Abstract
Interacting with a cluttered and dynamic environment requires making decisions about visual information at relevant locations while ignoring irrelevant locations. Typical adults can do this with covert spatial attention: prioritizing particular visual field locations even without moving the eyes. Deficits of covert spatial attention have been implicated in developmental dyslexia, a specific reading disability. Previous studies of children with dyslexia, however, have been complicated by group differences in overall task ability that are difficult to distinguish from selective spatial attention. Here, we used a single-fixation visual search task to estimate orientation discrimination thresholds with and without an informative spatial cue in a large sample (N=123) of people ranging in age from 5 to 70 years and with a wide range of reading abilities. We assessed the efficiency of attentional selection via the cueing effect: the difference in log thresholds with and without the spatial cue. Across our whole sample, both absolute thresholds and the cueing effect gradually improved throughout childhood and adolescence. Compared to typical readers, individuals with dyslexia had higher thresholds (worse orientation discrimination) as well as smaller cueing effects (weaker attentional selection). Those differences in dyslexia were especially pronounced prior to age 20, when basic visual function is still maturing. Thus, in line with previous theories, literacy skills are associated with the development of selective spatial attention.
Keywords: spatial attention, visual perception, dyslexia, development
1. Introduction
1.1. Spatial attention and reading ability
Complex and cluttered environments pose a challenge when using vision to navigate and interact: only a subset of the information entering the eyes is relevant to the task at hand. Fortunately, the brain is equipped with multiple mechanisms - generally referred to as “attention” - that prioritize relevant information and filter out irrelevant information. Selective spatial attention prioritizes information at specific locations in the scene, and is critical for everyday tasks. A prominent example is reading: pages of text are extremely cluttered, and letters are only identifiable in the central visual field (Legge et al., 2007). Even within the central visual field, it is difficult to fully process more than one word at a time (White, Palmer, & Boynton, 2018). Therefore, reading requires rapid shifts of spatial attention to select individual words on a page (Rayner, 2009).
Given its importance in reading, one might hypothesize that a deficiency in spatial attention would cause difficulty in reading. In fact, many researchers have argued for an association between spatial attention and developmental dyslexia, a reading disability that affects 5–10% of the population (e.g., Bosse, Tainturier, & Valdois, 2007; Facoetti, Paganoni, Turatto, Marzola, & Mascetti, 2000; Franceschini, Gori, Ruffino, Pedrolli, & Facoetti, 2012; Vidyasagar & Pammer, 2010). Some authors link the attentional deficits to a more general abnormality in the “dorsal visual stream” (e.g., Pammer, Hansen, Holliday, & Cornelissen, 2006; Vidyasagar & Pammer, 1999), based on performance in tasks thought to rely on the magnocellular visual pathway (Boden & Giaschi, 2007; Demb, Boynton, Best, & Heeger, 1998; Demb, Boynton, & Heeger, 1997; Simone Gori, Seitz, Ronconi, Franceschini, & Facoetti, 2015; Kevan & Pammer, 2008). However, many other etiological explanations for dyslexia have been proposed, involving the auditory system, phonological processing, or higher-level language skills (reviewed by Vellutino, Fletcher, Snowling, & Scanlon, 2004).
The goal of the present investigation was to evaluate the link between spatial attention and reading ability over development. Methods for measuring spatial attention have been inconsistent across studies of children and adults with dyslexia. That is important because visual spatial attention comes in several forms: (1) Overt spatial attention involves movements of the head and eyes to align the high-resolution fovea with task-relevant objects. While reading this page, for example, you are making roughly four rapid eye movements per second (Rayner, 2009). (2) Covert spatial attention is the selective prioritization of locations in the visual field without moving the eyes. There are two types of covert spatial attention: (a) Endogenous attention is voluntary and driven by knowledge and goals. While fixating on the word “endogenous” in the previous sentence, your attention shifted to the next word, “attention”, before your eyes moved on (Rayner, 2009). (b) Exogenous attention is involuntary, triggered by salient external events that may or may not be task-relevant, like an email notification appearing in the corner of the screen while you try to read a manuscript.
Although there is some evidence for abnormal eye movement control (overt attention) in dyslexia (e.g., Eden, Stein, & Wood, 1994; Hawelka & Wimmer, 2005; Rayner, 1985), here we focus on covert spatial attention. During reading, endogenous covert attention is required to begin processing parafoveal words before the eyes move, to plan the saccades themselves, and perhaps even to shift attention between letters within fixated words (Rayner, 2009; Vidyasagar & Pammer, 2010).
In the laboratory, covert spatial attention is often studied by requiring an observer to fixate and respond to target stimuli presented in the peripheral visual field. Prior to the stimuli, a cue directs attention to one or more locations (Posner, 1980). A cue could be an arrow that points to the location most likely to contain task-relevant stimuli, or a small shape that draws attention by flashing near a potential stimulus location. The effects of covert spatial attention include increased discrimination accuracy and decreased response time when the target stimulus’s location is cued compared to uncued (reviewed by Carrasco, 2011). Physiologically, neural responses are greater for stimuli at cued locations than at uncued locations (Maunsell, 2015; Reynolds & Chelazzi, 2004).
In this study, we are interested in how covert spatial attention differs between individuals with and without dyslexia, in childhood as well as in adulthood. Because our goal is to measure visual task performance across a wide age range, we must first consider more general developmental changes in visual perception and attention.
1.2. The development of covert spatial attention
Since the 1980s, psychologists have studied the development of covert spatial attention using spatial cueing paradigms (Akhtar & Enns, 1989; Brodeur & Boden, 2000; Brodeur & Enns, 1997; Enns & Brodeur, 1989; Iarocci, Enns, Randolph, & Burack, 2009; Leclercq & Sieroff, 2013; Pearson & Lane, 1990; Plude, Enns, & Brodeur, 1994; Ristic & Enns, 2015; Ristic & Kingstone, 2009; Wainwright & Bryson, 2002). Nearly all such studies assess spatial attention by comparing reaction times (RTs) across different cue conditions (e.g., valid vs. invalid). The question is how the differences in RT, which index attention effects, change across development. There is a general consensus that exogenous (automatic, stimulus-driven) cueing effects are present from at least pre-school age and are stable through the lifespan. Endogenous (voluntary, top-down) cueing effects show more gradual developmental change, suggesting an increase in strategic control over spatial attention.
Beyond that, there is little agreement on the details of the time course and which internal mechanisms are changing. Some studies claim that endogenous attention becomes “adult-like” by age 10 (Goldberg, Maurer, & Lewis, 2001; Landry, Johnson, Fleming, Crewther, & Chouinard, 2019; Michael, Lete, & Ducrot, 2013; Ristic & Enns, 2015; Wainwright & Bryson, 2005), but others suggest later maturation (Brodeur & Enns, 1997; Leclercq & Sieroff, 2013; Schul, Townsend, & Stiles, 2003).
A general challenge in this type of study is to separate developmental change in a specific mechanism of attention from developmental change in overall task performance. Younger participants tend to respond slower and less accurately to the same stimulus as older participants. Does a valid-invalid cueing effect of 50 ms reflect the same degree of attentional modulation in a 5-year-old as it does in a 20-year old? It is difficult to say, especially if their accuracies differ. Several previous studies have attempted to address this difficulty, for instance by normalizing RTs across age groups (Gaspelin, Margett-Jordan, & Ruthruff, 2015; Goldberg et al., 2001). It remains an open question, however, whether the mechanisms of selective spatial attention develop independently of basic visual sensitivity and task performance.
Moreover, cueing effects on reaction times could have many underlying causes: change in the quality of the sensory representation, the speed of evidence accumulation, response bias, and/or motor preparation. Only a handful of developmental studies have focused on how spatial cues affect detection or discrimination accuracy (Akhtar & Enns, 1989; Brodeur & Boden, 2000; Schul et al., 2003), which can give more information about the underlying mechanisms. Even then, it is difficult to compare cueing effects in units of proportion correct across groups that differ in their absolute levels of accuracy. The conclusion depends on an interaction between cue condition and age. Such interactions are difficult to interpret if the measurement (e.g., differences in proportion correct) may not be linearly related to the theoretical variable of interest (e.g., the effect of attention on visual processing; Loftus, 1978).
1.3. A paradigm to study visual attention in dyslexia and across development
Studies that compare spatial attention across good and poor readers face the same challenges as studies that compare across age groups. Many in the past have not precisely specified what differs in dyslexic individuals: overall visual sensitivity, motor ability, overt eye movements, or endogenous covert selection, etc. Many rely on RTs, and the participant groups often differ in their overall performance level (e.g., people with dyslexia can have slow processing speed in general; Peterson & Pennington, 2015) as well as any putative attention effects.
One research team overcame those challenges by measuring orientation discrimination thresholds in a simple visual search task with and without an informative spatial pre-cue (Roach & Hogben, 2004, 2007, 2008). The participants’ task was to report the tilt direction (clockwise or counterclockwise from vertical) of a single Gabor stimulus that was presented along with a variable number of vertical distractors, all equidistant from fixation (Baldassi & Burr, 2000). The display was presented briefly enough to avoid eye movements to the stimuli, and the target’s location varied randomly from trial to trial. On some trials, a 100% valid spatial cue (a small dot) flashed near the target’s location, just prior to the stimulus array. In each condition, the experimenters used a staircase procedure to estimate each participant’s orientation discrimination threshold: the degrees of tilt required to perform the task with ~75% correct. Without the spatial cue, thresholds rise with increasing set size, because the internal representation of each stimulus is noisy and each distractor could be mistaken for the target. With the spatial cue, thresholds in control participants are much lower (better performance) and less affected by increasing set size. This difference in threshold represents the benefit of spatial attention: the cue allows the participant to base their decision primarily on information at the target’s location and exclude noise from the distractors (Roach & Hogben, 2007, 2008).
Roach & Hogben found that although adults with dyslexia performed normally in the uncued condition, their thresholds were abnormally high (worse performance compared to control subjects) in the cued condition. In other words, adults with dyslexia failed to capitalize on the information conveyed by the cue to reduce uncertainty about the target’s location. Thresholds in the cued condition distinguished adults with dyslexia from controls better than a variety of other psychophysical and widely used clinical measures (Roach & Hogben, 2007). The difference between good and poor readers was strongest when the set size was largest (16 items). Importantly, the stimuli were not linguistic, which avoided a potential confound in comparing performance between good and poor readers. Overall, Roach & Hogben provided some of the strongest evidence to date that adults with dyslexia have an impairment in covert spatial attention.
One unusual feature of this paradigm is that the peripheral cue could potentially trigger both exogenous and endogenous spatial attention. The cue could be exogenous because it appears peripherally, adjacent to and immediately before the target. It could be endogenous because it always indicates the target’s location, so the participant can use that information to voluntarily select the most relevant information. Based on a series of additional experiments, Roach & Hogben (2008) concluded that the primary mechanism of the cueing effect in this paradigm is endogenous. We therefore chose to use this paradigm because the interval between the cue and the stimuli is short enough to prevent eye movement to the target, a potential pitfall when studying covert attention in young children. We will return to the endogenous vs. exogenous question in the Discussion.
1.4. The present study
We adapted and extended Roach & Hogben’s (2007) method to study the development of covert spatial attention and how it differs in children and adults with dyslexia. Although Roach & Hogben focused on set size 16, we used a fixed set size of 8 items in the cued and uncued conditions. With a larger set size, performance may be limited by crowding, which is also known to differ in dyslexia (Callens, Whitney, Tops, & Brysbaert, 2013; Cassim, Talcott, & Moores, 2014; S Gori & Facoetti, 2015; Joo, White, Strodtman, & Yeatman, 2018; Moores, Cassim, & Talcott, 2011). In addition, we increased the size and duration of the peripheral cue and made it red, so it would be salient enough for younger participants. In a subset of adult participants we also replicated a condition with the small black cue used by Roach & Hogben (2004, 2007). Finally, we added a “single stimulus” condition, in which the tilted target is presented alone.
There are advantages to measuring thresholds, which are “stimulus-referred” measurements, rather than reaction time or accuracy with a fixed stimulus. First, we set the stimulus difficulty on each trial with an adaptive staircase that converged on the orientation difference that yields ~75% correct performance. The level of overall task difficulty was therefore equalized across participants, regardless of age or reading skill. Second, we fit psychometric functions with separate parameters for the threshold and the upper asymptote. The latter parameter can capture differences the participants’ abilities to stay engaged and follow instructions. Third, we operationalized the effect of attention as the difference in log thresholds between cued and uncued conditions, which can be theoretically linked to a difference in the noise of the internal representations used to make the perceptual decision (see the Supplemental Material). That helps us interpret the interactions between cue condition and age or reading ability. Finally, thresholds in the single stimulus condition provide a baseline measure of group differences in the ability to make fine visual discriminations, independent of attention.
2. Materials and Methods
We report how we determined our sample size, all data exclusions, all inclusion/exclusion criteria, whether inclusion/exclusion criteria were established prior to data analysis, all manipulations, and all measures in the study.
2.1. Participants
All participants had normal or corrected-to-normal vision and gave written informed consent in accordance with the University of Washington Institutional Review Board. Flyers solicited participants with and without dyslexia, on campus and through local organizations that provide support to people with dyslexia, including the University of Washington Disability Resources for Students (DRS), Disabilities, Opportunities Internetworking and Technology (DO-IT), and Dyslexic Advantage (http://www.dyslexicadvantage.org/). Compensation was a fixed monetary payment.
We recruited a total of 131 participants (68 male) between the ages of 5 and 70 years. The sample size was guided by Roach & Hogben (2007), who used a similar design. We used their data to simulate an ROC analysis of cued thresholds with set size 8, and estimated the sample size required for an 80% probability of detecting a significant difference between the dyslexia group and the control group. That minimal sample size was 12 for each group. Seeking at least that many participants in each group, divided by reading ability and age bin (below and above 20 years), we recruited as many participants as possible in the time window available for the study. Of the 131 participants recruited, two chose to discontinue after only a few practice blocks. Three additional participants were excluded because they reported having an uncorrected vision problem, including amblyopia (criterion determined in advance). Of the remaining 126 participants, 3 children (ages 7–8 years) were excluded because in all conditions of the experiment their accuracy was not significantly above chance (criterion determined after data analysis). That suggested they were not engaged in the task or following instructions (more details in the Analysis section below). The final sample included 123 participants (64 male). Of those, 75 reported having a diagnosis of dyslexia, and 22 reported a diagnosis of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), which commonly co-occurs with dyslexia.
Each participant also completed a battery of psychometric tests. Following Roach & Hogben (2007), our primary measure of reading ability was the TOWRE-II Phonemic Decoding Efficiency (PDE) test, which requires speeded reading of pronounceable pseudowords (Torgesen, Rashotte, & Wagner, 1999). Participants also completed the TOWRE-II Sight Word Efficiency sub-test (SWE; speeded reading of real words), and the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI-III).
To examine the relationship between attention and reading skills, we divided our sample into two groups using a definition consistent with previous work (Roach & Hogben, 2007): individuals with dyslexia (DYS), and individuals with typical reading skills (control participants; CON). Participants in the DYS group (N=46) had a TOWRE PDE score <= 85 and either reported a history of reading difficulty or a diagnosis of dyslexia. A PDE score of 85 is 1 standard deviation below the age-adjusted norm of 100. Participants in the CON group (N=37) had a TOWRE PDE score > 85 and no diagnosis of dyslexia. This left out a third set of participants (“Neither”, N=40), all but one of whom were diagnosed with dyslexia but a PDE score in the normal range. Demographics and psychonomic data are reported in Table 1.
Table 1.
Group | Total N | N males | N ADHD Diagnosis | N Dyslexia Diagnosis | WASI Matrix Reasoning | WASI Full- scale 2 | TOWRE PDE | TOWRE SWE |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
DYS | 46 | 27 | 7 | 36 | 50.0 (12.3) | 102.4 (15.8) | 75.7 (6.8) | 78.6 (11.5) |
CON | 37 | 19 | 5 | 0 | 55.9 (9.7) | 114.6 (13.6) | 104.4 (11.3) | 101.5 (12.1) |
Neither | 40 | 18 | 10 | 39 | 55.4 (8.1) | 112.9 (12.1) | 95.4 (8.7) | 92.6 (12.1) |
The DYS group differed significantly from the CON group in both total IQ (WASI Full-Scale 2: t(78) = 3.66, p=0.0005) and non-verbal IQ (WASI Matrix Reasoning: t(78) = 2.35, p=0.021. Given that the full-scale IQ assessment includes a verbal component, it is not surprising that individuals with dyslexia scored lower. To be certain that any differences in our task performance were not confounded by non-verbal IQ differences, we included the Matrix Reasoning score as a covariate in our analyses of group differences. We use phonemic decoding (TOWRE PDE) as our primary measure of reading ability, to be consistent with a prior study (Roach & Hogben, 2007). In the Supplemental Material we present analogous results using the sight word efficiency (SWE) score instead.
A subset of the data from adult participants has been reported in a previous publication (Joo et al., 2018).
2.2. Equipment and Stimuli
We generated stimuli using MATLAB (The Mathworks Corporation, Natick, MA, USA) and the Psychophysics Toolbox (Brainard, 1997; Pelli, 1997) on a Linux PC (Mint Mate, version 17). We used an LG liquid crystal display (1920 * 1080 resolution, 120 Hz refresh rate) that subtended 51 degrees of visual angle (°) horizontally. The participant sat at a chinrest 53 cm from the monitor, and used two keys (the down arrow and the right arrow) to make the response. The keys were re-labeled with stickers to indicate the leftmost key was for reporting targets tilted counterclockwise of vertical, and the rightmost key for clockwise of vertical. The surrounding keys on the keyboard were covered with cardboard.
The screen background was set to medium gray. A black fixation cross (0.3°×0.3°) was present at all times except when feedback was given (see below). The target stimuli (Figure 1) were Gabor patches: 50% contrast sinusoidal gratings (2 cycles/°) windowed by a 2D Gaussian envelope (SD = 0.28°).
There were 3 conditions of the experiment: a Single Stimulus condition, and two conditions with eight stimuli: Uncued and Cued. The Cue was a bright red (full saturation) disk 0.6° in diameter presented at the same polar angle as the target, 3° from fixation (2° from the center of the target).
Participants over the age of 14 were also tested in a condition with a smaller cue designed to match Roach & Hogben (2007). The small cue was a black disk 0.18° in diameter, presented 3.88° from fixation (1.12° from the target) for only 17 ms. Results using this smaller cue were largely consistent as when cued with the bright red cue and are presented in the Supplemental Material.
2.3. Trial sequence and procedure
All trials began with an inter-trial interval, during which a fixation cross was displayed on a gray background (Figure 1). On the first trial of each block, this fixation interval lasted 1333 ms; in all subsequent trials it lasted 583 ms. Next was a 33 ms cue interval which was blank for the Uncued and Single Stimulus conditions and contained the large red cue in the Cued condition. Immediately after the cue interval, the stimulus array was presented for 83 ms. In all conditions except the Single Stimulus condition (see below), 8 Gabors were presented simultaneously, arranged equidistant from each other in a circle around fixation with radius 5°. Seven of the Gabors were oriented vertically (the distractors); one Gabor (the target) was tilted either clockwise or counterclockwise (50% probability).
The degree of target tilt was controlled by a staircase procedure (see below). The position of the target in the array was chosen randomly on each trial. In the Single Stimulus condition, the target was presented at a random one of the 8 positions. with no distractors. After the stimulus array, the observer reported the target’s tilt direction by pressing one of the two response keys. Response time was unlimited, and accuracy was emphasized. Immediately after the response, a 75 ms feedback tone was presented: a high-pitched (600 Hz) or low-pitched (180 Hz) tone for correct or incorrect responses, respectively. At the same time, visual feedback appeared at the screen center, in the form of the number of “points” won: if the response was correct, “+3” was displayed in green text; otherwise, “+0” in red text. The visual feedback was presented in 25 pt Arial font for 750 ms, after which the next inter-trial interval began.
The participant was instructed that the goal was to win as many points as possible, and that a total of 700 points was needed to “win the game.” At the end of each block, the total number of points won that block was displayed, as well as the total number of points gained so far in the session, along with the motivational phrase: “Great job! How many more can you get?”
Trials were conducted in blocks of 52†. During 48 of those trials, the degree of the target’s tilt was controlled by a weighted 1-up/1-down staircase procedure controlled by the Palamedes toolbox (Prins & Kingdom, 2009). The step size up was set to log10(4°) at the start of each staircase, and then halved after four staircase reversals. The step size down was always one third of the step size up, which makes the staircase converge on the 75% correct threshold. The tilt was free to vary between 0.1° and 25°. On a random 4 trials in each block, the target’s tilt was set to twice the current threshold estimate and the staircase was not updated. We included these “easy” trials to improve our estimate of the upper asymptote of the psychometric function and provide some relief to untrained observers who may find it difficult to perform at threshold for an extended period of time.
The condition was constant for each block of trials, and was announced before the block began with text on the screen: for Uncued, “There will be no dot, so you have to find the tilted stripes on your own.”; for Cued, “The red dot will appear near the tilted stripes.”; for Small Cue, “The small black dot will appear near the tilted stripes.”; for Single Stimulus, “There will only be one patch of stripes at a time.”
Over the course of data collection, we used three slightly different versions of the experiment that differ primarily in which conditions were included. All versions included the Cued and Uncued conditions. Version 2 was used in some of youngest participants and excluded the Single Stimulus condition. All participants 14 or older were tested in Version 3, which included the Small Cue condition. See the Supplemental Material for more detail.
Each participant was first trained in the task by viewing pictures of the stimuli. The task was introduced as a “game,” with the objective to find a tilted patch of stripes and report which way it is tilted. They practiced the single stimulus condition with easy tilt levels (8 trials per practice block). Then they practiced the uncued condition with increasingly difficult tilt levels until they could perform above chance with less than 15° tilt. Finally, they were introduced to the Cued condition. The instructions about the cue said, “In the next part of the game, we help you out and show a red dot next to the one patch of stripes that will be tilted. Your job will be to find that red dot, see the stripes that are tilted, and press the correct button. It’s important to note that in this game the red dot will ALWAYS be next to the stripes that are tilted and will appear just a split-second before the stripe patches.” For participants in Version 3, similar instructions followed about the Small Cue. Each session lasted roughly 25 minutes. No part of the study procedures or analyses was pre-registered prior to the research being conducted.
2.4. Analysis
Our primary measure was tilt thresholds: that is, the degree of tilt required for the participant to discriminate the direction of the target’s tilt with 75% correct accuracy, in each condition. To estimate thresholds we used a maximum likelihood procedure in the Palamedes toolbox (Prins & Kingdom, 2009) to fit the raw data in each condition with a log version of the Weibull function (also known as the Gumbel):
where F(x) is proportion correct, x is the tilt level in log10(degrees), γ is chance level (0.5), λ specifies the lapse rate (1 - the upper asymptote), β determines the slope of the curve, and α determines the location of the function along the x-axis. Fitting occurred in three stages: We first allowed the three parameters (β, λ and α) to vary freely, with the exception that λ was capped at 0.125. (If λ = 0.125, the subject guesses on 25% of trials, with p(correct) = 0.5). We then fixed the slope β to 1.77, which was the median of freely estimated slopes averaged across subjects and conditions (excluding poor fits with thresholds >45°) and refit the remaining two parameters, λ and α. Finally, we fit again by allowing only the parameter α to vary, with the parameter β still fixed to 1.77 and the lapse rate parameter λ fixed to the average of λ estimates across all conditions for that subject. Therefore, for each subject, only the parameter α varied across conditions, but λ could vary across subjects. After fitting, we computed the threshold as the tilt level t where the F(t) = 0.75. This procedure avoided over-fitting the data and improved reliability of threshold estimates.
We computed the effect of the cue on thresholds (the “cueing effect”) as the difference in t between the Uncued and Cued conditions, where t is in log10 units. This cueing effect indexes the observer’s ability to capitalize on the cue to process the target and filter out distractors. See the Supplemental Material for a computational model that justifies using the difference of log thresholds to measure the attention effect.
Finally, we estimated whether each participant was performing significantly above chance in each condition. For each condition, we estimated the 95% confidence interval of the binomial probability of a correct response. If that confidence interval included 0.5, then we concluded that the participant was not engaged in the task during those trials, and excluded that threshold from the analysis (but included other data from that subject). A total of 9 thresholds from 6 participants (ages 7–14) were excluded for this reason (3 Uncued, 5 Cued, and 1 Small Cue). The median of those excluded thresholds was 140°, and several were effectively infinite.
2.5. Reliability
78 participants completed a second session identical to the first, allowing us to compute test-retest reliability by correlating thresholds across the two sessions. With this smaller sample, the average reliability across conditions was r = 0.73. For the Uncued and Cued conditions (conditions for which we have the most data), r=0.75 and 0.82, respectively. For the cueing effect, r=0.46.
3. Results
3.1. Development of thresholds and spatial cueing effects
Before analyzing the effect of reading ability on task performance, we model the developmental time-course of visual spatial attention skills. We can then use this model to control for age in the analysis of reading ability. Figure 2A shows individual subject thresholds as a function of age in three main conditions: Uncued, Cued, and Single Stimulus. Lower thresholds imply better orientation discrimination. Overall, thresholds start out high in childhood, decrease through adolescence, and then plateau. We fit each condition with a piecewise linear function that assumes that thresholds (t) are a linear function of age (A) with slope s and y-intercept b, up until an inflection point c after which there is no more change with age. Specifically:
We used a least-squares cost function and a leave-one-out cross-validation to assess model fit. The piecewise model fit the data significantly better than a simple linear regression model (constant slope across the age range). For the simple linear model, cross-validated R2 in each condition (Uncued, Cued, and Single stimulus) was: 0.132, 0.118, and 0.070, respectively. R2 values were higher for the piecewise linear model: 0.382, 0.307, and 0.217. In addition to the superior fit to the data, the piecewise model provides an estimate of the age at which performance matures.
The least-squares fits are plotted as solid lines in Figure 2A, and the corresponding values of s, b, and c are listed in Table 2 along with 95% bootstrapped confidence intervals (CIs). Each CI is the 2.5th and 97.5th percentile on the bootstrapped distribution of parameter estimates (5000 independent samples with replacement). In all three conditions, the initial slope was significantly negative: the 95% CIs excluded 0. The inflection point c for all 3 conditions was between 18 and 20 years.
Table 2:
Condition | Slope s log10(deg)/year | Intercept b log10(deg) | Inflection c (years) |
---|---|---|---|
Uncued | −0.034 [−0.077 –0.023] | 1.41 [1.25 1.81] | 18.8 [13.4 20.4] |
Cued | −0.054 [−0.170 –0.036] | 1.39 [1.11 2450] | 18.9 [12.5 20.6] |
Single Stim. | −0.040 [−0.075 –0.025] | 0.98 [0.72 1.39] | 19.9 [16.3 22.8] |
The fitted functions reveal large differences in thresholds between the three conditions at the asymptotic level (that is, in adulthood). The 95% CIs were all non-overlapping for all three conditions. The large increase in thresholds from the Single Stimulus condition (set size 1) to the Uncued condition (set size 8) reflects the effect of adding the 7 vertical distractors to the target display. The distractors add noise to the orientation estimation process and may tax limited processing resources. The reduction in thresholds from the Uncued to the Cued condition is the effect of covert spatial attention. The mean cued threshold was 59% of the way between the Uncued threshold and the lower bound represented by the Single Stimulus threshold. Thus, the cue allows the typical adult to filter out much of the noise added by the distractors, but not all of it (Roach & Hogben, 2007).
To compare the rate of developmental change across conditions, we computed 95% bootstrapped CIs on the difference in slope parameters. The slope of developmental change in the Cued condition was 59% greater than in the Uncued condition, and the 95% CI of differences excluded zero: [0.001 0.119]. The slope in the Cued condition was 35% greater than in the Single Stimulus condition, but the CI included zero: [−0.012 0.15]. The slopes in the Uncued and Single Stimulus conditions did not significantly differ: CI = [−0.020 0.030].
To directly assess the development of spatial attention we analyzed the difference in log thresholds between the Uncued and Cued conditions (i.e., the cueing effect). Figure 2B plots the cueing effect for each participant as a function of age. (Note that this analysis includes 117 participants, excluding 6 who did not perform above chance in either the Cued or Uncued conditions). We fit those data with the same piecewise linear function as in Figure 2A. The best-fitting slope was 0.020 log10(deg)/year (95% CI = [0.008 0.166]), indicating a significant developmental increase in the cueing effect, above and beyond the change observed in each individual condition. The inflection point was 19.2 years (CI = [10.2 22.8]).
In sum, the developmental analysis of orientation discrimination thresholds revealed two patterns: (1) A large improvement in orientation discrimination ability, as measured in the Single Stimulus condition, from age 5 to about 20. The best-fitting developmental function predicts a threshold of 6.1° at age 5, and a threshold of 1.5° after age 20. (2) A difference in thresholds between the Cued and Uncued conditions that also increases with age up until about 20 years. Therefore, in addition to the overall improvement in visual sensitivity with age, there is also an improvement in the attentional selection of information from relevant peripheral visual field locations. However, as shown in the next section, the developmental increase in the cueing effect was primarily driven by participants with poor reading ability.
3.2. Comparison of spatial attention in good and poor readers
3.2.1. Thresholds in individual conditions
We next evaluated the association between reading ability and orientation discrimination thresholds, and between reading ability and the cueing effect. We took two complementary approaches: (1) comparing two sub-groups of our sample, those with and without dyslexia; (2) correlating the TOWRE PDE score with threshold measurements in the whole sample. The first group comparison approach is typical in the literature and was used in the prior study of adults that reported group differences in this task (Roach & Hogben, 2007). Here we examine whether those differences between good and poor readers are also present in children, before performance in this task matures. The second approach treats reading ability as a continuum (Shaywitz, Escobar, Shaywitz, Fletcher, & Makuch, 1992) and includes all participants.
Following the first approach, we defined two sub-groups of our sample: The “DYS” group included all individuals who had a PDE score <= 85 and either reported a history of reading difficulty or a diagnosis of dyslexia. The control group, “CON”, included all individuals with a PDE score > 85 and no diagnosis of dyslexia. This left out roughly 1/3 of our participants, who indicated a diagnosis of dyslexia but had a PDE score in the normal range. We further divided the DYS and CON groups into two age bins: 5–19 years and 20–70: based on the developmental patterns described above, the first age bin is in the range when orientation discrimination thresholds as well as cueing effects are still maturing. Table 3 reports the number of subjects in each age bin and reading ability group.
Table 3:
Ages | DYS | CON | Neither |
---|---|---|---|
5–19 | 31 | 18 | 24 |
20–70 | 15 | 19 | 16 |
The mean thresholds in each group are plotted in Figure 3, left column. In the younger age bin, thresholds are higher in the DYS group that the CON group, especially in the Cued condition. To analyze these differences in thresholds in the younger age bin we fit a LME model with fixed effects of condition and reading ability group, and a random effect of subject. We also included fixed effects of age, non-verbal IQ (matrix reasoning score), and ADHD diagnosis as covariates. Consistent with the developmental patterns reported above, the effect of age was significant (F(1,114)=12.3, p=0.0007). ADHD diagnosis had no significant effects or interactions (p>0.40). Higher non-verbal IQ was associated with lower thresholds overall (F(1,114)=3.59, p=0.061), but that did not interact with condition (F(2,114)=1.09, p=0.34). There was a large main effect of condition on thresholds (F(2,114)=32.8, p<10−11). On average, thresholds in the DYS group were 0.13 log10(deg) higher than in the CON group, but the main effect of reading ability group was not significant (F(1,114)=2.38, p=0.126). However, there was an interaction between condition and reading group (F(2,114)=3.56, p=0.032). As seen in the left column of Figure 3, the relative elevation of thresholds DYS group under 20 years old was greatest in the Cued condition.
The same analyses of thresholds within the adult age bin (over age 20) found no effect of age (F<1), consistent with the conclusion above that thresholds asymptote after age 20. ADHD diagnosis had no effect (F<1), but there was a main effect of non-verbal IQ (F(1,78)=18.5, p<10−4). Thresholds differed significantly across conditions (F(2,78)=36.1, p<10−11), but there was no main effect of reading ability group (F(1,78)=0.36, p=0.55; mean difference = 0.03). There was, however, an interaction between condition and reading group (F(2,78)=4.15, p=0.019).
The bar plots in the left column of Figure 3 suggest that the threshold difference between the DYS and CON groups is larger in participants below age 20 than in participants above age 20, at least for the Cued and Uncued conditions. Children with dyslexia may become more similar to control participants as they reach adulthood, in terms of visual search performance. Such a hypothesis predicts an interaction between age bin and reading ability group on thresholds. Controlling for ADHD diagnosis, non-verbal IQ, and age within each group, that interaction was significant in the Cued condition (F(1,68) = 4.34, p=0.041). The interaction was not significant in either the Uncued condition (F(1,70)=2.12, p=0.15) or the Single Stimulus condition (F(1,57)=0.33, p=0.57). To gain more power, we conducted a similar analysis but including all participants and using the TOWRE PDE score as a continuous measure of reading ability. The interaction between reading ability and age bin was significant for both the Uncued (F(1,106)=5.50, p=0.021) and Cued (F(1,105)=7.77, p=0.006) conditions, but not the Single Stimulus (F(1,91)=0.90, p=0.34). Therefore, for both conditions with set size 8, the relative impairment in poor readers becomes less severe after age 20.
In the next analysis, we ask how reading ability can explain the residual variance in thresholds (and cueing effects) that was not explained by age in the developmental model shown in Figure 2. This controls for the effect of age while maximizing statistical power to estimate the effect of reading ability by using the entire sample. In the middle column of Figure 3 we plot the relation between each subject’s reading score and the residuals from the piecewise linear models of thresholds as a function of age for each condition. The residuals are the differences between each subject’s threshold and the prediction of the developmental model. Each subject’s reading group (DYS, CON or Neither) is indicated by the color of the dot. In all three conditions (Uncued, Cued, and Single Stimulus), there was a significant negative correlation between reading score and threshold residuals. This means that better readers are likely to have lower thresholds compared to poor readers of the same age. Therefore, not all of the unexplained variance by the developmental model in Figure 2A is just noise; some of it is explained by reading ability. We further analyzed these residuals with a linear model that included reading score (TOWRE PDE), ADHD diagnosis, and non-verbal IQ. ADHD had no effects in any condition, but higher non-verbal IQ was associated with lower thresholds in the Uncued (slope=−0.005; t(108)=2.50, p=0.014) and Single Stimulus (slope=−0.009; t(91)=3.15, p=0.002) conditions. The effect of reading score was significant in the Cued condition (slope=−0.004; t(109)=2.02, p=0.046), but not in the Uncued (slope=−0.001, t(110)=1.05, p=0.30) or and Single Stimulus conditions (slope=−0.003, t(95)=1.58, p=0.12). The solid lines in the middle column of Figure 3 are the predictions based on reading score alone.
We conducted another analysis to examine how well these psychophysical measures distinguish dyslexic from control participants, as suggested by Roach & Hogben (2007). Specifically, we conducted a Receiver Operating Characteristic (ROC) analysis of the distributions of threshold residuals. The area under the ROC curve (AUC) is a measure of how accurately we can classify subjects into the DYS or CON groups, ranging from 0.5 (chance) to 1.0 (perfect).
In the right column of Figure 3, we plot smoothed distributions of residuals (same data as in the middle column) for the two groups (DYS in white, CON in the darker color). To evaluate whether each AUC was significantly higher than predicted by chance, we performed a permutation test to establish a null distribution: on each of 5000 repetitions, we randomly shuffled the group labels for all subjects and re-computed the AUC. The actual AUC exceeded the 95% CI on this null distribution in the Cued condition: AUC = 0.66 (null CI = [0.37 0.63]). As seen in Figure 3, the two groups’ distributions in the Cued condition greatly overlapped, with a rather subtle upward shift for the DYS group (mean = 0.10 ± 0.06) compared to the CON group (−0.08 ±0.04). AUC was not significantly above chance in Uncued (0.53, null CI = [0.37 0.63]) or Single Stimulus conditions (0.62, [0.36 0.64]).
In summary, poor readers have higher orientation discrimination thresholds than good readers, especially in the Cued condition. Moreover, in the Cued condition, the difference between good and poor readers is greater before age 20 than in adulthood. That interaction between reading ability and age is less robust in the Uncued condition and appears to be absent in the Single Stimulus condition.
3.2.2. The cueing effect on thresholds
To specifically evaluate spatial attention, we analyze the relation between reading ability and the cueing effect. Figure 4A plots the mean cueing effects for the DYS and CON groups, for those under and above 20 years of age (as in Figure 3). For each age bin, we analyzed the cueing effect with a linear model that included reading group, age, ADHD diagnosis, and non-verbal IQ as predictors. In the younger age bin, the model estimated that the cueing effect is 0.22 log10(deg) greater in the CON group than in the DYS group (t(41) = 2.17, p=0.036). The effect of age was not significant (t(41)=1.67, p=0.10), and there were no effects of ADHD diagnosis or non-verbal IQ (both t<1). Therefore, individuals under 20 with dyslexia have (on average) weaker attentional selection than those without dyslexia.
In the adult age bin (over 20), a similar linear model estimated that the cueing effect was 0.06 log10(deg) larger in the DYS group than the CON groups, but was not statistically reliable (t(26) = 1.04, p=0.31). The effect of age (t(26)=1.50, p=0.15) and non-verbal IQ (t(26)=1.86, p=0.074) also were weak. There was a reliable effect of ADHD diagnosis: the model estimated that adults with ADHD have a cueing effect 0.18 log10(deg) smaller than adults without ADHD (t(26)=2.15, p=0.041. However, note that this analysis only included 4 adults with ADHD.
Altogether, the data in Figure 4A show that children with dyslexia have a markedly smaller spatial cueing effect than typically-reading children, but adults with dyslexia have a cueing effect that is only slightly smaller than typical. That suggests that as individuals with dyslexia mature, they recover some spatial attention function; i.e., the differences in childhood reflect a developmental delay rather than a deficit per se. We urge caution in interpreting that result, however, because it is not supported by a statistical interaction between reading ability group and age bin. Specifically, we fit a linear model of the Uncued-Cued log threshold difference, with factors age bin (under 20; over 20), reading group (DYS, CON), and their interaction. We also include covariates of the normalized (mean 0) age within each age group, ADHD diagnosis, and non-verbal IQ. The cueing effect was smaller in the DYS group (F(1,69)=7.72, p=0.007). However, the interaction between age bin and reading group was not significant (F(1,69)=2.32, p=0.132), nor were any of the other main effects. To maximize power, we conducted a similar analysis but using all participants and using the TOWRE PDE score as a continuous measure (normalized to mean 0 within each age bin), rather than dividing by reading group. Consistent with the developmental pattern reported above, there was a significant effect of age bin (F(1,105) = 6.95, p=0.010). No other factor had a significant effect except reading score (F(1,105)=4.58, p=0.035). Again, the effect of reading score did not interact with age bin (F(1,105)=2.23, p=0.138).
Finally, we examined how the residuals of the cueing effect from the developmental model in Figure 2C relates to reading ability. Those residuals, for all participants, are plotted as a function of reading score in Figure 4B. The correlation was positive but not significant (r=0.15, p=0.10). The solid line is the prediction of a linear model that also included predictors for non-verbal IQ and ADHD. None of those factors were significant, including reading score (slope=0.002; t(108)=1.51, p=0.134). However, the ROC analysis yielded a significant difference in residuals between just the DYS and CON groups (Figure 4C). The AUC was 0.64 (null CI = [0.37 0.63]). The groups significantly differ even when controlling for ADHD and non-verbal IQ: the mean residual for DYS was −0.075 ± 0.04; and for CON it was 0.069 ± 0.03 (t(73) = 2.47, p=0.016). Again, the effect is subtle and the two distributions largely overlap.
In summary, we found that individuals with dyslexia benefit less from the cue than control participants, in line with an impairment of selective covert spatial attention. Previous studies have only reported such differences in selective attention for adults with dyslexia (Roach & Hogben, 2007). In our data, the difference associated with dyslexia actually appears stronger in people under 20. However, the interaction between age group and reading score on cueing effects was not statistically significant (p>0.05).
3.2.3. Analyses in a sliding age window
Given that there were significant interactions between reading ability and age bin in some conditions (Figure 3), we sought a finer-grained analysis of developmental changes in the effect of reading score on task performance. Figure 5 shows the association between reading score and residuals from the piecewise linear developmental models computed in a sliding window across the age range. At each step, the window included 40 participants (1/3 of the sample with usable thresholds). Using the threshold residuals controls for the effect of age per se. At each step of the window, we analyzed the Cued and Uncued conditions as well as the cueing effect, with the same three analyses as in the middle and right columns of Figures 3 and 4:
We fit the residuals with a linear model that had three predictors: TOWRE PDE reading score, ADHD diagnosis, and non-verbal IQ. In Figure 5A we plot the estimated slope relating reading score to the residuals, as a function of the median age in the window. Dots indicate points when the slope was significantly different from 0, correcting for multiple comparisons (false discovery rate = 0.05; Benjamini & Hochberg, 1995). There were significantly negative slopes for the Cued condition between median ages 10 and 20 years. Negative slopes mean better readers have lower thresholds (better sensitivity) than predicted by age alone. Within that range, the slope for the Cued condition was roughly twice the slope for the Uncued condition, indicating that good and poor readers differ in how well they can use the cue. After about age 20, the slopes approach 0. The slope relating the cueing effect to reading ability starts out positive and then declines after age 20, suggesting that better readers have larger cueing effects in late childhood and adolescence. However, the slopes for cueing effects were not significant at any individual time point.
We also computed the linear correlation between TOWRE PDE and the residuals in each time window (Figure 5B). The correlation coefficients show a similar pattern as the linear model slopes, but are roughly equivalent for the Cued and Uncued conditions, even before age 20. Therefore, reading ability accounts for the same amount of variance in thresholds for those two conditions, but the difference between good and poor readers is greater in the Cued condition (as reflected by the slopes in Fig. 5A).
Finally, within each time window we computed the area under the ROC curve that discriminates the DYS and CON groups (Figure 5C). Unlike the first two analyses, this takes into account dyslexia diagnosis and history of reading difficulty, and excludes the set of participants who fall into neither group. The Cued condition was significantly above chance between the median ages of 10 and 20, and remained at or above 0.85 between 11 and 19. The Uncued condition was not significantly above chance at any time-point. The cueing effect, however, was significantly above chance between median ages of 10 and 19, reaching a peak value of 0.84 at 16. Therefore, there may be a special link between spatial attention and dyslexia per se (Fig. 5C) that is not captured as cleanly by correlating the cueing effect with a single measure of reading ability (the TOWRE PDE score, as used in Figs. 5A and 5B).
This data-driven analysis confirms that the division into two age bins at age 20, as used in Figures 3 and 4, is appropriate. It is notable that the link between reading ability and age- normed task performance diminishes at about the same age as absolute thresholds reach mature levels (Fig. 2). However, note that when we analyzed cueing effects in just two age bins, we did not find a statistically significant interaction between age bin and reading ability group (Fig. 4A).
3.3. Testing the effect of cue salience
The ROC analysis above indicated that cued thresholds discriminate the DYS and CON groups. However, the accuracy of that discrimination diminishes in adulthood (Fig. 5C). A prior study with a similar design reported larger group differences in adults than we observed here (with set size 16; Roach & Hogben, 2007). Their cue was a small black dot presented for 20 ms. We increased the salience of the cue for use with children by making it larger and red, with duration 33 ms. These differences motivate an intriguing hypothesis that attentional deficits in dyslexia can be (partially) ameliorated by increasing the salience of the cue. To investigate whether participants with dyslexia are relatively more impaired with a less salient cue, 76 participants over the age of 14 were additionally tested in a Small Cue condition that was matched to Roach and Hogben (2007).
Overall, performance with the Small Cue was similar to performance with the big cue. Indeed, thresholds in those two conditions were highly correlated (r=0.68, p<10−11). Reading ability predicted thresholds in the Small Cue condition (r=−0.25, AUC = 0.71), replicating the phenomenon reported by Roach & Hogben (2007). However, the DYS and CON groups differed only slightly in the small cue’s effect (difference from Uncued). Overall, we found no evidence that individuals with dyslexia perform more differently from good readers when the cue is less salient. See Figure S1 and text in the Supplemental Material for more detail.
3.4. Lapse Rates and Response Times
When fitting psychometric functions we also estimated the difference λ between the upper asymptote and 1 (where 1 indicates perfect performance). λ estimates how often the subject’s response is uncorrelated with the stimulus, as would result from a lapse of focus. λ was fixed to be equal across conditions but varied across subjects. In our sample, λ decreased with age, suggesting that as children mature they become more consistently engaged in the task and therefore have lower lapse rates. This developmental pattern is in addition to the improvements to orientation sensitivity and selective spatial attention that were apparent in thresholds. Importantly, individuals with dyslexia do not differ from control participants in lapse rates. Thus, group differences in thresholds cannot be attributed to differences in ability to focus on the task. See the Supplemental Material for the full analysis of how λ varies with age and reading ability.
We also analyzed response times (RTs) on correct trials. Although the task was not speeded and not designed to measure attention effects on RTs, it is important to verify that there were no speed-accuracy trade-offs that could explain differences in thresholds between participant groups. For instance, individuals with dyslexia may have less of a cueing effect because they respond too fast in the Cued condition. Overall, there was no relation between reading ability and RT, although among adults over age 20, the DYS group tended to respond slower than the CON group. On average there was no cueing effect on RTs (difference between the Uncued and Cued conditions). Better readers tended to have larger cueing effects, consistent with the pattern in thresholds. Overall, there was no sign of differing speed-accuracy tradeoffs between groups. See the Supplemental Material for a full analysis of RTs.
3.5. Associations with phonemic decoding vs. real word reading
In the analyses presented above, we used the TOWRE phonemic decoding efficiency score (PDE) to define reading ability groups and to correlate with thresholds (as did prior studies with this paradigm; Roach & Hogben, 2007). The PDE test requires rapid reading aloud of pseudowords. Our participants also completed a similar test with real words (sight word efficiency, SWE). Sight word reading and phonemic decoding skills, respectively, may distinguish ‘surface’ and ‘phonological’ subtypes of dyslexia (McDougall, Borowsky, MacKinnon, & Hymel, 2005), and they may also be differentially related to the neural circuits of exogenous and endogenous spatial attention (Ekstrand, Neudorf, Gould, Mickleborough, & Borowsky, 2019).
Therefore, we conducted an exploratory analysis of our data using the SWE score in place of the PDE. Fewer subjects fell into the DYS group based on SWE (N=40) than based on PDE (N=46). Overall, the results were quite similar. One possible difference is that the association between SWE and cued thresholds does not decrease with age as much as it did for the PDE. More detail, including the sliding age window analysis, is reported in the Supplemental Material (Figure S5). Future studies should explore the hypothesis that adults with a specific impairment in real word reading (rather than just phonemic decoding) have a more robust deficiency in covert spatial attention.
4. Discussion
4.1. Summary
The primary findings of this study concern the development of orientation discrimination sensitivity and covert spatial attention, and how both of those measures relate to reading ability. First, when analyzing our entire sample together, we found that both orientation sensitivity and spatial attention improve gradually up until around age 20, consistent with previous findings (Brodeur & Enns, 1997; Leclercq & Siéroff, 2013; Schul et al., 2003). Although our sample included only 6 participants over age 50 (aging was not our focus), we observed no decline in task performance or attentional selection in the later years (Folk & Hoyer, 1992; Hartley, Kieley, & Slabach, 1990).
Second, individuals with dyslexia tended to have higher orientation discrimination thresholds and weaker spatial cueing effects than control participants with typical reading skills. Poor reading ability was associated with higher thresholds even in the single stimulus condition, when attentional selection was not necessary (there were no distracting stimuli to filter out). Nonetheless, the difference between groups was largest in the Cued condition, and reading ability correlated with the cueing effect (the benefit relative to the Uncued condition). This pattern shows that many individuals with dyslexia struggled to take advantage of the cue to base their decision on the relevant target stimulus and ignore irrelevant locations. Therefore, dyslexia is associated with a difference in the mechanisms of selective spatial attention.
Surprisingly, the links between reading ability and task performance were strongest in participants below age 20, prior to the maturation of absolute threshold levels (Fig. 5). That means that the attentional advantage in skilled readers is not a consequence of decades of fluent reading experience (Franceschini et al., 2012). That advantage may emerge early during the acquisition of literacy, or be linked to another trait that facilitates both reading and spatial attention from a young age.
4.2. Dyslexia in childhood and in adulthood
Another intriguing hypothesis consistent with our data is that individuals with dyslexia have a developmental delay in visual spatial attention. Eventually their spatial attention approaches normal function, but deficits present in childhood have a lasting impact on their reading ability. That hypothesis requires further investigation, because although we did find a statistically significant interaction between age bin and reading ability for thresholds in the cued condition, that interaction was not quite reliable for the cueing effect (relative to the Uncued condition).
Nonetheless, it is worth noting that we found relatively little age-related change in the cueing effect among skilled readers. That is consistent with prior findings that endogenous attention is adult-like by age 10 (Goldberg et al., 2001; Michael et al., 2013; Ristic & Enns, 2015; Wainwright & Bryson, 2005). The more gradual developmental patterns shown in Figure 2 were mostly driven by the poor readers, who were disproportionally represented in our sample. Another limitation is that our study was not longitudinal. The differences observed between children and adults could be due to sampling from different populations. In other words, it is possible that the particular dyslexic 10-year-olds in our sample will continue to have large deficits late into adulthood. Also, the differences between children and adults were somewhat less pronounced when using sight word efficiency to assess reading ability, rather than phonemic decoding efficiency (Figure S5).
For those reasons and others, we do not conclude that there is no association between spatial attention and reading ability in adulthood. Previous work with the same paradigm found large differences in cued thresholds between adults with and without dyslexia (Roach & Hogben, 2004, 2007, 2008). They varied the set size and found that the cueing effects and group differences were maximal with set size 16. We used set size 8 to reduce the influence of crowding in the periphery, and we were able to roughly replicate their finding that cued thresholds correlate with reading ability (Fig. S1). But the relatively small effects of reading ability in adults with set size 8 - especially for the uncued-cued threshold differences - suggest that the developmental trajectory of reading ability and spatial attention may interact with crowding.
When interpreting the differences between group averages, it is important to note the great deal of overlap in cueing effect magnitudes between individuals with and without dyslexia. Some individuals with dyslexia had large cueing effects, and some excellent readers had no cueing effect. A deficit of covert spatial attention is therefore unlikely to be the single cause of dyslexia. One possible interpretation is that a sub-type of dyslexia is associated with a specific deficit in visual-spatial attention, while many individuals struggle to read for a constellation of other reasons (e.g., crowding; Joo et al., 2018). Another interpretation is that a developmental delay in spatial attention interacts with other deficits to increase the risk that a child will struggle learning to read. Our data highlight the importance of taking a developmental approach. Longitudinal studies combining multiple measures in large samples of children and adults will be needed to resolve these different hypotheses.
4.3. Measurements and theory
We recommend that future studies also measure visual discrimination thresholds. When the primary dependent measure is response time or proportion correct, overall differences in motor function, perceptual ability or cognitive ability can complicate comparisons of attentional effects across groups. The study reported here is the first to measure thresholds in an investigation of spatial attention in children as well as adults with dyslexia. Because we assessed thresholds with an adaptive staircase, overall difficulty was equated across participants. We were also able to separately analyze lapse rates and found little differences between good and poor readers. Therefore, group differences in the cueing effect cannot be explained by differences in overall visual ability or focus on the psychophysical task.
The use of thresholds also allows us to make detailed inferences about the underlying mechanisms driving the effects in each condition. First, consider the age-related improvement in thresholds in the Single Stimulus condition. The target appeared alone and its contrast was supra-threshold, so performance was not limited by difficulty in target detection or localization. Rather, performance was limited by noise in the visual estimation of its orientation and the formation of a categorical decision (left or right of vertical). Improvement in thresholds with age can be interpreted as a sharpening of orientation-tuned mechanisms early in the visual system (Gilbert, Sigman, & Crist, 2001), or perhaps a more efficient read-out of visual neurons (Petrov, Dosher, & Lu, 2005). Independent of the effect of age, we also found that better readers had lower thresholds in the single stimulus condition (Figure 3, bottom middle panel). Although this effect is small, it suggests that better readers are generally better at discriminating fine visual details, which may not be surprising. There could be many reasons why an individual with dyslexia has elevated orientation discrimination thresholds, but it would be difficult to explain by a pure deficit in the ‘magnocellular’ visual stream (Skottun, 2000).
Second, consider the Uncued condition, when 8 Gabor patches are presented simultaneously. Seven are vertical distractors, and one is the tilted target that appears at an unpredictable location. To perform the task, the observer could either integrate the estimated orientations of all 8 items (by summing or averaging, as suggested by Baldassi & Burr, 2000), or they could report the tilt direction of the one item that appears to deviate most from vertical (search based on a max rule, as suggested by Roach & Hogben, 2007). Either way, the task is difficult because of noise in the estimated orientation of each item. Errors can occur when at least one distractor is mistakenly perceived as being tilted in the direction opposite the target and to a larger degree. Thresholds in the Uncued condition are therefore higher than in the Single Stimulus condition: more signal is required to overcome the noise added by each distractor. Another potential explanation of the threshold elevation in the Uncued condition is that the distractors tax limited processing resources, such that each item’s orientation estimate is noisier than in the Single Stimulus condition.
Third, consider the Cued condition and how the presence of the informative cue affects thresholds. The cue reveals the target’s location just prior to the onset of the eight Gabors. Cued thresholds are far below Uncued thresholds, but do not reach Single Stimulus levels. The benefit compared to the Uncued condition is the effect of selective spatial attention, and there are several potential explanations for it. One is signal enhancement: the cue increases the precision of the perceptual representation of the target (Cameron, Tai, & Carrasco, 2002; Cameron, Tai, Eckstein, & Carrasco, 2004; Carrasco & Yeshurun, 1998; Lu & Dosher, 1998). Another possibility is uncertainty reduction (or distractor exclusion; Morgan, Ward, & Castet, 1998; Palmer, 1994; Palmer, Ames, & Lindsey, 1993). Under this hypothesis, the cue allows the observer to base their decision on information at the target location and exclude the noise added by the distractors. Thus, the cue benefits performance even without changing the quality of the underlying perceptual representations. Roach & Hogben (2007, 2008) concluded that the cueing effect in this task is primarily due to uncertainty reduction, although that does not preclude some signal enhancement as well. In the Supplemental Material we describe in more detail a quantitative model that links cueing effects to differences in log thresholds.
4.4. Exogenous vs. endogenous spatial attention
To further understand the cueing effect we must classify it as exogenous (stimulus-driven, involuntary, and transient), endogenous (goal-driven, voluntary and sustained; Carrasco, 2011), or perhaps both. Our cues were peripheral and near the target location, so they could have an exogenous effect. But they were also 100% valid (predictive of the target location), so they could have an endogenous effect. Roach & Hogben (2008) manipulated several factors in this paradigm to probe the role of endogenous versus exogenous attention. First, they manipulated the validity of the cue (how often it appears near the target). When the cue’s location was totally random, there was no cueing effect, contrary to the prediction of an exogenous mechanism. They also varied the timing of the cue relative to the stimulus. The cueing effect was not transient, but rather was sustained and increased with more time between the cue and stimulus. The primary mechanism of the attentional effect in this paradigm is therefore endogenous.
Moreover, because in our experiments the cue appeared only 33 ms before the target array, there was not enough time for either endogenous or exogenous attention to focus on the cued location before the stimuli appear. Rather, endogenous attentional selection was likely based on stimulus representations held in a short-term memory trace (Roach & Hogben, 2008). Therefore, for a participant to benefit from the cue, they must be able to detect and localize the cue, understand the information it conveys, and efficiently filter out information that was seen at all other locations before making a decision. Given that we made the cue especially salient, it is unlikely that detection and localization are what differ between good and poor readers and between children and adults. Rather, our results reveal differences in the capacity to capitalize on the cue’s information to select the relevant stimulus.
4.5. Conclusion
In conclusion, children and adolescents with dyslexia are less able than their peers to select information from a task-relevant visual field location and filter out irrelevant information. That attentional skill improves along with fine discrimination ability up until about age 20. Individual differences in covert spatial attention are important not just in reading, but in any daily activity that requires finding objects that are not physically salient or ignoring irrelevant objects that are salient. The biological and environmental causes of the attentional deficits in dyslexia, as well as the cascading effects they may have on other cognitive functions throughout development, are all worthy of further investigation.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgements
This work was funded by: National Eye Institute grants EY026785 and EY029366 to A.L.W.; Microsoft, the Jacobs Foundation Early Career Fellowship, NSF BCS #1551330, and NICHD grant R21HD092771 to J.D.Y; and NEI grant EY12925 to G.M.B. We are grateful to Patrick M. Donnelly for invaluable assistance in data collection, and to John Palmer and Michael Grubb for insightful comments on the manuscript.
Footnotes
Conflict of Interest statement
The authors have no conflicts of interest to declare.
In Version 2 of the experiment, there was an additional “easy” trial that brought the total number of trials per block to 53 trials. Also in Version 2, the target’s tilt on easy trials was set to a fixed value of 25°, rather than twice the current threshold estimate.
Data and code availability
The experiment code, data, and MATLAB code to reproduce each figure and statistic, are all publicly available at the following website: https://github.com/alexlwhite/WhiteBoyntonYeatman2019_Repository (DOI: 10.5281/zenodo.3361729).
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
References
- Akhtar N, & Enns JT (1989). Relations between covert orienting and filtering in the development of visual attention. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 48, 315–334. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baldassi S, & Burr DC (2000). Feature-based integration of orientation signals in visual search. Vision Research, 40, 1293–300. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Benjamini Y, & Hochberg Y (1995). Controlling the False Discovery Rate: A Practical and Powerful Approach to Multiple Testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society. Series B (Methodological), 90, 289–300. [Google Scholar]
- Boden C, & Giaschi D (2007). M-stream deficits and reading-related visual processes in developmental dyslexia. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 346–366. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bosse M-L, Tainturier MJ, & Valdois S (2007). Developmental dyslexia: The visual attention span deficit hypothesis. Cognition, 104, 198–230. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brainard DH (1997). The psychophysics toolbox. Spatial Vision, 10, 443–446. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brodeur DA, & Boden C (2000). The effects of spatial uncertainty and cue predictability on visual orienting in children. Cognitive Development, 15, 367–382. [Google Scholar]
- Brodeur DA, & Enns JT (1997). Covert visual orienting across the lifespan. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 51, 20–35. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Callens M, Whitney C, Tops W, & Brysbaert M (2013). No deficiency in left-to-right processing of words in dyslexia but evidence for enhanced visual crowding. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66, 1803–1817. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cameron EL, Tai JC, & Carrasco M (2002). Covert attention affects the psychometric function of contrast sensitivity. Vision Research, 42, 949–67. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cameron EL, Tai JC, Eckstein MP, & Carrasco M (2004). Signal detection theory applied to three visual search tasks--identification, yes/no detection and localization. Spatial Vision, 17, 295–325. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carrasco M (2011). Visual attention: The past 25 years. Vision Research, 51, 1484–1525. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carrasco M, & Yeshurun Y (1998). The contribution of covert attention to the set-size and eccentricity effects in visual search. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 673–92. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cassim R, Talcott JB, & Moores E (2014). Adults with dyslexia demonstrate large effects of crowding and detrimental effects of distractors in a visual tilt discrimination task. PloS One, 9, e106191. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Demb JB, Boynton GM, Best M, & Heeger DJ (1998). Psychophysical evidence for a magnocellular pathway deficit in dyslexia. Vision Research, 38, 1555–1559. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Demb JB, Boynton GM, & Heeger DJ (1997). Brain activity in visual cortex predicts individual differences in reading performance. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 94, 13363–13366. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eden GF, Stein JF, & Wood FB (1994). Differences in eye movements and reading problems in dyslexic and normal children. Vision Research, 34, 1345–1358. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ekstrand C, Neudorf J, Gould L, Mickleborough M, & Borowsky R (2019). Where words and space collide: The overlapping neural activation of lexical and sublexical reading with voluntary and reflexive spatial attention. Brain Research, 1706, 1–12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Enns JT, & Brodeur DA (1989). A developmental study of covert orienting to peripheral visual cues. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 48, 171–189. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Facoetti A, Paganoni P, Turatto M, Marzola V, & Mascetti GG (2000). Visual- spatial attention in developmental dyslexia. Cortex, 36, 109–123. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Folk CL, & Hoyer WJ (1992). Aging and shifts of visual spatial attention. Psychology and Aging, 7, 453–465. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Franceschini S, Gori S, Ruffino M, Pedrolli K, & Facoetti A (2012). A Causal Link between Visual Spatial Attention and Reading Acquisition. Current Biology, 22, 814–819. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gaspelin N, Margett-Jordan T, & Ruthruff E (2015). Susceptible to distraction: Children lack top-down control over spatial attention capture. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 22, 461–468. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gilbert CD, Sigman M, & Crist RE (2001). The Neural Basis of Perceptual Learning. Neuron, 31, 681–697. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goldberg MC, Maurer D, & Lewis TL (2001). Developmental changes in attention: the effects of endogenous cueing and of distractors. Developmental Science, 4, 209219. [Google Scholar]
- Gori S, & Facoetti A (2015). How the visual aspects can be crucial in reading acquisition: The intriguing case of crowding and developmental dyslexia. Journal of Vision, 15, 1–20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gori Simone, Seitz AR, Ronconi L, Franceschini S, & Facoetti A (2015). Multiple Causal Links Between Magnocellular-Dorsal Pathway Deficit and Developmental Dyslexia. Cerebral Cortex, bhv206. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hartley AA, Kieley JM, & Slabach EH (1990). Age Differences and Similarities in the Effects of Cues and Prompts. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16, 523–537. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hawelka S, & Wimmer H (2005). Impaired visual processing of multi-element arrays is associated with increased number of eye movements in dyslexic reading. Vision Research, 45, 855–863. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Iarocci G, Enns JT, Randolph B, & Burack JA (2009). The modulation of visual orienting reflexes across the lifespan. Developmental Science, 12, 715–724. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Joo SJ, White AL, Strodtman DJ, & Yeatman JD (2018). Optimizing text for an individual’s visual system: The contribution of crowding to reading difficulties. Cortex, 103, 291–301. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kevan A, & Pammer K (2008). Making the link between dorsal stream sensitivity and reading. NeuroReport, 19, 467–470. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Landry O, Johnson KA, Fleming SJ, Crewther SG, & Chouinard PA (2019). A new look at the developmental profile of visual endogenous orienting. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 183, 158–171. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leclercq V, & Siéroff E (2013). Development of endogenous orienting of attention in school-age children. Child Neuropsychology, 19, 400–419. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Legge GE, Cheung S-HSH, Yu D, Chung STL, Lee H-WHW, & Owens DP (2007). The case for the visual span as a sensory bottleneck in reading. Journal of Vision, 7, 1–15. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Loftus GR (1978). On interpretation of interactions. Memory & Cognition, 6, 312–319. [Google Scholar]
- Lu ZL, & Dosher BA (1998). External noise distinguishes attention mechanisms. Vision Research, 38, 1183–1198. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maunsell JHR (2015). Neuronal Mechanisms of Visual Attention. Annual Review of Vision Science, 1, 373–391. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McDougall P, Borowsky R, MacKinnon GE, & Hymel S (2005). Process dissociation of sight vocabulary and phonetic decoding in reading: A new perspective on surface and phonological dyslexias. Brain and Language, 92, 185–203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Michael GA, Lété B, & Ducrot S (2013). Trajectories of attentional development: An exploration with the master activation map model. Developmental Psychology, 49, 615–631. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moores E, Cassim R, & Talcott JB (2011). Adults with dyslexia exhibit large effects of crowding, increased dependence on cues, and detrimental effects of distractors in visual search tasks. Neuropsychologia, 49, 3881–3890. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morgan MJ, Ward RM, & Castet E (1998). Visual Search for a Tilted Target: Tests of Spatial Uncertainty Models. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 51A, 347–370. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Palmer J (1994). Set-size effects in visual search: The effect of attention is independent of the stimulus for simple tasks. Vision Research, 34, 1703–1721. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Palmer J, Ames CT, & Lindsey DT (1993). Measuring the effect of attention on simple visual search. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 19, 108–130. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pammer K, Hansen P, Holliday I, & Cornelissen P (2006). Attentional shifting and the role of the dorsal pathway in visual word recognition. Neuropsychologia, 44, 2926–2936. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pearson DA, & Lane DM (1990). Visual Attention Movements: A Developmental Study. Child Development, 61, 1779–1795. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pelli DG (1997). The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: Transforming numbers into movies. Spatial Vision, 10, 437–442. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peterson RL, & Pennington BF (2015). Developmental Dyslexia. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 11, 283–307. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petrov A. a, Dosher BA, & Lu Z-L (2005). The dynamics of perceptual learning: an incremental reweighting model. Psychological Review, 112, 715–43. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Plude DJ, Enns JT, & Brodeur DA (1994). The development of selective attention: A life-span overview. Acta Psychologica, 86, 227–272. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Posner MI (1980). Orienting of Attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32, 3–25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Prins N, & Kingdom FAA (2009). Palamedes: Matlab routines for analyzing psychophysical data. [Google Scholar]
- Rayner K (1985). Do faulty eye movements cause dyslexia? Developmental Neuropsychology, 1, 3–15. [Google Scholar]
- Rayner K (2009). Eye movements and attention in reading, scene perception, and visual search. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62, 1457–1506. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reynolds JH, & Chelazzi L (2004). Attentional modulation of visual processing. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27, 611–647. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ristic J, & Enns JT (2015). Attentional Development. Handbook of Child Psychology and Developmental Science, 1–45. [Google Scholar]
- Ristic J, & Kingstone A (2009). Rethinking attentional development: Reflexive and volitional orienting in children and adults. Developmental Science, 12, 289–296. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roach NW, & Hogben JH (2004). Attentional modulation of visual processing in adult dyslexia. Psychological Science, 15, 650–654. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roach NW, & Hogben JH (2007). Impaired filtering of behaviourally irrelevant visual information in dyslexia. Brain, 130, 771–785. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roach NW, & Hogben JH (2008). Spatial cueing deficits in dyslexia reflect generalised difficulties with attentional selection. Vision Research, 48, 193–207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schul R, Townsend J, & Stiles J (2003). The development of attentional orienting during the school-age years. Developmental Science, 6, 262–272. [Google Scholar]
- Shaywitz SE, Escobar MD, Shaywitz BA, Fletcher JM, & Makuch R (1992). Evidence that dyslexia may represent the lower tail of a normal distribution of reading ability. The New England Journal of Medicine, 326, 145–150. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Skottun BC (2000). The magnocellular deficit theory of dyslexia: The evidence from contrast sensitivity. Vision Research, 40, 111–127. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Torgesen J, Rashotte C, & Wagner R (1999). TOWRE-2: Test of Word Reading Efficiency, 2nd Ed. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed. [Google Scholar]
- Vellutino FR, Fletcher JM, Snowling MJ, & Scanlon DM (2004). Specific reading disability (dyslexia): what have we learned in the past four decades? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 2–40. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vidyasagar TR, & Pammer K (1999). Impaired visual search in dyslexia relates to the magnocellular pathway in attention. Neuroreport, 10, 1283–1287. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vidyasagar TR, & Pammer K (2010). Dyslexia: a deficit in visuo-spatial attention, not in phonological processing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14, 57–63. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wainwright A, & Bryson SE (2002). The development of exogenous orienting: Mechanisms of control. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 82, 141–155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wainwright A, & Bryson SE (2005). The Development of Endogenous Orienting : Control Over the Scope of Attention and Lateral Asymmetries. Developmental Neuropsychology, 27, 237–255. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- White AL, Palmer J, & Boynton GM (2018). Evidence of serial processing in visual word recognition. Psychological Science, 29, 1062–1071. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.