Skip to main content
Schizophrenia Research: Cognition logoLink to Schizophrenia Research: Cognition
. 2019 May 1;19:100148. doi: 10.1016/j.scog.2019.100148

What variables predict cognitive remediation associated improvement in individuals with psychosis?

Joanna M Fiszdon a,b,, Matthew M Kurtz c, Lori Parente a, Jimmy Choi d
PMCID: PMC6889739  PMID: 31832338

Abstract

On average, cognitive remediation (CR) is effective in improving cognitive function in individuals with psychosis, though there is considerable variability in treatment response. No consensus has emerged to date about the potential influence of patient and illness characteristics on CR efficacy. In the current analyses, we examined baseline demographic, cognitive, clinical, and functional ability variables as potential moderators of cognitive improvements during a randomized, controlled trial of a hybrid drill-and-practice plus strategy training CR intervention.

In an attempt to disentangle non-specific vs. CR specific treatment effects, we separately examined potential predictors of cognitive improvement in individuals who received CR versus those in the control condition. Cognitive gains were predicted by a large array of demographic, symptom and cognitive variables, however this was true both in the CR and the control condition. CR-specific cognitive improvement was associated with more severe course of illness as indexed by higher number of hospitalizations, with poorer baseline cognition, and with less severe baseline negative symptoms.

Keywords: Psychosis, Schizophrenia, Cognitive remediation, Moderators, Treatment outcome

1. Introduction

A large body of research supports the efficacy of cognitive remediation (CR) for individuals with psychosis. The impact of CR has been examined on a wide range of outcomes, including symptoms, cognition, and functioning. Cognitive outcomes have likely been the most widely studied, and have generally have been associated with small to moderate range improvements (Wykes et al., 2011; McGurk et al., 2007; Prikken et al., in press). Recently, there has been increased interest in understanding variables that predict response to this intervention. Better understanding who, and under what conditions, is most likely to benefit from CR is particularly important given the time- and labor- intensive nature of this intervention, and the considerable heterogeneity in response, with some individuals normalizing their performance to healthy control levels, while others show no benefit (Fiszdon et al., 2005; Vita et al., 2013; Bosia et al., 2017). Better understanding individual variables that predict treatment response may help professionals match individuals to treatments most likely to be beneficial to them, as well as help identify variables which may need to be targeted before commencing cognitive remediation.

To date, the literature on moderators of CR effects has been limited, and no consensus has emerged regarding the potential influence of individual characteristics like demographics, baseline cognition, and symptoms on treatment response. While some studies indicate that CR may be more effective in improving cognition in younger individuals (Thomas et al., 2017; Wykes et al., 2009; Kontis et al., 2013; Corbera et al., 2017; McGurk and Mueser, 2008), others suggest it may be more effective in older individuals (Twamley et al., 2011), and others yet find no age differences in response to the intervention (Wykes et al., 2011; Medalia and Richardson, 2005; Fiszdon et al., 2005; Prikken et al., in press). Similarly mixed are results of studies that evaluate the impact of baseline cognitive function, with some suggesting that cognitive remediation may be more effective for individuals with poorer baseline cognition (Rodewald et al., 2014; Twamley et al., 2011), others finding better outcomes in those with better baseline cognition (Fiszdon et al., 2005; Vita et al., 2013), and several reports of no impact of neurocognitive abilities on treatment outcomes (Medalia and Richardson, 2005; Scheu et al., 2013). The two studies that examined current IQ both found that it does not impact cognitive remediation outcomes (Fiszdon et al., 2005; Twamley et al., 2011), while there is mixed data on the efficacy of CR for those with lower premorbid IQ estimates (Fiszdon et al., 2006; Kontis et al., 2013). With regard to baseline symptoms, there are also mixed reports, with some reporting better outcomes in those with more symptoms (Twamley et al., 2011), some reporting better outcomes in those with less symptoms (Fiszdon et al., 2005; Vita et al., 2013; Wykes et al., 2011), and several reporting that symptoms do not moderate treatment effects (Medalia and Richardson, 2005; Scheu et al., 2013). Data regarding illness chronicity is also inconclusive, with some reporting no evidence that illness duration impacts outcomes (Fiszdon et al., 2005; Twamley et al., 2011; Wykes et al., 2011), while others suggest that longer illness duration may adversely impact some cognitive outcomes (Bowie et al., 2014; Prikken et al., in press).

In sum, efforts to date have not identified any variables that have been consistently associated with a positive response to CR. However, this literature is still in its infancy, and more work is needed. In the current secondary analyses, we examine baseline demographic, cognitive, clinical, and functional ability variables as potential moderators of improvements in cognition. As the existing literature on CR moderators is limited and lacks consistency, we did not have directional hypotheses. We chose cognition as the primary outcome, as this variable is most consistently assessed and reported in CR trials. In an attempt to disentangle non-specific vs. specific CR treatment effects, we separately examine the predictive value of these variables in individuals who received CR versus those randomized to the control condition.

2. Methods

2.1. Participants

Participants were individuals with structured clinical interview for DSM-IV (SCID, First et al., 1996) confirmed psychotic spectrum illness, aged 18 to 65. All met the following criteria: English as primary language, no evidence of substance abuse or dependence in the past 30 days, and psychiatric stability as evidenced by no hospitalizations nor changes in medications or housing in the past 30 days. The study had been approved by the local Institutional Review Board, and all participants provided written informed consent.

2.2. Study design

For details of the original study, please refer to Fiszdon et al. (2016). Briefly, participants were randomized (2:1 ratio) to two months of either cognitive remediation (CR), or treatment as usual (TAU) condition. CR consisted of four weeks (five 1-h sessions per week) of computerized drill-and-practice attention and memory training using PSS CogReHab (Bracy, 1995), and four weeks (five, 1-h sessions per week) of manualized, strategy-focused training (Delahunty et al., 1993). For computerized training, participants were administered 5–7 tasks per session, with task difficulty titrated based on prior performance. For strategy-focused training, participants were administered paper-and-pencil memory tasks, and worked with a trainer to identify and practice strategies (e.g. verbal mediation, chunking, etc.) to enhance their task performance. The order of computerized drill-and-practice versus strategy-focused training was counterbalanced. TAU entailed continuation of whatever standard outpatient psychiatric and psychological treatment participants were receiving prior to enrollment in the study, which most often consisted of medication management, case management and/or group or individual therapy. Comprehensive assessments were conducted at study intake and end of the 2-month active phase.

2.3. Measures

The following demographic information was obtained at study intake: age, education, number of prior hospitalizations, age at first hospitalization, age at illness onset, premorbid IQ estimate (based on Wide Range Achievement Test reading, Jastak and Wilkinson, 1993) and current IQ estimate (based on 2-subtest Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence, Wechsler, 1999). Additional measures were collected at both intake and end of the 2-month active phase. Symptoms were assessed using the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS, Kay et al., 1987, Bell et al., 1994). Self-report task motivation and self-esteem were assessed using the Intrinsic Motivation Inventory for Schizophrenia Research (IMI, Choi et al., 2010) and the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (RSES, Rosenberg, 1965), respectively. Visuospatial memory was assessed using the Rey-Osterreith Complex Figure (Osterreith, 1944). Set shifting was assessed using the percent perseverative errors and percent conceptual level scores from the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (Heaton, 1981). Vigilance was assessed using the Continuous Performance Test, x/a (Loong, 1991). Processing speed/attention and speeded set-shifting were assessed using Trail Making Test Part A and B, respectively (TMT, Lezak, 1983). List learning was assessed using the California Verbal Learning Test-II, Trials 1–5 total (Delis et al., 2000). Immediate and delayed story memory was assessed using the Wechsler Memory Scale revised, Logical Memory I and II (Wechsler, 1987). Simple attention and working memory were assessed using the digits forward and digits backward subtests from the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, 3rd edition (WAIS-III, Wechsler, 1997). Emotion recognition was assessed using the Bell-Lysaker Emotion Recognition Task (BLERT, Bell and Lysaker, 1994). Functional ability was assessed using the UCSD Performance-Based Skills Assessment (UPSA, Patterson et al., 2001a), Social Skills Performance Assessment (SSPA, Patterson et al., 2001b), and the Medication Management Ability Assessment (MMAA, Patterson et al., 2002).

2.4. Data analysis

All variables were inspected for normality and, as needed to adjust for skew, a log 10 transformation or a reflect and log 10 transformation was applied (Schinka et al., 2003). Baseline demographic group differences were explored using t-tests and chi-squared tests. CR outcome measures selected as dependent variables for the current analyses were cognitive variables which we had previously reported improved significantly more for those in the CR vs. TAU condition at the end of active phase. These variables indexed visuospatial memory (Rey-Osterreith immediate recall), list learning (CVLT-II trials 1–5 total), and working memory (WAIS-III Digits backward.) For each of these three measures, post- scores were regressed on pre- scores in order to generate residualized change scores reflecting amount of change during the two-month active phase. These scores were then multiplied by −1, so that positive values reflected greater cognitive improvement from pre to post. Next, Pearson correlations were used to examine the degree of relationship between baseline demographic, self-report, symptom, cognitive, and functional ability measures and the residualized change scores for the CR condition. As these were exploratory analyses, we retained a p < 0.05 as the significance level in spite of multiple comparisons.

Next, in order to determine whether significant correlations found in the CR condition were unique to that sample and thereby more likely to reflect CR-specific factors, we re-ran Pearson correlations between baseline variables and residualized change scores in the TAU condition. For baseline variables that were significantly correlated with cognitive improvement in the CR condition, we then conducted Fisher r to z transformations to compare correlation coefficients between the two conditions. Lack of significant differences in correlation strength between the two conditions was interpreted as indicating that the predictor variable indexed non-specific cognitive improvement, meaning it was associated with change in cognition over time, but not necessarily as a result of CR.

3. Results

Of the 50 individuals randomized to CR and 25 to the TAU condition, 63 completed assessments conducted at the end of the 2-month active phase (CR = 40; TAU = 23). Demographic information for this completer sample as a whole and for each condition is presented in Table 1. There were no group differences in the examined baseline characteristics. On average, participants were in their late 40's, with 12 years of education. Nearly three quarters of the sample were male, with a little more than half never married and half Caucasian. Age at onset was early 20's with first hospitalization in the mid-twenties. Current psychiatric symptoms were mild. Participants in the CR condition completed an average of 33.48 (SD = 10.45) training sessions [14.39 (7.42) for computerized drill and practice and 15.59 (6.80) for strategy-based sessions].

Table 1.

Sample demographics.

Variable Full sample
CR condition
TAU condition
n = 63 n = 40 n = 23
Age 48.81 (8.84) 48.03 (8.87) 50.17 (8.81)
Age at onset 20.85 (6.81) 20.03 (6.27) 22.25 (7.61)
Number hosp 13.59 (20.85) 10.53 (11.29) 19.10 (31.21)
Age at 1st hosp 24.98 (9.13) 25.83 (9.81) 23.52 (7.85)
Education 12.30 (2.01) 12.50 (1.99) 11.96 (2.23)
PANSS grand total 52.44 (11.38) 52.08 (11.81) 53.09 (10.83)
Gender (male) 70% 70% 70%
Race (Caucasian) 49% 50% 48%
Marital status (never married) 62% 60% 65%

3.1. Visuospatial memory improvement

Please refer to Table 2 for details of moderator analyses. In the CR condition, significant correlations were observed between greater improvement in visuospatial memory (Rey-Osterreith) and poorer baseline visuospatial memory performance, poorer baseline delayed story memory (Logical Memory II), and more hospitalizations. When the strength of these correlations was compared to those observed in the TAU condition, the only condition difference was for number of hospitalizations, suggesting that this variable may uniquely predict visuospatial improvement that occurs in response to CR, while the other two variables may predict non-specific cognitive change.

Table 2.

Predictors of cognitive improvement, by condition.

Variable Visuospatial memory
Rey-Osterreith improvement
List learning
CVLT-II improvement
Working memory
Digits back improvement
CR (r) Control (r) Difference (p) CR (r) Control (r) Difference (p) CR (r) Control (r) Difference (p)
Demographics
Age 0.031 0.221 −0.085 0.069 0.299^ −0.164
Education −0.170 −0.113 −0.231 −0.210 −0.224 −0.177
No. hospitalizations 0.353 −0.267 0.0316 0.312^ 0.055 0.026 −0.068
Age at 1st hosp −0.265 −0.135 −0.154 −0.069 −0.070 0.322
Age onset −0.037 0.042 −0.052 0.062 −0.099 0.066
WRAT −0.130 −0.354 −0.042 0.193 −0.246 −0.058
WASI IQ −0.263 0.187 −0.265 −0.142 −0.085 −0.087



Symptoms
PANSS positive 0.053 −0.188 0.010 0.478 −0.114 0.002
PANSS negative −0.039 −0.087 −0.075 0.345 −0.399⁎⁎ 0.188 0.0264
PANSS cognitive 0.050 −0.069 −0.077 0.313 −0.274^ 0.299
PANSS emotional −0.100 −0.206 −0.060 −0.077 −0.224 −0.125
PANSS hostility −0.092 −0.280 −0.322 −0.017 0.2543 −0.398⁎⁎ −0.117 0.2713



Self report
IMI interest 0.093 −0.060 −0.007 −0.102 0.028 0.134
IMI effort 0.152 0.198 0.111 −0.125 0.153 0.235
IMI competence −0.124 0.028 −0.128 0.017 −0.026 0.147
Rosenberg SES 0.182 −0.301 0.169 0.048 0.287^ −0.110



Cognition
Rey-O immediate −0.642⁎⁎⁎⁎ −0.576⁎⁎⁎ 0.7039 −0.284^ −0.113 −0.201 0.072
Rey-O 30-min delay −0.585⁎⁎⁎⁎ −0.546⁎⁎ 0.8337 −0.288^ −0.256 −0.162 0.002
WCST % CL −0.157 −0.446 −0.342 −0.178 0.5287 −0.330 −0.011 0.2301
WCST % PE Log 0.151 0.370^ 0.357 0.242 0.6527 0.294^ −0.108
CPT RefLog 0.187 0.033 0.075 0.075 0.204 0.210
BLERT −0.203 −0.020 −0.274^ −0.136 −0.124 −0.120
Trails A Log 0.033 0.355^ 0.011 −0.018 0.293^ −0.050
Trails B Log 0.112 −0.086 0.057 0.357^ 0.443⁎⁎⁎ −0.078 0.0444
CVLT Trials 1–5 Tot −0.071 −0.033 −0.747⁎⁎⁎⁎ −0.789⁎⁎⁎⁎ 0.7114 −0.356 0.097 0.0891
Logical Memory I −0.291^ 0.013 −0.350 −0.448 0.6745 −0.514⁎⁎⁎⁎ 0.034 0.0293
Logical Memory II −0.415⁎⁎ −0.146 0.2891 −0.407 −0.334 0.7642 −0.486⁎⁎⁎⁎ −0.168 0.1902
WAIS Digits forward 0.032 0.286 −0.097 0.192 −0.132 −0.546⁎⁎
WAIS Digits back −0.229 0.012 −0.352 0.058 0.126 −0.718⁎⁎⁎⁎ −0.830⁎⁎⁎⁎ 0.303



Functional ability
UPSA −0.197 −0.318 −0.335 −0.222 0.6599 −0.294^ −0.115
SSPA 0.002 −0.054 −0.152 −0.402^ −0.031 0.045
MMAA RefLog 0.272^ 0.238 0.195 0.362 0.368 0.137 0.3843

Note: BLERT, Bell Lysaker Emotion Recognition Task; CL, conceptual level response; CPT, Continuous Performance Test; CRT, Cognitive remediation; CVLT, California Verbal Learning Test-II; ILSS, Independent Living Skills Survey; IMI, Intrinsic Motivation Inventory for Schizophrenia Research; Log, Log 10 transformation; MMAA, Medication Management Ability Assessment; PANSS, Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale; PE, perseverative errors; QLS, Quality of Life Scale; RefLog, Reflect and log 10 transformation; Rey-Osterreith; Rosenberg SES, Rosenberg Self Esteem Scale; SSPA, Social Skills Performance Assessment; UPSA, UCSD Performance-Based Skills Assessment; WAIS, Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, 3rd edition; WASI IQ, Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence, estimate based on two-test score for vocabulary and matrix reasoning; WCST, Wisconsin Card Sorting Task; WRAT, Wide Range Achievement Test.

^

p < 0.10.

p < 0.05.

⁎⁎

p < 0.01.

⁎⁎⁎

p < 0.005.

⁎⁎⁎⁎

p ≤ 0.001.

3.2. List learning improvement

In the CR condition, significant correlations were observed between greater improvement in list learning (CVLT-II) and poorer baseline list learning, immediate and delayed story memory (Logical Memory), working memory (WAIS digits backward), set shifting (WCST), functional ability (UPSA performance), and with less severe baseline PANSS hostility. The strength of these correlations was not significantly different than that observed in the TAU condition, suggesting these variables index non-specific change.

3.3. Working memory improvement

In the CR condition, significant correlations were observed between greater improvement in working memory (WAIS digits backward) and poorer baseline working memory, speeded set-shifting (Trail Making Test B), list learning (CVLT), set-shifting (WCST), immediate and delayed story memory (Logical Memory), functional ability (MMAA), and with less severe baseline PANSS hostility and negative symptoms. PANSS negative symptoms, Trail Making Test B, and immediate story memory emerged as the only variables where the strength of association was significantly greater in the CR than in the TAU condition, suggesting these variables may index CR-specific effects. As a post-hoc analysis, these three variables were entered into a stepwise regression. Immediate story memory entered first, accounting for 26.4% of the variance, with PANSS negative factor entering second, accounting for an additional 10.4% of variance. Trail Making Test B was not retained in the final model, which accounted for 36.9% of variance.

4. Discussion

This study is among the first to investigate the relationship between, and specificity of, clinical, demographic, cognitive and functional ability variables at study entry to the degree of cognitive improvement during a CR intervention consisting of both drill-and-practice and strategy-based training. Results indicated that in the CR condition, a large array of demographic, symptom, and cognitive variables predicted improvement in the three cognitive domains which showed improvement in the original clinical trial.

Only a few of these were CR specific however, showing a stronger relationship with cognitive change in the CR than in the TAU condition. Namely, greater visuospatial memory improvement was more strongly predicted in the CR than in the TAU condition by greater number of hospitalizations, while greater working memory improvement was more strongly predicted in the CR than in the TAU condition by less severe baseline negative symptoms, poorer baseline speeded set-shifting, and poorer baseline immediate story memory. Taken together, these findings suggest that a better response to CR, when indexed by cognitive outcomes, is predicted by a more severe course of illness (as measured by number of hospitalizations), by less severe levels of social withdrawal, blunted affect and other negative symptoms, and by poorer baseline speeded set-shifting and immediate story memory.

In terms of variables associated with working memory improvement, the relationship between negative symptoms and working memory is well established (Carter et al., 1996; Menon et al., 2001). Negative symptoms are known to limit cognitive performance, CR adherence, and even overall psychosocial rehabilitation outcomes (Cella et al., 2017; Evans et al., 2004; Milev et al., 2005). Also, given that working memory dysfunction and negative symptoms are similarly tied to disturbances in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (Menon et al., 2001; Perlstein et al., 2001), fewer negative symptoms may allow the person to benefit more from a cognitive intervention, particularly one that requires motivation to engage in a time-and labor-intensive therapy. At least one study has reported larger improvements in cognitive outcomes in response to a strategy-based program of CR in clients with poorer baseline cognitive function (Twamley et al., 2011). The authors of this study surmised that clients with greater cognitive impairment at baseline likely had greater room to improve on cognitive measures. In contrast to our study, the study by Twamley and colleagues found that greater negative symptoms predicted a better response to CR. Discrepancies between these findings and the current study may reflect the hybrid approach of strategy and drill-and-practice CR training used in the current study as compared to the group-based strategy-training selected in the 2011 study.

Finding that factors associated with greater illness burden (i.e. more hospitalizations, poorer baseline cognition) as well as factors associated with lesser illness burden (i.e. less severe negative symptoms) both predict improvement in CR was unexpected and somewhat counterintuitive. At the same time, it highlights our limited understanding of the complex interrelationships between predictor variables. As noted above, negative symptoms may be unique in that if present at high levels, they may limit actual exposure to the intervention. The predictive value of variables associated with higher illness burden, on the other hand, may stem from them indexing individuals who have the greatest room to improve.

The failure to find a link between participant age and degree of improvement was somewhat unexpected in light of several recent reports of stronger responses to CR among younger clients (Corbera et al., 2017; Wykes et al., 2009). For example, Kontis et al. (2013), in a study of 134 people with schizophrenia, investigated the effects of age (younger than 40 vs. older than 40) on improvements in cognition using a CR intervention that was one element of the hybrid intervention reported in the current study. The authors found only very limited effects of CR in the 40 or older group. Differences between this and our study may reflect the much more limited range of ages assessed in the current study (mean age = 48.81; SD = 8.84) reducing sensitivity, along with the inclusion of a drill-and-practice element to the intervention in addition to the strategy training.

Several limitations of the current study should be mentioned. First, sample size was moderate and the power to detect differential predictive relationships between baseline scores and cognitive change between CR and a TAU control condition was likely limited. Second, the study is correlational and it remains unclear to what degree modifying baseline characteristics (e.g., providing interventions to reduce negative symptoms before entry into sustained and complex programs of CR) would have any effect on cognitive improvements. Third, strategy-training and drill-and-practice CR represent opposite ends of a continuum in approaches to CR treatment. The combination of both approaches in our selected intervention precludes an analysis of the degree to which predictors may be different in one approach versus another, and potentially limits the generalizability of our findings. Fourth, the doses of the drill-and-practice intervention (20 h vs. target of 100 h in other studies, e.g. Bell et al., 2001, Kurtz et al., 2007) and the strategy training component of our intervention (20 h vs. 40 h in other studies, (e.g. Reeder et al., 2006) might have reduced the effect size of positive treatment gains and/or prevented us from seeing significant improvements in other cognitive domains. Fifth, specific characteristics of our sample could limit the generalizability of our findings. And lastly, there is still no agreement on how much, or to what level, cognition must improve to lead to functional improvements—our results could very well have been different had our outcome been reaching a specific cognitive threshold or had out outcome been change in functioning itself.

In spite of these limitations, this study contributes to the literature on moderators of CR outcome in schizophrenia. Unlike many other studies that more narrowly focus on predictors of improvement in individuals exposed to CR, we identified CR-specific baseline moderators by teasing out the predictive value of these moderators against a treatment as usual group and the associated non-specific effects. The findings highlight the impact that number of hospitalizations, negative symptoms, and verbal memory prior to the start of CR can have on benefiting from this intervention, particularly in the areas of visuospatial and working memory.

Conflict of interest statement

All authors declare that they have no conflicts to disclose.

Funding source

This study was funded by grant from Veterans Administration Rehabilitation Research and Development Service (#F3195V to JMF). The sponsor had no role in the planning or conduct of the study or in the interpretation of study results.

References

  1. Bell M., Lysaker P. 1994. Bell-Lysaker Emotion Recognition Task: Videotape and Scoring System. (available from author) [Google Scholar]
  2. Bell M.D., Lysaker P.H., Beam-Goulet J.L., Milstein R.M., Lindenmeyer J.P. Five-component model of schizophrenia: assessing the factorial invariance of the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale. Psychiatry Res. 1994;52:295–303. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(94)90075-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Bell M., Bryson G., Greig T., Corcoran C., Wexler B.E. Neurocognitive enhancement therapy with work therapy: effects on neuropsychological test performance. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 2001;58:763–768. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.58.8.763. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bosia M., Buonocore M., Bechi M., Spangaro M., Pigoni A., Croci M., Cocchi F., Guglielmino C., Bianchi L., Smeraldi E., Cavallaro R. Cognitive remediation and functional improvement in schizophrenia: is it a matter of size? Eur. Psychiatry. 2017;40:26–32. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2016.06.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bowie C.R., Grossman M., Gupta M., Oyewumi L.K., Harvey P.D. Cognitive remediation in schizophrenia: efficacy and effectiveness in patients with early versus long-term course of illness. Early Interv. Psychiatry. 2014;8:32–38. doi: 10.1111/eip.12029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bracy O. Psychological Software Services; Indianapolis, Indiana: 1995. CogReHab Software. [Google Scholar]
  7. Carter C.S., Robertson L., Nordahl T., Chaderjian M., Kraft L., O-Shora-Celaya L. Spatial working memory deficits and their relationship to negative symptoms in unmedicated schizophrenia patients. Biol. Psychiatry. 1996;40:930–932. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(96)00350-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Cella M., Preti A., Edwards C., Down T., Wykes T. Cognitive remediation for negative symptoms of schizophrenia: a network meta-analysis. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2017;52:43–51. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2016.11.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Choi J., Mogami T., Medalia A. Intrinsic Motivation Inventory: an adapted measures for schizophrenia research. Schizophr. Bull. 2010;36:966–976. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbp030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Corbera S., Wexler B.E., Poltorak A., Thime W.R., Kurtz M.M. Cognitive remediation for adults with schizophrenia: does age matter? Psychiatry Res. 2017;247:21–27. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2016.10.084. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Delahunty A., Morice R., Frost B. Specific cognitive flexibility rehabilitation in schizophrenia. Psychol. Med. 1993;23:221–227. doi: 10.1017/s0033291700039015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Delis D.C., Kramer J.H., Kaplan E., Ober B.A. Adult Version Manual. second edition. Psychological Corporation; San Antonio, TX: 2000. California verbal learning test. [Google Scholar]
  13. Evans J.D., Bond G.R., Meyer P.S., Kim H.W., Lysaker P.H., Gibson P.J., Tunis S. Cognitive and clinical predictors of success in vocational rehabilitation in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 2004;70:331–342. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2004.01.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. First, M. B. S., R.L, Gibbon M., Williams J.B. Biometrics Research Department, New York State Psychiatric Institute; New York: 1996. Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders—Patient Edition (SCID-I/P, Version 2.0) [Google Scholar]
  15. Fiszdon J.M., Cardenas A.S., Bryson G.J., Bell M.D. Predictors of remediation success on a trained memory task. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 2005;193:602–608. doi: 10.1097/01.nmd.0000177790.23311.ba. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Fiszdon J.M., Choi J., Bryson G., Bell M.D. Impact of intellectual status on response to cognitive task training in patients with schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 2006;87:261–269. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2006.04.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Fiszdon J.M., Choi K.H., Bell M.D., Choi J., Silverstein S.M. Cognitive remediation for individuals with psychosis: efficacy and mechanisms of treatment effects. Psychol. Med. 2016;46:3275–3289. doi: 10.1017/S0033291716001951. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Heaton R. Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc.; Odessa, FL: 1981. The Wisconsin Card Sorting Test Manual. [Google Scholar]
  19. Jastak J., Wilkinson G. Jastak Associates Inc.; Lutz, FL: 1993. Wide Range Achievement Test 3. [Google Scholar]
  20. Kay S.R., Fiszbein A., Opler L.A. The positive and negative syndrome scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 1987;13:261–276. doi: 10.1093/schbul/13.2.261. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Kontis D., Huddy V., Reeder C., Landau S., Wykes T. Effects of age and cognitive reserve on cognitive remediation therapy outcome in patients with schizophrenia. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatr. 2013;21:218–230. doi: 10.1016/j.jagp.2012.12.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Kurtz M.M., Seltzer J.C., Shagan D.S., Thime W.R., Wexler B.E. Computer-assisted cognitive remediation in schizophrenia: what is the active ingredient? Schizophr. Res. 2007;89:251–260. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2006.09.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Lezak M.D. 2nd ed. Oxford University Press; New York, NY: 1983. Neuropsychological Assessment. [Google Scholar]
  24. Loong J. Wang Neuropsychological Laboratory; San Louis Obispo, CA: 1991. The Manual for the Continuous Performance Test. [Google Scholar]
  25. McGurk S.R., Mueser K.T. Response to cognitive rehabilitation in older versus younger persons with severe mental illness. Am. J. Psychiatr. Rehabil. 2008;11:90–105. [Google Scholar]
  26. McGurk S.R., Twamley E.W., Sitzer D.I., Mchugo G.J., Mueser K.T. A meta-analysis of cognitive remediation in schizophrenia. Am. J. Psychiatr. 2007;164:1791–1802. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.07060906. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Medalia A., Richardson R. What predicts a good response to cognitive remediation interventions? Schizophr. Bull. 2005;31:942–953. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbi045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Menon V., Anagnoson R.T., Mathalon D.H., Glover G.H., Pfefferbaum A. Functional neuroanatomy of auditory working memory in schizophrenia: relation to positive and negative symptoms. Neuroimage. 2001;13:433–446. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2000.0699. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Milev P., Ho B.C., Arndt S., Andreasen N.C. Predictive values of neurocognition and negative symptoms on functional outcome in schizophrenia: a longitudinal first-episode study with 7-year follow-up. Am. J. Psychiatr. 2005;162:495–506. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.3.495. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Osterreith P.A. Le test de copie d'une figure complexe. Arch. Psychol. 1944;30:206–356. [Google Scholar]
  31. Patterson T.L., Goldman S., McKibbin C.L., Hughs T., Jeste D.V. UCSD Performance-Based Skills Assessment: development of a new measure of everyday functioning for severely mentally ill adults. Schizophr. Bull. 2001;27:235–245. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a006870. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Patterson T.L., Moscona S., McKibbin C.L., Davidson K., Jeste D.V. Social skills performance assessment among older patients with schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 2001;48:351–360. doi: 10.1016/s0920-9964(00)00109-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Patterson T.L., Lacro J., McKibbin C.L., Moscona S., Hughs T., Jeste D.V. Medication management ability assessment: results from a performance-based measure in older outpatients with schizophrenia. J. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 2002;22:11–19. doi: 10.1097/00004714-200202000-00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Perlstein W.M., Carter C.S., Noll D.C., Cohen J.D. Relation of prefrontal cortex dysfunction to working memory and symptoms in schizophrenia. Am. J. Psychiatr. 2001;158(7):1105–1113. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.7.1105. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Prikken M., Konings M.J., Lei W.U., Beagemann M.J.H., Sommer I.E.C. The efficacy of computerized cognitive drill and practice training for patients with a schizophrenia-spectrum disorder: a meta-analysis. Schizophr. Res. 2019;204:368–374. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2018.07.034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Reeder C., Smedley N., Butt K., Bogner D., Wykes T. Cognitive predictors of social functioning improvements following cognitive remediation for schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 2006;32(Suppl. 1):S123–S131. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbl019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Rodewald K., Holt D.V., Rentrop M., Roesch-Ely D., Liebrenz M., Funke J., Weisbrod M., Kaiser S. Predictors for improvement of problem-solving during cognitive remediation for patients with schizophrenia. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2014;20:455–460. doi: 10.1017/S1355617714000162. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Rosenberg M. Princeton University Press; Princeton, NJ: 1965. Society and the Adolescent Self-image. [Google Scholar]
  39. Scheu F., Aghotor J., Pfueller U., Moritz S., Bohn F., Weisbrod M., Roesch-Ely D. Predictors of performance improvements within a cognitive remediation program for schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 2013;209:375–380. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2013.04.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Schinka J.A., Velicer W.F., Weiner I.B. Wiley; Hoboken, NJ: 2003. Handbook of Psychology, Vol. 2. Research Methods in Psychology. [Google Scholar]
  41. Thomas K.R., Puig O., Twamley E.W. Age as a moderator of change following compensatory cognitive training in individuals with severe mental illnesses. Psychiatr. Rehabil. J. 2017;40:70–78. doi: 10.1037/prj0000206. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Twamley E.W., Burton C.Z., Vella L. Compensatory cognitive training for psychosis: who benefits? Who stays in treatment? Schizophr. Bull. 2011;37:S55–S62. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbr059. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Vita A., Deste G., De Peri L., Barlati S., Poli R., Cesana B.M., Sacchetti E. Predictors of cognitive and functional improvement and normalization after cognitive remediation in patients with schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 2013;150:51–57. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2013.08.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Wechsler D. Psychological Corporation; New York, NY: 1987. The Wechsler Memory Scale—Revised. [Google Scholar]
  45. Wechsler D. Psychological Corporation; San Antonio, TX: 1997. WAIS-III Manual: Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III. [Google Scholar]
  46. Wechsler D. Psychological Corporation; San Antonio, TX: 1999. Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI-III) Manual. [Google Scholar]
  47. Wykes T., Huddy V., Wykes T., Huddy V. Cognitive remediation for schizophrenia: it is even more complicated. Curr. Opin. Psychiatry. 2009;22:161–167. doi: 10.1097/YCO.0b013e328322fbf4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Wykes T., Huddy V., Cellard C., McGurk S.R., Czobor P. A meta-analysis of cognitive remediation for schizophrenia: methodology and effect sizes. Am. J. Psychiatr. 2011;168:472–485. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.10060855. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Schizophrenia Research: Cognition are provided here courtesy of Elsevier

RESOURCES