Abstract
We propose a novel approach to investigate the effects of sensitization, habituation and dishabituation in the brain using the analysis of the reaction-diffusion memory unit (RDMU). This unit consists of Morris-Lecar-type sensory, motor, interneuron and two input excitable cables, linked by four synapses with adjustable strength defined by Hebbian rules. Stimulation of the sensory neuron through the first input cable causes sensitization by activating two excitatory synapses, C1 and C2, connected to the interneuron and motor neuron, respectively. In turn, the stimulation of the interneuron causes habituation through the activation of inhibitory synapse C3. Likewise, dishabituation is caused through the activation of another inhibitory synapse C4. We have determined sensitization-habituation (BSH) and habituation-dishabituation (BHDH) boundaries as functions between synaptic strengths C2 and C3 at various strengths of C1 and C4. When BSH and BHDH curves shift towards larger values of C2, the RDMU can be easily inhibited. On the contrary, the RDMU can be easily sensitized or dishabituated if BSH and BHDH curves shift towards smaller values of C2. Our numerical simulations readily demonstrate that higher values of the Morris-Lecar relaxation parameter, greater leakage and potassium conductances, reduced length of the interneuron, and higher values of C1 all result in easier habituation of the RDMU. In contrast, we found that at higher values of C4 the RDMU becomes significantly more prone to dishabituation. Based on these simulations one can quantify BSH and BHDH curve shifts and relate them to particular neural outcomes.
Introduction
The formation of memory has been linked to long-term potentiation (LTP) and long-term depression (LTD), or the lasting increase or decrease of the strength of synaptic connections [1], [2]. Through LTP and LTD, information can be stored and behavioral patterns can become fixed in the brain. LTP and LTD occur after a period of learning, which is dominated by sensitization and habituation [2].
Sensitization and habituation are two basic processes in memory that are controlled by the strength of synaptic connections. Sensitization increases the probability that a given stimulus will produce a downstream transmembrane potential by increasing the connectivity of excitatory synapses while habituation decreases the probability of an transmembrane potential by increasing the connectivity of inhibitory synapses [3], [4]. This has been demonstrated in experiments on the gill withdrawal reflex in Aplysia californica, where repeated stimulation caused a prolonged withdrawal of the gill [5], [6].
The process of altering behavior based on changes in synaptic connection strengths is known as synaptic plasticity and is considered to be the underlying mechanism of the formation of memory [5], [7]. So far, the main stream approach to model the formation of memory was based on the threshold models of individual neurons [8], [9], [10]. Nevertheless, these models did not reflect physiological reaction-diffusion mechanisms which are responsible for the conduction of excitation in the neuronal environment. A recent effort to incorporate reaction-diffusion effects to quantify changes in the synaptic strength of isolated biological synapses [11] and synaptic-like memristive elements [12] was a step in the right direction. However, it still did not help to elucidate the reaction-diffusion origin of sensitization and habituation.
Another way to account for the spatial distribution of neuronal structures is the introduction of the concept of a meta-neuron. A meta-neuron consists of a relatively small group of several tens of neurons, which may be collectively involved in a particular macroscopic function. Such a structure includes Hodgkin-Huxley axons with added synaptic connections described by a set of gating equations [13]. In general, this approach may be considered as an adequate tool to describe large neuronal clusters, yet it entirely ignores the essential structural details which govern the balance between sensitization and habituation needed to process the information by a particular memory unit.
A typical example of sensitization and habituation can be seen in the startle response in zebrafish. The startle circuit is comprised of an auditory sensory neuron, a Mauthner cell (motor neuron) that triggers startle movement, and an inhibitory neuron that prevents familiar stimuli from triggering a startle response [14], [15]. The auditory neuron connects to both the inhibitory neuron and Mauthner cell with two excitatory synapse regions. The inhibitory neuron also connects to the Mauthner cell with an inhibitory synapse region. As a new incoming stimulus is repeated, the excitatory synapses connecting to the inhibitory neuron strengthen, and inhibitory synapses connecting to the Mauthner cell strengthen as well, resulting in less frequent triggering of the startle response. Although the described above circuit may be useful for explanation of a simple startle response in zebrafish [14], it lacks sufficient complexity to relate to the abrupt disappearance of habituation (dishabituation) which, according to Groves and Thompson dual-process theory, occurs even if the strength of excitatory synapses in the sensory neuron remains constant [4].
More complex examples of habituation are described in Sokolov’s comparator and Ramaswami’s negative-image models [16], [17]. The system for the formation of the model in Sokolov’s approach inhibits the excitatory process for recognized stimuli. When presented with unfamiliar stimuli, this inhibition ceases, resulting in dishabituation. Similarly, dishabituation may also occur in the negative-image model by inhibiting the negative image that negates incoming stimuli.
Based on these examples we propose a novel method based on the reaction-diffusion approach that is capable of quantifying combined effects of sensitization, habituation and dishabituation by connecting just a few axons with several synapses of adjustable strength. This model incorporates a circuit of three Morris-Lecar-type neurons [18] linked by four synapses defined by Hebbian synaptic strength rules [19]. The circuit is connected to two distinct Morris-Lecar-type input cables to allow a separate stimulation of sensory and inhibitory neurons. We will further refer to this circuit as the reaction-diffusion memory unit (RDMU, Fig 1).
Unlike conventional spiking threshold models, our reaction-diffusion approach eliminates the need for the use of purely phenomenological temporal delays associated with propagation of excitation from one neuron to another [20], [21]. Instead, these delays form naturally as a result of spatio-temporal evolution of excitation waves under the influence of different rates of cellular membrane polarization and re-polarization processes, various neuronal lengths, and altered strengths of synaptic connections between different neuronal fibers.
Model
It is possible to illustrate each individual synaptic connection as shown in Fig 2. To quantify cases with an arbitrary number of such connections, we define a variable C as a conglomerate synapse given by the sum in Eq (1)
(1) |
where excitatory and inhibitory synaptic wieghts si between two specific neurons have positive and negative signs, respectively. The sign of the conglomerate synapse C tells whether the net effect of all connections in the bundle is excitatory (+) or inhibitory (-) (Fig 2).
Using synaptic connections determined by Eq (1), we can link neurons to elucidate the processes of sensitization, habituation, and dishabituation (Fig 1). In this figure, the initial propagation of the transmembrane potential is provided by stimulating inputs A and B. The further evolution of the wave from input A causes sensitization through the passage of excitatory synapse C2. The wave also propagates to the interneuron through excitatory synapse C1, and produces habituation via inhibitory synapse C3. The evolution of the wave from input B causes dishabituation by passing through inhibitory synapse C4.
We study the RDMU mathematically by using a Morris-Lecar model with incorporated Hebbian conditions at the synaptic junctions, no flux boundary conditions at the ends of neurons, and additional diffusion terms at the sensory neuron’s branching point [18] (Fig 1). The equations for the Morris-Lecar model are as follows:
(2) |
(3) |
(4) |
(5) |
(6) |
(7) |
where I and tdur are the amplitude of the external current (stimulus) and its duration, respectively. Variables v and w represent the transmembrane voltage and dimensionless gating variable corresponding to the inhibitory response of the potassium channels. Parameters vL, vCa, and vK are equilibrium potentials for leakage, calcium, and potassium currents, respectively. Factors M∞ and W∞ are dimensionless constants which are determined by regulating voltages v1, v2, v3, and v4 [22].
By introducing specific time and spatial scales, one can define a set of dimensionless variables as follows:
(8) |
(9) |
(10) |
(11) |
(12) |
(13) |
Here v*, t*, and x* are dimensionless variables for transmembrane potential, time and spatial variables. Parameters gL* and gK* refer to dimensionless leakage and potassium conductance, respectively. Parameter gCa*, which determines dimensionless calcium conductance, is equal to one. The value of LD corresponds to the diffusion length. The value of L0 is the length of the main section of the RDMU, which is equal to the sum of the lengths of the sensory and motor neurons. The scales are given as follows: C0 = 10μF, vCa = 100mV, D = 1μS•cm2, gCa = 10mS, and L0 = 1mm.
For simplicity, we will further refer to dimensionless variables as v, w, t, x, gK, vK, etc., continuing with dimensionless Morris-Lecar equations in the following way:
(14) |
(15) |
(16) |
(17) |
(18) |
Table 1 summarizes the model dimensionless parameters which, unless stated otherwise, were used in all numerical experiments. These values are based on the dimensional values from [22].
Table 1. The dimensionless parameters used to solve model Eqs (14)–(22).
ϕ | gCa | gK | gL | vCa | vK | vL | v1 | v2 | v3 | v4 | I | vo | wo | vThr |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
0.017 | 1 | 1.8 | 0.45 | 1 | -0.84 | -0.6 | -0.012 | 0.18 | 0.02 | 0.30 | 1 | -0.58 | 0.0177 | -0.225 |
A set of boundary conditions for Eqs (14)–(18) includes no-flux conditions at the ends of the neurons (Eq (19)), and Hebbian links between the pre- and postsynaptic values of transmembrane potential at each of the RDMU synapses (Eq (20)) [19].
(19) |
(20A) |
(20B) |
Here Ci, vi,pre, vi,post, and vo are synaptic strengths, pre- and post-synaptic potentials of the ith synapse and resting value of transmembrane potentials, respectively. The first of Eq (20) describes the cumulative post-synaptic action of adjacent excitatory and inhibitory synapses C2 and C3, located at the beginning of the motor neuron. The second part describes the cumulative post-synaptic action of adjacent excitatory and inhibitory synapses C1 and C4, located at the beginning of the interneuron. The resting transmembrane potential, vo, as well as the resting value of the recovery variable, wo, are determined by the intersection of null-clines of the system of Eqs (14) and (15). The null-clines also define the excitation threshold, vthr, as shown in Fig 3.
To complete the formulation of synaptic conditions one needs an additional boundary condition to warrant that each synapse acts as a unidirectional gate which prevents the backward flow of transmembrane potentials. This condition is defined by Eq (22):
(21) |
Here x− is upstream with respect to the direction of the synaptic current.
Finally, we analyze the branching point located at the end of input A, where the sensory axon diverges (Fig 1). At this point we need to modify Eq (14) and consider two diffusion terms to account for cumulative two-dimensional effects comprised of two one-dimensional diffusion processes in the first (x) and the second (y) neuron branches, respectively:
(22) |
The rationale for considering a linear steady-state Hebbian rule (Eq (20)) is based upon the observation that behavioral and, to some extent, cognitive memories are associated with neural oscillations within theta and partial gamma ranges below 20 Hz [23], [24]. Under these conditions one can consider only isolated stimulating currents (Eq (7)) applied to the sensory neuron shown in Fig 1. Indeed, the transmembrane potentials induced by neuronal spikes in the hippocampus are on average 1-3ms in duration [25] and the intervals between successive spikes at frequencies below 20Hz are greater than 50ms. Therefore, the temporal evolution of the transmembrane potential resulted from a previous neuronal spike becomes completed well before the initiation of the next spiking activity. Accordingly, propagation of the transmembrane potentials in the RDMU branches evolves into transmission of the steady-state solitary pulses. Finally, since the propagation of transmembrane potentials is steady-state, a temporal derivative term in the Hebbian rule [19] can be omitted and resulting steady-state Hebbian links can be expressed as linear algebraic relations described by Eq (20).
Numerical method and system parameters
The system of Eqs (14)–(22) was solved numerically using an explicit finite difference method (See S1 Appendix). The dimensionless time and spatial steps were Δt = 2.5×10−5 and Δx = .01 for all experiments, respectively. Parameter in Eqs (14) and (22) was set to 0.01.
Unless stated otherwise, the sensory neuron and both inputs spanned 25 spatial intervals each, while the motor neuron and interneuron individually consisted of 50 spatial intervals (Fig 4). At the initial time t = 0 an external stimulus I of amplitude one was applied for a duration of 5×104Δt to nodes one through fifteen located at the beginning of inputs A and B.
Taking into consideration that the speed of transmembrane potentials in the brain is on average greater than 10m/s, parameters in Table 1 were set to reflect that the width of the excitation wave is much longer than a one millimeter total length of the sensory and motor neurons [26], [27]. Typical spatial and temporal evolutions of such waves are depicted in Fig 5.
Experimental protocol for numerical simulations
We studied the propagation of solitary pulses originated by identical input stimuli, I, applied to both inputs of the RDMU. A series of numerical simulations has been performed in order to evaluate the RDMU’s ability to reproduce the processes of sensitization, habituation and dishabituation. Depending on the values of synaptic strengths C1, C2, C3, and C4, the input stimuli propagated to the motor neuron and originated either sub-threshold or over-threshold responses, thus signifying the initiation of processes of sensitization, habituation and dishabituation.
The synaptic strength boundary between sensitization and habituation (BSH) was computed iteratively with the value of C4 fixed at zero. At fixed values of C2 we incrementally adjusted the value of C3 until regimes changed from sensitization to habituation, preventing the propagation of the over-threshold stimulus in the motor neuron. After that, values of C2 were increased by a set of sufficiently small increments and the process was repeated until values of C2 were equal to 1.5 or values of |C3| exceeded 5, beyond which the BSH and BHDH curves become linearly proportional. The boundary between habituation and dishabituation (BHDH) was calculated in the same manner at different values of C4.
Results
We determined whether the system was in sensitization, habituation, or dishabituation by comparing the maximum transmembrane potential to a threshold potential shown in Fig 3. The threshold potential was increased by 20% to account for wave propagation decay due to diffusion. BSH and BHDH curves were determined depending on whether the maximum transmembrane potential exceeded the modified threshold or remained below it. It was found that the differences between BSH and BHDH curves measured at 10 Δx from the end of the motor neuron and further away (30 Δx) did not exceed 5% and 16% at low and high values of C3, respectively. We chose to measure the magnitude of the transmembrane potentials closer to the end of the motor neuron at 10 Δx.
The influence of relaxation parameter, potassium and leakage conductance on shape of the excitation pulse
One of the main parts of our numerical simulations was focused on investigating the influence of the relaxation parameter ϕ and the potassium and leakage conductances on the dynamics of excitation pulses in the RDMU. As expected, we found that the magnitude of ϕ significantly affected the rate of relaxation of recovery variable w, and therefore invoked considerable changes in the width and speed of the excitation pulse. Potassium and leakage conductances also contributed to changes of the width of the pulse in a noticeable way. Fig 6 demonstrates various shapes of excitation pulses for different parameters ϕ, gK and gL. One can observe that smaller values of ϕ cause prolongation of pulses. Similar changes occur due to the decrease of either gK or gL.
Boundary between sensitization and habituation. The influence of ϕ, gK, gL and C1
We performed a series of numerical simulations to study the edge between sensitization and habituation processes in the RDMU, where the propagation of excitation waves is depicted in Fig 6. As shown in Fig 7 the BSH can be adequately described by Eq (23) (see also Table 2)
(23) |
Table 2. Constants a, b, and c for curves depicted in Figs 7 and 8.
ϕ, gK, gL, C1 | a | b | c | Figure/curve shape |
---|---|---|---|---|
0.0017, 2.5, 0.45, 0.8 | -3.81 | 2.76 | 1.59 | 8, filled circle |
0.0017, 1.8, 0.45, 0.8 | -3.37 | 3.14 | 1.06 | 8, filled square |
0.0017, 1.8, 0.3, 0.8 | -3.39 | 3.60 | 0.33 | 7, filled square |
0.017, 1.8, 0.3, 0.8 | -3.36 | 3.47 | 0.59 | 7, open square |
0.0017, 1.8, 0.3, 0.6 | -21.11 | 5.50 | 0.57 | 7, filled diamond |
0.017, 1.8, 0.3, 0.6 | -18.08 | 4.08 | 2.10 | 7, open diamond |
It should be noted that shorter (slower) pulses with higher values of ϕ correspond to lower absolute values of the inhibitory synaptic strength |C3|, thus indicating that it is easier to counter play an excitatory action of the synapse C2 for higher magnitudes of relaxation parameter ϕ. Alternatively, it was found that a decrease of leakage conductance gL resulted in an opposite shift of BSH towards higher values of |C3| associated with greater thresholds required to inhibit the RDMU at any given strength of C2 (Fig 7).
As shown in Fig 1, the excitatory synapse C1 plays a role as some type of a gate which regulates the flow of transmembrane potentials between the sensory and interneuron branches of the RDMU. Specifically, it varies the transmembrane potential’s diffusion flux, and therefore controls the amplitude of the excitation pulse which propagates through the inter-neuronal branch of the RDMU.
At lower values of C1, as well as in case of lesser gL, we again observed a significant shift of the BSH towards higher values of |C3| (Fig 7). Specifically, at C1 = 0.6 and C2 = 0.65 the value of |C3| required for the suppression of a pulse in the motor neuron was more than three times greater than a corresponding value of |C3| necessary for the suppression of a similar pulse at C1 = 0.8. It should be noted that all BSH curves depicted in Fig 7 are in agreement with approximation (23), since values of b are greater than one (Table 2). However, when the relaxation parameter ϕ and potassium conductance gK increase simultaneously the BSH curves turn to nearly directly proportional changes between inhibitory and excitatory synaptic strengths C2 and C3 (Fig 8).
Boundary between sensitization and habituation. The influence of the length of RDMU branches
It has been demonstrated above that the propagation of excitation waves from the sensory to motor neuron may significantly depend on both the strengths of the excitatory synapses C1 and C2, as well as on the influence of the inhibitory interneuron synaptic connection C3.
We also found that the lengths of the RDMU neurons can be additional important contributors into the balance between habituation and sensitization. Accordingly, the larger ratio of the interneuron’s length to the total length of the sensory and motor neurons results in more significant shift of the BSH curve to the left, making it more difficult to inhibit the RDMU even at smaller values of C2 (Fig 9).
Boundary between habituation and dishabituation. The influence of C4
To calculate the BHDH curves, we applied two stimuli through inputs A and B. As shown in Fig 1, input A connects directly to the sensory neuron while input B connects to the interneuron through inhibitory synapse C4. In this manner, C4 affects the BHDH curves by decreasing the responsiveness of the interneuron.
We found that increasing the strength of C4 resulted in a shift of BHDH curves towards smaller values of C2, thus reflecting dishabituation of the motor neuron. This effect is more pronounced for lower values of gL, where the shift in C2 is greater, and the slopes of the BHDH curves are consistently shallower (Fig 10).
As the strength of C4 further increases, the BHDH curves continue to shift to the left with steeper slopes, until the value of C4 is approximately 0.225, where the BHDH curves become vertical, as shown by dashed lines (Fig 10). Beyond this value, waves in the interneuron are unable to propagate to inhibitory synapse C3, resulting in complete dishabituation.
Discussion
Utilizing a novel approach, we studied variations in the BSH dependences in response to changes of parameters of the reaction-diffusion model with Hebbian type synaptic junctions between neurons. It was found that longer transmembrane potential waves (lower gL), which propagate in the motor neuron, caused the BSH curves to shift towards sensitization. On the contrary, shorter waves (greater gK) triggered the opposite shift of the BSH curves towards habituation. Also, we observed that synaptic strength C1 is another important parameter which has a significant effect on the positioning of BSH.
The value of C1 directly affects the transmembrane potential flux into the interneuron, thus changing its inhibitory influence on the RDMU. Specifically, it was found that different values of C1 either substantially reduced or increased the effectiveness of the inhibitory synapse C3, resulting in a state of the RDMU that is either significantly harder or easier to habituate. In addition to excitatory synapse C1, inhibitory synapse C4 also influences the RDMU through changing conditions for dishabituation. Indeed, just a small increase in C4 produces a notable shift in the BHDH curves towards lower values of excitatory strength in the synapse C2.
There are two possible approaches for incorporating dishabituation in the RDMU. These two approaches can be derived from the existing concepts of superimposition of sensitization and reversal of habituation described in [28]. The first approach is to increase the responsiveness of the motor neuron through an additional strong stimulus, which can be accomplished by adding an additional sensory neuron. In contrast, the second approach is to inhibit the interneuron that causes habituation. While the first approach results in the intertwining of sensitization and dishabituation, since the two processes share the same mechanism, the second one allows sensitization and dishabituation to be further distinguished.
There has been much debate about whether sensitization and dishabituation can be dissociated [28]-[32]. Once again, based on specific experimental procedures, these two processes could either occur through the same mechanisms [29], [30] or could have differing ones [28], [31], [32]. The classic dishabituation described in [4] could be an example of superimposed sensitization, where dishabituation and sensitization both result from direct stimulation to the habituated neuron. However, a more recent revision of this work emphasizes the reversal of habituation as another form of dishabituation [33], which we chose to model using additional input B (Fig 1).
A possible future enhancement to the RDMU would be the ability to model more complex behaviors than described above. Often described alongside sensitization and habituation is training, the process by which a weak input (trained input) becomes able to excite a target neuron by being repeatedly paired with a strong stimulus (training stimulus) [5]. After sufficient stimulations, the strength of the synapses in the trained input increases enough to excite the target neuron independently of the training stimulus. At present, our RDMU is unable to model training because the strengths of synaptic connections are fixed. Additionally, the current RDMU may need to be modified to accommodate series of periodic stimulations, as excitations due to isolated stimuli may not be capable of producing training.
Supporting information
Acknowledgments
We thank Andrei Starobin for the introduction of Eric Kandel's book "In search of memory", which played a significant role in shaping our approach to the problem.
Data Availability
All relevant data are within the manuscript.
Funding Statement
The authors received no specific funding for this work.
References
- 1.Takeuchi T, Duszkiewicz AJ, Morris RGM. The synaptic plasticity and memory hypothesis: encoding, storage and persistence. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B. 2014; 369: 1–14. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Kandel ER, Dudai Y, Mayford MR. The molecular and systems biology of memory. Cell. 2014; 157: 163–186. 10.1016/j.cell.2014.03.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Halberstadt AL, Geyer MA. Habituation and sensitization of acoustic startle: Opposite influences of dopamine D 1 and D 2 -family receptors. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory. 2009; 92: 243–248. 10.1016/j.nlm.2008.05.015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Groves PM, Thompson RF. Habituation: A dual-process theory. Physchological Review. 1970; 77: 419–450. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Kandel ER. The molecular biology of memory storage: a dialogue between genes and synapses. Science. 2001; 294: 1030–1038. 10.1126/science.1067020 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Kandel ER, Tauc L. Heterosynaptic facilitation in neurones of the abdominal ganglion of aplysia depilans. J. Phisiol. 1965; 181: 1–27. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Malenka RC, Bear MF. LTP and LTD: An embarrassment of riches. Neuron. 2004; 44: 5–21. 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.09.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Gerstner W, Naud R. How good are neuron models. Science 2009; 326: 379–380. 10.1126/science.1181936 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Izhikevich EM. Which model to use for cortical spiking neurons?. IEEE Transactions on Neural Networks. 2004; 15: 1063–1070. 10.1109/TNN.2004.832719 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Golomb D, Ermentrout GB. Bistability in pulse propogation in networks of excitatory and inhibitory populations. Physical Review Letters. 2001; 86: 4179–4182. 10.1103/PhysRevLett.86.4179 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Haselwandter CA, Kardar M, Triller A, Azeredo da Silveira R. Self-assembly and plasticity of synaptic domains through a reaction-diffusion mechanism. Physical Review E. 2015; 92: 1–19. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Adamatzky A, Chua L. Memristor Networks. Springer, New York: 2014 [Google Scholar]
- 13.Carnevale NT, Hines ML. The NEURON Book. Cambridge University Press; 2006. [Google Scholar]
- 14.Marsden KC, Granato M. In vivo Ca2+ imaging reveals that decreased dendritic excitability drives startle habituation. Cell Reports. 2015; 13: 1733–1740. 10.1016/j.celrep.2015.10.060 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Korn H, Faber DS. The mauthner cell half a century later: A neurobiological model for decision-making?. Neuron. 2005; 47: 13–28. 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.05.019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Sokolov EN. Neuronal models and the orienting influence In: Brazier MA., editor. The central nervous system and behavior: III. Macy Foundation; New York: 1960. [Google Scholar]
- 17.Ramaswami M. Network plasticity in adaptive filtering and behavioral habituation. Neuron. 2014; 82: 1216–1229. 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.04.035 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Morris C, Lecar H. Voltage oscillations in the barnacle giant muscle fiber. Biophys. J. 1981; 35: 193–213. 10.1016/S0006-3495(81)84782-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Dayan P, Abbott LF. Theoretical neuroscience: Computational and mathematical modeling of neural systems. London; 2001. [Google Scholar]
- 20.Anokhin KV, Burtsev MS, Ilyin VA, et al. A review of computational models of neuronal cultures in vitro. Mat. Biolog. Bioinform. 2012; 7: 372–397. (In Russian) [Google Scholar]
- 21.Li R, Cao J. Stability analysis of reaction-diffusion uncertain memristive neural networks with time-varying delays and leakage term. Appl. Math. and Comp. 2016; 278: 54–69. [Google Scholar]
- 22.Meier SR, Lancaster JL, Starobin JM. Bursting regimes in a reaction-diffusion system with action potential-dependent equilibrium. PLoS ONE. 2015; 10: 1–25. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Shuman T, Amendolara B, Golshani P. Theta rhythmopathy as a cause of cognitive disability in TLE. Epilepsy Currents. 2017; 17: 107–111. 10.5698/1535-7511.17.2.107 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Buzsáki G. Theta oscillations in the hippocampus. Neuron. 2002; 33: 325–340. 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00586-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Bean BP. The action potential in mammalian central neurons. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2007; 8: 451–465. 10.1038/nrn2148 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Firmin L, Field P, Maier MA, et al. Axon diameters and conduction velocities in the macaque pyramidal tract. J Neurophysiol. 2014; 112: 1229–1240. 10.1152/jn.00720.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Neuhuber B, Himes T, Shumsky JS, et al. Axon growth and recovery of function supported by human bone marrow stromal cells in the injured spinal cord exhibit donor variations. Brain Research. 2005; 1035: 73–85. 10.1016/j.brainres.2004.11.055 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Hawkins RD, Cohen TE, Kandel ER. Dishabituation in Aplysia can involve either reversal of habituation or superimposed sensitization. Learning & Memory. 2006; 13: 397–403. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Antonov I, Kandel ER, Hawkins RD. The contribution of monosynaptic PSPs to dishabituation and sensitization of the Aplysia siphon withdrawal reflex. J Neuroscience. 1999; 19: 10438–10450. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Castellucci V, Pinsker H, Kupfermann I, Kandel ER. Neuronal mechanisms of habituation and dishabituation of the gill-withdrawal reflex in Aplysia. Science. 1970; 167: 1745–1748. 10.1126/science.167.3926.1745 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Marcus EA, Nolen TG, Rankin CH, Carew TJ. Behavioral dissociation of dishabituation, sensitization, and inhibition in Aplysia. Science. 1988; 241: 210–213. 10.1126/science.3388032 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Cohen TE, Kaplan SW, Kandel ER, Hawkins RD. A simplified preparation for relating cellular events to behavior: mechanisms contributing to habituation, dishabituation, and sensitization of the Aplysia gill-withdrawal reflex. J Neuroscience. 1997; 17: 2886–2899. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Rankin CH, Abrams T, Barry RJ, et al. Habituation revisited: an updated and revised description of the behavioral characteristics of habituation. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2009; 92: 135–138. 10.1016/j.nlm.2008.09.012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
All relevant data are within the manuscript.