Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2019 Dec 16.
Published in final edited form as: Lang Linguist Compass. 2019 Sep 5;13(9):e12349. doi: 10.1111/lnc3.12349

Neuroimaging genetics studies of specific reading disability and developmental language disorder: A review

Nicole Landi 1, Meaghan Perdue 1
PMCID: PMC6913889  NIHMSID: NIHMS1057194  PMID: 31844423

Abstract

Developmental disorders of spoken and written language are heterogeneous in nature with impairments observed across various linguistic, cognitive, and sensorimotor domains. These disorders are also associated with characteristic patterns of atypical neural structure and function that are observable early in development, often before formal schooling begins. Established patterns of heritability point toward genetic contributions, and molecular genetics approaches have identified genes that play a role in these disorders. Still, identified genes account for only a limited portion of phenotypic variance in complex developmental disorders, described as the problem of “missing heritability.” The characterization of intermediate phenotypes at the neural level may fill gaps in our understanding of heritability patterns in complex disorders, and the emerging field of neuroimaging genetics offers a promising approach to accomplish this goal. The neuroimaging genetics approach is gaining prevalence in language- and reading-related research as it is well-suited to incorporate behavior, genetics, and neurobiology into coherent etiological models of complex developmental disorders. Here, we review research applying the neuroimaging genetics approach to the study of specific reading disability (SRD) and developmental language disorder (DLD), much of which links genes with known neurodevelopmental function to functional and structural abnormalities in the brain.

1. INTRODUCTION

Developmental disorders of spoken and written language are characterized by deficits in various linguistic domains such as reading, phonological processing, vocabulary, and grammatical skills (Hulme & Snowling, 2013), as well as atypical neural structure and function (Mayes, Reilly, & Morgan, 2015; Norton, Beach, & Gabrieli, 2015). A growing number of genes have been implicated in the etiology of these disorders, many of which play a role in neurodevelopmental processes such as neuronal migration, neurite outgrowth, cortical morphogenesis, and ciliary structure and function (Newbury, Monaco, & Paracchini, 2014). The developmental function of such genes is a likely source of neural anomalies, and the emerging field of neuroimaging genetics seeks to better understand the relationships among genetic and behavioral markers of disorders by establishing intermediate phenotypes at the neural level. Findings to date provide some compelling evidence for links among specific genes, brain structure and/or function, and reading- and language-associated phenotypes. Here, we review research employing a neuroimaging genetics approach to study specific reading disability (SRD) and developmental language disorder (DLD).1 These disorders are of interest to study in parallel due to overlap in characteristic deficits, such as phonological processing, as well as shared neural and genetic underpinnings. We provide a brief introduction on genetic approaches to the study of complex disorders (please see Appendix A for explanation of technical concepts), along with an overview of each disorder and the related neuroimaging genetic findings reported thus far. Given the complex polygenic underpinnings of SRD and DLD, we have organized our discussion of the relevant imaging genetic findings by gene, beginning with coverage of well-established candidate genes for SRD and DLD and followed by those more recently associated with reading and/or language (genes investigated in relation to SRD and DLD using a neuroimaging genetic approach are listed in Table 1, and findings are summarized in Table 2). In addition, we only include genes that have been studied using a neuroimaging genetics approach. In order to present a clear account within the scope of neurodevelopmental disorders of spoken and written language, we have limited our review to studies including typically developing populations and populations with SRD or DLD and no additional neurological or psychiatric diagnosis.

TABLE 1.

Gene variants discussed in this review

Gene Chr Location Function Imaging Phenotype
(language/reading)
References
BDNF 11p13 Neuronal survival
Neuronal proliferation
Synaptic growth
fMRI Reading Jasinska et al. (2016)
C2orf3/GCFC2/MRPL19 2p11-q11.2 Unknown MRI
DTI
Reading Eicher et al. (2016)
Scerri et al. (2012)
CMIP 16q23.2-q23.3 T-cell signaling MRI Reading Skeide et al. (2016)
COL4A2 13q34 Type IV collagen subunit encoding MRI Reading Skeide et al. (2016)
COMT 22q11.21 Dopamine metabolism fMRI Reading Landi et al. (2013)
NRSN1 6p22.3 Neurite growth MRI Reading Skeide et al. (2016)
ROBO1 3p12.3 Neuronal migration Axon guidance MEG Reading Lamminmäki et al. (2012)
CCDC136/FLNC 7q32.1 Unknown MRI Reading/Language Gialluisi, Guadalupe, Francks, and Fisher (2017)
DCDC2 6p22 Neuronal migration MRI

fMRI
rsMRI
EEG
DTI
Reading/Language Cope et al. (2012)
Czamara et al. (2011)
Darki et al. (2012)
Darki et al. (2014)
Cilia Marino et al. (2014)
Meda et al. (2008)
DYX1C1 15q21.3 Neuronal migration MRI Reading/Language Darki et al. (2012)
Cilia Darki et al. (2014)
FOXP2 7q31 Transcriptional regulation Neurogenesis MRI
fMRI
PET
Reading/Language Belton et al. (2003)
Liegois et al. (2003)
Pinel et al. (2012)
Skeide et al. (2016)
Vargha-Kadem et al. (1998)
Watkins et al. (2002)
KIAA0319 6p22 Neuronal migration MRI
fMRI
rsMRI
EEG
DTI
Reading/Language Wilcke et al. (2012)
Czamara et al. (2011)
Darki et al. (2012)
Darki et al. (2014)
Eicher et al. (2016)
Pinel et al. (2012)
RBFOX2 22q12.3 Alternative exon splicing regulation MRI Reading/Language Gialluisi et al. (2017)
SETBP1 18q12.3 DNA replication, apoptosis, transcription, nucleosome assembly fMRI Reading/Language Perdue et al. (2019)
SLC2A3 4q32.1 Neural glucose transport regulation EEG
rsMRI
DTI
Reading/Language Roeske et al. (2011)
Skeide et al. (2015)
ACOT13/THEM2 6p22.3 Cell proliferation MRI
DTI
Reading/Language Eicher et al. (2016)
Pinel et al. (2012)
CNTNAP2 7q35 Cell adhesion Voltage-gated channels MRI
fMRI

DTI
Language Dennis et al. (2011)
Koeda et al. (2015)
Skeide et al. (2016)
Tan et al. (2010) Udden et al. (2017)
Whalley et al. (2011)

Note. Publications focused on patient populations and/or individuals with disorders other than SRD and DLD were excluded from our review.

TABLE 2.

Summary of imaging genetics findings related to reading/language

Gene Reference(s) Variant(s) Design Imaging Na Agea,b Ethnic
Backgrounda
Imaging findings Behavioral findings
BDNF Jasinska et al. (2016) rs6265** Continuous fMRI 81 6–10 70 Caucasian
1 African-American
2 Hispanic
3 Asian
5 Mixed ethnicity
Increased activation bilaterally in Met carriers relative to Val/Val homozygotes Val allele homozygotes performed better than Met carriers on passage comprehension, phonological memory, and IQ
C2ORF3/MRPL19/GCFC2 Eicher et al. (2016) rs2298948*
rs917235*
rs6732511*
DYX3 locus upstream of GCFC2 and MRPL19
Continuous MRI
DTI
332 3-20 European Minor allele on rs2298948 associated with decreased CT and GMV. rs917235 associated with CT
rs6732511 minor allele associated with increased GMV
NR
Scerri et al. (2012) rs917235*****
rs714939
rs1000585
Continuous MRI
DTI
76 6–25 98% European G allele associated with reduced WMV rs714939 and rs917235 associated with verbal IQ
CMIP Skeide et al. (2016) rs12927866
rs6564903
rs3935802
rs4265801
rs16955705
rs7201632
Joint SNP effect*****
Case/control MRI 54 5–12 NR CMIP related to bilateral portions of cerebellar WM Associated with reading comprehension
COL4A2 Skeide et al. (2016) rs9521789***** Case/control MRI 54 5–12 NR Related to cerebellar GMV No significant associations reported
COMT Landi et al. (2013) rs4680**** Case/control fMRI 86 6–10 83 Caucasian
3 African-American
Greater activation in left hemisphere reading-related regions for Met carriers Significant differences between genotype groups in phonological awareness (PA) and spelling
NRSN1 Skeide et al. (2016) rs9356928
rs4285310
rs3178
Joint SNP effect*****
Case/control MRI 54 5–12 NR Joint SNP association with GMV and WMV NRSN1 associated with reading comprehension performance. GMV in VWFA ROI related to NRSN1 predicted reading outcomes
ROBO1 Lamminmäki et al. (2012) N/A Case/control MEG 10 19–51 Finnish Ipsilateral auditory suppression in both hemispheres related to ROBO1 expression levels NR
CCDC136/FLNC Gialluisi, Guadalupe, Francks, and Fisher 2017 rs59197085
rs56184882**
rs339054**
rs339046**
Continuous MRI 1275 18–35 NR Associated with cortical surface area in IFG NR
DCDC2 Cope et al. (2012) rs793862
rs807701
rs807724
BV677278 STR**
BV677278 del.
rs1087266
Continuous fMRI 82 7–12 European American DCDC2 variants associated with activation during print and auditory processing
Nominal associations between DCDC2 variants and activation during word reading and nonword reading tasks
Nominally significant associations between DCDC2 variants and measures of IQ, PA, word reading, and passage comprehension
Czamara et al. (2011) Chr6:24459391/rs1052724905**
Chr6:24564881**
Chr6:24571041**
Chr6:24581378/rs1033367480**
SRD EEG 200 8–19 German Four rare variants on DCDC2 or between DCDC2 and KIAA0319 were associated with late mismatch negativity (MMN) to speech NR
Darki et al. (2012) rs793842*****
rs793862
rs807701
rs2328819
rs2792682
rs7751169
rs9460974
Continuous MRI
DTI
76 6–25 98% European Associated with WMV Positive correlation between white matter volume and reading scores. No direct relationships between SNPs and behavior.
Darki et al. (2014) rs793842***** Continuous MRI
DTI
76 6–25 98% European Associated with WMV in left temporo-parietal region and with CT in left temporo-parietal and occipital cortices Associated with reading comprehension scores; WMV and CT correlated with reading scores
DCDC2 Eicher et al. (2016) rs707864 Continuous MRI
DTI
332 3–20 European NS NR
Marino et al. (2014) DCDC2-intron2 deletion** Case/control DTI 47 16–21 NR Reduced WM integrity in subjects with DCDC2 intron2 deletion Positive correlation between fractional anisotropy (FA) and average reading scores
Meda et al. (2008) DCDC2-intron2 deletion* Continuous MRI 56 19–85 NR Increased GMV in carriers of DCDC2 intron 2 deletion compared to those homozygous for no deletion NR
DYX1C1 Darki et al. (2012) rs3743204****
rs3743205
rs17819126
Continuous MRI
DTI
76 6–25 98% European Associated with WMV in bilateral temporo-parietal region Significant correlation between WMV and reading scores
Darki et al. (2014) rs3743204***** Continuous MRI
DTI
76 6–25 98% European Associated with bilateral temporo-parietal WMV, replicating findings of Darki et al. (2012) across an additional time point Significant correlation between WMV and reading scores
FOXP2 Belton et al. (2003) KE Family*** Case/control MRI 34 9–27 European Altered GM density in affected family members NR
Liegois et al. (2003) KE Family*** Case/control fMRI 20 19–56 European Affected family members showed an atypical pattern of activation in a verb generation task compared to unaffected family members and controls NR
FOXP2 Pinel et al. (2012) rs10249234***
rs7784315***
rs7812028*
rs17137135***
rs6980093***
rs6942634
rs2894699
rs1476535
rs10255943
rs10486026
rs10261780
rs10262103
rs4727799
rs17312686
rs2106900
rs17312861
rs12113612
rs10266297
rs10279936
rs7799109
rs12532920
rs17137124
rs10269986
rs1229761
rs1229758
rs12705966
rs10230087
rs7782412
rs1456029
rs12670585
rs6966051
rs17213159
rs1378771
rs12705971
rs12705973
rs2396766
rs12671330
Continuous fMRI 94 M = 24.7 European Associations with activation during a sentence reading task NR
FOXP2 Skeide et al. (2016) rs923875
rs12533005
rs6980093
rs10230558
rs7782412
rs936146
Joint SNP effect*****
Continuous MRI 54 5–12 NR Joint SNP effect related to GMV in left medial superior frontal gyrus NS
Vargha-Kadem et al. 1998 KE Family*** Case/control PET
MRI
34 NR European Atypical activation during a word repetition task in affected family members. Regional alterations in GM structure Affected family members show deficits in word repetition, nonword repetition, and simultaneous and sequential orofacial movements
Watkins et al. (2002) KE Family*** Case/control MRI 34 9–27 European Atypical pattern of GMV in affected family members compared to unaffected family members and controls Correlation between caudate nucleus volume and performance on test of oral praxis, nonword repetition, and coding subtest of Wechsler Intelligence Scale in affected family members
Wilcke et al. (2012) rs12533005** Case/control fMRI 33 M = 11 German Decreased activation in risk allele carriers during rhyme decision task NR
Darki et al. (2012) rs6935076*****
rs4504469
rs2143340
Continuous MRI
DTI
76 6–25 98% European Associated with left temporoparietal WMV Positive correlations between WMV and reading comprehension and WMV and timed single-word reading; No significant correlations between SNPs and reading
KIAA0319 Darki et al. (2014) rs6935076***** Continuous MRI
DTI
76 6–25 98% European Associated with bilateral temporoparietal WMV, replicating findings from Darki et al. (2012) across an additional time point Significant correlation between WMV and reading scores
Eicher et al. (2016) rs9461045****
rs9295626
rs10456309
rs4576240
Continuous MRI
DTI
332 3–20 European Associated with cortical thickness and WM integrity NR
Pinel et al., 2012 rs2235676
rs9467247
rs3756821
Continuous fMRI 94 M = 24.7 Primarily Caucasian NS NR
RBFOX2 Gialluisi et al. (2017) rs5995177***
rs78563107***
rs6000084***
rs6000085***
rs144006011***
Continuous MRI 1275 18–35 NR Associated with cortical thickness NR
SETBP1 Perdue et al. (2019) rs7230525*** Continuous fMRI 73 5–12 116 Caucasian
2 African American
3 Hispanic
4 Asian
8 mixed
2 unreported
Interaction between genotype, lexicality and modality in the right inferior parietal lobule Genotype associated with phonological working memory
SLC2A3 Roeske et al. (2011) rs4234898***
rs11100040***
SRD EEG 200 18 6c*** 8–19 German Altered late MMN response NR
Skeide et al. (2015) rs4234898
rs11100040***
Continuous rsfMRI
DTI
34 9–12 NR Weaker functional and structural connectivity in risk allele carriers WM integrity related to PA performance
THEM2/ACOT13 Eicher et al. (2016) rs3777663** Continuous MRI
DTI
332 3–20 European Associated with cortical thickness NR
Pinel et al. (2012) rs17243157***
rs3756819
rs1061925
rs3181227
rs2223588
rs6928074
rs9461049
rs926529
rs1885211
Continuous fMRI 94 M = 24.7 Primarily Caucasian Associated with functional asymmetry during reading and speech listening tasks NR
CNTNAP2 Dennis et al. (2011) rs2710102*** Continuous DTI 328 M = 23.4 Caucasian Atypical structural connectivity indices in risk allele carriers NR
Koeda et al. (2015) rs7794745***
rs2710102
Continuous fMRI 108 M = 26.3 Japanese Genotype group differences in activation during general auditory processing, human voice processing, and language processing NS
Skeide et al. (2016) rs7794745
rs10246256
rs2710102
rs759178
rs17236239
rs4431523
rs2710117
Joint SNP effect*****
Continuous MRI 54 5–12 NR Joint SNP effect related to WMV in left cerebral and cerebellar peduncles Association with reading comprehension
CNTNAP2 Tan et al. (2010) rs7794745*** Continuous MRI
DTI
114 NR NR Reduced GMV, WMV, and WM integrity in risk allele homozygotes NR
Udden et al. (2017) rs7794745**** Continuous MRI 1717 M = 24.3 Primarily European Caucasian Reduced GMV in AT/TT carriers compared to AA homozygotes NR
Whalley et al. (2011) rs7794745***
rs2710102***
Continuous fMRI 66 M = 20.5 Caucasian Atypical patterns of activation in risk allele carriers during sentence completion task NS

Note. Shading indicates relevant phenotype: white = reading, light gray = reading and/or language, dark gray = language. Boldface indicates SNPs for which significant imaging-genetic results were reported. For studies that included corrected and uncorrected p values, asterisks indicate corrected p to be consistent with other studies presented here.

Abbreviations: CT, cortical thickness; GMV, gray matter volume; NR, not reported; NS, no significant findings; WMV, white matter volume.

*

p < .01, uncorrected.

**

p < .001, uncorrected.

***

p < .05, corrected.

****

p < .01, corrected.

*****

p < .001, corrected.

a

Information listed refers to samples included in neuroimaging analyses.

b

Age ranges or means reported according to information available in each study.

c

Replication sample.

2. ∣. GENETICS IN DEVELOPMENTAL DISORDERS

The field of genetics took hold as a modern science following Mendel’s foundational work in the mid-late 1800s and was furthered by a number of remarkable discoveries throughout the 20th century that led to the discovery of the genetic basis for many diseases. Following early genetic sequencing efforts in the 1970s and 1980s, the successful sequencing of the human genome was accomplished in 2003 (Human Genome Sequencing Consortium, 2004). This sequencing did not uncover a deterministic map of human traits and diseases but instead revealed a basic architecture from which complex interactions among genes, behavior, and environment dictate human ontogenesis (Gottlieb & Lickliter, 2007). Uncovering this architecture has paved the way for advances in analytic approaches for exploring the nature of gene–behavior relations. Previously, the primary method for uncovering the genetic underpinnings of particular traits was through linkage analysis. This technique allowed researchers to use genetic markers to identify the locations of the segment(s) of DNA in chromosomes that are shared by individuals within a family who exhibit a trait of interest (Smith, 1953). Linkage analysis is a useful tool for establishing inheritance in single-gene Mendelian disorders because it is based on deviations from expected patterns of inheritance and it traces cotransmission of a DNA marker allele and a disorder. For single-gene disorders, linkage can be identified by using a few large family pedigrees; for the study of complex traits, the affected sib-pair linkage design which examines allele sharing for pairs of affected siblings in many different families is the most widely used linkage design (Plomin, DeFries, McClearn, & McGuffin, 2007). However, linkage approaches require a large gene effect for successful detection and thus have limited power for identification of genes of modest effect that contribute to complex traits and disorders (Risch & Merikangas, 1996). The vast majority of complex traits, conditions, and behaviors arise from the presence of multiple genetic variations or polymorphisms as well as gene-by-gene and gene-by-environment interactions. These include both common variants, which are present in greater than 1% of the genome, and rare variants, which occur in less than 1% of the genome. The most frequently occurring polymorphisms are single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs), in which there is a single base pair substitution in the structural units of DNA (The International SNP Map Working Group, 2001)—these represent approximately 90% of the variation in human DNA (The 1000 Genomes Project Consortium, 2011). Additional variation comes from insertion–deletion polymorphisms (indels) which affect one or more base pairs by the removal or addition of units (Weber et al., 2002) and copy number variations (CNVs) which represent alterations in the position or number of copies of larger sequences of DNA (Iafrate et al., 2004). SNPs and other common variants are of great scientific interest as markers of risk for genetic diseases or conditions in the general population because they occur relatively frequently and modest gene effects can be detected using association studies that do not require large family pedigrees.

Researchers who seek to understand the genetic origins of common diseases and traits are guided by two predominant hypotheses: the common disease/common variant (CDCV) hypothesis and the common disease/rare variant hypothesis (CDRV). According to CDCV, variants that are common in the population but have low penetrance (i.e., the probability that the carrier of the variant will express the disease) play a primary role in disease susceptibility (Lander, 1996; Reich & Lander, 2001; Schork, Murray, Frazer, & Topol, 2009). In contrast, CDRV suggests that rare variants with high penetrance are the major contributors to disease susceptibility (Schork et al., 2009). Evidence supports both of these models (Schork et al., 2009), and while ongoing research continues to contrast these models, a hybrid account for complex disease and trait susceptibility holds the most explanatory power. Consistent with the CDCV hypothesis, many of the identified genetic variants associated with SRD and DLD are (a) fairly common in the general population2; (b) carried by only a small subset of individuals with these disorders; and (c) not clearly linked to the behavioral phenotype of the individuals who carry them. There are, however, some rare variants associated with language and reading phenotypes which have been found in more severely impaired individuals, including some of the FOXP2 variants associated with speech and language impairment (e.g., see Estruch et al., 2016).

Under both the CDRV and CDCV approaches, two primary methods are used for the identification of genes associated with complex neurodevelopmental disorders. Hypothesis-driven candidate gene association studies investigating specific SNPs allow researchers to identify how alleles at one or more pre-identified locations on the gene are related to specific phenotypes (Kornilov & Grigorenko, 2016). Alternatively, an exploratory approach may be taken using a genome-wide association study (GWAS) to identify SNPs associated with a trait of interest; this approach is especially useful for detection of new variants (Hirschhorn & Daly, 2005). GWAS is a powerful tool, but it is costly and effortful to genotype thousands of SNPs per individual, and GWAS requires large sample sizes for sufficient power to detect small effects associated with complex traits (Hirschhorn & Daly, 2005). Recent efforts to build and share large genetic databases (e.g. the Pediatric Imaging, Neurocognition, and Genetics [PING] Data Repository; Jernigan et al., 2016) aim to reduce the burden and increase power of GWAS studies. The field of neuroimaging genetics applies these genetic methods in combination with neuroimaging techniques to improve characterization of complex traits through the identification of intermediate phenotypes that link genes to behaviors through neural mechanisms. Using these methods, geneticists, neuroscientists, and psychologists may work together to deepen the understanding of complex developmental disorders such as specific reading disability (SRD) and developmental language disorder (DLD) in a step to improve identification and treatment of these conditions.

3. ∣. SPECIFIC READING DISABILITY

Specific reading disability (SRD), or developmental dyslexia (DD), is a prevalent learning disability affecting around 7–16% of school-age children and is characterized by deficits in accurate and/or fluent word recognition, decoding and spelling which do not result from inadequate educational experiences (Fletcher, 2009; Lyon, Shaywitz, & Shaywitz, 2003; Pennington & Bishop, 2009). SRD is neurobiological in origin, and functional and structural magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI/MRI) reveal atypical brain characteristics (in both structure and function) associated with SRD in left temporo-parietal, occipito-temporal, and inferior frontal regions (e.g., Landi et al., 2010 Maisog, Einbinder, Flowers, Turkeltaub, & Eden, 2008; Pugh et al., 2001; Richlan, Kronbichler, & Wimmer, 2009, 2013). Specifically, individuals with SRD exhibit reduced activation during reading and phonological awareness tasks relative to controls in left temporo-parietal and occipito-temporal regions (e.g., Richlan et al., 2009). Mixed findings of over- and under-activation in the left inferior frontal cortex in SRD suggests that activation in this area may be task-specific and warrants further investigation (e.g., Richlan et al., 2009). Reports of anomalous cortical structure in SRD include reduced gray matter volume in bilateral superior temporal regions (Hoeft et al., 2007; Richlan et al., 2013), bilateral temporo-parietal regions and insula (Hoeft et al., 2007), bilateral occipito-temporal and right temporo-parietal regions (Kronbichler et al., 2008), and atypical sulcal pattern in left temporo-parietal and occipito-temporal regions (Im, Raschle, Smith, Grant, & Gaab, 2016). Advances in the use of diffusion-weighted MRI (including diffusion tensor imaging, or DTI) allow examination of white matter microstructure, indexed by a measure of white matter integrity called fractional anisotropy (FA), and measures of diffusivity along white matter tracts: mean diffusivity (MD), axial diffusivity (AD), and radial diffusivity (RD). The application of diffusion-weighted MRI to the study of SRD has shown decreased white matter integrity (reduced FA) in left hemisphere tracts including the arcuate fasciculus, inferior longitudinal fasciculus, inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus, and corona radiata, which structurally link language-related cortical areas (Vandermosten, Boets, Wouters, & Ghesquière, 2012; Yeatman et al., 2011). Although some of the observed neurobiological alterations in SRD may be a result of reading difficulties, much of the altered brain function and structure characteristic of SRD is observed in pre-reading children and consequently has been proposed to be rooted in underlying genetic risk factors (Ozernov-Palchik & Gaab, 2016).

3.1 ∣. Neuroimaging genetics studies of specific reading disability

3.1.1. ∣. SRD candidate genes: DCDC2, KIAA0319, THEM2, NRSN1, DYX1C1, and ROBO1

Variation on DCDC2 has been associated with performance on several measures of reading and reading-related skills including phonological skills, word/pseudoword reading, and spelling in samples with SRD (Chen, Zhao, Zhang, & Zuo, 2017; Ludwig, Roeske, et al., 2008; Marino et al., 2012; Matsson et al., 2015; Meng et al., 2005; Wilcke et al., 2009) and without SRD (Lind et al., 2010; Newbury et al., 2011; Powers et al., 2013; Scerri et al., 2011; Sun et al., 2014; Venkatesh, Siddaiah, Padakannaya, & Ramachandra, 2013; Zhang et al., 2016). Extant research suggests that variation in this gene is associated with subtle cortical malformations in brain areas important for reading (Meng et al., 2005). For example, in a study of typically developing Swedish-speaking children and young adults, one SNP in DCDC2 (rs7938423) was related to white matter volume in left temporo-parietal cortex, with the highest white matter volume indices in CC homozygotes and lowest in TT homozygotes, and heterozygotes falling in between (Darki, Peyrard-Janvid, Matsson, Kere, & Klingberg, 2012). In addition, the authors found a significant positive correlation between white matter volume and reading fluency; however, no direct associations between SNPs and behavioral performance were significant (Darki et al., 2012). The association between rs793842 and left temporo-parietal white matter volume was replicated in a follow-up analysis that examined the same sample with the inclusion of a third longitudinal time point, and a significant association between this SNP and cortical thickness in the left temporo-parietal and occipital cortices emerged in this follow-up analysis, such that T-allele carriers had thicker cortex in these regions (Darki, Peyrard-Janvid, Matsson, Kere, & Klingberg, 2014). Additionally, rs793842 was associated with reading comprehension scores.

A second polymorphism in DCDC2, a deletion in intron 2 (hereafter, DCDC2d), which encompasses BV677278, has been associated with SRD, and with interindividual variation reading performance and motion perception in children with SRD and typical development (TD; Brkanac et al., 2007; Cicchini, Marino, Mascheretti, Perani, & Morrone, 2015; Gori et al., 2015; Harold et al., 2006; Ludwig, Schumacher, et al., 2008; Marino et al., 2012; Meng et al., 2005; Wilcke et al., 2009).4 One imaging genetics study of the DCDC2d reports significantly increased gray matter volume in a number of regions related to language and reading, including bilateral temporo-parietal regions, in typically developing individuals heterozygous for DCDC2d (Meda et al., 2008). Moreover, a study of white matter microstructure in individuals with and without SRD found the DCDC2d to be associated with reduced white matter integrity in the left arcuate fasciculus and the posterior corpus callosum, regardless of reading impairment status. Within the reading impaired group, white matter integrity was reduced bilaterally in the inferior longitudinal fasciculus and anterior corpus callosum in those with the deletion relative to those without (Marino et al., 2014). Marino et al. (2014) also reported positive correlations between white matter integrity and average reading scores in several left-hemisphere tracts related to reading and language.

In addition to structural findings, recent fMRI and event-related potential (ERP) studies that engage the neural circuitry for reading and language provide evidence for links between DCDC2 and brain function. One ERP of particular interest in reading and language research is the mismatch negativity (MMN) response, a commonly used index of auditory discrimination (typically of tones or phonemes) that is characteristically reduced in individuals with reading and language impairment (Schulte-Körne, Deimel, Bartling, & Remschmidt, 2001). Czamara et al. (2011) reported an attenuated late MMN response to phonemic stimuli in minor allele carriers of one SNP in DCDC2 (chr6:24459391/rs1052724905) and three locations between DCDC2 and KIAA0319 (chr6:24564881, chr6:24571041, and chr6:24581378/rs1033367480) in a sample of children with SRD. Further, using fMRI, Cope et al. (2012) identified patterns of activation associated with alleles of BV677278 during a set of language and reading tasks and replicated previous findings of gene/reading behavioral associations. Specifically, BV677278 alleles were significantly associated with activation in the left temporo-parietal cortex (positive correlation) and the right occipitotemporal gyrus (negative correlation) during processing of printed words and negatively associated with activation in the right occipito-temporal gyrus during processing of spoken words, indicating relevance of this gene for both written and spoken language. The functional association in the temporo-parietal cortex is consistent with the localization of structural associations with DCDC2d discussed above (Meda et al., 2008), and together, these results link DCDC2 and temporo-parietal anomalies that are characteristic of SRD.

A close neighbor of DCDC2 within the same locus on chromosome 6p22.3, KIAA0319, is another well-studied candidate gene for SRD that has been associated with reading ability within SRD (Cope et al., 2005; Couto et al., 2010; Dennis et al., 2009; Ludwig, Roeske, et al., 2008; Mascheretti et al., 2014) and the general population (Lim, Wong, Ho, & Waye, 2014; Luciano et al., 2007; Newbury et al., 2011; Paracchini et al., 2008; Scerri et al., 2011; Venkatesh et al., 2013; Sun et al., 2014). Animal research shows that the expression pattern of KIAA0319 in the developing brain is consistent with its hypothesized role in neuronal migration, and recent bioinformatics analysis has suggested its involvement in additional neurodevelopmental and signaling functions (Peschansky et al., 2010; Poon et al., 2011; Szalkowski et al., 2013, 2012; Velayos-Baeza, Levecque, Kobayashi, Holloway, & Monaco, 2010). Consistent with KIAA0319’s hypothesized role from animal findings, structural associations with KIAA0319 have been reported in human MRI research. An association between white matter volume in the left temporo-parietal region and KIAA0319 SNP rs6935076 has been identified (Darki et al., 2012) and replicated with an additional right hemisphere effect when data from a third longitudinal time point were added to the analysis (Darki et al., 2014). Furthermore, Eicher et al. (2016) reported decreased cortical thickness in a left orbitofrontal region in carriers of the minor allele of the SNP rs9461045 (associated with single-word reading and spelling ability; Dennis et al., 2009) relative to those homozygous for the major allele within a sample of individuals ages 3–20 years from the Pediatrics Imaging Neurocognition Genetics study (PING) database.5 With regard to structural connectivity, the minor allele of the same SNP was associated with reduced white matter integrity in the corpus callosum. These findings point toward an effect of KIAA0319 on language-related brain structures that may underlie individual differences in language and reading abilities.

In addition to DCDC2 and KIAA0319, two additional genes within 6p22-21.3 (THEM2 and NRSN1) have been linked with reading-associated neural structure and/or function across multiple studies. Presence of the minor allele at rs3777663 in THEM2 (also known as ACOT13 and previously identified as a protective allele [Eicher et al., 2014]) has been linked to increased cortical thickness in the left inferior frontal region (Eicher et al., 2016). In a separate study, Pinel et al. (2012) found that THEM2 SNP rs17243157 was associated with functional asymmetry in a posterior temporal region during reading and speech listening tasks, with a stronger effect for reading.

NRSN1 has recently been implicated in reading-related cortical structure. Specifically, recent work has identified associations between variation on NRSN1 and gray matter volume in right parieto-occipital, left lateral occipital, and left occipito-temporal regions, as well as white matter volume in a left postcentral region (Skeide et al., 2016). In a follow-up classification analysis, left occipito-temporal volume performed significantly above chance in classifying subjects into SRD and control groups, and variation on NRSN1 was further associated with reading comprehension (Skeide et al., 2016). These structural and functional associations may be attributed to variation in NRSN1 expression, which has been linked to axon and dendrite growth (Araki et al., 2002). Together, these findings support THEM2 and NRSN1 as loci for further investigation of links between genes, neural structure/function, and reading.

The remaining candidate genes for SRD have received relatively little attention in human neuroimaging studies. Like those of other SRD candidate genes, the protein encoded by DYX1C1 has been linked to neurodevelopmental processes (Currier, Etchegaray, Haight, Galaburda, & Rosen, 2011; Rosen et al., 2007; Szalkowski et al., 2011; Tammimies et al., 2016; Tarkar et al., 2013; Threlkeld et al., 2007; Wang et al., 2006), and variation on this gene has been associated with reading and spelling abilities in both general population (Bates et al., 2010; Newbury et al., 2011; Zhang et al., 2012) and clinical samples (Lim, Ho, Chou, & Waye, 2011; Marino et al., 2007; Venkatesh, Siddaiah, Padakannaya, & Ramachandra, 2014). With respect to neuroimaging, Darki et al. (2012) found a relationship between SNP rs3743204 in DYX1C1 and white matter volume in bilateral temporo-parietal regions and correlations between white matter volume in these regions and reading scores. As with the KIAA0319 findings described above, these findings were replicated when a third longitudinal time point was added for the same sample (Darki et al., 2014).

ROBO1 appears to serve an axon guidance function that regulates the connections between brain hemispheres and between cortical and subcortical structures (Andrews et al., 2006; Hannula-Jouppi et al., 2005; Massinen et al., 2016; Whitford et al., 2002). Linkage studies suggest that this gene is related to SRD (Fisher et al., 2002; Mascheretti et al., 2014; Nopola-Hemmi et al., 2001; Tran et al., 2014) and speech-sound disorder (Stein et al., 2004), and research in the general population indicates an association with phonological skills (Bates et al., 2011). In the context of neuroimaging, ROBO1 has been investigated in one study that employed magnetoencephalography (MEG) to examine auditory processing in individuals with reading impairment from a family carrying a rare, weakly expressing haplotype of the ROBO1 gene relative to typical controls (Lamminmäki, Massinen, Nopola-Hemmi, Kere, & Hari, 2012). Ipsilateral auditory suppression in both hemispheres was related to ROBO1 expression levels in the haplotype carrier group, and these subjects showed significantly weaker ipsilateral suppression compared to the control group. These imaging findings point toward a possible auditory processing deficit as the source of previously observed associations between ROBO1, phonological skills, and speech-sound disorders.

3.1.2 ∣. Summary: Neuroimaging genetics studies of SRD

The early observation of heritability in SRD and subsequent identification of candidate genes have made SRD a model for studying genetic contributions to complex cognitive traits and understanding the genetic bases of heterogeneous neurodevelopmental disorders. Further, the extensive research on neural structure and function in SRD provides a basis for linking genes, brain structure and function, and behavior in this disorder. Associations between SRD risk genes and gray and white matter structure in reading-associated regions are consistent with the role of these genes in brain development and further support atypical neural development as a contributing factor to reading difficulties. Although cross-report comparisons of structural findings are mixed, inconsistencies are at least partially due to variability in SNPs investigated for a particular gene, the structural metrics that are used, ages of participants, and inclusion of individuals with SRD vs. use of a broader population sample. Additional research, including replication studies, is needed to clarify these ambiguities and more definitively establish links among specific genes and polymorphisms, and neural structure. While association among genetic variation and neural function is less well studied in SRD, extant fMRI and ERP studies linking polymorphisms on candidate genes to well-characterized auditory electrophysiological components and atypical neural function in reading-related regions provide some encouraging evidence for mapping of gene–brain–behavior relations.

4. ∣. DEVELOPMENTAL LANGUAGE DISORDER

Developmental language disorder (DLD) refers to difficulties in language acquisition that may affect comprehension and/or production of language across modalities (e.g., spoken and written; American Psychological Association, 2013). Because DLD often affects components of language that are also impaired in SRD, such as phonological awareness, similarities in the neural and genetic underpinnings of these two disorders are expected. The neurobiology of DLD has not been as well studied as that of SRD, but a small body of literature indicates decreased activation during language processing tasks (broadly construed) in individuals with DLD relative to typical controls in temporo-parietal and superior and middle temporal regions, as well as some mixed evidence for atypical functioning in the left inferior frontal gyrus (de Guibert et al., 2011; Hugdahl et al., 2004; Mayes et al., 2015). A number of genes have been linked to impairments in speech and language function, including FOXP2, CNTNAP2, ATP2C2, and CMIP (Newbury, Fisher, & Monaco, 2010). Because these genes play potentially wide-reaching roles in neurodevelopment and brain function (discussed below), imaging genetic research on these DLD-associated genes is not only helpful for linking genes to their specific neural functions in the context of expressive and receptive language but may also be helpful for understanding the potential cascading effects of these genes on reading.

4.1 ∣. Neuroimaging genetics studies of developmental language disorder

4.1.1. ∣ FOXP2

FOXP2 was first linked to language impairment in the KE family, which gained the attention of geneticists because half of the members are affected by a severe inherited speech and language disorder (Fisher, Vargha-Khadem, Watkins, Monaco, & Pembrey, 1998; Lai, Fisher, Hurst, Vargha-Khadem, & Monaco, 2001). Genetic associations with this speech and language disorder have been extensively investigated in a three-generation pedigree of the KE family that includes 27 family members (Lai et al., 2001). More recently, FOXP2 has been linked to variability in language and reading traits in both the general population and in a clinical sample (Mozzi et al., 2017). Using linkage analyses, researchers first identified the locus associated with inheritance of the disorder (SPCH1, chromosome 7q31), and later determined that a mutation in the gene FOXP2 was causally related to the disorder (Fisher et al., 1998; Lai et al., 2001). FOXP2 belongs to a family of genes that produce proteins that regulate expression of other genes during neurodevelopmental processes (Carlsson & Mahlapuu, 2002), leading to complex and widespread contribution of this gene to neurodevelopment (Newbury et al., 2010).

Among the series of studies investigating the nature of the KE family’s impairment are several examinations of neural structure and function. Studies of gray matter structure in the KE family have revealed atypical gray matter volume and density in affected compared to unaffected family members in cortical and subcortical regions associated with motor and language functions including the inferior frontal gyrus, pre-supplementary motor area, caudate nucleus, and cerebellum (Belton, Salmond, Watkins, Vargha-Khadem, & Gadian, 2003; Vargha-Khadem et al., 1998; Watkins et al., 2002). These early findings point toward a relationship of FOXP2 with brain structure in regions related to speech production and language; however, additional studies are needed to confirm whether these patterns hold in members of the general population who exhibit atypical expression of FOXP2.

In addition to structural anomalies, affected members of the KE family have shown altered functional neural activation during both overt and covert language production tasks in motor areas and language-related regions (Liegeois et al., 2003; Vargha-Khadem et al., 1998). Research in the KE family has also led to the investigation of functional associations with FOXP2 SNPs in the general population, with consistent findings related to activation in the left inferior frontal cortex (Pinel et al., 2012; Wilcke et al., 2012). For example, associations have been observed between rs6980093 and left inferior frontal activation and between rs7784315 and rs17137135 and left precentral activation in response to sentences presented in auditory and visual modalities in a study of typically developing young adults (Pinel et al., 2012). Moreover, Wilcke et al. (2012) included typically developing children and children with SRD in their fMRI study of FOXP2 SNPs. Results revealed decreased activation during a rhyme decision task in the temporo-parietal cortex, inferior frontal cortex, superior occipital gyrus, and lingual gyrus in carriers of the risk allele on rs1253305 (Wilcke et al., 2012). This evidence suggests that the neural alterations associated with FOXP2 are not limited to the mutation characteristic of the KE family, but may play a role in language disorders more broadly with possible implications for reading impairment.

4.1.2. ∣ CNTNAP2

CNTNAP2 has been identified as a risk-gene for DLD and autism (Alarcón et al., 2008; Strauss et al., 2006), and variation in CNTNAP2 has been associated with performance on nonsense word repetition (Vernes et al., 2008) and with rapid auditory processing (Riva et al., 2018). CNTNAP2 (also known as CASPR2), a gene proximal to and regulated by FOXP2, is involved in regulation of neuron–glia interaction related to myelination and localization of ion channels (Poliak et al., 2003; Rasband, 2004; Vernes et al., 2008). These functions have been taken as support for a role of this gene in structural connectivity and neural activity. Functional neuroimaging studies implicate a relationship between CNTNAP2 polymorphisms and atypical lateralization of language processing. For example, Whalley et al. (2011) found associations between lateralization for language and two CNTNAP2 polymorphisms in typically developing adults during a sentence completion task. Specifically, increased activation in the right inferior frontal cortex combined with decreased activation in the left superior parietal lobule was observed in individuals homozygous for the risk allele at rs2710102 compared to all other subjects; and increased activation in the right middle temporal gyrus was observed in those homozygous for risk allele at rs7794745 compared to all other subjects. Further evidence for atypical lateralization for language associated with CNTNAP2 olymorphisms comes from a study of language, voice, and general auditory processing. Koeda et al. (2015) reported increased activation in the right middle frontal gyrus during native language listening in Japanese carriers of the nondominant allele for rs7794745. This pattern of increased right hemisphere activation also held for human voice perception (reversed sentences) the right middle frontal gyrus. Further, these authors observed an interaction between lateralization of function during voice processing (forward and reversed speech), rs7794745 genotype, and handedness, suggesting a complex influence of this SNP on voice processing networks in relation to handedness and lateralization.6

With respect to brain structure, two studies have identified regional reductions in gray matter volume associated with the risk allele on rs7794745 primarily affecting occipital, fusiform, and cerebellar regions (Tan, Doke, Ashburner, Wood, & Frackowiak, 2010; Uddén, Snijders, Fisher, & Hagoort, 2017). Additionally, altered white matter structure associated with CNTNAP2 has been reported in fronto-occipital and thalamic tracts (Tan et al., 2010) as well as midbrain tracts that facilitate communication among the cortex, cerebellum, and other central nervous system structures (Skeide et al., 2016). Using diffusion MRI, Dennis et al. (2011) identified a pattern of white matter structure characterized by local rather than long-range connections in individuals homozygous for the risk allele on CNTNAP2 SNP rs2710102. These reductions in long-range connectivity may indicate a weakened link between frontal and temporal cortical regions important for language processing and production. These white matter findings are consistent with CNTNAP2’s role in myelination and suggest that CNTNAP2 may contribute to variation in language skills via modulation of connectivity among regions that are needed for effective language function.

4.1.2 ∣. Summary: Neuroimaging genetics studies of DLD

Neuroimaging genetic investigations of these two primary DLD candidate genes suggest specific roles of each gene in language-related neural function. Imaging genetics studies of FOXP2 indicate that this gene affects brain regions associated with language production and speech motor planning (Liegeois et al., 2003; Pinel et al., 2012; Vargha-Khadem, Gadian, Copp, & Mishkin, 2005), with preliminary links to activity in posterior language regions (Wilcke et al., 2012). CNTNAP2 has also been found to have effects on functional activation in language-related regions, including inferior frontal and middle temporal gyri (Whalley et al., 2011) and on gray matter morphology in occipital, fusiform, and cerebellar regions (Tan et al., 2010; Uddén et al., 2017); in addition, this gene has effects on structural and functional connectivity among language-associated regions and more domain general regions (Dennis et al., 2011; Skeide et al., 2016). The compelling neuroimaging genetic findings related to both of these genes may direct future research to further characterize their roles in DLD. Further examination of FOXP2 in the general population will be particularly useful to disentangle the specific effects of the KE Family mutation from more common polymorphisms that may contribute to individual differences in language abilities. Additional functional neuroimaging research is needed to clarify the associations of these genes with specific aspects of language processing. Furthermore, several additional DLD candidate genes (e.g., ATP2C2 and CMIP) have yet to be studied using neuroimaging genetics, and this approach may provide insight into the mechanisms by which they impact language function.

5. ∣. ADDITIONAL GENES LINKED TO READING AND LANGUAGE

Neuroimaging genetic research provides an informative approach to understanding the contributions of novel candidate genes for SRD and DLD and language/reading-related polymorphisms located in intergenic regions. Gialluisi et al. (2017) reported promising findings in several recently identified candidate genes for reading and language disorders. The minor allele of RBFOX2 SNP rs5995177, previously linked to reading/language skills (Gialluisi et al., 2014), was significantly associated with decreased cortical thickness in parietal, temporal, and inferior frontal regions of interest (Gialluisi et al., 2017). Furthermore, three SNPs located upstream of CCDC136 (rs56184882, rs339054, and rs339046) were nominally related to cortical surface area of the inferior frontal gyrus bilaterally (Gialluisi et al., 2017). Evidence for the role of a set of SNPs located in a noncoding region has also been revealed by neurogenetic methods. Variation on a haplotype formed by rs4234898 and rs11100040 in a noncoding region in chromosome 4q32.1 has been associated with the MMN response in children with SRD (Roeske et al., 2011). Both SNPs were associated with regulation of the glucose transport gene SLC2A3, leading the authors to propose that the attenuated MMN response observed in children with SRD may arise from reduced glucose resulting from the modulation of SLC2A3 expression by rs4234898 and rs11100040. In another investigation, Skeide et al. (2015) reported reduced functional and structural connectivity among left hemisphere reading/language regions in children carrying the risk-allele at rs11100040. Additional research will be needed to investigate a potential causal mechanism among these SNPs, regulation of SLC2A3, connectivity, and reading and language abilities.

Several studies have linked the DYX3 genetic locus on chromosome 2 containing GCFC2/C2Orf3 and MRPL19 to SRD (Anthoni et al., 2007; Fagerheim et al., 1999; Kaminen et al., 2003), but the role of these genes in neural function remains unknown. Two neuroimaging genetic studies of this locus suggest associations with neural structure. Eicher et al. (2016) reported suggestive associations between gray matter structure and SNPs in and upstream of GCFC2. A separate study linked one DYX3 SNP to decreased white matter volume in the posterior corpus callosum and cingulum, suggesting a disturbance in interhemispheric connectivity between posterior reading-related regions (Scerri et al., 2012). Scerri and colleagues also found an association between variation in DYX3 SNPs and verbal IQ performance, extending previous links of the DYX3 locus with reading to a broader cognitive scope.

Our laboratory has recently conducted a behavioral association and neuroimaging genetic study of the SETBP1 gene, which was previously associated with expressive language function in an isolated population in Russia with a high prevalence of DLD (Kornilov et al., 2016). This population is of interest for investigating a shared genetic cause of DLD because its remote location leads to reduced genetic diversity and increased heritability of traits within the population. A follow-up study revealed a significant association between the SETBP1 gene and reading-relevant skills (e.g., phonological working memory) in a group of typically developing children in the United States (Perdue et al., 2019). Further, we investigated brain activation for the SNP with the strongest association (rs7230525) in a subset of participants who completed an fMRI task that involved reading and listening to words and pseudowords. Our imaging analysis revealed a complex three-way interaction among genotype, word type, and presentation modality in the right inferior parietal lobule. Breaking down this interaction revealed greater activation for more difficult to process printed stimuli (pseudowords > words) for individuals in the genotype group associated with poorer phonological skills (Perdue et al., 2019). Although preliminary, these findings point to a relation between variability in SETBP1 and the role of attentional networks during decoding.

Finally, due to the complex nature of reading, it is likely that genes involved in regulating cognitive processes such as memory, executive functioning, and attention may subsequently be related to reading and language. Two such “generalist genes” are COMT, which is involved in dopamine regulation in the prefrontal cortex (Meyer-Lindenberg et al., 2005), and BDNF, which regulates a variety of processes involved in brain development and plasticity (Numakawa et al., 2010). One recent study showed an association between the COMT Val158Met polymorphism (SNP rs4680) and neural activation in the reading circuit (e.g., the left occipito-temporal and superior temporal/middle temporal regions) during a word and pseudoword reading task, in addition to associations with phonological awareness and spelling (Landi et al., 2013). A second study showed that a common variation at BDNF SNP rs6265 (the BDNF Val66Met polymorphism) is associated with reading-related behaviors, including passage comprehension, and neural activation during reading in children (Jasińska et al., 2016). In light of these reading-related findings for genes associated with more general cognitive factors, it seems important that the field continue to consider the contribution of so-called generalist genes in examinations of the genetic basis of individual differences in reading and language.

6. ∣. LIMITATIONS AND FUTURE DIRECTIONS

Although neuroimaging genetics provide a useful lens for investigating mechanisms that link genes to behavior in neurodevelopmental disorders, several important limitations must be addressed: reproducibility and convergence. Replication of genetic associations with language phenotypes has emerged as one key problem in the extant literature, both in genome-wide and targeted genetic association studies. Specifically, Carrion-Castillo et al. (2016) conducted a genetic association study of reading and language phenotypes with 17 of the most significant SNPs associated with these phenotypes in prior GWAS studies and failed to find any significant associations surviving correction for multiple comparisons in an independent sample from the Netherlands. Accordingly, neuroimaging genetics studies of reading and language should be interpreted with caution, especially because most do not include an independent replication cohort. Indeed, only one of the studies reviewed here included an independent replication cohort for neuroimaging-genetic analysis (Roeske et al., 2011). Furthermore, Grabitz et al. (2018) recently raised additional concerns with regard to genetics studies published in neuroimaging journals, namely, issues of sample size and power, calculation of effect size, correction for multiple comparisons, completeness of reporting, and complexity of analysis, in addition to the aforementioned replication problem. Indeed, very few of the studies reviewed here include reports of effect sizes, and several of those reported effect sizes for behavioral-genetic associations, but not imaging genetic associations, making it difficult to interpret the magnitude of reported effects.

Additionally, heterogeneity among study samples, measures, and methods likely contributes to a perceived lack of replication in some cases. The neuroimaging-genetic studies reviewed here include a great deal of variance in selection of samples, genetic markers, and analytic approaches that make it difficult to compare findings across studies. For example, Eicher et al. (2016) included a set of behaviorally associated SNPs on DCDC2, but not the DCDC2 intron 2 deletion (DCDC2d) in their neuroimaging genetic analysis. This omission could lead to perceived lack of convergence with previous imaging genetic work on DCDC2 by Meda et al. (2008) that found an association between gray matter volume and the DCDC2d. Likewise, differences in the acquisition and analysis of neuroimaging data such as examining white matter volume versus white matter microstructure (FA, RD, and AD) represent methodological heterogeneity that limits cross-study comparability. Large-scale studies accounting for such variations are needed to confirm or reject the associations of specific SNPs with reading and language phenotypes. This poses a practical challenge that will require more collaborative cross-disciplinary efforts among researchers to collect the data required for replication. Transparent reporting of methods may also address issues of convergence such that future studies may provide appropriate comparison to existing findings.

7. ∣. GENERAL CONCLUSION

The findings presented in this review illustrate the complex neurobiological underpinnings of reading and language disorders (See Table 2 for a summary of results from neuroimaging genetics studies reviewed in this paper). Though the application of neuroimaging genetic methods to research on SRD and DLD is in its early stages, common threads are emerging in the observed relations among gene, brain, and behavior. Indeed, imaging genetics research has linked variation on genes involved in aspects of neurodevelopment with brain structure and/or function in language-associated areas and reading/language performance. Although these links are not as clear for genes with less well-specified roles in neurodevelopmental processes or neuronal function, imaging genetics findings may guide new research that seeks to better understand the role of these genes in brain structure or function. Imaging genetics research also validates the involvement of a number of generalist genes, which contribute to individual differences in cognitive processing that affect reading and language performance. These contributions may explain additional genetic variance in reading and language performance beyond specific risk genes for dyslexia or DLD, thereby addressing the often-cited missing heritability problem.

The associations revealed between genes and neural activation suggest genetic sources of atypical neural functioning, but it remains unclear whether functional deficits arise due to anomalous brain structure and/or direct contributions of genes to neuronal excitation and metabolism. Limited by their correlational nature, neuroimaging genetic approaches cannot directly investigate causal mechanisms of SRD and DLD in humans, but these methods are key in building theoretical models of the etiologies of these prevalent neurodevelopmental disorders. Advances in neuroimaging technologies will help to fill some gaps in the mechanisms linking genes, brain, and behavior. One promising direction is the application of magnetic resonance spectroscopy (MRS) in neurogenetic research. This noninvasive tool allows for in vivo measurement of neurometabolites and neurotransmitters, thereby providing a neurochemical level of exploration for identification of connections between genes and neural function. This method affords human investigation of evidence linking genes and neurochemistry to atypical neural function in animal models such as the association between mutation of the DCDC2 homolog in mice and altered neural activity driven by dysfunction of the neurotransmitter glutamate (Che, Girgenti, & LoTurco, 2014; Che, Truong, Fitch, & Loturco, 2016). Indeed, initial work utilizing MRS in humans has identified links between glutamate and choline and SRD and formed the basis for the application of the neural noise hypothesis to the study of SRD (Hancock, Pugh, & Hoeft, 2017; Pugh et al., 2014).

Further insight may arise from the combined use of neuroimaging and advanced DNA sequencing methods, including next-generation whole genome sequencing and whole exome sequencing, which afford the advantage of detecting small structural variants in addition to mutations and SNPs (Kornilov & Grigorenko, 2016). Ongoing developments in the use of next-generation sequencing along with advances in statistical approaches will support the discovery of new risk genes as well as complex gene by gene interactions and gene by environment interactions that further our understanding of the genetic bases of SRD and DLD.

Finally, the field will benefit from continued attempts at replication of extant findings as well as data sharing and combining that will allow for mega- and meta-analyses. The aforementioned lack of replication, lack of methodological overlap, and limited details in statistical reporting pose challenges to neuroimaging genetics research, but these issues may be ameliorated through the application of rigorous research and reporting methods. With these limitations in mind, neuroimaging genetic methods can make an important contribution by helping to constrain exploratory findings through identification of intermediate phenotypes that elucidate brain-based mechanisms linking genes to behavior in SRD and DLD.

Supplementary Material

Appendix A

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

This work was supported in part by the following grants: NSF IGERT DGE (1144399); NSF GRFP DGE (1747453); NIH R01 HD (48830); NIH R03 HD (053409); and Florida Learning Disabilities Research Clinic, NIH P50 HD (052120 and NIH 2P50HD052120-11; and NIH R21 DA (030665).

Footnotes

1

For purposes of this review, specific reading disability is synonymous with reading disability (RD) and dyslexia, and developmental language disorder is synonymous with specific language impairment (SLI) and language impairment (LI).

2

Allele frequency data are publicly available in the dbSNP database: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/snp (Sherry et al., 2001).

3

Unique identifiers called reference SNP (rs) ID numbers are assigned by The National Center for Biotechnology Information (NCBI) for consistent identification and comparison of polymorphisms across individuals (Kitts, Phan, Ward, & Holmes, 2013).

4

Although negative findings have also been reported (Paracchini et al., 2011; Scerri et al., 2017).

5

PING is a multi-site study and corresponding database that includes standardized behavioral measures, imaging phenotypes, and whole genome genotyping (Jernigan et al., 2016).

6

The non-dominant allele (A) identified for this study according to the Japanese Hapmap ratio does not correspond to the rs7794745 risk-allele (T) identified in the Whalley et al. (2011) study described above.

REFERENCES

  1. Alarcón M, Abrahams BS, Stone JL, Duvall JA, Perederiy JV, Bomar JM, … Geschwind DH (2008). Linkage, association, and gene-expression analyses identify CNTNAP2 as an Autism-susceptibility gene. The American Journal of Human Genetics, 82(1), 150–159. 10.1016/j.ajhg.2007.09.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Andrews W, Liapi A, Plachez C, Camurri L, Zhang J, Mori S, … Richards LJ (2006). ROBO1 regulates the development of major axon tracts and interneuron migration in the forebrain. Development, 133, 2243–2252. 10.1242/dev.02379 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Anthoni H, Zucchelli M, Matsson H, Müller-Myhsok B, Fransson I, Schumacher J, Peyrard-Janvid M .(2007). A locus on 2p12 containing the co-regulated MRPL19 and C2ORF3 genes is associated to dyslexia. Human Molecular Genetics, 16(6), 667–677. 10.1093/hmg/ddm009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Araki M, Nagata K, Satoh Y, Kadota Y, Hisha H, Adachi Y, & Taketani S (2002). Developmentally regulated expression of Neuro-p24 and its possible function in neurite extension. Neuroscience Research, 44(4), 379–389. 10.1016/S0168-0102(02)00156-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Association AP (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing. [Google Scholar]
  6. Bates TC, Lind PA, Luciano M, Montgomery GW, Martin NG, & Wright MJ (2010). Dyslexia and DYX1C1: Deficits in reading and spelling associated with a missense mutation. Molecular Psychiatry, 15(12), 1190–1196. 10.1038/mp.2009.120 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bates TC, Luciano M, Medland SE, Montgomery GW, Wright MJ, & Martin NG (2011). Genetic variance in a component of the language acquisition device: ROBO1 polymorphisms associated with phonological buffer deficits. Behavior Genetics, 41, 50–57. 10.1007/s10519-010-9402-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Belton E, Salmond CH, Watkins KE, Vargha-Khadem F, & Gadian DG (2003). Bilateral brain abnormalities associated with dominantly inherited verbal and orofacial dyspraxia. Human Brain Mapping, 18(3), 194–200. 10.1002/hbm.10093 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Brkanac Z, Chapman NH, Matsushita MM, Chun L, Nielsen K, Cochrane E, … Raskind WH (2007). Evaluation of candidate genes for DYX1 and DYX2 in families with dyslexia. American Journal of Medical Genetics, Part B: Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 144B(4), 556–560. 10.1002/ajmg.b.30471 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Carlsson P, & Mahlapuu M (2002). Forkhead transcription factors: Key players in development and metabolism. Developmental Biology, 250(1), 1–23. 10.1006/dbio.2002.0780 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Carrion-Castillo A, van Bergen E, Vino A, van Zuijen T, de Jong PF, Francks C, & Fisher SE (2016). Evaluation of results from genome-wide studies of language and reading in a novel independent dataset. Genes, Brain and Behavior, 15(6), 531–541. 10.1111/gbb.12299 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Che A, Girgenti MJ, & LoTurco J (2014). The dyslexia-associated gene Dcdc2 is required for spike-timing precision in mouse neocortex. Biological Psychiatry, 76(5), 387–396. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.08.018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Che A, Truong DT, Fitch RH, & Loturco JJ (2016). Mutation of the dyslexia-associated gene Dcdc2 enhances glutamatergic synaptic transmission between layer 4 neurons in mouse neocortex. Cerebral Cortex, 26, 3705–3718. 10.1093/cercor/bhv168 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Chen Y, Zhao H, Zhang YX, & Zuo PX (2017). DCDC2 gene polymorphisms are associated with developmental dyslexia in Chinese Uyghur children. Neural regeneration research, 12(2), 259. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Cicchini GM, Marino C, Mascheretti S, Perani D, & Morrone MC (2015). Strong motion deficits in dyslexia associated with DCDC2 gene alteration. Journal of Neuroscience, 35(21), 8059–8064. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5077-14.2015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Cope N, Eicher JD, Meng H, Gibson CJ, Hager K, Lacadie C, … Gruen JR (2012). Variants in the DYX2 locus are associated with altered brain activation in reading-related brain regions in subjects with reading disability. NeuroImage, 63(1), 148–156. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.06.037 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Cope N, Harold D, Hill G, Moskvina V, Stevenson J, Holmans P, … Williams J (2005). Strong evidence that KIAA0319 on chromosome 6p is a susceptibility gene for developmental dyslexia. American Journal of Human Genetics, 76, 581–591. Retrieved from. http://ac.els-cdn.com/S0002929707628705/1-s2.0-S0002929707628705-main.pdf?_tid=a04e8030-0ff4-11e7-a503-00000aacb361&acdnat=1490293079_60766b9884cfedef4c1f730500158ba4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Couto JM, Livne-Bar I, Huang K, Xu Z, Cate-Carter T, Feng Y, … Barr CL (2010). Association of reading disabilities with regions marked by acetylated H3 histones in KIAA0319. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part B: Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 153B(2), 447–462. 10.1002/ajmg.b.30999 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Currier TA, Etchegaray MA, Haight JL, Galaburda AM, & Rosen GD (2011). The effects of embryonic knockdown of the candidate dyslexia susceptibility gene homologue Dyx1c1 on the distribution of GABAergic neurons in the cerebral cortex. Neuroscience, 172, 535–546. 10.1016/J.NEUROSCIENCE.2010.11.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Czamara D, Bruder J, Becker J, Bartling J, Hoffmann P, Ludwig KU, … Schulte-Körne G (2011). Association of a rare variant with mismatch negativity in a region between KIAA0319 and DCDC2 in dyslexia. Behavior Genetics, 41(1), 110–119. 10.1007/s10519-010-9413-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Darki F, Peyrard-Janvid M, Matsson H, Kere J, & Klingberg T (2012). Three dyslexia susceptibility genes, DYX1C1, DCDC2, and KIAA0319, affect temporo-parietal white matter structure. Biological Psychiatry, 72(8), 671–676. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.05.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Darki F, Peyrard-Janvid M, Matsson H, Kere J, & Klingberg T (2014). DCDC2 polymorphism is associated with left temporoparietal gray and white matter structures during development. Journal of Neuroscience, 34(43), 14455–14462. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1216-14.2014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. de Guibert C, Maumet C, Jannin P, Ferre J-C, Treguier C, Barillot C, … Biraben A (2011). Abnormal functional lateralization and activity of language brain areas in typical specific language impairment (developmental dysphasia). Brain, 134(10), 3044–3058. 10.1093/brain/awr141 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Dennis EL, Jahanshad N, Rudie JD, Brown JA, Johnson K, Mcmahon KL, … Thompson PM (2011). Altered structural brain connectivity in healthy carriers of the Autism risk gene, CNTNAP2. Brain Connectivity, 1(6), 447–459. 10.1089/brain.2011.0064 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Dennis MY, Paracchini S, Scerri TS, Prokunina-Olsson L, Knight JC, Wade-Martins R, … Monaco AP (2009). A common variant associated with dyslexia reduces expression of the KIAA0319 gene. PLoS Genetics, 5(3). 10.1371/journal.pgen.1000436 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Eicher JD, Montgomery AM, Akshoomoff N, Amaral DG, Bloss CS, Libiger O, … Gruen JR (2016). Dyslexia and language impairment associated genetic markers influence cortical thickness and white matter in typically developing children. Brain Imaging and Behavior, 10, 272–282. 10.1007/s11682-015-9392-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Eicher JD, Powers NR, Miller LL, Mueller KL, Mascheretti S, Marino C, … Olson RK (2014). Characterization of the DYX2 locus on chromosome 6p22 with reading disability, language impairment, and IQ. Human Genetics, 133(133), 869–881. 10.1007/s00439-014-1427-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Estruch SB, Graham SA, Chinnappa SM, Deriziotis P, & Fisher SE (2016). Functional characterization of rare FOXP2 variants in neurodevelopmental disorder. Journal of neurodevelopmental disorders, 8(1), 44. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Fagerheim T, Raeymaekers P, Egil Tønnessen F, Pedersen M, Tranebjaerg L, & Lubs HA (1999). A new gene (DYX3) for dyslexia is located on chromosome 2. Journal of Medical Genetics, 3636 Retrieved from. http://jmg.bmj.com/content/jmedgenet/36/9/664.full.pdf [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Fisher SE, Francks C, Marlow AJ, MacPhie IL, Newbury DF, Cardon LR, … Monaco AP (2002). Independent genome-wide scans identify a chromosome 18 quantitative-trait locus influencing dyslexia. Nature Genetics, 30(1), 86–91. 10.1038/ng792 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Fisher SE, Vargha-Khadem F, Watkins KE, Monaco AP, & Pembrey ME (1998). Localisation of a gene implicated in a severe speech and language disorder. Nature Genetics, 18(2), 168–170. 10.1038/ng0298-168 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Fletcher JM (2009). Dyslexia: The evolution of a scientific concept. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 15(4), 501–508. 10.1017/S1355617709090900 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Gialluisi A, Newbury DF, Wilcutt EG, Olson RK, DeFries JC, Brandler WM, … Fisher SE (2014). Genome-wide screening for DNA variants associated with reading and language traits. Genes, Brain and Behavior, 13(7), 686–701. 10.1111/gbb.12158 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Gialluisi A, Guadalupe T, Francks C, & Fisher SE (2017). Neuroimaging genetic analyses of novel candidate genes associated with reading and language. Brain and Language, 172, 9–15. 10.1016/j.bandl.2016.07.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Gori S, Mascheretti S, Giora E, Ronconi L, Ruffino M, Quadrelli E, … Marino C (2015). The DCDC2 intron 2 deletion impairs illusory motion perception unveiling the selective role of magnocellular-dorsal stream in reading (dis)ability. Cerebral Cortex, 25, 1685–1695. 10.1093/cercor/bhu234 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Gottlieb G, & Lickliter R (2007). Probabilistic epigenesis. Developmental Science, 10(1), 1–11. 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00556.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Grabitz CR, Button KS, Munafò MR, Newbury DF, Pernet CR, Thompson PA, & Bishop DVM (2018). Logical and methodological issues affecting genetic studies of humans reported in top neuroscience journals. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 30, 25–41. 10.1162/jocn_a_01192 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Hancock R, Pugh KR, & Hoeft F (2017). Neural noise hypothesis of developmental dyslexia. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 21(6), 434–448. 10.1016/j.tics.2017.03.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Hannula-Jouppi K, Kaminen-Ahola N, Taipale M, Eklund R, Nopola-Hemmi J, Kääriäinen H, & Kere J (2005). The axon guidance receptor gene ROBO1 is a candidate gene for developmental dyslexia. PLoS Genetics, 1(4),0467–0474. Retrieved from. http://journals.plos.org/plosgenetics/article/file?id=10.1371/journal.pgen.0010050&type=printable [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Harold D, Paracchini S, Scerri T, Dennis M, Cope N, Hill G, … Monaco AP (2006). Further evidence that the KIAA0319 gene confers susceptibility to developmental dyslexia. Molecular Psychiatry, 11, 1085–1091. 10.1038/sj.mp.4001904 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Hirschhorn JN, & Daly MJ (2005). Genome-wide association studies for common diseases and complex traits. Nature Reviews Genetics, 6, 95–108. 10.1038/nrg1521 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Hoeft F, Meyler A, Hernandez A, Juel C, Taylor-Hill H, Martindale JL, … Merzenich MM (2007). Functional and morphometric brain dissociation between dyslexia and reading ability. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104(10), 4234–4239. Retrieved from. http://www.pnas.org/content/104/10/4234.full.pdf, 10.1073/pnas.0609399104 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Hugdahl K, Gundersen H, Brekke C, Thomsen T, Rimol LM, Ersland L, & Niemi J (2004). fMRI brain activation in a Finnish family with specific language impairment compared with a normal control group. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 47(1), 162 10.1044/1092-4388(2004/014) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Hulme C, & Snowling MJ (2013). The interface between spoken and written language: developmental disorders. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 369(1634). Retrieved from). http://rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org/content/369/1634/20120395.short [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Human Genome Sequencing Consortium, I (2004). Finishing the euchromatic sequence of the human genome. Nature, 431(7011), 931–945. 10.1038/nature03001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Iafrate AJ, Feuk L, Rivera MN, Listewnik ML, Donahoe PK, Qi Y, … Lee C (2004). Detection of large-scale variation in the human genome. Nature Genetics, 36(9), 949–951. Retrieved from. http://www.nature.com/ng/journal/v36/n9/pdf/ng1416.pdf, 10.1038/ng1416 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Im K, Raschle NM, Smith SA, Grant PE, & Gaab N (2016). Atypical sulcal pattern in children with developmental dyslexia and at-risk kindergarteners. Cerebral Cortex, 26, 1138–1148. 10.1093/cercor/bhu305 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Jasińska KK, Molfese PJ, Kornilov SA, Mencl WE, Frost SJ, Lee M… Landi N(2016). The BDNF Val66Met polymorphism influences reading ability and patterns of neural activation in children. PLoS ONE, 11(8), e0157449 10.1371/journal.pone.0157449 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Jernigan TL, Brown TT, Hagler DJ, Akshoomoff N, Bartsch H, Newman E, … Dale AM (2016). The pediatric imaging, neurocognition, and genetics (PING) data repository. NeuroImage, 124, 1149–1154. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.04.057 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Kaminen N, Hannula-Jouppi K, Kestilä M, Lahermo P, Muller K, Kaaranen M, … Kere J (2003). A genome scan for developmental dyslexia confirms linkage to chromosome 2p11 and suggests a new locus on 7q32. Journal of Medical Genetics, 40, 340–345. Retrieved from. http://jmg.bmj.com/content/jmedgenet/40/5/340.full.pdf, 10.1136/jmg.40.5.340 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Kitts A, Phan L, Ward M, & Holmes JB (2013). The database of short genetic variation (dbSNP) In The NCBI Handbook [Internet] (2nd ed.). Bethesda, MD: National Center for Biotechnology Information; Retrieved from. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK174586/pdf/Bookshelf_NBK174586.pdf [Google Scholar]
  52. Koeda M, Watanabe A, Tsuda K, Matsumoto M, Ikeda Y, Kim W, … Okubo Y (2015). Interaction effect between handedness and CNTNAP2 polymorphism (rs7794745 genotype) on voice-specific frontotemporal activity in healthy individuals: An fMRI study. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience Www.Frontiersin.Org, 987(9). 10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00087 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Kornilov SA, & Grigorenko EL (2016). Molecular genetics methods for developmental scientists In Cicchetti D (Ed.), Developmental psychopathology (3rd ed.) (pp. 378–415). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. [Google Scholar]
  54. Kornilov SA, Rakhlin N, Koposov R, Lee M, Yrigollen C, Caglayan AO, … Grigorenko EL (2016). Genome-wide association and exome sequencing study of language disorder in an isolated population. Pediatrics, 137(4). Retrieved from), e20152469 http://magnuson.psy.uconn.edu/wp-content/uploads/sites/1140/2015/01/Kornilov_etal_peds.2015-2469.full_.pdf, 10.1542/peds.2015-2469 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Kronbichler M, Wimmer H, Staffen W, Hutzler F, Mair A, & Ladurner G (2008). Developmental dyslexia: Gray matter abnormalities in the occipitotemporal cortex. Human Brain Mapping, 29(5), 613–625. 10.1002/hbm.20425 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Lai CSL, Fisher SE, Hurst JA, Vargha-Khadem F, & Monaco AP (2001). A forkhead-domain gene is mutated in a severe speech and language disorder. Nature, 413(6855), 519–523. 10.1038/35097076 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Lamminmäki S, Massinen S, Nopola-Hemmi J, Kere J, & Hari R (2012). Human ROBO1 regulates interaural interaction in auditory pathways. The Journal of Neuroscience, 32(3), 966–971. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4007-11.2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Lander ES (1996). The new genomics: Global views of biology. Source: Science Genome Issue, 274(5287), 536–539. Retrieved from. http://www.jstor.org/stable/2899626 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Landi N, Frost SJ, Mencl WE, Preston JL, Jacobsen LK, Lee M, … Grigorenko EL (2013). The COMT Val/Met polymorphism is associated with reading-related skills and consistent patterns of functional neural activation. Developmental Science, 16(1), 13–23. 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2012.01180.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Landi N, Mencl WE, Frost SJ, Sandak R, & Pugh KR (2010). An fMRI study of multimodal semantic and phonological processing in reading disabled adolescents. Annals of Dyslexia, 60(1), 102–121. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Liegeois F, Baldeweg T, Connelly A, Gadian DG, Mishkin M, & Vargha-Khadem F (2003). Language fMRI abnormalities associated with FOXP2 gene mutation. Nature Neuroscience, 6(11), 1230–1237. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Lim CKP, Ho CSH, Chou CHN, & Waye MMY (2011). Association of the rs3743205 variant of DYX1C1 with dyslexia in Chinese children. Behavioral and Brain Functions, 7, 1–9. 10.1186/1744-9081-7-16 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Lim CKP, Wong AMB, Ho CSH, & Waye MMY (2014). A common haplotype of KIAA0319 contributes to the phonological awareness skill in Chinese children. Behavioral and Brain Functions, 10(1), 23. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Lind PA, Luciano M, Wright MJ, Montgomery GW, Martin NG, & Bates TC (2010). Dyslexia and DCDC2: Normal variation in reading and spelling is associated with DCDC2 polymorphisms in an Australian population sample. European Journal of Human Genetics, 18, 668–673. 10.1038/ejhg.2009.237 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Luciano M, Lind PA, Duffy DL, Castles A, Wright MJ, Montgomery GW, … Bates TC (2007). A haplotype spanning KIAA0319 and TTRAP is associated with normal variation in reading and spelling ability. Biological psychiatry, 62(7), 811–817. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Ludwig KU, Roeske D, Schumacher J, Schulte-Körne G, König IR, Warnke A, … Hoffmann P (2008).Investigation of interaction between DCDC2 and KIAA0319 in a large German dyslexia sample. Journal of Neural Transmission, 115(11), 1587–1589. 10.1007/s00702-008-0124-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Ludwig KU, Schumacher J, Schulte-Körne G, König IR, Warnke A, Plume E, … Hoffmann P (2008). Investigation of the DCDC2 intron 2 deletion/compound short tandem repeat polymorphism in a large German dyslexia sample. Psychiatric Genetics, 18(6), 310–312. 10.1097/YPG.0b013e3283063a78 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Lyon GR, Shaywitz SE, & Shaywitz BA (2003). A definition of dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 53(1), 1–14. 10.1007/s11881-003-0001-9 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  69. Maisog JM, Einbinder ER, Flowers DL, Turkeltaub PE, & Eden GF (2008). A meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies of dyslexia. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1145(1), 237–259. 10.1196/annals.1416.024 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Marino C, Citterio A, Giorda R, Facoetti A, Menozzi G, Vanzin L, … Molteni M (2007). Association of short-term memory with a variant within DYX1C1 in developmental dyslexia. Genes, Brain and Behavior, 6(7), 640–646. 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2006.00291.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Marino C, Meng H, Mascheretti S, Rusconi M, Cope N, Giorda R, … Gruen JR (2012). DCDC2 genetic variants and susceptibility to developmental dyslexia. Psychiatric Genetics, 22(1), 25–30. 10.1097/YPG.0b013e32834acdb2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Marino C, Scifo P, Della Rosa PA, Mascheretti S, Facoetti A, Lorusso ML, … Perani D (2014). The DCDC2/intron 2 deletion and white matter disorganization: Focus on developmental dyslexia. Cortex, 57, 227–243. 10.1016/j.cortex.2014.04.016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Mascheretti S, Riva V, Giorda R, Beri S, Lanzoni LFE, Cellino MR, & Marino C (2014). KIAA0319 and ROBO1: Evidence on association with reading and pleiotropic effects on language and mathematics abilities in developmental dyslexia. Journal of Human Genetics, 59(4), 189–197. 10.1038/jhg.2013.141 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Massinen S, Wang J, Laivuori K, Bieder A, Tapia Paez I, Jiao H, & Kere J (2016). Genomic sequencing of a dyslexia susceptibility haplotype encompassing ROBO1. Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders, 8(4), 4 10.1186/s11689-016-9136-y [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Matsson H, Huss M, Persson H, Einarsdottir E, Tiraboschi E, Nopola-Hemmi J, … Schulte-Körne G (2015). Polymorphisms in DCDC2 and S100B associate with developmental dyslexia. Journal of human genetics, 60(7), 399. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Mayes AK, Reilly S, & Morgan AT (2015). Neural correlates of childhood language disorder: A systematic review. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 57(8), 706–717. 10.1111/dmcn.12714 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Meda SA, Gelernter J, Gruen JR, Calhoun VD, Meng H, Cope NA, & Pearlson GD (2008). Polymorphism of DCDC2 reveals differences in cortical morphology of healthy individuals—A preliminary voxel based morphometry study. Brain Imaging and Behavior, 2(1), 21–26. 10.1007/s11682-007-9012-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Meng H, Smith SD, Hager K, Held M, Liu J, Olson RK, … Gruen JR (2005). DCDC2 is associated with reading disability and modulates neuronal development in the brain. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 102(47), 17053–17058. Retrieved from. http://www.pnas.org/content/102/47/17053.full.pdf [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Meyer-Lindenberg A, Kohn PD, Kolachana B, Kippenhan S, McInerney-Leo A, Nussbaum R, … Berman KF(2005). Midbrain dopamine and prefrontal function in humans: Interaction and modulation by COMT genotype. Nature Neuroscience, 8(5), 594–596. 10.1038/nn1438 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Mozzi A, Riva V, Forni D, Sironi M, Marino C, Molteni M, … Mascheretti S (2017). A common genetic variant in FOXP2 is associated with language-based learning (dis)abilities: Evidence from two Italian independent samples. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part B: Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 174(5), 578–586. 10.1002/ajmg.b.32546 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Newbury DF, Fisher SE, & Monaco AP (2010). Recent advances in the genetics of language impairment. Genome Medicine, 2, 6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Newbury DF, Monaco A, & Paracchini S (2014). Reading and language disorders: The importance of both quantity and quality. Genes, 5(2), 285–309. 10.3390/genes5020285 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Newbury DF, Paracchini S, Scerri TS, Winchester L, Addis L, Richardson AJ, … Monaco AP (2011). Investigation of dyslexia and SLI risk variants in reading- and language-impaired subjects. Behavior Genetics, 41(1), 90–104. 10.1007/s10519-010-9424-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Nopola-Hemmi J, Myllyluoma B, Haltia T, Taipale M, Ollikainen V, Ahonen T, … Widén E (2001). A dominant gene for developmental dyslexia on chromosome 3. Journal of Medical Genetics, 38(10), 658–664. Retrieved from. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11584043 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Norton ES, Beach SD, & Gabrieli JDE (2015). Neurobiology of dyslexia. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 30, 73–78. 10.1016/j.conb.2014.09.007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Numakawa T, Suzuki S, Kumamaru E, Adachi N, Richards M, & Kunugi H (2010). BDNF function and intracellular signaling in neurons. Histology and Histopathology, 25(2), 237–258. 10.14670/HH-25.237 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Ozernov-Palchik O, & Gaab N (2016). Tackling the “dyslexia paradox”: Reading brain and behavior for early markers of developmental dyslexia. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 7(2), 156–176. 10.1002/wcs.1383 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Paracchini S, Ang QW, Stanley FJ, Monaco AP, Pennell CE, & Whitehouse AJO (2011). Analysis of dyslexia candidate genes in the Raine cohort representing the general Australian population. Genes, Brain and Behavior, 10(2), 158–165. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Paracchini S, Steer CD, Buckingham L-L, Morris AP, Ring S, Scerri T, … Monaco AP (2008). Association of the KIAA0319 dyslexia susceptibility gene with reading skills in the general population. American Journal of Psychiatry, 165(12), 1576–1584. 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.07121872 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Pennington BF, & Bishop DVM (2009). Relations among speech, language, and reading disorders. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 283–306. 10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163548 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Perdue MV, Mascheretti S, Kornilov SA, Jasińska KK, Ryherd K Mencl WE … Landi N (2019). Common variation within the SETBP1 gene is associated with reading-related skills and patterns of functional neural activation. Neuropsychologia, 130, 44–51. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Peschansky VJ, Burbridge TJ, Volz AJ, Fiondella C, Wissner-Gross Z, Galaburda AM, … Rosen GD (2010). The effect of variation in expression of the candidate dyslexia susceptibility gene homolog Kiaa0319 on neuronal migration and dendritic morphology in the rat. Cerebral Cortex, 20(4), 884–897. 10.1093/cercor/bhp154 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Pinel P, Fauchereau F, Moreno A, Barbot A, Lathrop M, Zelenika D, … Dehaene S (2012). Genetic variants of FOXP2 and KIAA0319/TTRAP/THEM2 locus are associated with altered brain activation in distinct language-related regions. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 32(3), 817–825. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5996-10.2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Plomin R, DeFries JC, McClearn GE, & McGuffin P (2007). Behavioral genetics (fifth edit). New York: Worth Publishers. [Google Scholar]
  95. Poliak S, Salomon D, Elhanany H, Sabanay H, Kiernan B, Pevny L, … Peles E (2003). Juxtaparanodal clustering of Shaker-like K+ channels in myelinated axons depends on Caspr2 and TAG-1. The Journal of Cell Biology, 162(6). Retrieved from). http://jcb.rupress.org/content/162/6/1149 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Poon M-W, Tsang W-H, Chan S-O, Li H-M, Ng H-K, & Waye MM-Y (2011). Dyslexia-associated Kiaa0319-like protein interacts with axon guidance receptor Nogo Receptor 1. Cellular and Molecular Neurobiology, 31(1), 27–35. 10.1007/s10571-010-9549-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Powers NR, Eicher JD, Butter F, Kong Y, Miller LL, Ring SM, … Gruen JR (2013). Alleles of a polymorphic ETV6 binding site in DCDC2 confer risk of reading and language impairment. The American Journal of Human Genetics, 93(1), 19–28. 10.1016/j.ajhg.2013.05.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Pugh KR, Frost SJ, Rothman DL, Hoeft F, Del Tufo SN, Mason GF, … Preston JL (2014). Glutamate and choline levels predict individual differences in reading ability in emergent readers. Journal of Neuroscience, 34(11), 4082–4089. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Pugh KR, Mencl WE, Jenner AR, Katz L, Frost SJ, Lee JR, … Shaywitz BA (2001). Neurobiological studies of reading and reading disability. Journal of Communication Disorders, 34(6), 479–492. 10.1016/S0021-9924(01)00060-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Rasband MN (2004). It’s “juxta” potassium channel! Journal of Neuroscience Research, 76(6), 749–757. 10.1002/jnr.20073 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Reich DE, & Lander ES (2001). On the allelic spectrum of human disease. Trends in Genetics, 17(9), 502–510. Retrieved from. http://ac.els-cdn.com/S0168952501024106/1-s2.0-S0168952501024106-main.pdf?_tid=c63078d0-0e5a-11e7-b882-00000aacb35f&acdnat=1490117049_b00202deec633a7af5598534961dc938 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Richlan F, Kronbichler M, & Wimmer H (2009). Functional abnormalities in the dyslexic brain: A quantitative meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Human Brain Mapping, 30(10), 3299–3308. 10.1002/hbm.20752 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Richlan F, Kronbichler M, & Wimmer H (2013). Structural abnormalities in the dyslexic brain: A meta-analysis of voxel-based morphometry studies. Human Brain Mapping, 34(11), 3055–3065. 10.1002/hbm.22127 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Risch N, & Merikangas K (1996). The future of genetic studies of complex human diseases. Science, New Series, 273(13), 1516–1517. Retrieved from. http://links.jstor.org/sici?sici=0036-8075%2819960913%293%3A273%3A5281%3C1516%3ATFOGSO%3E2.0.CO%3B2-K [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Riva V, Cantiani C, Benasich AA, Molteni M, Piazza C, Giorda R, … Marino C (2018). From CNTNAP2 to early expressive language in infancy: The mediation role of rapid auditory processing. Cerebral Cortex, 28(6), 2100–2108. 10.1093/cercor/bhx115 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Roeske D, Ludwig K, Neuhoff N, Becker J, Bartling J, Bruder J, … Schulte-Körne G (2011). First genome-wide association scan on neurophysiological endophenotypes points to trans-regulation effects on SLC2A3 in dyslexic children. Molecular Psychiatry, 16, 97–107. 10.1038/mp.2009.102 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Rosen GD, Bai J, Wang Y, Fiondella CG, Threlkeld SW, LoTurco JJ, & Galaburda AM (2007). Disruption of neuronal migration by RNAi of Dyx1c1 results in neocortical and hippocampal malformations. Cerebral Cortex, 17(11), 2562–2572. 10.1093/cercor/bhl162 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Scerri TS, Macpherson E, Martinelli A, Wa WC, Monaco AP, Stein J, … Paracchini S (2017). The DCDC2 deletion is not a risk factor for dyslexia. Translational Psychiatry, 7(7), e1182 10.1038/tp.2017.151 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. Scerri TS, Darki F, Newbury DF, Whitehouse AJO, Peyrard-Janvid M, Matsson H, … Nichols T (2012). The dyslexia candidate locus on 2p12 is associated with general cognitive ability and white matter structure. PLoS ONE, 7(11), e50321 10.1371/journal.pone.0050321 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. Scerri TS, Morris AP, Buckingham L-L, Newbury DF, Miller LL, Monaco AP, … Paracchini S (2011). DCDC2, KIAA0319 and CMIP are associated with reading-related traits. BPS, 70, 237–245. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.02.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. Schork NJ, Murray SS, Frazer KA, & Topol EJ (2009). Common vs. rare allele hypotheses for complex diseases. Current Opinion in Genetics & Development, 19, 212–219. 10.1016/j.gde.2009.04.010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. Schulte-Körne G, Deimel W, Bartling J, & Remschmidt H (2001). Speech perception deficit in dyslexic adults as measured by mismatch negativity (MMN). International Journal of Psychophysiology, 40(1), 77–87. 10.1016/S0167-8760(00)00152-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  113. Sherry ST, Ward MH, Kholodov M, Baker J, Phan L, Smigielski EM, & Sirotkin K (2001). dbSNP: the NCBI database of genetic variation. Nucleic acids research, 29(1), 308–311. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. Skeide MA, Kirsten H, Kraft I, Schaadt G, Müller B, Neef N, … Friederici AD (2015). Genetic dyslexia risk variant is related to neural connectivity patterns underlying phonological awareness in children. NeuroImage, 118, 414–421. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.06.024 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Skeide MA, Kraft I, Müller B, Schaadt G, Neef NE, Brauer J, … Friederici AD (2016). NRSN1 associated grey matter volume of the visual word form area reveals dyslexia before school. Brain, 139(10), 2792–2803. 10.1093/brain/aww153 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Smith CAB (1953). The detection of linkage in human genetics. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series B: Methodological, 15(2), 153–192. Retrieved from. http://www.jstor.org/stable/2983767 [Google Scholar]
  117. Stein CM, Schick JH, Gerry Taylor H, Shriberg LD, Millard C, Kundtz-Kluge A, … Iyengar SK (2004). Pleiotropic effects of a chromosome 3 locus on speech-sound disorder and reading. The American Journal of Human Genetics, 74(2), 283–297. 10.1086/381562 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  118. Strauss KA, Puffenberger EG, Huentelman MJ, Gottlieb S, Dobrin SE, Parod JM, … Morton DH (2006). Recessive symptomatic focal epilepsy and mutant contactin-associated protein-like 2. New England Journal of Medicine, 354(13), 1370–1377. 10.1056/NEJMoa052773 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  119. Sun Y, Gao Y, Zhou Y, Chen H, Wang G, Xu J, … Tan LH (2014). Association study of developmental dyslexia candidate genes DCDC2 and KIAA0319 in Chinese population. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part B: Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 165(8), 627–634. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  120. Szalkowski CE, Fiondella CF, Truong DT, Rosen GD, LoTurco JJ, & Fitch RH (2013). The effects of Kiaa0319 knockdown on cortical and subcortical anatomy in male rats. International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience, 31(2), 116–122. 10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2012.11.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  121. Szalkowski CE, Fiondella CG, Galaburda AM, Rosen GD, LoTurco JJ, & Fitch RH (2012). Neocortical disruption and behavioral impairments in rats following in utero RNAi of candidate dyslexia risk gene Kiaa0319. International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience, 30(4), 293–302. 10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2012.01.009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  122. Szalkowski CE, Hinman JR, Threlkeld SW, Wang Y, LePack A, Rosen GD, … Fitch RH (2011). Persistent spatial working memory deficits in rats following in utero RNAi of Dyx1c1. Genes, Brain and Behavior, 10(2), 244–252. 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2010.00662.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  123. Tammimies K, Bieder A, Lauter G, Sugiaman-Trapman D, Torchet R, Hokkanen M-E, … Swoboda P (2016). Ciliary dyslexia candidate genes DYX1C1 and DCDC2 are regulated by Regulatory Factor X (RFX) transcription factors through X-box promoter motifs. The FASEB Journal, 30(10), 3578–3587. 10.1096/fj.201500124RR [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  124. Tan GCY, Doke TF, Ashburner J, Wood NW, & Frackowiak RSJ (2010). Normal variation in fronto-occipital circuitry and cerebellar structure with an autism-associated polymorphism of CNTNAP2. NeuroImage, 53(3), 1030–1042. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.02.018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  125. Tarkar A, Loges NT, Slagle CE, Francis R, Dougherty GW, Tamayo JV, … Omran H (2013). DYX1C1 is required for axonemal dynein assembly and ciliary motility. Nature Genetics, 45(9), 995–1003. 10.1038/ng.2707 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  126. The 1000 Genomes Project Consortium (2011). A map of human genome variation from population-scale sequencing. Nature, 467, 1061–1073. 10.1038/nature09534 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  127. The International SNP Map Working Group (2001). A map of human genome sequence variation containing 1.42 million single nucleotide polymorphisms. Nature, 409, 928–933. 10.1038/35057149 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  128. Threlkeld SW, McClure MM, Bai J, Wang Y, LoTurco JJ, Rosen GD, & Fitch RH (2007). Developmental disruptions and behavioral impairments in rats following in utero RNAi of Dyx1c1. Brain Research Bulletin, 71(5), 508–514. 10.1016/J.BRAINRESBULL.2006.11.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  129. Tran C, Wigg KG, Zhang K, Cate-Carter TD, Kerr E, Field LL, … Barr CL (2014). Association of the ROBO1 gene with reading disabilities in a family-based analysis. Genes, Brain and Behavior, 13(4), 430–438. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  130. Uddén J, Snijders TM, Fisher SE, & Hagoort P (2017). A common variant of the CNTNAP2 gene is associated with structural variation in the left superior occipital gyrus. Brain and Language, 172, 16–21. 10.1016/j.bandl.2016.02.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  131. Vandermosten M, Boets B, Wouters J, & Ghesquière P (2012). A qualitative and quantitative review of diffusion tensor imaging studies in reading and dyslexia. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 36(6), 1532–1552. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.04.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  132. Vargha-Khadem F, Watkins KE, Price CJ, Ashburner J, Alcock KJ, Connelly A, … Passingham RE (1998). Neural basis of an inherited speech and language disorder. Neurobiology, 95, 12695–12700. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  133. Vargha-Khadem F, Gadian DG, Copp A, & Mishkin M (2005). FOXP2 and the neuroanatomy of speech and language. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6(2), 131–138. 10.1038/nrn1605 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  134. Velayos-Baeza A, Levecque C, Kobayashi K, Holloway ZG, & Monaco AP (2010). The dyslexia-associated KIAA0319 protein undergoes proteolytic processing with γ-secretase-independent intramembrane cleavage. The Journal of Biological Chemistry, 285(51), 40148–40162. 10.1074/jbc.m110.145961 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  135. Venkatesh SK, Siddaiah A, Padakannaya P, & Ramachandra NB (2013). Analysis of genetic variants of dyslexia candidate genes KIAA0319 and DCDC2 in Indian population. Journal of human genetics, 58(8), 531. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  136. Venkatesh SK, Siddaiah A, Padakannaya P, & Ramachandra NB (2014). Association of SNPs of DYX1C1 with developmental dyslexia in an Indian population. Psychiatric Genetics, 24(1), 10–20. 10.1097/YPG.0000000000000009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  137. Vernes SC, Newbury DF, Abrahams BS, Winchester L, Nicod J, Groszer M, … Fisher SE (2008). A functional genetic link between distinct developmental language disorders. New England Journal of Medicine, 359(22), 2337–2345. 10.1056/NEJMoa0802828 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  138. Wang Y, Paramasivam M, Thomas A, Bai J, Kaminen-Ahola N, Kere J, … LoTurco JJ (2006). DYX1C1 functions in neuronal migration in developing neocortex. Neuroscience, 143, 515–522. Retrieved from. http://ac.els-cdn.com/S0306452206010918/1-s2.0-S0306452206010918-main.pdf?_tid=aac0d08a-1581-11e7-b37a-00000aacb361&acdnat=1490903412_e7cbb26a510030d1e3b94cf5cd72cd17 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  139. Watkins KE, Vargha-Khadem F, Ashburner J, Passingham RE, Connelly A, Friston KJ, … Watkins K (2002). MRI analysis of an inherited speech and language disorder: Structural brain abnormalities. Brain, 125, 465–478. 10.1093/brain/awf057 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  140. Weber JL, David D, Heil J, Fan Y, Zhao C, & Marth G (2002). Human diallelic insertion/deletion polymorphisms. The American Journal of Human Genetics, 71(4), 854–862. 10.1086/342727 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  141. Whalley HC, O’Connell G, Sussmann JE, Peel A, Stanfield AC, Hayiou-Thomas ME, … Hall J (2011). Genetic variation in CNTNAP2 alters brain function during linguistic processing in healthy individuals. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part B: Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 156(8), 941–948. 10.1002/ajmg.b.31241 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  142. Whitford KL, Marillat V, Stein E, Goodman CS, Tessier-Lavigne M, Chédotal A, & Ghosh A (2002). Regulation of cortical dendrite development by Slit-Robo interactions. Neuron, 33(1), 47–61. 10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00566-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  143. Wilcke A, Ligges C, Burkhardt J, Alexander M, Wolf C, Quente E, … Kirsten H (2012). Imaging genetics of FOXP2 in dyslexia. European Journal of Human Genetics, 20(10), 224–229. 10.1038/ejhg.2011.160 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  144. Wilcke A, Weissfuss J, Kirsten H, Wolfram G, Boltze J, & Ahnert P (2009). The role of gene DCDC2 in German dyslexics. Annals of Dyslexia, 59(1), 1–11. 10.1007/s11881-008-0020-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  145. Yeatman JD, Dougherty RF, Rykhlevskaia E, Sherbondy AJ, Deutsch GK, Wandell BA, & Ben-Shachar M (2011). Anatomical properties of the arcuate fasciculus predict phonological and reading skills in children. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(11), 3304–3317. 10.1162/jocn_a_00061 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  146. Zhang Y, Li J, Song S, Tardif T, Burmeister M, Villafuerte SM, … Shu H (2016). Association of DCDC2 polymorphisms with normal variations in reading abilities in a Chinese population. PLoS One, 11(4), e0153603. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  147. Zhang Y, Li J, Tardif T, Burmeister M, Villafuerte SM, McBride-Chang C, … Shu H (2012). Association of the DYX1C1 dyslexia susceptibility gene with orthography in the Chinese population. PLoS ONE, 7(9), e42969 10.1371/journal.pone.0042969 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Appendix A

RESOURCES