concerning persistence in a STEM major, the parameter estimates indicated that Substance Use coping had a negative effect for both URM and NURMs.
4. Discussion
Overall, our study demonstrates that students come to college already using a range of self-regulatory strategies to manage stressful situations. Our results suggest that self-regulation plays an important role in persistence of STEM majors, both in intentions to persist in STEM and in their actual persistence in a STEM major across their first year of college. These results suggest fruitful new approaches to supporting students in their persistence in STEM despite the challenges and strains involved in these majors (Holland and Piper, 2016).
4.1. STEM Students and Self-Regulation
STEM students reported moderately high use of some self-regulatory strategies as evidenced by mean scores above the midpoint for both ERQ scales and five of the nine Brief COPE scales. At the beginning of college, students appear to rely on strategies that are more problem-focused (Planning, Active coping) and that help them manage their emotions (Cognitive Reappraisal)--all of which are generally considered adaptive strategies (Carver, Scheier, and Weintraub, 1989; Cheng and Cheung, 2005; Davis et al., 2008). It is possible that first year STEM students employ these strategies in anticipation of experiencing more academic challenges, but it is also possible that first year students in general are more likely to endorse these strategies as they are consistent with the messages and preparation students receive prior to enrolling in college and during orientation (e.g., problem solve, think positively).
We had hypothesized that URMs would differ from NURMs in their self-regulatory styles, but in fact, no differences emerged. That URMs arrive at college with reasonably strong self-regulation styles and are not disadvantaged in that regard relative to NURMs means that baseline self-regulation does not explain URMs’ higher rates of attrition from STEM (Hurtado et al., 2012).
Our factor analysis of self-regulation styles yielded a large factor comprising multiple and diverse adaptive strategies, including cognitive reappraisal, positive reframing, and active coping, all aspects of self-regulation that have been shown to be associated with favorable outcomes, including college GPA (e.g., Casillas et al., 2012; Robbins et al., 2004; Tagney et al., 2004). While we had included cognitive, emotional and behavioral aspects of self-regulation, the fact that they loaded together rather than producing separate factors is consistent with theories of emotion generation and regulation that detail how these diverse aspects of regulation work together (e.g., Ellsworth and Scherer, 2003). The second self-regulation factor, seeking social support, is a common factor in studies of self-regulation in stressful situations (e.g., Lazarus and Folkman, 1984; Carver, et al., 1989; Skinner, Edge, Altman, and Sherwood, 2003). Although seeking social support typically predicts myriad positive aspects of adjustment (e.g., Aspinwall and Taylor, 1993; Ben-Zur, 2009), this factor was unrelated to our three indicators of success. Perhaps social support coping has a greater impact as STEM students progress through their college career or become more immersed in courses specific to their major. It may also be that seeking social support is more effective for some groups of students than others (e.g., women; Bonneville-Roussy, Evans, Verner-Filion, Vallerand, and Bouffard, 2017; Reevy and Maslach, 2001). We also found that many coping strategies did not fit into those two larger factors but rather are employed independently.
4.2. Self-Regulation Styles as Predictors of Success and Persistence in STEM
As assessed at baseline, cognitive-emotional self-regulation skills predicted students’ intentions to persist in STEM, even when controlling for students’ demographic characteristics, none of which predicted intentions to persist. In fact, students’ precollege enrichment experiences were the only other predictor of student STEM intentions. Importantly, this positive effect of cognitive-emotional self-regulation skills on intentions to persist was stronger for URMs than for NURMs.
Curiously, the only aspect of self-regulation that predicted actual persistence was use of alcohol and other drugs to cope, which predicted actual lower persistence in a STEM major across the first year after controlling for gender, first language status, precollege enrichment score, and URM status. These findings are consistent with other studies. As in our sample, although this type of self-regulation is typically reported at low levels of use by college students (e.g., Ham and Hope, 2003), it tends to be strongly associated with a host of negative outcomes, including stress, less perceived academic achievement, personal injuries, and unplanned sexual activity (Wills, 1986; El Ansari, Stock, and Mills, 2013; Wechsler, Dowdall, Davenport, and Castillo, 1995).
Our third outcome, cumulative GPA, is obviously highly important; this objective measure of performance will strongly influence students’ future prospects. The only aspect of baseline self-regulation that predicted cumulative first year GPA was using humor to cope. This type of coping has been inconsistently related to psychological well-being, with some studies finding that using humor in specific situations can be helpful (e.g., Martin and Lefcourt, 1983; Nezu, Wheeler, and Reis, 1988) while other studies have demonstrated its less positive or potentially adverse effects on well-being (e.g., Kuiper and Martin, 1998; Thorson, et al., 1997). These discrepancies may be due in part to the nature of the stressor (acute versus chronic) as well as situational context (benign versus threatening) (McGraw and Warren, 2010). In our study, using humor as a general way to cope with stress predicted lower academic performance in the first year, an effect that was stronger for URMs than for NURMs. These results suggest that a tendency to make light of potentially stressful situations might interfere with individuals’ abilities to engage in more adaptive academic strategies (e.g., study skills, support seeking).
We expected that self-regulation would be particularly important in predicting first-year outcomes for URMs relative to NURMs, and we found some support for this hypothesis: Cognitive-emotional self-regulation was more strongly related to intentions to persist for URMs, and use of humor coping was related to lower GPA especially for URMs.
4.3. Limitations
Study limitations must be noted. First, although we endeavored to include a full cohort of incoming first year students (approximately 3800 students), less than half (43%) participated in the study. Although the percentage of men, URMs, and STEM majors was comparable between those who participated and those who did not (i.e., 51 vs. 53%, 10.8 vs. 9.3%, and 40.4 vs. 37%, respectively), other sample characteristics may have biased our results in unknown ways. Second, because there is no consensus regarding conceptualization or measurement of self- regulation, we may have omitted some important aspects of regulatory style or skill that promotes STEM student persistence and success. Further, our analyses include only the first year’s metrics and only baseline self-regulation. Self-regulation strategies likely change across the college years and may have more meaningful relationships with persistence and success over time. Thus, self-regulation strategies may become more predictive of STEM student success as students delve deeper into their academic studies and the nature of their stressors evolves. Future analyses may reveal more about these developmental processes.
4.4. Strengths and Conclusions
This study has important strengths as well. Our sample of URM and NURM students is large enough to allow for in-depth examination of self-regulatory processes as they pertain to STEM persistence. Combining institutional data with self-report is very rare, yet illuminating (Linn et al., 2015). Our three outcomes were not strongly correlated—in fact, student intentions to persist in STEM and their actual persistence in a STEM major across the first year were correlated at r = .09. This lack of correspondence could be a result of the restricted range observed in both measures or it could suggest our measures tap into different aspects of STEM persistence and success. URM status and precollege enrichment experiences significantly predicted first year GPA, with lower first year GPAs for URMs and higher first year GPAs for STEM students with more precollege enrichment experiences. URMs also reported fewer precollege enrichment experiences which may have contributed to their lower first year GPA. Precollege enrichment may be especially impactful for URMs as these experiences not only provide opportunities for skill development but also facilitate self-regulation skills important for college success and STEM persistence (e.g., positive reframing, instrumental support). A focus on self-regulation skills is also a strength, given that these are highly amenable to change (Heatherton, 2011; Zimmerman, 1990) and can be measured early in students’ course of study to provide clear avenues for interventions, including opportunities to tailor those interventions to the needs of particular students. For example, some types of research experiences or programs may be more helpful to students with strong self-regulation skills (e.g., independent study) while other types may be more helpful to students with self-regulation deficits (e.g., course-based research experiences).
Future research is needed to understand how self-regulation abilities that STEM students arrive with predict their trajectories across their college career, how they develop and change, and how they may best be promoted to optimize student persistence and success in STEM. The present study is a step in this direction, in highlighting the different ways that self-regulation may influence different indicators of STEM persistence and success.
Supplementary Material
Author Bios
V. Bede Agocha, Ph.D., is an Assistant Professor-in-Residence in the Department of Psychological Sciences, and the Africana Studies Institute, at the University of Connecticut. His research interests focus on the role of culture in health behavior decision making and achievement.
Lauren Carney is a doctoral student in the Clinical Psychology program at the University of Connecticut. Her research interests include religious and spiritual coping and mindfulness-based interventions in the context of health-related conditions such as cancer survivorship.
Andrea DePetris, M.A., is a doctoral student in the department of Psychological Sciences at the University of Connecticut. Her research interests focus on the impact of race and culture on psychological, physical, and behavioral health.
Paul R. Hernandez, Ph.D., is an Assistant Professor of Educational Psychology in the Department of Learning Sciences and Human Development at West Virginia University. His research focuses on psychological factors that promote academic/career success (particularly among underrepresented groups in science) and on the design, measurement, and evaluation of interventions aimed at broadening participation in science.
Sharon Y. Lee, M.S., M.A. is a doctoral candidate in Clinical Psychology at the University of Connecticut, where she works under the advisement of Dr. Crystal Park. Broadly, Sharon’s research interests are the effects of trauma and stress on physical health and the mind-body processes underlying this relationship.
Crystal L. Park, Ph.D., is Professor of Psychological Sciences at the University of Connecticut. Her research focuses on coping and self-regulation in the context of daily stress and major life stressors, including trauma and health crises.
Michelle Williams, Ph.D., is an Associate Vice President for Research at the University of Connecticut where she is also an Associate Professor in the Department of Psychological Sciences and Africana Studies Institute. Her research interests focus broadly in the area of multicultural psychology with a specific emphasis on the impact of race and culture on psychological well-being, trauma recovery, and student outcomes.
Contributor Information
Crystal L. Park, University of Connecticut
Michelle Williams, University of Connecticut.
Paul Hernandez, West Virginia University.
V. Bede Agocha, University of Connecticut.
Lauren M. Carney, University of Connecticut
Andrea DePetris, University of Connecticut.
Sharon Y. Lee, University of Connecticut
References
- Aspinwall LG, & Taylor SE (1992). Modeling cognitive adaptation: A longitudinal investigation of the impact of individual differences and coping on college adjustment and performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 989–1003. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.63.6.989 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Augustine AA, & Hemenover SH (2009). On the relative effectiveness of affect regulation strategies: A meta-analysis. Cognition and Emotion, 23, 1181–1220. doi: 10.1080/02699930802396556 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Barlow AEL, & Villarejo M (2004). Making a difference for minorities: Evaluation of an educational enrichment program. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 41, 861–881. doi: 10.1002/tea.20029 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Ben-Zur H (2009). Coping styles and affect. International Journal of Stress Management, 16, 87–101. doi: 10.1037/a0015731 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Blom G (1958). Statistical estimates and transformed beta-variables New York, NY: Wiley. [Google Scholar]
- Bonneville-Roussy A, Evans P, Verner-Filion J, Vallerand RJ, & Bouffard T (2017). Motivation and coping with the stress of assessment: Gender differences in outcomes for university students. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 48, 28–42. doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2016.08.003 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Carver CS (1997). You want to measure coping but your protocol’s too long: Consider the Brief COPE. International Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 4, 92–100. doi: 10.1207/s15327558ij [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carver CS, & Scheier MF (1998). On the self-regulation of behavior New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Carver CS, & Connor-Smith J (2010). Personality and coping. Annual Review of Psychology, 61, 679–704. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.100352 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carver CS, & Scheier MF (2013). Goals and emotion. In Robinson MD, Watkins ER, & Harmon-Jones E (Eds.), Guilford handbook of cognition and emotion (pp. 176–194). New York, NY: Guilford. [Google Scholar]
- Carver CS, Scheier MF, & Weintraub JK (1989). Assessing coping strategies: A theoretically based approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 267–283. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.56.2.267 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Casillas A, Robbins S, Allen J, Kuo YL, Hanson MA, & Schmeiser C (2012). Predicting early academic failure in high school from prior academic achievement, psychosocial characteristics, and behavior. Journal of Educational Psychology, 104, 407–420. doi: 10.1037/a0027180 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Chang MJ, Cerna O, Han J, & Saenz V (2008). The contradictory roles of institutional status in retaining underrepresented minorities in biomedical and behavioral science majors. The Review of Higher Education, 31, 433–464. doi: 10.1353/rhe.0.0011 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Cheng C, & Cheung MWL (2005). Cognitive processes underlying coping flexibility: Differentiation and integration. Journal of Personality, 73, 859–886. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2005.00331.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chen X, & Soldner M (2014). STEM attrition: College students’ paths into and out of STEM: Fields Statistical Analysis Report Washington, D.C.: National Center for Education Statistics; Retrieved from: www/nces.ed.gov/pubs2014/201400.rev.pdf. [Google Scholar]
- Cohen GL, Garcia J, Apfel N, & Master A (2006). Reducing the racial achievement gap: A social psychological intervention. Science, 313, 1307–1310. doi: 10.1126/science.1128317 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen S, & Wills TA (1985). Stress, social support, and the buffering hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin, 98, 310–357. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.98.2.310 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davis HA, DiStefano C, & Schutz PA (2008). Identifying patterns of appraising tests in first-year college students: Implications for anxiety and emotion regulation during test taking. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100, 942–960. doi: 10.1037/a0013096 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Doherty RW, Orimoto L, Singelis TM, Hatfield I, Hebb J (1995). Emotional contagion: Gender and occupational differences. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 19, 355–371. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-6402.1995.tb00080.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Eccles JS (2004). Schools, academic motivation, and stage-environment fit. In Lerner RM & Steinberg L (Eds.), Handbook of adolescent psychology (pp. 125–153). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley & Sons. [Google Scholar]
- Eisenberg N, Smith CL, & Spinrad TL (2016). Effortful control: Relations with emotion regulation, adjustment, and socialization in childhood. In Vohs KD & Baumeister RF (Eds.), Handbook of Self-Regulation: Research, Theory, and Applications (3rd edition, pp. 458–478). New York, NY: Guilford. [Google Scholar]
- El Ansari W, Stock C, & Mills C (2013). Is alcohol consumption associated with poor academic achievement in university students? International Journal of Preventive Medicine, 4, 1175–1188. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ellsworth PC, & Scherer KR (2003). Appraisal processes in emotion. In Davidson RJ, Sherer KR, & Goldsmith HH (Eds.), Handbook of affective sciences (pp. 572–595). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Estrada M, Woodcock A, Hernandez PR, & Schultz P (2011). Toward a model of social influence that explains minority student integration into the scientific community. Journal of Educational Psychology, 103, 206–212. doi: 10.1037/a0020743 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Estrada M, Burnett M, Campbell AG, Campbell PB, Denetclaw WF, Gutierrez CG, Hurtado S, John GH, Matsui J, McGee R, Okpodu CM, Robinson TJ, Summer MF, Werner-Washburne M, & Zavala M (2016). Improving underrepresented minority student persistence in STEM. CBE Life Sciences Education, 9, es5. doi: 10.1187.cbe.16-01-0038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Espinosa LL (2011). Pipelines and pathways: Women of color in undergraduate STEM majors and the college experiences that contribute to persistence. Harvard Educational Review, 81, 209–240. doi: 10.17763/haer.81.2.92315ww157656k3u [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Galla BM, & Duckworth AL (2015). More than resisting temptation: Beneficial habits mediate the relationship between self-control and positive life outcomes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 109, 508–525. doi: 10.1037/pspp0000026 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gard T, Noggle JJ, Park CL, Vago DR, & Wilson A (2014). Potential self-regulatory mechanisms of yoga for psychological health. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 1–20. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00770 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Green SB, & Babyak MA (1997). Control of type I errors with multiple tests of constraints in structural equation modeling. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 32, 39–51. doi: 10.1207/s15327906mbr3201_2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gross JJ, & Thompson RA (2007). Emotion regulation: Conceptual foundations. In Gross JJ (Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation (pp. 3–24). New York, NY: Guilford. [Google Scholar]
- Gross JJ, & John OP (2003). Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: Implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 348–362. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.348 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ham LS, & Hope DA (2003). College students and problematic drinking: A review of the literature. Clinical Psychology Review, 23, 719–759. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Han S, Capraro R, & Capraro MM (2015). How science, technology, engineering, and mathematics (STEM) project-based learning (PBL) affects high, middle, and low achievers differently: The impact of student factors on achievement. International Journal of Science and Mathematics Education, 13, 1089–1113. doi: .1007/s10763-014-9526-0 [Google Scholar]
- Harackiewicz J, Canning EA, Tibbetts Y, Priniski SJ, & Hyde JS (2015). Closing achievement gaps with a utility-value intervention: Disentangling race and social class. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 111, 745–765. doi: 10.1037/pspp0000075 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harackiewicz JM, Barron KE, Tauer JM, & Elliot AJ (2002). Predicting success in college: A longitudinal study of achievement goals and ability measures as predictors of interest and performance from freshman year through graduation. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94, 562–575. doi: 10.1037//0022-0663.94.3.562 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Hayton JC, Allen DG, & Scarpello V (2004). Factor retention decisions in exploratory factor analysis: A tutorial on parallel analysis. Organizational Research Methods, 7, 191–205. doi: 10.1177/1094428104263675 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Heatherton TF (2011). Neuroscience of self and self-regulation. Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 363–390. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.121208.131616 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hernandez P, Schultz P, Estrada M, Woodcock A, & Chance R (2013) Sustaining optimal motivation: A longitudinal analysis of interventions to broaden participation of underrepresented students in STEM. Journal of Educational Psychology, 105, 89–107. doi: 10.1037/a0029691 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Horn JL (1965). A rationale and test for the number of factors in factor analysis. Psychometrika, 30, 179–185. doi: 10.1007/bf02289447 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holland DD, & Piper RT (2016). Testing a technology integration education model for millennial preservice teachers: Exploring the moderating relationships of goals, feedback, task value, and self-regulation among motivation and technological, pedagogical, and content knowledge competencies. Journal of Educational Computing Research, 54, 196–224. doi: 10.1177/0735633115615129 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Hu L, & Bentler PM (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indices in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6, 1–55. doi: 10.1080/10705519909540118 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Hurtado S, Cerna OS, Chang JC, Sàenz VB, Lopez LR, Mosqueda C, Oseguera L, Chang MJ, & Korn WS (2006). Aspiring scientists: Characteristics of college freshmen interested in the biomedical and behavioral sciences Los Angeles: Higher Education Research Institute; Retrieved from: https://heri.ucla.edu/PDFs/NIH/Summer%20Report.PDF [Google Scholar]
- Hurtado S, Egan K, & Hughes B, (2012). Priming the pump or the sieve: Institutional contexts and URM STEM degree attainments Paper presented at the Annual Forum of the Association of Institutional Research, New Orleans, LA. [Google Scholar]
- Hurtado S, Eagan MK, & Chang M (2010). Degrees of success: Bachelor’s degree completion rates among initial STEM majors Higher Education Research Institute at UCLA; Retrieved from: https://www.heri.ucla.edu/nih/downloads/2010%20-%20Hurtado,%20Eagan,%20Chang%20-%20Degrees%20of%20Success.pdf. [Google Scholar]
- Hurtado S, Eagan MK, Cabrera NI, Lin MH, Park J, & Lopez M (2008) Training future scientists: Predicting first year minority participation in health sciences research. Research in Higher Education, 49, 126–152. doi: 10.1007/s11162-007-9068-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huang G, Taddese N, & Walter E (2000). Entry and persistence of women and minorities in college science and engineering education (No. NCES 2000601) Washington, D.C: National Center for Education Statistics. [Google Scholar]
- IBM Corp. Released 2015. IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 23.0 Armonk, NY: IBM Corp. [Google Scholar]
- Jöreskog KG (1967). A general approach to confirmatory maximum likelihood factor analysis. ETS Research Report Series, 1967(2), 183–202. [Google Scholar]
- Judd CM, McClelland GH, & Ryan CS (2009). Outliers and ill-mannered error. In Judd CM, McClelland GH, & Ryan CS (Eds.), Data analysis: A model comparison approach Data analysis: A model comparison approach, 2nd ed. (pp. 328). New York, NY: Routledge. [Google Scholar]
- Kelley WM, Wagner DD, & Heatherton TF (2015). In search of a human self-regulation system. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 38, 389–411. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-071013-014243 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kinkead J (2003). Learning through inquiry: An overview of undergraduate research. In Kinkead J (Ed.), Valuing and supporting undergraduate research. New Directions for Teaching and Learning, 93 (pp. 5–17). San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass. [Google Scholar]
- Kline RB (2016). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling (4 ed.). New York, NY: Guilford. [Google Scholar]
- Kohn-Wood LP, Hammond WP, Haynes TF, Ferguson KK, & Jackson BA (2012). Coping styles, depressive symptoms, and race during transition to adulthood. Mental Health, Religion, & Culture, 15, 263–372. doi: 10.1080/13674676.2011.577059 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Koole S (2009). The psychology of emotion regulation: An integrative review. Cognition & Emotion, 23, 4–41. doi: 10.1080/02699930802619031 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Kuiper NA, & Martin RA (1998). Is sense of humor a positive personality characteristics? In Ruch W (Ed.), The sense of humor: Explorations of personality characteristics (pp. 159–178). New York, NY: Mouton de Gruyter. [Google Scholar]
- Larson LM, Pesch KM, Surapaneni S, Bonitz VS, Wu TF, & Werbel JD (2015). Predicting graduation: the role of mathematics/science self-efficacy. Journal of Career Assessment, 23, 399–409. doi: 10.1177/1069072714547322 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Linn MC, Palmer E, Baranger A, Gerard E, & Stone E (2015). Undergraduate research experiences: Impacts and opportunities. Science, 347(6222) 399–409. doi: 10.1126/science.1261757 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luszczynska A, Diehl M, Gutiérrez-Doña B, Kuusinen P, & Schwarzer R (2004). Measuring one component of dispositional self-regulation: Attention control in goal pursuit. Personality and Individual Differences, 37, 555–566. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2003.09.026 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- MacPhee D, Farro S, & Canetto SS (2013). Academic self-efficacy and performance of underrepresented STEM majors: Gender, ethnic, and social class patterns. Analysis of Social Issues and Public Policy, 13, 347–369. doi: 10.1111/asap.12033 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Martin RA, & Lefcourt HM (1983). Sense of humor as a moderator of the relation between stressors and mood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 1313–1324. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.45.6.1313 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Maton KI, Hrabowski FA, & Schmitt CL (2000). African American college students excelling in the sciences: College and postcollege outcomes in the Meyerhoff scholars. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 37, 629–654. doi: 10.1002/1098-2736(200009)37 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- McGraw AP, & Warren C (2010). Benign violations: Making immoral behavior funny. Psychological Science, 21, 1141–1149. doi: 10.1177/0956797610376073 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Muthén LK, & Muthén BO (1998–2015). MPlus user’s guide (7th ed.). Los Angeles, CA: Muthen & Muthen. National Science Foundation. STEM Classification of Instructional Programs Crosswalk; Retrieved from: https://ncsesdata.nsf.gov/webcaspar/Help/dataMapHelpDisplay.jsp?subHeader=DataSourceBySubject&type=DS&abbr=DEGSRACE&noHeader=1 Accessed April 11, 2018. [Google Scholar]
- Pargament KI, Falb MD, Ano GG, & Wachholtz AB (2013). The religious dimension of coping: Advances in theory. research and practice. In Paloutzian RF & Park CL (Eds.), Handbook of the psychology of religion and spirituality, 2nd ed. (pp. 560–578). New York: Guilford. [Google Scholar]
- Pew Research Center (2010). Religion among the millennials. Pew Forum on Religion and Public Life Retrieved from: http://www.pewforum.org/2010/02/17/religion-among-the-millennials/ [Google Scholar]
- Pew Research Center (2012). “Nones” on the rise: One-in-five ddults have no religious affiliation. Pew Forum on Religion & Public Life Retrieved from: http://www.pewforum.org/2012/10/09/nones-on-the-rise/ [Google Scholar]
- Pinquart M, & Sörensen S (2000). Influences of socioeconomic status, social network, and competence on subjective well-being in later life: A meta-analysis. Psychology and Aging, 15, 187–224. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.15.2.187 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- President’s Council of Advisors on Science and Technology (PCAST) (2012). Engage to Excel: Producing One Million Additional College Graduates with Degrees in Science, Technology, Engineering, and Mathematics Washington, DC: U.S. Government Office of Science and Technology. [Google Scholar]
- Reevy GM, & Maslach C (2001). Use of social support: Gender and personality differences. Sex Roles, 44, 437–459. doi: 10.1023/A:1011930128829 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Robbins SB, Lauver K, Le H, Davis D, Langley R, & Carlstrom A (2004). Do psychosocial and study skill factors predict college outcomes? A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 261–288. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.130.2.261 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robbins SR, Casillas A, Peterson CH, & Le H (2006). Unraveling the differential effects of motivational and skills, social, and self-management measures from traditional predictors of college outcomes. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 598–616. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.98.3.598 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Schultz PW, Hernandez PR, Woodcock A, Estrada M, Chance RC, Aguilar M, & Serpe RT (2011). Patching the pipeline: Reducing educational disparities in the sciences through minority training programs. Educational Evaluation Policy Analysis, 33, 95–114. doi: 10.3102/0162373710392371 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Skinner EA, Edge K, Altman J, & Sherwood H (2003). Searching for the structure of coping: a review and critique of category systems for classifying ways of coping. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 216–269. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.129.2.216 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Skinner EA, & Zimmer-Gembeck MJ (2007). The development of coping. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 119–144. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085705 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sitzmann T, & Ely K (2011). A meta-analysis of self-regulated learning in work-related training and educational attainment: What we know and where we need to go. Psychological Bulletin, 137, 421–442. doi: 10.1037/a0022777 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Swail WS, Redd KE, & Perna LW (2003). Retaining minority students in higher education: A framework for success. ASHE-ERIC Higher Education Report, 30(2). U.S. Department of Education Retrieved from: http://www.educationalpolicy.org/pdf/Swail_Retention_Book.pdf [Google Scholar]
- Tangney JP, Baumeister RF, & Boone AL (2004). High self‐control predicts good adjustment, less pathology, better grades, and interpersonal success. Journal of Personality, 72, 271–324. doi: 10.1111/j.0022-3506.2004.00263.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thompson B (2004). Exploratory and confirmatory factor analysis: Understanding concepts and applications Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. [Google Scholar]
- Thorson JA, Powell FC, Sarmany-Schuller I, & Hampes WP (1997). Psychological health and sense of humor. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 53, 605–619. doi: ::AID-JCLP9>3.0.CO;2-I [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Velicer WF (1976). Determining the number of components from the matrix of partial correlations. Psychometrika, 41, 321–327. doi: 10.1007/BF02293557 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Velicer WF, Eaton CA, & Fava JL (2000). Construct explication through factor or component analysis: A review and evaluation of alternative procedures for determining the number of factors or components. In Goffin RD & Helmes E (Eds.), Problems and solutions in human assessment (pp. 41–71). Boston, MA: Kluwer. [Google Scholar]
- Vermetten YJ, Vermunt JD, & Lodewijks HG (1999). A longitudinal perspective on learning strategies in higher education: Different viewpoints towards development. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 69, 221–242. doi: 10.1348/000709999157699 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Webb TL, Miles E, & Sheeran P (2012). Dealing with feeling: A meta-analysis of the effectiveness of strategies derived from the process model of emotion regulation. Psychological Bulletin, 138, 775–808. doi: 10.1037/a0027600 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wechsler H, Dowdall GW, Davenport A, & Castillo S (1995). Correlates of college student binge drinking. American Journal of Public Health, 85, 921–926. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.85.7.921 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wills TA (1986). Stress and coping in early adolescence: Relationships to substance use in urban school samples. Health Psychology, 5, 503–529. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.5.6.503 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wolniak GC (2016). Examining STEM bachelor’s degree completion for students with differing propensities at college entry. Journal of College Student Retention: Research, Theory & Practice, 18, 287–309. doi: 10.1177/1521025115622782 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Wood W (2016). The role of habits in self-control. In Vohs KD & Baumeister RF (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation: Research, theory, and applications (3rd edition, pp. 95–108). New York, NY: Guilford. [Google Scholar]
- Zell AL, & Baumeister RF (2013). How religion can support self-control and moral behavior. In Paloutzian RF & Park CL (Eds.), Handbook of the psychology of religion and spirituality (pp. 498–516). New York, NY: Guilford. [Google Scholar]
- Zimmerman BJ (1990). Self-regulated learning and academic achievement: An overview. Educational Psychologist, 25, 3–17. doi: 10.1207/s15326985ep2501_2 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Zimmerman BJ, & Bandura A (1994). Impact of self-regulatory influences on attainment in a writing course. American Educational Research Journal, 29, 845–862. doi: 10.2307/1163397 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.