Abstract
The zebrafish (Danio rerio) is a popular vertebrate model for studying visual development, especially at the larval stage. For many vertebrates, post-natal visual experience is essential to fine-tune visual development, but it is unknown how experience shapes larval zebrafish vision. Zebrafish swim with a moving texture; in the wild, this innate optomotor response (OMR) stabilises larvae in moving water, but it can be exploited in the laboratory to assess zebrafish visual function. Here, we compared spatial-frequency tuning inferred from OMR between visually naïve and experienced larvae from 5 to 7 days post-fertilisation. We also examined development of synaptic connections between neurons by quantifying post-synaptic density 95 (PSD-95) in larval retinae. PSD-95 is closely associated with N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptors, the neurotransmitter-receptor proteins underlying experience-dependent visual development. We found that rather than following an experience-independent genetic programme, developmental changes in visual spatial-frequency tuning at the larval stage required visual experience. Exposure to motion evoking OMR yielded no greater improvement than exposure to static form, suggesting that increased sensitivity as indexed by OMR was driven not by motor practice but by visual experience itself. PSD-95 density varied with visual sensitivity, suggesting that experience may have up-regulated clustering of PSD-95 for synaptic maturation in visual development.
Subject terms: Developmental neurogenesis, Sensorimotor processing, Sensory processing, Motion detection, Pattern vision
Introduction
Experience plays an important role in the early visual development of vertebrates1–6. For example, visual stimulation is required for the monostratification of retinal ganglion cell (RGC) dendrites in the inner plexiform layer (IPL), which contributes to the maturation of ON and OFF visual pathways in the mouse retina1. In ferrets, the emergence of cortical orientation selectivity is driven by early experiences with moving visual stimuli, enhancing sensitivity to their spatiotemporal features2. However, not all post-natal visual development depends on experience. While visual evoked binocular responses emerge at a similar post-natal age in both pre-term and full-term human infants (suggesting experience-dependence)6, monocular responses instead track time from conception, suggesting development independent of experience. The role of visual experience in larval zebrafish visual development has been controversial. Some previous studies have suggested that visual experience may not be necessary for early structural and functional development of some visual regions (e.g., tectum and retina) in larval zebrafish7,8. However, a recent study indicates that visual experience contributes to the reorganisation of functional circuits and shapes spontaneous neural activity in the developing larval tectum, and is crucial for the development of prey-capture behaviour9.
The molecular mechanisms underlying experience-dependent visual development are also coming to light. Up-regulated expression of brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) has been observed in Xenopus tadpole tectal neurons after exposure to visual stimulation, accompanied by increased visual acuity10. Activity-dependent expression of BDNF requires appropriate activation of N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptors, α-amino-3-hydroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazolepropionic acid (AMPA) receptors and γ-Aminobutyric acid A (GABAA) receptors in the developing visual cortex10–13. Excitatory NMDA and AMPA receptors interact with the scaffold protein post-synaptic density 95 (PSD-95) to form surface complexes that anchor the receptors on post-synaptic membranes for signalling14,15. During development, PSD-95 contributes to the maturation of excitatory synapses and stabilises synaptic contacts16,17. Expression of PSD-95 is up-regulated in the post-synaptic membrane of neurons in human visual cortex during childhood, pointing to an important role in early visual development18.
The zebrafish (Danio rerio) has become a popular vertebrate model in visual neuroscience. Its high conservation of genetics and retinal architecture with other vertebrate species make the zebrafish ideal for modelling the human visual system19. A prolific breeder, the zebrafish produces embryos that develop outside the mother and are transparent until 7 days post-fertilisation (dpf), allowing real-time observation of neurogenesis inside the embryonic body20–22. Thus, the early morphological development of the larval zebrafish visual system, including the retina, has been well documented19,21,22. In comparison, relatively few studies to our knowledge have characterised the functional, behavioural aspect of visual development in larval zebrafish23–26. The zebrafish visual system is behaviourally competent as early as 68 hours post-fertilisation (hpf)25, and the optomotor response (OMR) can be elicited from 4 dpf27. In the wild, larval zebrafish will follow a moving texture to stabilise themselves in the river bed; this OMR is often exploited in the lab as a behavioural screen for visual deficits24,27 or, less commonly, to characterise the spatiotemporal or chromatic response properties of the visual system28,29. Although studies of zebrafish neuroanatomy largely focus on the early developmental stages—especially those ages at which external food is not required (<7dpf)—the corresponding development of behavioural sensitivity as indexed by OMR has not yet been profiled. Further, the role of visual experience in larval zebrafish visual development and its underlying molecular mechanisms remain to be fully described.
Here, we report that visual experience accelerates development of visual sensitivity to contrast inferred from OMR in larval zebrafish from 5 to 7 dpf. We demonstrate that exposure to static visual form, which does not elicit the OMR, is sufficient to induce experience-dependent improvements in sensitivity; this indicates that the increased OMR reflects visual, rather than motor, development. In addition, we find that PSD-95 density in the retinal IPL increases with visual experience during this developmental period. These results reveal highly experience-dependent development of visual sensitivity at the behavioural level and point to a potential biomolecular mechanism.
Methods
Animal husbandry
Zebrafish (wild type St Kilda or AB strains) were bred and housed in the facility at the Walter and Eliza Hall Institute of Medical Research in accordance with local animal guidelines. Male and female zebrafish embryos were raised in egg water (60 mg/L sea salt) at 28.5 °C and reared in complete darkness when not exposed to visual stimulation within the experimental protocol. All procedures were performed according to the provisions of the Australian National Health and Medical Research Council code of practice for the care and use of animals, and were approved by the Faculty of Science Animal Ethics Committee at the University of Melbourne (No. 1614017.3).
Optomotor apparatus
The OMR apparatus was the same as described in our previous work30, adapted from one used by Orger and colleagues (Fig. 1a,b)27. A Power Mac G5 computer (Apple Computer, Inc., Cupertino, CA) ran MATLAB 2016b (MathWorks, Natick, MA) with Psychtoolbox extensions31. Stimuli were processed on an ATI Radeon HD 5770 graphics card, with the output sent to a BITS++ video processor (Cambridge Research Systems, Rochester, UK) for increased contrast resolution, and displayed on a M992 flat-screen cathode ray tube (CRT) monitor (Dell Computer Corporation, Round Rock, Texas, USA) with its screen facing upwards. During the experiment, zebrafish were contained in a five-lane arena (300 mm × 38 mm per lane; Fig. 1b) with a transparent base positioned 56.5 mm above the screen. A C922 Pro Stream webcam (Logitech Company, Lausanne, Switzerland; 1080p at 30 Hz) was fixed 366.5 mm above the base of the arena, controlled by the MATLAB program to take digital images (1080 × 1920 pixels) before and after each trial for calculation of swimming distance.
Optomotor response measurements
After collection, zebrafish embryos were randomly assigned to one of five exposure regimes (Table 1). There were 4–7 groups per regime, with 50–60 larvae in each group; thus, in total, there were over 200 larvae tested for each regime. Some measurements were derived from the same larvae at different timepoints; for example, measurements from larvae in Regime 2 at 6 dpf are referred to as ‘6-dpf naïve’ in the first analysis (experience-dependent development of visual sensitivity), while measurements from the same larvae at 7 dpf are referred to as ‘7-dpf 1-session moving’ in the second analysis (exposure to visual form versus motion).
Table 1.
Age | 5 dpf | 6 dpf | 7 dpf |
---|---|---|---|
Regime 1 (7 groups) | |||
Condition | Moving stimuli | Moving stimuli | Moving stimuli |
Group name (analysis 1)* | 5-dpf naïve | 6-dpf experienced | 7-dpf experienced |
Group name (analysis 2)** | — | — | 7-dpf 2-session moving |
Regime 2 (5 groups) | |||
Condition | No exposure | Moving stimuli | Moving stimuli |
Group name (analysis 1) | — | 6-dpf naïve | — |
Group name (analysis 2) | — | — | 7-dpf 1-session moving |
Regime 3 (5 groups) | |||
Condition | No exposure | No exposure | Moving stimuli |
Group name (analysis 1) | — | — | 7-dpf naïve |
Group name (analysis 2) | — | — | |
Regime 4 (5 groups) | |||
Condition | Static stimuli | Static stimuli | Moving stimuli |
Group name (analysis 1) | — | — | — |
Group name (analysis 2) | — | — | 7-dpf 2-session static |
Regime 5 (4 groups) | |||
Condition | No exposure | Static stimuli | Moving stimuli |
Group name (analysis 1) | — | — | — |
Group name (analysis 2) | — | — | 7-dpf 1-session static |
*Analysis 1: Experience-dependent development of visual sensitivity. **Analysis 2: Exposure to visual form versus motion.
On the day of an optomotor assay or static exposure, each group was transferred to one of the arena lanes. The transfer period took less than 10 minutes, after which all larvae were allowed to adapt to the arena for 10 minutes. Stimuli were random Gaussian-noise textures presented at 100% contrast (Fig. 1c). These were filtered to be spatial-frequency bandpass and isotropic using log-Gabor filters32. Contrast was flattened to remove large-scale fluctuations across the texture33,34. Centre frequencies were 0.005, 0.01, 0.02, 0.04, 0.08, 0.16 or 0.32 cycles per degree (c/°) with a standard deviation of 0.5 octaves. Velocities for moving stimuli were 25, 50 or 100 degrees per second (°/s), equivalent to 24.7, 49.3 and 98.6 mm/s in our setup, parallel to the long axis of the arena.
Prior to each trial, a corralling stimulus was shown for 30 s to guide larvae to the centre of the lane (Fig. 1d). This stimulus comprised several sinusoidal luminance gratings with spatial frequencies intermediate to those presented during the trials, superposed with randomised phases. One compound grating drifted from each end of the lane, converging on a central line (Fig. 1d). The corralling stimulus drifted at 25 °/s for sessions with moving stimuli and was static for sessions with static stimuli. This was followed by a 30-s presentation of a test stimulus. Images were taken by the camera before and after each presentation to capture the position of each larva. A blank grey screen was presented after the offset of the test stimulus as the texture for the next trial was computed. Each of the groups underwent no more than one session per day. In sessions with moving stimuli, each combination of spatial frequency and velocity was presented 6 times (3 times in each direction). In sessions with static stimuli, larvae were exposed to each spatial frequency 18 times. Trials were presented in random order. Each experimental session comprised a total of 126 trials and took about 3 h. After each session, larvae were immediately transferred back to dark incubators, or humanely killed in 0.1% tricaine at 7 dpf. All experiments were conducted between 9:30 a.m. and 6:00 p.m.
Histological processing and immunohistochemistry
Following humane killing in 0.1% tricaine, zebrafish embryos were fixed in 4% paraformaldehyde (PFA) in phosphate-buffered saline (PBS), cryoprotected in 30% sucrose in PBS solution, embedded in OCT (Tissue-Tek) and cryostat sectioned at 12 µm thickness. Antibody staining was carried out at room temperature using standard protocols. Antigen retrieval was performed by incubating slides in boiled 10 mM sodium citrate (pH 6) until the solution was cooled down to room temperature. Slides were then blocked in 5% foetal bovine serum (FBS) for 30 min and incubated overnight in rabbit anti-PSD95 antibody (1:100, Abcam, cat. number ab18258) diluted in the same block solution. On the second day, slides were rinsed using PBS and then incubated for 2 h in anti-Rabbit Alexa Fluor 488 (1:500, Thermo Fisher Scientific, cat. number A11001) diluted in 5% FBS. Nuclei were then stained with 4′,6-diamidino-2-phenylindole (DAPI; 1:10000, Sigma-Aldrich, cat. number D9542-10MG) in PBS solution. Sections were mounted in Mowiol (Sigma-Aldrich, cat. number 81381-250 G).
Retinal image acquisition
Stained retinal sections were imaged using an A1R confocal microscope (Nikon Corporation, Tokyo, Japan; 60× oil objective, NA = 1.4; excitation wavelengths 405, 488, 561 and 640 nm). The deconvolution function was used to enhance PSD-95 puncta against the staining background. Image brightness and contrast were adjusted using FIJI35 or Photoshop (Adobe, San Jose, CA, USA) software.
Optomotor analysis
Larval positions were extracted from the before and after images for each trial using a MATLAB algorithm. Image contrast was flattened, and a Laplacian-of-Gaussian was applied to highlight edges33. Thresholding was then applied (and manually adjusted if necessary) to segment larvae from the background, and the spatial centroid of the whole group was calculated using bwconncomp and regionprops commands from the MATLAB Image Processing Toolbox. On each trial, the change in position of the centroid in the direction of the texture motion was taken as the optomotor index. Within each stimulus speed, normalised optomotor indices were calculated by normalising data to the mean index at 0.02 c/° for 5 dpf larvae (for analysis of OMR development from 5 to 7 dpf) or to the index at 0.02 c/° for 7 dpf 2-session moving larvae (for the analysis of 7 dpf larval OMR as a function of visual experience). Following normalisation of measurements within each velocity, the optomotor index is equivalent to normalised gain (calculated as the ratio of swimming speed to stimulus speed) reported in some larval OMR studies. These linear transformations had no effect on the model fitting or statistical analysis but converted OMR indices into more readily interpretable units. The spatial-frequency tuning function was fitted as a log-Gaussian using a least-squares criterion. For each fitted model, the estimated parameters were amplitude (i.e., the height of the peak), peak frequency (i.e., the spatial frequency at which amplitude peaked) and bandwidth (i.e., the standard deviation; Supplementary Fig. S1). To determine whether parameter estimates differed between groups, we used an F test to compare a full model, in which each group’s estimates could vary independently, with a restricted model, in which one parameter was constrained to be the same across groups36. We applied Bonferroni correction to the set of pairwise comparisons across groups within a parameter (α = 0.05; αcorrected = 0.005). Reported P values are adjusted accordingly. There were between 24 and 42 trials per data point in each exposure condition (see Supplementary Tables S1 and S2).
Histological analysis
One randomly selected region of interest of the IPL (10 × 20 µm) was cropped from images of each retinal section using Photoshop software. The density of PSD-95 puncta in the IPL was then quantified using the Analyse particles function in FIJI35. Bayesian one-way ANOVA was performed using JASP37 to compare PSD-95 density between groups. There were between 10 and 12 retinae per group (see Supplementary Table S3).
Results
Experience-dependent development of visual sensitivity
To investigate whether visual experience affects early development of visual behaviour in larval zebrafish, we measured the spatial-frequency tuning function using OMR in visually naïve (i.e., dark-reared before measurements) and experienced (i.e., exposed to texture before measurements) larvae from 5 to 7 dpf. All spatial-frequency tuning functions were well fit by the spatially bandpass log-Gaussian model (Fig. 2a–c). An omnibus test indicated that tuning functions differed between groups for each speed (P = 0.045, P = 0.013 and P = 0.003 for 25, 50 and 100 °/s, respectively; Supplementary Table S1).
Examining individual parameters, visual experience had no discernible effect on peak frequencies or bandwidth at any stimulus speed (Supplementary Fig. S2). However, as can be seen in the fitted tuning curves (Fig. 2a–c), amplitude differed between groups at each stimulus speed. At 25 °/s (Fig. 2d), there was overall difference across groups (P = 0.023), although no individual pairwise comparison survived Bonferroni correction (Supplementary Table S1). At 50 °/s (Fig. 2e), there was an overall effect of group on amplitude (P = 0.006); pairwise comparisons revealed that the amplitude of the tuning function for 5 dpf larvae was lower than for 6 dpf (P = 0.032) and 7 dpf (P = 0.034) experienced larvae, but indistinguishable from 6 and 7 dpf naïve larvae. Similarly, at 100 °/s (Fig. 2f), there was an overall effect of group on amplitude (P = 0.002); amplitude was higher for 7 dpf experienced larvae than for 5 dpf larvae and 7 dpf naïve larvae (P = 0.039 and 0.019, respectively; Fig. 2f). Overall, visual experience was necessary for improvements in sensitivity from 5 to 7 dpf.
Exposure to visual form versus motion
Some proportion of the increased OMR in visually experienced larvae might be attributable to enhanced motor function from repeated stimulation of the response on previous days. In order to assess this possibility, we measured the spatial-frequency tuning function using OMR in 7 dpf larvae with different levels of previous experience (0, 1 or 2 sessions) of either static or moving stimuli. The static stimulus provided exposure to form vision without eliciting the OMR, while moving stimulus evoked a motor response.
Inspection of the spatial-frequency tuning functions (Fig. 3a–c) suggests that sensitivity in all experienced 7 dpf groups was higher than that of 7 dpf naïve larvae, with little difference between static and moving exposure groups. This observation was generally supported by formal comparison of the models (Supplementary Table S2). While models were not statistically different from each other at 25 °/s (Fig. 3a), they differed across groups at 50 °/s (P = 0.048; Fig. 3b) and 100 °/s (P < 0.001; Fig. 3c). Examining individual parameters, visual experience had no discernible effect on peak frequencies or bandwidth at any stimulus speed (Supplementary Fig. S3). At slower speeds, amplitude was numerically (but not significantly) higher in experienced groups than in the naïve group (Fig. 3d,e); but there was a statistical difference in amplitude between groups at 100 °/s (P < 0.001; Fig. 3f). The amplitude of the tuning function for the 2-session moving group was significantly higher than that of the naïve group (P = 0.019), and significantly higher for the 2-session static group than for both the 1-session moving group (P = 0.047) and the naïve group (P = 0.003). There was no significant difference in amplitude between groups with moving versus static stimulus experience. This suggests that the primary driver of increased OMR in experienced larvae was not motor development, but rather the visual experience itself.
Synaptic density in the inner plexiform layer
Our behavioural data demonstrate experience-dependent development of the larval zebrafish OMR from 5 to 7 dpf. Thus, we investigated whether differential development over this period is reflected in different levels of synaptic maturation. PSD-95 is known to play an important role in neuronal signalling by anchoring excitatory NMDA neurotransmitter receptors on the post-synaptic neural membrane during early synaptic development14,15,17. The retina is responsible for sensation and initial processing of visual inputs. In zebrafish, PSD-95 is expressed throughout the retina, but the inner plexiform layer (IPL) is particularly suited to quantification of PSD-95 density owing to an absence of cell bodies—bipolar (BC) and amacrine cells (AC) terminate in the IPL to contact ganglion cell (GC) dendrites38, making PSD-95 puncta much easier to observe. We thus measured PSD-95 density in the retinal IPL as a marker of synaptic maturation.
PSD-95 puncta were spread throughout the IPL of both naïve and experienced larvae from 5 to 7 dpf (Fig. 4a). However, quantification revealed that both 2-session static and 2-session moving groups at 7 dpf had higher PSD-95 densities than 6- and 7-dpf naïve groups (Fig. 4b, Supplementary Table S3). This raises the possibility that experience-dependent development of visual sensitivity is reflected at a molecular level in improved communication between synapses. In contrast, we found no evidence of a difference in PSD-95 density between 2-session static and 2-session moving 7-dpf larvae. While there was no evidence of a difference between the 5-dpf naïve group and other groups, this is likely a result of highly variable PSD-95 density at 5 dpf.
Discussion
This study demonstrates experience-dependent visual development in larval zebrafish from 5 to 7 dpf using the optomotor response (OMR). Larval zebrafish have several innate responses, including OMR, that can be used to study visual function27,39. Due to the advantages of the zebrafish model and continuing refinement of setups for behavioural measurement, larval zebrafish behavioural assays have been increasingly popular in drug and genetic screening27,40,41. In spite of this, much remains to be learned regarding the basic development of visual behaviour in larval zebrafish. We recently showed developmental changes in the larval zebrafish electroretinogram (ERG)42, with a considerable acceleration of neural responses generated by retinal photoreceptors and bipolar cells from 4 to 7 dpf. However, development of the larval OMR, its interaction with visual experience, and its subcellular correlates have not been investigated previously.
Zebrafish perform visual behaviours with an immature visual system, which requires visual experience for further maturation. Our data show significant increases in the amplitude of the spatial-frequency tuning function measured by OMR between 5 and 7 dpf in visually experienced larvae, with little discernible change in amplitude in naïve larvae (Fig. 2). This indicates that exposure to visual stimulation at the larval stage may be necessary for maturation of visual behaviour. In many mammals, such as humans, mice and cats, there is a critical period after eye opening during which the neural circuits of the immature visual system are refined3,12. During this period, the development of response selectivity in visual neurons depends on experience1,2; without appropriate experience (e.g., if the animal is dark-reared), the visual cortex remains immature, manifesting in lower visual sensitivity3,43. However, this is not so clear in the case of zebrafish, for which visual development is often considered to be hardwired. For example, the refinement of neural circuits in the optic tectum is almost complete at 78 hpf in dark-reared larvae, prior to the arborisation of retinal axons in the tectum7,8. Whether visual development in larval zebrafish is activity-dependent remains controversial7,44; however, physiological studies suggest that zebrafish larvae reared under dark or restricted spectral conditions manifest altered retinal functions, consistent with activity-dependent development45,46. A critical period at 5–6 dpf for larval zebrafish was also indicated by a recent study, which found that visual experience at this age was critical for the development of larval optic tectum and prey-capture behaviour9. Our study adds to this growing body of evidence for experience-dependent visual development in larval zebrafish.
The importance of exposure to light and form for early visual development has been indicated previously in several vertebrate species1,24,47–49. Our study extends these findings to show that exposure to visual form is the driving factor in the development of visual sensitivity at the larval stage. By assessing OMR in experienced 7-dpf larvae, we found that improvements in visual sensitivity were similar for larvae exposed to static and moving stimuli. This was clearest at the highest stimulus speed tested (100 °/s), at which improvements were more statistically robust than at the lowest speed (25 °/s). Unequal development of motion sensitivity across different stimulus speeds has been shown in human studies50, and on this basis we suspect that in zebrafish larvae, experience may improve visual sensitivity at higher stimulus speeds at a faster developmental rate.
Notably, experience of moving stimuli was not necessary to produce improvements in sensitivity. Exposure to moving stimuli at early ages contributes to development of circuits for direction selectivity, shown in developing ferret visual cortex and Xenopus tectal neurons2,51. Of course, in our experiment, larvae with only static exposure would have experienced some visual motion across the retina during spontaneous swimming. Perhaps supported by a largely hardwired optic tectum, this retinal motion may have been sufficient to drive the refinement of experience-dependent motion-sensing circuits in the retina7,8,44. In any case, the similar improvements for larvae exposed to static and moving stimuli—also reflected in our measurements of PSD-95 density—rule out the possibility that development of the spatial-frequency tuning function as indexed by the OMR reflected motor improvements from repeated invocation of the response.
We also found an experience-dependent increase in PSD-95 density in the IPL, which may be a subcellular marker of visual development. With visual experience, activity of neurons in the CNS induces up-regulation of BDNF synthesis52. Lack of BDNF expression in the developing rat retina can impair visual acuity53. This molecule plays a vital role in long-term potentiation (LTP) of neural responses and synaptic consolidation, which require high-frequency stimulation of excitatory inputs from the activation of NMDA receptors54. This activation also induces LTP at excitatory synapses for the experience-dependent refinement of developing retinal circuits in larval zebrafish55. Importantly, NMDA receptors rely on PSD-95 to anchor them on the post-synaptic membrane15, which may link PSD-95 to the BDNF-dependent LTP induced by early visual experience. Over-expression of PSD-95 can stabilise young synaptic contacts and promote excitatory synaptic maturation16,17,56. These results together suggest that PSD-95 plays a role in experience-dependent visual development. Future studies will clarify the molecular pathway that gives rise to increased PSD-95 density with visual experience. We also observed that variability in IPL PSD-95 density decreased for naïve larvae from 5 to 7 dpf, which may reflect increasing stability of PSD-95 expression with age even in the absence of visual experience. Quantifying PSD-95 density in the IPL provides a gross measure of overall synaptic changes; further investigations may reveal whether more subtle experience-dependent changes in synaptic density or distribution occur in certain neural subtypes, including direction-selective ganglion cells and starburst amacrine cells57,58, which are particularly important for processing information for motion detection.
The present study exemplifies some of the advantages of the larval zebrafish OMR in studying visual function and development. OMR measurement allows analysis of visual behaviour in relatively large samples compared to other assays such as the optokinetic response (OKR). For example, in our setup, every measurement in an OMR trial is averaged from ~50 larvae; this is advantageous in the context of rapid development, which can introduce considerable variability among larvae of the same age. Quantifying visual sensitivity at a given spatial frequency through swimming distance in response to a high-contrast stimulus—rather than calculating thresholds by varying contrast—improves efficiency by significantly reducing the number of experimental sessions required to measure the sensitivity of spatial vision. Furthermore, the OMR can be used to measure higher-order visual processing in zebrafish, targeting functions mediated by whole-brain visual circuits59,60. Using OMR in larval zebrafish, researchers can interrogate diverse visual-processing networks throughout the whole brain, especially when coupled with techniques such as two-photon calcium imaging60.
Conclusions
The present study provides robust evidence of experience-dependent development of visual sensitivity in larval zebrafish from 5 to 7 dpf. This development is primarily driven by exposure to light and form, even in the absence of the explicit stimulus motion (i.e., non-self-generated motion) required for development of neural direction selectivity in other species2,51. PSD-95 density in the IPL was increased in 7-dpf larvae with visual experience; we found no evidence of a similar increase from 5 to 7 dpf in visually naïve larvae. These histological results support our OMR findings and suggest that visual exposure can up-regulate PSD-95 expression for refining synaptic connections, and may thus improve visual behaviour. Our results also contribute to a growing body of work demonstrating the utility of the larval OMR assay in experimental visual neuroscience.
Supplementary information
Acknowledgements
We thank the Biological Optical Microscopy Platform (BOMP) of the Melbourne Advanced Microscopy Facility for providing instruments for confocal imaging, and Christopher Chia for his assistance in collection of preliminary data. This work was supported by a grant from the Melbourne Neuroscience Institute (to PTG, PRJ & BVB). PTG was supported by a Discovery Early Career Researcher Award from the Australian Research Council (DE160100125).
Author contributions
J.X. contributed to conception and design, carried out data collection, participated in data analysis, drafted and critically revised the manuscript. P.R.J. contributed to conception and design, supervised histological data collection, and critically revised the manuscript. B.V.B. contributed to conception and design, and critically revised the manuscript. P.T.G. contributed to conception and design, supervised behavioural data collection, participated in data analysis, and critically revised the manuscript. All authors gave final approval for publication.
Data availability
The datasets supporting this article are available on the Open Science Framework (https://osf.io/8emn7/).
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary information
is available for this paper at 10.1038/s41598-019-54958-6.
References
- 1.Tian N, Copenhagen DR. Visual stimulation is required for refinement of ON and OFF pathways in postnatal retina. Neuron. 2003;39:85–96. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(03)00389-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Li Y, Van Hooser SD, Mazurek M, White LE, Fitzpatrick D. Experience with moving visual stimuli drives the early development of cortical direction selectivity. Nature. 2008;456:952–956. doi: 10.1038/nature07417. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Crair MC, Gillespie DC, Stryker MP. The role of visual experience in the development of columns in cat visual cortex. Science. 1998;279:566–570. doi: 10.1126/science.279.5350.566. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Qin W, Liu Y, Jiang T, Yu C. The development of visual areas depends differently on visual experience. PLoS One. 2013;8:e53784. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0053784. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Xu H, Khakhalin AS, Nurmikko AV, Aizenman CD. Visual experience-dependent maturation of correlated neuronal activity patterns in a developing visual system. J. Neurosci. 2011;31:8025–8036. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.5802-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Jandó G, et al. Early-onset binocularity in preterm infants reveals experience-dependent visual development in humans. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2012;109:11049–11052. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1203096109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Nevin LM, Taylor MR, Baier H. Hardwiring of fine synaptic layers in the zebrafish visual pathway. Neural Dev. 2008;3:36. doi: 10.1186/1749-8104-3-36. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Niell CM, Smith SJ. Functional imaging reveals rapid development of visual response properties in the zebrafish tectum. Neuron. 2005;45:941–951. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.01.047. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Avitan L, et al. Spontaneous activity in the zebrafish tectum reorganizes over development and is influenced by visual experience. Curr. Biol. 2017;27:2407–2419. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.06.056. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Schwartz N, Schohl A, Ruthazer ES. Activity-dependent transcription of BDNF enhances visual acuity during development. Neuron. 2011;70:455–467. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.02.055. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Beston B, Jones D, Murphy K. Experience-dependent changes in excitatory and inhibitory receptor subunit expression in visual cortex. Front. Synaptic Neurosci. 2010;2:138. doi: 10.3389/fnsyn.2010.00138. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Hensch TK. Critical period plasticity in local cortical circuits. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2005;6:877–888. doi: 10.1038/nrn1787. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Jaffer S, Vorobyov V, Kind PC, Sengpiel F. Experience-dependent regulation of functional maps and synaptic protein expression in the cat visual cortex. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2012;35:1281–1294. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2012.08044.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Zheng C-Y, Seabold GK, Horak M, Petralia RS. MAGUKs, synaptic development, and synaptic plasticity. Neuroscientist. 2011;17:493–512. doi: 10.1177/1073858410386384. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Chen X, et al. PSD-95 family MAGUKs are essential for anchoring AMPA and NMDA receptor complexes at the postsynaptic density. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2015;112:E6983–E6992. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1517045112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Taft CE, Turrigiano GG. PSD-95 promotes the stabilization of young synaptic contacts. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 2014;369:20130134. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2013.0134. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.El-Husseini AE-D, Schnell E, Chetkovich DM, Nicoll RA, Bredt DS. PSD-95 involvement in maturation of excitatory synapses. Science. 2000;290:1364–1368. doi: 10.1126/science.290.5495.1364. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Pinto JGA, Jones DG, Williams CK, Murphy KM. Characterizing synaptic protein development in human visual cortex enables alignment of synaptic age with rat visual cortex. Front. Neural Circuits. 2015;9:3. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2015.00003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Gestri G, Link BA, Neuhauss SC. The visual system of zebrafish and its use to model human ocular diseases. Dev. Neurobiol. 2012;72:302–327. doi: 10.1002/dneu.20919. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Roper, C. & Tanguay, R. L. Zebrafish models of developmental biology and toxicology In Handbook of Developmental Neurotoxicology, 2nd Ed. (ed. Slikker, W., Paule, M. G. & Wang, C.) 143–151 (Academic Press, 2018).
- 21.Bilotta J, Saszik S. The zebrafish as a model visual system. Int. J. Dev. Neurosci. 2001;19:621–629. doi: 10.1016/S0736-5748(01)00050-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Chhetri J, Jacobson G, Gueven N. Zebrafish: On the move towards ophthalmological research. Eye (Lond.) 2014;28:367–380. doi: 10.1038/eye.2014.19. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Haug MF, Biehlmaier O, Mueller KP, Neuhauss SCF. Visual acuity in larval zebrafish: Behavior and histology. Front. Zool. 2010;7:8. doi: 10.1186/1742-9994-7-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Bilotta J. Effects of abnormal lighting on the development of zebrafish visual behavior. Behav. Brain Res. 2000;116:81–87. doi: 10.1016/S0166-4328(00)00264-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Easter SS, Jr, Nicola GN. The development of vision in the zebrafish (Danio rerio) Dev. Biol. 1996;180:646–663. doi: 10.1006/dbio.1996.0335. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Rinner O, Rick JM, Neuhauss SCF. Contrast sensitivity, spatial and temporal tuning of the larval zebrafish optokinetic response. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 2005;46:137–142. doi: 10.1167/iovs.04-0682. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Orger Michael B., Gahtan Ethan, Muto Akira, Page-McCaw Patrick, Smear Matthew C., Baier Herwig. Methods in Cell Biology. 2004. Behavioral Screening Assays in Zebrafish; pp. 53–68. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Orger MB, Baier H. Channeling of red and green cone inputs to the zebrafish optomotor response. Vis. Neurosci. 2005;22:275–281. doi: 10.1017/S0952523805223039. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Maaswinkel H, Li L. Spatio-temporal frequency characteristics of the optomotor response in zebrafish. Vision Res. 2003;43:21–30. doi: 10.1016/s0042-6989(02)00395-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Xie J, Goodbourn PT, Bui BV, Sztal TE, Jusuf PR. Correspondence between behavioral, physiological, and anatomical measurements of visual function in inhibitory neuron-ablated zebrafish. Invest. Opthalmol. Vis. Sci. 2019;60:4681–4690. doi: 10.1167/iovs.19-27544. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Kleiner M, et al. What’s new in Psychtoolbox-3? Perception. 2007;36:1. [Google Scholar]
- 32.Field DJ. Relations between the statistics of natural images and the response properties of cortical cells. J. Opt. Soc. Am. A. 1987;4:2379–2394. doi: 10.1364/josaa.4.002379. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Bizrah M, et al. A semi-automated technique for labeling and counting of apoptosing retinal cells. BMC Bioinformatics. 2014;15:169. doi: 10.1186/1471-2105-15-169. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Dakin SC, Turnbull PR. Similar contrast sensitivity functions measured using psychophysics and optokinetic nystagmus. Sci. Rep. 2016;6:34514. doi: 10.1038/srep34514. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Schindelin J, et al. Fiji: An open-source platform for biological-image analysis. Nat. Methods. 2012;9:676–682. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.2019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Lu, Z.-L. & Dosher, B. Visual Psychophysics: From Laboratory to Theory (MIT Press, 2013).
- 37.Quintana DS, Williams DR. Bayesian alternatives for common null-hypothesis significance tests in psychiatry: A non-technical guide using JASP. BMC Psychiatry. 2018;18:178. doi: 10.1186/s12888-018-1761-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Masland RH. The neuronal organization of the retina. Neuron. 2012;76:266–280. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.10.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Neuhauss SC. Behavioral genetic approaches to visual system development and function in zebrafish. J. Neurobiol. 2003;54:148–160. doi: 10.1002/neu.10165. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Neuhauss SC, et al. Genetic disorders of vision revealed by a behavioral screen of 400 essential loci in zebrafish. J. Neurosci. 1999;19:8603–8615. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.19-19-08603.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Muto A, et al. Forward genetic analysis of visual behavior in zebrafish. PLoS Genet. 2005;1:e66. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.0010066. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Xie J, Jusuf PR, Goodbourn PT, Bui BV. Electroretinogram recording in larval zebrafish using a novel cone-shaped sponge-tip electrode. J. Vis. Exp. 2019;145:e59487. doi: 10.3791/59487. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Tian N, Copenhagen DR. Visual deprivation alters development of synaptic function in inner retina after eye opening. Neuron. 2001;32:439–449. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00470-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Gnuegge L, Schmid S, Neuhauss SCF. Analysis of the activity-deprived zebrafish mutant macho reveals an essential requirement of neuronal activity for the development of a fine-grained visuotopic map. J. Neurosci. 2001;21:3542–3548. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.21-10-03542.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Saszik S, Bilotta J. Constant dark-rearing effects on visual adaptation of the zebrafish ERG. Int. J. Dev. Neurosci. 2001;19:611–619. doi: 10.1016/S0736-5748(01)00051-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Dixon LJ, McDowell AL, Houchins JD, Bilotta J. Effects of restricted spectral rearing on the development of zebrafish retinal physiology. Doc. Ophthalmol. 2004;109:17–33. doi: 10.1007/s10633-004-1124-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Powers MK, Bassi C, Raymond P. Lighting conditions and retinal development in goldfish: Absolute visual sensitivity. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 1988;29:37–43. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Mower GD. The effect of dark rearing on the time course of the critical period in cat visual cortex. Dev. Brain Res. 1991;58:151–158. doi: 10.1016/0165-3806(91)90001-Y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Borges S, Berry M. The effects of dark rearing on the development of the visual cortex of the rat. J. Comp. Neurol. 1978;180:277–300. doi: 10.1002/cne.901800207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Ellemberg D, et al. Putting order into the development of sensitivity to global motion. Vision Res. 2004;44:2403–2411. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2004.05.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Engert F, Tao HW, Zhang LI, Poo M-M. Moving visual stimuli rapidly induce direction sensitivity of developing tectal neurons. Nature. 2002;419:470–475. doi: 10.1038/nature00988. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Greenberg ME, Xu B, Lu B, Hempstead BL. New insights in the biology of BDNF synthesis and release: Implications in CNS function. J. Neurosci. 2009;29:12764–12767. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.3566-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Landi S, et al. Retinal functional development is sensitive to environmental enrichment: A role for BDNF. FASEB J. 2007;21:130–139. doi: 10.1096/fj.06-6083com. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Bramham CR, Messaoudi E. BDNF function in adult synaptic plasticity: The synaptic consolidation hypothesis. Prog. Neurobiol. 2005;76:99–125. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2005.06.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Wei H-P, Yao Y-Y, Zhang R-W, Zhao X-F, Du J-L. Activity-induced long-term potentiation of excitatory synapses in developing zebrafish retina in vivo. Neuron. 2012;75:479–489. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.05.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Béïque J-C, Andrade R. PSD-95 regulates synaptic transmission and plasticity in rat cerebral cortex. J. Physiol. 2003;546:859–867. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2002.031369. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Sun W, Deng Q, Levick WR, He S. ON direction-selective ganglion cells in the mouse retina. J. Physiol. 2006;576:197–202. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2006.115857. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Yoshida K, et al. A key role of starburst amacrine cells in originating retinal directional selectivity and optokinetic eye movement. Neuron. 2001;30:771–780. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00316-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Orger MB, Smear MC, Anstis SM, Baier H. Perception of Fourier and non-Fourier motion by larval zebrafish. Nat. Neurosci. 2000;3:1128–1133. doi: 10.1038/80649. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Naumann EA, et al. From whole-brain data to functional circuit models: The zebrafish optomotor response. Cell. 2016;167:947–960. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2016.10.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The datasets supporting this article are available on the Open Science Framework (https://osf.io/8emn7/).