Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2019 Dec 13;14(12):e0226418. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0226418

The Intersection of Human Disturbance and Diel Activity, with Potential Consequences on Trophic Interactions

Michael A Patten 1,2,*, Jutta C Burger 3,¤, Milan Mitrovich 4
Editor: Paulo Corti5
PMCID: PMC6910683  PMID: 31834919

Abstract

Direct effects of human disturbance on animal populations are well documented across habitats, biomes, and species, but indirect effects of diel have received less attention. An emerging field in applied ecology involves behavioral avoidance of or attraction to humans and their trappings. We posit trophic consequences, in terms of relative risk, for four species of mammals, each of which strongly avoids human activity, in urban reserves of coastal southern California. Two species, one predator and one prey, avoid human activity via a temporal shift to become “more nocturnal”—the species’ activity is centered near dawn on days without human activity but nearer to midnight on days with human activity. Diel shifts have brought the species into greater overlap, respectively, with a key prey and a key predator, overlap that may increase encounter rate and thus increase relative risk of predation, with potential consequences for trophic dynamics and cascades: increased risk of predation may depress prey population, either directly (e.g., mortality) or indirectly (e.g., “landscape of fear”). Human use of reserves, especially in high population density regions, needs to be reconsidered either to reduce access or to restrict access entirely to areas that may provide refuge to both predators and prey.

Introduction

Predation is a principal force that shapes an ecosystem. Predator–prey dynamics are complex; many pieces must fall into place before any individual is attacked and consumed by a predator. In this light predation can be thought of as a multi-stage process, the stages being encounter between prey and predator, active choice of prey species by a predator, and success of any given attack [1]. Because prey pay the ultimate price, it is expected that prey reduce their exposure to predators, and there is a good deal of evidence in support of this expectation [2]. Key anti-predator defenses include a decrease in activity or an increase in time spent in hiding [3]. Seclusion in a refuge need not be spatial but may be temporal.

Humans can disrupt natural predator-prey dynamics. Animals respond to human presence sometimes positively [4] but usually negatively [56]. Avoidance is a common response, particularly avoidance of areas with high levels of human activity or infrastructure [79], and avoidance can have the same ecological effects as predation [1011]. Wildlife response also is affected by the type of activity; for example, domestic dogs that accompany hikers have an additive effect on wildlife displacement relative to hikers alone [12], and bicyclists plus hikers have a comparable additive or interactive effect relative to hikers alone [13].

Yet an animal cannot avoid an area of high human activity if accessible alternative habitat does not exist [6], a problem exacerbated in regions that have been developed extensively. In coastal southern California, for instance, human population continues to grow and so habitat loss continues apace. In this region wildlife response tends toward avoidance of areas of high human activity or heavy urbanization [1417], with some species displaced temporally: they are more active at night is areas of high human activity [1415,18]. Such temporal shifts have been documented in other regions [1921] and could be ubiquitous in areas with high human activity [22].

Although diel or temporal shifts in activity in response to humans are well documented [2225], much of this documentation comes from single-species studies or from multi-species studies in which each species was considered serially rather than in parallel. Moreover, in many cases shifts have been predicted or documented only for predators [2627], even though various studies have shown that prey species (typically primary consumers) shift their activities in time or space to avoid humans [21,2829]. It is reasonable to conclude that any species not a human commensal (or otherwise disturbance tolerant) will alter its behavior if humans are present. An outstanding question is not whether species shift their use of time or space in response to human presence but how such shifts alter trophic interactions and dynamics. Kuijper and colleagues [30] metaphorically referred to this effect as the “human shadow” cast on how predators and prey interact, and Magle and colleagues [29] speculated that human activities altered predator–prey dynamics when predator and prey were relegated to share habitat patches as a result of human encroachment.

An unanswered question is whether anthropogenic-induced diel shifts alter predator–prey dynamics, specifically in terms of altering encounter rate. Because “factors that increase a prey’s encounter rate… should have a positive effect” on predation rate [3], high levels of human activity could, in principle, depress further recruitment levels depressed already by habitat loss and fragmentation. Our key research goal, then, was to lay groundwork to study the extent to which human disturbance alters the probability of encounter between prey species and their predators. Groundwork was provided by a large set (nearly 90,000 records) of camera-trap data collected across eight years at >50 stations situated in coastal southern California. We examined patterns across seven mammal species but focused on two pairs of predator and prey, the Puma (Puma concolor) and the Mule Deer (Odocoileus hemionus), its principal food in the region [31], and the Coyote (Canis latrans) and the Gray Fox (Urocyon cinereoargenteus), of which the Coyote is behaviorally dominant and a frequent predator [3233]. We assessed not only diel shifts but further explored how encounter rates might change and speculated on the consequences of any a change.

Methods

Study area

The study was conducted in Orange County, California, within a complex of urban and urban-adjacent preserves once privately owned and now publicly owned by the County of Orange, City of Irvine, and City of Newport Beach [17]. Parks in our study span ~12,000 ha (~30,000 acres) in central and coastal Orange County and are protected further through either the Orange County Central and Coastal Natural Community Conservation Plan (NCCP) or by conservation easements, as well as deed restrictions, park abandonment ordinances, and other legal mechanisms. These lands are adjacent both to a heavily urbanized landscape, which supports over three million people, and near to ~28,300 ha (70,000 acres) of U.S. National Forest. Public access is not only permitted but encouraged, chiefly via a managed access program with permit-only entry, regular docent-led programs, and monthly self-guided wilderness access days. Dogs are not permitted on managed-access lands. Climate is Mediterranean, and habitats consist of coastal sage scrub, chaparral, grassland, and oak woodland, with some ephemeral streams and permanent water sources (natural and man-made).

Data set

Fixed location digital wildlife cameras (Cuddeback Expert 3300, Non Typical, Inc., Green Bay, Wisconsin, and HCO Scoutguard SG-565F, HCO Outdoor Products, Norcross Georgia) were installed across the landscape to monitor human and wildlife activity long term in the reserve system (S1 Table, S1 Fig). Cameras were installed and monitored as part of a reserve-wide wildlife and human activity monitoring program managed by the Irvine Ranch Conservancy long-term land management agreements with the following land owners: The Irvine Company (no contract number), Orange County Parks (MA-012-13012148), City of Irvine (MA-4950), and City of Newport Beach (contract 03012008). Additional access and implementation permits were not needed because monitoring activities were encompassed in the scope of work for each management agreement. All landowners and federal and state agencies associated with the project team knew of and supported the project as a tool to inform adaptive management of wildlife and human activity in the NCCP and the adjacent reserve system.

Cuddeback cameras were used from 2007–2011, Scoutguard from 2012–2015 (the former were no longer available commercially). Different makes and models of cameras respond to stimulus in different ways, which could affect estimates of population size or occupancy, but our analyses and comparisons were relative (e.g., averages within-camera captures), so a correction factor was unnecessary. Cuddeback cameras had a flash range of 18 m, were triggered instantly by motion (6–30 m distance) and heat, and a had narrow field of view of 2 m at 10 m distance; Scoutguard cameras had a flash range of 15 m, were triggered at 2–10 m, and had a wide detection angle of 52°. Sensitivity of each camera was adjusted to maximize probability of species detections but minimize superfluous photographs of moving vegetation or shadows. Each camera was set for a 1-min delay between photographs to minimize duplicates of the same individual. Images were stamped with date and time on 1-GB compact flash cards, which were collected every two weeks. For each identifiable photograph, date, time, species detected, number of individuals, trap location, and notes were entered into the relational biodiversity database program Biota 2.04®.

Data from 50 cameras were analyzed from June 2007–March 2015, although not all cameras operated continuously over that period. Thirty cameras were sited along trails, six adjacent to water troughs or water sources, and the remainder off-trail. Trail cameras were positioned obliquely (i.e., not perpendicularly) to improve capture rates of humans and wildlife, and all cameras were sited to maximize probability of detection and to provide a representative sample of managed urban-adjacent wildlands (for a map of camera placement, see ref. 17). Twenty-eight cameras recorded activity continuously since June 2007. If a human, bicycle, or vehicle was detected repeatedly within 1 h then all such photos in that hour were summed as a single record, chiefly to avoid over-counting people and their accouterments. If a wildlife species occurred repeatedly within a 5-min period, then photos were tallied as a single record; only ~15% (4792 of 30519) of mammal detections, species for species, occurred >5 min. and <1 h, and reducing the data set to a 1-h cutoff did not materially change results (S2 Fig).

Analyses

Diel patterns of seven species—the Bobcat (Lynx rufus), Puma, Gray Fox, Coyote, Striped Skunk (Mephitis mephitis), Northern Raccoon (Procyon lotor), and Mule Deer—in relation to human activity were analyzed using the full data set from June 2007–March 2015, which yielded 87,881 photos identifiable to species or activity. We considered all human activities—hiker, bicyclists, equestrian, vehicle, or domestic dog (despite prohibitions, many were detected, typically accompanied by a person on foot)—to constitute anthropogenic disturbance; in the study area, effects of each activity differed but all are negative [17]. We classified each focal mammal occurrence as either “undisturbed” or “disturbed,” the latter defined as human activity at the same camera <24 h prior to a focal mammal detection. (We also defined disturbance in two other ways, as human activity <12 h prior or human activity between noon and sunrise the following morning, but results were consistent irrespective of definition. In no case did we consider human presence after a mammal detection; i.e., a human had to have been detected first.)

We analyzed daily activity using circular statistics, the “circle” being a 24-h clock. For each species we used Rayleigh’s test to determine if activity departed from a null of random occurrence across a day. Circular analogs to ANOVA have restrictive assumptions that our data did not meet, so we used a non-parametric (and hence conservative) rank sum test of difference in mean angle to compare mean activity on undisturbed vs. disturbed days [34]. We used SAS 9.3 (SAS Institute, Cary, N.C.) for the rank sums test and package ‘circular’ in R or a spreadsheet for other tests. We quantified extent of activity overlap between species A and species B in undisturbed vs. disturbed conditions. To do so we used the polygon tool in ImageJ shareware to estimate shared activity (i.e., A ∩ B) against total activity (A U B). The quotient of the intersection and union is an estimate of proportion of overlap. We supplemented this simple descriptive step with a Bayesian estimate of a statistic, d, the trigonometric relationship at the heart of the Watson–Wheeler two-sample test of difference in mean vector, the logic of which is described and illustrated in Batschelet [34]. In short, the statistic assess whether the mean angle for species A differs from the mean angle for species B by comparing their sum to a composite angle (a “grand mean” vector). Our JAGS code to estimate d used a von Mises process for the likelihood and flat priors for mean angle.

We express encounter between predator and prey as odds ratios—which for these data is a measure of relative risk—of joint probability of disturbed vs. undisturbed occurrences binned at 15-min intervals. For any given species, the probability of occurrence in an interval is the quotient of the sum of occurrences in that interval and the total occurrences of that species. The joint probability of occurrence of predator and prey is the product of probabilities per interval, with overall joint probability the sum of all within-interval joint probabilities (i.e., the odds of two odds ratios). In this way we calculate a joint probability for undisturbed (pu) and disturbed (pd), with the odds ratio estimated as [pd ⋅(1- pu)]/[pu ⋅(1- pd)]. We estimated 95% Bayesian credible intervals (CI)—in JAGS and R, with a Bernoulli process for the likelihood—for the odds ratios to assess statistical significance [35]; i.e., we determined whether the odds ratio differed from 1.0 (i.e., no relative risk) by means of whether the CI overlapped 1.0.

The predator–prey system of the Puma and Mule Deer is coupled tightly, so we focused on it to examine potential effects of diel shifts. We plotted annual estimated abundance from an N-mixture model (package ‘unmarked’ in R) of each species and of human disturbance. An N-mixture model is, in effect, an occupancy model wherein the response variable is abundance rather than presence/absence, with an occupancy model being an approach to estimate the probability of occupancy (Ψ) conditional on the probability of detection (p). We can extract from an N-mixture model both an estimate of abundance and an estimate of Ψ, both of them adjusted for how easy or difficult it is to detect a species. Annual data for the N-mixture model were detections per quarter (March, June, September, and December) per camera-day, such that each year had only two (2016), three (2007), or four (2008–2015) “repeat” surveys per camera. If a diel shift increased encounter odds, then we predicted deer detections to decrease while Puma detections would be stable or increase. We explored this prediction both via a plot of mean estimated abundance across years and via slopes from a Bayesian beta regression of Ψ against year (in JAGS and R, with flat priors, a burn-in of 100,000, and MCMC iterations of 1,000,000 thinned by 1000). Lastly, we calculated expected versus observed joint occurrence, meaning detections of both a deer and a Puma at the same camera on the same day, to infer whether deer co-occur as often as expected if their presence was independent of the Puma’s presence.

Results

As expected, human activity was concentrated between mid-morning and noon, with ~95% of activity during daylight hours (S3 Fig). Disturbance varied among cameras: the proportion of days with human presence averaged 0.788 (SD ≈ 0.285, estimated from the weighted mean reported here), with a range of 0.128 to 1.000. With regard to the maximum proportion, human presence was detected daily at two of the fifty cameras, although neither of these cameras was operated particularly long (31 days and 165 days).

Four of the seven mammal species for which we had sufficient data shifted their temporal distribution in response to human presence (Table 1, S4 Fig). Shifts were particularly striking for two species, the Coyote, from shortly before dawn to shortly before midnight (Fig 1), and the Mule Deer, from around sunrise to shortly after sunset (Fig 2). Marked diel shifts in these two species increased overlap from ~⅓ to ~½ with their respective interactors in predator–prey dynamics, the Gray Fox and the Puma (Figs 1 and 2), neither of which shifted temporally (Table 1). Under undisturbed conditions mean activity of the Coyote and fox differed (d = 148 with Bayesian 95% CI of [125, 171]), but under disturbed conditions mean activity did not differ (d = 1 [–14, 16]). Likewise, under undisturbed conditions mean activity of the Puma and deer differed (d = 222 [122, 317]), but under disturbed conditions mean activity did not differ (d = 11 [–4, 26]). Each diel shift increased the relative risk of predation of both the Gray Fox (odds ratio [95% Bayesian credible intervals]: 1.39 [1.14, 1.64]) and the Mule Deer (1.31 [1.06, 1.56]).

Table 1. Temporal distribution across undisturbed and disturbed days for seven mammal species in coastal Orange County, California.

All means (from angle θ, with it and 95% confidence intervals, CI, converted to time of day, t, on a 24-h clock) differed significantly (Rayleigh test: P < 0.0001) from a random distribution of occurrences around the clock. Statistic r, which varies from 0 to 1, is a standardized measure of concentration at the mean. At the right, results are presented a rank sum test of temporal shift between undisturbed vs. disturbed occurrences.

undisturbed disturbed
species mean t r n mean t r n Z P
Lynx rufus 00:24 0.20 2140 22:44 0.42 992 -3.53 <0.01
Puma concolor 00:15 0.40 514 22:58 0.59 157 -1.36 0.17
Urocyon cinereoargenteus 00:34 0.60 971 23:10 0.70 268 -1.42 0.16
Canis latrans 04:04 0.26 3671 23:08 0.40 2440 -9.04 <0.01
Mephitis mephitis 01:30 0.61 280 00:52 0.74 108 -2.06 0.04
Procyon lotor 00:23 0.55 599 23:59 0.62 91 -1.65 0.10
Odocoileus hemionus 06:44 0.15 15689 20:56 0.24 2471 -6.48 <0.01

Fig 1. Diel activity of the Gray Fox (Urocyon cinereoargenteus) and Coyote (Canis latrans) with or without human disturbance.

Fig 1

Arrows indicate time (direction) and proportional magnitude (length) of mean activity, and the “net” displays the spread of activity on a 24-h clock, binned at 30-min. intervals. Note the predator’s (the Coyote) nocturnal shift when disturbance was present. The extent of overlap in activity increased from 33% to 45%.

Fig 2. Diel activity of the Mule Deer (Odocoileus hemionus) and Puma (Puma concolor) with and without human disturbance; see Fig 1 for an explanation of the graphs.

Fig 2

Note the prey’s (the deer) nocturnal shift when disturbance was present. The extent of overlap in activity increased from 35% to 54%.

With probability 0.94, the true and underlying slope (β1 = -0.13) of a beta regression of deer occupancy against year was negative, whereas with probability 0.87, the true and underlying slope (β1 = 0.07) of a beta regression of Puma occupancy against year was positive (Fig 3). Also, the observed number of days on which both the Mule Deer and Puma were recorded at the same camera consistently was lower than an expectation if presence was independent of each other (Table 2).

Fig 3. Mean estimated abundance (from an N-mixture model, ±SE, and on a common log scale) of the Mule Deer (Odocoileus hemionus) and Puma (Puma concolor), as well as human disturbance for reference, with heuristic plots of mean slopes from a linear regression.

Fig 3

It is probable (Bayesian posterior P = 0.94) that deer occupancy (Ψ) has decreased over time whereas Puma occupancy has increased (Bayesian posterior P = 0.87). (Occupancy estimates for the Puma were unstable for 2007 and so abundance could not be estimated. Also, probability of detection was not correlated with year, although it may have increased slightly for the deer.).

Table 2. Presence of the Mule Deer (Odocoileus hemionus) and the Puma (Puma concolor) across camera traps, with expected versus observed incidence of joint presence.

Data were aggregated by calendar day for the focal species alone (i.e., camera-trap records of species other than the deer and Puma were not considered) and restricted to cameras that had at least one presence of the Puma. Expected values were estimated from a joint probability under the assumption that the deer and Puma occur independently, meaning observed joint (i.e., same day) presence much lower than expected implies that occurrence is not independent.

proportion of days with a presence joint probability
camera n Mule Deer Puma expected observed
AG CH 77 0.675 0.325 17 0
AU TR 509 0.986 0.022 11 4
BL ST 66 0.924 0.091 6 1
BO SP 618 0.989 0.013 8 1
BO TR 582 0.930 0.124 67 31
CO MI 182 0.967 0.038 7 1
CO TR 173 0.908 0.104 16 2
DO CA 106 0.830 0.179 16 1
DO CA2 50 0.900 0.120 5 1
DO CA3 29 0.897 0.103 3 0
DR SP 207 0.807 0.232 39 8
DR SP2 149 0.926 0.121 17 7
EA MW 445 0.971 0.043 18 6
EA MW2 41 0.976 0.024 1 0
FR RO 231 0.939 0.087 19 6
FU BR 204 0.941 0.069 13 2
GY FO 243 0.930 0.119 27 12
LA RO 319 0.937 0.085 25 7
LI ME 131 0.832 0.198 22 4
LI ME2 35 0.771 0.286 8 2
LI SI 141 0.816 0.270 31 12
LO WE1 121 0.992 0.008 1 0
LO WE2 274 0.737 0.281 57 5
ME SP 10 0.800 0.200 2 0
MO FR1 215 0.837 0.181 33 4
MO FR3 178 0.781 0.247 34 5
OV TR 195 0.964 0.036 7 0
RA TR 77 0.987 0.026 2 1
RO CA 134 0.993 0.007 1 0
RO CA2 35 0.857 0.171 5 1
SO GY 204 0.868 0.157 28 5
UP WE 133 0.932 0.075 9 1
WE SP 458 0.985 0.026 12 5
WE TR1 791 0.991 0.059 47 40
WE TR2 627 0.994 0.038 24 20
WE TR3 359 0.997 0.028 10 9
WE WI 71 0.930 0.070 5 0

Discussion

Response of large and medium-sized mammals to human activity is difficult to characterize because it varies in time and space and with species and type of activity [36], yet an increasing body of literature suggests that the response is negative: in general, wild animals avoid humans, a pattern well established in coastal southern California [9,1517]. A pattern is emerging, yet mechanisms by which animals avoid humans or come into conflict with humans often (but not always [37]) are not. Our data suggest that one mechanism involves diel shifts away from daylight hours, when human activity dominates—e.g., in our study area, nearly 95% (54,170 of 57,359 occurrences) of human disturbance was detected between 07:00 a.m. and 07:00 p.m. As a result, when disturbance was present the temporal distribution of two species, the Coyote and Mule Deer, shifted away from early morning to nearer the middle of the night (Figs 2 and 3). Such diel shifts may be commonplace [22], yet potential ecological implications of avoidance or conflict is little explored.

Our finding that relative risk of predation increases after human-induced diel shifts suggest important ramifications for predator–prey dynamics. We couch potential effects in terms of relative risk to draw a parallel between predation and disease transmission. In the latter, exposure to a disease is no guarantee of transmission, but as risk of exposure increases the probability of contracting that disease increases. Likewise, increased risk of predation via diel shifts does not mean that a given individual will be killed, but that individuals’ risk of mortality has increased. In this light, it is crucial to note that shifts we detected were not solely of species in one trophic level (e.g., secondary consumers); instead, we uncovered evidence for a shift in a primary consumer that brought it into greater temporal alignment with its chief predator (the Mule Deer into the Puma’s activity window) and for a shift in a secondary consumer that brought it into greater temporal alignment with prey (the Coyote into the Gray Fox’s activity window). In either case the post-shift increase in relative risk can be seen as an increase in encounter rate. All else being equal, on the basis of Holling’s disc equation—a means to estimate a predator’s potential functional response to an increase in prey—increased encounter yields a higher rate of predation [38], which ultimately may depress prey populations.

Recent studies have underscored how altered predation regimes affect ecosystems [3942]. We lack direct evidence for increased rates of predation in our system, yet indirectly we see Mule Deer decreases and Puma increases coincident with increased human activity (Fig 3). And even if predation rates change little, increased presence of predators—or, indeed, increased perceived predation risk [3940]—may nonetheless adversely affect prey species’ behavior (e.g., habitat occupancy). For example, the risk of predation may itself be detrimental in that the mere presence of predators can decrease survival or fecundity of prey species [43], and temporal shifts of competitively or behaviorally dominant species, such as the Coyote, may shrink niche space of subordinate species, such as the Gray Fox [44]. Lastly, given that the Coyote and Gray Fox are potential prey for the Puma, the Coyote’s shift to a more nocturnal pattern may further alter predator–prey dynamics relative to undisturbed systems.

We posit that subtle stress on prey populations that results from increased temporal overlap of predator and prey in combination with other population stressors, such as habitat loss, reduced habitat connectivity, and increased human activity [1517], has ramifications for wildlife in urban wildlands where spatial and temporal refugia are limited. Human disturbance adds a stressor to fragmented landscapes that often support compromised habitats, and its effect may be unsustainable if not managed properly. It is possible that some prey find refuge from predators when humans are numerous because predators also avoid humans [26], but reserves in our study area are not large, so opportunity for spatial shifts is limited. In our system, human presence was as strongly negatively associated with the spatial and temporal activities of a primary consumer as it was for any of predators [17]. This effect was stark regardless of how we measured “disturbance”: the pattern held whether humans were on foot, on bicycles, in vehicles, or on horseback or whether dogs accompanied them, the last a finding consistent with other work [45]. Further, we found that activity of the Mule Deer and Coyote shifted temporally in a manner that increased probability of a predator–prey encounter in the absence of other avoidance behavior, adding a previously unrecognized stressor to wildlife prey species.

Despite the strong signal, our quantification may underestimate effects because we examined human presence rather than human abundance. Studies elsewhere have found that behavioral response of large mammals may not be apparent at low densities of humans and their accouterments but are as density increased [21,28]. Reserves in Orange County and other urban areas are visited by considerably more people than are protected areas in the studies cited above, so it may be that effects on wild animals of increased human density are stronger in urban areas. Yet access to reserves is important if we hope to foster a sense of stewardship and passion for natural habitats and their denizens, even if we must manage access carefully (and perhaps even strictly) if we hope to conserve natural habitats and their denizens. The majority of the protected landscapes in this study are under managed access and therefore generally subject to a clumped distribution of human activity and a lower level of overall activity relative to neighboring parks that have 7-day access. As human use of parks increases, more research will be needed on both community-level and potential cascading effects of recreation [46] Further studies that test specifically for spatial effects of recreation on predator-prey dynamics, such as using a grid of cameras, could test the degree and scale of spatial impact on predator-prey behavioral shifts we suggest herein.

Supporting information

S1 Table. Specific locations of camera traps in coastal Orange County, California, 2007–2016.

(PDF)

S1 Fig. Active cameras 2007–2015.

(PDF)

S2 Fig. Parameter estimates of peak activity of the Mule Deer (Odocoileus hemionus) and Coyote (Canis latrans) when camera trap images are excluded within 1 hour of the previous image of that same species or only within 5 minutes of the previous image.

(PDF)

S3 Fig. Temporal distributions for human activity in this study.

(PDF)

S4 Fig. Temporal distributions for “non focal” species in this study.

(PDF)

Acknowledgments

Special thanks to Irvine Ranch Conservancy staff members M. Fowler, D. Swenson, E. Sheehan, K. Brtalik, G. Salgado, A. Nelson, S. Lagarile, C. McCammon, R. Pratt, and S. Anon for their assistance with data gathering, and to UCSB Bren Intern Nico Alegria for help with data management. J. Paludi, A. and M. Fowler installed most cameras, and Y. C. Fang drafted maps. IRC volunteers, especially D. Newell, D. Millar, P. Wetzel, C. Carter, J. Hanson, D. Mahalik, and E. Hill, serviced wildlife cameras after installation. Our thanks to project team members W. Miller (USFWS), C. Beck (CDFW), J. Gump (OC Parks), and E. Boydston (USGS) for discussion and input and to members of the Patten lab (A. E. Adams, M. Dantzler-Kyer, D. C. Hille, E. A. Hjalmarson, G. Shahrokhi) for feedback on some ideas presented herein. We particularly appreciate a thorough and thoughtful review provided by W. Vickers of a pre-submission draft of the manuscript.

Data Availability

Data cannot be shared publicly because they are co-owned by multiple land owners and the Irvine Ranch Conservancy (IRC), but they are available from IRC for researchers who meet approval for data access. Contact Yi-Chin Fang (yfang@irconservancy.org) for access requests.

Funding Statement

This work was funded by Local Assistance Grant #P1482109 awarded by the California Department of Fish and Wildlife. https://www.wildlife.ca.gov/Conservation/Planning/NCCP/Grants The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1.Sih A, Christensen B. Optimal diet theory: when does it work, and when and why does it fail? Anim Behav. 2001; 61:379–390. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Lima SL. Stress and decision-making under the risk of predation: recent developments from behavioral, reproductive, and ecological perspectives. Adv Stud Behav. 1998; 27:215–290. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Mittelbach GG Community ecology. 2012. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates. [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Boarman WI, Patten MA, Camp RJ, Collis SJ. Ecology of a population of subsidized predators: Common Ravens in the central Mojave Desert, California. J Arid Environ. 2006; 67:248–261. [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Whittaker D, Knight RL. Understanding wildlife responses to humans. Wildl Soc Bull. 1998; 26:312–317. [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Stankowich T. Ungulate flight response to human disturbance: a review and meta-analysis. Biol Conserv. 2008; 141:2159–2173. [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Pruett CL, Patten MA, Wolfe DH. Avoidance behavior by prairie grouse: implications for wind energy development. Conserv Biol. 2009; 23:1253–1259. 10.1111/j.1523-1739.2009.01254.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Patten MA, Kelly JF. Habitat selection and the perceptual trap. Ecol Appl. 2010; 20:2148–2156. 10.1890/09-2370.1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Poessel SA, Burdett CL, Boydston EE, Lyren LM, Alonso RS, Fisher RN, et al. Roads influence movement and home range of a fragmentation-sensitive carnivore, the Bobcat, in an urban landscape. Biol Conserv. 2014; 180:224–232. [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Frid A, Dill LM. Human-caused disturbance stimuli as a form of predation risk. Conserv Ecol. 2002; 6(1):11. [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Beale CM, Monaghan P. Human disturbance: people as predation-free predators? J Appl Ecol. 2004; 41:335–343. [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Miller SG, Knight RL, Miller CK. Wildlife responses to pedestrians and dogs. Wildl Soc Bull. 2001; 29:124–132. [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Taylor AR, Knight RL. Wildlife response to recreation and associated visitor perceptions. Ecol Appl. 2008; 13:951–963. [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Riley SJ, Sauvajot RM, Fuller TK, York EC, Kamradt DA, Bromley C, et al. Effects of urbanization and habitat fragmentation on Bobcats and Coyotes in southern California. Conserv Biol. 2003; 17:566–576. [Google Scholar]
  • 15.George SL, Crooks KR. Recreation and large mammal activity in an urban nature reserve. Biol Conserv. 2006; 133:107–117. [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Ordeñana MA, Crooks KR, Boydston EE, Fisher RN, Lyren LM, Siudyla S, et al. Effects of urbanization on carnivore species distribution and richness. J Mammal. 2010; 91:1322–1331. [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Patten MA, Burger JC. Reserves as double-edged sword: avoidance behavior in an urban-adjacent wildland. Biol Conserv. 2018; 218:233–239. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Tigas LA, Van Vuren DH, Sauvajot RM. Behavioral responses of Bobcats and Coyotes to habitat fragmentation and corridors in an urban environment. Biol Conserv. 2002; 108:299–306. [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Naugle DE, Jenks JA, Kernohan BJ, Johnson RR. Effects of hunting and loss of escape cover on movements and activity of female White-tailed Deer, Odocoileus virginianus. Can Field-Nat. 1997; 111:595–600. [Google Scholar]
  • 20.McClennen N, Wigglesworth RR, Anderson SH, Wachob DG. The effect of suburban and agricultural development on the activity patterns of Coyotes (Canis latrans). Am Midl Nat. 2001; 146:27–36. [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Rogala JK, Hebblewhite M, Whittington J, White CA, Coleshill J, Musiani M. Human activity differentially redistributes large mammals in the Canadian Rockies national parks. Ecol Soc. 2011; 16(3):16. [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Gaynor KM, Hojnowski CE, Carter NH, Brashares JS. The influence of human disturbance on wildlife nocturnality. Science. 2018; 360:1232–1235. 10.1126/science.aar7121 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Carter NH, Shrestha BK, Karki JB, Pradhan NMB, Liu J. Coexistence between wildlife and humans at fine spatial scales. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2012; 109:15360–15365. 10.1073/pnas.1210490109 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Poudel BS, Spooner PG, Matthews A. Temporal shift in activity patterns of Himalayan marmots in relation to pastoralism. Behav Ecol. 2015; 26:1345–1351. [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Reilly ML, Tobler MW, Sonderegger DL, Beier P. Spatial and temporal response of wildlife to recreational activities in the San Francisco Bay ecoregion. Biol Conserv. 2017; 207:117–126. [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Muhly TB, Semeniuk C, Massolo A, Hickman L, Musiani M. Human activity helps prey win the predator–prey space race. PLoS one. 2011; 6(3):e17050 10.1371/journal.pone.0017050 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Monterroso P, Célio Alves P, Ferreras P. Plasticity in circadian activity patterns of mesocarnivores in Southwestern Europe: implications for species coexistence. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2014; 68:1403–1417. [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Ciuti S, Northrup JM, Muhly TB, Simi S, Musiani M, Pitt JA, et al. Effects of humans on behaviour of wildlife exceed those of natural predators in a landscape of fear. PLoS one. 2012; 7(11):e50611 10.1371/journal.pone.0050611 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Magle SB, Simoni LS, Lehrer EW, Brown JS. Urban predator–prey association: Coyote and deer distributions in the Chicago metropolitan area. Urban Ecosyst. 2014; 17:875–891. [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Kuijper DPJ, Bubnicki JW, Churski M, Mols B, van Hooft P. Context dependence of risk effects: wolves and tree logs create patches of fear in an old-growth forest. Behav Ecol. 2015; 26:1558–1568. [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Dickson BG, Beier P. Home-range and habitat selection by adult cougars in southern California. J Wildl Manage. 2002; 66:1235–1245. [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Fedriani JM, Fuller TK, Sauvajot RM, York CE. Competition and intraguild predation among three sympatric carnivores. Oecologia. 2000; 125:258–270. 10.1007/s004420000448 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Farias V, Fuller TK, Wayne RK, Sauvajot RM. Survival and cause-specific mortality of Gray Foxes (Urocyon cinereoargenteus) in southern California. J Zool. 2005; 266:249–254. [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Batschelet E. Circular statistics in biology. 1981. London, UK: Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Morris JA, Gardner MJ. Calculating confidence intervals for relative risks (odds ratios) and standardised ratios and rates. Brit Med J. 1988; 296:1313–1316. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Tracey JA, Zhu J, Boydston E, Lyren L, Fisher RN, Crooks KR. Mapping behavioral landscapes for animal movement: a finite mixture modeling approach. Ecol Appl. 2013; 23:654–669. 10.1890/12-0687.1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Goswami VR, Medhi K., Nichols JD, Oli MK. Mechanistic understanding of human–wildlife conflict through a novel application of dynamic occupancy models. Conserv Biol. 2015; 29:1100–1110. 10.1111/cobi.12475 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Dale BW, Adams LG, Bowyer RT. Functional response of wolves preying on barren-ground caribou in a multiple-prey ecosystem. J Anim Ecol. 1994; 63:644–652. [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Suraci JP1, Clinchy M, Dill LM, Roberts D, Zanette LY. Fear of large carnivores causes a trophic cascade, Nat Comm. 2016; 7:10698. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Atkins JL, Long RA, Pansu J, Daskin JH, Potter AB, Stalmans ME, et al. Cascading impacts of large-carnivore extirpation in an African ecosystem. Science. 2019; 364:173–177. 10.1126/science.aau3561 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Koel TM, Tronstad LM, Arnold JL, Gunther KA, Smith DW, Syslo JM, et al. Predatory fish invasion induces within and across ecosystem effects in Yellowstone National Park. Sci Adv. 2019; 5:eaav1139 10.1126/sciadv.aav1139 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Suraci JP, Clinchy M, Zanette LY, Wilmers CC. Fear of humans as apex predators has landscape-scale impacts from mountain lions to mice. Ecol Lett. 2019; 22:1578–1586. 10.1111/ele.13344 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.MacLeod KJ, Krebs CJ, Boonstra R, Sheriff MJ. Fear and lethality in snowshoe hares: the deadly effects of non-consumptive predation risk. Oikos. 2018; 127:375–380. [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Smith JA, Thomas AC, Levi T, Wang Y, Wilmers CC. Human activity reduces niche partitioning among three widespread mesocarnivores. Oikos. 2018; 127:890–901. [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Reed SE, Merenlender AM. Effects of management of domestic dogs and recreation on carnivores in protected areas in northern California. Conserv Biol. 2011; 25:504–513. 10.1111/j.1523-1739.2010.01641.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Larson CL, Reed SE, Merenlender AM, Crooks KR. Effects of recreation on animals revealed as widespread through a global systematic review. PLoS one. 2016; 11(12):e0167259 10.1371/journal.pone.0167259 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Paulo Corti

10 Jul 2019

PONE-D-19-14511

The Intersection of Human Disturbance and Diel Activity, with Potential Consequences on Trophic Interactions

PLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Patten,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

​Both reviewers found that this study is valuable and well written, so they provided constructive comments to improve the MS. However, both referees manifested that methodology and statistical analyses need to be reviewed carefully. Then, please check your MS throughly to asses referees' comments, doubts, and suggestions, paying attention to following issues: human activity patterns (disturbed vs undisturbed must be clarified); diel patterns you found should be related at temporal or spatial level; sharing space between prey and predators; the use of peak activity as measurement; analyses using abundance time series.

We would appreciate receiving your revised manuscript by Aug 24 2019 11:59PM. When you are ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter.

To enhance the reproducibility of your results, we recommend that if applicable you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io, where a protocol can be assigned its own identifier (DOI) such that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). This letter should be uploaded as separate file and labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. This file should be uploaded as separate file and labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. This file should be uploaded as separate file and labeled 'Manuscript'.

Please note while forming your response, if your article is accepted, you may have the opportunity to make the peer review history publicly available. The record will include editor decision letters (with reviews) and your responses to reviewer comments. If eligible, we will contact you to opt in or out.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Paulo Corti, Ph.D.

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

1. When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at

http://www.journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and http://www.journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2.  In your Methods section, please provide additional location information of study area, including geographic coordinates for the data set if available.

3. In your Methods section, please provide additional information regarding the permits you obtained for the work. Please ensure you have included the full name of the authority that approved the field site access and, if no permits were required, a brief statement explaining why.

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Partly

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: No

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: Main comments to authors

The authors analyze a very large data set of large-mammal pictures taken by authomatic cameras in protected areas in California. The authors look at temporal patterns of activity to analyze if human presence is coupled with a shift in anuimal activity, and if this kind of changes can change predator-prey and agonisitic behaviour/intra-guild predation risks. Data are robust and methods in general appropriate, though some improvements are needed. See more detailed comments below.

- Please, include in results the diel activity graph of human activity. It is central to your discussion and readers only know that human activity is 95% diurnal. Also, indicate the percentage of disturbed vs. Undisturbed days of your cameras, and the variability among them.

- The methodological observation in l. 120-125. pinpoints something rather relevant that has to be taken into account, discussed and potentially analyzed in the paper. The consistency of results either defining the ‘disturbed/undisturbed’ as presence of human activity before or after the animal detection shows that it is not the case that the (recent) presence of human disturbance shapes the response you describe in the paper. Thus, the differences in diel patterns you find must derive from a correlation/response at a higher level, either temporal or spatial.

In the case that there is ‘temporal correlation in your observations’ (the same camera takes repeatedly pictures from the same animal species with different beahaviours -disturbed/undisturbed-, but observation of the former and latter are aggregated), your results show that the behaviour of animals changes seasonally in sites visited by humans during one season (e.g. Easter vacation, summer…). That’s why you find that it is the same to analyse data looking to the previous or the next day (most probably both are high- or low-human occupancy)

The other option (spatial correlation) is that you always get animals in ‘disturbed’ conditions in some points (e.g. cameras located closest to parking lots at entrances) and ‘undisturbed’ animals always in other sites (deeper in the reserve where fewer hikers reach). In this case, you may have individuals that always behave in one of the two conditions.

The above does not invalidate your research at all, but it is rather important to know about it to understand the potential implications. I suggest analysing the spatial and temporal distribution of disturbance as well as those of animal observations, and including descriptive data on that in the paper to properly discuss the issue. In a (somewhat) similar vein, I suggest informing also the frequency of cameras with detections of species in pairs for a somewhat narrower time window (e.g. number of coincidences in season and site of puma-Odocoileus). All this information can be very valuable and worth discussing.

- l. 125-129. I suggest revising the use you do (in results and discussion) to the concept of ‘peak activity’ and ‘change in peak activity’ due to the statistics involved. The Rayleigh’s test formally gives you the presence of directionality in the clock vs. randomness, but it is not fully informative of ‘a peak hour’ (see Odocoileus case in Figure 2a for a clear case of ‘peak from a bi-modal distribution’). The point with this test is that it computes an average vector for all observations and analyses if its length is larger than expected under random conditions (some bi-modalities may even show lack of peak!!). The second analysis based on ranks (it is not fully explained but from my experience with circular statstics I guess the details) also looks for differences in ‘temporal distribution’ rather than differences in ‘peak’. As a conclusion, I suggest better dealing with it as changes in temporal distributions.

- l. 148-150. Do not include the option for ‘spatial avoidance’ since you can’t say about that with this approach: In case prey spatially avoided predators, then their presence in other cameras would compensate for this movement. The case that they ‘move away’ of your cameras (but they stay in the area) can’t be discussed on scientific grounds (you would need a posteriori explanations based on imperfect sampling with your cameras added to a biased presence of predators close to your cameras). Moreover, with your data you can’t blame changes in diel activity as responsible in population abundances. You just describe a reduction in deer numbers occurring at the same time that puma numbers increase, but we do not have any clue about the reason (ok, it can be increased predation, but higher numbers of puma simply predate more to survive and we are not informed if diel activity of deer has gradually changed since 2007 until 2016, or about changes in attack success of puma along the years)

- Methods in l. 143-153need to be clarified (and/or re-organized). I understand (right? wrong? And readers should not face many doubts) that you first did a kind of Bayesian occupancy modelling to estimate probability of detection and presence using raw 0-1 data. From that you extract the slopes –interannual change- (those painted in fig. 3? Why don’t you include a second y-axis scaled accordingly?) and their plausabilities of being different from 0. After you have the detection probabilities for each year, you use the raw data of number of detections per quarter per camera corrected for detectability to compute abundance data. Finally, it is not clear if lines in fig 3 are just regression lines fitted to the (log-)abundance data and drawn ignoring any kind of their fit statistics. In short: explain it a bit better and in the logical order of construction (I may have passed several issues).

- I encourange including (it can be as supplementary) the diel activity graphs for all species in both situations (one figure by species).

Other comments

- Include a map of the study area with the distribution of protected areas-working sites and urban areas (not needed in a very precise detail that may compromise the robbery of cameras)

- l. 108-111. Further justify why you do not use the 1-h lapse time between animal snaps to differentiate ‘animal visits’ since it is the time window usually applied in studies of carnivores. Moreover, two observations of the same species in the same place in 5-10’ when you work with large mammals (puma, deer) will correspond in most cases to the same individual just moving around (e.g. puma) or feeding (deer). On the contrary, you applied the one-hour rule for people. It would be also needed to know how many observations you have that do not fit the one-hour rule but only the 5’ one.

- Figures 1 and 2 apparently show data by 30’ intervals. If this is the case (in order to somewhat smooth their appearance) explain it. In the caption, substitute ‘…relative to human disturbance’ by something like ‘… under the disturbance of human and its absence’. The present captions give the idea of confronting in the same plate diel activity of the animals and human activity.

- You can easily compute the percentage of overlap shown in figures 1 and 2, and include in in results. This features will have the advantage of being rather easy to understand for readers.

Typographical errors and other minor issues

- l. 137-139. In fact you compute ‘the odds of two odd ratios’. I do not know if it could be better to find another name or explain it better (it is a suggestion, it first surprised me and I took a bit time to understand it… but I am not English native)

- l. 192 … potential increase in encounter rate

- Table 1. Do not include second in the peak time. Use the same number of decinal positions in all ‘P’ values

Reviewer #2: Patten et al. use a long-term camera trapping dataset to evaluate shifts in species diel activities in response to human disturbance and assess the consequences for predator-prey relationships. The question being addressed – the influence of humans on ecological interactions – is important, and the data used are extensive. The manuscript is clearly written overall.

However, I have three major concerns with this manuscript:

1) A prey species can share the same diel pattern that its predator, yet avoid the latter in space. This mechanism is hinted at in the Puma-Mule Deer analysis, but I am not convinced by that analysis (see point 2 below). Until the authors show that these diel shifts are or aren’t accompanied by changes in space use, conclusions regarding changes in predation risk are unfortunately not supported. Unless I have missed something, I do not see this kind of analysis in the manuscript.

2) Using the “peak activity” as the sole metric with which to compare activity distributions seems a bit restrictive. What if the distribution is bimodal, hypothetically?

3) The analysis of abundance time series could be improved. First, given the cyclic and non-independent nature of the time series, I’m not sure a simple linear regression is the most appropriate approach to quantify trends. On top of this, there is added variation due to seasonality/quarter that does not seem to be accounted for. Second, why not implement a single model assessing prey abundance as a function of predator abundance, human disturbance, with random effects to account for year and quarter. The temporal unit would be the quarter. Another approach could be to look at cross-correlations (possibly lagged) between the time series considered. Overall, I admit that this analysis was a bit unclear to me.

Minor comments:

l. 28 – not clear what “avoidance can mirror predation” means here. Could you please clarify or reword?

l. 52 – “basic ecological terms” is a bit vague. What would these be?

l. 92 – Cuddeback instead of “Cuddleback”

l. 108-11 – I think the difference in independence thresholds needs justifying.

l. 117-9 - why wasn’t the disturbance caused by the researchers setting/checking the cameras also included?

l. 143 – a reference to support this statement is needed

l. 164-66 – use of the present tense is awkward, I suggest changing to past.

l. 166 – perhaps use “puma” instead of “lion” to maintain consistency.

l. 172-174 – Awkward sentence, I suggest rewording.

l. 192 – not if space use shifts as well (see major concern 1)

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: Yes: Jeremy Cusack

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files to be viewed.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email us at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

Decision Letter 1

Paulo Corti

29 Oct 2019

PONE-D-19-14511R1

The Intersection of Human Disturbance and Diel Activity, with Potential Consequences on Trophic Interactions

PLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Patten,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

==============================

I have revised your answers to both reviewers and most of them fulfilled their requests. However, I believe some of them still need some work, so here what I think is necessary to take into account to improve your manuscript:

- Line 108-111. Please exclude the 15% from your data set or show the analysis where is probed that results are not affected when the 15% that doesn't fulfill the 1 h rule is included in the analyses.

- Animal common names should be in small caps.

- Please provide citations to back up information in between lines 149 and 154, and also in 165-167.

- In lines 259-262 Please, avoid redundancy and speculation, so rewrite these sentences.

I think it is importan to correctly answer this request from reviewer #1:

Requests: You can easily compute the percentage of overlap shown in figures 1 and 2, and include in in results. This features will have the advantage of being rather easy to understand for readers.

Response: I do not feel it is easy to calculate overlap, apart from program ‘overlap’ in R, which is designed to do just that. I have concerns about this package, though, because it does not account for the circular nature of the data, and hence I have not reported results from it. I still do not feel this paper is the place to air concerns about that approach, particularly because Fig. 1 and Fig. 2 “speak for themselves,” as the old trope goes.

Please reduce speculations so show the percentage of change and also there were differences between treatments.

==============================

We would appreciate receiving your revised manuscript by Dec 13 2019 11:59PM. When you are ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter.

To enhance the reproducibility of your results, we recommend that if applicable you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io, where a protocol can be assigned its own identifier (DOI) such that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). This letter should be uploaded as separate file and labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. This file should be uploaded as separate file and labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. This file should be uploaded as separate file and labeled 'Manuscript'.

Please note while forming your response, if your article is accepted, you may have the opportunity to make the peer review history publicly available. The record will include editor decision letters (with reviews) and your responses to reviewer comments. If eligible, we will contact you to opt in or out.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Paulo Corti, Ph.D.

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email us at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2019 Dec 13;14(12):e0226418. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0226418.r004

Author response to Decision Letter 1


25 Nov 2019

- Line 108-111. Please exclude the 15% from your data set or show the analysis where is probed that results are not affected when the 15% that doesn't fulfill the 1 h rule is included in the analyses.

RESPONSE: I did the latter; see S2 Fig.

- Animal common names should be in small caps.

RESPONSE: Okay.

- Please provide citations to back up information in between lines 149 and 154, and also in 165-167.

RESPONSE: We fear a misunderstanding here. Lines 149–154 only restate basic mathematical definitions of joint probability and odds in terms of our data. On lines 165–167 we proffered a specific prediction as opposed to a relationship from the literature. To avoid further confusion, we changed “expected” to “predicted.”

- In lines 259-262 Please, avoid redundancy and speculation, so rewrite these sentences.

RESPONSE: We rewrote the closing of the final paragraph.

- I think it is importan to correctly answer this request from reviewer #1: Requests: You can easily compute the percentage of overlap shown in figures 1 and 2, and include in in results. This features will have the advantage of being rather easy to understand for readers. Response: I do not feel it is easy to calculate overlap, apart from program ‘overlap’ in R, which is designed to do just that. I have concerns about this package, though, because it does not account for the circular nature of the data, and hence I have not reported results from it. I still do not feel this paper is the place to air concerns about that approach, particularly because Fig. 1 and Fig. 2 “speak for themselves,” as the old trope goes.

Please reduce speculations so show the percentage of change and also there were differences between treatments.

RESPONSE: Reviewer comments to the contrary, computing overlap is not easy, and I do not wish to air publicly my concerns about the ‘overlap’ package in R. I nonetheless understand the desire to see how % overlap changed. I accomplished this by porting graphs into ImageJ, a shareware package, to estimate overlap area (A *intersection* B) and total area (A *union* B). Their quotient approximates proportion of overlap, which I present in the text. As for “differences between treatments,” that is addressed already in the odds ratio analysis, but to supplement that analysis I wrote code to obtain a Bayesian analog to the Watson–Wheeler two-sample test of difference in mean angle. I report those results now, too.

Decision Letter 2

Paulo Corti

27 Nov 2019

The Intersection of Human Disturbance and Diel Activity, with Potential Consequences on Trophic Interactions

PONE-D-19-14511R2

Dear Dr. Patten,

We are pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it complies with all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you will receive an e-mail containing information on the amendments required prior to publication. When all required modifications have been addressed, you will receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will proceed to our production department and be scheduled for publication.

Shortly after the formal acceptance letter is sent, an invoice for payment will follow. To ensure an efficient production and billing process, please log into Editorial Manager at https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the "Update My Information" link at the top of the page, and update your user information. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to enable them to help maximize its impact. If they will be preparing press materials for this manuscript, you must inform our press team as soon as possible and no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

With kind regards,

Paulo Corti, Ph.D.

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Acceptance letter

Paulo Corti

3 Dec 2019

PONE-D-19-14511R2

The Intersection of Human Disturbance and Diel Activity, with Potential Consequences on Trophic Interactions

Dear Dr. Patten:

I am pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper at this point, to enable them to help maximize its impact. If they will be preparing press materials for this manuscript, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org.

For any other questions or concerns, please email plosone@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE.

With kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. Paulo Corti

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 Table. Specific locations of camera traps in coastal Orange County, California, 2007–2016.

    (PDF)

    S1 Fig. Active cameras 2007–2015.

    (PDF)

    S2 Fig. Parameter estimates of peak activity of the Mule Deer (Odocoileus hemionus) and Coyote (Canis latrans) when camera trap images are excluded within 1 hour of the previous image of that same species or only within 5 minutes of the previous image.

    (PDF)

    S3 Fig. Temporal distributions for human activity in this study.

    (PDF)

    S4 Fig. Temporal distributions for “non focal” species in this study.

    (PDF)

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: letter1.pdf

    Data Availability Statement

    Data cannot be shared publicly because they are co-owned by multiple land owners and the Irvine Ranch Conservancy (IRC), but they are available from IRC for researchers who meet approval for data access. Contact Yi-Chin Fang (yfang@irconservancy.org) for access requests.


    Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES