Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2020 Nov 1.
Published in final edited form as: Ageing Soc. 2018 Jun 27;39(11):2520–2540. doi: 10.1017/S0144686X18000612

Marital Power and Depressive Symptoms among Older Mexican Adults

Joseph L Saenz 1, Sunshine Rote 2
PMCID: PMC6892469  NIHMSID: NIHMS963842  PMID: 31802784

Abstract

An extensive body of research documents marital status differences in health among older adults. However, few studies have investigated the heterogeneity in depressive symptomatology among older married adults living in developing countries. Our study investigates the interplay of gender and marital power dynamics for mental health among older Mexican adults. Our sample includes older married couples in the 2015 Wave of the Mexican Health and Aging Study (n=3,621 dyads). We use seemingly unrelated regression to model the association between self-reported distributions of decision-making power within marriages and depressive symptoms for husbands and wives. For approximately 41 per cent of couples, the husband and wife both reported an equal distribution of power in the marriage. Compared to those who reported an equal power distribution, husbands and wives who reported an imbalance of power (having more power or less power than their spouse) reported more depressive symptoms. Levels of depressive symptoms were higher in marriages characterised by an unequal balance of power. The relationship between equality in power and depressive symptoms is not explained by health care needs or living arrangements. Marital quality is an important factor for understanding depressive symptoms among older Mexican adults.

Keywords: MHAS, Mexico, aging, marital power, depression

Introduction

Population ageing is a global phenomenon. Between 2015 and 2050, the proportion of the older adult population in the world is projected to increase from 12 per cent to 22 per cent (World Health Organization 2016a). Further, population ageing is occurring especially rapidly in developing countries. In Mexico, the population aged 60 and older is expected to triple to over 20 per cent of the total population by 2050 (Angel et al. 2017). Importantly, population ageing in Mexico is taking place with limited public health and social security systems for older adults (Wong and Palloni 2009) and demographic changes limiting family size. Given these changes, studies of mental health in Mexico and at a global level are becoming increasingly important. Globally, depression is expected to become the leading cause of loss of disability-adjusted life years as soon as 2030 (World Health Organization 2004). Depression can negatively impact social functioning (Lewinsohn et al. 1991), increase suicide risk and overall mortality, and can worsen the progression of many medical conditions (Alexopoulos 2005). Depression among older adults is often underdiagnosed and is less likely to be treated (Lebowitz et al. 1997). In Mexico, approximately one out of every eight older adults has major depressive symptoms (García-Peña et al. 2008). As in other countries, depression rates in Mexico are higher for women and are closely linked to family structure and dynamics, including marital status (Medina-Mora et al. 2005).

One major area of research on depression focuses on the role of marital status. While married adults tend to report lower levels of depression than the unmarried (Gore and Mangione 1983; Gove et al. 1983; Kessler and Essex 1982; Ross et al. 1990), the relationship between marriage and health is also highly dependent on marital quality (Robles 2014). Individuals in high quality marriages, characterised by reciprocity and support, tend to benefit in terms of both health and psychological well-being. However, marriages characterised by distress are associated with depression (Beach 2014). The effects of marital functioning on health may also vary for men and women. Marital functioning may be more strongly related to mental health for men than women (Kiecolt-Glaser and Newton 2001) which can be attributed to gendered perceptions on the importance of social relationships (Kiecolt-Glaser and Newton 2001), gender roles in domestic work and childcare, and gender inequality which results in power differentials within marriages (Wanic and Kulik 2011).

In the present study, we seek to understand how the distribution of marital power in couples influences depression in gendered ways. We focus on Mexico as several societal-level characteristics make the context of marital power in Mexico unique. First, Mexico is a developing country and research on marriage and depression has tended to focus on developed countries. Second, Mexico has a strong history of traditional general roles that place much of the social and economic power in the hands of men (Stern 1997) which is especially pronounced among older cohorts of the population (Seedat et al. 2009). Based on the gender inequality index, Mexico ranks in the top half of countries in terms of gender inequality, but ranks lower than other Latin American and Caribbean countries (United Nations 2014). Third, Mexico is undergoing demographic changes that may influence family structure and function. There have been declines in the percentage of married individuals, rises in divorce (Organization for Economic Cooperation and Development 2016), more women entering the paid labour force (Organization for Economic Cooperation and Development 2004), and declines in adherence to traditional notions of gender roles (Seedat et al. 2009) which may shift gendered expectations of power within marriage. Finally, the strong reliance on spouses and family members for support and care in old age in Mexico (Villegas et al. 2014) can ultimately influence gendered power dynamics and the resulting relationship to mental health.

Our theoretical framework for understanding the gendered influence of marital power on depressive symptoms is rooted in equity theory. In general, equity theory argues that human relationships characterised by unequal exchange are distressing (Adams 1965). Hence, negative emotions may result from either under-benefiting or over-benefiting from imbalanced relationships (DeMaris et al. 2010). This is because under-benefiting may create feelings of unfairness while over-benefiting may create feelings of guilt. Applied to the context of marriage, previous research in the United States has found that women report lower marital power than men (Bulanda 2011; Kaufman and Taniguchi 2006), and that both husbands and wives tend to report lower levels of depression when household power is shared between spouses (Mirowsky 1985). While there is evidence that over-benefiting is associated with higher depressive symptoms (Longmore and Demaris 1997; Mirowsky 1985), under-benefiting may be more distressing than over-benefiting (Adams 1965; Longmore and Demaris 1997; Mirowsky 1985). Additionally, perceptions of marital inequality may be more relevant for women than men (DeMaris 2010).

Many previous investigations of equity theory have relied on younger samples that are more likely to be free of chronic conditions, functionally able, participating in the labour force, and raising children. While men may have more marital power in midlife due to greater earnings and participation in the formal labour sector, by late life, many men may have retired, developed health problems, and may rely on their wife for care (Bulanda 2011). In late life then, having less power in a marriage may be indicative of either “under-benefiting” from the marriage by having less power, or that one may rely on their spouse due to cognitive and physical pathologies/limitations. Therefore, power dynamics may shift and be more influenced by health status and reflect level of support or care provided by one spouse to aid the other in living independently. Previous research on older Mexican adults has found higher education to be related to greater marital power (particularly for women) while poor health was related to having less marital power (particularly for men) (Lührmann and Maurer 2008).

The Current Study

We investigate the association between marital power and depression in the context of a developing country, explicitly examining the simultaneous influences of one’s own characteristics and the characteristics of one’s spouse including background and physical health factors, and health dependency. We control for many important factors related to depression, including cognitive and physical health, socioeconomic position, and social factors including living arrangements and number of children (Cui et al. 2008). We seek to understand, using a dyadic approach, the ways in which the balance of marital power affects the depressive symptomatology of both the husband and the wife in the context of Mexico. The aims of this analysis are, first, to describe the distribution of marital power among older married adults in Mexico. Given the presence of traditional gender roles in Mexico, we hypothesise that husbands will report greater marital power than their wives. Second, we determine how distributions of marital power are associated with depressive symptoms among married adults in Mexico. Based on previous studies of equity theory, we hypothesise that depressive symptoms will be elevated in marriages with an imbalanced distribution of power. That is, we hypothesise a U-shaped relationship between marital power and depressive symptoms.

Data and methods

Data is drawn from the 2015 Wave of the Mexican Health and Aging Study (MHAS Mexican Health and Aging Study 2012). The MHAS is a large, household based, longitudinal, nationally representative study of older Mexican adults (age 50+) and their spouses (regardless of age) living in Mexico. The first wave of data collection began in 2001 (original cohort age 50+ in 2001). The sample was re-interviewed in 2003, 2012, and most recently in 2015. The MHAS added an additional cohort of respondents born 1952–1962 in 2012 to refresh the sample and regain representation of the population age 50+. The survey protocols of the MHAS are highly comparable with the United States Health and Retirement Study (HRS). The MHAS is partly sponsored by the National Institutes of Health/National Institute on Aging (grant number NIH R01AG018016). Data files and documentation are public use and available at www.MHASweb.org. The 2015 Wave of the MHAS includes 4,245 married couples. However, after excluding households with missing information on independent or dependent variables, our analytic sample consists of 3,621 dyads.

Marital Power

Marital power has been conceptualised in previous work as a spouse’s ability to influence the other, and to influence decisions that affect both members of the couple (Blood and Wolfe 1960; Mirowsky 1985). For this reason, we measure marital power using household decision-making dynamics. This is ascertained in the survey by asking both spouses, individually, whether he/she feels that one) his/her opinion on important family decisions is given more weight, two) both his/her and his/her spouse’s opinions are given equal weight, or three) his/her spouse’s opinion is given more weight.

Covariates

Demographic covariates in the analysis include rural/urban residence, age, sex, educational attainment, and economic well-being. Respondents are classified as urban if they live in a community with 100,000+ residents while those who reside in a community with fewer than 100,000 residents are classified as rural. Educational attainment is classified based on elementary education in Mexico. Respondents with zero years of education are classified as no education, those with one to five years of education are classified as incomplete elementary education, those with six years of education are classified as elementary education, while those with seven or more years of education are classified as beyond elementary education. We also include the difference in years of education between the spouses as the husband’s years of education minus the wife’s years of education to account for differences in levels of education which may influence household power distributions. We account for the relative age of the spouses by calculating the age difference of the couple as the husband’s age minus the wife’s age.

We measure economic well-being at the couple and individual level. First, we include a couple level count of owned consumer durables (including a radio, television, refrigerator, washing machine, telephone, water heater, internet, and computer) as income in late-life among older Mexicans is often quite low and previous studies have used consumer durables in dwellings as a proxy for socioeconomic status (Bollen et al. 2001). We also include individual level self-assessed financial situation. Respondents are asked: “Would you say your financial situation is…” one) excellent, two) very good, three) good, four) fair, or five) poor. Because few respondents report excellent or very good financial situations, we combine the excellent, very good, and good categories into one to create a three-level variable (good or better, fair, or poor financial situation). Self-assessed financial measures have been shown to be stronger predictors of health than income in prior work (Balabanova and McKee 2002; Gilmore et al. 2002), particularly in countries with large informal economies where income may not fully capture financial well-being (Gilmore et al. 2002) such as Mexico (Instituto Nacional de Estadística y Geografía 2017). We also include a categorical variable indicating whether each spouse is either retired, currently working, or has never worked for pay, as previous work has shown that labour force participation, primarily for women, may influence the distribution of power within marriages (Lührmann and Maurer 2008),

Previous research has suggested that household living arrangements and number of children may be associated with depression among older Mexican adults (Díaz-Venegas et al. 2017). Following this study, we calculate the total number of children ever born for each spouse and create a categorical variable indicating whether each spouse had zero to two children, three to four children, or five or more children. To capture living arrangements, we create binary variables indicating whether the couple had a child living in the household, and whether the couple had any other relatives besides children living in the household at the time of the survey. In sensitivity analyses (not shown) we also controlled for the number of persons in the household (as well as squared and cubic terms), but none of these measures were associated with the husband’s or wife’s depressive symptoms and, thus, were not included in our final regression results.

We also include a count of chronic conditions as a measure of health status based on self-reported hypertension, cancer, diabetes, stroke, heart attack, and respiratory conditions. To capture the physical functionality of respondents, we include the presence of any Activities of Daily Living (ADL) (Katz et al. 1963) and Instrumental Activities of Daily Living (IADL) limitations. ADLs are assessed by reporting trouble dressing, bathing, eating, getting out of bed, or using the toilet. Following the example of previous research, respondents who report problems performing the activity, not being able to perform the activity, or receiving help performing the activity are classified as having a limitation while those who report no problems performing the activity are considered disability-free (Díaz-Venegas et al. 2015). IADLs are assessed by difficulty preparing meals, shopping, taking medications, or managing money. We also include two measures of cognitive function including verbal learning (respondents immediately recall a list of eight-words three times, the average number of words recalled correctly across trials is calculated) and verbal recall (the respondent recalls the eight-word list after a delay).

Depressive Symptoms

Our outcome variable, depressive symptoms, is measured using a nine-item version of the Center for Epidemiologic Studies - Depression Scale (CES-D) (Radloff 1977). Study participants were asked whether they experienced the following symptoms of depression in the previous week: one) felt depressed, two) felt that everything [he/she] did was an effort, three) felt [his/her] sleep was restless, four) felt happy, five) felt lonely, six) felt that [he/she] enjoyed life, seven) felt sad, eight) felt tired, nine) felt that [he/she] had a lot of energy. Positive items are reverse coded such that the sum of depressive symptoms ranges from zero to nine with higher values representing a greater level of depressive symptomatology. The validity of the nine-item CES-D scale among older Mexican adults has been established in previous work (Aguilar-Navarro et al. 2007).

Statistical Analysis

Because we analyse couples which produce two outcome variables (depressive symptoms for husbands and wives), we use Seemingly Unrelated Regression (SUR) (Zellner 1962). SUR allows the error terms of models to correlate to improve estimation efficiency as knowledge about the error term of one equation should reduce the predicted value of the error term in the other equation if error terms are correlated. This method takes into account unobserved factors which are shared by husbands and wives at the household level which may affect depressive symptoms for both spouses (Siegel et al. 2004).

In Model 1, we regress the husband’s and wife’s depressive symptoms as a function of each spouse’s own perception of marital power, own age, own education, own employment history, own number of children ever born, and own self-assessed financial situation. We also include couple level controls including whether the couple lives in an urban or rural area, household count of consumer durables, age difference of couple, difference in years of education between spouses, whether the couple has children living at home, and whether the couple has other relatives living at home. In Model 2, we add one’s own chronic condition count, ADL limitations, IADL limitations, verbal learning, and verbal recall scores to assess whether differences in depressive symptoms across levels of marital power can be attributed to differences in health status, physical functioning, and cognitive function. In Model 3, we allow depressive symptoms to vary as a function of the spouse’s chronic conditions, ADL, IADL, verbal learning, and verbal recall. While we tested the use of negative-binomial and Poisson models of depressive symptoms, we report the results from Seemingly Unrelated Linear Regressions to facilitate interpretability as results were similar across estimation methods. In sensitivity analyses (not shown), we also dichotomised depressive symptoms using cut-points established in previous work (Aguilar-Navarro et al. 2007) and obtained similar results. To determine whether our results were robust to missing data, we also estimated our models using full information maximum likelihood (FIML), allowing us to include all 4,245 married couples, and obtained similar results.

Results

Descriptive Results

We begin by presenting the cross-tabulation of the husband’s report of marital power versus the wife’s report of marital power in Table 1. The most common type of household was one in which both the husband and the wife reported that both spouses shared decision-making power equally (40.8 per cent). However, there was evidence of a more patriarchal power structure among the households. Of the husbands, 26.1 per cent reported having more marital power than the wife while only 10.5 per cent reported having less power than the wife. For wives on the other hand, only 19.9 per cent reported more marital power than the husband while 24.8 per cent reported less power than the husband. While 55.3 per cent of wives reported an equal distribution of power, 63.4 per cent of husbands reported equal power between spouses. Approximately 57.5 per cent of households were in perfect agreement about the distribution of power (both report equality or both spouses agree on which spouse has more power), 5.4 per cent were in perfect disagreement (both spouses report having more power or both spouses report having less power), while the remaining 37.1 per cent were in between (one spouse reports an equal distribution of power while the other reports either themself or their spouse to have either more or less power). On average, the husbands in our sample were 4.3 years older and had 0.7 more years of education than their wives. The mean number of depressive symptoms among husbands was 2.6 (inter-quartile range [IQR]: 1–4) while the mean number of depressive symptoms for wives was 3.5 (IQR: 1–6).

Table 1.

Prevalence of household types based on husbands’ and wives’ reports of which spouse has more marital power.

Wife Report

Husband Report More Equal Less Total

More 148 389 409 946
4.1% 10.7% 11.3% 26.1%
Equal 377 1,477 441 2,295
10.4% 40.8% 12.2% 63.4%
Less 195 136 49 380
5.4% 3.8% 1.4% 10.5%
Total 720 2,002 899 3,621
19.9% 55.3% 24.8% 100.0%

Note: authors’ own calculations using data from the 2015 Mexican Health and Aging Study (n=3,621 dyads).

We then present the sociodemographic, psychological, and health characteristics of husbands and wives by their report of which spouse has more power in Table 2. Results for the husbands are shown on the left-hand side of Table 2. Husbands who reported less (mean: 3.3) or more (mean: 2.8) power than the wife reported more depressive symptoms than those who reported an equal distribution of power (mean: 2.4). Husbands were most likely to report an equal distribution of power across all levels of education. Husbands who reported either an ADL or an IADL limitation were less likely to report an equal distribution of power compared to their disability-free counterparts. Among husbands with no ADL limitation, 26.0 per cent and 9.5 per cent reported more and less power, respectively. However, these percentages increase to 27.1 per cent and 16.8 per cent among husbands reporting an ADL limitation. A similar trend was observed for IADL limitations. Husbands who reported an equal distribution of power reported slightly fewer chronic conditions.

TABLE 2.

Sociodemographic, psychosocial, and health characteristics of husbands and wives by own report of who has more marital power.

Husbands Wives


Own report of marital power: More Equal Less Sig More Equal Less Sig


Depressive symptoms (mean, SD) 2.8 2.3 2.4 2.2 3.2 2.6 *** 4.0 2.8 3.3 2.6 3.9 2.7 ***
Educational attainment
No education (n, %) 152 26.6 352 61.6 67 11.7 ** 115 19.1 319 52.9 169 28.0 ***
Incomplete elementary (n, %) 332 29.1 704 61.6 106 9.3 205 17.6 609 52.3 350 30.1
Elementary (n, %) 268 29.3 572 62.4 76 8.3 199 20.6 500 51.8 267 27.6
Beyond elementary (n, %) 346 24.1 917 63.9 172 12.0 327 24.6 785 59.0 219 16.5
Urban
Lives in a more rural area (n, %) 504 27.4 1,188 64.6 148 8.0 *** 316 17.2 1,033 56.1 491 26.7 ***
Lives in a more urban area (n, %) 594 26.7 1,357 61.0 273 12.3 530 23.8 1,180 53.1 514 23.1
Age (mean, SD) 66.0 9.1 65.9 8.8 67.0 9.6 *** 61.5 9.5 61.1 9.5 61.8 9.5 ***
Consumer durables (mean, SD) 5.3 2.0 5.6 1.9 5.8 1.9 5.6 1.9 5.6 2.0 5.3 1.9
Employment history
Retired (n, %) 414 26.0 996 62.4 185 11.6 364 21.6 911 54.1 410 24.3 ***
Currently working (n, %) 674 27.7 1,529 62.9 229 9.4 305 28.6 575 53.9 187 17.5
Never worked (n, %) 10 27.0 20 54.1 7 18.9 177 13.5 727 55.4 408 31.1
Fertility history
0–2 children (n, %) 193 25.5 490 64.8 73 9.7 152 18.9 474 59.0 178 22.1 **
3–4 children (n, %) 365 26.3 867 62.4 158 11.4 331 22.4 811 55.0 333 22.6
5+ children (n, %) 540 28.2 1,188 61.9 190 9.9 363 20.3 928 52.0 494 27.7
Self-assessed financial situation
Good or better (n, %) 259 26.6 621 63.8 93 9.6 195 18.7 612 58.7 235 22.6 ***
Fair (n, %) 699 26.4 1,672 63.1 277 10.5 537 20.1 1,453 54.4 682 25.5
Poor (n, %) 140 31.6 252 56.9 51 11.5 114 32.6 148 42.3 88 25.1
Children living at home
No kids at home (n, %) 390 27.2 900 62.7 146 10.2 269 18.7 764 53.2 403 28.1 ***
Kids at home (n, %) 708 26.9 1,645 62.6 275 10.5 577 22.0 1,449 55.1 602 22.9
Relatives living at home
No relatives at home (n, %) 743 27.4 1,701 62.8 263 9.7 ** 528 19.5 1,500 55.4 679 25.1 *
Relatives at home (n, %) 355 26.2 844 62.2 158 11.6 318 23.4 713 52.5 326 24.0
Chronic condition count (mean, SD) 1.0 1.0 0.9 0.9 1.0 1.0 1.2 1.0 1.1 1.0 1.2 1.0
ADL limitation
No ADL limitation (n, %) 932 26.6 2,241 64.1 325 9.3 *** 679 20.0 1,892 55.7 824 24.3 **
ADL limitation (n, %) 166 29.3 304 53.7 96 17.0 167 25.0 321 48.0 181 27.1
IADL limitation
No IADL limitation (n, %) 1,009 26.8 2,388 63.4 367 9.8 *** 746 20.6 2,010 55.5 866 23.9 ***
IADL limitation (n, %) 89 29.7 157 52.3 54 18.0 100 22.6 203 45.9 139 31.4
Verbal learning (mean, SD) 4.6 1.2 4.7 1.2 4.6 1.3 * 5.1 1.1 5.1 1.1 4.9 1.2 ***
Verbal recall (mean, SD) 3.8 2.1 4.0 2.1 3.7 2.2 * 4.8 2.0 4.8 1.9 4.5 2.1 **

Note: authors' own calculations using data from the 2015 Mexican Health and Aging Study (n=4,064 dyads). All percentages are row percentages calculated for husbands and wives, separately.

Sig represents whether differences in independent and dependent variables by own report of marital power were significant at *: p<0.05,

**

: p<0.01,

***

: <0.001.

ADL = activity of daily living limitation. IADL = instrumental activity of daily living limitation.

We then present the sociodemographic, psychological, and health characteristics of the wives by their report of which spouse has more power in the right-hand side of Table 2. Wives who reported either less (mean: 3.8) or more power (mean: 3.9) than the husband reported more depressive symptoms than those who reported an equal distribution of power (mean: 3.2). While wives were most likely to report an equal distribution of power across all levels of education, the percent reporting more power than the husband generally increased with increasing level of education. While 17.3 per cent of wives with no education reported more power than the husband, this increased to 23.8 per cent among those with beyond an elementary education. Wives who reported an ADL or an IADL limitation were also less likely to report an equal distribution of power. Among wives with no ADL limitation, 19.1 per cent and 24.4 per cent reported more and less power than the husband, respectively. However, these percentages increase to 23.8 per cent and 26.9 per cent among wives reporting an ADL limitation. Among wives with no IADL limitation, 19.6 per cent and 24.1 per cent reported more and less power than the husband, respectively. However, these percentages increase to 22.0 per cent and 30.8 per cent among wives reporting an IADL limitation. Wives who reported an equal distribution of power were slightly younger. For wives, less marital power was associated with reports of no employment history and having more children. Further, marital power was associated with worse self-assessed for financial situation, having children or relatives living in the home, and cognitive function.

Regression Results: Depressive Symptoms of Husbands

We then present the results of our SUR models in Tables 3. Estimates for the husband’s and wife's depressive symptoms are shown in the left and right sides of the table, respectively. It should be noted that Models 1–3 for husbands and wives are estimated simultaneously. Focusing first on the husbands, those who reported having either less or more power than the wife reported more depressive symptoms than those who reported an equal distribution of marital power in Model 1. Husbands who reported having less power than the wife reported the highest level of depressive symptomatology. Having fewer consumer durables, having less education, having five or more (compared to zero to two) children, and reporting a fair or poor (compared to good or better) financial situation were associated with higher depressive symptomatology.

TABLE 3.

Seemingly unrelated regression of depressive symptoms for married adults

Husband's depressive symptoms Wife's depressive symptoms
Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 1 Model 2 Model 3

Marital power (ref: equal power) β1 SE2 β SE β SE β SE β SE β SE

More 0.29*** (0.08) 0.22** (0.07) 0.21** (0.07) 0.50*** (0.10) 0.43*** (0.10) 0.41*** (0.10)
Less 0.74*** (0.11) 0.53*** (0.11) 0.53*** (0.11) 0.37*** (0.10) 0.32*** (0.09) 0.32*** (0.09)
Education (ref: beyond elementary)
No education 0.68*** (0.14) 0.36** (0.14) 0.32* (0.14) 0.47** (0.17) 0.21 (0.16) 0.14 (0.17)
Incomplete elementary 0.58*** (0.11) 0.40*** (0.11) 0.37*** (0.11) 0.32* (0.13) 0.12 (0.13) 0.08 (0.13)
Complete elementary 0.32** (0.10) 0.24* (0.10) 0.22* (0.10) 0.32** (0.12) 0.16 (0.11) 0.14 (0.11)
Demographics
More urban (ref: more rural) 0.02 (0.08) −0.02 (0.07) −0.02 (0.07) 0.09 (0.09) 0.05 (0.09) 0.04 (0.09)
Age 0.00 (0.00) −0.01* (0.00) −0.01* (0.00) 0.00 (0.01) −0.03*** (0.01) −0.03*** (0.01)
Age difference3 −0.01 (0.01) −0.01 (0.01) −0.01 (0.01) −0.01 (0.01) −0.02* (0.01) −0.02** (0.01)
Education difference4 0.01 (0.01) 0.01 (0.01) 0.01 (0.01) −0.01 (0.01) −0.00 (0.01) 0.00 (0.01)
Consumer durables −0.12*** (0.02) −0.10*** (0.02) −0.09*** (0.02) −0.15*** (0.03) −0.13*** (0.03) −0.12*** (0.03)
Employment history (ref: retired)
Currently working −0.45*** (0.07) −0.12 (0.07) −0.12 (0.07) −0.23* (0.10) −0.10 (0.10) −0.11 (0.10)
Never worked −0.06 (0.35) −0.04 (0.33) −0.03 (0.33) −0.18 (0.10) −0.16 (0.09) −0.16 (0.09)
Fertility history (ref: 0–2 children)
3–4 children 0.18 (0.10) 0.21* (0.09) 0.21* (0.09) 0.20 (0.11) 0.14 (0.11) 0.14 (0.11)
5+ children 0.33*** (0.10) 0.31** (0.10) 0.30** (0.10) 0.39** (0.12) 0.25* (0.12) 0.25* (0.12)
Self-assessed financial situation (ref: good)
Fair 0.48*** (0.08) 0.42*** (0.08) 0.41*** (0.08) 0.80*** (0.10) 0.71*** (0.09) 0.69*** (0.09)
Poor 1.68*** (0.13) 1.40*** (0.12) 1.39*** (0.12) 2.07*** (0.16) 1.72*** (0.16) 1.67*** (0.16)
Living arrangements
Kids at home 0.04 (0.08) 0.04 (0.07) 0.03 (0.07) −0.17 (0.09) −0.14 (0.09) −0.14 (0.09)
Relatives at home −0.03 (0.07) −0.05 (0.07) −0.05 (0.07) 0.20* (0.09) 0.13 (0.09) 0.12 (0.09)
Own health variables
Chronic condition count 0.29*** (0.04) 0.30*** (0.04) 0.37*** (0.04) 0.36*** (0.04)
ADL5 1.06*** (0.10) 1.11*** (0.10) 1.29*** (0.12) 1.26*** (0.12)
IADL6 0.99*** (0.13) 1.00*** (0.14) 0.70*** (0.14) 0.75*** (0.14)
Verbal learning −0.16*** (0.03) −0.16*** (0.04) −0.11* (0.04) −0.12** (0.04)
Verbal recall 0.02 (0.02) 0.02 (0.02) −0.05 (0.02) −0.04 (0.02)
Spouse's health variables
Chronic condition count −0.02 (0.03) 0.07 (0.04)
ADL 0.01 (0.10) 0.33** (0.13)
IADL 0.23 (0.12) 0.15 (0.16)
Verbal learning −0.04 (0.04) −0.05 (0.04)
Verbal recall 0.00 (0.02) 0.01 (0.02)

Note: Authors’ own calculations using data from the 2015 Mexican Health and Aging Study (n=4,064 dyads).

*

denotes p<0.05,

**

denotes p<0.01,

***

denotes p<0.001.

1

β indicates parameter estimate.

2

SE indicates standard error.

3

Age difference = husband’s age minus wife’s age.

4

Education difference = husband's years of education minus wife's years of education.

5

ADL = activity of daily living limitation.

6

IADL = instrumental activity of daily living limitation.

We add own health characteristics (chronic condition count, ADL limitation, IADL limitation, verbal learning, and verbal recall) in Model 2 to determine whether differences in the husband’s depressive symptoms across the marital power distribution were explained by his health characteristics. For husbands, reporting more chronic conditions, an ADL or IADL limitation, and scoring lower on the verbal learning task were associated with elevated depressive symptomatology. Reporting either more or less power remained statistically significant predictors of elevated depressive symptomatology, although both parameter estimates decreased after accounting for the health variables. In the full model (Model 3), we add cross-spouse effects by adding the wife’s health conditions to the model. The inclusion of the cross-spouse effects did not affect the size or statistical significance of the marital power parameters and none of the wife’s health characteristics were associated with the husband’s depressive symptomatology. In sensitivity analyses (results not shown), we changed the reference group for marital power to reporting more power than the wife. Across models, having less (compared to more) marital power than the wife was associated with elevated depressive symptomatology for husbands.

Regression Results: Depressive Symptoms of Wives

We then shift our focus to the models of the wife’s depressive symptoms which are shown in the right-hand side of Table 3. Similar to the husbands, reporting either more or less power than the husband (compared to equal) was associated with elevated depressive symptomatology for wives. In contrast to the husbands, wives who reported more power than the husband tended to report the highest levels of depression. Similar to husbands, having fewer consumer durables, lower education, having five or more (compared to zero to two) children, and reporting a fair or poor (compared to good or better) financial situation were associated with elevated depressive symptomatology.

We then add own health conditions in Model 2. Having more chronic conditions, having an ADL or IADL limitation, and poorer performance on the verbal learning task were associated with elevated depressive symptomatology. While in Model 1, having relatives living in the household and having five or more (compared to zero to two children) were associated with higher depressive symptomatology, these parameters lost statistical significance when we added own health conditions in Model 2. Further, the negative association between education and depression for wives lost statistical significance after accounting for own health conditions. Regarding marital power, reporting either more or less power than the husband remained statistically significant predictors of depression for wives, even after accounting for own health conditions in Model 2. The marital power-depression association remained statistically significant even in Model 3 when spousal health conditions were added to the model. Interestingly, while none of the wife’s health conditions were significant predictors of depression for husbands, having a husband with an ADL limitation was associated with the wife’s depression. In sensitivity analyses, we changed the reference group for marital power to having less power than the husband. Across models, having more (compared to less) marital power was not associated with elevated depression.

Discussion

In this analysis, we explored the association between marital power, measured through household decision-making power, and depressive symptoms using a dyadic approach. We find that about 41 per cent of married couples in Mexico report an equitable distribution of marital power (both spouses reported equal power). Consistent with equity theory (Longmore and Demaris 1997; Mirowsky 1985), both husbands and wives who reported that their marriage involved an equal distribution of power reported the fewest depressive symptoms. Therefore, depressive symptomatology was significantly higher among married adults who reported having either more or less power than their spouse. This supports prior studies outside of Mexico that find that egalitarian marriages tend to be associated with better mental health (Longmore and Demaris 1997; Mirowsky 1985). In terms of concordance, we find that over half of the couples in the MHAS agreed upon the power dynamics of their relationship.

We also note other important differences by gender. Married men were more likely than married women to report an equal balance of marital power. However, married women (25 per cent) were much more likely than married men (11 per cent) to report having less power in the marriage. We also found gender differences in the influence of marital power on individual mental health. Primarily, for men having less marital power than the wife was significantly more distressing than having more marital power. Contrastingly, for women, having less marital power was not significantly more distressing than having more marital power than the husband. In fact, for wives, having more marital power seemed to be most distressing (although the difference between having more versus less marital power for wives was not statistically significant).

It is not surprising that depression was higher among those reporting either more or less power than their spouse. Previous work using younger samples suggests that under-benefiting in relationships creates feelings of unfairness while over-benefiting may lead to feelings of guilt. This perspective may be applicable for our current analysis. Spouses who feel that their opinion on important family decisions is not given weight may perceive the situation as unjust, which may ultimately affect depression (Longmore and Demaris 1997). On the other hand, spouses who report more power may have higher levels of depression because they feel guilty about the relative lack of power of their spouse. Treating a spouse unfairly also contradicts social norms and may elicit negative reactions from the under-benefited spouse and their loved ones (Mirowsky 1985). Having more decision-making power is also not necessarily positive. Certain decision-making activities such as financial decisions may elicit stress (Starcke and Brand 2012) which is associated with depression (Hammen 2005). Consequently, depression may be higher for both spouses in marriages with an unequal power distribution.

Among older adults, however, marital power may be more dependent on the abilities and disabilities of partners (Bulanda 2011; Lührmann and Maurer 2008). Married adults may report that their spouse has more influence on important family decisions because they may rely on their spouse to make decisions due to their own physical or cognitive limitations. Hence, higher levels of depression may stem from the negative psychological effects of loss of autonomy and independence (Bruce 2001; Yang and George 2005). On the other hand, spouses who report more influence on family decisions may have taken this role due to the physical and cognitive limitations of his/her spouse. If married adults with more power are serving as a caregiver for their spouse, the stress associated with caregiving may influence their depressive symptomatology (Beeson 2003).

Health dependency may also explain gender differences we note in the influence of marital power on depression. The husbands in our sample were, on average, 4.3 years older than their wives Life expectancy for males also lags behind females in Mexico, where women can expect to live nearly six years longer than men as of 2015 (World Health Organization 2016b). This suggests that wives are likely to outlive their husbands. Gender differences in both life expectancy and age at marriage, combined with traditional notions of gender divisions in care work, indicate that more wives may then serve as caregivers and provide assistance to their husbands in his final years of life. The finding that husbands with less marital power than the wife reported the highest levels of depression may be explained, in part, by his loss of autonomy throughout health deterioration and the resulting changes in marital power. Some evidence for this can be provided by the declining parameter estimate for the husband’s report of less marital power after accounting for his own health, including physical and cognitive functioning. Research on caregiving also shows that wives tend to be the sole or primary caregivers for their husbands, while husbands rely on adult children and others for help with caregiving for their spouses (Feld et al. 2010). For wives, on the other hand, elevated depression among those reporting more marital power may be attributed to the negative effects of caregiving and more care being provided to their husbands as their health fails. Further, previous research has suggested that caregiving may be more detrimental for women’s physical and mental health (Kaufman and Taniguchi 2006; Miller 1990; Pinquart and Sörensen 2006). For wives’ depressive symptoms, husbands’ ADL disability mattered while none of the wife’s health conditions mattered for the husband’s depressive symptoms. Wives may then be more likely to serve as a caregiver throughout the course of disability, and may experience greater distress due to this role, contributing to the gender differences we note. Gendered patterns in intensity and type of caregiving may be implicated in our findings and we suggest this area for future research.

Gender differences may also be explained by societal norms and expectations of marital power. Traditional gender roles (Stern 1997), as well as social structural factors leading to gender disparities in income and formal labour force participation in Mexico (Hausmann 2009), may lead to gendered expectations of a patriarchal distribution of marital power (Bernard 1981; Ferree 1990; Halloran 1998; Sussman et al. 1999; Tichenor 2005). For husbands who carry a more traditional gender role ideology, having less power than the wife may be particularly distressing as this may conflict with his expectations of power within marriages (Hyde 2016). For wives on the other hand, having more power than the husband may lead to more depression as this violation of gender roles may elicit negative reactions from friends and relatives who may attempt to “correct” this (Kemper 1977). Although our results provide some evidence of a patriarchal power structure among Mexican couples, it should be noted that both husbands and wives were most likely to report an equal distribution of power. Further, shared decision-making among Mexican couples was common as far back as 1966–1967 (de Leñero 1969), 1973 (Cromwell et al. 1973), and more recently in 1992 (Oropesa 1997). Gender roles are also becoming less traditional in more recent cohorts in Mexico, and decreases in society level gender role traditionality have been associated with shrinking gender gaps in the prevalence of major depressive disorder (Seedat et al. 2009). Given these demographic trends, it remains to be seen whether future cohorts of older Mexican adults will demonstrate similar gender differences in the associations between marital power and depressive symptoms.

Our study comes with several limitations. The construct we aim to measure (marital power) is quite broad, and we assess it using a single item (household balance of decision-making power). While respondents are asked to report which spouse’s opinion on important family decisions is given more weight, there are many types of decisions including economic, social, health and family planning, among others. It is possible that while one spouse may have greater influence on certain types of decisions, he/she may have less influence on other types and some types may be more consequential than others. Further, the influence of each spouse on individual decision types is distributed in a gendered way, and we cannot explore how domain specific household power imbalances influence psychosocial health. Additionally, we cannot detect “hidden power” among the couples in our sample. Hidden power is observed when spouses (particularly wives) change their responses to agree with the other spouse as a result of observing the response of the other spouse, which has been observed in previous studies (Komter 1989; Zipp et al. 2004; Zipp and Toth 2002). While MHAS interviews are, ideally, conducted alone (Lührmann and Maurer 2008), there are likely to be many cases in which the respondent’s spouse or relatives were present at the time of the interview. The presence of one’s spouse or relatives may then influence one’s report of the household balance of decision-making power towards a more socially acceptable response.

Notwithstanding these limitations, our study comes with several strengths. First, the MHAS sample is large, nationally representative, and contains many dyads for analysis. Second, the household based nature of the MHAS provides the opportunity to utilise both the husband and the wife’s characteristics in models of depressive symptoms. This also affords us the opportunity to investigate cross-spouse effects to better understand how health and functional limitation influence psychosocial well-being at the household level. Third, the collection of data across a variety of domains in the MHAS allows us to test a broad range of covariates including sociodemographic factors, chronic conditions, functional limitations, living arrangements, and mental health.

This research has several policy and public health implications. First, our study helps to identify older married adults that are at risk for depression. This is especially important as depression is prevalent among older adults and is likely to be undiagnosed. Second, although we did not directly measure caregiving in our analyses, our results infer the need for more focus on older adults with physical and cognitive limitations, as well as spousal caregivers. Third, although education among women is increasing (Wong and Palloni 2009) and traditional gender roles may be relaxing (Seedat et al. 2009) in Mexico, our descriptive results still point to a patriarchal power structure among older married adults in Mexico. Future policy efforts should push for greater education of women and enhancing the influence of women on household decisions. Last, our results suggest that egalitarian marriages are beneficial for the mental health of married adults. Therefore, interventions to promote egalitarian marriages may then aid in reducing the burden of depression among older adults in Mexico.

Understanding the factors that influence depressive symptomatology among older Mexican adults is becoming increasingly important given the rapid ageing of the Mexican population, and the considerable health, social, and economic costs associated with depression among aged adults. While many studies report lower levels of depressive symptoms among married adults compared to their non-married counterparts, we explored the heterogeneity among married adults in Mexico. While a large portion of respondents reported being in a marriage with shared decision-making power, depressive symptomatology for both spouses was higher among those residing in households with an unequal distribution of power. Future work should continue to examine the characteristics of marriages that are protective or problematic for the mental health of the older population in developing countries experiencing rapid demographic change.

Acknowledgments

This study was conducted with the support of the Davis School of Gerontology at the University of Southern California, and by the Multidisciplinary Research Training in Gerontology Training Grant (T32AG000037) from the National Institutes of Health/National Institute on Aging.

The research reported in this publication was supported by the National Institute on Aging of the National Institutes of Health under Award Number P30AG043073. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health.

The MHAS is partly sponsored by the National Institutes of Health/National Institute on Aging (grant number NIH R01AG018016). Data files and documentation are public use and available at www.MHASweb.org.

Footnotes

Ethical approval

The Mexican Health and Aging Study was approved by the Institutional Review Boards or Ethics Committees of the University of Texas Medical Branch in the United States, the Instituto Nacional de Estadistica y Geografia (INEGI) and the Instituto Nacional de Salud Publica (INSP) in Mexico.

References

  1. Adams JS. Inequity In Social Exchange. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology. 1965;2:267–99. [Google Scholar]
  2. Aguilar-Navarro SG, Fuentes-Cantú A, Ávila-Funes JA, García-Mayo EJ. Validity and reliability of the screening questionnaire for geriatric depression used in the Mexican Health and Age Study. Salud Pública de México. 2007;49(4):256–62. doi: 10.1590/s0036-36342007000400005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Alexopoulos GS. Depression in the elderly. The Lancet. 2005;365(9475):1961–70. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(05)66665-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Angel JL, Vega W, López-Ortega M. Aging in Mexico: Population Trends and Emerging Issues. The Gerontologist. 2017;57(2):153–62. doi: 10.1093/geront/gnw136. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Balabanova DC, McKee Self-reported health in Bulgaria: levels and determinants. Scandinavian Journal of Public Health. 2002;30(4):306–12. doi: 10.1080/14034940210164867. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Beach S. The Couple and Family Discord Model of Depression. Oxford University Press; 2014. [Google Scholar]
  7. Beeson RA. Loneliness and depression in spousal caregivers of those with Alzheimer’s disease versus non-caregiving spouses. Archives of Psychiatric Nursing. 2003;17(3):135–43. doi: 10.1016/s0883-9417(03)00057-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bernard J. The good-provider role: Its rise and fall. American Psychologist. 1981;36(1):1–12. [Google Scholar]
  9. Blood R, Wolfe D. Husbands and wives: the dynamics of married living 1960 [Google Scholar]
  10. Bollen KA, Glanville JL, Stecklov G. Socioeconomic Status and Class in Studies of Fertility and Health in Developing Countries. Annual Review of Sociology. 2001;27(1):153–85. [Google Scholar]
  11. Bruce ML. Depression and Disability in Late Life: Directions for Future Research. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. 2001;9(2):102–12. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Bulanda JR. Gender, Marital Power, and Marital Quality in Later Life. Journal of Women & Aging. 2011;23(1):3–22. doi: 10.1080/08952841.2011.540481. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Cromwell RE, Corrales R, Torsiello PM. Normative Patterns of Marital Decision Making Power and Influence in Mexico and the United States: A Partial Test of Resource and Ideology Theory. Journal of Comparative Family Studies; Calgary, Alta. 1973;4(2):177–96. [Google Scholar]
  14. Cui X, Lyness JM, Tang W, Tu X, Conwell Y. Outcomes and Predictors of Late-Life Depression Trajectories in Older Primary Care Patients. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. 2008;16(5):406–15. doi: 10.1097/JGP.0b013e3181693264. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. DeMaris A. The 20-year trajectory of marital quality in enduring marriages: Does equity matter? Journal of Social and Personal Relationships. 2010;27(4):449–71. doi: 10.1177/0265407510363428. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. DeMaris A, Mahoney A, Pargament KI. Sanctification of marriage and general religiousness as buffers of the effects of marital inequity. Journal of Family Issues. 2010;31(10):1255–78. doi: 10.1177/0192513x10363888. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Díaz-Venegas C, De La Vega S, Wong R. Transitions in activities of daily living in Mexico, 2001–2012. Salud Pública de México. 2015;57:s54–s61. doi: 10.21149/spm.v57s1.7590. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Díaz-Venegas C, Sáenz JL, Wong R. Family size and old age well-being: Effects of the fertility transition in Mexico. Ageing and Society. 2017;37(3):495–516. doi: 10.1017/S0144686X15001221. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Feld S, Dunkle RE, Schroepfer T, Shen H-W. Does Gender Moderate Factors Associated with Whether Spouses Are the Sole Providers of IADL Care to Their Partners? Research on Aging. 2010;32(4):499–526. doi: 10.1177/0164027510361461. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Ferree MM. Beyond Separate Spheres: Feminism and Family Research. Journal of Marriage and the Family; Minneapolis. 1990;52(4):866. [Google Scholar]
  21. García-Peña C, Wagner FA, Sánchez-Garcia S, Juárez-Cedillo T, Espinel-Bermúdez C, García-Gonzalez JJ, Gallegos-Carrillo K, Franco-Marina F, Gallo JJ. Depressive Symptoms Among Older Adults in Mexico City. Journal of General Internal Medicine. 2008;23(12):1973–80. doi: 10.1007/s11606-008-0799-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Gilmore ABC, McKee M, Rose R. Determinants of and inequalities in self-perceived health in Ukraine. Social Science & Medicine. 2002;55(12):2177–88. doi: 10.1016/s0277-9536(01)00361-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Gore S, Mangione TW. Social Roles, Sex Roles and Psychological Distress: Additive and Interactive Models of Sex Differences. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 1983;24(4):300–312. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Gove WR, Hughes M, Style CB. Does Marriage Have Positive Effects on the Psychological Well-Being of the Individual? Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 1983;24(2):122–31. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Halloran EC. The role of marital power in depression and marital distress. The American Journal of Family Therapy. 1998;26(1):3–14. [Google Scholar]
  26. Hammen C. Stress and Depression. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology. 2005;1(1):293–319. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.1.102803.143938. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Hausmann R. The Global Gender Gap Report 2009. World Economic Forum; 2009. [Google Scholar]
  28. Hyde JS. Gender and Couple Relationships. National Symposium on Family Issues; Springer, Cham: 2016. Women, Men, Work, and Family: Expansionist Theory Updated; pp. 93–109. [Google Scholar]
  29. Instituto Nacional de Estadística y Geografía. Resultados de la encuesta nacional de ocupación y empleo. Cifras durante el cuarto trimestre de 2016 2017 [Google Scholar]
  30. Katz S, Ford AB, Moskowitz RW, Jackson BA, Jaffe MW. Studies of Illness in the Aged: The Index of ADL: A Standardized Measure of Biological and Psychosocial Function. JAMA. 1963;185(12):914–19. doi: 10.1001/jama.1963.03060120024016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Kaufman G, Taniguchi H. Gender and Marital Happiness in Later Life. Journal of Family Issues. 2006;27(6):735–57. [Google Scholar]
  32. Kemper RV. Migration and Adaptation: Tzintzuntzan Peasants in Mexico City. Sage: 1977. [Google Scholar]
  33. Kessler RC, Essex M. Marital Status and Depression: The Importance of Coping Resources. Social Forces. 1982;61(2):484–507. [Google Scholar]
  34. Kiecolt-Glaser JK, Newton TL. Marriage and Health: His and Hers. Psychological Bulletin. 2001;127(4):472–503. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.127.4.472. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Komter A. Hidden Power in Marriage. Gender & Society. 1989;3(2):187–216. [Google Scholar]
  36. Lebowitz BD, Pearson JL, Schneider LS, Reynolds CF, Alexopoulos GS, Bruce ML, Conwell Y, Katz IR, Meyers BS, Morrison MF, Mossey J, Niederehe G, Parmelee P. Diagnosis and Treatment of Depression in Late Life: Consensus Statement Update. JAMA. 1997;278(14):1186–90. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. De Leñero M, del CE. ? Hacia dónde va la mujer Mexicana?:(resultados de una encuesta nacional) Instituto Mexicano de Estudios Sociales, AC; 1969. [Google Scholar]
  38. Lewinsohn PM, Rohde P, Seeley JR, Fischer SA. Age and depression: Unique and shared effects. Psychology and Aging. 1991;6(2):247–60. doi: 10.1037//0882-7974.6.2.247. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Longmore MA, Demaris A. Perceived Inequity and Depression in Intimate Relationships: The Moderating Effect of Self-Esteem. Social Psychology Quarterly. 1997;60(2):172–84. [Google Scholar]
  40. Lührmann M, Maurer J. Working paper. Mannheim: 2008. Who wears the trousers? A semiparametric analysis of decision power in couples. [Google Scholar]
  41. Medina-Mora ME, Borges G, Lara C, Benjet C, Blanco J, Fleiz C, Villatoro J, Rojas E, Zambrano J. Prevalence, service use, and demographic correlates of 12-month DSM-IV psychiatric disorders in Mexico: results from the Mexican National Comorbidity Survey. Psychological Medicine. 2005;35(12):1773–83. doi: 10.1017/S0033291705005672. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. MHAS Mexican Health and Aging Study. Data Files and Documentation (public use): Mexican Health and Aging Study, Wave 3 2012 [Google Scholar]
  43. Miller B. Gender Differences in Spouse Caregiver Strain: Socialization and Role Explanations. Journal of Marriage and the Family; Minneapolis. 1990;52(2):311–21. [Google Scholar]
  44. Mirowsky J. Depression and Marital Power: An Equity Model. American Journal of Sociology. 1985;91(3):557–92. [Google Scholar]
  45. Organization for Economic Cooperation and Development. Female Labour Force Participation: Past Trends and Main Determinants in OECD Countries 2004 [Google Scholar]
  46. Organization for Economic Cooperation and Development. Marriage and Divorce Rates 2016 [Google Scholar]
  47. Oropesa RS. Development and Marital Power in Mexico. Social Forces. 1997;75(4):1291–1318. [Google Scholar]
  48. Pinquart M, Sörensen S. Gender Differences in Caregiver Stressors, Social Resources, and Health: An Updated Meta-Analysis. The Journals of Gerontology: Series B. 2006;61(1):P33–P45. doi: 10.1093/geronb/61.1.p33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Radloff LS. The CES-D Scale: A Self-Report Depression Scale for Research in the General Population. Applied Psychological Measurement. 1977;1(3):385–401. [Google Scholar]
  50. Robles TF. Marital Quality and Health: Implications for Marriage in the 21st Century. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2014;23(6):427–32. doi: 10.1177/0963721414549043. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Ross CE, Mirowsky J, Goldsteen K. The Impact of the Family on Health: The Decade in Review. Journal of Marriage and the Family; Minneapolis. 1990;52(4):1059. [Google Scholar]
  52. Seedat S, Scott KM, Angermeyer MC, Berglund P, Bromet EJ, Brugha TS, Demyttenaere K, Girolamo G de, Haro JM, Jin R, Karam EG, Kovess-Masfety V, Levinson D, Mora MEM, Ono Y, Ormel J, Pennell B-E, Posada-Villa J, Sampson NA, Williams D, Kessler RC. Cross-National Associations Between Gender and Mental Disorders in the World Health Organization World Mental Health Surveys. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2009;66(7):785–95. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.36. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Siegel MJ, Bradley EH, Gallo WT, Kasl SV. The Effect of Spousal Mental and Physical Health on Husbands’ and Wives’ Depressive Symptoms, Among Older Adults: Longitudinal Evidence From the Health and Retirement Survey. Journal of Aging and Health. 2004;16(3):398–425. doi: 10.1177/0898264304264208. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Starcke K, Brand M. Decision making under stress: A selective review. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2012;36(4):1228–48. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.02.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Stern SJ. The Secret History of Gender: Women, Men, and Power in Late Colonial Mexico. Univ of North Carolina Press; 1997. [Google Scholar]
  56. Sussman MB, Steinmetz SK, Peterson GW. Handbook of Marriage and the Family. Springer Science & Business Media; 1999. [Google Scholar]
  57. Tichenor V. Maintaining Men’s Dominance: Negotiating Identity and Power When She Earns More. Sex Roles. 2005;53(3–4):191–205. [Google Scholar]
  58. United Nations. Gender Inequality Index 2014 [Google Scholar]
  59. Villegas SG, Zavala VM de O, Guillén J. Social Support and Social Networks Among the Elderly in Mexico. Journal of Population Ageing. 2014;7(2):143–59. [Google Scholar]
  60. Wanic R, Kulik J. Toward an Understanding of Gender Differences in the Impact of Marital Conflict on Health. Sex Roles. 2011;65(5–6):297–312. [Google Scholar]
  61. Wong R, Palloni A. Aging in Mexico and Latin America. In: Uhlenberg P, editor. International Handbook of Population Aging. International Handbooks of Population; Springer Netherlands: 2009. pp. 231–52. [Google Scholar]
  62. World Health Organization. Aging and Health 2016a [Google Scholar]
  63. World Health Organization. Life Expectancy Data by Country 2016b [Google Scholar]
  64. World Health Organization. The Global Burden of Disease: 2004 Update 2004 [Google Scholar]
  65. Yang Y, George LK. Functional Disability, Disability Transitions, and Depressive Symptoms in Late Life. Journal of Aging and Health. 2005;17(3):263–92. doi: 10.1177/0898264305276295. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Zellner A. An Efficient Method of Estimating Seemingly Unrelated Regressions and Tests for Aggregation Bias. Journal of the American Statistical Association. 1962;57(298):348–68. [Google Scholar]
  67. Zipp JF, Prohaska A, Bemiller M. Wives, Husbands, and Hidden Power in Marriage. Journal of Family Issues. 2004;25(7):923–48. [Google Scholar]
  68. Zipp JF, Toth J. She Said, He Said, They Said: The Impact of Spousal Presence in Survey Research. Public Opinion Quarterly. 2002;66(2):177–208. [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES