Skip to main content
eNeuro logoLink to eNeuro
. 2019 Dec 2;6(6):ENEURO.0335-18.2019. doi: 10.1523/ENEURO.0335-18.2019

From State-to-Trait Meditation: Reconfiguration of Central Executive and Default Mode Networks

C C C Bauer 1,2,3,4,, S Whitfield-Gabrieli 1,4, J L Díaz 5, E H Pasaye 2, F A Barrios 1,2,
PMCID: PMC6893234  PMID: 31694816

While brain default mode network (DMN) activation in human subjects has been associated with mind wandering, meditation practice has been found to suppress it and to increase psychological well-being. In addition to DMN activity reduction, experienced meditators (EMs) during meditation practice show an increased connectivity between the DMN and the central executive network (CEN).

Keywords: experienced meditators, fALFF, functional connectivity, mind-wandering, mindfulness, top-down control

Abstract

While brain default mode network (DMN) activation in human subjects has been associated with mind wandering, meditation practice has been found to suppress it and to increase psychological well-being. In addition to DMN activity reduction, experienced meditators (EMs) during meditation practice show an increased connectivity between the DMN and the central executive network (CEN). However, the gradual change between DMN and CEN configuration from pre-meditation, during meditation, and post-meditation is unknown. Here, we investigated the change in DMN and CEN configuration by means of brain activity and functional connectivity (FC) analyses in EMs across three back-to-back functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) scans: pre-meditation baseline (trait), meditation (state), and post-meditation (state-to-trait). Pre-meditation baseline group comparison was also performed between EMs and healthy controls (HCs). Meditation trait was characterized by a significant reduction in activity and FC within DMN and increased anticorrelations between DMN and CEN. Conversely, meditation state and meditation state-to-trait periods showed increased activity and FC within the DMN and between DMN and CEN. However, the latter anticorrelations were only present in EMs with limited practice. The interactions between networks during these states by means of positive diametric activity (PDA) of the fractional amplitude of low-frequency fluctuations (fALFFs) defined as CENfALFF¯DMNfALFF¯ revealed no trait differences but significant increases during meditation state that persisted in meditation state-to-trait. The gradual reconfiguration in DMN and CEN suggest a neural mechanism by which the CEN negatively regulates the DMN and is probably responsible for the long-term trait changes seen in meditators and reported psychological well-being.

Significance Statement

We introduce a positive diametric activity (PDA) metric to measure the relation between two anticorrelated rs-fMRI networks in experienced meditators (EMs). PDA as well as functional connectivity (FC) are increased during meditation compared to resting state and persists in the post-meditation resting state. Conversely, meditation trait is characterized by positive PDA but with significant reduction in activity and FC within default mode network (DMN) and increased anticorrelations between DMN and central executive network (CEN). PDA and FC between DMN and CEN distinguish meditation state-to-trait effects.

Introduction

What does the mind do when all physiologic needs are taken care of and there are no immediate demands? Does it part into an empty void, a dormant state to conserve resources until something disturbs it and activity is needed? We all know that this is not the case. Every moment of our waking experience challenges this explanation of mental life. When nothing requires active cogitative processing, our mind generally tends to think about what is not going on in the present moment, i.e., it wanders, recapitulates events that happened in the past, or fantasizes about what might happen in the future, jumping from one thought to the next with amazing speed and apparent randomness (Mason et al., 2007). Undeniably, this never stopping stream of consciousness appears to be our brain’s idle mode of being, our default mode when not otherwise engaged (Mason et al., 2007; Christoff et al., 2016; Kane et al., 2017). Although this ability is as an evolutionary achievement that allows us to learn, reason, and plan (Mantini et al., 2013; Rilling, 2014), it swiftly fails to serve its adaptive function, and turns into a risk factor for health and psychological well-being whenever it becomes a rigid and inflexible pattern with tremendous emotional costs (Smallwood et al., 2009; Killingsworth and Gilbert, 2010; Ottaviani et al., 2013; Simon and Engström, 2015; Jazaieri et al., 2016). Consequently, the underlying mental processes that keep the brain active when not otherwise engaged have been a source of significant theoretical surmise. Since the advent of neurophysiological recordings, it has been determined that the brain is never truly at rest (Berger and Hans, 1933). From a functional imaging perspective, the remarkable properties of the brain’s intrinsic activity were first noted by Biswal et al. (1995). They observed that the “noise” in the spontaneous functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) signal exhibited striking patterns of spatial coherence corresponding, in their case, to the sensorimotor regions of the cerebral cortex. Subsequently, Raichle and colleagues first took notice that during the commonly used “baseline” in research paradigms, the synchronous behavior of a number of anatomic regions were observed to deactivate during task and thus initially identified as a network of task-specific deactivations, later named the default mode network (DMN; Gusnard et al., 2001; Buckner et al., 2008). Soon it was determined that these task specific deactivations of the DMN also showed patterns of coherent activation during periods of rest that included anterior (medial prefrontal cortex, MPFC) and posterior midline structures (posterior cingulate, PCC) as well as lateral temporal cortex (LTC) and the hippocampus. Additionally, in healthy individuals, DMN activity has been shown to be anticorrelated (negatively correlated) with brain regions activated during attention demanding tasks (e.g., the central executive network, CEN; Greicius et al., 2003; Fox et al., 2005; Fransson, 2005; Kelly et al., 2008; Uddin et al., 2009). Specifically, the CEN, typically including the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) and posterior parietal cortex (PPC), supports these attention demanding tasks, i.e., attentional control and working memory. Across individuals greater magnitude of DMN-CEN anticorrelations is associated with superior cognitive function such as complex working memory (Whitfield-Gabrieli et al., 2009; Hampson et al., 2010; Keller et al., 2015). Abnormal DMN activity, such as competitive, antagonistic DMN activation during CEN activity or changes in connectivity between subregions of the DMN, has also been associated not only with lower levels of happiness (Killingsworth and Gilbert, 2010; Smallwood and O’Connor, 2011), but with a number of psychological disorders such as anxiety (Zhao et al., 2007), depression (Sheline et al., 2009), schizophrenia (Garrity, 2007; Pomarol-Clotet et al., 2008; Whitfield-Gabrieli et al., 2009; Camchong et al., 2011; Bastos-Leite et al., 2015), epilepsy (Liao et al., 2011), autism (Assaf et al., 2010), attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD; Uddin et al., 2008), and Alzheimer’s disease (AD; Greicius et al., 2004; Sheline and Raichle, 2013). Recent studies further suggest that abnormal DMN activity and connectivity plays a role in neuropsychiatric disorders (Whitfield-Gabrieli and Ford, 2012; Raichle, 2015). These associations have led to the suggestion of using the DMN as method by which to study mental disorders, resulting in a growing body of literature concerning disorder-specific variations within the DMN (Arens et al., 2003; Greicius, 2008; Broyd et al., 2009; Fox and Greicius, 2010; Whitfield-Gabrieli and Ford, 2012; Simon and Engström, 2015).

Given the interrelationship between mind-wandering, DMN activity, and the risk to health and psychological well-being, a question arises: Is it possible to change this maladaptive mode into one that is more pleasant and healthier? According to many philosophical and contemplative traditions, yes, this is possible. Thus, these teach that happiness is to be found by “living in the moment,” i.e., to be here and now without losing oneself in past or future thought. For more than two millennia, meditation has been practiced as a means of achieving this ephemeral mind state, psychological equanimity, and self-awareness, yet it has only recently become the target of systematic Western-world research for its relevance to mental and physical health in fields such as medicine, psychology, and neuroscience (Van Dam et al., 2018). Undeniably, meditation is becoming increasingly well regarded for its therapeutic promise (Buchholz, 2015; Creswell, 2015; Gu et al., 2015; Simon and Engström, 2015) and meditation methods have been beneficial in the treatment of psychological disorders such as schizophrenia (Chien and Thompson, 2014), depression (Teasdale et al., 2000; Ma and Teasdale, 2004; Eisendrath et al., 2008; Kuyken et al., 2008; Yang et al., 2016), anxiety (Baer, 2003; Grossman et al., 2004; Ludwig and Kabat-Zinn, 2008; Shen et al., 2014), addiction (Bowen et al., 2014), alcoholism (Witkiewitz et al., 2005; Garland et al., 2010), smoking (Tang et al., 2013), mild cognitive impairment (MCI; Wells et al., 2013), and ADHD (Zylowska et al., 2008; Bueno et al., 2015; Janssen et al., 2015). Preliminary findings have suggested that the effects of meditation include better emotion regulation (Lutz et al., 2014; Turner, 2014; Prakash et al., 2015), self-regulation (Tang et al., 2014), awareness and self-perception (Hölzel et al., 2011b), memory and cognition (Zeidan et al., 2010), attention (Moore et al., 2012), working memory (Mrazek et al., 2013; Banks et al., 2015), as well as gray and white matter differences in experienced meditators (EMs; Luders et al., 2009; Hölzel et al., 2011a; Fox et al., 2014). Functional imaging studies on meditation practice have examined two distinct effects of meditation. The first, called a state effect, refers to the short-term consequences of meditation practice on the individual’s state. This would include short lived changes in bodily awareness, relaxation, emotion regulation, attention and in BOLD activation or functional connectivity (FC) when measured with fMRI. During this active state of meditation, studies have consistently found that within-network connectivity of the DMN as well as between-network connectivity of DMN, CEN and salience network (SAL) are increased (Brewer et al., 2011; Jang et al., 2011; Garrison et al., 2014; Jao et al., 2016). The second, called a trait effect, refers to long-lasting changes in these same dimensions, which continue after practice and during the meditator’s daily life (Lutz et al., 2007). However, the findings for this trait effect have not been so clear when measured with resting-state FC (rsFC), with mixed results (for review, see Mooneyham et al., 2016). Furthermore, little to nothing is known about the activity and connectivity change of DMN and CEN that intertwine the transitory state effects of meditation with the lasting trait effects of meditation practice.

Here, we hypothesized that activity of the CEN is an important modulator of DMN activity and connectivity, both during the meditation state and during the transition phase post-meditation. Support for this idea comes on the one hand from correlational neuroimaging studies looking into attentional and cognitive control (McKiernan et al., 2003; Fox et al., 2005; Fransson, 2005; Dosenbach et al., 2007; Northoff et al., 2007; Sonuga-Barke and Castellanos, 2007; Sridharan et al., 2008; Anticevic et al., 2012), but specifically from a study by Chen et al. (2013) that provides direct evidence for this neural mechanism. In their study, they specifically tested this mechanism by exciting or inhibiting nodes within the CEN using noninvasive brain stimulation and observed the results using simultaneous brain imaging. They found that the DMN is under inhibitory control specifically from a node in CEN (posterior middle frontal gyrus pMFG; BA 9/10). Accordingly, we predicted that brain activation in nodes of the CEN during meditation would: (1) modulate activity of the DMN and increase connectivity between DMN and CEN, (2) these changes would persist after meditation and finally, (3) that there are trait differences in DMN and CEN activity and connectivity characteristic of meditation experience. To test these predictions, we conducted three back-to-back fMRI runs in EMs: pre-meditation baseline (trait), meditation (state), and post-meditation (state-to-trait). The pre-meditation baseline was also performed on a group of healthy controls (HCs) as a comparison.

Materials and Methods

Participants

After standard exclusion criteria for fMRI research were applied, 16 EMs (six females, mean age 41.12 years, SD 10.5, average of 1677 ± 367 h of Vipassana meditation experience) and 17 meditation naive HCs (seven females, mean age 35.70 years, SD 4.7) participated in the study. There was no significant age difference between groups (t(21) = –1.68, p = 0.10). All subject groups were recruited and scanned in the same time period as part of the same experiment and all subjects gave informed consent for the experimental procedure, and the protocol had IRB approval.

Experiment design

BOLD fMRI data were collected from all participants during a 5-min, eyes-open resting-state period (rsBase). In addition, immediately following the rsBase scan, EM participants were also scanned during a 20-min, eyes-open period while engaged in Vipassana meditation (Med). Vipassana meditation emphasizes focused attention typically involving the deliberate focus of attention to a chosen target, such as general body sensations or sensations related to breathing, and the voluntary redirection of attention each time it lapses (Hart, 2011). Finally, EM underwent a second 5-min eyes-open resting-state period (rsPost).

Meditation trait, state, and state-to-trait operationalizations (Austin, 1999; Shapiro and Walsh, 1984; West, 2016)

Meditation trait

The lasting changes in sensory, cognitive, and self-referential awareness and their underlying brain activity and connectivity that persist in the meditator irrespective of being actively engaged in meditation. Contrast HC rsBase < EM rsBase (Fig. 1Ca).

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

State-to-trait meditation brain activity changes of the fractional Amplitude of Low-Frequency Fluctuations (fALFFs). Aa, Brain regions showing trait changes in fALFF between meditators and HCs at baseline (HC rsBase < EM rsBase). Ab, Brain regions that show significant changes in fALFF during the meditation state in meditators (EM rsBase < EM Med). Ac, Brain regions that show significant changes in fALFF during the transition from state-to-trait meditation in meditators (EM rsBase < EM rsPost). B, Boxplot showing the mean fALFF z-scores in blue (DMN) and red (CEN) and PDA scores in green during baseline (rsBase), meditation (Med) and post-meditation (rsPost) for HCs and EMs. C, Schematic representation of state-to-trait contrasts. All stats shown are nonparametric (5000 permutations) with height threshold p < 0.05 and cluster-size FDR-corrected p < 0.05. Black dots represent subjects that lie beyond the whiskers.

Meditation state

Refers to the altered sensory, cognitive, and self-referential awareness that can arise during meditation practice and their underlying brain activity and connectivity. In the present study this will correspond to the contrast EM rsBase < EM Med (Fig. 1Cb).

Meditation state-to-trait

Refers to the changes in sensory, cognitive, and self-referential awareness and their underlying brain activity and connectivity that persist in the meditator after an active engaged meditation session. Contrast EM rsBase < EM rsPost (Fig. 1Cc).

MRI data acquisition

MRI imaging was performed on a 3.0T GE MR750 instrument (General Electric) using a 32-channel head coil. Functional imaging for resting state included 35 slices, acquired using a T2*-weighted EPI sequence with TR/TE 2000/40 ms, a 64 × 64 matrix and 4-mm slice thickness, resulting in a 4 × 4 × 4 mm3 isometric voxel and a total of 151 volumes. For meditation imaging included 35 slices, acquired using a T2*-weighted EPI sequence with TR/TE 1500/40 ms, a 64 × 64 matrix and 4-mm slice thickness, resulting in a 4 × 4 × 4 mm3 isometric voxel and a total of 804 volumes. The shorter TR during meditation was chosen according to the parameters in Hasenkamp et al. (2012) to adhere to additional experiential sampling during meditation and thus needed a faster acquisition. Importantly, this discrepancy has been shown not to affect fractional amplitude of low-frequency fluctuation (fALFF) nor FC correlation coefficients (CCs) since multiple repetition times have been empirically compared to address the issue of temporal mismatch (Wu et al., 2011). Additionally, high-resolution structural 3D-T1-weighted images were acquired for anatomic localization (resolution of 1 × 1 × 1 mm3, TR = 2.3 s, TE = 3 ms) covering the whole brain. The images were acquired with an acceleration factor = 2.

Preprocessing

The preprocessing of resting-state images was done using SPM 12 software (http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm) implemented in a MATLAB suite (MathWorks, Inc.). It included slice time correction, head motion correction, co-registration to subjects’ structural images, segmentation, normalization, linear detrending, and smoothing (FWHM = 8 mm).

Brain activity analysis

To detect regional brain activity changes we used an improved approach of the ALFFs method, fALFF (Zou et al., 2008 ). This method takes the ratio of power spectrum of low-frequency (here: 0.008–0.09 Hz) to that of the entire frequency range. fALFF analysis was conducted with AFNI’s 3dRSFC (Taylor and Saad, 2013 ). Similar to the procedures of previous literature (Zou et al., 2008; Shpaner et al., 2014; Kong et al., 2015), the time series of each voxel was transformed to a frequency domain after the linear trend was removed without bandpass filtering. The square root was then calculated at each frequency of the power spectrum, and finally the sum of amplitude across 0.008–0.09 Hz was divided by that across the entire frequency range (0–0.25 Hz for TR = 2 s and 0–0.33 Hz for TR = 1.5 s, see fMRI data acquisition) to obtain fALFF. Importantly, the TR discrepancy has been shown not to affect fALFF since multiple repetition times have been empirically compared to address the issue of temporal mismatch ( Wu et al., 2011 ). Next, we obtained the meditation trait differences of the fALFF maps of HC and EM at baseline (rsBase) using two-sample t tests. The thresholded images were then converted into binarized masks and were used as the target regions of interest (ROIs) from which to extract the mean fALFF maps for all subjects and states. All imaging analysis were performed with FSL’s randomize tool for nonparametric permutation (5000 permutations) inference on neuroimaging data (Winkler et al., 2014 ) and masked with the binarized DMN and CEN templates of Yeo et al. (2011 ) and threshold-free cluster enhancement (TFCE; Smith and Nichols, 2009 ) cluster corrected (p < 0.05) . We first obtained the meditation trait differences of the fALFF maps of HC and EM at baseline (rsBase) using two-sample t test and then extracted the mean fALFF maps for all subjects and states. The extracted values were converted to normally distributed z-scores to allow for second-level analyses using multilevel modeling one-way repeated measures ANOVA and post hoc Tukey’s test. Statistical analysis was performed with the open-source R package (www.R-project.org).

Positive diametric activity (PDA)

To further assess the information processing during meditation, we introduce a PDA metric to determine the activation change of CEN and DMN according to their mean fALFF. The PDA metric is based on the hypothesis that there is a causal neural mechanism by which the CEN negatively regulates the DMN (Chen et al., 2013). This is to say that when CEN activity is increased this produces an inhibitory effect on the DMN and its activity is decreased. Accordingly, we define the PDA as follows:

PDA=CENfALFF¯DMNfALFF¯,

where PDA will always be positive as long as the mean activity of CEN is greater than the mean activity of DMN, reflecting precisely this negative relation between CEN and DMN. Meditation trait differences of the PDA for HC and EM at baseline (rsBase) was assessed using two-sample t tests. The PDAs for EM rsBase, Med, and rsPost were analyzed using multilevel modeling one-way repeated measures ANOVA and post hoc Tukey’s test.

FC analysis

FC analysis was performed using a seed-driven approach with in-house, custom software CONN 16.b (Chai et al., 2012; Whitfield-Gabrieli et al., 2012). We performed seed-voxel correlations by estimating maps showing temporal correlations between the BOLD signal from the target ROIs and that of every brain voxel (i.e., whole-brain analysis). The specific target ROI clusters were obtained from the previous fALFF analysis two-sample t tests between HC and EM at rsBase for the DMN and CEN respectively (see above, Brain activity analysis). This yielded seed ROIs for the DMN in the medial prefrontal cortices (see clusters 1 and 2 in Fig. 1Aa and Table 1), which are nodes that have been implicated in processing of self-referential stimuli and in generating a model of the self (Northoff and Bermpohl, 2004) and for the CEN in bilateral inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) and inferior parietal lobule (IPL; see clusters 3, 4, and 5 in Fig. 1Aa and Table 1). Physiologic and other spurious sources of noise were estimated and regressed out using the anatomic CompCor method (aCompCor; Chai et al., 2012). Global signal regression, a widely used preprocessing method, was not used because it artificially creates negative correlations that prevent the interpretation of anticorrelation (Behzadi et al., 2007; Chai et al., 2012; Whitfield-Gabrieli et al., 2012) and can contribute to group differences in positive correlations (Saad et al., 2012). Instead, aCompCor allows for interpretation of anticorrelations and yields higher specificity and sensitivity compared with global signal regression (Chai et al., 2012). A temporal bandpass filter of 0.008 to 0.09 Hz was applied simultaneously to all regressors in the model. We used methods that minimize the influence of motion and artifact and that allow for valid identification of correlated and anticorrelated networks (Behzadi et al., 2007; Chai et al., 2012; Whitfield-Gabrieli et al., 2012). To address the spurious correlations in resting-state networks caused by head motion we used quality assurance software artifact detection tools (http://www.nitrc.org/projects/artifact_detect; http://www.nitrc.org/projects/conn; Whitfield-Gabrieli et al., 2012) to identify problematic time points during the scan. Specifically, an image was defined as an outlier if the head displacement in x, y, or z direction was greater than 0.5 mm from the previous frame, or if the global mean intensity in the image was >3 SDs from the mean image intensity for the entire resting scan. A single regressor for each outlier image was included in the first level general linear model along with motion parameters and first order derivatives (there were no significant differences between groups and runs; Fig. 1). The anatomic image for each participant was segmented into white matter, gray matter, and CSF masks using SPM 12. To minimize partial voluming with gray matter, the white matter and CSF masks were eroded by one voxel, which resulted in substantially smaller masks than the original segmentations (Chai et al., 2012). The eroded white matter and CSF masks were then used as noise ROIs. Signals from the white matter and CSF noise ROIs were extracted from the unsmoothed functional volumes to avoid additional risk of contaminating white matter and CSF signals with gray matter signals. Previous results showed that aCompCor signals were considerably different from the global signal, as regressing higher order principal components of the global signal diminished both positive and negative correlations whereas regressing aCompCor signals resulted in stronger anticorrelations and eliminated spurious correlations (Behzadi et al., 2007). Time series of all the voxels within each seed were averaged, and first-level correlation maps were produced by extracting the residual BOLD time course from each seed and computing Pearson correlation coefficients (CCs) between that time course and the time course of all other voxels. CCs were converted to normally distributed z-scores using the Fisher transformation to allow for second-level general linear model analyses. Meditation Trait differences of HC and EM at baseline (rsBase) were compared using two-sample t tests. Second-level analyses for EM rsBase, Med, and rsPost, were compared using with a one-way repeated measures ANOVA implemented in CONN. All contrasts are nonparametric (1000 permutations) with height threshold p < 0.05 and cluster-size FDR-corrected p < 0.05, unless otherwise stated.

Table 1.

Differences in fractional Amplitude of Low-Frequency Fluctuations (fALFF) for the different meditation states

Region BA Voxels MNI (x,y,z mm) p value*
Meditation trait effects
HC rsBase > EM rsBase
Default Mode Network (DMN)
Medial Temporal Gyrus L 21 1302 –66,–34,–6 0.02
Medial Prefrontal Cortex R 10 18 6,60,26 0.01
Medial Prefrontal Cortex L 10 147 –2,58,12 0.05
Central Executive Network (CEN)
Inferior Frontal Gyrus L 9 405 –34,6,34 0.01
Inferior Frontal Gyrus R 9 144 54,20,20 0.03
Inferior Parietal Lobule L 40 74 –54,–38,50 0.05
Meditation state effects
EM Med > EM rsBase
Default Mode Network (DMN)
No significant differences N/A N/A N/A N/A
Central Executive Network (CEN)
Inferior Frontal Gyrus R 9 115 46,18,30 0.02
Inferior Frontal Gyrus L 9 82 –44,10,26 0.03
Meditation state-to-trait effects
EM rsPost > EM rsBase
Default Mode Network (DMN)
No significant differences N/A N/A N/A N/A
Central Executive Network (CEN)
Inferior Frontal Gyrus L 9 17 –46,18,34 0.02
Inferior Frontal Gyrus R 9 4 50,28,38 0.05

Brodmann areas (BA), number of voxels and Montreal Neurological Institute coordinates (MNI).

*All statistics are nonparametric (5000 permutations) with height threshold p < 0.05 and cluster-size FDR-corrected p < 0.05.

Correlation between PDA and FC

To assess whether there is a relationship between brain activity (fALFF) and FC, we correlated the individual PDA scores (see above) with differences in FC of DMN and CEN ROIs for the different meditation states.

Results

Meditation trait effects

fALFF analysis

DMN

EM showed reduced activity in the left medial temporal gyrus (MTG; BA 21), right superior frontal gyrus (SFG; BA 6), left MPFC; BA 10, Fig. 1Aa1,Aa2, blue box plots in Fig. 1B (HC rsBase < EM rsBase) rs Base and Table 1.

CEN

EM showed reduced activity in bilateral DLPFC; BA 9, Fig. 1Aa3,Aa4; red box plots (HC rsBase < EM rsBase) in Fig. 1B; Table 1 and left IPL (BA 40, Fig. 1Aa5; Table 1). For this and all other fALFF significant MNI coordinates, see Table 1.

FC analysis

DMN

EM showed reduced connectivity with left SFG (BA 11), right medial frontal gyrus (MFG; BA 10), IPL (BA 40), and superior temporal gyrus (STG; BA 38), Fig. 2Aa; blue box plots (HC rsBase < EM rsBase) in Fig. 2B and Table 2. For this and all other significant connectivity coordinates, see Table 2.

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

State-to-trait meditation functional connectivity (FC) changes. Aa, Brain regions showing trait FC changes between experienced meditators (EM) and healthy controls (HCs) at baseline (HC rsBase < EM rsBase). Ab, Brain regions that show significant FC changes during the meditation state in meditators (EM rsBase < EM Med). Ac, Brain regions that show significant FC changes during the transition from state-to-trait meditation in meditators (EM rsBase < EM rsPost). Dark green (DMN ROIs 1 and 2) and light green (CEN ROIs 3 and 4) clusters show in each case the seeds used to determine the shown contrast (Fig. 1Aa). B, Boxplot showing mean FC z-scores in blue (DMNs 1 and 2) and red (CENs 3 and 4) during baseline (rsBase), meditation (Med), and post-meditation (rsPost) for HCs and EMs. C, Schematic representation of state-to-trait contrasts. All stats shown are nonparametric (1000 permutations) with height threshold p < 0.05 and cluster-size FDR-corrected p < 0.05. Black dots represent subjects that lie beyond the whiskers.

Table 2.

Differences in functional connectivity (FC) for the different meditation states

Region Connectivity BA Voxels MNI (x,y,z mm) p value*
Meditation trait effects
EM rsBase > HC rsBase
Default Mode Network (DMN)
Superior Frontal Gyrus Reduced L 11 >1000 –16,60,26 0.01
Middle Frontal Gyrus Reduced L 9 >200 –26,26,32 0.01
Inferior Parietal Lobule Reduced L 40 >200 –38,–48,26 0.02
Superior Temporal Gyrus Reduced R 38 >200 48,16,–20 0.03
Central Executive Network (CEN)
No significant differences N/A N/A N/A N/A N/A
Meditation state effects
EM Med > EM rsBase
Default Mode Network (DMN)
No significant differences N/A N/A N/A N/A N/A
Central Executive Network (CEN)
Middle frontal gyrus Increased L 10 >200 –29,48,15 0.02
Middle frontal gyrus Increased R 10 >200 31,52,12 0.02
Anterior cingulate cortex Increased L 32 >200 4,30,24 0.02
Posterior cingulate cortex Increased R 31 >200 3,–31,38 0.03
Inferior Parietal Lobule Increased L 40 >200 –38,–46,46 0.03
State-to-trait effects
EM rsPost > EM rsBase
Default Mode Network (DMN)
No significant differences N/A N/A N/A N/A N/A
Central Executive Network (CEN)
Precuneus Increased L 23 >1000 –2,–42,28 0.02
Angular gyrus Increased R 39 >1000 50,–66,33 0.02

Brodmann areas (BA), number of voxels and Montreal Neurological Coordinates (MIN).

*nonparametric (1000 permutations) with height threshold p < 0.05 and cluster-size FDR-corrected p < 0.05.

CEN

No differences were found.

PDA analysis

No significant differences were found.

Correlations between PDA and FC

DMN

No significant correlations were found.

CEN

No significant correlations were found.

Correlations between hours of meditation practice and FC

DMN

No significant correlations were found.

CEN

Whole-brain FC analysis revealed a significant correlation (R = 0.87) between the hours of meditation practice and connectivity between CEN and MPFC at rsBase for EM (Fig. 4A).

Figure 4.

Figure 4.

Correlations between meditation hours (MedHrs) and functional connectivity (FC). A, Brain regions showing the correlation of MedHrs and FC at baseline for meditators. B, Brain regions that show significant correlation between MedHrs and FC during the meditation state in meditators (EM Med). C, Brain regions that show significant correlation between MedHrs and the change in FC during the transition from state-to-trait meditation in meditators (ΔEM = rsBase - rsPost). Dark green clusters at the Default Mode Network (DMN ROIs 1 and 2 from Fig. 1Aa) and bright green clusters at the Central Executive Network (CEN ROIs 3 and 4 from Fig. 1Aa) show in each case the seeds used to determine the estimated contrast. **nonparametric (1000 permutations) with height threshold p < 0.05 and cluster-size FDR-corrected p < 0.05.

To better understand this positive correlation between meditation experience and increased connectivity between CEN and MPFC, which also has been reported in other studies comparing novice versus expert meditators (Brewer et al., 2011; Jang et al., 2011; Shaurya Prakash et al., 2013), we divided our EM group into two sub-groups (median = 1130 h): EMs with >1130 h of daily practice (EM > 1130; approximately three years) and intermediate meditators (EM < 1130) and conducted a multilevel modeling one-way repeated measures ANOVA and post hoc Tukey’s test for the connectivity z-scores between CEN and MPFC. The results confirmed a significant difference between HC (mean = 0.13) and EM < 1130 (mean = –0.04, b = –0.16, p < 1e-3) but no difference between HC and EM > 1130 (mean = 0.04, b = –0.09, p = 0.08). Furthermore, there was also a significant difference between EM < 1130 (median = –0.25) and EM > 1130 (median = –0.07, b = 0.17, p = 0.04; Fig. 2).

Correlations between hours of meditation practice and PDA

No significant correlations were found for meditation trait, state, or state-to-trait PDA and meditation hours.

Meditation state effects

fALFF analysis

DMN

No significant differences were found.

CEN

The multilevel modeling one-way repeated measures ANOVA yielded significant variation among rsBase, Med, and rsPost, χ2(2) = 24.57, p < 1e-04. The post hoc analysis showed that CEN fALFF was significantly increased in bilateral DLPFC (BA 9, Fig. 1Ab) during Med (mean = 0.3) compared to rsBase (mean = –0.55, b = 0.84, p < 1e-04). See red box plots (EM rsBase < Med) in Figure 1B.

FC analysis

DMN

No significant differences were found.

CEN

EM showed increased connectivity of posterior cingulate cortex (PCC; BA 31) and IPL; BA 40; Fig. 2Ab and red box plot (EM rsBase < Med) of Fig. 2B.

PDA analysis

The multilevel modeling one-way repeated measures ANOVA yielded that there was significant variation among rsBase, Med, and rsPost, χ2(2) = 12.54, p = 0.005. The post hoc analysis showed that PDA was significantly increased during Med (mean = 0.65) compared to rsBase (mean = 0.23, b = 0.57, p < 0.003) and rsPost (mean = 0.36, b = 0.42, p < 0.02). See green box plots (EM rsBase < Med) in Figure 1B.

Correlations between PDA and FC

DMN

No significant correlations were found.

CEN

There was positive correlation (R = 0.91, parametric stats with height threshold p < 0.05 and cluster-size FDR-corrected p < 0.05) between PDA during Med and FC during Med of bilateral DLPFC (BA 9) and left posterior cingulate cortex (PCC; BA 31), left IFG (BA 45), left STG (BA 22), and left inferior parietal lobe (IPL; BA 40; Fig. 3A). For this and all other significant correlation analysis coordinates, see Table 3.

Figure 3.

Figure 3.

Correlations between Positive Diametric Activity (PDA) and Functional Connectivity (FC). A, Brain regions that show significant correlation between PDA and FC during the meditation state in experienced meditators (EM) (EM Med). B, Brain regions that show significant correlation between the change in PDA and change in FC during the transition from state-to-trait meditation (ΔEM = rsBase - rsPost). Dark green clusters at the Default Mode Network (DMN ROIs 1 and 2 from Fig. 1Aa) and bright green clusters at the Central Executive Network (CEN ROIs 3 and 4 from Fig. 1Aa) show in each case the seeds used to determine the estimated contrast. *nonparametric (1000 permutations) with height threshold p < 0.05 and cluster-size FDR-corrected p < 0.05; **parametric stats with height threshold p < 0.05 and cluster-size FDR-corrected p < 0.05.

Table 3.

Correlation between Positive Diametric Activity (PDA) and Functional Connectivity (FC) in experienced meditators (EM)

Region BA Voxels MNI (x,y,z mm) p value*
Meditation state effects
EM Med PDA and EM FC
Default Mode Network (DMN)seeds 1 and 2
No correlation N/A N/A N/A N/A
Central Executive Networkseeds 3, 4, and 5
Inferior Frontal Gyrus L 45 >200 –56,14,18 0.01
Superior temporal lobe L 22 >200 –46,–16,8 0.03
Posterioir cingulate cortex L 31 >200 –12,43,23 0.01
Inferior Parietal Lobule L40 >200 –48,–64,50 0.02
State-to-trait effects
Δ PDA and Δ FC
Default Mode Network (DMN)seeds 1 and 2
Posterior Cingulate Cortex L 31 >200 0,–36,34 0.02
Precuneus L 19 >200 –35,–74,34 0.02
Limbic lobe, uncus L 20 >200 –28,–22,–34 0.03
Central Executive Networkseeds 3, 4, and 5
Medial frontal lobe L 6 >200 –16,–12,60 0.02
Superior temporal lobe R 22 >200 56,2,6 0.01

ROIs 1,2,3,4 and 5 from Fig. 1Aa.

*nonparametric (1000 permutations) with height threshold p < 0.05 and cluster-size FDR-corrected p < 0.05.

Correlations between hours of meditation practice and FC

DMN

Whole-brain FC analysis showed a significant correlation (R = 0.87) between the hours of meditation and the increase in connectivity between rDLPFC and MPFC during meditation (Fig. 4B).

CEN

No significant correlations were found.

Correlations between hours of meditation practice and PDA

No significant correlations were found for meditation trait, state, or state-to-trait PDA and meditation hours.

Meditation state-to-trait effects

fALFF analysis

DMN

No significant differences were found.

CEN

The multilevel modeling one-way repeated measures ANOVA yielded that there was significant variation among rsBase, Med, and rsPost, χ2(2) = 24.57, p < 1e-04. The post hoc analysis showed that CEN fALFF was significantly increased in bilateral DLPFC (BA 9, Fig. 1Ac) during rsPost (mean = 0.09) compared to rsBase; mean = –0.55, b = 0.63, p = 0.006; red box plots (EM rsBase < EM rsPost) in Fig. 1B.

FC analysis

DMN

No differences were found.

CEN

EM showed increased connectivity (χ2(2) = 14.47, p < 0.001) with the Precuneus (BA 23) and right angular gyrus (BA 39) at rsPost compared to rsBase; Fig. 2Ac and red box plots (EM rsBase < EM rsPost) of Fig. 2B.

PDA analysis

PDA

The multilevel modeling one-way repeated measures ANOVA yielded no significant differences. However a two-sample paired t test showed a significant increase relative to rsBase; t(15) = 1.97, p = 0.03, green box plots (EM rsBase < EM rsPost) of Fig. 1B, uncorrected.

Correlations between PDA and FC

DMN

There was a significant negative correlation (R = –0.70) between the change in PDA from rsBase to rsPost and the change in FC from rsBase to rsPost between MPFC (BA 10) and posterior cingulate cortex (PCC; BA 31), precuneus (BA 19), and limbic lobe (BA 20; Fig. 3B).

Correlations between hours of meditation practice and FC

DMN

No significant correlations were found.

CEN

Whole-brain FC analysis showed a significant correlation (R = 0.63) between the change in CEN and PCC connectivity from rsBase to rsPost and the hours of meditation practice (Fig. 4C).

Correlations between hours of meditation practice and PDA

No significant correlations were found for meditation trait, state, or state-to-trait PDA and meditation hours. For this and all other significant correlation analysis coordinates, see Table 4.

Table 4.

Correlation between meditation hours and Functional Connectivity (FC) in experienced meditators (EM)

Region R BA Voxels MNI (x,y,z mm) p value*
Meditation state effects
Default Mode Network (DMN)seeds 1 and 2
Middle frontal gyrus 0.87 R 10 >200 45,44,12 0.02
Central Executive Network (CEN)seeds 3, 4, and 5
No correlations N/A N/A N/A N/A N/A
State-to-trait effects
Default Mode Network (DMN)seeds 1 and 2
No correlations N/A N/A N/A N/A N/A
Central Executive Network (CEN)seeds 3, 4, and 5
Posterior Cingulate Cortex 0.63 L 29 >200 –06,–44,14 0.03
Trait effects
Default Mode Network (DMN)seeds 1 and 2
No correlations N/A N/A N/A N/A N/A
Central Executive Network (CEN)seeds 3, 4, and 5
Medial Frontal Gyrus 0.87 L 10 >200 –06,44,8 0.01

ROIs 1,2,3,4 and 5 from Fig. 1Aa.

*nonparametric (1000 permutations) with height threshold p < 0.05 and cluster-size FDR-corrected p < 0.05.

Discussion

Meditation trait was characterized by a significant reduction in activity and FC within the DMN and increased anticorrelations between DMN and CEN in EMs. However, the latter anticorrelations were only present in meditators with less than three years of practice. Conversely, the meditation state and the meditation state-to-trait periods showed increased activity and FC within the DMN and between DMN and CEN. While there were no meditation trait PDA metric differences, we found significant increase during meditation state that persisted in meditation state-to-trait. The gradual reconfiguration in DMN and CEN suggest a neural mechanism by which the CEN negatively regulates the DMN and is probably responsible for the long-term trait changes seen in meditators and reported psychological well-being.

To our knowledge, no previous study has directly compared trait, state, and state-to trait conditions in EMs using fALFF and FC. In so doing, our findings demonstrate the following for each stage.

Meditation trait

We found that meditation trait is characterized by (1) a significant reduction in brain activity of specific nodes of the DMN, most prominently the MPFC and MTG as well as CEN nodes in the right and left DLPFC and (2) a reduction in FC, both within DMN and between DMN and CEN. Thus, the former reduction in intrinsic DMN connectivity would speak for trait reduced synchrony of DMN regions for meditators and is in accordance with previous studies (Hasenkamp and Barsalou, 2012; Doll et al., 2015) which found that a region in MPFC showed decreased connectivity with the PCC in mindfulness experts compared to novices. Similarly, the latter finding of reduced FC between DMN and CEN for meditators in the baseline resting state is in accordance with two recent studies (Doll et al., 2015; Kemmer et al., 2015), suggesting a critical interplay between DMN and CEN for repeated engagement of attention on present moment experience. Hence, DMN activity and FC, instead of being engaged in task-unrelated thought or mind-wandering, resulting in activation and synchronization of the DMN (Mason et al., 2007), rather is again and again suppressed to re-engage in present moment awareness and hence reflects the stronger anti-correlated coupling between CEN and DMN (Mooneyham et al., 2017; Marusak et al., 2018). This is idea is in accordance with preliminary analysis using dynamic FC on the meditation state data that suggest that meditation trait is characterized by more transitions between brain states (mindful vs mind-wandering) over time, and thus meditators probably spent overall less time in a mind-wandering or a mindless state (Martínez et al., 2019). However, and most importantly, the additional finding of a positive correlation between meditation experience and increased connectivity between CEN and MPFC, which also has been reported in other studies comparing novice versus expert meditators (Brewer et al., 2011; Jang et al., 2011; Shaurya Prakash et al., 2013) and, which at first seems to contradict the increase in anticorrelations for meditators in general, rather is a subtle difference that points to additional network reconfiguration occurring as practice increases for expert meditators (Brewer et al., 2011; Hasenkamp and Barsalou, 2012). We then disentangled this finding by searching for the differences between intermediate (<1130 h of practice, approximately three years of 1 h daily practice) and more EMs (>1130 h) FC in these nodes. The finding, hence suggests that the trait state of intermediate meditators is characterized by a stronger reduction in DMN connectivity and significant increases in anticorrelations between CEN and MPFC. In more EMs (>1130 h of practice) the reduction in DMN connectivity still remains, however the anticorrelations have returned to a pre-meditation state. This finding, we think, is of major importance and points to the evolution of brain activity and connectivity changes as meditation progresses from an intermediate to more advanced stages, and that this change is rather a slow one, with the final sole reduction of DMN activity and connectivity independent of CEN suppression of at least three years of practice and can even evolve to a sustained increase in connectivity between CEN and DMN in meditators with more than three years of experience (Brewer et al., 2011; Creswell et al., 2016). Additionally, we think that this trait suppression of DMN in more EMs independent of anticorrelations with the CEN, is the ultimate network reconfiguration without any active and repeated suppression of the DMN by the CEN and thus, a more effective, consolidated effortless baseline, reflecting a completely transformed and stable mindful state with suppression of DMN without the more active and repeated transitions between mindful versus mind-wandering brain states in intermediate meditators. However, this hypothesis has to be tested in longitudinal studies.

Meditation state

We found that meditation state is characterized by the following: (1) increase in the activity of specific CEN nodes (bilateral DLPFC), (2) increased FC between CEN nodes and DMN nodes (MPFC and PCC), (3) increased PDA, (4) PDA positively correlates with the strength in FC between the CEN and the DMN, and (5) that meditation experience (meditation hours) positively correlates with connectivity between MPFC and right DLPFC during meditation. These findings are in accordance with previous literature (Brewer et al., 2011; Hasenkamp and Barsalou, 2012) and suggest that the neural mechanisms underlying the active meditation state is associated with differential activation and connectivity of CEN nodes modulate activity and connectivity of the DMN. Importantly, although we cannot rule out that other brain networks mediate the effect of CEN on the DMN, our results indicate that activity in the CEN, specifically in the DLPFC effects on the activity and connectivity of the DMN hub nodes, i.e., MPFC and PCC. This hypothesis is in line with previous meditation studies (Brewer et al., 2011; Hasenkamp and Barsalou, 2012; Hasenkamp et al., 2012) but is of particularly interest given a study by Chen et al. (2013), where they used TMS and fMRI to demonstrate a directional causal relationship by which a DLPFC node situated within the CEN inhibits the MPFC portion of the DMN. This in turn is further supported by evidence of monosynaptic projections between CEN and DMN structures in rhesus monkeys, which indicate that these efferent pathways are part of an elaborate anatomic circuit which could mediate aspects of attention, memory, and external or internal perception (Selemon and Goldman-Rakic, 1988). What is more, recent evidence has shown that there is a direct relation between GABAergic inhibition within the mPFC and the reactivity of amygdala during emotional processing. Delli Pizzi et al. (2017) investigated the mPFC-amygdala circuit both with resting-state fMRI (rs-fMRI) and proton MR spectroscopy (MRS) as well as trait anxiety. Their results showed that the rs-fMRI signals of the amygdala and the mPFC were significantly anti-correlated and that this negative functional coupling between the two regions was inversely correlated with the GABA+/tCr level within the MPFC and the STAI-Y2 scores. This suggests a close relationship between MPFC GABA levels and functional interactions within the MPFC-amygdala circuit. Although this is still an open question and needs additional research with simultaneous MRS acquisition during and after meditation, we propose that a meditation state: (1) increases activity within the CEN, (2) this increase in CEN activity directly downregulates the two major DMN nodes (i.e., MPFC and PCC), (3) this down regulation of MPFC is coupled with a reduction in GABA+/tCr levels and reduced amygdala reactivity which, and (4) have a direct impact on anxiety scores and the physiology of emotion regulation.

Meditation state-to-trait

We found that the meditation state-to-trait is characterized by shoeing remnants of the meditation state effect both in activity and connectivity. Specifically, (1) increased activity of the CEN; (2) increased FC between CEN nodes and DMN, although only with the PCC, which has been suggested to be more related to internal meditation practices (Scheibner et al., 2017); and (3) an increased PDA. Moreover, the change in PDA from rsBase to rsPost was negatively correlated with change in intrinsic connectivity of the DMN (MPFC and PCC), i.e., the greater the increase in PDA the less the intrinsic connectivity of the DMN at rsPost. This is also in line with previous studies that suggest that the post-task resting-state network activity and connectivity reflect an aspect of the immediately preceding brain state (Waites et al., 2005) and that these brain changes support the role of learning from a recently performed task as a concomitant process in expertise development (Muraskin et al., 2016). Hence, it further supports the hypothesis that the activity within CEN nodes (specifically DLPFC) actively suppresses DMN nodes and that this suppression carries over during a restful state after meditation, although it shifts to a more posterior node, namely the PCC, probably because this node is more engaged in this particular type of meditation, i.e., internal focused meditation (Scheibner et al., 2017). Finally, we found that meditation experience positively correlates with change in CEN-DMN connectivity. Hence, the more EMs seem to have a larger homeostatic rebound after meditation in CEN-DMN connectivity (for a discussion of this hypothesis, see below).

Finally, and because of the design of the present study, we propose that both, the increased PDA as well as the increased connectivity between DMN and CEN during meditation, as well as the remnants after meditation, at the beginning of practice, effectively produce a “homeostatic rebound” to what we identify as the meditation trait, namely the reduced activity and connectivity within DMN and between DMN and CEN. We think that brain activity, brain connectivity and long-term ensurance of homeostasis depend on intrinsic properties that determine the functionality of these neuronal networks. Homeostatic factors are inherently important and involve complex self-regulatory mechanisms (Davis, 2013). Consequently, it seems reasonable to view this homeostatic rebound as the plasticity that results from a repeated meditation practice within a context of otherwise stable network configurations. First, without the existence of potent mechanisms that perturb this normal balance between networks, and secondly, similarly potent mechanisms that in turn stabilize this perturbation, our capacity to learn and change brain function would be lost. Hence, we suggest, that there is a two stage reconfiguration or homeostatic plasticity (Davis, 2013; Hellyer et al., 2017), that evolves with meditation practice. First, as meditation practice begins, it produces stronger anti-correlated coupling between CEN and DMN (Mooneyham et al., 2017; Marusak et al., 2018) and thus increases the frequency of repeated periods of DMN suppression, even without being actively engaged in meditation, resulting in increased number of moments during the day of engagement in present moment awareness and less mindlessness. This initial change is in accordance with additional evidence showing that increased anticorrelations between DMN and CEN are associated with a healthy development (Chai et al., 2014) and aging (Keller et al., 2015; Esposito et al., 2018), cognitive reserve (Franzmeier et al., 2017), superior cognitive performance (Chai et al., 2014), reduced risk of psychopathology (Whitfield-Gabrieli et al., 2009; Andrews-Hanna et al., 2014), increased emotional stability (Servaas et al., 2017), and overall physiologic and psychological well-being (Fountain-Zaragoza and Prakash, 2017). As meditation increases, a second reconfiguration occurs where the frequency of repeated periods of DMN suppression start to overlap and rather become prolonged periods of engagement in present moment awareness up until the point where it is just continued present moment awareness. Once this point has been reached, we think that there is no more need of active monitoring if one is in a mindless state or mindful state and thus the anticorrelations start to return to a normal pre-meditation level.

The plasticity resulting from a repeated meditation practice, i.e., the suggested homeostatic rebound, may also be the underlying mechanism of many of the benefits reported with meditation practice (Goldberg et al., 2018) that depend on the top-down regulation of the DMN by CEN (Chen et al., 2013; Garrison et al., 2015). In doing so, it is possible that functional abnormalities in cortical and subcortical regions involved in emotion regulation such as anxiety, depression, or other types of psychopathology that relate to deficiencies in noradrenergic and serotonergic function are also recalibrated and thus enhancing brain noradrenergic or serotonergic transmission (Whitfield-Gabrieli and Ford, 2012; Willner et al., 2013). Meditation training, thus, with time, may lead to observable changes in the brain and in neurotransmitter levels (Guglietti et al., 2013; Jindal et al., 2013) accounting for its antidepressant effects and use in psychotherapy. However, this last hypothesis has to be corroborated with further studies that particularly look at this process.

We also want to note some limitations of this study. First off, when we talk about “activity” in the present study we do this with complete knowledge of the limitations of BOLD imaging as the standard technique used to generate images in fMRI studies, and that relies on regional differences in cerebral blood flow to indirectly delineate regional activity (Arthurs and Boniface, 2002; Huettel, 2004). Thus, activity in the present study refers to BOLD activity. There were a modest number of participants. In turn, this motivated a ROI approach focused on specific nodes from the DMN and CEN, based on the prior literature, so that a conservative level of statistics could be employed. A strength of this study is that it represents a first step in elucidating the potential neurobehavioral mechanisms mediating the practice of meditation on DMN activity and the FC. Specifically, it suggests that homeostatic rebound of anticorrelated DMN and CEN networks after meditation could be the compensatory mechanisms operating in the brain that may account for the trait changes in meditators.

Conclusion

Here, we examined the brain changes underlying the state-to-trait experience and training of meditation. We focused on the activity and connectivity of the default and executive networks given their respective roles in internal cognition, self-regulation, and awareness. The findings presented reveal that active meditation practice deliberately engages networks related to cognitive and attentional control that effectively directs the focus of attention and curbs our usual mode of getting carried away by the endless stream of internal and external distractions. Furthermore, we showed that this active state is carried over to an immediate and passive restful state with similar network relationships than the active state. The trait effects of meditation suggest a recalibration and reconfiguration of network structure, or homeostatic plasticity (Davis, 2013; Hellyer et al., 2017), that produces in particular reductions in DMN activity and connectivity. However, the way this suppression is achieved depends on the stage of meditation. Intermediate meditators show stronger anti-correlated coupling between CEN and DMN (Mooneyham et al., 2017; Marusak et al., 2018), which suggests increased frequency between states of mindlessness and mindfulness and thus moments of DMN suppression, even without being actively engaged in meditation. Finally, with more experience in meditation, the moments of mindfulness become so frequent that no more transitions are needed and thus become the new default mode with sustained reduction in DMN activity and connectivity without effort. These, we think are the trait characteristics that ultimately underlie the beneficial effects of meditation, yet allowing the initial practitioner to increase the number of moments of mindful and clear reality to finally achieve a sustained mindful state, even to see reality as it actually is, without the perturbing vail of past or future illusion.

Acknowledgments

Acknowledgements: We thank MSc Leopoldo González-Santos, MSc Juan J. Ortiz, and Dr. Sarael Alcauter for technical support and Dr. M.C. Jeziorski for his manuscript editing.

Synthesis

Reviewing Editor: Bradley Postle, University of Wisconsin

Decisions are customarily a result of the Reviewing Editor and the peer reviewers coming together and discussing their recommendations until a consensus is reached. When revisions are invited, a fact-based synthesis statement explaining their decision and outlining what is needed to prepare a revision will be listed below. The following reviewer(s) agreed to reveal their identity: Sungshin Kim.

Here are the full narrative reviews:

Reviewer #1:

The manuscript is written well enough for comprehension but there are different misspellings and grammatical errors, so a revision of an expert in English editing is recommended.

The results summarized in the abstract should be described more clearly (were the comparisons performed between groups or across conditions?)

Which was the rationale of using fMRI sequences with different parameters for resting state and meditation acquisition? Although it was stated that the data sets with the shorter TR were finally truncated at 151 volumes to match the resting state acquisition this should be better clarified in both methods and limitations discussion.

At line 199 the numerator of fALFF should be (0.01-0.1 Hz).

The sentence at lines 214-216 : “We first obtained the meditation trait differences of the fALFF maps of HC and EM at baseline (rsBase) using two-sample t-tests. The thresholded images were then converted into binarized masks in order to extract the mean fALFF maps for all subjects and states” appear to be a repetition.

Was the Fisher transformation applied to fALFF values prior to perform the t-tests? Also, the Fisher transformation is usually reported for correlation values, I'm not sure it can be applied to every value ranging from -1 to 1.

The sentence at line 261: “as opposed to global signal regression, a widely used preprocessing method known to artificially introduce negative correlations” is redundant as the concept has been already clarified in the same paragraph. 


Which is the rationale for using different bands for temporal filtering (in some sections it is 0.01-0.1 Hz, in others 0.008-0.09 Hz or 0.009- 0.08 Hz)?

The limitation of the BOLD technique in giving information about “activity level” should be pointed out in the discussion. Indeed, even the ALFF approach is a measure of signal fluctuation amplitude, not of a steady state activity.

Finally, there are recent studies investigating the role of medial prefrontal cortex with GABA/Glutamatergic neurotransmission in situations that meditation is supposed to relieve, such as anxiety, that can be taken into account when discussing the results.

Reviewer #2:

This manuscript presented a fMRI study showing a gradual configuration of central executive and default mode networks from pre-meditation, during meditation, and post-meditation. For this, the authors designed back-to-back fMRI experiments with experienced meditators, and a control experiment of pre-meditation resting state scanning with novice healthy participants. The authors characterized network configuration using two measurements, fALFF which is power of low-frequency components of BOLD and functional connectivity (FC), then these were related with meditation experiences. The manuscript contributes to the literature as it provides novel data sets regarding how networks are reconfigured by meditation.

Using a novel back-to-back fMRI experiment design, the authors elucidated neural mechanism of meditation, showing how the default mode network interacts with the central executive network. The correlation results with high goodness of fit are very impressive. Also, their new suggestion regarding homeostatic rebound of anticorrelated DMN-CEN networks after meditation is an interesting hypothesis worthy of further investigation. However, I have several concerns in the experiment design and analysis that should be addressed for publication.

Major points:

1. For experienced meditators, the authors compared three data sets, pre-meditation, during-meditation, and post-meditation. My major concern is discrepancy of scanning protocols between pre-/post-meditation and during-meditation in terms of TR, scanning length, and so on. The authors briefly mentioned they used the initial portion of data for during-meditation to match the length of pre-/post-meditation data, 151 volumes (in line 281). Because they used different TR, 2 sec vs. 1.5 sec for pre-/post-meditation and during-meditation, the actual duration in time was different given the same number of volumes. Especially, due to the different TRs, the frequency ranges were also different as they described in line 205 and 206. This affects the calculation of fALFF and may induce bias of lowering fALFF of during-meditation, acquired by higher TR (larger range of frequency), than that of pre-/post-meditation.

However, there was no explanation why they used different protocols. If this was done by mistakes, they should provide quantitative evidences that difference in protocols had no significant effects on the results. They also need to explain why only the initial portion of the data was used. What if using the entire data of duration-meditation? Will it still support their conclusions, at least regarding the results for during-meditation?

2. Another major concern is the definition of ROIs, which I think unclear (lines 209-216). As I understand, the ROIs were marked as green solid circles in Figure 2A and these were obtained from the 2-sample whole-brain t-test between fALFF maps of HC rsBase and EM rsBase. The whole-brain t-test results were shown in Figure 1A and the significant clusters were intersected with CEN and DMN masks adapted from a previous study.

However, this seems not the case in Table 1. I guess clusters marked with superscripts (1-6) are the ROIs which were used as seeds in subsequent FC analysis. However, ROIs in CEN were listed under Meditation State-To-Trait Effects and two ROIs (bilateral IPL) marked as 5 and 6 were not shown in the table, which I did not expect. The authors explicitly described the target ROI's were obtained from the two sample t-test between HC rsBase vs. EM rsBase. Additionally, it is unclear whether they used peak coordinates (maximum t-value) and generated spherical ROIs from the coordinate or used the entire clusters. All the results were dependent of the ROIs in DMN and CEN because the authors described their results based on these.

The author summarized the results separately for DMN and CEN. Were all the results of fALFF analysis and FC analysis calculated by taking the mean of two ROIs for each of DMN (figure 1.A.1 and 1.A.2) and CEN (figure 1.A.3 and 1.A.4)? Then, why did the author not use ROIs in CEN marked as 5 and 6, which correspond to bilateral IPL.

3. It is unclear how the authors calculated correlation between PDA and FC for different regions in the same network. For example, I do not understand why R^2 (maybe R is correct) is exactly the same for all the region in the same network in Table 3.

4. The authors report correlations between FC and hours of meditation as well as PDA. I wonder if there is any significant correlation between hours of meditation and PDA.

5. There is no explanation for the whole-brain analysis shown in figure 4.A. The authors described a correlation result only.

6. I suggest to reorganize the result section incorporating the correlation results as separate analysis under three effects. Specifically, it would be better to move sections 3.4 and 3.5 under 3.1-3.3 so that results of trait (3.4.1. and 3.5.1), state(3.4.2. and 3.5.2), and state-to-trait effects (3.4.3. and 3.5.3) can move to 3.1.4, 3.1.5, 3.2.4, 3.2.5, 3.3.4, 3.3.5.

By doing this, the result section will be more organized in the order of trait, state, and state-trait effects.

7. In discussion, interpretation of their results is rather difficult to understand. Specifically, I do not understand how more transition between mindful vs. mind-wandering is related with stronger anti-correlation. I wonder if the authors can show this more quantitatively using dynamic connectivity analysis with their data sets. I think 20-minute fMRI is long enough to perform this analysis although the authors used only initial portion of data, which are only 151 volumes.

8. Other studies comparing novice vs. expert meditators showed increased connectivity between DMN-CEN. However, the current result found no significant difference after rebounding from the anticorrelation. The authors did not explain this discrepancy. Did the expert meditators in other studies have more experiences than meditators in the current study? Then, can we say connectivity between DMN-CEN continue increasing beyond the level of novice after rebounding from anti-correlation?

Minor points:

1. Please give a reference for Free Cluster Enhancement (TFCE) method in line 213.

2. The authors introduced a new measurement, PDA based on the hypothesis of CEN negatively regulating the DMN (lines 221-226). However, it would be helpful to conceptualize PDA in a way of giving better insight into understanding their results.

3. Were the signals from CSF and the white matter regressed out when calculating FC as described in lines 266-268?

4. typo in line 289, “differents” -> different

5. typo in line 303, left Superior Frontal Gyrus (MFG) -> left Superior Frontal Gyrus (SFG)

6. In the description of ANOVA results (e.g., line 313), what do you mean by “Meditators”? I think this should be three experimental conditions, pre-, during, post-meditation. Meditator seems to mean subjects, which I think not.

7. I am not sure why there is no ANOVA result in FC analysis of Meditation

8. In 3.2.2. FC analysis, MFG and ACC were not shown in figure 2.A. Only PCC and IPL were shown.

9. In post-hoc Tukey test, please explain the meanings of “M” and “b” (e.g., line 316).

10. In line 345 of 3.3.3. PDA, was the t-test result (p = .03) corrected for multiple comparison?

11. Which of R^2 and R is correct? The main text used R^2 but the figure used R. I guessed the latter is correct.

12. In line 369, the authors need to briefly explain why they divided EM group into two groups. They used an expression such as “to further disentangle this finding”, but the authors need to briefly remind that connectivity between CEN-MPFC at rsBase decreased from novice to experienced meditators, which seem contradictory to the finding. In discussion, the author did provide this explanation, but here it is little bit awkward to use the expression, “further disentangle” without any explanation.

13. What is the criterion of selecting 1130 hours differentiating experienced and intermediate meditators?

14. In discussion, line 390 describing the anticorrelations should be right after line 388 for better logical flow.

15. In line 486, Fist -> First

16. In line 515, there is a typo “ts”

17. In figure 2.A, the index of a color bar (red to yellow) may be wrong. -0.5 should be 0.5

18. What do black dots indicate in figure 1.B and 2.B?

References

  1. Andrews-Hanna JR, Smallwood J, Spreng RN (2014) The default network and self-generated thought: component processes, dynamic control, and clinical relevance. Ann NY Acad Sci 1316:29–52. 10.1111/nyas.12360 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Anticevic A, Cole MW, Murray JD, Corlett PR, Wang X-J, Krystal JH (2012) The role of default network deactivation in cognition and disease. Trends Cogn Sci 16:584–592. 10.1016/j.tics.2012.10.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Arens J, Moar KM, Eiden S, Weide K, Schmidt I, Mercer JG, Simon E, Korf H-W (2003) Age-dependent hypothalamic expression of neuropeptides in wild-type and melanocortin-4 receptor-deficient mice. Physiol Genomics 16:38–46. 10.1152/physiolgenomics.00123.2003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Arthurs OJ, Boniface S (2002) How well do we understand the neural origins of the fMRI BOLD signal? Trends Neurosci 25:27–31. 10.1016/s0166-2236(00)01995-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Assaf M, Jagannathan K, Calhoun VD, Miller L, Stevens MC, Sahl R, O’Boyle JG, Schultz RT, Pearlson GD (2010) Abnormal functional connectivity of default mode sub-networks in autism spectrum disorder patients. Neuroimage 53:247–256. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.05.067 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Austin JH (1999) Zen and the brain: toward an understanding of meditation and consciousness. Cambridge: The MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
  7. Baer RA (2003) Mindfulness training as a clinical intervention: a conceptual and empirical review. Clin Psychol Sci Pract 10:125–143. 10.1093/clipsy/bpg015 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  8. Banks JB, Welhaf MS, Srour A (2015) The protective effects of brief mindfulness meditation training. Conscious Cogn 33:277–285. 10.1016/j.concog.2015.01.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bastos-Leite AJ, Ridgway GR, Silveira C, Norton A, Reis S, Friston KJ (2015) Dysconnectivity within the default mode in first-episode schizophrenia: a stochastic dynamic causal modeling study with functional magnetic resonance imaging. Schizophr Bull 41:144–153. 10.1093/schbul/sbu080 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Behzadi Y, Restom K, Liau J, Liu TT (2007) A component based noise correction method (CompCor) for BOLD and perfusion based fMRI. Neuroimage 37:90–101. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.04.042 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Berger H, Hans B (1933) Über das Elektrenkephalogramm des Menschen. Arch Psychiatr Nervenkr 100:301–320. 10.1007/BF01814740 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  12. Biswal B, Yetkin FZ, Haughton VM, Hyde JS (1995) Functional connectivity in the motor cortex of resting human brain using echo-planar MRI. Magn Reson Med 34:537–541. 10.1002/mrm.1910340409 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Bowen S, Witkiewitz K, Clifasefi SL, Grow J, Chawla N, Hsu SH, Carroll HA, Harrop E, Collins SE, Lustyk MK, Larimer ME (2014) Relative efficacy of mindfulness-based relapse prevention, standard relapse prevention, and treatment as usual for substance use disorders: a randomized clinical trial. JAMA Psychiatry 71:547–556. 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.4546 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Brewer JA, Worhunsky PD, Gray JR, Tang YY, Weber J, Kober H (2011) Meditation experience is associated with differences in default mode network activity and connectivity. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 108:20254–20259. 10.1073/pnas.1112029108 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Broyd SJ, Demanuele C, Debener S, Helps SK, James CJ, Sonuga-Barke EJS (2009) Default-mode brain dysfunction in mental disorders: a systematic review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 33:279–296. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.09.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Buchholz L (2015) Exploring the promise of mindfulness as medicine. JAMA 314:1327–1329. 10.1001/jama.2015.7023 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Buckner RL, Andrews-Hanna JR, Schacter DL (2008) The brain’s default network. Ann NY Acad Sci 1124:1–38. 10.1196/annals.1440.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Bueno VF, Kozasa EH, da Silva MA, Alves TM, Louzã MR, Pompéia S (2015) Mindfulness meditation improves mood, quality of life, and attention in adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Biomed Res Int 2015:962857. 10.1155/2015/962857 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Camchong J, MacDonald AW 3rd, Bell C, Mueller BA, Lim KO (2011) Altered functional and anatomical connectivity in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 37:640–650. 10.1093/schbul/sbp131 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Chai XJ, Castañón AN, Ongür D, Whitfield-Gabrieli S (2012) Anticorrelations in resting state networks without global signal regression. Neuroimage 59:1420–1428. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.08.048 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Chai XJ, Ofen N, Gabrieli JDE, Whitfield-Gabrieli S (2014) Selective development of anticorrelated networks in the intrinsic functional organization of the human brain. J Cogn Neurosci 26:501–513. 10.1162/jocn_a_00517 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Chen AC, Oathes DJ, Chang C, Bradley T, Zhou Z-W, Williams LM, Glover GH, Deisseroth K, Etkin A (2013) Causal interactions between fronto-parietal central executive and default-mode networks in humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 110:19944–19949. 10.1073/pnas.1311772110 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Chien WT, Thompson DR (2014) Effects of a mindfulness-based psychoeducation programme for Chinese patients with schizophrenia: 2-year follow-up. Br J Psychiatry 205:52–59. 10.1192/bjp.bp.113.134635 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Christoff K, Irving ZC, Fox KCR, Spreng RN, Andrews-Hanna JR (2016) Mind-wandering as spontaneous thought: a dynamic framework. Nat Rev Neurosci 17:718–731. 10.1038/nrn.2016.113 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Creswell JD (2015) Biological pathways linking mindfulness with health In: Handbook of mindfulness: science and practice. New York: Guilford Publications. [Google Scholar]
  26. Creswell JD, Taren AA, Lindsay EK, Greco CM, Gianaros PJ, Fairgrieve A, Marsland AL, Brown KW, Way BM, Rosen RK, Ferris JL (2016) Alterations in resting-state functional connectivity link mindfulness meditation with reduced interleukin-6: a randomized controlled trial. Biol Psychiatry 80:53–61. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2016.01.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Davis GW (2013) Homeostatic signaling and the stabilization of neural function. Neuron 80:718–728. 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.09.044 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Delli Pizzi S, Chiacchiaretta P, Mantini D, Bubbico G, Ferretti A, Edden RA, Di Giulio C, Onofrj M, Bonanni L (2017) Functional and neurochemical interactions within the amygdala-medial prefrontal cortex circuit and their relevance to emotional processing. Brain Struct Funct 222:1267–1279. 10.1007/s00429-016-1276-z [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Doll A, Hölzel BK, Boucard CC, Wohlschläger AM, Sorg C (2015) Mindfulness is associated with intrinsic functional connectivity between default mode and salience networks. Front Hum Neurosci 9:461. 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00461 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Dosenbach NUF, Fair DA, Miezin FM, Cohen AL, Wenger KK, Dosenbach RAT, Fox MD, Snyder AZ, Vincent JL, Raichle ME, Schlaggar BL, Petersen SE (2007) Distinct brain networks for adaptive and stable task control in humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 104:11073–11078. 10.1073/pnas.0704320104 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Eisendrath SJ, Delucchi K, Bitner R, Fenimore P, Smit M, McLane M (2008) Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for treatment-resistant depression: a pilot study. Psychother Psychosom 77:319–320. 10.1159/000142525 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Esposito R, Cieri F, Chiacchiaretta P, Cera N, Lauriola M, Di Giannantonio M, Tartaro A, Ferretti A (2018) Modifications in resting state functional anticorrelation between default mode network and dorsal attention network: comparison among young adults, healthy elders and mild cognitive impairment patients. Brain Imaging Behav 12:127–141. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Fountain-Zaragoza S, Prakash RS (2017) Mindfulness training for healthy aging: impact on attention, well-being, and inflammation. Front Aging Neurosci 9:11. 10.3389/fnagi.2017.00011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Fox KCR, Nijeboer S, Dixon ML, Floman JL, Ellamil M, Rumak SP, Sedlmeier P, Christoff K (2014) Is meditation associated with altered brain structure? A systematic review and meta-analysis of morphometric neuroimaging in meditation practitioners. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 43:48–73. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.03.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Fox MD, Greicius M (2010) Clinical applications of resting state functional connectivity. Front Syst Neurosci 4:19. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Fox MD, Snyder AZ, Vincent JL, Corbetta M, Van Essen DC, Raichle ME (2005) The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 102:9673–9678. 10.1073/pnas.0504136102 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Fransson P (2005) Spontaneous low-frequency BOLD signal fluctuations: an fMRI investigation of the resting-state default mode of brain function hypothesis. Hum Brain Mapp 26:15–29. 10.1002/hbm.20113 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Franzmeier N, Caballero MÁA, Taylor ANW, Simon-Vermot L, Buerger K, Ertl-Wagner B, Mueller C, Catak C, Janowitz D, Baykara E, Gesierich B, Duering M, Ewers M; Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (2017) Resting-state global functional connectivity as a biomarker of cognitive reserve in mild cognitive impairment. Brain Imaging Behav 11:368–382. 10.1007/s11682-016-9599-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Garland EL, Gaylord SA, Boettiger CA, Howard MO (2010) Mindfulness training modifies cognitive, affective, and physiological mechanisms implicated in alcohol dependence: results of a randomized controlled pilot trial. J Psychoactive Drugs 42:177–192. 10.1080/02791072.2010.10400690 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Garrison KA, Scheinost D, Constable RT, Brewer JA (2014) BOLD signal and functional connectivity associated with loving kindness meditation. Brain Behav 4:337–347. 10.1002/brb3.219 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Garrison KA, Zeffiro TA, Scheinost D, Constable RT, Brewer JA (2015) Meditation leads to reduced default mode network activity beyond an active task. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 15:712–720. 10.3758/s13415-015-0358-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Garrity A (2007) Aberrant “default mode” functional connectivity in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 164:450. 10.1176/ajp.2007.164.3.450 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Goldberg SB, Tucker RP, Greene PA, Davidson RJ, Wampold BE, Kearney DJ, Simpson TL (2018) Mindfulness-based interventions for psychiatric disorders: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Clin Psychol Rev 59:52–60. 10.1016/j.cpr.2017.10.011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Greicius M (2008) Resting-state functional connectivity in neuropsychiatric disorders. Curr Opin Neurol 21:424–430. 10.1097/WCO.0b013e328306f2c5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Greicius MD, Krasnow B, Reiss AL, Menon V (2003) Functional connectivity in the resting brain: a network analysis of the default mode hypothesis. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 100:253–258. 10.1073/pnas.0135058100 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Greicius MD, Srivastava G, Reiss AL, Menon V (2004) Default-mode network activity distinguishes Alzheimer’s disease from healthy aging: evidence from functional MRI. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 101:4637–4642. 10.1073/pnas.0308627101 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Grossman P, Niemann L, Schmidt S, Walach H (2004) Mindfulness-based stress reduction and health benefits. A meta-analysis. J Psychosom Res 57:35–43. 10.1016/S0022-3999(03)00573-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Guglietti CL, Daskalakis ZJ, Radhu N, Fitzgerald PB, Ritvo P (2013) Meditation-related increases in GABAB modulated cortical inhibition. Brain Stimul 6:397–402. 10.1016/j.brs.2012.08.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Gu J, Strauss C, Bond R, Cavanagh K (2015) How do mindfulness-based cognitive therapy and mindfulness-based stress reduction improve mental health and wellbeing? A systematic review and meta-analysis of mediation studies. Clin Psychol Rev 37:1–12. 10.1016/j.cpr.2015.01.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Gusnard DA, Raichle ME, Raichle ME (2001) Searching for a baseline: functional imaging and the resting human brain. Nat Rev Neurosci 2:685–694. 10.1038/35094500 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Hampson M, Driesen N, Roth JK, Gore JC, Constable RT (2010) Functional connectivity between task-positive and task-negative brain areas and its relation to working memory performance. Magn Reson Imaging 28:1051–1057. 10.1016/j.mri.2010.03.021 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Hart W (2011) The art of living: Vipassana meditation as taught by S.N. Goenka. Onalaska: Pariyatti. [Google Scholar]
  53. Hasenkamp W, Barsalou LW (2012) Effects of meditation experience on functional connectivity of distributed brain networks. Front Hum Neurosci 6:38. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Hasenkamp W, Wilson-Mendenhall CD, Duncan E, Barsalou LW (2012) Mind wandering and attention during focused meditation: a fine-grained temporal analysis of fluctuating cognitive states. Neuroimage 59:750–760. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.07.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Hellyer PJ, Clopath C, Kehagia AA, Turkheimer FE, Leech R (2017) From homeostasis to behavior: Balanced activity in an exploration of embodied dynamic environmental-neural interaction. PLoS Comput Biol 13:e1005721. 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1005721 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  56. Hölzel BK, Carmody J, Vangel M, Congleton C, Yerramsetti SM, Gard T, Lazar SW (2011a) Mindfulness practice leads to increases in regional brain gray matter density. Psychiatry Res 191:36–43. 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2010.08.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Hölzel BK, Lazar SW, Gard T, Schuman-Olivier Z, Vago DR, Ott U (2011b) How does mindfulness meditation Wwork? Proposing mechanisms of action from a conceptual and neural perspective. Perspect Psychol Sci 6:537–559. 10.1177/1745691611419671 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Huettel SA (2004) Linking hemodynamic and electrophysiological measures of brain activity: evidence from functional MRI and intracranial field potentials. Cereb Cortex 14:165–173. 10.1093/cercor/bhg115 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Jang JH, Jung WH, Kang D-H, Byun MS, Kwon SJ, Choi C-H, Kwon JS (2011) Increased default mode network connectivity associated with meditation. Neurosci Lett 487:358–362. 10.1016/j.neulet.2010.10.056 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Janssen L, Kan CC, Carpentier PJ, Sizoo B, Hepark S, Grutters J, Donders R, Buitelaar JK, Speckens AEM (2015) Mindfulness based cognitive therapy versus treatment as usual in adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). BMC Psychiatry 15:216. 10.1186/s12888-015-0591-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Jao T, Li CW, Vértes PE, Wu CW, Achard S, Hsieh CH, Liou CH, Chen JH, Bullmore ET (2016) Large-scale functional brain network reorganization during Taoist meditation. Brain Connect 6:9–24. 10.1089/brain.2014.0318 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Jazaieri H, Hooria J, Lee IA, Kelly M, Thupten J, Doty JR, Gross JJ, Goldin PR (2016) A wandering mind is a less caring mind: daily experience sampling during compassion meditation training. J Posit Psychol 11:37–50. 10.1080/17439760.2015.1025418 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  63. Jindal V, Gupta S, Das R (2013) Molecular mechanisms of meditation. Mol Neurobiol 48:808–811. 10.1007/s12035-013-8468-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Kane MJ, Gross GM, Chun CA, Smeekens BA, Meier ME, Silvia PJ, Kwapil TR (2017) For whom the mind wanders, and when, varies across laboratory and daily-life settings. Psychol Sci 28:1271–1289. 10.1177/0956797617706086 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Keller JB, Hedden T, Thompson TW, Anteraper SA, Gabrieli JDE, Whitfield-Gabrieli S (2015) Resting-state anticorrelations between medial and lateral prefrontal cortex: association with working memory, aging, and individual differences. Cortex 64:271–280. 10.1016/j.cortex.2014.12.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Kelly AMC, Uddin LQ, Biswal BB, Castellanos FX, Milham MP (2008) Competition between functional brain networks mediates behavioral variability. Neuroimage 39:527–537. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.08.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Kemmer PB, Guo Y, Wang Y, Pagnoni G (2015) Network-based characterization of brain functional connectivity in Zen practitioners. Front Psychol 6:603. 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00603 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Killingsworth MA, Gilbert DT (2010) A wandering mind is an unhappy mind. Science 330:932. 10.1126/science.1192439 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Kong F, Hu S, Wang X, Song Y, Liu J (2015) Neural correlates of the happy life: the amplitude of spontaneous low frequency fluctuations predicts subjective well-being. Neuroimage 107:136–145. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.11.033 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Kuyken W, Byford S, Taylor RS, Watkins E, Holden E, White K, Barrett B, Byng R, Evans A, Mullan E, Teasdale JD (2008) Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy to prevent relapse in recurrent depression. J Consult Clin Psychol 76:966–978. 10.1037/a0013786 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Liao W, Zhang Z, Pan Z, Mantini D, Ding J, Duan X, Luo C, Wang Z, Tan Q, Lu G, Chen H (2011) Default mode network abnormalities in mesial temporal lobe epilepsy: a study combining fMRI and DTI. Hum Brain Mapp 32:883–895. 10.1002/hbm.21076 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Luders E, Toga AW, Lepore N, Gaser C (2009) The underlying anatomical correlates of long-term meditation: larger hippocampal and frontal volumes of gray matter. Neuroimage 45:672–678. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Ludwig DS, Kabat-Zinn J (2008) Mindfulness in medicine. JAMA 300:1350–1352. 10.1001/jama.300.11.1350 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Lutz A, Antoine L, Dunne JD, Davidson RJ (2007) Meditation and the neuroscience of consciousness: an introduction In: The Cambridge handbook of consciousness, pp 499–552. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  75. Lutz J, Herwig U, Opialla S, Hittmeyer A, Jäncke L, Rufer M, Grosse Holtforth M, Brühl AB (2014) Mindfulness and emotion regulation–an fMRI study. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 9:776–785. 10.1093/scan/nst043 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Mantini D, Corbetta M, Romani GL, Orban GA, Vanduffel W (2013) Evolutionarily novel functional networks in the human brain? J Neurosci 33:3259–3275. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4392-12.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Martínez AY, Demertzi A, Bauer CCC, Gracia-Tabuenca Z, Alcauter S, Barrios FA (2019) The Time Varying Networks of the Interoceptive Attention and Rest. BioRxiv. Advance online publication Team. Retrieved November 13, 2019. doi:10.1101/840645.
  78. Marusak HA, Thomason ME, Elrahal F, Peters CA, Kundu P, Lombardo MV, Calhoun VD, Goldberg EK, Cohen C, Taub JW, Rabinak CA (2018) Mindfulness and dynamic functional neural connectivity in children and adolescents. 336:211–218. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Ma SH, Teasdale JD (2004) Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for depression: replication and exploration of differential relapse prevention effects. J Consult Clin Psychol 72:31–40. 10.1037/0022-006X.72.1.31 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Mason MF, Norton MI, Van Horn JD, Wegner DM, Grafton ST, Macrae CN (2007) Wandering minds: the default network and stimulus-independent thought. Science 315:393–395. 10.1126/science.1131295 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. McKiernan KA, Kaufman JN, Kucera-Thompson J, Binder JR (2003) A parametric manipulation of factors affecting task-induced deactivation in functional neuroimaging. J Cogn Neurosci 15:394–408. 10.1162/089892903321593117 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Mooneyham BW, Mrazek MD, Mrazek AJ, Schooler JW (2016) Signal or noise: brain network interactions underlying the experience and training of mindfulness. Ann NY Acad Sci 1369:240–256. 10.1111/nyas.13044 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Mooneyham BW, Mrazek MD, Mrazek AJ, Mrazek KL, Phillips DT, Schooler JW (2017) States of mind: characterizing the neural bases of focus and mind-wandering through dynamic functional connectivity. J Cogn Neurosci 29:495–506. 10.1162/jocn_a_01066 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Moore A, Gruber T, Derose J, Malinowski P (2012) Regular, brief mindfulness meditation practice improves electrophysiological markers of attentional control. Front Hum Neurosci 6:18. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Mrazek MD, Franklin MS, Phillips DT, Baird B, Schooler JW (2013) Mindfulness training improves working memory capacity and GRE performance while reducing mind wandering. Psychol Sci 24:776–781. 10.1177/0956797612459659 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Muraskin J, Dodhia S, Lieberman G, Garcia JO, Verstynen T, Vettel JM, Sherwin J, Sajda P (2016) Brain dynamics of post-task resting state are influenced by expertise: insights from baseball players. Hum Brain Mapp 37:4454–4471. 10.1002/hbm.23321 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Northoff G, Bermpohl F (2004) Cortical midline structures and the self. Trends Cogn Sci 8:102–107. 10.1016/j.tics.2004.01.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Northoff G, Walter M, Schulte RF, Beck J, Dydak U, Henning A, Boeker H, Grimm S, Boesiger P (2007) GABA concentrations in the human anterior cingulate cortex predict negative BOLD responses in fMRI. Nat Neurosci 10:1515–1517. 10.1038/nn2001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Ottaviani C, Shapiro D, Couyoumdjian A (2013) Flexibility as the key for somatic health: from mind wandering to perseverative cognition. Biol Psychol 94:38–43. 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2013.05.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Pomarol-Clotet E, Salvador R, Sarró S, Gomar J, Vila F, Martínez A, Guerrero A, Ortiz-Gil J, Sans-Sansa B, Capdevila A, Cebamanos JM, McKenna PJ (2008) Failure to deactivate in the prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia: dysfunction of the default mode network? Psychol Med 38:1185–1193. 10.1017/S0033291708003565 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Prakash RS, Whitmoyer P, Aldao A, Schirda B (2015) Mindfulness and emotion regulation in older and young adults. Aging Ment Health 21:77–87. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Raichle ME (2015) The brain’s default mode network. Annu Rev Neurosci 38:433–447. 10.1146/annurev-neuro-071013-014030 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Rilling JK (2014) Comparative primate neuroimaging: insights into human brain evolution. Trends Cogn Sci 18:46–55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Saad ZS, Gotts SJ, Kevin M, Gang C, Jo HJ, Alex M, Cox RW (2012) Trouble at rest: how correlation patterns and group differences become distorted after global signal regression. Brain Connect 2:25–32. 10.1089/brain.2012.0080 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Scheibner HJ, Bogler C, Gleich T, Haynes J-D, Bermpohl F (2017) Internal and external attention and the default mode network. Neuroimage 148:381–389. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.01.044 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Selemon LD, Goldman-Rakic PS (1988) Common cortical and subcortical targets of the dorsolateral prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices in the rhesus monkey: evidence for a distributed neural network subserving spatially guided behavior. J Neurosci 8:4049–4068. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Servaas MN, Riese H, Renken RJ, Wichers M, Bastiaansen JA, Figueroa CA, Geugies H, Mocking RJ, Geerligs L, Marsman J-B, Aleman A, Schene AH, Schoevers RA, Ruhé HG (2017) Associations between daily affective instability and connectomics in functional subnetworks in remitted patients with recurrent major depressive disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology 42:2583–2592. 10.1038/npp.2017.65 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Shapiro DH Jr, Walsh RN (1984) Meditation: classic and contemporary perspectives. Piscataway: Aldine Transactions Publishers. [Google Scholar]
  99. Shaurya Prakash R, De Leon AA, Klatt M, Malarkey W, Patterson B (2013) Mindfulness disposition and default-mode network connectivity in older adults. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 8:112–117. 10.1093/scan/nss115 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Sheline YI, Raichle ME (2013) Resting state functional connectivity in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. Biol Psychiatry 74:340–347. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.11.028 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Sheline YI, Barch DM, Price JL, Rundle MM, Vaishnavi SN, Snyder AZ, Mintun MA, Wang S, Coalson RS, Raichle ME (2009) The default mode network and self-referential processes in depression. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 106:1942–1947. 10.1073/pnas.0812686106 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Shen H, Li Z, Zeng LL, Yuan L, Chen F, Liu Z, Hu D (2014) Internetwork dynamic connectivity effectively differentiates schizophrenic patients from healthy controls. Neuroreport 25:1344–1349. 10.1097/WNR.0000000000000267 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Shpaner M, Kelly C, Lieberman G, Perelman H, Davis M, Keefe FJ, Naylor MR (2014) Unlearning chronic pain: a randomized controlled trial to investigate changes in intrinsic brain connectivity following cognitive behavioral therapy. Neuroimage Clin 5:365–376. 10.1016/j.nicl.2014.07.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Simon R, Engström M (2015) The default mode network as a biomarker for monitoring the therapeutic effects of meditation. Front Psychol 6:776. 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00776 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Smallwood J, Fitzgerald A, Miles LK, Phillips LH (2009) Shifting moods, wandering minds: negative moods lead the mind to wander. Emotion 9:271–276. 10.1037/a0014855 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Smallwood J, O’Connor RC (2011) Imprisoned by the past: unhappy moods lead to a retrospective bias to mind wandering. Cogn Emot 25:1481–1490. 10.1080/02699931.2010.545263 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Smith SM, Nichols TE (2009) Threshold-free cluster enhancement: addressing problems of smoothing, threshold dependence and localisation in cluster inference. Neuroimage 44:83–98. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.03.061 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Sonuga-Barke EJS, Castellanos FX (2007) Spontaneous attentional fluctuations in impaired states and pathological conditions: a neurobiological hypothesis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 31:977–986. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.02.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. Sridharan D, Levitin DJ, Menon V (2008) A critical role for the right fronto-insular cortex in switching between central-executive and default-mode networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 105:12569–12574. 10.1073/pnas.0800005105 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. Tang YY, Tang R, Posner MI (2013) Brief meditation training induces smoking reduction. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 110:13971–13975. 10.1073/pnas.1311887110 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. Tang YY, Posner MI, Rothbart MK (2014) Meditation improves self-regulation over the life span. Ann NY Acad Sci 1307:104–111. 10.1111/nyas.12227 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. Taylor PA, Saad ZS (2013) FATCAT: (an efficient) functional and tractographic connectivity analysis toolbox. Brain Connect 3:523–535. 10.1089/brain.2013.0154 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  113. Teasdale JD, Segal ZV, Williams JMG, Ridgeway VA, Soulsby JM, Lau MA (2000) Prevention of relapse/recurrence in major depression by mindfulness-based cognitive therapy. J Consult Clin Psychol 68:615–623. 10.1037//0022-006X.68.4.615 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. Turner K (2014) Mindfulness skills training: a pilot study of changes in mindfulness, emotion regulation, and self-perception of aging in older participants. Act Adapt Aging 38:156–167. 10.1080/01924788.2014.901074 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  115. Uddin LQ, Kelly AMC, Biswal BB, Margulies DS, Shehzad Z, Shaw D, Ghaffari M, Rotrosen J, Adler LA, Castellanos FX, Milham MP (2008) Network homogeneity reveals decreased integrity of default-mode network in ADHD. J Neurosci Methods 169:249–254. 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2007.11.031 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Uddin LQ, Kelly AM, Biswal BB, Castellanos FX, Milham MP (2009) Functional connectivity of default mode network components: correlation, anticorrelation, and causality. Hum Brain Mapp 30:625–637. 10.1002/hbm.20531 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  117. Van Dam NT, van Vugt MK, Vago DR, Schmalzl L, Saron CD, Olendzki A, Meissner T, Lazar SW, Kerr CE, Gorchov J, Fox KCR, Field BA, Britton WB, Brefczynski-Lewis JA, Meyer DE (2018) Mind the hype: a critical evaluation and prescriptive agenda for research on mindfulness and meditation. Perspect Psychol Sci 13:36–61. 10.1177/1745691617709589 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  118. Waites AB, Stanislavsky A, Abbott DF, Jackson GD (2005) Effect of prior cognitive state on resting state networks measured with functional connectivity. Hum Brain Mapp 24:59–68. 10.1002/hbm.20069 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  119. Wells RE, Kerr CE, Wolkin J, Dossett M, Davis RB, Walsh J, Wall RB, Kong J, Kaptchuk T, Press D, Phillips RS, Yeh G (2013) Meditation for adults with mild cognitive impairment: a pilot randomized trial. J Am Geriatr Soc 61:642–645. 10.1111/jgs.12179 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  120. West MA (2016) The psychology of meditation: research and practice. New York: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  121. Whitfield-Gabrieli S, Ford JM (2012) Default mode network activity and connectivity in psychopathology. Annu Rev Clin Psychol 8:49–76. 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032511-143049 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  122. Whitfield-Gabrieli S, Thermenos HW, Milanovic S, Tsuang MT, Faraone SV, McCarley RW, Shenton ME, Green AI, Nieto-Castanon A, LaViolette P, Wojcik J, Gabrieli JDE, Seidman LJ (2009) Hyperactivity and hyperconnectivity of the default network in schizophrenia and in first-degree relatives of persons with schizophrenia. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 106:1279–1284. 10.1073/pnas.0809141106 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  123. Whitfield-Gabrieli S, Susan WG, Alfonso NC (2012) Conn: a functional connectivity toolbox for correlated and anticorrelated brain networks. Brain Connect 2:125–141. 10.1089/brain.2012.0073 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  124. Willner P, Scheel-Krüger J, Belzung C (2013) The neurobiology of depression and antidepressant action. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 37:2331–2371. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.12.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  125. Winkler AM, Ridgway GR, Webster MA, Smith SM, Nichols TE (2014) Permutation inference for the general linear model. Neuroimage 92:381–397. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.01.060 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  126. Witkiewitz K, Alan Marlatt G, Walker D (2005) Mindfulness-based relapse prevention for alcohol and substance use disorders. J Cogn Psychother 19:211–228. 10.1891/jcop.2005.19.3.211 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  127. Wu CW, Chen CL, Liu PY, Chao YP, Biswal BB, Lin CP (2011) Empirical evaluations of slice-timing, smoothing, and normalization effects in seed-based, resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging analyses. Brain Connect 1:401–410. 10.1089/brain.2011.0018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  128. Yang CC, Barrós-Loscertales A, Pinazo D, Ventura-Campos N, Borchardt V, Bustamante JC, Rodríguez-Pujadas A, Fuentes-Claramonte P, Balaguer R, Ávila C, Walter M (2016) State and training effects of mindfulness meditation on brain networks reflect neuronal mechanisms of its antidepressant effect. Neural Plast 2016:9504642. 10.1155/2016/9504642 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  129. Yeo BTT, Krienen FM, Sepulcre J, Sabuncu MR, Lashkari D, Hollinshead M, Roffman JL, Smoller JW, Zöllei L, Polimeni JR, Fischl B, Liu H, Buckner RL (2011) The organization of the human cerebral cortex estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. J Neurophysiol 106:1125–1165. 10.1152/jn.00338.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  130. Zeidan F, Johnson SK, Diamond BJ, David Z, Goolkasian P (2010) Mindfulness meditation improves cognition: evidence of brief mental training. Conscious Cogn 19:597–605. 10.1016/j.concog.2010.03.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  131. Zhao XH, Wang PJ, Li CB, Hu ZH, Xi Q, Wu WY, Tang XW (2007) Altered default mode network activity in patient with anxiety disorders: an fMRI study. Eur J Radiol 63:373–378. 10.1016/j.ejrad.2007.02.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  132. Zou QH, Zhu CZ, Yang Y, Zuo XN, Long XY, Cao QJ, Wang YF, Zang YF (2008) An improved approach to detection of amplitude of low-frequency fluctuation (ALFF) for resting-state fMRI: fractional ALFF. J Neurosci Methods 172:137–141. 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2008.04.012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  133. Zylowska L, Ackerman DL, Yang MH, Futrell JL, Horton NL, Hale TS, Pataki C, Smalley SL (2008) Mindfulness meditation training in adults and adolescents with ADHD: a feasibility study. J Atten Disord 11:737–746. 10.1177/1087054707308502 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from eNeuro are provided here courtesy of Society for Neuroscience

RESOURCES