Abstract
Recent theories of episodic memory (EM) posit that the hippocampus provides a spatiotemporal framework necessary for representing events. If such theories hold true, then does the development of EM in children depend on the ability to first bind spatial and temporal information? And does this ability rely, at least in part, on normal hippocampal function? We investigated the development of EM in children 2–8 years of age (Study 1) and its impairment in Williams Syndrome, a genetic neurodevelopmental disorder characterized by visuospatial deficits and irregular hippocampal function, (Study 2) by implementing a nonverbal object-placement task that dissociates the what, where, and when components of EM. Consistent with the spatiotemporal-framework view of hippocampal EM, our results indicate that the binding of where and when in memory emerges earliest in development, around the age of 3, and is specifically impaired in WS. Space-time binding both preceded and was critical to full EM (what + where + when), and the successful association of objects to spatial locations seemed to mediate this developmental process.
Subject terms: Cognitive control, Human behaviour
Introduction
Episodic memory (EM) contains various details of an event, such as the objects or people involved (“what”), the spatial setting (“where”) and the temporal sequence (“when”) in which the event unfolded. To create an EM representation, however, it is not enough to independently remember such details; they must be remembered as a coherently bounded, continuous episode. It is widely known that the hippocampal formation (HF) is crucial for memory binding processes and that hippocampal damage disrupts such processes1–3. At the same time, the HF has been intensively studied as the neural basis of spatial navigation in both humans and nonhuman animals4–6. To reconcile the role of the hippocampus in EM and spatial navigation, some researchers have suggested that the HF uses space and time as a primary scaffold for coding episodic memories and that other event-defining components are subsequently incorporated into this spatiotemporal framework7,8.
As animals move through their surroundings, representations of sequentially visited locations (and the properties that define those locations) contributes to a memory of traveled navigational paths. In the same way, episodic memories are created while individuals interact with their environment in specific contexts and settings. Although the HF has long been regarded as critical for spatial mapping (e.g.,4,5), there is now a large consensus that it is also involved in the temporal organization of memories. Recent findings illustrating that hippocampal and entorhinal cells represent episodic time9–16 and studies showing specific impairment in the temporal organization of memories in patients with hippocampal damage (e.g.,17), provide considerable support for the HF’s role in representing both spatial and temporal information.
Due to the physical nature of our experiences, humans and other animals cannot avoid being immersed in a spatiotemporal context at all times. Therefore, without the tight binding of spatial locations with time (and the objects that co-occur at each timepoint) to guide our goal-oriented behavior, we would end up wandering helplessly about, with no contiguity in the memory of our own experiences. If a spatiotemporal context represented by basic navigation mechanisms provides a scaffold even for coherent memories of non-navigational experiences, it follows that the ability to bind spatial and temporal information into a continuous representation is a requisite for EM. From a developmental perspective, therefore, it can be hypothesized that the ability to bind where and when should develop first, before the ability to represent a fully integrated memory of an episode (e.g., what + where + when).
Previous studies on memory development in children have shown that while basic recognition abilities appear to be already present early in infancy, both for objects18–21 and spatial locations22, associative memory binding processes have a longer, more protracted maturation18,23–27. The variegated developmental trajectories of hippocampal subregions28,29 and the frontal-hippocampal network (e.g.,30) produce a gradual yet differentiated emergence of distinct memory processes and competences that, once integrated, can support a rich, coherent adult-like EM by around 9-to-11 years of age. However, studies with wider age ranges e.g.,18 suggest that memory for spatial contexts of events may occur earlier in development.
Olson and Newcombe31 suggested that among all the stimuli that are processed by our perceptual systems, spatial information might be more biologically salient and therefore play an organizing role in the structuring of our memories across development. Whether due to biological salience or an intrinsic feature of navigation-based hippocampal representations, there may be an advantage for binding space with time in early development. Such a process may provide the basis for episodic time, allowing memories of what happened (and the objects and people involved) to be remembered in a spatiotemporally continuous way. Moreover, if such abilities rely on the HF, subjects with atypical hippocampal development may be impaired in representing episodic time, particularly in the binding of spatial and temporal information.
The present study explored the above possibilities by investigating the developmental origins of EM in both typical and atypical development. In Study 1, we tested the binding of what, where, and when information in memory, across typically developing children between the ages 2 to 8, using a nonverbal object-hiding task (Figure1). In Study 2, we investigated how atypical hippocampal development may affect the development of episodic memory binding by conducting a similar test in young adult subjects with Williams Syndrome (WS), a genetic neurodevelopmental disorder characterized by a severe deficit in spatial cognition and irregular hippocampal function. If the HF is critical to the spatiotemporal binding of EM, it follows that WS subjects may be impaired in combining where + when, which consequently disrupts their EM. In contrast, WS may be relatively spared in their recognition memory, such as their ability to remember people and objects.
Study 1: Typical Development of EM Binding Components
Previous studies have reported that episodic memory in typically-developing (TD) children begins to mature around 6 years of age and reaches adult-like levels at around 9 years (e.g.,18). This improvement in memory representations of single events is attributed to hippocampal maturation at around 6 years32,33; nevertheless, memory binding may occur even before that age, but perhaps in a considerably less reliable or less strongly integrated fashion31. However, most studies on EM have focused on their maturation in older children in comparison to memory abilities in adults but not on their initial emergence in early childhood, making it unclear what is the exact developmental trajectory of memory-binding process in EM. This may be partly due to the limitations of the commonly used EM paradigms which often rely on designs that make them challenging to use with younger or children with atypical populations (computer-based or verbal tasks, e.g.34). Moreover, most nonverbal EM studies have utilized designs involving objects and locations on a computer screen18,35 and only few of them have tested EM development in first-person 3D space (e.g.21,36,37). To overcome the limitations of past studies, we created a simple nonverbal task, involving actual physical movement of the participant in real space with real objects (rather than just observation) to induce true EM of personal experiences (see Figure1 and Method section for a detailed description).
Results
Age-related differences in binding components across TD
Figure 2A shows performance in the three tests across development. With age in months as a continuous variable, a Spearman’s correlation between age and each memory-binding component was performed. We found a strong correlation between the variables (age and space-time: r = 0.536, p < 0.001; age and object-time: r = 0.417, p < 0.001; age and full EM: r = 0.459, p = <0.001). To assess developmental change, we split the subjects into three age groups, 2–4, 4–6, and 6–8 years old. First, we calculated effect sizes (using the average of the three tests), comparing two groups at a time: 2–4 vs. 4–6-year-olds (large Cohen’s d = 0.93); 4–6 vs. 6–8-year-olds (large Cohen’s d = 1.14); 2–4 vs. 6–8-year-olds (large Cohen’s d = 2.32; see descriptive statistics in Supplementary Information).
Performance on all three tasks varied significantly by age-group (Kruskal Wallis one-way ANOVA (Fig. 2B) H(2) = 12.278, p < 0.01; H(2) = 23.575, p < 0.001; H(2) = 12.178, p < 0.01, respectively). 2–4-year-olds and 4–6-year-olds differed significantly only in Space-Time Test (H(2) = −20.042, p = 0.001, Bonferroni-corrected). In the other two tests, 2–4- and 4–6-year-olds performed equally well (Object-Time Test: (H(2) = −5.565, p = 1.000; EM Test: H(2) = −4.208, p = 1.000) and were both significantly worse compared to 6–8-year-olds children (Object-Time Test: 2–4 year-olds, H(2) = −18.934, p < 0.01; 4–6-year-olds, H(2) = −13.278, p < 0.05; EM Test: 2–4-year-olds, H(2) = −18.282, p < 0.01; 4–6-year-olds, H(2) = −14.074, p < 0.05). These results suggest an early maturation of space-time binding between 2–4 years of age, followed by a protracted development of object-time binding and EM. In order to ensure that these patterns of results cannot be attributed simply to a memory of motor movements, we tested a subset of subjects in a “gated condition” during which a plastic gate was introduced right in front of the child between the encoding and retrieval phases. In order to perform the task, the child was required to walk around the gate forcing him to take a roundabout path that was different from the encoding phase (see Method section). We found no differences in performance between the gated and default conditions (Space-Time Test U = 9.500 p = 0.548; Object-Time Test U = 5.500 p = 0.151; EM U = 8.000 p = 0.421, Mann-Whitney U Test).
In the default condition, when looking just at children’s choice of locations and objects, without taking their temporal sequence into consideration, children performed well over chance across all ages (locations: 2–4-year-olds: 0.69 ± 0.08; 4–6-year-olds: 0.90 ± 0.04; 6–8-year-olds: 0.97 ± 0.02 paired two-tailed t-test p < 0.001, p < 0.001, p < 0.001, respectively; objects: 2–4-year-olds: 0.67 ± 0.06; 4–6-year-olds: 0.70 ± 0.05; 6–8-year-olds: 1 ± 0.00 paired two-tailed t-test p < 0.001, p < 0.001, p < 0.001, respectively) indicating that young children do not have difficulty recognizing the objects and places themselves (Fig. 2C, gray lines). Nevertheless, there is significant improvement for both over development (Kruskal Wallis one-way ANOVA, H(2) = 16.835, p < 0.0011; H(2) = 28.696, p < 0.001, respectively), with 2–4-year-olds performing worse than both 4–6- and 6–8-year-olds in remembering spatial locations (H(2) = −14.229, p < 0.01, H(2) = −18.991, p < 0.001, respectively, Bonferroni-corrected), and 6–8-year-olds performing better than both 2–4- and 4–6-year-olds in object recognition (H(2) = −24.750, p = 0.0001, H(2) = −21.188, p < 0.001, respectively, Bonferroni-corrected).
In order to address the possibility that early development of space-time binding could simply be an artifact of improvement in children’s spatial memory (and not in its temporal organization), a Generalized Linear Model (GZLM) was performed using space-time binding accuracy as the dependent variable, age-groups as the independent factor and spatial location accuracy as a covariate (Fig. 2C black line, left). Performance in the Space-Time Test still varied significantly between the three age-groups (F(2) = 6.755, p < 0.01). 2–4-year-olds (0.48 ± 0.07) differed significantly from 4–6- and 6–8-year-olds in space-time binding, even when adjusted for spatial location accuracy (0.74 ± 0.05, p < 0.05; 0.82 ± 0.05, p < 0.01, respectively, Bonferroni-corrected), suggesting that the variance in the model is not explained simply by spatial memory, but by the developmental changes in the binding of space and time. Similar analyses were performed on the Object-Time Test (Fig. 2C black line, right), using object-time binding accuracy as the dependent variable, age-groups as the independent factor and object identity accuracy as a covariate. In contrast with the Space-Time Test, the Object-Time Test showed no significant differences across age-groups once it was adjusted for object choice accuracy (F(2) = 0.871, p = 0.423), suggesting that the age effect in object-time binding is mostly driven by improvements in object recognition. Altogether, these results suggest that the development of temporally ordered representation of places does not simply rely on spatial memory but on the specific development in space-time binding (and that this developmental pattern is not true for object-time binding).
Additionally, we wanted to address the possibility that the difference among age-groups could simply stem from age-related differences in working memory (and not long-term hippocampus-based memory). Because we had conducted the Corsi Block-Tapping Task in each subject, we used children’s performance in that task as a measure of visuospatial working memory38 and conducted a GZLMusing space-time binding accuracy as the dependent variable and age-group as the independent factor, with Corsi performance as a covariate. Similar analyses were conducted with Object-Time and the full EM Test. The age effect remained significant in all three tests (F(2) = 5.290, p = 0.009; F(2) = 4.981, p = 0.011; F(2) = 4.045, p = 0.025, respectively) even after accounting for age differences in WM, suggesting that the variance in the model is not explained by improvements in visuospatial short-term memory but by the developmental changes in long-term EM.
Effects of space-time binding in EM representation
In order to investigate whether space-time binding ability might play a role in EM development, we conducted an ordinal regression analysis using performance in the Space-Time Test and Object-Time Test as predictors and performance in the EM Test as the dependent variable, with age as a covariate (see Fig. 3A,B). All the variables, with the exception of age, had an ordinal distribution representing four scores ranging from 0 (“Not Accurate”) to 1 (“Strongly Accurate”). The model was statistically significant (χ2(7) = 24.417, p = 0.001), showing a significant relationship between EM (scored 1, as the referent value) and total failure in the Space-Time Test (Wald χ2(1) = 4.067, p < 0.05; negative logit regression: Exp (B) = 0.137) (the Wald test evaluates whether it is likely that the estimated effect could be zero). This result means that the failure in the Space-Time Test reduces the probability of being strongly accurate in the EM Test. In other words, successful EM rarely occurs without successful space-time binding, but this is not true for object-time binding. These results are in line with past studies7,8 emphasizing the role of space-time binding as a scaffold for episodic memories and shed light on the developmental emergence of this cognitive ability.
Detailed analysis on the EM Test components: The binding of objects to space-time.
We performed a detailed analysis on the EM Test components (i.e. spatial location, object identity, space-time binding, object-time binding, and object-space binding) in order to investigate whether developmental changes in space-time binding can be observed even within the EM Test itself. Figure 4A shows the distribution of each
EM Test binding component in children, divided by age-groups. Performance on space-time and object-space binding varied significantly by age-group, while object-time did not show any significant differences (Kruskal Wallis one-way ANOVA, object-time: H(2) = 5.437, p = 0.066; space-time: H(2) = 8.678,; p < 0.05 object-space: H(2) = 12.029, p < 0.01). 2–4-year-olds and 6–8-year-olds differed significantly in space-time and object-space (H(2) = −13.133, p < 0.05; H(2) = −19.075, p < 0.01, respectively), while 4–6-year-olds and 6–8-year-olds differed significantly only in space-time (H(2) = −11.769, p < 0.05). Interestingly, 2–4-year-olds perform as well as 4–6-year-olds children in their ability to bind space and time components in the EM Test. Surprisingly, if we compare these results with those obtained in the Space-Time Test (Fig. 4B, left), we observe that 2–4-year-olds do better in the space-time component of the EM Test (choosing the correct location in the correct order, regardless of object identity) than the Space-Time Test, in which object identity was held constant (Z = −2.137, p < 0.05, Wilcoxon signed-ranks test). In contrast, there is no significant difference between the object-time component of the EM Test (choosing the correct objects in the correct order, regardless of their location) in comparison with the Object-Time Test, in which spatial location was held constant (Z = −1.742, p = 0.082, Wilcoxon signed-ranks test); Fig. 4B, right).
How are even the youngest children able to bind space and time in the EM Test when the task requires a higher cognitive load, compared to the Space-Time Test? One possibility may be that because the EM Test was always conducted last, there is a practice effect. However, it is noted that such a practice effect is not found for object-time component. An alternative explanation is that the presence of objects strengthens spatiotemporal memory through the association of object content to spatial location. In fact, that may be the very mechanism by which objects get bound onto the spatiotemporal continuum of episodic memory. In Fig. 4C, it is possible to see that object-space binding follows a developmental pattern that highly resembles that of object-time. Consistent with this, space-time accuracy is significantly higher than both object-time and object-space accuracy, while object-time and object-space are not different (Z = 2.058, p < 0.05; Z = 2.795, p < 0.01; Z = −0.715, p = 0.475, respectively, Wilcoxon signed-ranks test).
When we conducted Spearman’s correlations between the binding components of the EM Test, we saw that, while these variables were strongly all related to one another according to zero-level correlations (object-time and object-space: r = 0.636, p < 0.001; object-time vs space-time: r = 0.361, p < 0.01; object-space vs space-time: r = 0.467, p < 0.001; Table 1), their partial correlations show a different result (see Table 2). In particular, if we partial out the effects of object-space binding, then object-time and space-time binding are no longer significantly correlated (r = 0.093, p = 0.456), suggesting that the successful binding of objects with time is mediated by the successful association between objects and its spatial context. In contrast, the other correlations remain largely unchanged. In other words, in the development of EM, the association of “what happened” with its spatial context may bring with it the temporal context (which has been already bounded to space) for free.
Table 1.
object-time binding | space-time binding | object-space binding | EM | |
---|---|---|---|---|
object-time binding | 1 | 0.361** | 0.636** | 0.819** |
space-time binding | 0.361** | 1 | 0.467** | 0.674** |
object-space binding | 0.636** | 0.467** | 1 | 0.859** |
EM | 0.819** | 0.674** | 0.859** | 1 |
Pearson’s zero-order correlation for object-time, object-space, and space-time binding components and the total score of the EM Test in the TD sample.
Table 2.
Controlled for object-space binding |
Controlled for object-time binding |
Controlled for space-time binding |
||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
object-space binding | object-time binding | object-space binding | object-time binding | object-space binding | object-time binding | |
object-space binding | 1 | 0.093 (ns) | 1 | 0.330** | 1 | 566** |
object-time binding | ,093 (ns) | 1 | 0.330** | 1 | 566** | 1 |
Partial correlations between object-time and object-space binding components controlled for the effect of object-space, object-time and space-time binding. ns; p < 0.05 *; p < 0.01 **.
Study 2: Atypical development of hippocampal binding components in Williams Syndrome
Williams Syndrome is a rare genetic disorder caused by a microdeletion of about 25 genes in region q11.23 of chromosome 7. WS patients are a particularly interesting population for investigating EM because of their abnormal hippocampal development and their unique cognitive profile. Individuals with WS show relative strengths in domains such as language production and face recognition but exhibit severe deficits in a range of spatial functions19–22. Pervasive impairment in the ability to construct arrays of objects with specific spatial relations among them39–42 is one of the most characteristic features of WS. Consistent with their visuospatial deficits, WS individuals are impaired in short43,44 and long-term45 tests of visuospatial memory. Importantly, however, their impaired long-term visuospatial performance is not merely due to difficulties in spatial working memory: WS show a greater memory decay effect between short and long delays, suggesting a specific deficit of consolidation of spatial memory. Such memory deficits documented in WS extend into their allocentric spatial navigation abilities46, as well as their associative memory for pairs of items47 and word-location pairs48. While there is a large body of literature on spatial memory in WS, no study has specifically investigated the ability to represent temporally continuous memories in this population. Therefore, Study 2 tested WS subjects (compared to TD controls) in the same task described in Study 1, in order to selectively assess the role of single EM components and their binding with time, as well as to speculate about the effects of atypical hippocampal development.
Results
Impairment in space-time binding and its impact on EM in WS
We investigated memory binding processes in WS subjects, by comparing their performance with those of mental age-matched (MA) and chronological age-matched (CA) control subjects (Fig. 5A).
We first calculated effect sizes using the average of the three tests, comparing two groups at a time: WS vs. MA (large Cohen’s d = 0.68); WS vs CA (large Cohen’s d = 1.67); MA vs. CA (large Cohen’s d = 1.15; see descriptive statistics in Supplementary Information). Performance on the three tests significantly differed across groups (Independent-samples Kruskal Wallis one-way ANOVA: Object-Time Test: H(2) = 6.823, p < 0.05; Space-Time Test: H(2) = 14.658, p = 0.001; EM Test: H(2) = 23.004, p < 0.001). WS participants performed significantly worse compared to MA controls in space-time binding and in EM (H(2) = −10.409, p < 0.05; H(2) = −13.568, p < 0.05, respectively, Bonferroni-corrected), while their memory for ordered recall of items was only delayed compared to CA (H(2) = −11.591, p < 0.05; Fig. 5A). Interestingly, spatial location and object identity accuracy were both highly accurate and did not show any significant difference across the three groups (Fig. 5B).
In order to address the possibility that the failure in space-time binding could be driven simply by a deficit in WS subjects’ spatial memory and not its relation to temporal continuity, we performed a GZLM with accuracy in the Space-Time Test as the dependent variable, groups (WS, MA, CA) as the independent factor and spatial location accuracy as a covariate (Fig. 5B above). Performance in the Space-Time Test differed significantly by group, having adjusted for spatial accuracy (F(2) = 6.927, p < 0.001), with WS subjects (mean 0.75 ± 0.04) performing significantly worse than MA and CA controls (means and t-tests against WS: 0.92 ± 0.04, p = 0.01; 0.95 ± 0.04, p < 0.01, respectively, Bonferroni-corrected). These results suggest that the difference across groups is not solely driven by differences in spatial accuracy but by those in space-time binding. A similar analysis was conducted using performance in the Object-Time Test as a dependent variable, groups as an independent factor and object identity accuracy as a covariate (Fig. 5B below). We found that the difference in object-time binding across groups was no longer significant once it has been adjusted for object accuracy (F(2) = 2.299, p = 0.109). All together, these results suggest that the deficit in space-time memory in WS subjects does not simply rely on spatial memory difficulties but on binding deficits. As was the case for typically developing children in Study 1 (see Fig. 2C), temporal information was particularly relevant for explaining group differences when the task required recalling a sequence of locations rather than objects.
In order to investigate whether WS subjects’ performance can be explained by general cognitive ability, we conducted Spearman’s rank order correlations between WS subjects’ mental age, performance in Space-Time and Object-Time tests, and spatial location and object identity recognition accuracy. A significant correlation was found only between mental age and spatial location accuracy (r = 0.54, p < 0.01) but not between mental age and space-time binding (r = 0.24, p = 0.355). This means that as mental age (and general intelligence) increases in WS, location (but not temporal) memory improved; in other words, space-time binding deficits in WS patients is specific and is not related to their general cognitive abilities.
Discussion
In Study 1, we set out to investigate whether space-time binding provides a scaffold upon which EM is constructed over development. First, we found that the ability to fully bind the what, when, and where components of an event develops around 6 years of age, consistent with other studies of episodic memory in children e.g.,49. Additionally, we showed here for the first time that the emergence of EM is preceded by the development of a competence in coherently binding together spatial and temporal information from 2 to 4 years of age and that this particular memory-binding component has a strong relationship with EM ability across individuals. Moreover, children’s ability to embed objects with time is significantly correlated with their ability to bind those objects to space, which may indicate that the binding of what happened with a spatiotemporal framework is necessary for successful EM.
The interpretation that the association of object content to spatial context is what allows objects to be embedded in EM is supported by direct recordings of spatially-selective neuronal activity in the hippocampus of both human and non-human animals during object memory recall7,50 and is consistent with theories that posit that the spatiotemporal context functions as a primary scaffold for memories of events7,8. However, our results add to that view behavioral and neuropsychological evidence on the origins of the basis for such EM representations in early human development. Our findings are also consistent with previous studies on cognitive development indicating a significant improvement in the use of multiple cues (e.g., environmental boundaries and landmarks) in spatial navigation around 5 or 6 years of age51; such changes in cue-binding abilities could be related to the maturation of EM in early childhood. Researchers suggest52 that the development of two shared mechanisms underlies both episodic memory and allocentric navigation: pattern separation, necessary to distinguish different memories and supported by the dentate gyrus53, and pattern completion, needed to integrate the elements of a memory and supported by CA3 and CA154. While the early binding of space with time could be supported by temporal representations which have been found both in the hippocampus and in the entorhinal cortex [e.g.,15], the protracted maturation of the dentate gyrus55 as well as its connections with CA356 might underlie the late emergence of fully bounded EM.
In Study 2, we investigated how the emergence of memory-binding abilities is affected by a deficit in spatial cognition accompanied by abnormal hippocampal development in WS. We showed that WS participants, unlike 4-year-old TD children, struggle with space-time binding and perform even worse when attempting to bind object contents inthe full EM Test.
Importantly, our results showed that WS was not impaired on their recognition of spatial location, indicating that their memory binding deficit is not driven solely by their spatial cognitive impairment. In fact, children and WS subjects were highly accurate in picking out the correct boxes and objects; they just could not embed them into a continuous episode. This finding is consistent with past work that shows a dissociation between recognition processes and memory-binding in TD children18,34,49 and a preserved object recognition ability in WS47. Accurate memory for the spatial context of objects has been linked to activity in the anterior hippocampus33,57, while the posterior hippocampus is correlated with navigation expertise58. Given that the hippocampus of WS individuals exhibits structural and functional abnormalities along the anterior-posterior axis, we might speculate that their impairment in spatiotemporal binding may be related to their smaller posterior hippocampus and that their difficulty in further binding elements of memory into a spatiotemporal context may be related to the decreased activity in the anterior hippocampus.
Interestingly, despite a clear and consistent deficit in spatiotemporal and episodic recall, the spared memory for sequences of objects in WS suggests an alternative strategy of object-related processing that might rely, in part, on connections with the prefrontal cortex59. This object-related success is consistent with spatial navigation studies that show better performance46 and qualitatively different behavior60 in the use of landmarks in WS compared with TD children.
Conclusions
Time seems to have a special relation to space in that the first component of episode-like memory to emerge in development and to be impaired in WS is the binding of spatial and temporal information. Not only that, time seems to depend on its binding with space in order to weave together conceptual content, such as object identity, into its folds. These findings raise the possibility that the neural circuitry underlying episodic memory, in which the HF acts as a hub of information32,61, prioritizes spatiotemporal binding, which is ultimately what makes mental time travel possible. Further progress in our understanding of how EM develops in children and how they become impaired with hippocampal dysfunction is crucial for several reasons. First, the neural mechanisms underlying EM has been implicated not only in memory but in imagination or future planning62. Understanding how EM abilities can change over the course of development or neurological disorders will indirectly shed light on the emergence (and impairment) of high-level cognitive abilities such as creativity and imagination. Second, it has been proposed that the hippocampus adaptively organizes episodic memories into conceptual knowledge that drives adaptive novel behavior63. Insight into the origins of hippocampus-dependent memory binding mechanisms may help characterize how abstract conceptual maps can form and be modified in human cognition.
Methods
Participants
Study 1
67 children (34 females) between the ages of 2.52 and 8.74 (M = 5.53; SD = 1.63) participated in Study 1. Children were recruited through fliers delivered to daycares, preschools and elementary schools. Once parents agreed to have their child participate by signing up on the lab’s online form, they were contacted for an appointment. All children were tested at the Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience Lab at the Center for Mind/Brain Sciences, University of Trento. Children received a small gift for their participation. Most children came from Rovereto and its surrounding areas. 11 additional children from 2 to 4 years of age were excluded from the study due to medical problems, extreme prematurity, important language development delays, failure to complete the task, or refusal to follow instructions.
Study 2
27 Williams Syndrome subjects participated in Study 2 and were matched with TD control participants with similar mental and chronological ages. Because the age range proved to be too heterogeneous, ranging from children as young as 6 year old to 40-year-old adults, we decided to exclude all child participants (under the age of 12) in order to create a more homogeneous sample. Therefore, the final sample considered for analysis consisted of 22 WS subjects (11 females), 22 mental-age-matched controls (11 females) and 22 chronological-age-matched controls (11 females).
The WS participants belonged to two different Italian Williams Syndrome associations: AGSW (Associazione Genitori Sindrome di Williams) and AFSW (Associazione Famiglie Sindrome di Williams). Families were contacted by telephone or email after parents had agreed to have their child participate in the study. Subjects were tested in three different locations: during two different yearly WS retreats in Fano and Peschiera and by appointment at the Clinical Genetic Unit, Fondazione IRCCS Ca Granda Ospedale Maggiore Policlinico in Milan. TD control children and adults were tested at the Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience Lab of the Center for Mind/Brain Sciences, University of Trento. They were recruited either from Rovereto and surrounding areas or by contacting families of children who had previously participated in studies at the lab. Importantly, in each location we recreated the same experimental environment in terms of room size and landmark positions. Each WS participant was individually matched for mental and chronological age, gender and trial order with TDcontrols. WS subjects ranged in chronological age from 12.86 to 43.78 (M = 24.84; SD = 8.11) and mental age range from 5.11to 8.96 (M = 6.44; SD = 0.98). For a measure of non-verbal mental age, WS participants were administered four subscales of the Brief IQ Visualization and Reasoning tests from the Leiter International Performance Scale Revised (Leiter-R).
Procedure
Participants were tested on a non-verbal object-placement task developed to allow isolated testing of the what, where, and when components that make up an episodic memory. Each experimental session was composed of three tests (Fig. 1): The Space-Time Test was designed to measure the ability to remember spatial locations and organize them in time but did not require object recognition. The Object-Time Test measured the ability to remember different objects in a temporal order, while keeping spatial location constant. Finally, the EM Test assessed the ability to combine object identity, spatial locations, and their temporal order together into one representation. All subjects completed the three tests. The Space-Time Test and Object-Time Test were counterbalanced in their order, and then the EM Test was always presented last. The selected objects, locations, and temporal sequences were always randomized across subjects and within trials. The task was straightforward: the experimenter sequentially placed three objects in boxes laid out in the experimental room while the participant was watching (demonstration phase), then the participant was asked to do the same thing by placing the objects himself/herself (encoding phase). After a delay during which a verbal interference task was administered, the participant was asked to re-enact his own previous actions (retrieval phase).
Before beginning the experiment, two training tasks were provided in order to elicit imitation behaviors in children. In the first one, the experimenter hid one of three colored crayons in one of three grey plastic cups, while in the second one the experimenter demonstrated some motor sequences while standing up and facing the participant. The participant was then asked to repeat the experimenter’s behavior. If the participant struggled to understand or failed to reproduce the correct behavior, the experimenter repeated the sequence while emphasizing the relevant information. The training ended after the participant had successfully copied the experimenter twice.
To start the experiment, the experimenter brought the participant to the starting position located at one end of a rectangular room, in front of a large door. Once the participant was standing in front of the door and facing the room, the experimenter proceeded to give the instructions verbally by using an animal finger puppet (e.g., a lion) and telling the following short story: “You know, this little lion loves cookies; she always bakes too many and then has to hide them. Now, pay attention carefully to how I hide them, because afterwards you will have to do it just like me. Otherwise the lion will not be able to find them later!” The participant was then shown a plastic tray with the “cookies” (objects) that the animal had “baked” and was given about five seconds to observe them or manipulate them. The tray was then placed on an adjacent shelf, out of view from the child. Then the participant watched the experimenter hide three objects into a subset of the five boxes that were distributed across the room. For the Space-Time Test, children were provided with three identical objects that they had to place into three of the five boxes in the room. For the Object-Time Test, children had to choose three of five different objects to place into a single box nearby. For the EM Test, children had to choose three of five different objects, and place them into three of five different boxes. The objects were always hidden one at the time: the experimenter would pick up one, show it to the child for a couple seconds, before proceeding to hide it, and then return to the starting point. After each of the three objects were placed into a box, the experimenter led the child to face away from the boxes under the pretense of hiding from strong winds, while another experimenter collected the objects and returned them to the tray.
For the participant to encode his/her own episodic experience, the encoding phase involved the participant being presented with the same objects in the tray and instructed to “hide the cookies” just like the experimenter did. If the participant attempted to pick up more than one object at once, the experimenter would remind him/her to hide them one at the time. Once each of the three objects had been hidden, child and experimenter would again face away while they were picked up by a second experimenter. After the encoding episode, an interference task was administered. The participant and the experimenter sat down together at a table present in the room under the pretense of teaching the animal puppet a few words. A verbal naming task was performed for a total of 3 minutes. Finally, during the retrieval phase, the participant and experimenter returned to their starting position by the door, and the participant was presented with the objects and asked to “hide the cookies” as she/he had previously done. After each trial, the participant was rewarded with a sticker (regardless of his/her performance).
In order to investigate the potential effect of learning a particular trajectory or motor sequence, 5 subjects were tested in a “gated condition” during which a plastic gate was introduced right in front of the child between the encoding and retrieval phases. In order to perform the task, the child was required to walk around the gate, through a different route than the one previously taken during the encoding phase. There were no differences children’s memory performance in this condition, as compared to the rest (Space-Time Test U = 9.500 p = 0.548; Object-Time Test U = 5.500 p = 0.151; EM U = 8.000 p = 0.421, Mann-Whitney U Test).
Scoring
By comparing the subject’s accuracy between the encoding and retrieval phases, we calculated the accuracy in the participant’s memory for the single components (object identity or spatial location) and the extent to which they were able to bind them in a temporal sequence (object-time binding; space-time binding; EM). For the individual element analysis, the space location and object identity accuracy were calculated by giving a point for each correct remembered location or object, respectively. For temporal binding, three scores were calculated: space-time binding, for each correctly-remembered location in the correct place in the sequence; object-time binding, for each correctly-remembered object in its correct temporal sequence; EM, giving a point for each different object that is correctly remembered in its correct spatial and temporal position (general EM score). The general EM score was decomposed into three further component scores representing space-time, object-time, and object-space binding. For simplicity, each index was calculated on the basis of a proportion of correct scores (maximum raw score being 3). See the Supplementary Information for a detailed description of procedures and scoring system.
This study was conducted in accordance with the guidelines of the Human Research Ethics Committee of the University of Trento (internal review board approval No. 2016-005). The experimental protocol was approved by the internal review board, and informed consent was obtained from both the participants and their parents or legal guardians.
Supplementary information
Acknowledgements
We would like to thank the Center for Mind/Brain Sciences (CIMeC) at the University of Trento for supporting this research from its initial stages and our colleague Carlo Miniussi who provided valuable insight and expertise throughout. We would also like to express our profound gratitude to all the participants, their families, and the Italian Williams Syndrome associations (AGSW and AFSW) for making this study possible. Finally, we would like to thank our editor and reviewers for their constructive comments and suggestions. This work was funded, in part, by a grant to S.A.L. from the National Research Foundation (NRF-2019R1F1A1062801: Neural Correlates of Human Spatial Cognition and Episodic Memory : Effects of Cognitive Training) and by the Institute for Information & Communications Technology Planning & Evaluation(IITP) grant to S.A.L., funded by the Korean government (MSIT) (2019-0-01371, Development of Brain-Inspired AI with Human-like Intelligence).
Author contributions
M.M. and S.A.L. conceived and designed the study; M.M., N.B. and M.F.B. organized subject recruitment; M.F.B. performed clinical duties associated with Williams Syndrome subjects; M.M. and N.B. performed data collection; M.M. and N.B. analyzed the data; M.M. and S.A.L. wrote the paper.
Data availability
The datasets generated during and/or analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary information
is available for this paper at 10.1038/s41598-019-53823-w.
References
- 1.Brizzolara D, Casalini C, Montanaro D, Posteraro F. A case of amnesia at an early age. Cortex. 2003;39:605–625. doi: 10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70856-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Brown MW, Aggleton JP. Recognition memory: what are the roles of the perirhinal cortex and hippocampus? Nat Rev Neurosci. 2001;2:51–61. doi: 10.1038/35049064. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Aggleton JP, Brown MW. Episodic memory, amnesia, and the hippocampal-anterior thalamic axis. Behavioural Brain Sci. 1999;22:425–444. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X99002034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Bird CM, Burgess N. The hippocampus and memory: insights from spatial processing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2008;9:182–194. doi: 10.1038/nrn2335. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.McNaughton BL, Battaglia FP, Jensen O, Moser EI, Moser M. Path integration and the neural basis of the ‘cognitive map’. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2006;ume 7:663–678. doi: 10.1038/nrn1932. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.O’Keefe, J., & Nadel, L. The hippocampus as a Cognitive Map. Oxford Univ. Press, New York. (1978).
- 7.Miller JF, et al. Neural activity in human hippocampal formation reveals the spatial context of retrieved memories. Science. 2013;342:1111–1114. doi: 10.1126/science.1244056. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Healey MK, Long NM, Kahana MJ. Contiguity in episodic memory. Psychon Bull Rev. 2019;26:1–22. doi: 10.3758/s13423-018-1537-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Thavabalasingama S, O’Neil EB, Lee ACH. Multivoxel pattern similarity suggests the integration of temporal duration in hippocampal event sequence representations. Neuroimage. 2018;178:136–146. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.05.036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Lehn H, et al. A specific role of the human hippocampus in recall of temporal sequences. J Neurosci. 2009;29:3475–3484. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5370-08.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Tubridy S, Davachi L. Medial temporal lobe contributions to episodic sequence encoding. Cerebral cortex. 2010;21:272–80. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhq092. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Bellmund JLS, Deuker L, Doeller CF. Mapping sequence structure in the human lateral entorhinal cortex. eLife. 2019;8:e45333. doi: 10.7554/eLife.45333. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Montchal ME, Reagh ZM, Yassa MA. Precise temporal memories are supported by the lateral entorhinal cortex in humans. Nature Neuroscience. 2019;22:284–288. doi: 10.1038/s41593-018-0303-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Thavabalasingam, S., O’Neil, E. B., Tay, J., Nestor, A. & Lee, A. C. H. Evidence for the incorporation of temporal duration information in human hippocampal long-term memory sequence representations. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 116, 6407–6414 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 15.Rolls ET, Mills P. The Generation of Time in the Hippocampal Memory System. Cell Reports. 2019;28:1649–1658. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2019.07.042. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Clewett D, DuBrow S, Davachi L. Transcending time in the brain: How event memories are constructed from experience. Hippocampus. 2019;29:162–183. doi: 10.1002/hipo.23074. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Spiers HJ, Burgess N, Hartley T, Vargha-Khadem F, O’Keefe J. Bilateral hippocampal pathology impairs topographical and episodic memory but not visual pattern matching. Hippocampus. 2001;11:715–25. doi: 10.1002/hipo.1087. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Picard L, Cousin S, Guillery‐Girard B, Eustache F, Piolino P. How do the different components of episodic memory develop? Role of executive functions and short‐ term feature‐binding abilities. Child Development. 2012;83:1037–1050. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01736.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Sluzenski, J., Newcombe, N. S., & Kovacs, S. L. Binding, relational memory, and recall of naturalistic events: A developmental perspective. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition 32, 89–100 (2006). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 20.Lloyd ME, Doydum AO, Newcombe NS. Memory binding in early childhood: Evidence for a retrieval deficit. Child development. 2009;80:1321–1328. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01353.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Hayne H, Imuta K. Episodic memory in 3‐and 4‐year‐old children. Developmental Psychobiology. 2011;53:317–322. doi: 10.1002/dev.20527. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Newcombe N, Huttenlocher J, Drummey AB, Wiley JG. The development of spatial location coding: Place learning and dead reckoning in the second and third years. Cognitive Development. 1998;13:185–200. doi: 10.1016/S0885-2014(98)90038-7. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Shing YL, Werkle-Bergner M, Li SC, Lindenberger U. Associative and strategic components of episodic memory: a life-span dissociation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 2008;137:495–513. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.137.3.495. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Lorsbach TC, Reimer JF. Feature binding in children and young adults. The Journal of genetic psychology. 2005;166:313–328. doi: 10.3200/GNTP.166.3.313-328. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Friedman WJ, Lyon T. D Development of Temporal‐Reconstructive Abilities. Child Development. 2005;76:1202–1216. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00844.x-i1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Guillery-Girard B, et al. Developmental trajectories of associative memory from childhood to adulthood: a behavioral and neuroimaging study. Frontiers in behavioral neuroscience. 2013;7:126. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00126. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Ghetti S, Lee J. Children’s episodic memory. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science. 2011;2:365–373. doi: 10.1002/wcs.114. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Lavenex P, Lavenex PB. Building hippocampal circuits to learn and remember: insights into the development of human memory. Behavioural brain research. 2013;254:8–21. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2013.02.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Jabès A, Nelson CA. 20 years after “The ontogeny of human memory: A cognitive neuroscience perspective,” where are we? International Journal of Behavioral Development. 2015;39:293-–303. doi: 10.1177/0165025415575766. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Blankeship SL, Redcay E, Dougherty LR, Riggins T. Development of hippocampal functional connectivity during childhood. Human Brain Mapping. 2016;38:182–201. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23353. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Olson, I.R., & Newcombe, N.S. Binding together the elements of episodes: Relational memory and the developmental trajectory of the hippocampus. The Wiley Handbook and the Development of Children’s Memory, Volume I/II. Patricia J. Bauer and Robyn Fivush (2014).
- 32.Gogtay N, et al. Dynamic mapping of normal human hippocampal development. Hippocampus. 2006;16:664–72. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20193. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.DeMaster DM, Pathman T, Ghetti S. Development of memory for spatial context: hippocampal and cortical contributions. Neuropsychologia. 2013;51:2415–2425. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.05.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Hayne H, Imuta K. Episodic memory in 3- and 4-year-old children. Developmental Psychobiology. 2011;53:317–322. doi: 10.1002/dev.20527. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Lee JK, Wendelken C, Bunge SA, Ghetti S. A time and place for everything: developmental differences in the building blocks of episodic memory. Child development. 2016;87:194–210. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12447. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Russell J, Thompson D. Memory development in the second year: for events or locations? Cognition. 2003;87:97–105. doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(02)00238-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Newcombe NS, Balcomb F, Ferrara K, Hansen M, Koski J. Two rooms, two representations? Episodic-like memory in toddlers and preschoolers. Developmental Science. 2014;17:743–756. doi: 10.1111/desc.12162. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Kessels RPC, Van Zandvoort MJE, Postma A, Kappelle LJ, De Haan EHF. The Corsi Block-Tapping Task: Standardization and normative data. Applied Neuropsychology. 2000;7:252–258. doi: 10.1207/S15324826AN0704_8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Farran EK, Blades M, Boucher J, Tranter LJ. How do individuals with Williams syndrome learn a route in a real-world environment? Dev Sci. 2010;13:454–468. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00894.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Nardini M, Atkinson J, Braddick O, Burgess N. Developmental trajectories for spatial frames of reference in Williams syndrome. Dev Sci. 2008;11:583–595. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00662.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Bellugi U, Lichtenberger L, Jones W, Lai Z, George MS. I. The Neurocognitive Profile of Williams Syndrome: A Complex Pattern of Strengths and Weaknesses. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2000;12:7–29. doi: 10.1162/089892900561959. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Udwin O, Yule W. A cognitive and behavioral phenotype in Williams syndrome. Journal of clinical and experimental Neuropsychology. 1991;13:232–244. doi: 10.1080/01688639108401040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Wang PP, Bellugi U. Evidence from two genetic syndromes for a dissociation between verbal and visual-spatial short-term memory. Journal of clinical and experimental Neuropsychology. 1994;16:317–322. doi: 10.1080/01688639408402641. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Edgin JO, Pennington BF, Mervis CB. Neuropsychological components of intellectual disability: the contributions of immediate, working, and associative memory. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research. 2010;54:406–417. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.2010.01278.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Vicari S, Brizzolara D, Carlesimo GA, Pezzini G, Volterra V. Memory abilities in children with Williams syndrome. Cortex. 1996;32:503–514. doi: 10.1016/S0010-9452(96)80007-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Lakusta L, Dessalegn B, Landau B. Impaired geometric reorientation caused by genetic defect. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2010;107:2813–2817. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0909155107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Costanzo F, et al. Executive functions in intellectual disabilities: A comparison between Williams syndrome and Down syndrome. Research in Developmental Disabilities. 2013;34:1770–1780. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2013.01.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Edgin JO, Pennington BF, Mervis CB. Neuropsychological components of intellectual disability: the contributions of immediate, working, and associative memory. Journal of intellectual disability research. 2010;54:406–417. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.2010.01278.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Lloyd ME, Doydum AO, Newcombe NS. Memory binding in early childhood: Evidence for a retrieval deficit. Child Development. 2009;80:321–1328. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01353.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Komorowski RW, Manns JR, Eichenbaum H. Robust conjunctive item-place coding by hippocampal neurons parallels learning what happens where. The Journal of neuroscience: the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 2009;29:9918–29. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1378-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Lee SA, Spelke ES. Two systems of spatial representation underlying navigation. Experimental Brain Research. 2010;206:179–188. doi: 10.1007/s00221-010-2349-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Lambert FR, Lavenex P, Lavenex PB. Improvement of allocentric spatial memory resolution in children from 2 to 4 years of age. International Journal of Behavioral Development. 2015;39:318–331. doi: 10.1177/0165025415584808. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Ribordy F, Jabe’s A, Lavenex PB, Lavenex P. Development of allocentric spatial memory abilities in children from 18 months to 5 years of age. Cognitive Psychology. 2013;66:1–29. doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2012.08.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Hindy NC, Ng FY, Turk-Browne NB. Linking pattern completion in the hippocampus to predictive coding in visual cortex. Nature neuroscience. 2016;19:665–667. doi: 10.1038/nn.4284. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Jabès A, Lavenex PB, Amaral DG, Lavenex P. Quantitative analysis of postnatal neurogenesis and neuron number in the macaque monkey dentate gyrus. Eur J Neurosci. 2010;31:273–85. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.07061.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Clelland CD, et al. A functional role for adult hippocampal neurogenesis in spatial pattern separation. Science. 2009;325:210–3. doi: 10.1126/science.1173215. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.DeMaster DM, Ghetti S. Developmental differences in hippocampal and cortical contributions to episodic retrieval. Cortex. 2012;49:1482–93. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2012.08.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Maguire EA, et al. Navigation-related structural change in the hippocampi of taxi drivers. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2000;97:4398–403. doi: 10.1073/pnas.070039597. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Naya Y, Chen H, Yang C, Suzuki WA. Contributions of primate prefrontal cortex and medial temporal lobe to temporal-order memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2017;114:13555–13560. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1712711114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Ferrara K, Landau B. Geometric and featural systems, separable and combined: Evidence from reorientation in people with Williams syndrome. Cognition. 2015;144:123–33. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2015.07.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Battaglia FP, Benchenane K, Sirota A, Pennartz CM, Wiener SI. The hippocampus: hub of brain network communication for memory. Trends Cogn Sci. 2011;15:310–8. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.05.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Mullally SL. Maguire, & E.A. Memory, Imagination, and Predicting the Future: A Common Brain Mechanism? The Neuroscientist. 2013;20:220–234. doi: 10.1177/1073858413495091. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Bellmund JLS, Gärdenfors P, Moser EI, Doeller CF. Navigating cognition: Spatial codes for human thinking. Science. 2018;362:eaat6766. doi: 10.1126/science.aat6766. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The datasets generated during and/or analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.