Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2021 Feb 1.
Published in final edited form as: J Behav Med. 2019 Jun 8;43(1):34–43. doi: 10.1007/s10865-019-00064-x

HIV microaggressions: A novel measure of stigma-related experiences among people living with HIV

Lisa A Eaton 1, Aerielle Allen 1, Jessica L Maksut 1, Valerie Earnshaw 2, Ryan J Watson 1, Seth C Kalichman 1
PMCID: PMC6899209  NIHMSID: NIHMS1531409  PMID: 31177373

Abstract

Since the beginning of the HIV epidemic stigma has served as a strong barrier to effectively delivering HIV prevention and treatment. Due in part to its complex nature, stigma is difficult to address and novel methods of understanding stigma are needed. Based on formative and empirical research with N=236 primarily Black men living with HIV, a HIV microaggressions scale was developed and evaluated in order to assess this component of stigma. Factor analysis resulted in a 13-item scale (α=.83) with 3 subscales explaining 51% of the total variance. The microaggressions scale demonstrated convergent validity (with internalized, enacted, and anticipated stigmas) and discriminant validity (with social support). HIV microaggressions was associated with longer gaps since last care appointment and depressive symptoms, and subscales were associated with barriers to accessing health care, disclosure, and HIV care self-efficacy. The HIV microaggressions scale is a novel tool for assessing a critical subcomponent of stigma.

Keywords: HIV microaggressions, stigma, scale development, health care


Oppression in all of its forms affects the health and well-being of individuals. There exist well-documented disparities in a variety of physical and mental health outcomes between oppressed persons and their non-oppressed counterparts (Babyar, 2018). Minority stress theories consider the ways in which stress and oppression are experienced among persons with marginalized identities, and how these factors are related to health outcomes and health disparities (Friedman, Williams, Singer, & Ryff, 2009; Hendricks & Testa, 2012; Meyer, 2003b). Prior studies suggest that minority-based stressors may affect minority individuals’ health through biological stress mechanisms leading to psychological distress, and/or by influencing individuals’ health behaviors and their use (or non-use) of health services (Balsam, Molina, Beadnell, Simoni, & Walters, 2011; Hausmann, Jeong, Bost, & Ibrahim, 2008; Krieger et al., 2008; Meyer, 2003a).

Much of the literature to date has focused on the impacts of oppression on minority persons in the form of macroaggressions, or major discriminatory experiences. Recently, however, literature examining the physical and mental health impacts of oppression in the form of microaggressions, or subtle forms of discrimination that occur in daily life, has expanded substantially (Sue et al., 2007). Much of this work has stemmed from the need to recognize both acute and chronic stigmas (Williams, Neighbors, & Jackson, 2003). Although not a conceptual framework, different types of microaggressions have been identified; microassaults (e.g., name-calling, shunning, purposeful discrimination), microinsults (e.g., implications of negativity, rudeness), and microinvalidations (e.g., minimizing or ignoring target individuals’ feelings and statements, denying oppression) (Lilienfeld, 2017). Studies show that microaggressions can have a multitude of negative consequences for individuals among marginalized, stigmatized groups, including racial/ethnic, gender, and sexual orientation minorities. To date, studies of microaggressions have focused primarily on race-based and sexual orientation-based microaggressions. Documented consequences of these types of microaggressions include emotional, psychological, and physiological distress, and substance abuse and substance use disorders (Blume, Lovato, Thyken, & Denny, 2012; Nadal, Whitman, Davis, Erazo, & Davidoff, 2016; Smith, Rossetto, & Peterson, 2008; Torres & Driscoll, 2010).

While the literature regarding race- and sexual orientation-based microaggressions is growing, relatively little is known about other types of microaggressions. For example, race and sexual orientation minority persons who are living HIV may be subject to both macroaggressions and microaggressions directed toward their race and/or sexual orientation minority status(es), in addition to macroaggressions and microaggressions directed toward their HIV status. Furthermore, prior studies that have considered how race, sexual orientation, and HIV status-related oppression affects people living with HIV have focused on stigma in the form of macroaggressive experiences. These types of macroaggressions have been shown to discourage disclosure of one’s status, seeking and establishing social support networks, and adherence to HIV medications among people living with HIV (Sayles, Wong, Kinsler, Martins, & Cunningham, 2009; Smith et al., 2008).

While research has delineated pathways between macroaggressions and various health outcomes, additional work to better understand whether and to what extent people living with HIV experience HIV-based microaggressions, and whether HIV-based microaggressions impede health seeking behaviors among this population, is needed. There exist validated measures of microaggressions toward persons of color (i.e., the Racial Microaggressions Scale, the Racial and Ethnic Microaggressions Scale, Inventory of Microaggressions Against Black Individuals), (Mercer, Zeigler-Hill, Wallace, & Hayes, 2011; Nadal et al., 2016; Torres-Harding, Andrade, & Romero Diaz, 2012), as well as measures of microaggressions toward lesbian, gay, bisexual and trans individuals of color (i.e., the LGBT People of Color Microaggressions Scale) (Hausmann et al., 2008), to date there exists no formalized measure of HIV-based microaggressions.

The literature specific to HIV stigma related measures is extensive and largely focuses on stereotypes, prejudice, discrimination, internalized stigma, enacted stigma, and anticipated stigma (Earnshaw & Chaudoir, 2009; Earnshaw, Smith, Chaudoir, Amico, & Copenhaver, 2013). Further review of this literature, however, demonstrates that much of the work in the area of stigma measurement has focused on experiences of internalized stigma among people living with HIV, and examining stereotyping among the general population towards people living with HIV. Other areas of stigma are relatively under-represented in the literature. Moreover, HIV microaggressions are a form of enacted stigma, yet enacted stigma in particular has been overlooked in the literature (Nyblade, 2006). When enacted stigma is measured it is typically infrequently reported relative to other measures (Earnshaw et al., 2013), however, measures of enacted stigma tend to focus on major events (e.g., job loss, housing loss, or relationship loss as a result of living with HIV). A scale focused on HIV microaggression fills this overlooked area of enacted stigma.

Previous scholarship has documented a link between increased levels of alcohol use and depression to experiences of stigma and macro/microaggressions. For example, Grov et al. 2010 found that a significant portion of depression among HIV-positive men and women was attributable to HIV/AIDS-related stigma, which included personalized stigma and concerns of aggressions/negative reactions to one’s HIV status. Specific to microaggressions, Blume et al. 2012 found that among a sample of ethnic minority college students, race/ethnic microaggressions predicted more binge drinking and associated alcohol-related consequences for students of color in particular. Given previous associations between stigma and macro/microaggressions among minority populations, we perform preliminary analyses to explore the relationship between the microaggression measure and several health outcomes to demonstrate the potential utility of this new measure that investigates HIV-specific microaggressions among black men living with HIV.

Study Objectives.

Prior research suggests that microaggressions affect the health of people with HIV, yet few studies have been able to systematically measure this association. Given the state of the available research, the present study’s objectives were four-fold. The first objective was to develop items for a measure of HIV-based microaggressions among a sample of primarily black men living with HIV, which included a principal components analysis (PCA). The second objective was to test the initial reliability of the measure. The third objective was to test the validity of the measure by exploring correlations between the measure and measures of internalized, anticipated, and enacted stigmas, as well as social support. The fourth objective was to explore relationships between the HIV-based microaggression measure and multiple HIV-related health outcomes, including HIV health care behaviors and status disclosure. The final objective was determine the extent to which microaggression was associated with problematic alcohol use.

METHODS

Initial scale development procedures.

Four focus groups (N=7–8 per group) and individual interviews (N=17) (Guest, Namey, Taylor, Eley, & McKenna, 2017) were convened with black men living with HIV from the Atlanta, GA area in February 2017. Participants were recruited from LGBT focused online and in-person venues. Participants were provided with an overview of the designated topic (subtle experiences of mistreatment related to living with HIV), an overview of the format of the discussion, and for focus groups, general ground rules for maintaining respect/privacy. All participants provided written informed consent and were compensated $50 for participation. Participants were encouraged to discuss their personal and observed experiences with HIV stigma microaggressions. The first and last authors developed the guide for the focus groups and semi-structured individual interviews, and therefore, content areas were kept consistent across data collection methods. Focus groups were conducted by the first and last authors, and research staff conducted individual interviews which were coded for content and then evaluated by the first author. Based on memos created from data collected from the groups and individual interviews, a preliminary scale was established. The preliminary scale was reviewed and discussed by LAE and SCK based on interpretation of the qualitative data. Twenty-five items that reflected microaggressive experiences were generated from the focus group and individual interview data. From these items, 11 were removed due to ambiguity or redundancy leaving a total of 14 items for the scale. Item development and elimination were directed by the first and last author. Item response set for all items included 0-Never, 1-Rarely, 2-Sometimes, and 3-Often, and items were asked with a 3 month timeframe.

Scale testing procedures.

Participants were 189 men residing in and around Atlanta, GA, recruited at LGBT venues (e.g., bars/clubs/parties) and LGBT online sites (e.g., dating sites and apps), and over the phone through fliers and participant referrals. Study activities occurred between September 2017 and July 2018. Participants provided written informed consent and all procedures were approved by an Institution Review Board. Study entry criteria included: 16 years of age or older, assigned male gender at birth and identified as male, and living with HIV. Scale administration occurred during the baseline assessment of a larger, ongoing behavioral intervention trial to improve access to health care for people with HIV. Study procedures also included providing medical documentation of HIV positive status or administration of a rapid HIV self-test (OraQuick HIV 1/2 antibody test) to confirm HIV status, and completing an Audio Computer-Assisted Self-Interview (ACASI).

Measures

Demographic and HIV care characteristics.

Participants reported on age, race/ethnicity, gender identity, sexual orientation, relationship status, education, and income. Participants also reported on the date of their most recent HIV care appointment (analyzed as months since last appointment) and whether they knew their most recent viral load (yes/no).

Anticipated and enacted stigmas.

Participants were administered a battery of 24 HIV stigma related items adapted from Earnshaw et al. 2013 (e.g., mistreated by healthcare providers, made fun of or insulted, rejected by sex partners, lost friends, lost housing, etc., due to living with HIV). Participants were asked how often they experienced these stigmas (enacted stigma). Response set included never, rarely, sometimes, and often. Next, participants were asked to report on if they believed that experience would occur in the future (anticipated stigma). Example item included, “How likely is it that you will lose friends in the future for telling them you have HIV?” All items were based on a 6-point Likert scale (1=Strongly Disagree to 6=Strongly Agree). Items were averaged across the enacted stigma items (Cronbach’s α = .93) and the anticipated stigma items1 forming two separate scales.

Internalized stigma.

Participants reported on their agreement with internalized HIV stigma items (Earnshaw et al., 2013). Six items were used to assess this construct. Example items included “Having HIV makes me feel like I’m a bad person” and “I feel ashamed of having HIV”. All items were based on a 6-point Likert scale (1=Strongly Disagree to 6=Strongly Agree). The scale demonstrated good internal reliability (Cronbach’s α = .89).

Social support.

Participants reported on their experiences with social support. Fourteen items were used to assess this construct. Example items included, “When I need suggestions on how to deal with a personal problem, I know someone I can turn to” and “I feel a strong emotional bond with at least one other person” (Cutrona & Russell, 1987). Response set ranged from 1=completely true to 4=completely false, and responses were recoded such that higher scores indicated higher levels of social support. The scale demonstrated good internal reliability (Cronbach’s α = .91).

Alcohol use disorders identification test (AUDIT).

The AUDIT is a 10-item questionnaire designed to assess hazardous and harmful alcohol use (Saunders, Aasland, Babor, de la Fuente, & Grant, 1993). Items are focused on experiences with alcohol use from the past year. Scores of an 8 or higher on the AUDIT signal the need for a more in-depth alcohol assessment. Response sets include value ranges of 0–4 with answers varying based on question. Total scores range from 0–40.

Depressive symptoms.

The Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale (CES-D) for assessing self-reported depressive symptomology consists of 20 items (Radloss, 1977). Items focus on depressive symptoms from the prior week. Scores of 20 or higher indicate the need for further assessment of mental health. Responses range from 0–3, with a possible total score range of 0–60 (Cronbach’s α = .82).

Barriers to accessing health care.

Four items were asked to assess common barriers to attending HIV care appointments. Items included, “How hard is it for you to pay for transportation to go to a doctor or clinic for HIV care?” and “How hard is it to understand the information you are given at the doctor’s office or clinic about HIV (such as information from staff, pamphlets, or posters)?” Response set included 0=very easy to 4=very hard. Responses were averaged over all items, and the scale demonstrated good reliability (Cronbach’s α=.80).

HIV Disclosure.

Participants were asked if they had told some, none, or all of the following groups of people: sex partners, parents, siblings, children, relatives, close friends, casual friends, healthcare providers, and employers, about their living with HIV. For groups of people were who were not applicable, these items were not included in the participants’ average. The scale demonstrated good reliability (Cronbach’s α=.85).

HIV care self-efficacy.

Five items were asked in order to assess their self-efficacy for maintaining HIV care related behaviors (adapted from Johnson et al., 2007). Items included, “How certain are you that you can follow all of your HIV doctor’s orders?” and “How certain are you that you can keep your next HIV doctor’s appointment?”. Response set included an 11 point scale ranging from 0=not certain at all to 10=very certain. The scale demonstrated good reliability Cronbach’s α=.78.

Data Analyses

Primary analyses were conducted using IBM SPSS version 25. The overall sample included 189 male participants (see Table 1, participants who self-identified as transwomen [n = 4] and participants with incomplete data [n=2] were excluded from analyses). Bivariate correlations were conducted and results demonstrated that while the items were significantly correlated overall, the values did not exceed r = .80, suggesting no multicollinearity. A principal components analysis and tests of multiple validities were conducted.

Table 1.

Sociodemographics of men living with HIV in the Atlanta, GA and surrounding areas.

N %

Education
 ≤ High School 74 39.2
 > High School 115 60.8

Race/ethnicity
 Black 182 96.3
 Other/Non-specified 7 3.7

Sexual Orientation
 Heterosexual 10 5.3
 Gay/Same gender loving 161 85.0
 Bisexual 18 9.5

Income
 ≤ $20,000 112 59.3
 > $20,000 77 40.7

Pay for healthcare
 No coverage 17 9.0
 Private insurance 47 24.9
 Public insurance 107 56.6
 Other 18 9.5

Without healthcare in past two years 80 42.3

Seen a provider for HIV care in past 4 months 165 87.3

Employed 123 65.1

AUDIT (8 or higher) 32 16.9

Depression (16 or higher) 93 49.2

M SD

Years since diagnosis 7.01 4.51

Internalized stigma 2.54 1.49

Anticipated stigma 3.10 1.42

Enacted stigma .39

Social support 2.28 .33

Barriers to accessing healthcare 1.24 .98

HIV status disclosure 1.11 .45

HIV care self-efficacy 8.67 1.61

Note: Enacted stigma ranged from 1 (never) to 4 (often). Internalized stigma and anticipated stigma ranged from 1 (strongly disagree) to 6 (strongly agree). Social support ranged from 1 (completely true) to 4 (completely false). Barriers to accessing health care ranged from 0 (very easy) to 4 (very hard). HIV status disclosure ranged from 1 (none) to 3 (all). HIV care self-efficacy ranged from 0 (not certain at all) to 10 (very certain). N=number, M=mean, SD=standard deviation.

RESULTS

Initial tests of factorability.

Prior to running factor analyses, Pearson’s correlation coefficients were examined. It was observed that of the 14 items, 11 correlated >.3 with one or more other items suggesting reasonable factorability (Tabachnik & Fidell, 2001) (see Table 2 for correlations, see Table 3 for each item description). Second, the Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin measure of sampling adequacy was .84, above the commonly recommended value of .6, and Bartlett’s test of sphericity was significant (χ2 (91) = 692.23, p < .05). These tests indicate the suitability of the data for structure detection (Tabachnik & Fidell, 2001). Lastly, the communalities of 13 items were above .3 (see Table 2), further confirming that close to all of the items shared some common variance with other items. Although, item 5 did not have a communality above .3, it was significantly correlated with most of the other microaggression items. Given these overall indicators, factor analysis was deemed to be suitable with all 14 items.

Table 2.

Summary of correlations, means, and standard deviations for scores of the HIV microaggression items

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14
1. You heard someone say, “I’m HIV negative, I’m clean.” -
2. You avoided circumstances where people might ask questions about your health. .23** -
3. You avoided reading the comments section on online articles about HIV. .01 .43** -
4. You saw an HIV positive person portrayed negatively in the media. .24** .19** .14* -
5. You heard about someone being outed about their HIV status. .35** .21** .16* .45** -
6. You heard someone say, “but he or she doesn’t look HIV positive.” .35** .15* .01 .38** .60** -
7. Someone assumed you must be depressed because of your HIV status. .13 .22** .14 .26** .30** .35** -
8. Someone assumed you don’t or shouldn’t have sex because of your HIV status. .23** .20** .17* .30** .37** .46** .47** -
9. Someone’s body language showed you that they were bothered because of your HIV status. .18* .18* .21** .25** .33** .36** .53** .55** -
10. You weren’t included in a group event because of your HIV status. .01 .13 .23** .23** .29** .20** .30** .21** .44** -
11. You felt that other health conditions (such as breast cancer or heart disease) received more public support than HIV. .04 .20** .15* .26** .34** .30** .26** .36** .40** .20** -
12. You heard about people trying to make or enforce laws that harm people with HIV. .16* .07 .13 .31** .28** .26** .18* .27** .20** .16* .26** -
13. In an online dating profile, someone wrote “drug/disease free, UB2” or neg for neg only,” etc. .25** .21** .03 .30** .37** .34** .22** .23** .24** .13 .22** .26** -
14. Someone seemed surprised to learn that people living with HIV would want to have children. .20** .22** .09 .42** .36** .46** .39** .43** .40** .28** .36** .31** .31** -
M 2.44 1.56 .91 1.75 2.15 2.38 1.38 1.49 1.26 .42 1.62 1.40 2.50 1.80
SD .86 1.09 1.04 1.02 .92 .85 1.18 1.17 1.17 .84 1.23 1.16 .90 1.10

Note.

**

. Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed).

*

. Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (2-tailed).

M=mean, SD=standard deviation.

Table 3.

Items, component loadings, item means, standard deviations, and communalities for HIV microaggressions

Item Factor loadings Scale if items are deleted Factor Item parameters
1 2 3 All 1 2 3 M SD h2
1. Someone assumed you must be depressed because of your HIV status. .71 .12 .37 .79 .69 - - 1 1.38 1.18 .53
2. Someone assumed you don’t or shouldn’t have sex because of your HIV status. .69 .09 .46 .78 .69 - - 1 1.49 1.17 .66
3. Someone’s body language showed you that they were bothered because of your HIV status. .81 .16 .30 .78 .64 - - 1 1.26 1.17 .43
4. You weren’t included in a group event because of your HIV status. .63 .23 .09 .80 .75 - - 1 .42 .84 .37
5. You felt that other health conditions (such as breast cancer or heart disease) received more public support than HIV. .60 .14 .29 .80 .75 - - 1 1.62 1.22 .23
6. You avoided circumstances where people might ask questions about your health. .19 .81 .31 .80 - n/a - 2 1.56 1.09 .72
7. You avoided reading the comments section on online articles about HIV. .27 .84 .03 .81 - n/a - 2 .91 1.04 .74
8. You heard about people trying to make or enforce laws that harm people with HIV. .35 .03 .44 .81 - - .73 3 1.40 1.16 .35
9. In an online dating profile, someone wrote “drug/disease free, UB2” or neg for neg only,” etc. .24 .09 .59 .80 - - .69 3 2.50 .90 .51
10. Someone seemed surprised to learn that people living with HIV would want to have children. .59 .07 .58 n/a n/a n/a n/a n/a 1.80 1.09 .51
11. You heard someone say, “I’m HIV negative, I’m clean.” .01 .13 .67 .81 - - .70 3 2.44 .86 .52
12. You saw an HIV positive person portrayed negatively in the media. .37 .16 .63 .79 - - .67 3 1.75 1.02 .42
13. You heard about someone being outed about their HIV status. .46 .14 .71 .79 - - .64 3 2.15 .92 .61
14. You heard someone say, “but he or she doesn’t look HIV positive.” .49 -.10 .72 .79 - - .66 3 2.38 .85 .51

Note. Values in boldface type are component loadings at or above the criteria for selection (.40). Item 14 was deleted due to double loading.

Factor analysis.

A principal components factor analysis with an oblimin rotation was used because the primary purpose was to identify and determine whether these items loaded on to one factor or consisted of multiple underlying factors (Table 3). The first three factors cumulatively explained approximately 52% of the variance. The first factor explained 31.67% of the variance, the second factor explained 10.49% of the variance resulting in 42.16% of cumulative variance explained, and the third factor explained 9.35% of the variance resulting in 51.51% of cumulative variance explained. All other factors were below the eigenvalue cutoff of 1.00. All items significantly contributed to the factor structure and had a primary factor loading of .4 or above with a factor. A few items demonstrated cross-loadings, loading onto more than one factor above .4. For this reason, we grouped items based on loadings above .5 (Larcker, 1981). For item 10, loadings were above .50 for two factors (and did not vary substantially from each other), indicating a lack of distinction in the factor structure; item 10 was, subsequently, deleted. We evaluated our factor loading structure as fair (.45), good (.55), very good (.63) and excellent (.71) (Tabachnik & Fidell, 2001).

Internal consistency for all factors.

Internal consistency for the overall microaggression scale and the three underlying factors was examined using Cronbach’s alpha. The overall alpha for the entire scale was high (α = .83) and the other alphas were moderate to high: .78 (Factor 1 – direct microaggressions; negative treatment towards individual living with HIV), .60 (Factor 2 – self-protection from microaggressions, avoiding circumstances where stigmatizing experiences are assumed to occur), .73 (Factor 3 – indirect microaggressions, negative treatment towards all people living with HIV). No substantial increases in alpha for any of the scales could have been achieved by eliminating items.

Composite scores were created for each of the three factors based on the mean of the items which had their primary loadings on each factor. An additional composite score was created based on the means of all the HIV microaggression items. Higher scores indicated greater experiences of HIV microaggression. Small to moderate correlations were observed between each of the composite scores: r=.30 between direct microaggressions (factor 1) and self-protection from microaggressions (factor 2); r=.55 between direct microaggressions and indirect microaggressions (factor 3); and r=.25 between self-protection from microaggressions and indirect microaggressions.

Scale validities.

In order to examine convergent and discriminate validity we correlated the microaggression composites with composite scores created from the enacted stigma, anticipated stigma, internalized stigma, and social support measures. Social support was assessed to determine discriminant validity as microaggressions and social support are theoretically different constructs and, therefore, considered separate areas of inquiry, yet potentially impactful on each other, making this construct fitting for discriminant validity (i.e., social support has been shown to serve as a buffer to the effects of stigmatizing experiences). Similar to the microaggression composites, these composites were based on the means of the items for each measure respectively. Results supported convergent validity among the microaggression items and the enacted, anticipated, and internalized stigma measures, showing significant positive correlations. In regards to social support, results demonstrated discriminate validity, showing no significant correlation with the HIV microaggressions scale overall, and significant negative correlations with two of the subscales (indirect microaggressions and self-protection from microaggressions) (Table 4).

Table 4.

Correlations between the microaggression scale and other psychosocial measures

Measures HIV Microaggression Scale Direct Microaggression Self-Protection from Microaggression Indirect Microaggression
HIV Microaggression -
Scale
Direct Microaggressions - -
Self-protection from - .30** -
Microaggressions
Indirect - .55** .25** -
Microaggressions
Stigma measures
 Internalized stigma .36** .31** .40** .21**
 Anticipated stigma .26** .23** .24** .13
 Enacted stigma .54** .61** .30** .32**
Social support −.12 −.15* −.29** .04
AUDIT .11 .09 −.02 .16*
Depression .22** .25** .23** .10
Barriers to accessing .13 .16* .24** −.01
health care
Disclosure .09 .20** −26** .08
HIV care self-efficacy −.14 −.16* −.23** −.05

Note.

**

. Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed).

*

. Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (2-tailed).

Psychosocial health related associations.

The microaggressions scale and subscales were also evaluated for their associations with depression, accessing HIV care, HIV status disclosure, and HIV care self-efficacy. The total microaggressions scale was found to be positively associated with depression. The direct microaggressions subscale was positively associated with depression, experiencing barriers to health care, and HIV status disclosure, and was negatively associated with HIV care self -efficacy. The self-protection subscale was positively associated with depression and barriers to accessing health care, and negatively associated with HIV status disclosure and HIV care self-efficacy. The indirect microaggressions subscale was positively associated with problematic alcohol use (Table 4).

HIV microaggressions and healthcare engagement.

Using generalized linear modeling, HIV microaggressions was significantly associated with months since last HIV care appointment, with greater endorsement of microaggressions being associated with greater amount of time since last visit (RR=1.33, 95%CI=1.09–1.83, p<.01).

DISCUSSION

For the current study, we present a new 13-item microaggression scale that we evaluated in order to understand how this form of stigma relates to other measures of stigma, and HIV health behaviors and beliefs. This measure allows for assessing stigmatizing experiences that have been overlooked by prior scales that primarily capture overt forms of stigma. Findings demonstrate a novel approach to understanding experiences of stigma among people living with HIV. Further, this measure can be used in the evaluation of stigma reduction interventions (Stangl, Lloyd, Brady, Holland, & Baral, 2013). Three distinct factors emerged from the principal components factor analysis. There are important nuances across each factor: microaggressions can be experienced as either direct or indirect, and people with HIV also report taking steps to protect themselves from these experiences. Subscales provide further insight into this new approach to understanding the complex nature of stigma.

Similar to prior literature (Berger, Ferrans, & Lashley, 2001), our HIV microaggressions scale demonstrated positive correlations with depression and other stigma measures, and negative correlations with social support (among subscales). These findings provide important insight into how microaggressions are related to other psychosocial measures. Further, it has been hypothesized that the burden of stigma on mental health is buffered by social support (Kondrat, Sullivan, Wilkins, Barrett, & Beerbower, 2017; Smith et al., 2008). These pathways may explain findings in the current paper and offer an explanation for how experiences of stigma result in varied outcomes (i.e., as a result of varied levels of social support) among individuals experiencing stigma (Stutterheim et al., 2011).

The HIV microaggressions scale was also both positively and negatively associated with health related and HIV specific behaviors. For example, indirect microaggressions was positively associated with alcohol use, a relationship that may exist through coping (Wardell, Shuper, Rourke, & Hendershot, 2018). Prior research proposes that addressing stigma could impact problematic alcohol use and is a novel avenue for intervention research in this area (Wardell et al., 2018). Interestingly, we conceptualize indirect microaggressions as a particularly subtle forms of stigma, which is likely exceedingly challenging to articulate as having experienced. The “invisible” nature of microaggressions makes them hard to identify, process, and address, but they can be equally, if not more, emotionally harmful as macroaggressions (Wing Sue, 2010). Likewise, and an extension of the current literature (Eaton et al., 2018), stigma subscales varied in their relationship to barriers to accessing health care, with direct and self-protection from microaggressions being associated with barriers. Considerable work has examined the affective states, typically stress, in making seemingly logic-based health care related decision making (Ferrer & Mendes, 2018). Experiencing HIV microaggressions likely contributes to emotional states that have an impact on health care decision making.

We note inverse relationships in the direction of correlations between direct microaggressions and HIV status disclosure (positive relationship), and self-protection from microaggressions and HIV status disclosure (negative relationship). Although we can’t be certain of the cause of the relationship, the direct microaggressions subscale includes items that assume the ‘stigmatizer’ is aware of the ‘targets’ HIV status, and therefore, in order for these items to be endorsed, an individual would likely have disclosed his/her status. This finding represents a unique area of microaggressions research where consequential outcomes of disclosure are identified. Likewise, self-protection from microaggressions conveys an avoidance of status disclosure, which is, of course, consistent with lower rates of disclosure, as was observed. Overstreet et al. 2013 noted related findings between disclosure and internalized stigma, however experiencing microaggressions as a result of disclosure is a novel area of research.

HIV microaggressions was associated with a longer gap in time since last HIV care appointment. Prior work has identified experiences of stigma as being associated with lack of accessing health care, however, much of this work has specifically focused on stigma from health care providers and/or stigma experienced in health care settings (Fay et al., 2011; Kinsler, Wong, Sayles, Davis, & Cunningham, 2007; Nyblade, Stangl, Weiss, & Ashburn, 2009). For the current paper, not only did we find microaggressions to be associated with health care gaps, but our measure of microaggressions does not focus on health care providers or settings, which suggests the possibility of a generalized impact of microaggressions. That is to say, experiencing microaggressions in one area (e.g., in online or in person social environments) may impact beliefs and behaviors in other areas (e.g., accessing health care).

Limitations.

Our research is cross-sectional and, therefore, causality can’t be inferred from the current data. Our sample primarily focused on individuals who identified as black males which limits generalizability of our findings. Further validation of the current scale needs to be conducted with individuals across genders, races/ethnicities, and sexual orientations. Likewise, our sample was recruited from individuals residing in the southeastern US, an area that may not be comparable to other US regions. In terms of scale development, one of our subscales consisted of two-items. There is some debate regarding the psychometrics of a two-item subscale, however, general consensus supports the use of two-item scales (Eisinga, Grotenhuis, & Pelzer, 2013) (in particular, when two-item scales are part of a larger more complex scale), and numerous studies have demonstrated the utility and reliability of these scales (e.g., Fick et al., 2015; Kroenke, Spitzer, Williams, & Lowe, 2009).

Findings from the current study expand the literature in multiple ways. Although we developed the HIV microaggression scale as a result of formative research specific to HIV related oppressive experiences among men living with HIV, the findings lay the groundwork for the development or adaptability of this scale for other oppressed and vulnerable populations. For example, other marginalized groups (e.g., gender minority individuals) may experience similar microaggressions (e.g., fear of being outed or being aware of laws to oppress a certain groups), and population-specific microaggression scales may be key in explaining health disparities. We also highlight the need to understand potential burdens experienced by people living with HIV that are not immediately apparent or that are difficult to articulate due to their subtle nature. Further, a critical area of future research would include a focus on the impact of intersectionality on interpreting experiences of microaggressions. Finally, it is known that stigma is a complex phenomenon and developing measures that aim to capture this complexity provides a roadmap towards addressing and ending stigma as a barrier to HIV prevention and treatment.

Acknowledgments

Research was funded by NIH grants T32MH074387, R01DA043068, and K01DA042881. This research involved Human Subjects and was conducted with the approval of the University of Connecticut Institutional Review Board (Protocol #16–130MER) and research was conducted in accordance with the ethical standards as laid down in the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Footnotes

The authors have no conflicts of interest to report.

Publisher's Disclaimer: This Author Accepted Manuscript is a PDF file of an unedited peer-reviewed manuscript that has been accepted for publication but has not been copyedited or corrected. The official version of record that is published in the journal is kept up to date and so may therefore differ from this version.

1

Because the anticipated stigma items were only asked if the participants had previously experienced the stigma (enacted stigma), reliability analyses could not be conducted.

REFERENCES

  1. Babyar J (2018). Equitable health: let’s stick together as we address global discrimination, prejudice and stigma. Arch Public Health, 76, 44. doi: 10.1186/s13690-018-0291-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Balsam KF, Molina Y, Beadnell B, Simoni J, & Walters K (2011). Measuring multiple minority stress: the LGBT People of Color Microaggressions Scale. Cultur Divers Ethnic Minor Psychol, 17(2), 163–174. doi: 10.1037/a0023244 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Berger BE, Ferrans CE, & Lashley FR (2001). Measuring stigma in people with HIV: psychometric assessment of the HIV stigma scale. Res Nurs Health, 24(6), 518–529. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Blume AW, Lovato LV, Thyken BN, & Denny N (2012). The relationship of microaggressions with alcohol use and anxiety among ethnic minority college students in a historically White institution. Cultur Divers Ethnic Minor Psychol, 18(1), 45–54. doi: 10.1037/a0025457 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. C, F., & Larcker D (1981). Evaluating Structural Equation Models with Unobsevable Variables and Measurement Error. Journal of Marketing Research, 18(1), 39–50. [Google Scholar]
  6. Cutrona C, & Russell D (1987). The provisions of social relationships and adaptation to stress. Advances in Personal Relationships, 1, 37–67. [Google Scholar]
  7. Earnshaw VA, & Chaudoir SR (2009). From conceptualizing to measuring HIV stigma: a review of HIV stigma mechanism measures. AIDS Behav, 13(6), 1160–1177. doi: 10.1007/s10461-009-9593-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Earnshaw VA, Smith LR, Chaudoir SR, Amico KR, & Copenhaver MM (2013). HIV stigma mechanisms and well-being among PLWH: a test of the HIV stigma framework. AIDS Behav, 17(5), 1785–1795. doi: 10.1007/s10461-013-0437-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Eaton LA, Earnshaw VA, Maksut JL, Thorson KR, Watson RJ, & Bauermeister JA (2018). Experiences of stigma and health care engagement among Black MSM newly diagnosed with HIV/STI. J Behav Med, 41(4), 458–466. doi: 10.1007/s10865-018-9922-y [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Eisinga R, Grotenhuis M, & Pelzer B (2013). The reliability of a two-item scale: Pearson, Cronbach, or Spearman-Brown? Int J Public Health, 58(4), 637–642. doi: 10.1007/s00038-012-0416-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Fay H, Baral SD, Trapence G, Motimedi F, Umar E, Iipinge S, … Beyrer C (2011). Stigma, health care access, and HIV knowledge among men who have sex with men in Malawi, Namibia, and Botswana. AIDS Behav, 15(6), 1088–1097. doi: 10.1007/s10461-010-9861-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Ferrer RA, & Mendes WB (2018). Emotion, health decision making, and health behaviour. Psychol Health, 33(1), 1–16. doi: 10.1080/08870446.2017.1385787 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Fick DM, Inouye SK, Guess J, Ngo LH, Jones RN, Saczynski JS, & Marcantonio ER (2015). Preliminary development of an ultrabrief two-item bedside test for delirium. J Hosp Med, 10(10), 645–650. doi: 10.1002/jhm.2418 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Friedman EM, Williams DR, Singer BH, & Ryff CD (2009). Chronic discrimination predicts higher circulating levels of E-selectin in a national sample: the MIDUS study. Brain Behav Immun, 23(5), 684–692. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2009.01.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Grov C, Golub SA, Parsons JT, Brennan M, & Karpiak SE (2010). Loneliness and HIV-related stigma explain depression among older HIV-positive adults. AIDS Care, 22(5), 630–639. doi: 10.1080/09540120903280901 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Guest G, Namey E, Taylor J, Eley N, & McKenna K (2017). Comparing focus groups and individual interviews: findings from a randomized study. International Journal of Social Research Methodology, 20(6), 693–708. [Google Scholar]
  17. Hausmann LR, Jeong K, Bost JE, & Ibrahim SA (2008). Perceived discrimination in health care and health status in a racially diverse sample. Med Care, 46(9), 905–914. doi: 10.1097/MLR.0b013e3181792562 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Hendricks M, & Testa R (2012). A conceptual framework for clinical work with transgender and gender nonconforming clients: An adaptation of the Minority Stress Model. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 43(5), 460. [Google Scholar]
  19. Johnson MO, Neilands TB, Dilworth SE, Morin SF, Remien RH, & Chesney MA (2007). The role of self-efficacy in HIV treatment adherence: validation of the HIV Treatment Adherence Self-Efficacy Scale (HIV-ASES). J Behav Med, 30(5), 359–370. doi: 10.1007/s10865-007-9118-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Kinsler JJ, Wong MD, Sayles JN, Davis C, & Cunningham WE (2007). The effect of perceived stigma from a health care provider on access to care among a low-income HIV-positive population. AIDS Patient Care STDS, 21(8), 584–592. doi: 10.1089/apc.2006.0202 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Kondrat DC, Sullivan WP, Wilkins B, Barrett BJ, & Beerbower E (2017). The Mediating Effect of Social Support on the Relationship Between the Impact of Experienced Stigma and Mental Health. . Stigma and Health
  22. Krieger N, Chen JT, Waterman PD, Hartman C, Stoddard AM, Quinn MM, … Barbeau EM (2008). The inverse hazard law: blood pressure, sexual harassment, racial discrimination, workplace abuse and occupational exposures in US low-income black, white and Latino workers. Soc Sci Med, 67(12), 1970–1981. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2008.09.039 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Kroenke K, Spitzer RL, Williams JB, & Lowe B (2009). An ultra-brief screening scale for anxiety and depression: the PHQ-4. Psychosomatics, 50(6), 613–621. doi: 10.1176/appi.psy.50.6.613 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Lilienfeld S (2017). Microaggressions: Strong claims, inadequate evidence. Perspectives on psychological science, 12(1), 138–169. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Mercer SH, Zeigler-Hill V, Wallace M, & Hayes DM (2011). Development and initial validation of the inventory of Microaggressions Against Black Individuals. J Couns Psychol, 58(4), 457–469. doi: 10.1037/a0024937 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Meyer IH (2003a). Prejudice, social stress, and mental health in lesbian, gay, and bisexual populations: conceptual issues and research evidence. Psychol Bull, 129(5), 674–697. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.129.5.674 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Meyer IH (2003b). Prejudice, social stress, and mental health in lesbian, gay, and bisexual populations: conceptual issues and research evidence. Psychol Bull, 129(5), 674. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Nadal KL, Whitman CN, Davis LS, Erazo T, & Davidoff KC (2016). Microaggressions Toward Lesbian, Gay, Bisexual, Transgender, Queer, and Genderqueer People: A Review of the Literature. J Sex Res, 53(4–5), 488–508. doi: 10.1080/00224499.2016.1142495 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Nyblade L, Stangl A, Weiss E, & Ashburn K (2009). Combating HIV stigma in health care settings: what works? J Int AIDS Soc, 12, 15. doi: 10.1186/1758-2652-12-15 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Nyblade LC (2006). Measuring HIV stigma: existing knowledge and gaps. Psychol Health Med, 11(3), 335–345. doi: 10.1080/13548500600595178 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Overstreet NM, Earnshaw VA, Kalichman SC, & Quinn DM (2013). Internalized stigma and HIV status disclosure among HIV-positive black men who have sex with men. AIDS Care, 25(4), 466–471. doi: 10.1080/09540121.2012.720362 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Radloss L (1977). CES-D scale: A self report depression scale for research in the general populations. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1, 385–401. [Google Scholar]
  33. Saunders JB, Aasland OG, Babor TF, de la Fuente JR, & Grant M (1993). Development of the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT): WHO Collaborative Project on Early Detection of Persons with Harmful Alcohol Consumption--II. Addiction, 88(6), 791–804. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Sayles JN, Wong MD, Kinsler JJ, Martins D, & Cunningham WE (2009). The association of stigma with self-reported access to medical care and antiretroviral therapy adherence in persons living with HIV/AIDS. J Gen Intern Med, 24(10), 1101–1108. doi: 10.1007/s11606-009-1068-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Smith R, Rossetto K, & Peterson BL (2008). A meta-analysis of disclosure of one’s HIV-positive status, stigma and social support. AIDS Care, 20(10), 1266–1275. doi: 10.1080/09540120801926977 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Stangl AL, Lloyd JK, Brady LM, Holland CE, & Baral S (2013). A systematic review of interventions to reduce HIV-related stigma and discrimination from 2002 to 2013: how far have we come? J Int AIDS Soc, 16(3 Suppl 2), 18734. doi: 10.7448/IAS.16.3.18734 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Stutterheim SE, Bos AE, Pryor JB, Brands R, Liebregts M, & Schaalma HP (2011). Psychological and social correlates of HIV status disclosure: the significance of stigma visibility. AIDS Educ Prev, 23(4), 382–392. doi: 10.1521/aeap.2011.23.4.382 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Sue DW, Capodilupo CM, Torino GC, Bucceri JM, Holder AM, Nadal KL, & Esquilin M (2007). Racial microaggressions in everyday life: implications for clinical practice. Am Psychol, 62(4), 271–286. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.62.4.271 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Tabachnik B, & Fidell L (2001). Principal components and factor analysis. . Using multivariate statistics, 4, 582–633. [Google Scholar]
  40. Torres-Harding SR, Andrade AL Jr., & Romero Diaz CE (2012). The Racial Microaggressions Scale (RMAS): a new scale to measure experiences of racial microaggressions in people of color. Cultur Divers Ethnic Minor Psychol, 18(2), 153–164. doi: 10.1037/a0027658 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Torres L, Driscoll M, & AL B (2010). Racial microaggressions and psychological functioning among highly achieving African-Americans: A mixed-methods approach. . Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 29(10), 1074–1099. [Google Scholar]
  42. Wardell JD, Shuper PA, Rourke SB, & Hendershot CS (2018). Stigma, Coping, and Alcohol Use Severity Among People Living With HIV: A Prospective Analysis of Bidirectional and Mediated Associations. Ann Behav Med, 52(9), 762–772. doi: 10.1093/abm/kax050 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Williams DR, Neighbors HW, & Jackson JS (2003). Racial/ethnic discrimination and health: findings from community studies. Am J Public Health, 93(2), 200–208. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Wing Sue D (2010). Microaggressions in Everyday Life: Race, Gender, and Sexual Orientation Wiley. [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES