Abstract
Interoception – the process of sensing bodily signals – has gained much interest in recent years, due to its role in physical and mental well-being. Here, we focus on the role of interoception in social connection, which is a relatively new and growing research area. Studies in this area suggest that interoception may help in appraising physiological signals in social situations, but also that (challenging) social situations may reduce interoceptive processing by shifting attention from internally- to externally- focused. We discuss potential mechanisms for the influence of interoception on social connection and highlight that flexibility in engaging interoception in social situations may be particularly important. We end with a discussion of loneliness – an extreme case of poor social connection, which is associated with physiological decline and increased mortality risk, and propose that interoceptive dysregulation is involved. We suggest that interventions aimed to improve interoceptive abilities, such as mindfulness-based meditation practices, may be key for alleviating loneliness and improving social connection.
Keywords: interoception, health, emotion, loneliness, social cognition, social connection
Introduction
Interoception – the process of sensing bodily signals – holds importance for physiological functioning (Craig, 2015). In recent years, there has been a surge of interest in interoception because of its impact on physical health (Quadt et al., 2018), mental health (Khalsa et al., 2018), as well as emotional functioning in general (Critchley and Garfinkel, 2017). Here, we focus on a growing literature investigating the link between interoception and social connection. This represents an important focus within the broader area of how interoception influences many types of social phenomena, or “social interoception.” We review some illustrative studies and discuss some potential mechanisms of this link. We then end with a discussion about why understanding the role of interoception in social connection is important for physical and mental health, with a focus on loneliness.
Social Interoception
Before turning to the role of interoception in social connection, we define interoception as the sensing of internal physiological states of the body (e.g., hunger, micturition, thirst, temperature), which serves as a means of regulating and maintaining homeostatic conditions (Craig, 2009; Sterling, 2011). Interoception involves the central processing of bottom-up, afferent signals from the body along with top-down regulatory directives. These physiological signals may (or may not) be represented as subjective feelings, and then may lead to behaviors to adjust the current state. Interoception is functionally distinct from exteroceptive senses (e.g., vision, audition), as well as proprioception (i.e., sensing the position of muscles/joints), although it interacts with these senses through multimodal sensory integration (Craig, 2015). A commonplace example of interoception is sensing a full bladder. In a healthy human, this results in a subjective feeling, with the behavioral adjustment being the action of emptying the bladder (though note that in some cases such behavior may bypass conscious feeling and volitional action; Griffiths, 2015).
But how does interception function in social situations? Take the example of a threatening social interaction. Here, the bodily response might entail an increase in heart rate – a reaction that serves to enhance alertness (e.g., preparing one to either argue one’s case or walk away). These physiological changes may (or may not) be felt and they may (or may not) generate affect, as we elaborate below. Interestingly, there is also a prediction component to this process and the role of “predictive interoceptive coding” is gaining understanding, but outside the scope of this review (see Barrett et al., 2016).
Dimensions of Interoception
Before reviewing studies that have investigated the relationship between interoception and social connection, we describe dimensions of interoception and ways to measure it in the laboratory. This is to highlight the multidimensionality of the construct. Garfinkel et al. (2015) define three key dimensions. Two are more objective – interoceptive accuracy (IAcc) and interoceptive awareness (IAw). One is more subjective – interoceptive sensibility (IS). Each of these are separate dimensions and do not necessarily correlate with one another (e.g., see Garfinkel et al., 2016).
Interoceptive accuracy refers to an individual’s ability to shift attention internally and accurately track their physiological state. The most commonly used task involves individuals monitoring their heartbeat by asking them to either count their number of heartbeats during variable time intervals, referred to as heartbeat counting (HBC1), or determine whether an external signal is synchronous with their own heartbeat, referred to as heartbeat discrimination. Note that unless stated otherwise, the IAcc studies discussed in this review used HBC.
Interoceptive awareness is a metacognitive construct. It is assessed by conducting the IAcc task, as well as asking individuals to rate their confidence in their guesses, collecting data across many trials, and testing the correlation between trial-by-trial IAcc scores and confidence ratings. A strong correlation indicates one has good metacognitive awareness of interoceptive accuracy.
Interoceptive sensibility is one’s subjective tendency to perceive, appraise, and use physiological signals, as measured by self-report questionnaires. One such questionnaire is the MAIA (Mehling et al., 2012) which has eight subscales, including: noticing, not-distracting, not-worrying, attention regulation, emotional awareness, self-regulation, body listening, and body trusting. These subscales index different attentional and appraisal “styles” with regard to processing and using physiological signals. Research shows that going beyond IAcc and IAw and distinguishing various attentional and regulatory aspects of interoception is important for understanding its role in typical social processing and psychopathological conditions (Mehling, 2016).
The Role of Interoception in Social Connection
There are several studies suggesting an intriguing link between interoception and social connection. The first type of research is correlational in nature, showing that trait interoceptive ability (IAcc) is associated with social reactivity. One study used the Trier social stress test to induce negative affect through impromptu public speaking (Werner et al., 2009), while another study subjected participants to social exclusion in a conversation with confederates, which is also presumably stressful (Werner et al., 2013). In both studies, people higher in IAcc reported less negative affect after the challenging social situation, despite their physiological reactivity being comparable to participants with lower IAcc. Such results are consistent with the idea that it is not the physiological response per se that causes social stress (in the form of negative affect), but one’s subjective appraisal of it (Kudielka et al., 2007). This leads to the interesting idea that perhaps greater IAcc allows one to identify the physiological response as resulting from an objective, external “social situation” rather than an attribute of oneself. This could reflect better emotional regulation in social situations (Werner et al., 2013). Finally, another study induced social exclusion with the Cyberball game and similarly found that higher IAcc was associated with less negative affect as well as behavioral affiliation tendency, as measured by preferred interpersonal distance (Pollatos et al., 2015). Recently, conflicting (null) results on the effect of IAcc buffering against negative affect from Cyberball were found – although these researchers used different measures for independent and dependent variables for affect, this may underscore the (contextual) complexity of interoceptive contributions to emotional resilience (Zamariola et al., 2019).
Although not directly manipulating social interaction, another intriguing study showed how trait interoceptive accuracy (IAcc) relates to peripersonal space – the region just surrounding one’s body in which multisensory integration is heightened for salient processing (Ferri et al., 2013). This study showed that the higher IAcc is associated with greater autonomic reactivity in response to seeing another person’s hand entering their peripersonal space (i.e., 20 cm from their own hand). Critically, this result was not seen when the entering object is a metal stick. Given that an approaching human hand is a proxy for social connection, this result is consistent with the possibility that interoception facilitates noticing of a potential social connection. Interestingly, social context has also been reported to shape the mapping of peripersonal space – becoming more expansive and inclusive when participants interact with someone perceived to be cooperative or moral (Teneggi et al., 2013; Ardizzi and Ferri, 2018; Pellencin et al., 2018). These results open the door for the use of peripersonal space tasks as a way to measure the degree of social connection in future studies, an idea we return to below.
The second type of research addressing the link between interoception and social connection (or lack thereof) is experimental in nature, showing that social situations can elicit acute changes in interoceptive ability. One study found that social exclusion (manipulated by the Cyberball game) caused an acute decrease in IAcc, compared to baseline levels (Durlik and Tsakiris, 2015). These results can be explained by a “shifting” of attention from interoceptive to exteroceptive processing, increasing social attention as a means to re-affiliate after being excluded (Powers and Heatherton, 2012), which we expand upon below. It is important to note that a decrease in IAcc in response to social exclusion is not necessarily contradictory to findings showing that reaction to social exclusion is less negative in people with higher trait IAcc (described above). After all, it is entirely possible that both are true: (i) better IAcc skills allow one to properly appraise one’s internal state in response to social exclusion, and (ii) social exclusion leads to an overall reduction in IAcc due to a switch from internal to external (social) attention. These considerations bring us to a broader discussion of mechanisms linking interoception and social connection, below.
Mechanisms for Linking Interoception With Social Connection: Enhanced Emotional Discernment and Attentional Switching
Given that there are links between interoception and social processing, what might be the underlying mechanisms? One idea, which we refer to as the “enhanced emotional discernment hypothesis,” is that better ability to feel bodily reactions translates to having a richer emotional experience. In turn, richer emotional experience may facilitate greater understanding of others’ emotions, and empathy – the process of understanding, sharing, and/or responding to others’ emotions (Decety, 2011; Zaki, 2014). Such empathic understanding and emotion sharing should then facilitate social connection. There is some evidence that is consistent with this hypothesis. First, with respect to whether heightened interoception is associated with rich emotional experience, it has been reported that people higher in IAcc tend to feel emotions more intensely (Barrett et al., 2004) and use emotion regulation strategies of reappraisal and suppression more frequently (Kever et al., 2015). In addition, interoceptive dysregulation appears to underlie alexithymia – difficulty identifying one’s own emotions and sensations (Brewer et al., 2016). Alexithymia is a common hallmark of psychological disorders such as Autism Spectrum Conditions that present with social difficulties (Bird and Cook, 2013). With respect to the connection between understanding one’s own emotions and those of others, a meta-analysis has shown that alexithymia predicts difficulties in recognizing emotions in others (Grynberg et al., 2012).
Two other studies more directly tie interoceptive abilities with feeling emotions of others. Grynberg and Pollatos (2015) found that IAcc is positively correlated with the degree to which one reports experiencing cognitive and affective empathy. In a similar vein, Terasawa et al. (2014) found that IAcc is positively associated with the degree of emotional contagion participants experience when identifying and rating faces expressing joy and sadness, but not anger and disgust. Interestingly, joy and sadness seem to provide the greatest potential for social understanding and connection (Hess and Fischer, 2013). Consistently, spontaneous mimicry of other people’s expressions of joy is reduced in people who are socially disconnected – i.e., lonely (Arnold and Winkielman, unpublished). This result, in contrast to the last, may suggest interoceptive dysregulation in loneliness – a topic we explore below. In short, such findings are consistent with the possibility that interoceptive ability facilitates social connection. In fact, one can imagine a positive feedback loop – once one’s social life starts to “feel right,” one can start to trust/rely on their interoceptive signals even more in order to learn from social affective experience.
A second possible mechanism, which we refer to as the “attentional switching hypothesis,” supposes that if one’s attention is drawn toward external events (including social situations that are challenging), this may reduce attentional resources for monitoring internal states (i.e., interoception). A few lines of evidence support this view. As described above, studies have shown that social exclusion causes reduction in interoceptive abilities (IAcc). Another study demonstrated that repeated exposure to angry faces leads to a decrease on a neural measure of interoceptive processing – the heartbeat-evoked brain potential (Pollatos and Schandry, 2004), and also an increase in visually evoked potential to the angry faces (Marshall et al., 2018). Given that the heartbeat-evoked potential indexes interoceptive processing, these data provide more direct evidence for the switching hypothesis – i.e., challenging social situations lead to an acute switch from interoceptive to exteroceptive attention. Another relevant study showed that administering oxytocin, which is implicated in social salience (Shamay-Tsoory and Abu-Akel, 2016), decreases IAcc in participants when they are being presented with emotional faces, but not in a control condition when they are exposed to a blank screen (Yao et al., 2018). Given that oxytocin increases social attention, these results are also consistent with exteroceptive social attention drawing resources away from interoceptive focus.
Since the switching hypothesis suggests a tradeoff between internal and external attention, it is important to consider which may best serve social connection. We propose that the key to interoception facilitating social connection is the ability to flexibly shift between interoceptive (emotional signals) attention and exteroceptive (social) attention. For example, when encountering a stranger or developing a friendship, it is important to be able to adaptively (and quickly) read the emotions and intentions of another person, as well as one’s own reactions to them. Interestingly, Ainley et al. (2016) recently proposed that IAcc scores reflect the ability to adaptively weight interoceptive signals over others competing for attention, which is in line with our account. One possible way to test the effectiveness of interoceptive vs. exteroceptive attention might involve directly instructing participants to shift from one to the other, and seeing how that affects social connection (measured with peripersonal space, for example). Interacting attentional modalities, as well as attentional field fluctuations within peripersonal space, that are potentially involved in a social situation are depicted in Figure 1. Note that these attentional investments can be dynamic and interactive.
Social Connection in Mental and Physical Health: Potential for Interoception Interventions
Social connection and belonging motivate much of human behavior and cognition (Baumeister and Leary, 1995). Social connection is required for health, with some of the best evidence coming from studies on loneliness – perceived social isolation – which results when one’s social relationships do not fulfill one’s desired level of social connectedness (Cacioppo and Patrick, 2008). The feeling of loneliness, thus, is subjective and distinct from objective measures of social isolation. Given the strong human need for social connection, it is not surprising that loneliness is associated with physiological decline and increased mortality risk (Luo et al., 2012). In longitudinal studies that track the onset of negative psychological states, loneliness has been shown to predict increases in depression, but not vice versa (Cacioppo et al., 2010), and shows a reciprocal relationship with subjective well-being (VanderWeele et al., 2012). Loneliness is also associated with low self-reported empathy (Beadle et al., 2012), which could imply interoceptive dysregulation. Given the negative health consequences of poor social connectedness, it is important to consider ways to alleviate (increasing) loneliness in society (Will, 2018).
With regard to the above “attentional switching hypothesis,” could loneliness involve heightened exteroceptive attention, and reduced interoceptive attention? Interestingly, Cacioppo and Hawkley (2009) proposed an (exteroceptive) attentional bias as key to maintaining loneliness and making it hard to overcome: hypervigilance for social threat (Cacioppo and Hawkley, 2009). This implicit attentional bias focuses on negative over positive social stimuli, and may coexist with altered interoceptive attention. We speculate that in chronic loneliness, the maladaptive attentional focus that shifts processing away from positive social signals to external negative social signals, could also reduce the degree and flexibility of interoceptive processing.
In order to facilitate social sensitivity and reduce loneliness, we suggest that improving one’s interoceptive abilities may be key. This idea also follows from the “enhanced emotional discernment hypothesis,” as outline above. Being able to accurately “tune in” to one’s own internal (emotional) states and properly used them in social judgments may improve social connection and decrease loneliness. In fact, data from our lab show a strong negative correlation between IS (as assessed with the MAIA scales) and loneliness (as assessed with the UCLA questionnaire). Interestingly, the subscale that most strongly predicts loneliness is “body trusting,” suggesting that trust (appraisal of acceptance) in one’s bodily signals may help “buffer” one from loneliness (Arnold and Dobkins, unpublished). As mentioned, trust and proper use of bodily signals could facilitate adaptive learning from social and affective experiences.
If good interoception does buffer against loneliness, interventions to reduce loneliness should focus on improving interoception. While loneliness reduction interventions remain difficult (Cacioppo et al., 2015), some of the most effective interventions have used mindfulness-based stress reduction (MBSR) techniques (Creswell et al., 2012; Lindsay et al., 2019). Similar meditation techniques have been shown to improve both IS (measured with MAIA; Bornemann et al., 2015) and interoceptive accuracy (Bornemann and Singer, 2017). This suggests that the effectiveness of MBSR in reducing loneliness may be mediated in part by improvements in interoception. Of course, mindfulness involves multiple cognitive and affective processes and influences many different aspects of subjective and social experience (Singer and Engert, 2019). Plainly, more research is needed to identify specific mechanisms associated with loneliness and poor interoception that could be targeted in further interventions for psychosocial function. Nevertheless, it appears that the growing field of “social interoception” represents a fruitful area of research for better understanding and treating not only loneliness, but other psychopathological conditions, as well as improving social health for all.
Author Contributions
AA conceived the study and drafted the manuscript, which was written with KD and PW. All authors agreed to the current form of the manuscript.
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
We thank Ashley Woodward for assistance with Figure 1, as well as colleagues who provided feedback on earlier versions of the manuscript.
Footnotes
Recently, some researchers expressed concerns about HBC as a measure of interoceptive accuracy because knowledge of one’s own resting heart rate may confound HBC performance (Ring et al., 2015). Specifically, participants tend to underestimate their heartbeat counts and re-test reliability is not high (Zamariola et al., 2018). We appreciate this point but highlight other qualitative dimensions of interoception as relevant to social cognition, in addition to citing papers that successfully used HBC as a measure of interoceptive accuracy.
References
- Ainley V., Apps M. A. J., Fotopoulou A., Tsakiris M. (2016). ‘Bodily precision’: a predictive coding account of individual differences in interoceptive accuracy. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond B Biol. Sci. 371:20160003. 10.1098/rstb.2016.0003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ardizzi M., Ferri F. (2018). Interoceptive influences on peripersonal space boundary. Cognition 177 79–86. 10.1016/j.cognition.2018.04.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barrett L. F., Quigley K. S., Bliss-Moreau E., Aronson K. R. (2004). Interoceptive sensitivity and self-reports of emotional experience. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 87 684–697. 10.1037/0022-3514.87.5.684 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barrett L. F., Quigley K. S., Hamilton P. (2016). An active inference theory of allostasis and interoception in depression. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 371:20160011. 10.1098/rstb.2016.0011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baumeister R. F., Leary M. R. (1995). The need to belong: desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychol. Bull. 117 497–529. 10.1037//0033-2909.117.3.497 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beadle J. N., Brown V., Keady B., Tranel D., Paradiso S. (2012). Trait empathy as a predictor of individual differences in perceived loneliness. Psychol. Rep. 110 3–15. 10.2466/07.09.20.PR0.110.1.3-15 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bird G., Cook R. (2013). Mixed emotions: the contribution of alexithymia to the emotional symptoms of autism. Transl. Psychiatry 3:e285. 10.1038/tp.2013.61 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bornemann B., Herbert B. M., Mehling W. E., Singer T. (2015). Differential changes in self-reported aspects of interoceptive awareness through 3 months of contemplative training. Front. Psychol. 5:1504. 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01504 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bornemann B., Singer T. (2017). Taking time to feel our body: steady increases in heartbeat perception accuracy and decreases in alexithymia over 9 months of contemplative mental training: contemplative training improves interoception. Psychophysiology 54 469–482. 10.1111/psyp.12790 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brewer R., Cook R., Bird G. (2016). Alexithymia: a general deficit of interoception. R. Soc. open sci. 3:150664. 10.1098/rsos.150664 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cacioppo J. T., Hawkley L. C. (2009). Perceived social isolation and cognition. Trends Cogn. Sci. 13 447–454. 10.1016/j.tics.2009.06.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cacioppo J. T., Hawkley L. C., Thisted R. A. (2010). Perceived social isolation makes me sad: 5-year cross-lagged analyses of loneliness and depressive symptomatology in the Chicago health. Psychol. Aging 25 453–463. 10.1037/a0017216 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cacioppo J. T., Patrick W. (2008). Loneliness: Human Nature and the Need for Social Connection. New York, NY: W. W. Norton & Company. [Google Scholar]
- Cacioppo S., Grippo A. J., London S., Goossens L., Cacioppo J. T. (2015). Loneliness: clinical import and interventions. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 10 238–249. 10.1177/1745691615570616 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Craig A. D. (2009). How do you feel — now? the anterior insula and human awareness. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10 59–70. 10.1038/nrn2555 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Craig A. D. (2015). How Do You Feel? An Interoceptive Moment With Your Neurobiological Self. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Creswell J. D., Irwin M. R., Burklund L. J., Lieberman M. D., Arevalo J. M. G., Ma J., et al. (2012). Mindfulness-based stress reduction training reduces loneliness and pro-inflammatory gene expression in older adults: a small randomized controlled trial. Brain Behav. Immun. 26 1095–1101. 10.1016/j.bbi.2012.07.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Critchley H. D., Garfinkel S. N. (2017). Interoception and emotion. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 17 7–14. 10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.04.020 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Decety J. (2011). Dissecting the neural mechanisms mediating empathy. Emot. Rev. 3 92–108. 10.1177/1754073910374662 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Durlik C., Tsakiris M. (2015). Decreased interoceptive accuracy following social exclusion. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 96 57–63. 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2015.02.020 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ferri F., Ardizzi M., Ambrosecchia M., Gallese V. (2013). Closing the gap between the inside and the outside: interoceptive sensitivity and social distances. PLoS One 8:e75758. 10.1371/journal.pone.0075758 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garfinkel S. N., Seth A. K., Barrett A. B., Suzuki K., Critchley H. D. (2015). Knowing your own heart: distinguishing interoceptive accuracy from interoceptive awareness. Biol. Psychol. 104 65–74. 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2014.11.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garfinkel S. N., Tiley C., O’Keeffe S., Harrison N. A., Seth A. K., Critchley H. D. (2016). Discrepancies between dimensions of interoception in autism: implications for emotion and anxiety. Biol. Psychol. 114 117–126. 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2015.12.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Griffiths D. (2015). Neural control of micturition in humans: a working model. Nat. Rev. Urol. 12 695–705. 10.1038/nrurol.2015.266 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grynberg D., Pollatos O. (2015). Perceiving one’s body shapes empathy. Physiol. Behav. 140 54–60. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2014.12.026 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grynberg D., Chang B., Corneille O., Maurage P., Vermeulen N., Berthoz S., et al. (2012). Alexithymia and the processing of Emotional Facial Expressions (EFEs): systematic review. PLoS One 7:e42429. 10.1371/journal.pone.0042429 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hess U., Fischer A. (2013). Emotional mimicry as social regulation. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 17 142–157. 10.1177/1088868312472607 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kever A., Pollatos O., Vermeulen N., Grynberg D. (2015). Interoceptive sensitivity facilitates both antecedent- and response-focused emotion regulation strategies. Pers. Individ. Diff. 87 20–23. 10.1016/j.paid.2015.07.014 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Khalsa S. S., Adolphs R., Cameron O. G., Critchley H. D., Davenport P. W., Feinstein J. S., et al. (2018). Interoception and mental health: a roadmap. Biol. Psychiatry Cogn. Neurosci. Neuroimag. 3 501–513. 10.1016/j.bpsc.2017.12.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kudielka B. M., Hellhammer D. H., Kirschbaum C. (2007). “Ten years of research with the trier social stress test–revisited,” in Social Neuroscience: Integrating Biological and Psychological Explanations of Social Behavior, eds Harmon-Jones E., Winkielman P. (New York, NY: The Guilford Press; ), 56–83. [Google Scholar]
- Lindsay E. K., Young S., Brown K. W., Smyth J. M., Creswell J. D. (2019). Mindfulness training reduces loneliness and increases social contact in a randomized controlled trial. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 116 3488–3493. 10.1073/pnas.1813588116 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luo Y., Hawkley L. C., Waite L. J., Cacioppo J. T. (2012). Loneliness, health, and mortality in old age: a national longitudinal study. Soc. Sci. Med. 74 907–914. 10.1016/j.socscimed.2011.11.028 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marshall A. C., Gentsch A., Schröder L., Schütz-Bosbach S. (2018). Cardiac interoceptive learning is modulated by emotional valence perceived from facial expressions. Soc. Cogn. Aff. Neurosci. 13 677–686. 10.1093/scan/nsy042 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mehling W. E. (2016). Differentiating attention styles and regulatory aspects of self-reported interoceptive sensibility. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 371:20160013. 10.1098/rstb.2016.0013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mehling W. E., Price C., Daubenmier J. J., Acree M., Bartmess E., Stewart A. (2012). The multidimensional assessment of interoceptive awareness (MAIA). PLoS One 7:e48230. 10.1371/journal.pone.0048230 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pellencin E., Paladino M. P., Herbelin B., Serino A. (2018). Social perception of others shapes one’s own multisensory peripersonal space. Cortex 104 163–179. 10.1016/j.cortex.2017.08.033 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pollatos O., Schandry R. (2004). Accuracy of heartbeat perception is reflected in the amplitude of the heartbeat-evoked brain potential. Psychophysiology 41 476–482. 10.1111/1469-8986.2004.00170.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pollatos O., Matthias E., Keller J. (2015). When interoception helps to overcome negative feelings caused by social exclusion. Front. Psychol. 6:786. 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00786 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Powers K. E., Heatherton T. F. (2012). Characterizing socially avoidant and affiliative responses to social exclusion. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 6:46. 10.3389/fnint.2012.00046 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Quadt L., Critchley H. D., Garfinkel S. N. (2018). The neurobiology of interoception in health and disease: neuroscience of interoception. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1428 112–128. 10.1111/nyas.13915 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ring C., Brener J., Knapp K., Mailloux J. (2015). Effects of heartbeat feedback on beliefs about heart rate and heartbeat counting: a cautionary tale about interoceptive awareness. Biol. Psychol. 104 193–198. 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2014.12.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shamay-Tsoory S. G., Abu-Akel A. (2016). The social salience hypothesis of oxytocin. Biol. Psychiatry 79 194–202. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.07.020 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Singer T., Engert V. (2019). It matters what you practice: differential training effects on subjective experience, behavior, brain and body in the resource project. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 28 151–158. 10.1016/j.copsyc.2018.12.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sterling P. (2011). Allostasis: a model of predictive regulation. Physiol. Behav. 106 5–15. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2011.06.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Teneggi C., Canzoneri E., di Pellegrino G., Serino A. (2013). Social modulation of peripersonal space boundaries. Curr. Biol. 23 406–411. 10.1016/j.cub.2013.01.043 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Terasawa Y., Moriguchi Y., Tochizawa S., Umeda S. (2014). Interoceptive sensitivity predicts sensitivity to the emotions of others. Cogn. Emot. 28 1435–1448. 10.1080/02699931.2014.888988 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- VanderWeele T. J., Hawkley L. C., Cacioppo J. T. (2012). On the reciprocal association between loneliness and subjective well-being. Am. J. Epidemiol. 176 777–784. 10.1093/aje/kws173 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Werner N. S., Duschek S., Mattern M., Schandry R. (2009). Interoceptive sensitivity modulates anxiety during public speaking. J. Psychophysiol. 23 85–94. 10.1027/0269-8803.23.2.85 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Werner N. S., Kerschreiter R., Kindermann N. K., Duschek S. (2013). Interoceptive awareness as a moderator of affective responses to social exclusion. J. Psychophysiol. 27 39–50. 10.1027/0269-8803/a000086 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Will G. (2018). We have an Epidemic of Loneliness. How can we fix it? The Washington Post. Available at: https://www.washingtonpost.com/ (accessed May 18, 2019). [Google Scholar]
- Yao S., Becker B., Zhao W., Zhao Z., Kou J., Ma X., et al. (2018). Oxytocin modulates attention switching between interoceptive signals and external social cues. Neuropsychopharmacology 43 294–301. 10.1038/npp.2017.189 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zaki J. (2014). Empathy: a motivated account. Psychol. Bull. 140 1608–1647. 10.1037/a0037679 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zamariola G., Luminet O., Mierop A., Corneille O. (2019). Does it help to feel your body? evidence is inconclusive that interoceptive accuracy and sensibility help cope with negative experiences. Cogn. Emot. 33 1627–1638. 10.1080/02699931.2019.1591345 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zamariola G., Maurage P., Luminet O., Corneille O. (2018). Interoceptive accuracy scores from the heartbeat counting task are problematic: evidence from simple bivariate correlations. Biol. Psychol. 137 12–17. 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2018.06.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]