Abstract
Objectives
Most research investigating children’s experiences of stress and coping has utilized a quantitative approach. This study aimed to examine children’s experiences of stress by conducting interviews with children and their parents.
Design
Dyadic child–parent interviews, embedded within a multiphase design.
Methods
Thirty‐eight children (22 boys) aged 7–11 years and 38 parents (34 mothers) completed in‐depth dyadic interviews about stressful life events, adversity, and coping, analysed using inductive thematic analysis with a phenomenological lens.
Results
Four themes emerged: (1) navigating the social minefield; (2) pressure to thrive in the modern world; (3) fear of the unknown; and (4) learning life’s lessons. The first suggested that social relationships are a major feature of children’s stress experiences; however, social support was also found to be a beneficial coping mechanism. The second theme highlighted multiple sources of pressure on young children (including school, extracurricular activities, pressure from self and others); the impact of such pressure was dependent upon children’s coping resources. The third theme emphasized the difficulty of coping with novel stressors, and how awareness can help reduce this fear. The final theme highlighted important lessons that children can learn from stressful experiences and how to cope with stress.
Conclusions
This study addresses the importance of the person and context‐dependent nature of stress and coping in order for children to survive and thrive following stressful experiences. These findings contribute to existing knowledge that could be used to develop a toolkit for coping with stress, designed specifically for children, parents, schools, and services.
Statement of contribution.
What is already known on this subject?
Stress experienced in childhood can have a significant impact on psychological and physiological outcomes across the life course. It is known that individual differences are vital for understanding the effects of stress on health, for children as well as adults. Qualitative methods enable deeper understanding of children’s experiences of stress and coping.
What does the study add?
Depth and breadth to understanding children’s experiences of stressful events.
An individual differences focus on the early stress experience that is frequently overlooked.
Support for the use of a dyadic interview approach for assessing children’s stress experiences.
Keywords: child, coping, parent, resilience, stress, qualitative
Stress experienced in childhood can have a significant impact on psychological and physiological outcomes across the life course (Braveman & Barclay, 2009; Nurius, Green, Logan‐Greene, & Borja, 2015). Childhood stressors can take the form of major life events, such as parental divorce or moving house, through to daily hassles, including falling out with friends and struggling with schoolwork. These stressful early experiences can have a negative effect or may result in the development of positive coping strategies, depending on situational and individual factors, which can act as psychosocial moderators between stress and health (Turner‐Cobb & Steptoe, 1998) with implications for later life. Coping strategies are known to evolve across the life course, with different strategies employed in childhood, adolescence, and adulthood (Aldwin, 2009). Therefore, it is imperative to explore children’s early experiences of stress and coping.
Much of the literature examining childhood stress and coping utilizes quantitative methods, including physiological responses to laboratory stressors and life event scales. While questionnaire‐assessed life events report the number of major stressors children have encountered, they do not reveal the extent to which the stressors have influenced the child, capturing data on only certain aspects of children’s stress experience (Holmes & Rahe, 1967). The relatively small amount of qualitative research that has been conducted on stress in childhood has focused on the effect of different types of stress (e.g., chronic vs. episodic) on psychiatric disorders (Gershon et al., 2011), the impact of illness‐related stress on depression and anxiety (LeBovidge, Lavigne, & Miller, 2005), and comparison of quantitative (life events checklist) and qualitative measures (life events interview) (Wagner, Abela, & Brozina, 2006). These studies provide a basic overview of particular aspects of children’s stress experience; research questions focus on mental health or methodology and they fail to explore the depth or breadth of children’s stress. Research that goes beyond describing the nature of stressors and investigates how children make sense of stressful experiences is needed to advance the field of child stress research.
The importance of interviewing child–parent dyads together has been highlighted as providing the most illuminating and rich data as children and their parents interact to build a narrative (Cheetham & Turner‐Cobb, 2016). For example, children often struggle to remember examples of times when they felt stressed but parents are able to prompt memory for specific events. Once recalled, children are often very good at describing how they felt about an event, something the parent would have been less able to do. This ‘story scaffolding’ generates a rich depth of data which would not be possible to access if either of the dyad were not present (Irwin & Johnson, 2005).
The present study aimed to better understand children’s experiences of stress, adversity, and coping, through in‐depth interviews with child and parent dyads to ensure that the perspectives of both children and parents could be garnered.
Method
Design
This study is part of an embedded multiphase design encompassing qualitative and quantitative data collection, in which children completed questionnaires and interviews about their experiences of stress and coping. Interview data are reported in this paper; questionnaire data are reported elsewhere (Cheetham‐Blake, Turner‐Cobb, Family, & Turner, 2019).
Participants
Ethical approval for this study was granted by the University’s Psychology Department ethics committee. An opt‐in recruitment method with press releases in local newspapers, advertisements in schools, on relevant websites, and through social media platforms was used to recruit healthy children and their parents.
Thirty‐eight children (16 females, aged 7–11 years), each accompanied by one parent, took part in the interviews. Thirty‐four of the participants were interviewed with their mothers, and four were interviewed with their fathers. The sample was White European with a median socio‐economic status (SES) score of 54 (possible range = 8–66; higher scores correspond to higher SES) and IQR of 11 (Hollingshead, 1975). Table 1 shows age and sex of the sample.
Table 1.
Males (22 participants) | Females (16 participants) | |
---|---|---|
7 years | C12, C16, C32, C34 | C14, C22, C33 |
8 years | C17, C26, C30 | C2, C7, C20, C27, C29 |
9 years | C19, C31, C37 | C5, C15, C24, C36, C38 |
10 years | C9, C10, C11, C21, C25 | C23 |
11 years | C3, C4, C6, C8, C13, C28, C35 | C1, C18 |
C3, C8, C24, and C25 were interviewed with their fathers; the other 34 children were interviewed with their mothers.
Measures
Questionnaires
Socio‐demographic data (child age, sex, ethnicity, and parental SES) and details of children’s experiences of stressful life events (Social Readjustment Rating Scale; Holmes & Rahe, 1967), daily hassles (Children's Hassles Scale; Kanner, Feldman, Weinberger, & Ford 1987), and coping strategies (Kidcope; Spirito et al., 1988) were collected. Children completed questionnaires with the assistance of their parents. Questionnaire topics were used to guide the interview protocol.
Interview protocol
Several interview protocols investigate early life stress and adversity, health, and coping strategies in young children (Gershon et al., 2011; LeBovidge et al., 2005; Wagner et al., 2006). However, none of the existing protocols fully cover all topics together in one interview schedule and they focus on the impact of stress on mental health. Existing protocols also often use parents as proxy respondents for their children. It is important to speak directly to children about their experiences, rather than relying on parental proxies (Docherty & Sandelowski, 1999). In the few instances when children were interviewed in previous literature, they participated separately from their parents (LeBovidge et al., 2005).
To address these issues, a new dyadic interview schedule was developed for the present study. This enabled the interviews to be semi‐structured, with some predetermined questions and topics, but also allowing the interviewer’s questions to be led by topics that participants felt to be significant or relevant. The interview questions complemented the topics covered in the questionnaires, but were not constrained by them; the questionnaire was a starting point for a broader discussion. For example, the first part of the interview focussed on stressful experiences, so the researcher would ask about a life event or daily hassle that the participant had indicated in the questionnaire to have been highly stressful. Table 2 shows interview questions and prompts.
Table 2.
Topic | Questions |
---|---|
1. About you (rapport building) |
Could you tell me a bit about yourself and your family? Prompts: Who do you live with? Has this changed since you were little? What kind of things to do like doing at the weekend? What subjects do you like at school? What are your hobbies/things you like doing? Complete ‘about you’ scale |
2. Stressful life events and daily hassles |
For stressful life events: You mentioned that (type of event) happened. Could you describe what happened? How did you/your family feel about (the event)? Prompts: Length of the event? Coping: Could you describe how you coped with (the event)? For example, what did you do to make yourself feel better? How do you think (your child) coped with (the event)? Comparisons: Could you compare what you felt like before (the event) and how you feel now? For example, compare how you felt about school/relationships with friends/family before and after. Could you compare your feelings/behaviour before and after (the event)? Effect on present life: Could you compare how you/your child deals with new challenges or problems before and after (the event)? Could you describe how you/your child reacts if they are reminded of (the event)? Prompts: Interference with day‐to‐day life? For daily hassles: You said that (type of hassle) made you feel (quite bad/very bad). Could you tell me a bit more about it/how it made you feel? Prompts: Are there other times when you’ve felt similar to that? Are there any other stressful events that have happened to you that we’ve not talked about? Prompts: How old were you/your child when this happened? |
3. Coping | When you told me about how you coped with that problem on the coping questionnaire you said that you found (top three strategies) most helpful. Go through each strategy in turn: Could you tell me about any other times you’ve used this strategy? Can you tell me how you normally cope with problems? What do you normally do if something bad happens to you? Prompts: Can you tell me a bit more about that? Do you use any of the things on the list? Can you describe a time when you’ve used this strategy? Can you describe how you feel once you’ve used them? |
4. Health and illness | Can you tell me a bit about what it’s like to be ill? On the health questionnaire you said that you/your child (go through each question, e.g., had some health problems). Could you tell me a bit more about that? For each specific illness: Complete health and illness scale |
5. Early life events (questions for the parent) Note: the child was asked the closing questions and left the interview prior to these parental questions) | Were there any stressful events that happened during your pregnancy? Were there any stressful life events that happened to you or (your child) during the first year of their life? Prompts: How did these events make you feel? |
Closing questions | Is there anything I’ve missed or not asked you about that you think is very important? Do you have any final questions or comments? |
Adaptations were made to the interview format and style to make it more appropriate for young children and reduce fatigue. In addition to the use of brief scales to break up the interview, the protocol comprised a combination of open and closed questions, with prompt cards for the interview topics placed in front of the participant throughout. These techniques are recommended as best practice for dividing up interviews with young children into manageable sections and keeping children engaged and focused throughout (London School of Economics, 2010; Shaw, Brady, & Davey, 2011).
Procedure
Parents were given detailed information about the project during their initial contact with the researcher; the interview was scheduled at least a week after the initial contact so that parents, and children had adequate time to consider their participation. The researcher met with participants at their homes or in a meeting room at the University. Parents received an information sheet to read while the researcher verbally explained the study to the child. Verbal assent was gained from the child and written consent from the parent. The child–parent dyads completed the questionnaires together and then took part in the interview that ranged in length from 20 to 55 min depending on how much life stress was reported and their ability to expand on answers; the average length of the interviews was 30‐min. Interviews were audio‐recorded and transcribed verbatim. Afterwards, children and their parents were verbally debriefed and given a £10 shopping voucher to thank them for their participation.
Data analysis
Interviews were analysed in NVivo version 10: QSR International. An inductive thematic analysis was conducted using a phenomenological lens (Guest, MacQueen, & Namey, 2012). This approach was selected due to the exploratory nature of the study, as no interviews previously addressed all the topics. This exploratory nature is part of the underlying philosophical presuppositions of the phenomenological approach; its purpose in the present study was to describe children’s lived experiences of the phenomenon of stress and coping (Creswell, 2013). The researchers did not want to impose a predetermined framework but allow the data to determine the analysis structure and to understand the sense that children made of their experiences of stress and coping. Analysis involved the lead researcher familiarizing themselves with the data (in part through interview transcription), generating initial codes, searching for themes, reviewing themes, defining and naming themes, and writing up the analysis (Braun & Clarke, 2006). Transcripts were fully coded and a coding manual developed and used to guide the analysis.
Inter‐rater reliability
Two independent qualitative researchers coded six of the transcripts (15% of the data) chosen at random. The lead researcher met with each coder to compare all the codes created. Any codes that appeared in one person’s analysis but not the others were discussed, and a decision made about their inclusion. This method of code checking (Silverman, 2013) yielded a high level of congruence in the codes developed by the coders and lead researcher; all original codes were agreed upon, the wording of five codes was improved upon, and five new codes were included in the analysis. For further details of how the study complied with quality criteria please, see the Appendix which maps the study to the COREQ criteria of reporting qualitative research.
Results
Four overarching themes emerged from the data: (1) navigating the social minefield; (2) pressure to thrive in the modern world; (3) fear of the unknown; and (4) learning life’s lessons. The first two themes represent the broader social elements of children’s stress experiences, whereas the latter two themes reflect more individual features of children’s experiences. Each theme encapsulates two to four candidate themes (see Figure 1).
All quotations are verbatim and in order to maintain anonymity, identified by a participant number alongside ‘C’ for child participants and ‘P’ for parents. The participant identification number for each parent corresponds to their child’s identification number, for example, P1 is the parent of C1 and so on.
1. Navigating the social minefield
For many children in this study, their social world was a difficult area of their lives to navigate. Managing dynamic relationships with people was an important skill for children to learn, especially as their relationships with friends were often far from smooth. Bullying was also a source of stress for children, particularly in relation to the negative feelings that result from bullying, and the potential reasons for its occurrence.
Complex friendship dynamics
The dynamic and changing nature of children’s friendships was evident in how they spoke about their friends, for example, ‘sometimes he annoys me but sometimes he’s also my friend’ (C10). Friendship was seen as fluid, with falling out as a normal everyday part of friendship. Despite their frequency, these fallings out were not taken lightly. Children found falling out upsetting, difficult, and stressful with several participants referring to fallings out as ‘break ups’ (C7, C23). In these children’s narratives, friendships were restored after parents or teachers intervened (C22, C29, C30, C35), children talked through their problems (C20), or forgot about the falling out (C38).
Levels of friendship and the social hierarchy were an interesting feature of the interviews. Distinctions were made between ‘best’ friends and regular friends (C23). ‘Enemies’ was a term used by some participants (C26, C28). There appeared to be a need to categorize friends based on closeness in a personal social ranking or hierarchy, with fallings out often occurring when a friend’s actions suggested they did not value their place in the social ranking (C26).
Bullying
Many children experienced negative social interactions such as bullying. The bullying described in the interviews ranged from mean comments and disrespectful behaviour, ‘she would also back‐chat you and be horrible about people’ (C1), destruction of personal property, ‘it’s like [participant] had this thing that he thought was cool so they wanted to destroy it’ (P6), and physical fighting and violence (P28). The language children used to describe bullying and its various forms are evidenced in this discussion between child and mother.
P: Your use of the term ‘beat me up’ also means speaking badly at you doesn't it (.) it's not only a physical activity (.) if they look at you in a certain way it means ‘beat you up’
C: And they like talk about me (.) ‘oh yeah [participant]'s a horrible person don't go and play with him’ (.) just behind my back when I'm not there (C28 and P28).
Bullying often had very serious consequences, with some children being so worried that they could not eat or sleep (C1), feigning illness or running away from school to avoid the bullies, and even changing schools (C2, C6, C28). Often bullying led to feelings of exclusion and isolation (C6, C12, C23, C28, C31, C35).
Participants frequently discussed why they thought they were bullied, often because they were ‘different’ from other people (C28) or because the bullies were trying to hide their own insecurities (C36). One participant felt his bullying was caused by his perceived social standing and that when he moved schools he felt a change in his social status, ‘a definite elevation, I’m not sure how to describe it but I was respected more. I was treated like a person’ (C6).
2. Pressure to thrive in the modern world
Children experienced pressure to do well at school, in extracurricular activities and taking responsibility for their own health; placed on them by themselves (internal pressure) and by those around them (external pressure). This pressure to thrive was shown to have positive effects (e.g., increased motivation and self‐worth) and negative effects (e.g., negative self‐esteem, worry, and rumination) on children’s psychological well‐being.
Pressure from multiple sources
School‐related pressure was a major feature of children’s narratives, especially in relation to struggling with schoolwork in the classroom (C15, C17, C23, C31) and difficult homework (C21, C25, C29, C34). Examinations and revision were reported to be a particularly stressful aspect of school (C4, C21, C36).
Ever since year five almost the school’s been preparing us for an exam to go to the same school, just into the senior school and then it all came down to this one day almost. It was a lot of pressure and it was quite stressful because we did lots of mock exams. (C36)
This participant’s stress resulted from combined internal and external pressure, wanting to do well for himself but also feeling pressure from the school. Parental pressure also played a role in children’s narratives, for example, participating in a sporting event that they did not want to take part in (C15).
Sources of pressure external to schoolwork included rehearsing for a play (C1), competing in gymnastics competitions (C16), public speaking as part of a role on the school council (C30), and health‐related responsibility (C7). As children’s perceived competence increased, they were given more responsibility for their own health, for example, in relation to food allergies (C7, C10) and avoiding situations which would exacerbate eczema by ‘trying to make the right choices as to whether you should go on the field to play with your friends or just stay on the playground’ (P5).
Impact of pressure on self‐esteem
The pressure to thrive could have a positive or negative effect on children’s self‐esteem and feelings of self‐worth, depending on the coping strategies available to them. Many of the children were motivated to overcome adversity to demonstrate their self‐worth to others and themselves, for example, one child went above and beyond when applying for an IT support role at school by preparing a presentation to show to her teachers (C27). In response to not doing well during a swimming lesson, one participant was primarily concerned about what others would think of their ability but soon positively reframed the situation in terms of future opportunities to showcase their skills:
I didn’t have a good day, so I told myself not to worry because there’s always another chance to prove that you’re better than that particular Saturday. (C14)
Pressure to achieve can have a negative impact on children’s well‐being and self‐esteem. Some parents felt that the modern school system put too much pressure on young children and turned to alternative forms of schooling (such as forest schools) which were noted for their ability to improve children’s self‐esteem, feelings of self‐worth, coping strategies, and behaviour (P26, P28, P29) and provide a way for parents to introduce some balance into children’s lives.
P: He was off school for one day a week to learn in a different way, in a different environment, and that helped with his emotional development and his ability to cope with stress and I think that has given him a lot of resilience, and he now knows he can cope with anything
C: I try to have positive thoughts and that helps (C13 and P13).
Worry and rumination
One of the negative outcomes of this pressure to thrive was worry and rumination, a key feature in the narratives of the children in this sample. Some participants remarked that they worried about everything (C31), and that worrying was a part of their daily life, ‘every day I worry about something: school stuff, home stuff, everything’ (C22). These worries and anxieties often transformed into rumination, with children dwelling on particular stressors. Parents commented on how their children ruminated on problems for a long time (P25) and one remarked that their child ‘carries stuff with him’ (P37). One of the features of rumination noted by children in the sample was how well they coped with things during the day but ruminated at night (C18, C31).
When I go to sleep I panic, like as soon as my head hits the pillow if I don’t have internet and I can’t go on YouTube, my head, I start on like a mini panic attack cos you think about everything and it’s really annoying. You need to make your life perfect but it’s never going to be perfect, but you really start panicking and it’s not very nice (C18)
As shown here, rumination clearly has a negative impact on the participant’s sleep and well‐being. Although she does describe a distraction coping mechanism, her concerns about being good at everything and wanting to be perfect are an indication of the pressure that she feels. The type of distraction chosen by the participant (the internet and YouTube videos) could also be increasing the pressure to be perfect, as social media often perpetuates a positive bias with contributors only presenting highlights of their life.
3. Fear of the unknown
Unknown stressors were a significant feature of children’s narratives about their experiences of stress, particularly in relation to coping. If a stressor was familiar, it was deemed as easier to cope with whereas a novel stressor was harder to adapt to. A strategy that was found to be helpful for dealing with unknown future stressors was increasing awareness through information gathering.
The future as an unknowable entity
The unknowable and unpredictable nature of the future can lead to feelings of worry. Children are still learning about stress and how to cope with it and therefore have fewer experiences to draw on to make estimations about the future. Children reported worrying about major life events, such as changing schools (C13, C26), a parent being away for long periods of time (C7, C34), and parents changing their jobs (C30, C31). Smaller hassles that affected children’s daily routines were also found to be stressful, for example, when a participant was summoned to the head teacher’s office but did not know why. In this extract, it is evident that it was the unknown nature of the situation that made the experience more stressful.
I went in and I was quite scared cos I didn’t know what I was going to talk to her about, and I thought it was a different situation that I was going to talk to, but it was a situation I wasn’t actually ready for (C20)
Novelty versus familiarity of stressors
Experiencing an event for the first time can feel more challenging than an event that is familiar, and this link between stress and novelty was referred to by several of the participants.
I felt really scared and nervous [about giving a speech] cos it was my first time standing in front of the class. Other people like my friend, it's about like her fourth time and it was my first (C30)
Numerous participants remarked on stressful situations which they were familiar with, such as people making mean comments to them: ‘cos it happens so frequently I don’t really feel that offended’ (C8) and in response to a recurring illness ‘I can cope because I had it a lot’ (C15). Familiarity with an event, even an unpleasant one such as bullying or illness, made participants better able to cope with it. Therefore, experiencing stress can sometimes have beneficial outcomes in terms of reducing novelty and improving coping strategies.
Awareness helps overcome fear
One method for making novel or unknown situations more bearable for children was through increasing awareness and knowledge by gathering information about the stressor. One participant described, with great insight, the benefit of awareness for her ability to cope with her father’s terminal illness and death.
I already knew that he had an illness though so that kind of helped, but I know a few people who have had it happen sudden…they don’t like to talk about it cos, he literally just died, their dad died just suddenly, they didn’t have any warning (C1)
This participant noted the benefit of knowing that the death was going to happen and being able to mentally prepare for it, comparing her advance knowledge favourably to the experience of someone whose father died unexpectedly. This participant’s ability to positively reframe an immensely stressful experience, and even point to others for whom the situation was worse, highlights how well she has coped and the resilience to adversity she displayed.
Enhancing awareness through information gathering was found to be a helpful coping mechanism by many of the participants, for example, one participant asked his older sister questions about the new school he was starting so he could prepare for his first day (C16). Increasing awareness using the knowledge and expertise of others was a source of comfort, particularly in relation to novel experiences, such as a school caving trip, ‘they were really prepared. They made us hold on to a rope so we’d know where we were going’ (C1).
4. Learning life’s lessons
The context‐dependent nature of stress was evident in children’s narratives with an emphasis placed on how major life events and daily hassles were perceived differently by different people. The ability to learn from stressful experiences and how to cope with stress are two important life lessons that can be acquired during this time. The passage of time was also found to be helpful in children’s recovery from stress.
Stress is context‐dependent
The type of stressor was frequently noted when children discussed the context of their stress and coping, for example, whether the stressor was a major life event or a daily hassle. For some participants, major life events were more stressful than daily hassles, ‘he copes very well with the day to day stuff but if there’s a big change coming he gets anxious about that’ (P32). Conversely, some children were fully capable of dealing with major life stress but did not cope well with daily hassles and stressors (P2, C11, C18, C36): ‘I’d say that some of the day to day roller‐coasters of emotions can be quite stressful’ (P20). Similar comments were made by other parents who had noticed that their children struggled most with daily hassles, such as falling out with siblings which ‘has the biggest continued impact on her, on an almost daily basis’ (P5, also C5, C7, P17).
Learning from stressful experiences
Children experience a wide range of adversities which, although stressful, can be beneficial in terms of learning life skills. For example, one child’s experience of bullying was very upsetting but the parent hoped that it could also be viewed as a learning experience, ‘I like to think that, in the long term, she will learn lots from it’ (P2). In the sample, parents often commented on the importance of children viewing their stressful experiences as life lessons. Examples included a bad gymnastics competition being a way to learn that more practice was needed for the next competition (P20) and that some children may appear overly confident about an exam to hide their own nerves which is a ‘useful life lesson as you learn the filter to put on that stuff’ (P36). Children also used this technique themselves, as one child explained, being told off by a teacher, ‘it feels bad but it also feels ok because it means that if you get told off you know that for next time you don't do that’ (C27).
Learning how to cope with stress
Problem‐focussed coping skills such as problem‐solving, cognitive restructuring, and social support were widely used by participants and this was encouraged by parents. Participants sometimes used several coping strategies to deal with a problem, which takes the form of problem‐solving and cognitive restructuring in the example below.
I think he’s more of a try to sort out the problem, try to see why it went wrong so that it doesn’t happen the next time, definitely tries to control himself. He does try and see the good things, you try and see what you can learn from it don’t you? (P16)
Over half of the participants reported using social support as a coping mechanism during times of stress. For several participants (C1, C38), it was not just the act of sharing their problems with another person that they found beneficial, but the process of feeling listened to, understood, and receiving reassurance. Verbal reassurance included assuring children that things would be okay (C38), that when bad things happened ‘it’s not the end of the world’ (C17, also C37), and help with refocussing and restructuring their thoughts in a more positive way (P14, P36). Reassurance was also gained through physical closeness; for some participants, merely being in the presence of their social companions was comforting (C16) and playing with friends helped distract them (C11, C32). Hugging was something that many children found particularly comforting, whether it was hugging their parents, friends, pets, or favourite toys (C15, C17, C20, C26, C31).
Emotion‐focussed coping strategies were also discussed by children and their parents, for example, blaming others, repressing negative thoughts, crying, shouting and screaming, smashing things, fighting, and violence towards oneself.
One more tactic that I sometimes do, I’ll go outside sometimes and like smash a ball, kick the ball as hard as I can or something. If you’re all contained just let it all out and then once I run around and get tired that’s when the problem goes away (C10)
For this participant, releasing anger by kicking an inanimate object was a cathartic experience that helped him to recover from stressful experiences. Another cathartic experience for many participants was crying, allowing them to release their feelings and diminish the impact of the stressor (C35). Creative techniques such as drawing and writing about one’s feelings were also used as a form of emotional expression to aid coping (C10, C20).
Avoidant coping strategies used by the participants included finding distractions, trying to forget the problem, dealing with problems alone, keeping quiet, leaving stressful situations, and blaming oneself. Distractions were frequently alluded to by the participants (C3, C6, C12, C17, C26, C37) and were found to be particularly helpful, for example,
We’ve been trying to teach him if you just try and do something that makes you feel good, to take your mind off it, like skateboarding. (P37)
The healing nature of time
Another key life lesson discussed in the interviews was the importance of the passage of time for coping with, and moving on from, stressful events. Some of the participants were acutely aware of how their feelings towards a stressor had changed over time, knowing that they felt differently now to how they felt when they had encountered a stressful situation in the past (C8), even those who had encountered very serious stressors, such as the death of a parent.
But now I deal with it so much better, it’s kind of like it happened, it’s finished, it’s done and everyone’s always like ‘are you okay’ and I’m like ‘it happened two years ago, it’s gone, don’t worry. (C1)
Mostly, parents tried to encourage their children in this behaviour as moving on was seen as advantageous, and children were aware of their parents preference for this approach (C7, C29), as opposed to dwelling on past events which ‘belong in the past’ (P32). Many parents remarked on their child’s ability to cope with stress and move on, with their children’s comments reflecting their own capacity for resilience (C2, C6, C8, C10, C13).
Discussion
1. Navigating the social minefield
This theme highlighted the importance of successfully negotiating social relationships with friends, parents, and in many children’s cases, bullies.
Managing complex relationships and social hierarchies makes up a considerable proportion of the early life stress and adversity experienced by young children. This finding is supported in evolutionary psychology, with the power dynamics and social dominance created by social hierarchies regarded as a major source of psychosocial stress with health implications for childhood (Hawley, 2015) and adolescence (Koski, Xie, & Olson, 2015).
Bullying was mentioned throughout children’s narratives, which is unsurprising given that 22% of young people have reported being bullied and 22% have witnessed bullying (Annual bullying survey, 2018). It was evident that bullying had a negative psychological impact on children, principally through increasing their feelings of isolation and exclusion. Previous work suggests that if children do not learn how to cope effectively with social stressors (e.g., bullying), it can have an impact on their physiological, as well as psychological, health (Knack, Jensen‐Campbell, & Baum, 2011). Bullying and stressful social relationships were mentioned by both genders.
Interventions created to build resilience in young people often focus their efforts on increasing positive peer relationships along with other skills such as self‐efficacy, creativity, and coherence, illustrating the importance of being able to successfully navigate social relationships (Waaktaar, Christie, Helmen Borge, & Torgersen, 2004). Similar findings have been found in research with adolescents which show that close friendships and perceived friendship quality were associated with higher resilience (Graber, Turner, & Madill, 2015). Higher feelings of social belonging were also correlated with fewer instances of physical illness, showing the impact of social relationships on health (Began & Turner‐Cobb, 2012).
2. Pressure to thrive in the modern world
This theme demonstrated the multiple sources of pressure that children experience and how this pressure could have a positive or negative impact on children’s well‐being.
Pressure for children to thrive came from themselves and others and applied to multiple areas of their lives, including school, extracurricular activities, and taking responsibility for their own health. Research with adolescents has found similar diversity in sources of pressure, for example, peer pressure, home life, school performance, and adult responsibilities (Moksnes, Moljord, Espnes, & Byrne, 2010). These varied and substantial stressors during childhood have led some researchers to suggest that children are being ‘hurried’ to grow up too quickly (Elkind, 2001). Just as social interactions were found to be difficult to navigate, they could contribute to the pressure to achieve that children feel.
Such pressure can have a positive or negative effect on children’s self‐esteem and feelings of self‐worth, depending on their coping resources. Research has placed an emphasis on children’s personal resources for coping and resilience particularly enhancing skills such as self‐efficacy, creativity, and self‐regulation, which can help children to succeed under pressure (Lavoie, Pereira, & Talwar, 2014; Waaktaar et al., 2004; Yendork & Somhlaba, 2015). The relationship between coping and self‐esteem is also bidirectional with high self‐esteem helping lead to successful coping in adolescents confronted with a social stressor (Moksnes et al., 2010).
Pressure to thrive can also have a negative impact on children, leading to worry and rumination. Striving to achieve goals can be taxing on children’s coping resources (Masten, 2014). Similarly, the act of striving for perfection can cause intense distress if the goals are not achieved, with rumination acting as a mediating factor in this relationship (O’Connor, O’Connor, & Marshall, 2007).
3. Fear of the unknown
This theme outlined the stress created by the unknown, how children cope with familiar and novel stressors differently, with information gathering highlighted as a coping mechanism for novel stressors.
The present study suggests that not knowing the outcome of a situation can be stressful and worrying. This finding is supported in the wider literature, with fear of the unknown being a major worry during childhood hospitalization (Hart & Bossert, 1994). Coping was enhanced if the stressor was familiar rather than novel, for instance, when children encounter positive early experiences, these events shift from novel to familiar, which enhances their coping ability (Kent, Davis, & Reich, 2014). Problem‐focussed coping strategies such as information gathering are able to enhance awareness in the face of unknown stressors, for example, children awaiting surgery (Thompson, 1994).
4. Learning life’s lessons
This theme highlighted how children respond differently to major and minor life stresses, with learning about stress and how to cope with it given as two important life lessons, along with the healing nature of time.
Stress and coping theories have emphasized the impact of the characteristics of the stressor on coping ability, suggesting the context‐dependent nature of stress and coping (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984). The coping conceptualization used in the present research is the three‐factor model encompassing problem‐focussed coping, emotion‐focussed coping, and avoidant coping (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984; Spirito et al., 1988; Turner‐Cobb & Steptoe, 1998). All three forms of coping were reported by children in the sample, with varying degrees of success in helping them cope with stress. There was no difference in the coping strategies used by boys and girls; both genders used a mixture of coping strategies. The success of different coping strategies is situational and person dependent. Successful coping could be part of the pathway to developing resilience, for example, by reducing negative chain reactions and the negative impact of stress (Rutter, 1999). Individual differences (e.g., personality), sensitivity to context and environment are also factors in the development of resilience (Boyce & Ellis, 2005; Lionetti et al., 2018; Masten, 2014).
In the present study, social support from friends and family was reported as a useful coping strategy for children when faced with a variety of stressors. Much work has focussed on parental social support; however, one study linked both friend and family social support to higher levels of resilience in children who have experienced early life adversity (Yendork & Somhlaba, 2015). A review of children’s resilience and adversity found that children who relied on parental or caregiver social support coped more successfully with early life adversity (Masten, Best, & Garmezy, 1990). The involvement of parents in the development of children’s coping resources was a prominent feature in the narratives, for example, helping children to learn and use certain coping strategies such as reframing stressful experiences as positive life lessons.
The passing of time was found to help children recover from stressful experiences, perhaps by letting go of stressful experiences or developing resilience. Research has highlighted the importance of both time and stressful experiences for the development of resilience (Egeland, Carlson, & Sroufe, 1993).
Strengths and limitations
The present study is the first to investigate children’s experiences of stress and coping using dyadic child–parent interviews, and it provides a unique discourse on issues of importance to young children. Using this method allowed for story scaffolding and a deeper understanding of child and parent perspectives on stress; these four themes formulated in analysis are supported by the wider literature. The study benefitted from a large sample size, unusual for qualitative psychology research. The sample of 38 children and 38 parents allowed for a comprehensive exploration of the topics from which complementary and divergent narratives could be established.
Despite efforts to recruit a diverse sample, participants were predominantly from higher SES backgrounds in a small relatively affluent geographical area. Future research would benefit from using a broader SES sample and sampling populations across different cultures, to examine whether the themes found in the present study apply across a broader range of backgrounds. Future research could also consider the gender of the parent being interviewed and the impact this may have on children’s narratives. Although efforts were made to reduce study fatigue, many topics were covered, in interview which could have been tiring for young children. Coverage of fewer topics may have enabled greater depth to be examined.
Applications of the research
The findings of this research have implications for policy and practice, particularly for services that work directly with young people such as schools and mental health services (e.g., CAMHS). Considering the person and context‐dependent nature of stress and coping strategies is crucial if children are to successfully cope with and thrive following stressful early experiences. The findings from this study contribute to existing knowledge that could be used to develop a toolkit for children, their parents, schools, and services that acknowledges individual differences and parental input in coping with stress.
The use of child–parent dyadic interviews provides a valuable methodological approach in health psychology research, as evidenced by the deepened understanding of stress experience in children and the potential health interventions this enables.
Conflict of interest
All authors declare no conflict of interest.
Acknowledgements
The authors would like to acknowledge the children and parents who took part in this research. The authors would like to thank Tamsyn Hawken for coding a subsection of the data and for her assistance with transcription, and Imogen Cobb for assistance with transcription. The research was conducted as part of a University of Bath Research Studentship funded PhD programme.
COREQ criteria for reporting qualitative research
Domains | Item | Guide questions/description | Responses |
---|---|---|---|
Domain 1: Research team and reflexivity | |||
Personal characteristics | 1. Interviewer/facilitator | Which author/s conducted the interview or focus group? | The lead author (TCB) conducted the interviews |
2. Credentials | What were the researcher’s credentials? For example, PhD, MD | The researcher had completed a BSc and an MSc prior to data collection | |
3. Occupation | What was their occupation at the time of the study? | The researcher was a PhD student at the time of data collection | |
4. Gender | Was the researcher male or female? | The researcher is female | |
5. Experience and training | What experience or training did the researcher have? | The researcher had completed modules in qualitative and quantitative methods during their BSc and MSc. Further methodological training was completed during the PhD. The researcher had extensive experience working with young children and had completed previous projects with child participants | |
Relationship with participants | 6. Relationship established | Was a relationship established prior to study commencement? | There was no relationship with participants (children and parents) prior to the study commencement |
7. Participant knowledge of the interviewer | What did the participants know about the researcher? For example, personal goals, reasons for doing the research | Before the interviews were conducted the researcher explained the study and their reasons for doing it to the participants | |
8. Interviewer characteristics | What characteristics were reported about the interviewer/facilitator? For example, bias, assumptions, reasons, and interests in the research topic | The lead researcher was the interviewer in the study. Bias was reduced by using independent qualitative researchers to code a subsection of the data. No assumptions were made by the researcher. The researcher had an academic interest in the topics | |
Domain 2: study design | |||
Theoretical framework | 9. Methodological orientation and Theory | What methodological orientation was stated to underpin the study? For example, grounded theory, discourse analysis, ethnography, phenomenology, content analysis | The study was part of an embedded multiphase design encompassing qualitative and quantitative data collection. The qualitative data were analysed using an inductive approach to thematic analysis |
Participant selection | 10. Sampling | How were participants selected? For example, purposive, convenience, consecutive, snowball | Participants were selected through convenience sampling |
11. Method of approach | How were participants approached? For example, face‐to‐face, telephone, mail, email | An opt‐in recruitment method using press releases in local newspapers, advertisements in schools and on relevant websites, and through social media platforms were used to recruit participants | |
12. Sample size | How many participants were in the study? | Thirty‐eight children and thirty‐eight parents | |
13. Non‐participation | How many people refused to participate or dropped out? Reasons? | All participants who approached the researcher about the project participated. No one withdrew from the study | |
Setting | 14. Setting of data collection | Where was the data collected? For example, home, clinic, workplace | The interviews were carried out either in participants’ homes or in a meeting room at the university |
15. Presence of non‐participants | Was anyone else present besides the participants and researchers? | One child and one parent participant were present for each interview. In a few instances a younger sibling was present during the interview when alternative childcare arrangements could not be made. The siblings did not contribute to the interview | |
16. Description of sample | What are the important characteristics of the sample? For example, demographic data, date | The participants were children aged 7–11 (16 females and 22 males) and one parent of each child (34 mothers and 4 fathers). The participants came from a high socio‐economic (SES) background. The interviews were conducted in 2015 | |
Data collection | 17. Interview guide | Were questions, prompts, guides provided by the authors? Was it pilot tested? | Interview questions and prompts were created by the researcher and can be found in Table 2 in the main article. The interview was pilot tested within the research team prior to data collection |
18. Repeat interviews | Were repeat interviews carried out? If yes, how many? | One interview per participant was carried out in the present study. Participants took part in a second brief interview in another study with the same research team (the findings of which are currently in press) | |
19. Audio/visual recording | Did the research use audio or visual recording to collect the data? | Audio recording | |
20. Field notes | Were field notes made during and/or after the interview or focus group? | Brief field notes were made during the interview and reflections were noted after each interview | |
21. Duration | What was the duration of the interviews or focus group? | The length of the interviews ranged from 20 to 55 min depending on how much life stress children had experienced and their ability to expand on their answers, with the average length of the interviews being 30‐min | |
22. Data saturation | Was data saturation discussed? | Data saturation was achieved due to the large sample size in this study | |
23. Transcripts returned | Were transcripts returned to participants for comment and/or correction? | Transcripts were not returned to participants as this was not deemed appropriate for research conducted with young children | |
Domain 3: analysis and findings | |||
Data analysis | 24. Number of data coders | How many data coders coded the data? | The lead researcher (TCB) coded all the data extracts. Two independent qualitative researchers (HF and TH) coded six of the transcripts (15% of the data) chosen at random and inter‐coder reliability was assessed |
25. Description of the coding tree | Did authors provide a description of the coding tree? | A coding manual was created which included the overarching themes, the candidate themes, definitions of each theme and examples of data extracts for each candidate theme | |
26. Derivation of themes | Were themes identified in advance or derived from the data? | The themes were derived from the data as an inductive approach to thematic analysis was taken | |
27. Software | What software, if applicable, was used to manage the data? | NVivo version 10 | |
28. Participant checking | Did participants provide feedback on the findings? | No, participants did not provide feedback on the findings | |
Reporting | 29. Quotations presented | Were participant quotations presented to illustrate the themes/findings? Was each quotation identified? For example, participant number | Quotations are provided throughout the results of the paper to illustrate the themes. Each quotation is identified by the participant number and a ‘C’ for child participants and a ‘P’ for parents |
30. Data and findings consistent | Was there consistency between the data presented and the findings? | The themes were derived from the data therefore there is consistency between the data and findings presented. Data extracts were chosen to illustrate themes discussed in the findings | |
31. Clarity of major themes | Were major themes clearly presented in the findings? | The four overarching themes are evident within the results and discussion | |
32. Clarity of minor themes | Is there a description of diverse cases or discussion of minor themes? | Each overarching theme has two to four candidate themes which are clearly discussed |
References
- Aldwin, C. M. (2009). Developmental studies of coping In The Guildford Press (Ed.), Stress, coping, and development: An integrative perspective (pp. 271–304). London, UK: The Guildford Press. [Google Scholar]
- Annual bullying survey (2018). The annual benchmark of bullying in the United Kingdom. Brighton, UK: Ditch the Label Anti-Bullying Charity. [Google Scholar]
- Began, F. M. , & Turner‐Cobb, J. M. (2012). The need to belong and symptoms of acute physical health in early adolescence. Journal of Health Psychology, 17, 907–916. 10.1177/1359105311431176 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boyce, W. T. , & Ellis, B. J. (2005). Biological sensitivity to context: I. An evolutionary‐developmental theory of the origins and functions of stress reactivity. Development and Psychopathology, 17, 271–301. 10.1017/S0954579405050145 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Braun, V. , & Clarke, V. (2006). Using thematic analysis in psychology. Qualitative Research in Psychology, 3(2), 77–101. 10.1191/1478088706qp063oa [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Braveman, P. , & Barclay, C. (2009). Health disparities beginning in childhood: A life‐course perspective. Pediatrics, 124, 163–175. 10.1542/peds.2009-1100D [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cheetham, T. J. , & Turner‐Cobb, J. M. (2016). Panel manipulation in social stress testing: The Bath Experimental Stress Test for Children (BEST‐C). Psychoneuroendocrinology, 63, 78–85. 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2015.09.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cheetham-Blake, T. J. , Turner-Cobb, J. M. , Family, H. E. , & Turner, J. E. (2019). Resilience characteristics and prior life stress determine anticipatory response to acute social stress in children aged 7–11 years. British Journal of Health Psychology, 24(2), 282–297. 10.1111/bjhp.12353 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Creswell, J. W. (2013). Qualitative inquiry and research design: Choosing among five approaches (3rd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. [Google Scholar]
- Docherty, S. , & Sandelowski, M. (1999). Focus on qualitative methods: Interviewing children. Research in Nursing and Health, 2, 177–185. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Egeland, B. , Carlson, E. , & Sroufe, L. A. (1993). Resilience as process. Development and Psychopathology, 5, 517–528. 10.1017/s0954579400006131 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Elkind, D. (2001). The hurried child: Growing up too fast too soon (3rd ed.). Cambridge, MA: Perseus. [Google Scholar]
- Gershon, A. , Hayward, C. , Schraedley‐Desmond, P. , Rudolph, K. D. , Booster, G. D. , & Gotlib, I. H. (2011). Life stress and first onset of psychiatric disorders in daughters of depressed mothers. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 45, 855–862. 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2011.03.016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Graber, R. , Turner, R. , & Madill, A. (2015). Best friends and better coping: Facilitating psychological resilience through boys’ and girls’ closest friendships. British Journal of Psychology, 107, 338–358. 10.1111/bjop.12135 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guest, G. , MacQueen, K. M. , & Namey, E. E. (2012). Applied thematic analysis. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. [Google Scholar]
- Hart, D. , & Bossert, E. (1994). Self‐reported fears of hospitalized school‐age children. Journal of Pediatric Nursing, 9(2), 83–90. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hawley, P. H. (2015). Social dominance in childhood and its evolutionary underpinnings: Why it matters and what we can do. Pediatrics, 135(2), 31–38. 10.1542/peds.2014-3549D [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hollingshead, A. A. (1975). Four‐factor index of social status. New Haven, CT: Yale University. [Google Scholar]
- Holmes, T. H. , & Rahe, R. H. (1967). The social readjustment rating scale. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 11, 213–218. 10.1037/t02251-000 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Irwin, L. G. , & Johnson, J. (2005). Interviewing young children: Explicating our practices and dilemmas. Qualitative Health Research, 15, 821–831. 10.1177/1049732304273862 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kanner, A. D. , Feldman, S. S. , Weinberger, D. A. , & Ford, M. E. (1987). Uplifts, hassles, and adaptational outcomes in early adolescents. The Journal of Early Adolescence, 7, 371–394. 10.1177/0272431687074002 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Kent, M. , Davis, M. C. , & Reich, J. W. (2014). The resilience handbook: Approaches to stress and trauma. New York, NY: Routledge. [Google Scholar]
- Knack, J. M. , Jensen‐Campbell, L. A. , & Baum, A. (2011). Worse than sticks and stones? Bullying is associated with altered HPA axis functioning and poorer health. Brain and Cognition, 77, 183–190. 10.1016/j.bandc.2011.06.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koski, J. , Xie, H. , & Olson, I. R. (2015). Understanding social hierarchies: The neural and psychological foundations of status perception. Social Neuroscience, 10, 527–550. 10.1080/17470919.2015.1013223 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lavoie, J. , Pereira, L. C. , & Talwar, V. (2014). Understanding healthy development in children and adolescents: Themes of resilience. American Psychological Association Newsletter. 10.1037/e578812014-003 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Lazarus, R. S. , & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal and coping. New York, NY: Springer. [Google Scholar]
- LeBovidge, J. S. , Lavigne, J. V. , & Miller, M. L. (2005). Adjustment to chronic arthritis of childhood: The roles of illness‐related stress and attitude toward illness. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 30, 273–286. 10.1093/jpepsy/jsi037 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lionetti, F. , Aron, A. , Aron, E. N. , Burns, G. L. , Jagiellowicz, J. , & Pluess, M. (2018). Dandelions, tulips and orchids: Evidence for the existence of low‐sensitive, medium‐sensitive and high‐sensitive individuals. Translational Psychiatry, 8(24), 1–11. 10.1038/s41398-017-0090-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- London School of Economics (2010). Best practice guide: What are the best ways to interview children? Retrieved from http://www.Lse.Ac.Uk/Media@lse/Research/EUKidsOnline/BestPracticeGuide/FAQ15.Aspx [Google Scholar]
- Masten, A. S. (2014). Ordinary magic: Resilience in development. New York, NY: Guildford Press. [Google Scholar]
- Masten, A. S. , Best, K. M. , & Garmezy, N. (1990). Resilience and development: Contribution from the study of children who overcome adversity. Development and Psychopathology, 2, 425–444. 10.1017/S0954579400005812 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Moksnes, U. K. , Moljord, I. E. O. , Espnes, G. A. , & Byrne, D. G. (2010). The association between stress and emotional states in adolescents: The role of gender and self‐esteem. Personality and Individual Differences, 49, 430–435. 10.1016/j.paid.2010.04.012 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Nurius, P. S. , Green, S. , Logan‐Greene, P. , & Borja, S. (2015). Life course pathways of adverse childhood experiences toward adult psychological well‐being: A stress process analysis. Child Abuse & Neglect, 45, 143–153. 10.1016/j.chiabu.2015.03.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O’Connor, D. B. , O’Connor, R. C. , & Marshall, R. (2007). Perfectionism and psychological distress: Evidence of the mediating effects of rumination. European Journal of Personality, 21, 429–452. 10.1002/per.616 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Rutter, M. (1999). Resilience concepts and findings: Implications for family therapy. Journal of Family Therapy, 21, 119–144. 10.1111/1467-6427.00108 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Shaw, C. , Brady, L. , & Davey, C. (2011). Guidelines for research with children and young people. London, UK: National Children’s Bereau Research Centre. [Google Scholar]
- Silverman, D. (2013). Doing qualitative research (4th ed.). London, UK: SAGE. [Google Scholar]
- Spirito, A. , Stark, L. J. , & Williams, C. (1988). Development of a brief coping checklist for use with pediatric populations. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 13, 555–574. 10.1093/jpepsy/13.4.555 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thompson, M. L. (1994). Information‐seeking coping and anxiety in school‐age children anticipating surgery. Children’s Health Care, 23(2), 87–97. 10.1207/s15326888chc2302_2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Turner‐Cobb, J. M. , & Steptoe, A. (1998). Psychosocial influences on upper respiratory infectious illness in children. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 45, 319–330. 10.1016/s0022-3999(97)00311-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Waaktaar, T. , Christie, H. J. , Helmen Borge, A. I. , & Torgersen, S. (2004). How can young people’s resilience be enhanced? Experiences from a clinical intervention project. Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 9, 167–183. 10.1177/1359104504041917 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Wagner, C. , Abela, J. R. Z. , & Brozina, K. (2006). A comparison of stress measures in children and adolescents: A self‐report checklist versus an objectively rated interview. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 28, 251–261. 10.1007/s10862-005-9010-9 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Yendork, J. S. , & Somhlaba, N. Z. (2015). Do social support, self‐efficacy and resilience influence the experience of stress in Ghanian orphans? An exploratory study. Child Care in Practice, 21, 140–159. 10.1080/13575279.2014.985286 [DOI] [Google Scholar]