Abstract
Background
Recreational cannabis has been legalized in 11 states and Washington DC in the US. However, little is known about individual preferences for legal cannabis products. This study estimated the impacts of tetrahydrocannabinol (THC), cannabidiol (CBD), warning messages, and price on preferences for cannabis flowers.
Methods
A cross-sectional online survey with discrete choice experiments was implemented in October 2017. A sample of 2,400 adults aged 21 years or older were recruited from 6 US states with recreational cannabis legalization, consisting of 1,200 past-year nonusers and 1,200 past-year users. Each respondent was randomly assigned to 12 discrete choice scenarios, each asking them to choose from an opt-out option and 3 cannabis flower products with varying levels in THC, CBD, warning messages, and price. The impacts of product attributes on individual choices were analyzed with nested logit regressions.
Results
Both cannabis nonusers and users preferred higher CBD and lower price. Users also preferred higher THC. The results on warning messages were mixed: graphic warning on drugged driving and text warning message had positive impacts on nonusers’ and users’ preferences for cannabis flowers, respectively, whereas FDA disapproval disclaimer had negative impacts on nonusers’ preferences. Heterogeneities in preferences were revealed among nonusers by former use status and among users by reason of use. Particularly, medical cannabis users were not as responsive to THC as recreational cannabis users or dual users were. Regarding relative importance of the attributes, all respondents but medical cannabis users perceived price as the most important attribute (relative importance 51–64%), whereas medical cannabis users perceived CBD as the most important attribute (relative importance 47%).
Conclusion
The findings indicated that product characteristics may have influences on US adults’ choices of legal cannabis flower products and may deserve consideration for cannabis regulatory framework.
Keywords: Cannabis legalization, discrete choice experiments, potency, warning message, price
INTRODUCTION
Cannabis is the most commonly used illicit drug in the US that is prohibited at the federal level. During 2002–2017, past-year users increased from 11% to 15% (37.8 million in 2017) and past-month users increased from 6% to 10% (24.4 million in 2017) (SAMHSA, 2017). There is some evidence on therapeutic effects of cannabis on certain conditions such as chronic pain, chemotherapy-induced nausea and vomiting, and multiple sclerosis (NationalAcademies, 2017), but recreational cannabis use is associated with cannabis use disorders, respiratory and cardiovascular diseases, and increased risk of motor vehicle accidents (Hall & Degenhardt, 2009).
Despite the federal prohibition against cannabis in the US, since 2012, recreational cannabis has been legalized in 11 states and Washington DC where over a quarter of US population live (LawAtlas, 2019). Recreational cannabis legalization has been also adopted by Uruguay and Canada at national level recently (Austen, 2017; Miroff, 2017) and considered in additions states in the US. The major provision of state laws in the US was to allow retail sale. By the end of 2018, Alaska, California, Colorado, Massachusetts, Nevada, Oregon, and Washington had opened their retail stores to adults aged 21 years or older (LawAtlas, 2019). The other legalizing states, with the only exceptions of Washington DC and Vermont, were planning to do so in the next 2 to 3 years.
Retail cannabis markets have expanded rapidly in the US. The number of licensed retail stores has grown nearly 400% in Colorado since retail markets opened in early 2014 (Orens, Light, Lewandowski, Rowberry, & Saloga, 2018). Annual retail sales in Colorado increased from $303 million in 2014 to $1,213 million in 2018 (StateofColorado, 2019). In Washington, monthly retail sales reached $98 million two years after retail markets opened in July 2014 (Smart, Caulkins, Kilmer, Davenport, & Midgette, 2017). Cannabis flowers have been dominating the retail markets, accounting for two thirds of the market share (Orens, et al., 2018; Smart, et al., 2017). Other types of cannabis products on legal markets include concentrates, cannabis-infused edibles and drinks, etc. In the US, flowers are primarily consumed by smoking, and smoking flowers is the most common method to consume cannabis. Over 92% recreational cannabis users and over 82% medical cannabis users reported that, when they used cannabis last time, they smoked cannabis flowers (Pacula, Jacobson, & Maksabedian, 2016).
Studies evaluating the impacts of recreational cannabis legalization were just emerging. Most of them used a dichotomous measure of legalization status to examine the changes before and after the legalization. The findings suggested that the legalization may be associated with increased cannabis use in some subpopulation or states but had no impacts on motor vehicle crashes or prescription opioid use in adults (Aydelotte, et al., 2017; Cerda, et al., 2017; Hansen, Miller, & Weber; Kerr, Bae, Phibbs, & Kern, 2017; Shi, et al., 2019). There was a paucity of research on cannabis commercialization. One study in Denver, Colorado examined the density of cannabis retail stores and found its associations with cannabis-related crime rates (Freisthler, Gaidus, Tam, Ponicki, & Gruenewald, 2017). Another study in Colorado suggested an increasing trend of cannabis use among trauma patients following cannabis commercialization (Chung, et al., 2019).
The lack of knowledge regarding how individuals make purchase decisions and what policies would be effective tools for reducing problem cannabis use significantly hinders evidence-based policymaking after cannabis is commercialized. Current regulatory frameworks were primarily based on lessons learned from tobacco and alcohol control (Pacula, Kilmer, Wagenaar, Chaloupka, & Caulkins, 2014), with little support from empirical evidence of policy effects. To fill the knowledge gap, in this study, we examined the following product characteristics that may have potential to modify cannabis purchase behaviors and inform policymaking (Kilmer, 2017; Pacula, et al., 2014).
Potency
Tetrahydrocannabinol (THC) and cannabidiol (CBD) are the two major chemical compounds in cannabis. Almost all states with retail markets required that manufactured cannabis products should list both THC and CBD levels on package labels. THC is the primary psychoactive constituent. Because of its influences on certain physiological and pathophysiological processes, THC could generate intoxicating psychoactive effects but also have potential to relieve health conditions such as pain (Izzo, Borrelli, Capasso, Di Marzo, & Mechoulam, 2009). THC increased from 3.4% in 1993 to 8.8% in 2008 (Mehmedic, et al., 2010) and increased more dramatically after cannabis was commercialized, from 16% to nearly 20% during 2014–2017 (Orens, et al., 2018). CBD is the primary non-psychoactive constitute that partially offsets THC’s psychoactive properties (Hall & Degenhardt, 2009). As CBD has no known intoxicating psychoactive effects, there have been increasing interests in its therapeutic application. Some beneficial effects have been suggested (Niesink & van Laar, 2013). In the US, 17 states have passed state-wide laws allowing for high CBD concentrates with minimal THC for the treatment of epilepsy or seizures (Procon.org, 2019).
Primarily due to the concern of accidental poisonings, most states with retail markets have set limits on THC in edible products. Currently no state limits THC in flowers. In 2016, Colorado proposed to limit all types of cannabis products to 15–16% THC but the initiative failed (Ingold, 2016). Little is known about the impacts of THC and CBD levels on consumers’ preferences and whether the impacts differ between recreational and medical users.
Warning messages
Currently all states with retail markets require text warning messages displayed on cannabis packages but the information varies across states (ShatterLabels, 2017). Some messages remind consumers that cannabis is a Schedule I drug, some describe the health risks, such as impaired driving and drug dependence, and others include statements that cannabis is not approved by the FDA. Tobacco and alcohol researchers have studied the impacts of warning labels extensively. Health warning messages prevent smoking initiation and promote smoking cessation (Hammond, 2011) but have little effects on drinking behaviors (Wilkinson & Room, 2009). There is a dearth of research regarding consumers’ responsiveness to different types of warning messages.
Price
Prices of cannabis products on retail markets vary considerably across and within states (Hunt & Pacula, 2017; Smart, et al., 2017). States also adopt different approaches to levy taxes (Nevada, 2018; Oregon, 2017; Rough, 2017; TaxFoundation, 2017). Sales tax is the most common form, ranging from 6.25% in Massachusetts to 17% in Oregon. In addition to sales tax, Colorado, Massachusetts, Nevada, and Washington also impose excise tax, with Washington’s tax rate being the highest (37%). California has a combination of 15% sales tax and $9.25 per ounce flat tax rate for flower products. Alaska is the only state with a single flat tax rate. Many states also allow local municipalities to levy local sales or excise tax. The different prices and taxes across and within states may lead to substantial variations in total costs faced by consumers. The literature on tobacco and alcohol control repeatedly suggest that higher prices deter initiation and reduce consumption (Elder, et al., 2010; Wagenaar, Salois, & Komro, 2009; Wagenaar, Tobler, & Komro, 2010; Wilson, et al., 2012). Previous research on cannabis relied on illicit market data (Gallet, 2014; Pacula & Lundberg, 2014) with only one exception (Smart, et al., 2017). It remains unclear, given the large declines in production costs in legalizing states (Smart, et al., 2017), the extent to which changes in price influence cannabis purchase behaviors.
This study aimed to identify the impacts of product attributes on individual preferences for cannabis products on legal retail markets in the US. Particularly, we aimed to 1) examine the impacts of THC, CBD, warning messages, and price on adults’ preferences for cannabis flowers; 2) examine the heterogeneities of the preferences among subgroups. We focused on cannabis flowers as flowers are dominating the US retail markets.
METHODS
Study Design
We used a cross-sectional online survey with discrete choice experiments (DCEs) to simulate individual decisions on purchasing hypothetical cannabis products. DCE is a stated-preference approach widely used in health economics to ascertain the relative importance of product attributes to consumers (Clark, Determann, Petrou, Moro, & de Bekker-Grob, 2014; de Bekker-Grob, Ryan, & Gerard, 2012; Ryan & Gerard, 2003; Soekhai, de Bekker-Grob, Ellis, & Vass, 2019). Particularly, it has been increasingly adopted in research on tobacco and alcohol use (Helter & Boehler, 2016; Lockshin, Wade, d’Hauteville, & Perrouty, 2006; Pechey, Burge, Mentzakis, Suhrcke, & Marteau, 2014; Regmi, Kaphle, Timilsina, & Tuha, 2018), which are arguably comparable to cannabis use after cannabis is commercialized.
Compared to observational data from population surveys or administrative records, DCE has the following advantages. 1) It can manipulate choice scenarios shown to the same individual such that within- and between- individual variations are enabled. In contrast, policy variations in observational data are typically limited and only available at macro level such as state and county. 2) Because of its hypothetical nature, DCE provides opportunities to examine potentially influential attributes or policies that are not yet available or implemented. 3) DCE can infer causality. By randomly varying choice scenarios shown to the same individual, we can ensure that the systematic differences in choices are solely attributable to the experimental manipulation. 4) Surveys along with DCEs provide rich individual information that are often not available in administrative records.
Study Sample
A convenience sample of 2,400 adults were recruited from a US online panel in October 2017. Maintained by a marketing company, the panel consists of millions of adult panelists who voluntarily participate in online surveys like the one in our study. To be eligible for this study, respondents must be at least 21 years old, passing the age limit for legal purchase of recreational cannabis in the US. Respondents also must be living in one of the 6 US states that had legalized recreational cannabis at the time of interview, including California, Colorado, Massachusetts, Nevada, Oregon, and Washington. These states represented 96% of total population in all the 9 jurisdictions in the US that had legalized recreational cannabis by the end of 2017.1 The panelists’ age and state information were held by the marketing company before this survey was conducted. Only those meeting the inclusion criteria received email invitations. The respondents were prevented from participating multiple times using digital identify technology. A series of measures were also implemented to improve data quality. For example, respondents who completed survey in less than 3 minutes were considered paying insufficient attention to questions and removed. We also removed those who failed attention check questions. Respondents redeemed monetary incentives through loyalty programs upon successful completion of the survey.
We recruited equal sample sizes of those who did not use cannabis in the past 12 months (cannabis nonusers, N=1,200) and those who used cannabis at least once in the past 12 months (cannabis users, N=1,200). These 2 samples might have differential responses to product attributes. The sample size of 1,200 exceeded most published DCE studies as well as the minimum sample size (~150) calculated using the recommended statistical procedures in de Bekker-Grob et al (de Bekker-Grob, Donkers, Jonker, & Stolk, 2015). After excluding 55 respondents with incomplete or inconsistent information, the study sample was reduced to 1,186 nonusers and 1,159 users.
Experiment Design
The attributes and corresponding levels in the DCE design are illustrated in Table 1. We focused on 4 attributes: (1) THC level, (2) CBD level, (3) warning messages, and (4) price.
Table 1.
Attributes | Levels |
---|---|
Tetrahydrocannabinol (THC) Level | 1. 15% |
2. 20% | |
3. 30% | |
Cannabidiol (CBD) Level | 1. 0% (CBD free) |
2. 0.4% | |
3. 15% | |
Warning Message | 1. None |
2. Text warning currently adopted by Washington and Colorado: There may be health risks associated with consumption of this product. Should not be used by women that are pregnant or breast feeding. For use only by adults twenty-one and older. Keep out of reach of children. Marijuana can impair concentration, coordination, and judgment. Do not operate a vehicle or machinery under the influence of this drug. Warning: This product has intoxicating effects and may be habit forming. Smoking is hazardous to your health. This product may be unlawful outside of your state. Caution: When eaten or swallowed, the intoxicating effects of this drug may be delayed by two or more hours. | |
3. Graphic warning on drugged driving: | |
4. FDA disapproval disclaimer: This product is not approved by the FDA to treat, cure, or prevent any disease. | |
Price | 1. $20 |
2. $35 | |
3. $50 | |
4. $65 |
Notes: The dominant and dominated combinations ($20 and THC 30%, $65 and THC 15%) were prohibited from the discrete choice experiment.
For each of the 4 product attributes, we varied attribute levels based on available information on products sold in the US. The THC levels (15%, 20%, and 30%) represented common low, medium, and high levels of THC in published online product menus (Weedmaps, 2018). Similarly, prices ($20, $35, $50, and $65) were selected as values from within quartile distributions on online product menus (Weedmaps, 2018) for cannabis flowers sold in 1/8th ounce units (roughly equivalent to 3.5 grams or 5 joints), the most popular unit size on retail markets (Smart, et al., 2017). In the case of CBD, 0.4% was the median level of products sold on market (Smart, et al., 2017), and ranges around that (0% and 15% CBD) were selected to represent CBD free products and products with high CBD, respectively. The first warning message (none) represented the default current federal requirement for cannabis product labeling, which is none given the federal prohibition. The second warning message (text warning) represented the style currently required by Colorado and Washington. The third warning message (graphic warning on drugged driving) was a graphic image adopted by Colorado in a state-wide campaign in 2016 to highlight the legal consequence of drugged driving. The fourth warning message (FDA disapproval disclaimer) was a modified FDA disclaimer stressing the fact that cannabis products received no evaluation or approval from the FDA (FDA, 2017); a similar disclaimer is required in Massachusetts (ShatterLabels, 2017). Each alternative in DCE represented a unique hypothetical cannabis product with the 4 attributes varying in levels as described above.
The DCEs were designed in accordance with good practices recommended by the Task Force (Bridges, et al., 2011). Sawtooth software was used to construct DCEs with a Balanced Overlap method and Blocked Fractional Factorial design (Jaynes, Wong, & Xu, 2016). We prohibited dominant alternatives (always desirable) that contained the combination of THC 30% and price $20 and dominated alternatives (always undesirable) that contained the combination of THC 15% and price $65. The total number of possible alternatives excluding dominant and dominated alternatives was 120.
It is recommended that DCE choice scenarios are limited to 8–16 to avoid cognitive burden (Reed Johnson, et al., 2013). Respondents were therefore randomly assigned to 1 out of 6 versions of surveys, with each containing 12 randomly ordered DCE choice scenarios. Each choice scenario asked respondents to choose from 4 alternatives: opt-out or any of the 3 hypothetical cannabis alternatives. An example of choice scenario is presented in Figure S1. These 72 (12*6) unique choice scenarios resulted in a D-efficiency of 81.4% relative to the full factorial design that includes all possible choice scenarios.
To increase the quality of DCE responses, prior to DCE questions, we provided narrative information introducing the general context and purpose of the research. We explained that “marijuana is also called cannabis, weed, herb, pot, grass, etc.”. Respondents were reminded that, despite the hypothetical nature of the questions, their responses are important to research and policymaking and honest responses are needed. The choice alternatives would be the only options they could get in each choice scenario; they could select “none” to opt out of selecting any cannabis alternatives; they should select an alternative based on its characteristics; they should consider only warning messages associated with the alternatives in a choice scenario and should not carry them to other scenarios. We briefly explained that THC and CBD were the primary psychoactive and non-psychoactive ingredients in cannabis, respectively. Even though respondents may have limited or even inaccurate pre-existing knowledge, detailed information and scientific evidence on health effects were not provided. The purpose was to simulate choices in the real world where consumers were not educated.
Online Survey
In addition to DCE questions, the survey also asked about cannabis use history and pattern, other drug use, cannabis-related perceptions, and socioeconomic background. The entire survey took 15 minutes on average to complete. The Institutional Review Board at University of California San Diego approved the study.
Measures
The primary outcome was a dichotomous variable indicating whether an alternative was chosen in a choice scenario. Because a respondent was randomly assigned to 12 choice scenarios, each including 3 cannabis alternatives and an opt-out option, the total number of alternatives for the primary outcome analysis was 56,928 for nonusers and 55,632 for users. To examine the characteristics of respondents who chose to opt-out in all the 12 assigned choice scenarios, we created a secondary outcome measure, also a dichotomous variable, indicating whether a respondent always opted out. The analysis of this secondary outcome was conducted at individual level.
For the 4 attribute variables (alternative-specific variables) that varied across choice alternatives, the levels of price were modeled as a continuous variable, whereas the levels of THC, CBD, and warning messages were dummy coded. The following respondents’ socioeconomic characteristics were considered as case-specific variables that were constant across alternatives and scenarios for the same respondent: gender, age, race/ethnicity, education, household income, knowledge about cannabis, past-month cigarette smoking, past-month binge drinking (drinking 5 or more alcoholic drinks in a single occasion), and state of residence. Among past-year nonusers, we also created an indicator for never users and former users. Among past-year users, past-month regular cannabis use was defined as using cannabis for at least 20 days in the past month (i.e., almost daily use); we identified reason of use with the question: “when you used marijuana, was it primarily for medical reasons to treat or decrease symptoms of a health condition, or was it primarily for recreational reasons to get pleasure or satisfaction?” The answers categorized users into recreational users (primarily using for recreational reasons), medical users (primarily using for medical reasons), and dual users (using for both recreational and medical reasons).
Statistical Analysis
Descriptive statistics were provided on individual characteristics and chosen alternatives. Nested logit regressions (Hensher, Rose, & Greene, 2015) on alternative-level observations were used to model choice selections. Nested logit model was preferred to multinomial logit model in this study because it relaxes the assumption of independence of irrelevant alternatives which requires that the probability of choosing between two choice alternatives is independent of the presence of an additional alternative in the choice scenario. Nested logit model relaxes this assumption by dividing the decision into two sequential steps: the first step chooses between opt-out and any cannabis products, then conditional on choosing any cannabis products, the second step chooses between three cannabis alternatives. Nested logit regressions are especially appropriate for the analysis of past-year nonusers, whose decision about opt-out is likely separate from the decision of choosing which cannabis product. They were commonly used in tobacco research to estimate nonsmokers’ choices (Salloum, et al., 2015; Shang, Huang, Chaloupka, & Emery, 2017). To account for multiple choice decisions made by the same respondent, standard errors were clustered at individual level.
We first conducted regressions among nonusers and users, respectively, then among nonusers by former use status and among users by reason of use. To formally detect heterogeneities across subsamples, we also interacted alternative-specific variables with subsample indicators in separate regressions. Among users, we also added interaction terms between THC and CBD to identify the most preferred combination of potency measures.
Based on nested logit models, we computed willingness-to-pay (WTP) and relative importance. WTP quantifies monetary values associated with the marginal change in product attribute from the reference level to another level (e.g., THC from 15% to 20%). It was calculated as dividing the coefficient of an attribute level of interest by the negative of the coefficient of the price attribute. To assess the relative importance of an attribute, we first calculated the range of estimated coefficient values (part-worth utilities) for the attribute, then divided it by the sum of all the attribute ranges (Czoli, Goniewicz, Islam, Kotnowski, & Hammond, 2016). A higher relative importance indicates a higher weight that respondents placed on a product attribute in cannabis purchase decisions.
Logistic regressions on individual-level observations were used to examine the associations between always-opt-out and individual characteristics.
RESULTS
Descriptive Statistics of Study Sample and DCE Responses
Table 2 presents descriptive statistics of individual-level characteristics. About 47% of nonusers reported former cannabis use. Among users, 30% used cannabis regularly in the past month; 40% were recreational users, 24% were medical users, and the remaining 36% were dual users. Significant differences were observed in most characteristics by former use status among nonusers and by reason of use among users. Table S1 reports demographic differences between our study sample and national sample from the 2016 National Survey on Drug Use and Health. Our sample had a greater proportion of females, non-Hispanic Whites, and college graduates in both nonusers and users.
Table 2.
Characteristics | Past-year Nonusers |
Past-year Users |
|||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
All Past-year Nonusers (N=1,186) | By Former Use |
All Past-year Users (N=1,159) | By Reason of Use |
||||||
Never Users (N=625) | Former Users (N=561) | Between-group Test P Value | Primarily Used for Recreational Reasons Only (N=459) | Primarily Used for Medical Reason Only (N=282) | Used for Both Recreational and Medical Reasons (N=418) | Between-group Test P Value | |||
Gender | .33 | <.001 | |||||||
Male | 32.04% | 33.28% | 30.66% | 31.49% | 37.91% | 31.56% | 24.40% | ||
Female | 67.96% | 66.72% | 69.34% | 68.51% | 62.09% | 68.44% | 75.60% | ||
Age | <.001 | .004 | |||||||
21–29 | 15.50% | 18.08% | 12.83% | 34.43% | 37.25% | 28.01% | 35.65% | ||
30–44 | 26.90% | 28.96% | 24.60% | 37.01% | 35.73% | 35.46% | 39.47% | ||
45–59 | 22.93% | 18.08% | 28.34% | 17.00% | 14.81% | 20.57% | 16.99% | ||
60+ | 34.57% | 34.88% | 34.22% | 11.56% | 12.20% | 15.96% | 7.89% | ||
Race/ethnicity | .029 | .40 | |||||||
Non-Hispanic White | 79.68% | 77.12% | 82.53% | 74.46% | 72.98% | 75.53% | 75.36% | ||
Hispanic | 8.01% | 8.00% | 8.02% | 12.51% | 12.64% | 10.28% | 13.88% | ||
Non-Hispanic Black | 2.28% | 2.40% | 2.14% | 5.35% | 5.66% | 4.96% | 5.26% | ||
Non-Hispanic other minority | 10.03% | 12.48% | 7.31% | 7.68% | 8.71% | 9.22% | 5.50% | ||
Education | .40 | .17 | |||||||
High school or less | 15.35% | 15.20% | 15.51% | 20.79% | 19.17% | 21.99% | 21.77% | ||
Some college | 24.37% | 22.88% | 26.02% | 31.49% | 29.41% | 29.79% | 34.93% | ||
College degree or more | 60.29% | 61.92% | 58.47% | 47.71% | 51.42% | 48.23% | 43.30% | ||
Household income | .005 | .039 | |||||||
<$25,000 | 14.59% | 15.68% | 13.37% | 21.14% | 16.78% | 21.63% | 25.60% | ||
$25,000-$50,000 | 19.22% | 15.84% | 22.99% | 24.42% | 23.75% | 23.05% | 26.08% | ||
$50,000–75,000 | 22.34% | 24.00% | 20.50% | 20.28% | 21.13% | 19.50% | 19.86% | ||
>$75,000 | 35.83% | 34.88% | 36.90% | 30.63% | 34.86% | 31.91% | 25.12% | ||
Did not answer | 8.01% | 9.60% | 6.24% | 3.54% | 3.49% | 3.90% | 3.35% | ||
Ever used cannabis in lifetime | NA | NA | |||||||
Yes | 47.30% | 0% | 100% | 100% | 100% | 100% | 100% | ||
No | 52.70% | 100% | 0% | 0% | 0% | 0% | 0% | ||
Past-month regular cannabis use | NA | <.001 | |||||||
Yes | NA | NA | NA | 30.37% | 16.12% | 26.95% | 48.33% | ||
No | NA | NA | NA | 69.63% | 83.88% | 73.05% | 51.67% | ||
Past-year cannabis users by reason of use | NA | NA | |||||||
Recreational users | NA | NA | NA | 39.60% | 100% | 0% | 0% | ||
Medical users | NA | NA | NA | 24.33% | 0% | 100% | 0% | ||
Dual users | NA | NA | NA | 36.07% | 0% | 0% | 100% | ||
Knowledge about cannabis | <.001 | <.001 | |||||||
Moderate to High | 45.70% | 37.76% | 54.55% | 78.34% | 71.02% | 79.79% | 85.41% | ||
Low | 54.30% | 62.24% | 45.45% | 21.66% | 28.98% | 20.21% | 14.59% | ||
Past-month smoking | <.001 | <.001 | |||||||
Yes | 11.13% | 4.32% | 18.72% | 29.94% | 25.71% | 25.53% | 37.56% | ||
No | 88.87% | 95.68% | 81.28% | 70.06% | 74.29% | 74.47% | 62.44% | ||
Past-month binge drinking | <.001 | <.001 | |||||||
Yes | 14.17% | 8.48% | 20.50% | 35.38% | 40.96% | 19.50% | 39.95% | ||
No | 85.83% | 91.52% | 79.50% | 64.62% | 59.04% | 80.50% | 60.05% | ||
State of residence | .45 | .10 | |||||||
California | 18.55% | 20.64% | 16.22% | 19.84% | 17.86% | 24.47% | 18.90% | ||
Colorado | 16.61% | 16.16% | 17.11% | 17.52% | 17.21% | 15.25% | 19.38% | ||
Massachusetts | 20.24% | 20.16% | 20.32% | 14.84% | 18.30% | 13.12% | 12.20% | ||
Nevada | 9.78% | 8.80% | 10.87% | 13.72% | 14.38% | 12.06% | 14.11% | ||
Oregon | 16.53% | 16.32% | 16.76% | 17.17% | 14.81% | 19.50% | 18.18% | ||
Washington | 18.30% | 17.92% | 18.72% | 16.91% | 17.43% | 15.60% | 17.22% |
The descriptive statistics conditional on chosen alternatives are provided in Table S2. Among all choice scenarios presented to nonusers, roughly two thirds (63.44%) of the decisions were to opt out with a higher proportion in never users (75.11%) than former users (50.45%). In contrast, only 18.82% decisions were to opt out among past-year users. The patterns of attributes and levels in chosen alternatives were similar between subgroups. In general, selection of cannabis products increased with CBD level but decreased with THC level and price. The pattern by warning messages was not obvious.
Preferences among Nonusers and Users
Table 3 shows the results from nested logit regressions for cannabis nonusers and users, respectively. Compared to THC 15%, products with THC 20% and 30% were more likely to be chosen by users (p<.001), but only those with THC 30% were more likely to be chosen by nonusers (p=.034). Relative to CBD free, products with CBD 0.4% and 15% were more likely to be chosen by both nonusers and users (p<.001). Relative to no warning message, text warning was associated with a higher utility among users (p=.028) and graphic warning on drugged driving was associated with a higher utility among nonusers (p=.010); FDA disapproval disclaimer was associated with a lower utility among nonusers (p=.050). Products with high price were less likely to be chosen by both nonusers and users (p<.001). Results with interaction terms between attributes and past-year use indicator (Table S3) confirmed statistical differences in preferences between nonusers and users: compared to nonusers, users were more likely to choose products with higher THC, higher CBD, waring messages, and higher price (p<.001).
Table 3.
Attributes | Past-year Nonusers |
Past-year Users |
Past-year Nonusers |
Past-year Users |
---|---|---|---|---|
Coefficient Estimate (95% CI) | Willingness to Pay Estimate ($) (95% CI) | |||
Tetrahydrocannabinol (THC) Level, dichotomous | ||||
15% (reference) | ||||
20% | .016 (−.0089, .041) | .18*** (.12, .24) | 1.24 (−.60, 3.0) | 4.88 (3.39, 6.37) |
30% | .041* (.0030, .079) | 42*** (.33, .52) | 3.10 (.35, 5.86) | 11.57 (9.36, 13.79) |
Cannabidiol (CBD) Level, dichotomous | ||||
0% (reference) | ||||
0.4% | .066*** (.032, .10) | .24*** (.17, .31) | 5.02 (2.82, 7.23) | 6.49 (4.80, 8.17) |
15% | .27*** (.19, .35) | .78*** (.64, .93) | 21.08 (17.29, 24.86) | 21.11 (18.44, 23.79) |
Warning Message, dichotomous | ||||
None (reference) | ||||
Text warning | −.0069 (−.045, .031) | .079* (.0083, .15) | −.52 (−3.44, 2.38) | 2.12 (.19, 4.05) |
Graphic warning on drugged driving | .050** (.012, .088) | .040 (−.030, .11) | 3.82 (.99, 6.65) | 1.08 (−.82, 2.98) |
FDA disclaimer | −.040* (−.080, −.00) | .011 (−.053, .075) | −3.03 (−5.95, −.12) | .29 (−1.42, 2.01) |
Price, continuous | −.013*** (−.016, −.0096) | −.037*** (−.042, −.031) | ||
Number of Respondents | 1,186 | 1,159 | ||
Number of Choice Scenarios | 14,232 | 13,908 | ||
Number of Choice Alternatives | 56,928 | 55,632 |
p<.05
p<.01
p<.001
Preferences among Nonusers by Former Use Status and among Users by Reason of Use
Table 4 reports subgroup analyses on cannabis nonusers by former use. Never users and former users both preferred higher CBD and lower price but were unresponsive to THC and text warning message. Never users were more likely to choose products with graphic warning on drugged driving (p=.005) and former users were less likely to choose products with FDA disapproval disclaimer (p=.027). Table S4 reports results with interaction terms between attributes and former use indicator. Compared to never users, former users were more likely to choose products with higher CBD, text warning, FDA disclaimer, and higher price. Preferences for THC and graphic warning did not differ between never users and former users.
Table 4.
Attributes | Never Users |
Former Users |
Never Users |
Former Users |
---|---|---|---|---|
Coefficient Estimate (95% CI) | Willingness to Pay Estimate ($) (95% CI) | |||
Tetrahydrocannabinol (THC) Level, dichotomous | ||||
15% (reference) | ||||
20% | .020 (−.0072, .048) | .0073 (−.035, .049) | 2.40 (−.32, 5.13) | .42 (−2.01, 2.86) |
30% | .016 (−.018, .052) | .064 (−.00094, .12) | 1.96 (−1.92, 5.85) | 3.72 (.028, 7.41) |
Cannabidiol (CBD) Level, dichotomous | ||||
0% (reference) | ||||
0.4% | .045* (.0093, .081) | .087** (.031, .14) | 5.32 (1.78, 8.86) | 5.06 (2.26, 7.87) |
15% | .17*** (.070, .27) | .37*** (.25, .49) | 19.98 (14.10, 25.87) | 21.82 (16.90, 26.75) |
Warning Message, dichotomous | ||||
None (reference) | ||||
Text warning | .021 (−.024, .067) | −.041 (−.10, .019) | 2.53 (−2.74, 7.81) | −2.43 (−5.85, .99) |
Graphic warning on drugged driving | .078** (.023, .13) | .012 (−.048, .072) | 9.15 (4.45, 13.85) | .71 (−2.79, 4.22) |
FDA disclaimer | −.010 (−.055, .034) | −.070* (−.13, −.0080) | −1.23 (−6.44, 3.96) | −4.11 (−7.58, −.64) |
Price, continuous | −.0085*** (−.013, −.0037) | −.017*** (−.022, −.012) | ||
Number of Respondents | 625 | 561 | ||
Number of Choice Scenarios | 7,500 | 6,732 | ||
Number of Choice Alternatives | 30,000 | 26,928 |
p<.05
p<.01
p<.001
Table 5 reports subgroup analyses on users by reason of use. All the three subgroups preferred higher CBD and lower price. Recreational and dual users also preferred higher THC (p<.001), but medical users were not responsive to THC. Regarding warning messages, medical users preferred text warning (p=.005), recreational users preferred graphic warning on drugged driving (p=.049). All the three subgroups were unresponsive to FDA disclaimer. Results with interaction terms between attributes and reason of use indicators (Table S5) confirmed statistical differences in preferences by reason of use. Compared to medical users, recreational users preferred products with higher THC, lower CBD, graphic warning, and lower price, whereas dual users preferred products with higher THC, higher CBD, graphic warning, FDA disclaimer, and lower price.
Table 5.
Attributes | Recreational Cannabis Users |
Medical Cannabis Users |
Dual Users |
Recreational Cannabis Users |
Medical Cannabis Users |
Dual Users |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Coefficient Estimate (95% CI) | Willingness to Pay Estimate ($) (95% CI) | |||||
Tetrahydrocannabinol (THC) Level, continuous | ||||||
15% (reference) | ||||||
20% | .17*** (.081, .26) | .041 (−.12, .20) | .29*** (.17, .41) | 4.82 (2.57, 7.07) | 1.19 (−3.38, 5.77) | 6.24 (4.07, 8.41) |
30% | .37*** (.23, .51) | .15 (−.084, .38) | .70*** (.48, .91) | 10.44 (7.08, 13.79) | 4.32 (−2.47, 11.12) | 15.05 (11.82, 18.29) |
Cannabidiol (CBD) Level, dichotomous | ||||||
0% (reference) | ||||||
0.4% | .092* (.0093, .17) | .50*** (.25, .75) | .37*** (.23, .51) | 2.59 (.38, 4.80) | 14.54 (8.05, 21.03) | 8.05 (5.52, 10.57) |
15% | .36*** (.21, .50) | 1.66*** (1.16, 2.16) | 1.09*** (.81, 1.37) | 10.09 (6.90, 13.28) | 47.79 (36.43, 59.15) | 23.45 (19.38, 27.52) |
Warning Message, dichotomous | ||||||
None (reference) | ||||||
Text warning | −.0037 (−.10, .098) | .30** (.090, .52) | .079 (−.044, .20) | −.10 (−2.96, 2.76) | 8.76 (2.07, 15.45) | 1.70 (−.99, 4.40) |
Graphic warning on drugged driving | .096* (.00021, .19) | −.0064 (−.23, .21) | −.022 (−.15, .10) | 2.71 (−.035, 5.46) | −.18 (−6.62, 6.25) | −.48 (−3.21, 2.24) |
FDA disclaimer | −.042 (−.13, .050) | .10 (−.090, .29) | .042 (−.074, .15) | −1.18 (−3.72, 1.35) | 2.96 (−2.72, 8.65) | .90 (−1.61, 3.42) |
Price, continuous | −.035*** (−.0446, −.026) | −.034*** (−.044, −.025) | −.046*** (−.057, −.035) | |||
Number of Respondents | 459 | 282 | 418 | |||
Number of Choice Scenarios | 5,508 | 3,384 | 5,016 | |||
Number of Choice Alternatives | 22,032 | 13,536 | 20,064 |
p<.05
p<.01
p<.001
Table S6 reported results with THC and CBD interactions among users by reason of use. The most preferred combination was THC 30% and CBD 15% among recreational users and dual users, but medical users preferred THC 20%/CBD 15% combination and THC 15%/ CBD 15% combination more than THC 30%/CBD 15% combination.
Willingness to Pay (WTP)
WTP estimates are reported in Tables 3–5. For example, among users, medical users had the highest WTP for higher CBD products ($14.54 more for CBD 0.4% and $47.79 more for CBD 15% relative to CBD free), and dual users had the highest WTP for higher THC products ($6.24 more for THC 20% and $15.05 more for THC 30% relative to THC 15%).
Relative Importance
Figure S2 depicts the relative importance of product attributes. Overall, price accounted for over half of cannabis purchase decisions, followed by CBD (nearly 30%). The only exception was medical users, who perceived CBD as the most important attribute (47%) and price as the second most important attribute (41%).
Predictors of Always Opt-Out
In Table S7, we assessed the predictors of always opt-out. A larger proportion of cannabis nonusers (53.88%) always opted out, while a considerably smaller share of cannabis users (7.85%) did so. Age and knowledge about cannabis were the two biggest drivers for opting out among both nonusers and users.
DISCUSSION
This study examined the impacts of THC, CBD, warning messages, and price on individual preferences for legal cannabis flowers among US adults. The study fills important research gaps. It is the first study using DCEs to investigate cannabis product attributes that are important to consumers and policymakers. It is the first to examine heterogeneities in preferences for cannabis between nonusers and users and among users by reason of use.
The findings suggested that, recreational cannabis users and dual users preferred cannabis flowers with higher THC, but never users, former users, and medical cannabis users were not responsive to THC level. This is corroborated with clinical evidence that THC is the primary psychoactive constituent that gets people high (Hall & Degenhardt, 2009). Those who were not interested in getting high were therefore not influenced by THC. Regarding CBD, products with higher CBD were consistently more likely to be chosen by all types of respondents, regardless of their cannabis use history and reason of use. CBD is typically considered providing therapeutic effects without intoxication. Not surprisingly, medical users had the highest WTP for high CBD products and placed the highest relative weight to CBD.
The results on warning messages were mixed and most were counterintuitive. The only type of warning message that discouraged cannabis choice was FDA disapproval disclaimer, with significant effects only in former users. Text warning message was preferred among medical users. We were not able to provide empirical evidence to explain this finding. It is likely that the message was perceived by medical users as drug use instructions that endorse medical use rather than as warnings of health risks. Graphic warning had null associations among most subgroups but positive associations among never users and recreational users. It may be that individuals appropriately separated the decision to use any cannabis from the decision to drive impaired; the graphic may even draw their attention and promote product choice. Future research is warranted to validate the puzzling results in other DCEs or observational data. It is worth noting that the warning messages investigated in our study were very basic, similar to those used on alcohol packages which had little impacts on drinking behaviors (Wilkinson & Room, 2009). The findings should not be generalized to comprehensive and graphic warning messages that have been proven effective in tobacco control (Hammond, 2011).
Consistent with tobacco and alcohol research, products with a higher price were significantly less likely to be chosen in all subgroups. Price also accounted for approximately half of the decisions in all subgroups. Future research is encouraged to evaluate price effects on youth and effects of different tax forms.
This study has limitations. Respondents may have made hypothetical decisions that deviated from their choices in the real world. Studies have suggested satisfactory predictability of behavioral economics experiments on real-world health behaviors in general and drug use behaviors in particular (Olmstead, Alessi, Kline, Pacula, & Petry, 2015; Quaife, Terris-Prestholt, Di Tanna, & Vickerman, 2018), but there is still concern about the external validity of our DCE data. DCEs oversimplify the real world. Respondents typically had more options in the real world than those presented in hypothetical scenarios. Cannabis flowers still dominate the legal retail markets in the US (Schauer, King, Bunnell, Promoff, & McAfee, 2016; Smart, et al., 2017), but other forms of cannabis such as concentrates and edibles have become increasingly popular (Schauer, et al., 2016). We also ignored cannabis alternatives from illicit markets, which might be cheaper without taxes. Future studies should evaluate to what extent the decisions of cannabis purchase in DCEs could be replicated in the reality. Studies are also encouraged to examine alternative forms of cannabis and alternative sources of cannabis.
Another limitation of this study is the ambiguity of the opt-out option. Opting-out to select any cannabis products in the choice scenario may mean choosing not to consume cannabis at all or alternatively mean choosing cannabis from other sources including illicit market. The findings cannot be used to project the extensive margin (whether or not to consume cannabis) or intensive margin (how much cannabis to consume) of consumer behaviors in reality.
In our DCE design, different warning messages were attached to cannabis flowers with different characteristics in the same choice scenario. While this is a common practice in tobacco research (Regmi, et al., 2018), respondents likely carried over the warning message in one choice alternative to another within the same choice scenario. For example, if one choice alterative carried drugged driving warning message and another carried FDA disapproval disclaimer, respondents may interpret that the two messages applied to both alternatives at the same time in the same choice scenario. To our knowledge, however, no studies have assessed how such design may impact the validity of DCE studies. We look forward to future research to address this concern.
Like other studies relying on online convenience panels, the study sample who selfselected to participate in the survey does not fully represent the study population of interest. To what extent the study findings could be generalized to the general population is unknown. Future research is encouraged to utilize representative samples. Furthermore, our findings may not be generalizable to other states that adopted recreational cannabis legalization later than this study. They may not be generalizable to other countries with legal cannabis markets (Uruguay and Canada) either, because these countries regulate retail sale with different approaches.
We were not able to examine the full range of attribute levels. For example, THC free products, which were rare on market, were not considered.
In the introduction of DCEs, respondents were instructed that 1/8th ounce of cannabis flowers was roughly equivalent to 5 joints, as suggested by an empirical study (Mariani, Brooks, Haney, & Levin, 2011). Some users may use bigger or smaller joints in real life. This individual variation should not have influenced individual choices between different choice alternatives, as the 1/8th ounce or 5 joints were invariant across alternatives and scenarios. However, we may have the endogeneity issue if respondents interpreted that the total THC and CBD intake were also varied with the amount of cannabis flowers they put in a joint. Whether this potential endogeneity issue influenced individual decisions is worth further investigations.
Finally, we restricted the study sample to adults aged 21 or older, who can legally purchase cannabis in retail outlets. Nonetheless, youth may have been increasingly exposed to cannabis after the states opened legal markets. The factors influencing their purchase decisions are likely different from adults. For example, they might be more responsive to price but less responsive to health benefits of cannabis. Future studies are encouraged to examine behaviors of younger population.
Given the limitations, the findings derived from DCE hypothetical scenarios may not be directly extrapolated to the real-world setting. Nonetheless, knowing consumer preferences could still inform policymaking. Cannabis with high THC increased severity of dependence, impaired executive function and motor control, and increased risks of anxiety, depression, and psychosis (Di Forti, et al., 2009; Freeman & Winstock, 2015; Hall & Degenhardt, 2009; Ramaekers, et al., 2006; Volkow, Baler, Compton, & Weiss, 2014). Because medical users were not responsive to high THC in our study, policymakers could consider designing policies to restrict THC level. Such policies may have potential to discourage the consumption of high-THC products among recreational users and dual users without influencing medical cannabis use. Warning messages had mixed influences on cannabis purchase decisions. It is suggestive that the warning messages currently adopted by states might not have the expected preventive effects. The consistent effects of price provided support to taxation policies. Of course, whether these potential policy impacts can be realized in the real world need to be validated with observational data.
CONCLUSION
Our study suggested that, for adults in US states with recreational cannabis legalization, potency, warning messages, and price may have influences on their choices of flower products and the influences might differ across different types of consumers. While more research is needed to substantiate the findings in representative samples and observational data, these product attributes may deserve consideration for cannabis regulatory framework.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgement
SurveySamplingInternational. Inc was paid for recruiting respondents from their online panel. Keith Chrzan at Sawtooth Analytics was paid to help design the discrete choice experiment. We thank Dr. James Sallis for edits and comments.
DISCLOSURES
Funding
This research was supported by grant R01DA042290 (PI: Shi) from the US National Institute on Drug Abuse. Shang was supported by grant K99AA024810 from the US National Institute on Alcohol and Alcoholism. This article is the sole responsibility of the authors and does not reflect the views of the US National Institute on Drug Abuse or the US National Institute on Alcohol and Alcoholism.
Footnotes
Conflict of Interest
The authors declared no conflict of interest.
Declaration of Competing Interests: None
The other 3 jurisdictions that had legalized recreational cannabis by the end of 2017 but were not included in this study were Alaska, Maine, and Washington DC. After the survey was implemented, 3 additional states (Illinois, Michigan, and Vermont) also passed laws to legalize recreational cannabis.
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
REFERENCES
- Austen I (2017). Ready or Not, Recreational Marijuana Use Is Coming to Canada. Available at https://www.nytimes.com/2017/11/04/world/canada/canada-marijuana-legal-justin-trudeau.html. Accessed on November 4, 2017 Archived by WebCite at http://www.webcitation.org/71Iw4pewW. In New York Times
- Aydelotte JD, Brown LH, Luftman KM, Mardock AL, Teixeira PGR, Coopwood B, & Brown CVR (2017). Crash Fatality Rates After Recreational Marijuana Legalization in Washington and Colorado. Am J Public Health, 107, 1329–1331. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bridges JF, Hauber AB, Marshall D, Lloyd A, Prosser LA, Regier DA, Johnson FR, & Mauskopf J (2011). Conjoint Analysis Applications in Health - A Checklist: A Report of the ISPOR Good Research Practices for Conjoint Analysis Task Force. Value Health, 14, 403–413. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cerda M, Wall M, Feng T, Keyes KM, Sarvet A, Schulenberg J, O’Malley PM, Pacula RL, Galea S, & Hasin DS (2017). Association of State Recreational Marijuana Laws With Adolescent Marijuana Use. Jama Pediatrics, 171, 142–149. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chung C, Salottolo K, Tanner A 2nd, Carrick MM, Madayag R, Berg G, Lieser M, & Bar-Or D (2019). The impact of Recreational Marijuana Commercialization on Traumatic Injury. Inj Epidemiol, 6, 3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clark MD, Determann D, Petrou S, Moro D, & de Bekker-Grob EW (2014). Discrete Choice Experiments in Health Economics: A Review of the Literature. Pharmacoeconomics, 32, 883–902. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Czoli CD, Goniewicz M, Islam T, Kotnowski K, & Hammond D (2016). Consumer Preferences for Electronic Cigarettes: Results from A Discrete Choice Experiment. Tob Control, 25, e30–36. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Bekker-Grob EW, Donkers B, Jonker MF, & Stolk EA (2015). Sample Size Requirements for Discrete-Choice Experiments in Healthcare: a Practical Guide. Patient, 8, 373–384. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Bekker-Grob EW, Ryan M, & Gerard K (2012). Discrete Choice Experiments in Health Economics: A Review of the Literature. Health Econ, 21, 145–172. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Di Forti M, Morgan C, Dazzan P, Pariante C, Mondelli V, Marques TR, Handley R, Luzi S, Russo M, Paparelli A, Butt A, Stilo SA, Wiffen B, Powell J, & Murray RM (2009). High-potency Cannabis and the Risk of Psychosis. Br J Psychiatry, 195, 488–491. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elder RW, Lawrence B, Ferguson A, Naimi TS, Brewer RD, Chattopadhyay SK, Toomey TL, Fielding JE, & Task Force on Community Preventive, S. (2010). The Effectiveness of Tax Policy Interventions for Reducing Excessive Alcohol Consumption and Related Harms. Am J Prev Med, 38, 217–229. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- FDA. Fda Warns Companies Marketing Unproven Products, Derived From Marijuana, That Claim To Treat Or Cure Cancer. Available at https://www.fda.gov/NewsEvents/Newsroom/PressAnnouncements/ucm583295.htm. Accessed on January 8, 2018 Archived by WebCite at http://www.webcitation.org/71J6JWaVy. Retrieved January 2018 from https://www.fda.gov/NewsEvents/Newsroom/PressAnnouncements/ucm583295.htm.
- Freeman TP, & Winstock AR (2015). Examining The Profile Of High-Potency Cannabis And Its Association With Severity Of Cannabis Dependence. Psychol Med, 45, 3181–3189. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Freisthler B, Gaidus A, Tam C, Ponicki WR, & Gruenewald PJ (2017). From Medical to Recreational Marijuana Sales: Marijuana Outlets and Crime in an Era of Changing Marijuana Legislation. J Prim Prev, 38, 249–263. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gallet CA (2014). Can Price Get The Monkey Off Our Back? A Meta-Analysis Of Illicit Drug Demand. Health Econ, 23, 55–68. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hall W, & Degenhardt L (2009). Adverse Health Effects Of Non-Medical Cannabis Use. Lancet, 374, 1383–1391. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hammond D (2011). Health Warning Messages On Tobacco Products: A Review. Tob Control, 20, 327–337. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hansen B, Miller K, & Weber C Early Evidence on Recreational Marijuana Legalization and Traffic Fatalities. Economic Inquiry, 0. [Google Scholar]
- Helter TM, & Boehler CE (2016). Developing Attributes For Discrete Choice Experiments In Health: A Systematic Literature Review And Case Study Of Alcohol Misuse Interventions. J Subst Use, 21, 662–668. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hensher DA, Rose JM, & Greene WH (2015). Applied Choice Analysis: Second Edition Cambridge: University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Hunt P, & Pacula RL (2017). Early Impacts of Marijuana Legalization: An Evaluation of Prices in Colorado and Washington. J Prim Prev, 38, 221–248. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ingold J (2016). One Attempt At Colorado Marijuana Potency Limit Fails. Available at https://www.denverpost.com/2016/04/08/one-attempt-at-colorado-marijuana-potency-limit-fails/. Accessed on January 8, 2018 Archived by WebCite at http://www.webcitation.org/71J5dhYto. In The Denver Post.
- Izzo AA, Borrelli F, Capasso R, Di Marzo V, & Mechoulam R (2009). Non-psychotropic plant cannabinoids: new therapeutic opportunities from an ancient herb. Trends Pharmacol Sci, 30, 515–527. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jaynes J, Wong WK, & Xu HQ (2016). Using Blocked Fractional Factorial Designs To Construct Discrete Choice Experiments For Healthcare Studies. Statistics in Medicine, 35, 2543–2560. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kerr DCR, Bae H, Phibbs S, & Kern AC (2017). Changes In Undergraduates’ Marijuana, Heavy Alcohol And Cigarette Use Following Legalization Of Recreational Marijuana Use In Oregon. Addiction, 112, 1992–2001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kilmer B (2017). Recreational Cannabis - Minimizing the Health Risks from Legalization. N Engl J Med, 376, 705–707. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- LawAtlas. Recreational Marijuana Laws. Retrieved July 2019 from http://lawatlas.org/datasets/recreational-marijuana-laws.
- Lockshin L, Wade JA, d’Hauteville F, & Perrouty JP (2006). Using Simulations From Discrete Choice Experiments To Measure Consumer Sensitivity To Brand, Region, Price, And Awards In Wine Choice. Food Quality and Preference, 17, 166–178. [Google Scholar]
- Mariani JJ, Brooks D, Haney M, & Levin FR (2011). Quantification and comparison of marijuana smoking practices: blunts, joints, and pipes. Drug Alcohol Depend, 113, 249–251. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mehmedic Z, Chandra S, Slade D, Denham H, Foster S, Patel AS, Ross SA, Khan IA, & ElSohly MA (2010). Potency Trends Of Delta9-Thc And Other Cannabinoids In Confiscated Cannabis Preparations From 1993 To 2008. J Forensic Sci, 55, 1209–1217. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miroff N (2017). In Uruguay’s Marijuana Experiment, The Government Is Your Pot Dealer. Available at https://www.washingtonpost.com/world/the_americas/in-uruguays-marijuana-experiment-the-government-is-your-pot-dealer/2017/07/07/6212360c-5a88-11e7-aa69-3964a7d55207_story.html?utm_term=.870987e95d47. Accessed on January 8, 2018 Archived by WebCite at http://www.webcitation.org/71J4cxXCa. In Washington Post [Google Scholar]
- NationalAcademies. (2017). The Health Effects Of Cannabis And Cannabinoids: The Current State Of Evidence And Recommendations For Research. In: US National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, Medicine. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nevada. FAQs for Marijuana Establishments. Available at https://tax.nv.gov/FAQs/Retail_Marijuana/. Accessed on January 8, 2018 Archived by WebCite at http://www.webcitation.org/71J6Aof0H. Retrieved January 2018 from https://tax.nv.gov/FAQs/Retail_Marijuana/.
- Niesink RJM, & van Laar M (2013). Does Cannabidiol Protect Against Adverse Psychological Effects of THC? Frontiers in Psychiatry, 4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Olmstead TA, Alessi SM, Kline B, Pacula RL, & Petry NM (2015). The price elasticity of demand for heroin: Matched longitudinal and experimental evidence. J Health Econ, 41, 59–71. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oregon. Marijuana Tax. Available at http://www.oregon.gov/dor/press/documents/marijuana_fact_sheet.pdf. Accessed on January 8, 2018 Archived by WebCite at http://www.webcitation.org/71J66Afic. Retrieved January 2018.
- Orens A, Light M, Lewandowski B, Rowberry J, & Saloga C (2018). Market Size and Demand for Marijuana in Colorado 2017 Market Update. Available at https://www.colorado.gov/pacific/sites/default/files/MED%20Demand%20and%20Market%20%20Study%20%20082018.pdf. Accessed on May 5, 2019 In: Marijuana Policy Group. [Google Scholar]
- Pacula RL, Jacobson M, & Maksabedian EJ (2016). In The Weeds: A Baseline View Of Cannabis Use Among Legalizing States And Their Neighbours. Addiction, 111, 973–980. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pacula RL, Kilmer B, Wagenaar AC, Chaloupka FJ, & Caulkins JP (2014). Developing Public Health Regulations For Marijuana: Lessons From Alcohol And Tobacco. Am J Public Health, 104, 1021–1028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pacula RL, & Lundberg R (2014). Why Changes in Price Matter When Thinking About Marijuana Policy: A Review of the Literature on the Elasticity of Demand. Public Health Rev, 35, 1–18. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pechey R, Burge P, Mentzakis E, Suhrcke M, & Marteau TM (2014). Public Acceptability Of Population-Level Interventions To Reduce Alcohol Consumption: A Discrete Choice Experiment. Soc Sci Med, 113, 104–109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Procon.org. 17 States with Laws Specifically about Legal Cannabidiol (CBD). Available at https://medicalmarijuana.procon.org/view.resource.php?resourceID=006473. Accessed on May 5, 2019 Archived by WebCite at http://www.webcitation.org/78ApvZqQc.
- Quaife M, Terris-Prestholt F, Di Tanna GL, & Vickerman P (2018). How well do discrete choice experiments predict health choices? A systematic review and meta-analysis of external validity. Eur J Health Econ, 19, 1053–1066. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ramaekers JG, Kauert G, van Ruitenbeek P, Theunissen EL, Schneider E, & Moeller MR (2006). High-Potency Marijuana Impairs Executive Function And Inhibitory Motor Control. Neuropsychopharmacology, 31, 2296–2303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reed Johnson F, Lancsar E, Marshall D, Kilambi V, Muhlbacher A, Regier DA, Bresnahan BW, Kanninen B, & Bridges JF (2013). Constructing Experimental Designs For Discrete-Choice Experiments: Report Of The Ispor Conjoint Analysis Experimental Design Good Research Practices Task Force. Value Health, 16, 3–13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Regmi K, Kaphle D, Timilsina S, & Tuha NAA (2018). Application of Discrete-Choice Experiment Methods in Tobacco Control: A Systematic Review. Pharmacoecon Open, 2, 5–17. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rough L Cannabis Tax Rates: A State-By-State Guide. Available at https://www.leafly.com/news/industry/marijuana-tax-rates-by-state. Accessed on January 7, 2018 Archived by WebCite at http://www.webcitation.org/71J5z541W. Retrieved January 2018 from https://www.leafly.com/news/industry/marijuana-tax-rates-by-state.
- Ryan M, & Gerard K (2003). Using Discrete Choice Experiments To Value Health Care Programmes: Current Practice And Future Research Reflections. Appl Health Econ Health Policy, 2, 55–64. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Salloum RG, Maziak W, Hammond D, Nakkash R, Islam F, Cheng X, & Thrasher JF (2015). Eliciting Preferences For Waterpipe Tobacco Smoking Using A Discrete Choice Experiment: Implications For Product Regulation. BMJ Open, 5, e009497. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- SAMHSA. (2017). Key Substance Use And Mental Health Indicators In The United States: Results From The 2017 National Survey On Drug Use And Health (HHS Publication No. SMA 18–5068, NSDUH Series H-53). In. Rockville, MD: Center for Behavioral Health Statistics and Quality, Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration. [Google Scholar]
- Schauer GL, King BA, Bunnell RE, Promoff G, & McAfee TA (2016). Toking, Vaping, and Eating for Health or Fun: Marijuana Use Patterns in Adults, U.S., 2014. Am J Prev Med, 50, 1–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shang C, Huang J, Chaloupka FJ, & Emery SL (2017). The Impact Of Flavour, Device Type And Warning Messages On Youth Preferences For Electronic Nicotine Delivery Systems: Evidence From An Online Discrete Choice Experiment. Tob Control. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- ShatterLabels. A State-By State Guide To Cannabis Packaging And Labeling Laws. Available at https://shatterlabels.com/news/a-state-by-state-guide-to-cannabis-packaging-and-labeling-laws. Accessed on January 7, 2018 Archived by WebCite at http://www.webcitation.org/71J5hyyNA. Retrieved January 2018.
- Shi Y, Liang D, Bao Y, An R, Wallace MS, & Grant I (2019). Recreational Marijuana Legalization And Prescription Opioids Received By Medicaid Enrollees. Drug Alcohol Depend, 194, 13–19. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smart R, Caulkins JP, Kilmer B, Davenport S, & Midgette G (2017). Variation In Cannabis Potency And Prices In A Newly Legal Market: Evidence From 30 Million Cannabis Sales In Washington State. Addiction, 112, 2167–2177. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Soekhai V, de Bekker-Grob EW, Ellis AR, & Vass CM (2019). Discrete Choice Experiments in Health Economics: Past, Present and Future. Pharmacoeconomics, 37, 201–226. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- StateofColorado. (2019). Marijuana Sales January 2014 to Date. Available at https://www.colorado.gov/pacific/sites/default/files/0219_MJSalesCalendarReport_PUBLISH.pdf. Accessed on May 5, 2019 In: Colorado Department of Revenue. [Google Scholar]
- TaxFoundation. Massachusetts Increases Marijuana Tax Rate. Available at https://taxfoundation.org/massachusetts-marijuana-tax-increase/. Accessed on January 7, 2018 Archived by WebCite at http://www.webcitation.org/71J6Eom2o. Retrieved January 2018.
- Volkow ND, Baler RD, Compton WM, & Weiss SR (2014). Adverse Health Effects Of Marijuana Use. N Engl J Med, 370, 2219–2227. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wagenaar AC, Salois MJ, & Komro KA (2009). Effects Of Beverage Alcohol Price And Tax Levels On Drinking: A Meta-Analysis Of 1003 Estimates From 112 Studies. Addiction, 104, 179–190. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wagenaar AC, Tobler AL, & Komro KA (2010). Effects Of Alcohol Tax And Price Policies On Morbidity And Mortality: A Systematic Review. Am J Public Health, 100, 2270–2278. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weedmaps. weedmaps.com. Retrieved January 2018 from https://weedmaps.com/.
- Wilkinson C, & Room R (2009). Warnings On Alcohol Containers And Advertisements: International Experience And Evidence On Effects. Drug Alcohol Rev, 28, 426–435. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson LM, Avila Tang E, Chander G, Hutton HE, Odelola OA, Elf JL, Heckman-Stoddard BM, Bass EB, Little EA, Haberl EB, & Apelberg BJ (2012). Impact Of Tobacco Control Interventions On Smoking Initiation, Cessation, And Prevalence: A Systematic Review. J Environ Public Health, 2012, 961724. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.