Abstract
The impacts of war and displacement on executive function (EF)—what we might call the cognitive signatures of minds under siege—are little known. We surveyed a gender‐balanced sample of 12‐ to 18‐year‐old Syrian refugees (n = 240) and Jordanian non‐refugees (n = 210) living in Jordan. We examined the relative contributions of poverty, trauma exposure, posttraumatic stress, and insecurity to variance in inhibitory control and working memory. We observed associations between poverty and WM, suggesting that, even in populations exposed to substantial violence and fear, poverty is a specific pathway to WM deficit. We did not, however, find associations between EFs and exposures to trauma. Careful distinction between childhood adversities may illuminate which neurocognitive pathways matter for measures of cognitive function.
In this study, we seek to understand what might be called the cognitive signatures of minds under siege—how young minds are affected by exposures to war and protracted displacement. We examine executive functions (EF), a set of higher order cognitive skills necessary for abstract thinking, decision making, and executing complex plans (Diamond, 2013; Gruszka, Szymura, & Matthews, 2010), known to associate with improved psychosocial health (McTeague, Goodkind, & Etkin, 2016), academic performance (Blair & Razza, 2007; Patrick, Blair, & Maggs, 2008), and success on the labor market (Mercy Corps, 2014). Two critical component processes of EF are inhibitory control (IC) and working memory (WM; Miyake & Friedman, 2012), which we measured in Syrian refugee and Jordanian non‐refugee adolescents in northern Jordan. Seventy‐one million children are displaced worldwide (UNHCR, 2019), and a whole generation of children and adolescents from Syria are at risk of compromised psychosocial well‐being (Save The Children, 2017). Researchers have emphasized that the Syrian crisis and other prolonged conflicts present urgent challenges for survival, health, and development pathways (Verma & Petersen, 2018). To better understand these pathways and the influence of war and displacement on EF, we examined the extent to which different indicators of adversity—poverty, war‐related trauma, posttraumatic stress, and insecurity—predicted EF in refugees and non‐refugees.
In the United States, poverty is robustly associated with EF deficits in childhood and adolescence (Daneri, Blair, Kuhn, & FLP Key Investigators, 2018; Haft & Hoeft, 2017; Wolf & Suntheimer, 2019). Recent work, however, shows that exposures to poverty and violence have distinct impacts on IC and WM (Lambert, King, Monahan, & McLaughlin, 2016; Sheridan, Peverill, Finn, & McLaughlin, 2017). Exposures to violence at home and in the community do not predict WM or IC task performance when controlling for poverty (Sheridan et al., 2017). These findings have been interpreted as distinguishing between the impact of adversities involving threat (“harm or threat of harm”) and those involving deprivation (“absence of expected inputs from the environment”), the latter being associated with low socioeconomic status (McLaughlin & Sheridan, 2016). To date, it is unknown whether exposures to violence and poverty have distinct impacts on EF in conflict‐affected children and adolescents, or whether the association between poverty and EF identified in other populations will hold true in this population.
In lower‐ and‐middle income countries (LMICs), few studies have been able to distinguish the cognitive signatures of poverty and violence, or for that matter, the cognitive signatures of posttraumatic stress and other psychosocial stressors. Adolescent and child refugees experience a host of adversities, including pervasive poverty, exposure to war‐related violence, and feelings of insecurity with regard to their safety and future opportunities (Verma & Petersen, 2018). These exposures are associated with mental health problems and difficulties in school (Dajani, Hadfield, van Uum, Greff, & Panter‐Brick, 2018; Miller‐Graff & Cummings, 2017; Wong, Schweitzer, & Khawaja, 2018). Few studies of conflict‐affected youth have measured performance on cognitive tasks, with the notable exception of work with Palestinian adolescents linking Intifada participation with attention and memory difficulties (Qouta, Punamäki, & Sarraj, 1995). For Syrian refugees, specifically, it is not known whether posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) or levels of insecurity are independently associated with cognitive performance, over and above exposure to traumatic events and poverty. This is important, as PTSD‐prevalence rates reported for child and adolescent refugees in the Middle East are high (41%–87%; Reed, Fazel, Jones, Panter‐Brick, & Stein, 2012), while levels of human insecurity—a measure of fear and worry for oneself and one’s family (Ziadni et al., 2011)—are salient in this environment, and predict mental health recovery (Panter‐Brick, Dajani, et al., 2018).
Conceptual Model and Hypotheses
Figure 1 presents a conceptual model linking types of childhood adversity with two measures of EF. It guided our theoretical expectations as we examined the relative strength of associations with poverty, trauma exposure, PTSD, and insecurity. We hypothesized that—controlling for trauma, PTSD, and human insecurity—higher levels of poverty would be associated with weaker IC and WM task performance. Given previous work in the United States demonstrating a lack of association between trauma exposure and EF, we hypothesized that associations between trauma exposure or PTSD and EF task performance would likely be seen in Syrian refugees, but not Jordanians, as the former had higher exposures to war violence. Given research documenting the regional importance of human insecurity, we hypothesized that higher levels of human insecurity would be negatively associated with IC and WM task performance. Finally, we examined resilience and migration history as potential moderators. We first examined associations in our whole sample (n = 450), then for refugees (n = 240) and non‐refugees (n = 210) separately. The analysis of data collected among refugee and non‐refugee populations living side‐by‐side in Jordan provides the variation needed to disentangle the effects of different dimensions of childhood adversity on EF (Table 1).
Table 1.
Syrian refugees (n = 240) | Jordanian non‐refugees (n = 210) | Both samples combined (n = 450) | |
---|---|---|---|
Age in years | 14.18 (1.92) | 14.42 (1.61) | 14.29 (1.79) |
% Male | 57.5 | 57.6 | 57.6 |
Years in school (child) | 6.60 (2.23) | 7.78 (1.77) | 7.15 (2.11)* |
Primary education or less (mother), % | 35.0 | 8.6 | 22.7* |
Primary education or less (father), % | 30.1 | 11.9 | 21.6* |
Predictors | |||
Household wealth, n household items | 6.65 (2.23) | 10.12 (2.07) | 8.27 (2.76)* |
War‐related trauma exposure, n events | 5.81 (3.07) | 0.67 (1.47) | 3.37 (3.55)* |
% Posttraumatic stress disorder | 65.1 | 16.2 | 42.2* |
Human insecurity | 68.25 (20.79) | 62.69 (21.55) | 65.65 (21.31)* |
Executive function outcomes | |||
Inhibitory control [IC] | 0.83 (0.28) | 0.84 (0.26) | 0.84 (0.27) |
Working memory [WM] | 67.84 (25.39) | 68.75 (22.49) | 68.27 (23.94) |
Unless otherwise stated, values are means (SD). The measure for IC is the performance across trials, where 0 is an incorrect and 1 is a correct response on each trial. The measure for WM is the average distance deviated from the target (in pixels) across trials.
p<.05 difference between refugees and non‐refugees, using an independent samples t‐test for continuous variables and a chi‐square for categorical variables.
method
The study was designed as a partnership between Mercy Corps, an international nongovernmental organization, and researchers with expertise in health and cognitive development. Fieldwork was conducted in 2015–2016 in northern Jordan, in areas heavily affected by the Syria crisis (Mercy Corps, 2013). We contacted refugee and non‐refugee participants through a Mercy Corps registry of youth considered high risk on the basis of their mental health difficulties and poor access to basic services. All study participants were eligible for Mercy Corps humanitarian programming. Syrian refugee youth had been in Jordan for 2.97 years on average (SD = 0.98 years).
We surveyed participants prior to the start of Mercy Corps activities (a program called Advancing Adolescents; Arabic: Nubader; Mercy Corps, 2013). We tracked mental health and stress outcomes over a 1‐year period for a cohort of 817 participants, 12–18 years old, representing half (48%) of all youth enrolled by Mercy Corps at the time of study. We asked 58% of our participants (n = 474), as many youth as could be accommodated, to additionally complete tablet‐based measures of WM and IC at the first time‐point. Given some data losses due to difficult field conditions—including lack of electricity and Internet—450 youth (Syrian refugees, n = 240, 57.5% male; Jordanian non‐refugees, n = 210, 57.6% male) had usable EF data (Table 1).
Data were collected in four cities: Irbid (20.0%), Jarash (37.6%), Mafraq (21.8%), and Zarqa (20.7%), using community centers as a base for operations. Research team members were Syrian and Jordanian women with relevant degrees and ≥3 years of experience working with refugee children. They received extensive training on the Rapid Assessment of Cognitive and Emotional Regulation (RACER) tasks. Written informed consent was obtained from community leaders and primary caregivers, and verbal assent from adolescents. The study was approved by the Prime Minister’s Office of Jordan and Yale University. We conducted assessments in a private location; during a single session (~50 min), adolescents completed survey instruments, as well as tablet‐based RACER tasks (first IC, then WM), optimized for use in LMIC and humanitarian settings (Ford, Kim, Brown, Aber, & Sheridan, 2019).
For the predictor variables, we used a checklist of 12 household items to measure poverty (Panter‐Brick, Eggerman, Gonzalez, & Safdar, 2009; Panter‐Brick, Hadfield, et al., 2018); the Traumatic Events Checklist to measure trauma (Panter‐Brick et al., 2009); the eight‐item Child Revised Impact of Events Scale (Smith, Perrin, Yule, Hacam, & Stuvland, 2002) to measure PTSD symptoms; and the Human Insecurity Scale, developed in the West Bank for Palestinian youth (Hamayel, Ghandour, Abu Rmeileh & Giacaman, 2014; Ziadni et al., 2011) to measure fear, worry, and perceived insecurity. For moderators, we used the Arabic Child and Youth Resilience Measure to measure resilience (Panter‐Brick, Hadfield, et al., 2018; Ungar & Liebenberg, 2011) and asked Syrian caregivers when they had arrived in Jordan to measure recency of forced migration. For sociodemographic covariates, we recorded refugee status, age, gender, child education, mother education, and father education. The first three were self‐reported, while the latter three were reported by caregivers (see Appendix S1).
The tablet‐based RACER cognitive tasks took 2–4 min each. While participants had limited experience with these tablet‐based games, they were broadly familiar with computerized games (32.0% had a computer and 78.8% had a cell phone at home). To learn the rules of each game, adolescents completed practice trials until reaching 75% accuracy or practicing four times—whichever came first. We assessed IC and WM by measuring performance on “challenge trials” while controlling for performance on baseline trials. Thus, our measure of IC and WM is the decrement in performance caused by increasing the IC or WM task demands, controlling for all other aspects of baseline task performance, such as visual–spatial ability or familiarity with computerized games. Performance on baseline trials (Table S1) was not central to our investigation.
Inhibitory Control
The RACER IC game is based on the “Simon task” (O’Leary & Barber, 1993; Simon & Rudell, 1967). On each trial of 60 trials, the adolescent was shown a dot on either the left or the right side of the screen, appearing in a random order (Figure S1a). On half the trials, the adolescent was presented with a solid dot, which participants were taught to touch as fast as possible (baseline trials). On the other half, a striped dot was presented; participants were taught to touch on the opposite side of the screen in this instance (challenge trials; Figure S1a). Dots were presented in a pseudorandom order, counterbalanced for side of screen and condition. Dots were presented for 2.5 s or as soon as the respondent touched the screen, whichever came first. Each trial was followed by a 0.5 s intertrial interval. The outcome for this task is accuracy, a variable indicating whether participants got the trial wrong or right (0 or 1 for each trial). Higher values indicate better accuracy.
Working Memory
The RACER WM task is a variant of the widely used “spatial delayed match to sample task” (Goldman‐Rakic, 1996; Thomason et al., 2009). In each of 42 trials, participants were shown a screen that displayed one, two, or three dots for 2 s and instructed to remember where the dot(s) were located (Figure S2b). The screen then went blank (for 0.1 or 3 s), after which it changed color indicating that participants should touch the screen precisely where the dot(s) had appeared. Because remembering one item is easier than remembering multiple items, higher load trials are understood to tax WM more than lower load trials. One, two, and three load trials were presented an equal number of times, as were 0.1‐ and 3‐s delays. Conditions were fully counterbalanced. The primary outcome for the WM task is precision (log‐transformed), a continuous variable reflecting the distance between the participants’ touch and the target location on each trial. Lower values indicate better precision.
Analysis
We examined bivariate associations between all variables of interest in the population (Table S2) and for refugees and non‐refugees (Table S3). We fit two regression models using generalized estimating equations with SPSS v.24 (IBM, Portsmouth, UK): one predicting IC and one predicting WM. Data were entered at the level of trial, with 60 trials per person for the IC models and 42 trials per person for the WM models. Cognitive trials were clustered within participants. We controlled for performance on each EF task at baseline, at the person level. For example, the measure of participants’ WM skill is their precision on the multiple‐dot trials, when controlling for their performance on the one‐dot trials (baseline). This is a strength of our analysis; each experimental test having multiple trials, we controlled for each participant’s baseline performance for EF outcomes.
We tested the effect of trial type (opposite‐ or same‐side in the IC task, number of balls in the WM task) on performance, regardless of person‐level predictors (i.e., childhood adversities, sociodemographic covariates; Table 2). We tested which covariates predicted our outcomes (IC and WM), keeping those with p < .05 for either IC or WM for all subsequent analyses. Baseline task performance and refugee status were included as covariates in all analyses, as were gender and child education (other covariates were not significant; Model 1, Table 3). We also tested four person‐level indicators of adversity (poverty, trauma exposure, PTSD, and human insecurity) together in a single model (Model 2, Table 3), then individually (Table S4). Based on initial associations, we tested for outcome differences by gender and PTSD (Table S5), and for interactions with resilience (Table S6) and time displaced in Jordan (Table S7).
Table 2.
Task |
Baseline M (SD) |
Challenge M (SD) |
p |
---|---|---|---|
Inhibitory control (IC) | 0.96 (0.11) | 0.84 (0.27) | < .001 |
Working memory (WM) | 52.65 (31.75) | 68.27 (23.94) | < .001 |
The measure for IC is the performance across trials, where 0 is an incorrect and 1 is a correct response on each trial. The measure for WM is the average distance deviated from the target (in pixels) across trials. Differences between baseline and challenge trials were compared using paired samples t‐tests.
Table 3.
Measure of adversity | EF | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Inhibitory control (IC) | Working memory (WM) | |||||
β (SE) | 95% CI | p | β (SE) | 95% CI | p | |
Model 1 | ||||||
Baseline task performance | 0.12 (0.06) | 0.003, 0.25, | .045 | 15.74 (1.52) | 12.77, 18.72 | < .001 |
Refugee status | −0.05 (0.20) | −0.44, 0.34 | .813 | −1.65 (1.61) | −4.80, 1.51 | .307 |
Age | −0.02 (0.19) | −0.35, 0.38 | .931 | 0.68 (1.21) | −1.69, 3.04 | .575 |
Gender | 0.47 (0.19) | 0.08, 0.86 | .017 | −8.22 (1.58) | −11.32, −5.12 | < .001 |
Child education | −0.06 (0.18) | −0.41, 0.30 | .756 | −3.67 (1.24) | −6.11, −1.23 | .003 |
Mother education | 0.37 (0.26) | −0.15, 0.88 | .165 | −3.18 (1.90) | −6.90, 0.54 | .094 |
Father education | −0.06 (0.29) | −0.62, 0.50, | .827 | −1.46 (2.06) | −5.36, 2.50 | .477 |
Model 2 | ||||||
Baseline task performance | 0.14 (0.08) | −0.01, 0.29 | .067 | 15.27 (1.15) | 13.02, 17.53 | < .001 |
Refugee status | −0.19 (0.38) | −0.93, 0.55 | .614 | −5.33 (2.82) | −10.86, 0.20 | .059 |
Gender | 0.57 (0.24) | 0.11, 1.04 | .016 | −7.46 (1.76) | −10.90, −4.03 | < .001 |
Child education | 0.02 (0.18) | −0.24, 0.21 | .868 | −3.15 (1.04) | −5.19, −1.11 | .002 |
Household wealth | −0.14 (0.16) | −0.44, 0.17 | .379 | −2.30 (1.13) | −4.52, −0.09 | .041 |
War‐related trauma exposure | −0.09 (0.19) | −0.46, 0.27 | .616 | −0.94 (1.48) | −3.84, 1.95 | .523 |
PTSD | 0.07 (0.25) | −0.41, 0.56 | .768 | 0.26 (2.19) | −4.02, 4.55 | .904 |
Human insecurity | −0.01 (0.11) | −0.22, 0.21 | .961 | 0.69 (0.79) | −0.86, 2.24 | .384 |
Model 1 tests which covariates predict EF in the combined sample. Model 2 controls for the significant covariates from Model 1 (refugee status, child gender, and gender education) to test the extent to which measures of adversity predict EF. For refugee status, Jordanians are the reference group. For gender, female is the reference group. For parents’ education, primary school or less is the reference group. Higher scores indicate greater household wealth. For posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), not having symptoms consistent with PTSD is the reference group. Models present standardized coefficients. IC has a binary logistic outcome; WM has a linear outcome (log of the distance deviated). A higher score indicates better IC; a lower score indicates better WM. Bold values indicate p<.05.
Results
As expected, Syrian refugees experienced more adversity (as measured by poverty, trauma events, PTSD, and insecurity) compared to non‐refugees (Table 1). Specifically, Syrian refugees differed from Jordanian peers with respect to material wealth (6.65 vs. 10.10 household items), exposure to war‐related trauma (5.81 vs. 0.67 events), PTSD (65.0% vs. 16.2%), and human insecurity (68.25 vs. 62.91), p < .05 (Figure S2). While similar in age and gender breakdown, the Syrian refugees had completed fewer years in school (6.60 vs. 7.78), and their mothers (35.0% vs. 8.6%) and fathers (30.1% vs. 11.9%) were more likely to have reached only primary education, compared to the Jordanians (Table 1).
Trial Type
Trial type impacted task performance on both tasks. Average IC performance was 12.2% less accurate on opposite‐side trials than same‐side trials (p < .001, Table 2). For WM, average precision on trials with multiple dots deviated on average 15.62 pixels more than trials with only one dot (p < .001, Table 2).
Contrary to expectations, no associations were found between IC and poverty, trauma exposure, PTSD, or human insecurity (Table 3) in the overall population. Only gender was associated with IC: Controlling for all other variables, males showed better performance than females (Table 3). By contrast, WM was associated with poverty, as well as gender and child education (Table 3). Thus, participants with greater household wealth had better performance on the WM task (β = −2.30, p = .041, Table 3); this effect was not moderated by levels of resilience (p > .05, Table S5). The association is negative, because smaller values for WM indicate less distance between touch and target, thereby better performance. Controlling for all other variables, boys and youth with more years of schooling performed better on the WM task. There were no significant associations between WM and levels of trauma exposure, PTSD, or human insecurity.
We examined these same associations separately for refugees and non‐refugees. Results for IC were the same for Syrian refugees as those for the overall sample. Poverty, male gender, and years of schooling were associated with WM (Table 4). The wealthier the household, the better the WM task performance (β = −2.38, p = .045), an association that was not moderated by resilience or time since forced displacement to Jordan (p > .05, Tables S6 and S7). There were no associations with levels of trauma exposure, PTSD, or human insecurity (Table 4). For Jordanian non‐refugees, we found no significant associations, save for boys performing better on IC and WM tasks, and participants with more years of schooling performing better on WM (Table 4).
Table 4.
Measure of adversity | EF | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Inhibitory control (IC) | Working memory (WM) | |||||
β (SE) | 95% CI | p | β (SE) | 95% CI | p | |
Syrian refugees | ||||||
Baseline task performance | 0.21 (0.10) | 0.02, 0.40 | .034 | 16.54 (1.86) | 12.90, 20.19 | < .001 |
Gender | 0.44 (0.33) | −0.20, 1.08 | .177 | −7.48 (2.44) | −12.26, −2.70 | .002 |
Child education | 0.02 (0.16) | −0.28, 0.32 | .894 | −3.03 (1.48) | −5.94, −0.12 | .041 |
Household wealth | −0.12 (0.20) | −0.51, 0.26 | .527 | −2.38 (1.19) | −5.94, −0.12 | .045 |
War‐related trauma exposure | −0.02 (0.21) | −0.43, 0.40 | .934 | −1.38 (1.41) | −4.15, 1.38 | .327 |
PTSD | 0.26 (0.28) | −0.29, 0.81 | .355 | −0.42 (2.56) | −5.44, 4.59 | .869 |
Human insecurity | −0.12 (0.15) | 0.42, 0.18 | .447 | 1.45 (1.05) | −0.60, 3.50 | .166 |
Jordanian non‐refugees | ||||||
Baseline task performance | 0.06 (0.11) | −0.15, 0.26 | .601 | 13.78 (1.80) | 10.26, 17.31 | < .001 |
Gender | 0.73 (0.35) | 0.04, 1.42 | .039 | −8.66 (2.57) | −13.70, −3.62 | .001 |
Child education | −0.13 (0.18) | −0.48, 0.22 | .470 | −3.20 (1.24) | −5.64, −0.77 | .010 |
Household wealth | −0.14 (0.30) | −0.73, 0.44 | .631 | −1.66 (1.25) | −4.12, 0.79 | .185 |
War‐related trauma exposure | −0.30 (0.39) | −1.07, 0.47 | .447 | 0.13 (1.79) | −3.38, 3.65 | .940 |
PTSD | −0.21 (0.43) | −1.04, 0.63 | .630 | 0.08 (4.41) | −8.55, 8.71 | .986 |
Human insecurity | 0.13 (0.17) | −0.30, 0.45 | .447 | 0.01 (1.20) | −2.34, 2.36 | .994 |
For gender, female is the reference group. Higher scores indicate greater household wealth. For posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), not having symptoms consistent with PTSD is the reference group. Models present standardized coefficients. IC has a binary logistic outcome; WM has a linear outcome (log of the distance deviated). A higher score indicates better IC; a lower score indicates better WM. Bold values indicate p<.05.
Discussion
This study is the first to disentangle the relative impact of poverty, violence, PTSD, and human insecurity on EF, for adolescents in a LMIC. It draws on measures in a population of 450 refugee and non‐refugee adolescents, sampled at the community level, in cities close to an active war zone. Syrian refugees had experienced what risk and resilience researchers consider to be “extreme adversity and stress” (Masten et al., 2012, p. 381). By contrast, Jordanian peers experienced less exposure to both poverty and the violence of war. We measured EF using computerized assessments (RACER tasks), observing task manipulation effects similar to those observed in Lebanon (Ford et al., 2019), Niger (Ford et al., 2019), and the United States (Sheridan, Kharitonova, Martin, Chatterjee & Gabrieli, 2014).
Recent work based on the United States has demonstrated a disassociation between trauma and poverty in predicting EF, whereby exposure to poverty but not traumatic violence predicts EF task performance (Lambert et al., 2016; Sheridan et al., 2017). This disassociation had never been tested in a sample with high levels of war trauma exposure and related ongoing stressors, as we did here. Consistent with such work, we did not observe associations between levels of traumatic events, PTSD, or insecurity and EF. Also, we did not observe group‐level differences between refugees and non‐refugees in EF, suggesting that war‐related trauma did not negatively impact EF.
We did find that participants in relatively poorer households performed worse on the WM task, controlling for baseline task performance, refugee status, gender, years of schooling, and other adversities (trauma exposure, PTSD, and insecurity). This finding remained significant within the most violence‐exposed group, Syrian refugees. We did not observe this association among Jordanian non‐refugees, potentially because there was not enough variance in the distribution of household wealth in this subgroup (Figure S1). We also observed that male gender and years of education were associated with better WM in both samples. These findings are consistent with other studies in LMICs showing that household material wealth, gender, and schooling can influence learning outcomes, such as language and mathematics skills, in middle childhood and early adolescence (Boyden, Dawes, & Tredoux, 2018; Obermeyer, Bott, & Sassine, 2015). However, it is striking that even with very high levels of traumatic violence exposure, we replicate distinctions between poverty and violence observed in US‐based samples (Sheridan et al., 2017). This confirms the importance of distinguishing between types of adversity exposure (McLaughlin & Sheridan, 2016).
While consistent with recent studies of U.S. children living in adversity, our finding that poverty, and not exposure to violence and trauma, worsened WM was somewhat unexpected, given the high levels of war exposure in Syrian refugees. Our study suggests that where young minds are under siege, it is the siege of ongoing poverty that is most important to skills of EF.
Limitations
This study had many strengths, including assessments with a gender‐balanced, representative sample, using regionally validated measures of mental health and psychosocial well‐being (Panter‐Brick, Dajani, et al., 2018; Panter‐Brick, Hadfield, et al., 2018) and measures of EF sensitive to the demands of cognitive testing in under‐resourced areas (Ford et al., 2019). It also has several limitations. First, we relied on a small set of measures to assess EF. The RACER tasks do not represent the entirety of adolescents’ EF, and we did not measure other potentially important variables, such as IQ. Additional or longer tasks may have led to a more reliable estimate of each adolescent’s EF capability. Second, the RACER battery does not include normative interpretations of where EF levels would be for same‐aged adolescents: Our results should be interpreted as relative differences in WM and IC, rather than absolute measures. Third, our measure of war‐related trauma exposure was based on self‐reports of lifetime traumatic, frightening, and distressing events, data which can be biased by memory and PTSD experiences (Panter‐Brick, Grimon, Kalin, & Eggerman, 2015). Fourth, Syrian refugees in our cohort had been displaced for an average 3 years; it is possible that more nuanced measures, for more recently displaced youth, would find associations between war‐related violence and EF.
Conclusion
Documenting the impacts of war and forced displacement on cognitive function contributes to a growing understanding of the ontology of EF skills. This in turn may guide approaches to interventions designed to help young refugees. This study is the first to identify predictors of EF skills in war‐affected adolescents using computerized task performance. It underscores the need to carefully document which dimensions of childhood adversity matter for EF. We find that the disassociation between violence and poverty in predicting EF, found in US‐based research, is also found in adolescents affected by the Syrian crisis. This would suggest that even in populations exposed to substantial violence and fear, poverty is uniquely associated with EF development.
Supporting information
This research was funded by Elrha’s Research for Health in Humanitarian Crises (R2HC) Programme (https://www.elrha.org/project/yale-psychosocial-call2/), which aims to improve health outcomes by strengthening the evidence base for public health interventions in humanitarian crises (elrha.org/r2hc). The R2HC programme is funded equally by the Wellcome Trust and the UK Government (DFID). We thank Jon Kurtz, Noura Shahed, and Natasha Shawarib at Mercy Corps; Jane MacPhail, Director of the Advancing Adolescents program implemented in Jordan; and the fieldworkers affiliated to the Taghyeer Organization, especially Dima Hamadmad, Ghufran Abudayyeh, Sana’a Bakeer, and Rahmeh Alhyari.
References
- Blair, C. , & Razza, R. P. (2007). Relating effortful control, executive function, and false belief understanding to emerging math and literacy ability in kindergarten. Child Development, 78, 647–663. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01019.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boyden, J. , Dawes, A. , & Tredoux, C. (2018). Improving children’s chances: Using evidence from four low‐and middle‐income countries to set priorities for the Sustainable Development Goals In Verma S. & Petersen A. C. (Eds.), Developmental science and sustainable development goals for children and youth (pp. 257–275). Cham, Swtizerland: Springer. [Google Scholar]
- Dajani, R. , Hadfield, K. , van Uum, S. , Greff, M. , & Panter‐Brick, C. (2018). Hair cortisol concentrations in war‐affected adolescents: A prospective intervention trial. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 89, 138–146. 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2017.12.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Daneri, M. P. , Blair, C. , Kuhn, L. J. ; FLP Key Investigators . (2018). Maternal language and child vocabulary mediate relations between socioeconomic status and executive function during early childhood. Child Development. 10.1111/cdev.13065 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology, 135–168, 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ford, C. B. , Kim, H. Y. , Brown, L. , Aber, J. L. , & Sheridan, M. A. (2019). A cognitive assessment tool designed for data collection in the field in low‐and middle‐income countries. Research in Comparative and International Education, 14, 141–157. 1177/1745499919829217 [Google Scholar]
- Goldman‐Rakic, P. (1996). Regional and cellular fractionation of working memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 93, 13473–13480. 10.1073/pnas.93.24.13473 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gruszka, A. , Szymura, B. , & Matthews, G. (2010). Handbook of individual differences in cognition: Attention, memory, and executive control. New York, NY: Springer. [Google Scholar]
- Haft, S. L. , & Hoeft, F. (2017). Poverty's impact on children's executive functions: Global considerations. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 2017, 69–79. 10.1002/cad.20220 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hamayel, L. , Ghandour, R. , Abu Rmeileh, N. , & Giacaman, R. (2014). Assessment of the psychosocial health status of adolescents 12–18 years old in the occupied Palestinian Territory: A cross sectional baseline survey. Birzeit, Palestine: Institute of Community and Public Health, Birzeit University. [Google Scholar]
- Lambert, H. K. , King, K. M. , Monahan, K. C. , & McLaughlin, K. A. (2016). Differential associations of threat and deprivation with emotion regulation and cognitive control in adolescence. Development and Psychopathology, 29, 929–940. 10.1017/S0954579416000584 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Masten, A. S. , Herbers, J. E. , Desjardins, C. D. , Cutuli, J. J. , McCormick, C. M. , Sapienza, J. K. , … Zelazo, P. D. (2012). Executive function skills and school success in young children experiencing homelessness. Educational Researcher, 41, 375–384. 10.3102/0013189X12459883 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- McLaughlin, K. A. , & Sheridan, M. A. (2016). Beyond cumulative risk: A dimensional approach to childhood adversity. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 25, 239–245. 10.1177/0963721416655883 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McTeague, L. M. , Goodkind, M. S. , & Etkin, A. (2016). Transdiagnostic impairment of cognitive control in mental illness. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 83, 37–46. 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2016.08.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mercy Corps . (2013). Mapping of host community‐refugee tensions in Mafraq and Ramtha, Jordan. Retrieved from https://data2.unhcr.org/en/documents/download/38301 [Google Scholar]
- Mercy Corps . (2014). Advancing Adolescents approach: Resilience to keep adolescents safe, learning and valued—Lebanon, Jordan & Syria. Author; Retrieved from https://www.mercycorps.org/sites/default/files/Mercy_Corps_Advancing_Adolescents_Approach_Lebanon_Jordan_Syrian-10-2016.pdf [Google Scholar]
- Miller‐Graff, L. E. , & Cummings, E. M. (2017). The Israeli‐Palestinian conflict: Effects on youth adjustment, available interventions, and future research directions. Developmental Review, 43, 1–47. 10.1016/j.dr.2016.10.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Miyake, A. , & Friedman, N. P. (2012). The nature and organization of individual differences in executive functions: Four general conclusions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21, 8–14. 10.1177/0963721411429458 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O’Leary, M. J. , & Barber, P. J. (1993). Interference effects in the Stroop and Simon paradigms. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 19, 830–844. 10.1037/0096-1523.19.4.830 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Obermeyer, C. M. , Bott, S. , & Sassine, A. J. (2015). Arab adolescents: Health, gender, and social context. Journal of Adolescent Health, 57, 252–262. 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2015.01.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Panter‐Brick, C. , Dajani, R. , Eggerman, M. , Hermosilla, S. , Sancilio, A. , & Ager, A. (2018). Insecurity, distress and mental health: Experimental and randomized controlled trials of a psychosocial intervention for youth affected by the Syrian crisis. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 59, 523–541. 10.1111/jcpp.12832/full [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Panter‐Brick, C. , Eggerman, M. , Gonzalez, V. , & Safdar, S. (2009). Violence, suffering, and mental health in Afghanistan: A school‐based survey. Lancet, 374, 807–816. 10.1016/S0140-6736(09)61080-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Panter‐Brick, C. , Grimon, M. , Kalin, M. , & Eggerman, M. (2015). Trauma memories, mental health, and resilience: A prospective study of Afghan youth. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 56, 814–825. 10.1111/jcpp.12350 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Panter‐Brick, C. , Hadfield, K. , Dajani, R. , Eggerman, M. , Ager, A. , & Ungar, M. (2018). Resilience in context: A brief and culturally grounded measure for Syrian refugee and Jordanian host‐community adolescents. Child Development, 89, 1803–1820. 10.1111/cdev.12868/epdf [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Patrick, M. E. , Blair, C. , & Maggs, J. L. (2008). Executive function, approach sensitivity, and emotional decision making as influences on risk behaviors in young adults. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 30, 449–462. 10.1080/13803390701523109 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Qouta, S. , Punamäki, R. L. , & El Sarraj, E. (1995). Relations between traumatic experiences, activity and cognitive and emotional responses among Palestinian children. International Journal of Psychology, 30, 289–304. 10.1080/00207599508246571 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Reed, R. V. , Fazel, M. , Jones, L. , Panter‐Brick, C. , & Stein, A. (2012). Mental health of displaced and refugee children resettled in low and middle‐income countries: Risk and protective factors. The Lancet, 379, 250–265. 10.1016/S0140-6736(11)60050-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Save The Children . (2017). Invisible wounds: The impact of six years of war on the mental health of Syria’s children. Retrieved from https://www.savethechildren.org.uk/content/dam/global/reports/emergency-humanitarian-response/invisible-wounds.pdf [Google Scholar]
- Sheridan, M. , Kharitonova, M. , Martin, R. E. , Chatterjee, A. , & Gabrieli, J. D. (2014). Neural substrates of the development of cognitive control in children ages 5–10 years. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 26, 1840–1850. 10.1162/jocn_a_00597 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sheridan, M. A. , Peverill, M. , Finn, A. S. , & McLaughlin, K. A. (2017). Dimensions of childhood adversity have distinct associations with neural systems underlying executive functioning. Development and Psychopathology, 29, 1777–1794. 10.1017/S0954579417001390 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simon, J. R. , & Rudell, A. P. (1967). Auditory SR compatibility: The effect of an irrelevant cue on information processing. Journal of Applied Psychology, 51, 300–304. 10.1037/h0020586 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith, P. , Perrin, S. , Yule, W. , Hacam, B. , & Stuvland, R. (2002). War exposure among children from Bosnia‐Hercegovina: Psychological adjustment in a community sample. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 15, 147–156. 10.1023/A:1014812209051 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thomason, M. E. , Race, E. , Burrows, B. , Whitfield‐Gabrieli, S. , Glover, G. H. , & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2009). Development of spatial and verbal working memory capacity in the human brain. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21, 316–332. 10.1162/jocn.2008.21028 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ungar, M. , & Liebenberg, L. (2011). Assessing resilience across cultures using mixed methods: Construction of the child and youth resilience measure. Journal of Mixed Methods Research, 5, 126–149. 10.1177/1558689811400607 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- UNHCR . (2019). Global forced displacement tops 70 million. Retrieved from https://www.unhcr.org/news/stories/2019/6/5d08b6614/global-forced-displacement-tops-70-million.html [Google Scholar]
- Verma, S. , & Petersen, A. C. (2018). Developmental science and pathways to sustainable development for children and youth In Verma S. & Petersen A. C. (Eds.), Developmental science and sustainable development goals for children and youth (pp. 1–35). Cham, Swtizerland: Springer. [Google Scholar]
- Wolf, S. , & Suntheimer, N. M. (2019). A dimensional risk approach to assessing early adversity in a national sample. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 62, 270–281. 10.1016/j.appdev.2019.03.004 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Wong, C. W. , Schweitzer, R. D. , & Khawaja, N. G. (2018). Individual, pre‐migration, and post‐settlement factors in predicting academic success of adolescents from refugee backgrounds: A 12‐month follow‐up. Journal of International Migration and Integration, 19, 1095–1117. 10.1007/s12134-018-0589-6 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Ziadni, M. , Hammoudeh, W. , Abu Rmeileh, N. , Hogan, D. , Shannon, H. , & Giacaman, R. (2011). Sources of human insecurity in post‐war situations: The case of Gaza. Journal of Human Security, 7, 23–36. 10.3316/JHS0703023 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.