Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2019 Dec 17;14(12):e0226405. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0226405

Acute kidney injury in Ugandan children with severe malaria is associated with long-term behavioral problems

Meredith R Hickson 1,#, Andrea L Conroy 2,*,#, Paul Bangirana 3, Robert O Opoka 4, Richard Idro 4,5, John M Ssenkusu 6, Chandy C John 2
Editor: Alister G Craig7
PMCID: PMC6917349  PMID: 31846479

Abstract

Background

Acute kidney injury (AKI) is a risk factor for neurocognitive impairment in severe malaria (SM), but the impact of AKI on long-term behavioral outcomes following SM is unknown.

Methods

We conducted a prospective study on behavioral outcomes of Ugandan children 1.5 to 12 years of age with two forms of severe malaria, cerebral malaria (CM, n = 226) or severe malarial anemia (SMA, n = 214), and healthy community children (CC, n = 173). AKI was defined as a 50% increase in creatinine from estimated baseline. Behavior and executive function were assessed at baseline and 6, 12, and 24 months later using the Child Behavior Checklist and Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function, respectively. Age-adjusted z-scores were computed for each domain based on CC scores. The association between AKI and behavioral outcomes was evaluated across all time points using linear mixed effect models, adjusting for sociodemographic variables and disease severity.

Results

AKI was present in 33.2% of children with CM or SMA at baseline. Children ≥6 years of age with CM or SMA who had AKI on admission had worse scores in socio-emotional function in externalizing behaviors (Beta (95% CI), 0.52 (0.20, 0.85), p = 0.001), global executive function (0.48 (0.15, 0.82), p = 0.005) and behavioral regulation (0.66 (0.32, 1.01), p = 0.0002) than children without AKI. There were no behavioral differences associated with AKI in children <6 years of age.

Conclusions

AKI is associated with long-term behavioral problems in children ≥6 years of age with CM or SMA, irrespective of age at study enrollment.

Introduction

More than 200 million children worldwide are at risk of failing to meet their developmental potential [1]. Malaria due to Plasmodium falciparum is a major cause of mortality and neurodevelopmental morbidity in sub-Saharan Africa—the region with the highest percentage of children at increased risk for developmental delay [14]. Severe malaria (SM) is associated with long-term impairment in multiple domains, including cognition, memory, attention, and behavior [4, 5]. Both parasite and host factors appear to contribute to the pathophysiology of SM [6]; however the mechanisms by which SM acts on the developing brain are not well understood.

Acute kidney injury (AKI) is a common, life-threatening complication of SM in both children and adults [713]. Recent studies in children have shown an association between AKI and neurodisability at discharge [7] and long-term neurocognitive impairment [14]. The limited availability of renal replacement therapy in malaria-endemic countries increases the risk of mortality in SM patients who present with AKI [15, 16]. This association between AKI and cognition in children with both CM and SMA may help to explain the broader impacts of SM on neurodevelopment. The effects of AKI on long-term behavioral problems in children is less well-defined, and we are unaware of any studies addressing the association of AKI with long-term behavioral disorders in any disease process.

Given the lack of data on AKI and long-term behavioral problems in children with SM, we conducted a long-term longitudinal cohort study, in which we assessed whether AKI is associated with long-term effects on behavioral outcomes in Ugandan children with SM.

Materials and methods

Participant enrollment

The study enrolled children between 2008 and 2013 at Mulago National Referral Hospital in Kampala, Uganda [4]. We enrolled all eligible children between the ages of 18 months and 12 years who presented to Mulago with two forms of severe malaria, cerebral malaria (CM) or severe malarial anemia (SMA). Inclusion criteria for cerebral malaria (CM) were 1) coma (Blantyre Coma Scale ≤2, Glasgow Coma Scale ≤8) with 2) P. falciparum on blood smear, and without 3) another identifiable cause of coma (white blood cells<10; negative lumbar puncture gram stain and culture; if hypoglycemic, no response to glucose bolus after 1 hour) [4, 17]. Inclusion criteria for severe malarial anemia (SMA) were 1) P. falciparum on blood smear, with 2) serum hemoglobin ≤5g/dL. Children with both CM and SMA were classified as CM. We recruited healthy community children (CC) from the same nuclear family or extended family household as a participant with CM or SMA, who were between the ages of 18 months and 12 years, and within 1 year of age of the household participant with CM or SMA.

We excluded children who lived further than 50km from Mulago and children for whom the primary caregiver reported: 1) chronic illness requiring medical care, 2) history of developmental delay, 3) history of coma (of any etiology, reported by the child’s caregiver), head trauma, or cerebral palsy, or 4) history of hospitalization for malnutrition. Additional exclusion criteria for SM without coma were: 1) impaired consciousness on exam, 2) any seizure activity prior to presentation, or 3) any other clinical evidence of central nervous system disease. Additional exclusion criteria for CC were: 1) illness requiring medical care within the previous 4 weeks or 2) major medical or neurological abnormality on screening exam. Due to the high prevalence of asymptomatic malaria in Ugandan children, we treated CC with a blood smear positive for Plasmodium species and did not exclude them from the study [18].

Initial clinical assessment and treatment

At enrollment, all children underwent a physical examination inclusive of the cardiovascular, pulmonary, gastrointestinal, and nervous systems. Study clinical officers collected a medical history from the participant’s primary caregiver (parent or guardian). Nutritional status was evaluated with weight- and height-for-age z-scores for all children during the initial physical exam using 2007 WHO reference values (Epi Info v. 3.5.3, CDC, Atlanta GA). Per the Ugandan Ministry of Health treatment guidelines in effect at the time, children with SM received IV quinine followed by oral quinine during the initial admission and/or parenteral artemether, and oral artemisinin combination therapy after discharge. In 2012, the treatment guidelines were updated recommending intravenous artesunate as the first-line therapy for severe malaria, and intravenous artesunate was introduced in early 2013.

Assessment of renal function

Creatinine was tested on cryopreserved plasma samples using a Beckman Coulter AU680 using the modified Jaffe method (Indiana University, Pathology Laboratory). AKI was defined using the Kidney Disease: Improving Global Outcomes (KDIGO) guidelines based on a single admission sample with premorbid creatinine estimated using the CC as previously described [14]. Briefly, we constructed a creatinine for height curve using the CC to generate a population-specific estimate of baseline creatinine and used the child’s height on admission to estimate baseline creatinine using the Bedside Schwartz equation [14]. AKI was defined as a 1.5-fold increase in creatinine from baseline [19].

Assessment of socioeconomic status and home environment

To determine participants’ socioeconomic status (SES), a scored questionnaire was used at enrollment as previously described [20]. SES assessed using this is related to cognitive functioning in healthy Ugandan children [20]. In addition, levels of cognitive stimulation and emotional support provided by caregivers was assessed using the Home Observation of the Environment (HOME) questionnaire [21].

Behavioral assessment

We selected 2 behavioral domains for inclusion in this study based on our group’s work on SM [4], and prior studies of neurodevelopment in kidney disease [22]. Behavioral assessments were conducted according to standard operating procedures by trained research assistants with a Bachelor’s degrees in psychology. Testing was subjected to a continuous quality control program by a quality control officer that provided feedback on tests to ensure standardization. Children <6 years were tested with preschool instruments and children 6 years or older were tested with school-aged instruments. We evaluated socio-emotional function with the preschool (18 months-5 years, 11 months) and school-aged (6–12 years) Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) [23], and executive function with the preschool (2–6 years) and school-aged (6–12 years) Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF) [24]. Higher z-scores on these tools indicate poorer performance and more problematic behavior. Children with SM were evaluated one week after hospital discharge and CC were evaluated at enrollment. We repeated testing at 6, 12 months, and 24 months follow-up for all participants. Children who turned 6 during the study were therefore tested with the preschool instruments initially and then with the school-aged instruments. Primary outcomes included internalizing and externalizing behavior (CBCL) and executive function and emergent metacognition (BRIEF) for all ages; and for children <6 the BRIEF-P flexibility index and inhibitory self-control index, and for children ≥6, the BRIEF behavioral regulation index. Secondary outcomes were the CBCL sub-scales for internalizing and externalizing behavior and the BRIEF sub-scales.

Statistical analysis

All analyses were performed with Stata/SE 14.2 (StataCorp LP). We compared baseline demographic and clinical characteristics based on AKI status using Student’s t-test for continuous measures, and Pearson’s χ2 test or Fisher’s exact test for categorical variables as appropriate. Age-adjusted z-scores for the HOME and all behavioral outcomes were created based on the scores from CC as previously described [4]. Linear mixed effects models were used to compare longitudinal behavioral outcomes among children with SM based on AKI status. All linear mixed models were fitted with a subject specific random intercept and visit as a categorical variable to allow for non-linearity between visits. Behavioral models for children <6 years included a caretaker random effect. In all models, we controlled for covariates selected a priori for their reported or hypothesized impact on neurodevelopment: age, sex, weight-for-age z-score, height-for-age z-score, SES total score, HOME z-score, maternal education level, preschool exposure, presence of coma, number of seizures during hospitalization, study year of enrollment, and parenteral anti-malarial therapy (quinine vs. artemisinin), and test administrator [35, 25]. Using Holm’s procedure, we adjusted for the number of comparisons within each age group (<6 years and ≥6 years) for primary outcomes (children <6 years, n = 6; children ≥6 years, n = 5) and secondary outcomes (children <6 years, n = 12; children ≥6 years, n = 14).

Ethics and consent

Written informed consent was obtained from the primary caregivers (parents or guardians), and assent in children older than 8 years of age whose primary caregivers consented to participation. This study was approved by the Uganda National Council for Science and Technology, and Institutional Review Boards from Makerere University (School of Medicine Research and Ethics Committee), and the University of Minnesota.

Results

A total of 440 children with SM (CM, n = 226, or SMA, n = 214) and 173 CC were included in our analysis (Fig 1, Table 1). The prevalence of AKI in the study cohort was 35.1% overall [14] and 33.2% among surviving children with baseline behavioral testing. Overall children with SM were comparable in age to the CC with a mean age (SD) of 4.00 (2.03) years in CC compared to 3.71 (1.84) years of age in children with SM. A lower proportion of children with SM were female compared to CC (40.2% vs. 54.3% respectively, p = 0.002). At enrollment, 51 (29.5%) of the CC had asymptomatic malaria detected by PCR. The presence of asymptomatic parasitemia in the CC was not associated with differences in our primary behavioral outcomes (S1 Table).

Fig 1. Flow chart of the study population.

Fig 1

Abbreviations, acute kidney injury (AKI), lost-to-follow-up (LTFU), child behavior checklist (CBCL), Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF).

Table 1. Baseline characteristics of children with severe malaria by AKI status.

  Community children
N = 173
Severe Malaria
No AKI (n = 294) AKI (n = 146) P value
No AKI vs. AKI
Age in years 4.00 ± 2.03 3.74 ± 1.84 3.65 ± 1.84 0.64
Duration of illness, days N/A 4.1 ± 3.0 3.5 ± 1.9 0.03
Female sex, No. (%) 94 (54.3) 119 (40.5) 58 (39.7) 0.88
Weight-for-age z-score -0.95 ± 0.98 -1.23 ± 1.11 -1.40 ± 1.00 0.13
Height-for-age z-score -1.05 ±1.14 -1.15 ± 1.46 -0.86 ± 1.35 0.037
Weight-for-height z-score -0.34 ± 1.04 -0.87 ± 1.34 -0.89 ± 1.25 0.86
Socioeconomic status (SES) 9.92 ± 3.03 9.90 ± 3.23 8.92 ± 2.89 0.003
Home environment z score 0.11 ± 1.03 0.08 ± 1.00 -0.20 ± 1.01 0.005
Maternal education level, No. (%)
    Primary 6 or lower 47 (27.2) 108 (36.7) 60 (41.1) 0.34
    Primary 7 42 (24.3) 61 (20.8) 31 (21.2)
    Secondary or higher 74 (42.8) 116 (39.5) 47 (32.2)
    Not known 10 (5.8) 9 (3.1) 8 (5.5)
Paternal education level, No. (%)
    Primary 6 or lower 24 (23.9) 63 (21.4) 25 (17.1) 0.54
    Primary 7 33 (19.1) 43 (14.6) 28 (19.2)
    Secondary or higher 90 (52.0) 131 (44.6) 65 (44.5)
    Not known 26 (15.0) 57 (19.4) 28 (19.2)
Preschool education, No. (%) 72 (41.9) 102 (35.3) 39 (27.1) 0.09
HIV positive at baseline, No. (%) 3 (1.2) 6 (2.1) 5 (3.5) 0.28
Hemoglobin SS genotype, No. (%) 0 (0.0) 16 (5.4) 4 (2.8) 0.10
Hypotensive on admissiona, No. (%) --- 0 (0.0) 1 (0.7) 0.16
Hypoglycemia on admission, No. (%) --- 12 (4.1) 8 (5.5) 0.28
Severe anemia on admission, No. (%) --- 178 (60.5) 86 (58.9) 0.74
Coma on admission, No. (%) --- 134 (45.6) 92 (63.0) <0.001
No. seizures during hospitalization --- 0.38 ± 1.00 1.11 ± 2.51 <0.001

Abbreviations: N/A, not applicable; P, P-value; HIV, Human Immunodeficiency Virus; AKI, acute kidney injury

Data presented as mean ± standard deviation or median (interquartile range)

Continuous measures compared with Student’s t-test (mean, standard deviation) or Wilcoxon Rank sum test (median, interquartile range). Categorical measures compared with Pearson’s Χ2 or Fisher’s exact test (for any comparison in which any cell count <5).

aHypotension defined as systolic blood pressure < 50 mmHg children <5 years, <70 mmHg children <12 years

Table 1 summarizes the baseline demographic and clinical characteristics of children with severe malaria, with and without AKI on enrollment. Children with AKI had higher mean height-for-age z-scores (p = 0.037), lower socioeconomic status (p = 0.003), and lower mean home environment quality z scores (p = 0.005) than children without AKI. Children with AKI reported a shorter duration of fever prior presentation suggesting that AKI was not related to prolonged duration of illness (p = 0.03). Children with AKI were also more likely to be in coma (i.e., have CM, p<0.001) and had a higher mean number of seizures during admission (p<0.001) than children without AKI. Most children presented with a BUN-to-creatinine ratio suggestive of prerenal injury with 92.2% of children without AKI and 95.2% of children with AKI having a BUN-to-creatinine ratio >20. Repeated creatinine measurements were not available for the children, and creatinine was measured retrospectively so AKI status was not known at the time of hospitalization.

Longitudinal behavioral outcomes among children with severe malaria and acute kidney injury

Children ≥6 years with AKI exhibited poorer externalizing behavior than those without AKI (Beta (95% CI), 0.52 (0.20, 0.85), p = 0.001) (Table 2), characterized by more aggression (Beta (95% CI), 0.49 (0.18, 0.81), p = 0.001) (Table 3). The executive functioning (global executive composite) of children ≥6 years with AKI was also worse compared with those without AKI (Beta (95% CI), 0.48 (0.15, 0.82), p = 0.005) (Table 2). Their caregivers reported poorer behavioral regulation (Beta (95% CI), 0.66 (0.32, 1.01), p = 0.0002), as evidenced by poorer impulse control (Beta (95% CI), 0.64 (0.30, 0.97), p = 0.0002) and emotional control (Beta (95% CI), 0.60 (0.28, 0.93), p = 0.0003) (Table 3). All models adjusted for child age, sex, nutritional status (height-for-age and weight-for-age z scores), socioeconomic status, enrichment in the home environment, preschool exposure, neurological complications including coma on admission and number of seizures during hospitalization, parenteral antimalarial treatment (quinine vs. artemisinin), year of enrollment and test administrator.

Table 2. Primary behavioral outcomes for children with severe malaria by acute kidney injury (AKI) status.

Unadjusted Estimates Adjusted Estimates
N (obs.), N Coefficient (95% CI) P N (obs.), N Coefficient (95% CI) P
Children aged <6 years
Socio-emotional functiona
    Internalizing behavior 1296, 379 0.09 (-0.07, 0.26) 0.28 1276, 370 -0.08 (-0.25, 0.07) 0.27
    Externalizing behavior 1296, 379 0.19 (0.02, 0.37) 0.03 1276, 370 0.06 (-0.12, 0.24) 0.49
Executive functionb
    Global Executive Composite 726, 298 0.41 (0.13, 0.69) 0.004 571, 283 0.04 (-0.24, 0.32) 0.76
    Emergent Metacognition Index 726, 298 0.48 (0.20, 0.76) 0.0008 571, 283 0.14 (-0.14, 0.43) 0.32
    Flexibility Index 726, 298 0.24 (0.003, 0.48) 0.05 571, 283 -0.06 (-0.30, 0.17) 0.60
    Inhibitory Self-Control Index 726, 298 0.27 (0.005, 0.54) 0.05 571, 283 -0.08 (-0.37, 0.21) 0.58
Children aged ≥6 years
Socio-emotional functiona
    Internalizing behavior 358, 150 0.11 (-0.12, 0.34) 0.35 357, 149 -0.01 (-0.23, 0.22) 0.95
    Externalizing behavior 358, 150 0.58 (0.25, 0.90) 0.0006 357, 149 0.52 (0.20, 0.85) 0.002
Executive functionb
    Global Executive Composite 251, 137 0.43 (0.09, 0.77) 0.01 251, 137 0.48 (0.15, 0.82) 0.005
    Emergent Metacognition Index 251, 137 0.30 (-0.04, 0.65) 0.08 251, 137 0.27 (-0.08, 0.62) 0.13
    Behavior Regulation Index 251, 137 0.52 (0.17, 0.88) 0.004 251, 137 0.66 (0.32, 1.01) 0.0002

Abbreviations: CI, confidence interval; P, P-value; N (obs.), number of observations in the model; N, the number of children in the analysis.

aAssessed using the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL)

bAssessed using the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF)

All linear mixed models were fitted with a subject specific random intercept and a caretaker random effect (for children <6 years of age) and visit as a categorical variable (baseline, 6 months, 12 months, 24 months).

Adjusted models included age, sex, height-for-age, weight-for-age, socioeconomic status, home environment, maternal education, preschool exposure, presence of coma on admission, number of seizures during hospitalization, parenteral antimalarial treatment (quinine vs. artemisinin), year of enrollment and test administrator as fixed effects.

† Significant following Holm’s adjustment for multiple comparisons within each age group (children <6, n = 6; children ≥6, n = 5).

Table 3. Secondary socio-emotional outcomes for children ≥6 years with severe malaria by acute kidney injury (AKI) status.

Unadjusted models Adjusted Models
N (obs.), N Coefficient (95% CI) P N (obs.), N Coefficient (95% CI) P
Socio-emotional functiona
Internalizing scales
    Anxious/depressed 358, 150 0.06 (-0.19, 0.31) 0.64 357, 149 0.01 (-0.24, 0.27) 0.93
    Withdraw/depressed 358, 150 0.16 (-0.11, 0.44) 0.25 357, 149 0.05 (-0.22, 0.32) 0.71
    Somatic complaints 358, 150 -0.03 (-0.26, 0.19) 0.77 357, 149 -0.07 (-0.30, 0.16) 0.55
Externalizing scales
    Aggressive behavior 358, 150 0.54 (0.21, 0.86) 0.001 357, 149 0.49 (0.18, 0.81) 0.003
    Rule breaking 358, 150 0.38 (0.11, 0.66) 0.007 357, 149 0.32 (0.03, 0.61) 0.03
Social problems 358, 150 0.45 90.12, 0.78) 0.007 357, 149 0.32 (-0.02, 0.65) 0.06
Thought problems 358, 150 0.14 (-0.13, 0.41) 0.31 357, 149 0.07 (-0.21, 0.36) 0.61
Attention problems 358, 150 0.20 (-0.19, 0.59) 0.32 357, 149 0.16 (-0.22, 0.53) 0.41
Executive functionb
Behavior Regulation Index
    Inhibit 251, 137 0.50 (0.17, 0.83) 0.003 251, 137 0.64 (0.30, 0.97) 0.0002
    Task shifting 251, 137 0.20 (-0.14, 0.54) 0.25 251, 137 0.31 (-0.03, 0.65) 0.07
    Emotional control 251, 137 0.52 (0.19, 0.84) 0.002 251, 137 0.60 (0.28, 0.93) 0.0003
Metacognition Index
    Initiate 251, 137 0.27 (-0.05, 0.59) 0.10 251, 137 0.31 (-0.01, 0.62) 0.05
    Plan/organize 251, 137 0.10 (-0.20, 0.40) 0.52 251, 137 0.07 (-0.23, 0.36) 0.66
    Organization of materials 251, 137 0.07 (-0.28, 0.42) 0.69 251, 137 -0.06 (-0.43, 0.31) 0.74
    Monitor 251, 137 0.38 (0.06, 0.71) 0.02 251, 137 0.32 (-0.04, 0.69) 0.08

Abbreviations: CI, confidence interval; P, P-value; N (obs.), number of observations in the model; N, the number of children in the analysis.

aAssessed using the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL)

bAssessed using the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF)

All linear mixed models were fitted with a subject specific random intercept and visit as a categorical variable (baseline, 6 months, 12 months, 24 months).

Adjusted models included age, sex, height-for-age, weight-for-age, socioeconomic status, home environment, maternal education, preschool exposure, presence of coma on admission, number of seizures during hospitalization, parenteral antimalarial treatment (quinine vs. artemisinin), year of enrollment and test administrator as fixed effects.

† Significant following Holm’s adjustment for multiple comparisons (n = 14).

To determine whether the relationship between AKI and behavioral differences reflected increased disease severity rather than a relationship between AKI per se, we considered models where we adjusted for disease severity using the Lambaréné Organ Dysfunction Score (LODS) [26, 27], a validated prognostic score reflecting disease severity in malaria. We also considered models where other individual SM complications were included: jaundice, lactic acidosis (Table 4). In each instance, inclusion of the LODS or other SM complications did not affect the relationship between AKI and behavioral differences. We further considered whether differences in fluid resuscitation or transfusion in AKI [28] affected behavioral outcomes, and found no differences in outcomes based on fluid resuscitation or transfusion and the relationship between behavioral differences and AKI remained significant (Table 4).

Table 4. Behavioral outcomes adjusting for severe malaria complications and treatment.

Children ≥6 years
N (obs.), N Coefficient (95% CI) P
Adjusting for disease severity
Socio-emotional functiona
    Internalizing behavior 357, 149 0.01 (-0.21, 0.24) 0.919
    Externalizing behavior 357, 149 0.55 (0.22, 0.87) 0.001
Executive functionb
    Global Executive Composite 251, 137 0.48 (0.14, 0.82) 0.006
    Emergent Metacognition Index 251, 137 0.27 (-0.09, 0.62) 0.139
    Behavior Regulation Index 251, 137 0.67 (0.32, 1.01) 0.0002
Adjusting for acidosis
Socio-emotional functiona
    Internalizing behavior 334, 139 -0.06 (-0.28, 0.16) 0.605
    Externalizing behavior 334, 139 0.51 (0.18, 0.85) 0.003
Executive functionb
    Global Executive Composite 228, 127 0.45 (0.09, 0.81) 0.016
    Emergent Metacognition Index 228, 127 0.23 (-0.15, 0.62) 0.225
    Behavior Regulation Index 228, 127 0.64 (0.27, 1.01) 0.0008
Adjusting for jaundice
Socio-emotional functiona
    Internalizing behavior 357, 149 0.01 (-0.22, 0.23) 0.957
    Externalizing behavior 357, 149 0.51 (0.18, 0.83) 0.003
Executive functionb
    Global Executive Composite 251, 137 0.45 (0.12, 0.79) 0.009
    Emergent Metacognition Index 251, 137 0.23 (-0.12, 0.59) 0.190
    Behavior Regulation Index 251, 137 0.64 (0.30, 0.99) 0.0003
Adjusting for fluid resuscitation and transfusion
Socio-emotional functiona
    Internalizing behavior 357, 149 -0.01 (-0.23, 0.22) 0.957
    Externalizing behavior 357, 149 0.54 (0.22, 0.87) 0.001
Executive functionb
    Global Executive Composite 251, 137 0.48 (0.14, 0.82) 0.006
    Emergent Metacognition Index 251, 137 0.26 (-0.10, 0.62) 0.151
    Behavior Regulation Index 251, 137 0.68 (0.34, 1.02) 0.0001

Abbreviations: CI, confidence interval; P, P-value; N (obs.), number of observations in the model; N, the number of children in the analysis.

aAssessed using the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL)

bAssessed using the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF)

All linear mixed models were fitted with a subject specific random intercept and visit as a categorical variable (baseline, 6 months, 12 months, 24 months).

Adjusted models included age, sex, height-for-age, weight-for-age, socioeconomic status, home environment, maternal education, preschool exposure, number of seizures during hospitalization (in cerebral malaria), parenteral antimalarial treatment (quinine vs. artemisinin), year of enrollment and test administrator as fixed effects. As coma is part of LODS, the presence of coma was not included in the model with LODS.

In contrast to the findings in older children, there were no behavioral differences children <6 years in primary (Table 2) or secondary outcomes (S2 Table) based on AKI status at admission. The longitudinal mixed effects model includes data from all baseline and follow-up assessments, so children who had assessments done at ≥6 years were not necessarily ≥6 years at the time of their severe malaria episode. 63% of the assessments done at ≥6 years were in children who were <6 years at the time of their enrollment SM episode. Effect sizes of differences in externalizing behavior, executive function and behavioral regulation were similar for these children as for those ≥6 years at initial testing (S3 Table).

To determine whether the relationship between AKI and behavior was consistent in children with CM and SMA, we performed a stratified analysis by severe malaria group. The association with diminished executive function and AKI remained significant for children with both cerebral malaria and severe malaria (S4 Table). AKI remained an independent risk factor for externalizing behavior in children 6 years of age with CM.

Discussion

AKI is increasingly being recognized as an important complication in children hospitalized with severe malaria and is an independent risk factor for long-term neurocognitive deficits in survivors [14]. We have previously shown that children with CM or SMA had worse behavioral outcomes compared to CC [25]. In this study, we explored whether the presence of AKI on admission was related to the behavioral differences previously observed. Among children with CM or SMA, AKI was associated with behavioral problems, notably externalizing behavior, poor behavioral regulation, and executive function (global executive composite). This effect was independent of other measures of disease severity and known risk factors for worse behavioral outcomes. The long-term behavioral changes associated with AKI in children with SM were evident in school-age children (≥6 years of age) irrespective of the age at insult.

Our finding that children ≥6 years with AKI in SM exhibited more externalizing (aggressive, rule-breaking) behavior, were more impulsive, and had poorer control over their emotional responses is consistent with our findings that AKI is also associated with neurocognitive impairment in children following SM [7, 14]. Developmentally disabled and otherwise cognitive impaired children struggle more than non-impaired children with behavioral auto-regulation, and can be more prone to behavioral escalation [29, 30]. That we did not find the same behavioral differences in children <6 years with AKI may be due in part to recall bias introduced by our caregiver-report measures. More is expected socially and behaviorally of older children, and undesirable behaviors might have been more frequently recalled by caregivers of children ≥6 years as compared with those <6 years. In addition, the neuro-architecture of infants and toddlers is more plastic than that of school-aged children [31], and thus the younger children in our cohort may have recovered better from the initial developmental insult. However, when stratifying children based their age at enrollment, the effect sizes for differences in externalizing behavior, executive function and behavioral regulation were similar for children <6 years of age as those ≥6 years at initial testing (S3 Table), suggesting that differences associated with AKI were related to the ability to elicit specific behaviors in older children, rather than to AKI-associated behavioral problems occurring only in children with SM ≥6 years of age. We previously reported that both cerebral malaria and severe malarial anemia are associated with poorer developmental outcomes in children <5 years, when compared with CC [25]. The current analysis, however, compares outcomes associated with a specific severe malaria complication among children with severe malaria on enrollment, which may explain the difference in impacted age groups.

We identified developmental impacts that persisted 2 years after SM infection. This persistence may be a function of the pathophysiology of AKI in SM, but should also be contextualized as part of the impacted children’s developmental milieu. Atypically developing children interact differently with caregivers, who, in turn, interact differently with them, potentially leading to further developmental disparities [32]. The families of cognitively impaired children in resource- and education-limited settings like Uganda often experience insufficient support services and negative socio-cultural attitudes that may impair recovery from an acute developmental insult [33, 34].

Although AKI was more common among children with coma (CM) than those without (SMA) in our cohort of children with these two forms of SM, the behavioral changes in children with AKI were significant after controlling for the presence of coma and number of seizures during hospitalization. In addition, the relationship between AKI and behavior remained unchanged in models when adjusting for other SM complications, disease severity, or fluid resuscitation and transfusion (Table 4). AKI may therefore be an independent contributor to the well-documented developmental consequences of SM and may explain, at least in part, why children without neurologic complications on admission are at risk for long-term cognitive and behavioral problems.

Microvascular obstruction and endothelial activation have been implicated in both neurologic manifestations of SM [3537], and the pathogenesis of malaria-associated AKI [37, 38]. AKI has been associated with endothelial activation and blood-brain-barrier integrity in various inflammatory models including sepsis, liver failure, and neurologic disease (reviewed in [39]). Kidney injury itself may lead to systemic inflammation and endothelial activation [39, 40], but can also slow the clearance of inflammatory and neurotoxic metabolites thus exacerbating inflammatory responses [41].

The design of this study enabled us to track neurodevelopment long-term, allowing us to explore what happens after discharge, the primary endpoint for previous studies of AKI and development [7, 42]. Our model took into consideration social and clinical factors that might also have explained the cognitive and behavioral effects we observed. This study benefited from the inclusion of healthy, age-matched community children who were used to derive population-specific kidney function and neurodevelopmental performance curves, allowing us to define impairment in a way that avoided comparisons with children from different social and economic contexts. Additional studies are needed to determine whether long-term neurocognitive and behavior problems associated with AKI are specific to severe malaria or generalizable to other populations with critical illness. Further, as AKI is a risk factor for subsequent development of chronic kidney disease in children [28, 43, 44], follow-up studies are needed to delineate the long-term impact of chronic kidney disease on child development in survivors.

While caregiver-report behavioral measures are subject to recall and reporter bias, both the BRIEF and CBCL were validated against tester/observer-based measures of children’s behavior [23, 24], and have been used successfully in Uganda before [45]. For children <6 years, we included a random caregiver effect to help address reporter bias. Asymptomatic malaria infection, per admission PCR, was present in almost 30% of our community children. Asymptomatic parasitemia was not associated with worse behavioral outcomes (S3 Table), and given the high prevalence of asymptomatic malaria in Ugandan children, we believe that these community children did represent “typical” development for this demographic [18].

Conclusion

Our study demonstrates that AKI is associated with poorer long-term socio-emotional and executive function in children with SM, independent of sociodemographic factors, neurologic risk factors (e.g. presence of coma, number of seizures during hospitalization), and other SM complications. Our findings suggest that prevention or early management of AKI in these children could mitigate the pervasive, long-term developmental consequences associated with SM, and that expanding access to renal replacement therapy may help children both clinically and developmentally. In endemic regions, however, where both acute care and developmental services are extremely limited, eradication remains the most powerful way to protect children from both the acute and chronic consequences of malaria.

Supporting information

S1 Data. AKI Behavior Data Export.

File containing primary data used in the analysis in wide and long format, and an accompanying codebook.

(XLS)

S1 Table. Primary behavioral outcomes for community children based on the presence of malaria by PCR on enrollment.

(DOCX)

S2 Table. Secondary socio-emotional outcomes for children <6 years with severe malaria by acute kidney injury (AKI) status.

(DOCX)

S3 Table. Behavioral outcomes for children ≥6 years with severe malaria by acute kidney injury (AKI) status stratified by age at enrollment.

(DOCX)

S4 Table. Behavioral outcomes for children ≥6 years with severe malaria by acute kidney injury (AKI) status stratified by study group at enrollment.

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

We thank the children and their parents who participated in this study, the study team for their dedicated effort in treating the children and collecting the data.

Data Availability

All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting information files.

Funding Statement

This work was supported by the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (R01NS055349 to CCJ, https://www.ninds.nih.gov/) and the Fogarty International Center (D43 NS078280 to CCJ, https://www.fic.nih.gov/). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1.Lu C, Black MM, Richter LM. Risk of poor development in young children in low-income and middle-income countries: an estimation and analysis at the global, regional, and country level. The Lancet Global Health. 2016;4(12):e916–e22. 10.1016/S2214-109X(16)30266-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.McCoy DC, Peet ED, Ezzati M, Danaei G, Black MM, Sudfeld CR, et al. Early Childhood Developmental Status in Low- and Middle-Income Countries: National, Regional, and Global Prevalence Estimates Using Predictive Modeling. PLoS Med. 2016;13(6):e1002034 10.1371/journal.pmed.1002034 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Walker SP, Wachs TD, Gardner JM, Lozoff B, Wasserman GA, Pollitt E, et al. Child development: risk factors for adverse outcomes in developing countries. Lancet. 2007;369(9556):145–57. 10.1016/S0140-6736(07)60076-2 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.John CC, Bangirana P, Byarugaba J, Opoka RO, Idro R, Jurek AM, et al. Cerebral malaria in children is associated with long-term cognitive impairment. Pediatrics. 2008;122(1):e92–9. 10.1542/peds.2007-3709 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Bangirana P, Opoka RO, Boivin MJ, Idro R, Hodges JS, Romero RA, et al. Severe malarial anemia is associated with long-term neurocognitive impairment. Clin Infect Dis. 2014;59(3):336–44. Epub 2014/04/29. 10.1093/cid/ciu293 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Plewes K, Royakkers AA, Hanson J, Hasan MM, Alam S, Ghose A, et al. Correlation of biomarkers for parasite burden and immune activation with acute kidney injury in severe falciparum malaria. Malar J. 2014;13:91 10.1186/1475-2875-13-91 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Conroy AL, Hawkes M, Elphinstone RE, Morgan C, Hermann L, Barker KR, et al. Acute Kidney Injury Is Common in Pediatric Severe Malaria and Is Associated With Increased Mortality. Open Forum Infect Dis. 2016;3(2):ofw046 10.1093/ofid/ofw046 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.von Seidlein L, Olaosebikan R, Hendriksen IC, Lee SJ, Adedoyin OT, Agbenyega T, et al. Predicting the clinical outcome of severe falciparum malaria in african children: findings from a large randomized trial. Clin Infect Dis. 2012;54(8):1080–90. Epub 2012/03/14. 10.1093/cid/cis034 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Jallow M, Casals-Pascual C, Ackerman H, Walther B, Walther M, Pinder M, et al. Clinical features of severe malaria associated with death: a 13-year observational study in the Gambia. PLoS One. 2012;7(9):e45645 10.1371/journal.pone.0045645 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Kapoor K, Gupta S. Malarial acute kidney injury in a paediatric intensive care unit. Trop Doct. 2012;42(4):203–5. 10.1258/td.2012.120196 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Imani PD, Odiit A, Hingorani SR, Weiss NS, Eddy AA. Acute kidney injury and its association with in-hospital mortality among children with acute infections. Pediatr Nephrol. 2013;28(11):2199–206. 10.1007/s00467-013-2544-2 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Waller D, Krishna S, Crawley J, Miller K, Nosten F, Chapman D, et al. Clinical features and outcome of severe malaria in Gambian children. Clin Infect Dis. 1995;21(3):577–87. 10.1093/clinids/21.3.577 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Dondorp A, Nosten F, Stepniewska K, Day N, White N, South East Asian Quinine Artesunate Malaria Trial g. Artesunate versus quinine for treatment of severe falciparum malaria: a randomised trial. Lancet. 2005;366(9487):717–25. 10.1016/S0140-6736(05)67176-0 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Conroy AL, Opoka RO, Bangirana P, Idro R, Ssenkusu JM, Datta D, et al. Acute kidney injury is associated with impaired cognition and chronic kidney disease in a prospective cohort of children with severe malaria. BMC Med. 2019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Trang TT, Phu NH, Vinh H, Hien TT, Cuong BM, Chau TT, et al. Acute renal failure in patients with severe falciparum malaria. Clin Infect Dis. 1992;15(5):874–80. Epub 1992/11/01. 10.1093/clind/15.5.874 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Anand S, Bitton A, Gaziano T. The gap between estimated incidence of end-stage renal disease and use of therapy. PLoS One. 2013;8(8):e72860 10.1371/journal.pone.0072860 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.World Health Organization. Severe Malaria. Trop Med Int Health. 2014;19:7–131. 10.1111/tmi.12313_2 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Nsobya SL, Parikh S, Kironde F, Lubega G, Kamya MR, Rosenthal PJ, et al. Molecular evaluation of the natural history of asymptomatic parasitemia in Ugandan children. J Infect Dis. 2004;189(12):2220–6. 10.1086/421281 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.KDIGO. Kidney Disease: Improving Global Outcomes (KDIGO) Acute Kidney Injury Work Group. KDIGO Clinical Practice Guideline for Acute Kidney Injury. Kidney Int. 2012;Supplement(2):1–138. [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Bangirana P, John CC, Idro R, Opoka RO, Byarugaba J, Jurek AM, et al. Socioeconomic predictors of cognition in Ugandan children: implications for community interventions. PLoS One. 2009;4(11):e7898 10.1371/journal.pone.0007898 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Caldwell BM BR. Home Inventory Administration Manual (3rd ed). Little Rock, AR: University of Arkansas; 2001. [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Gerson AC, Butler R, Moxey-Mims M, Wentz A, Shinnar S, Lande MB, et al. Neurocognitive outcomes in children with chronic kidney disease: Current findings and contemporary endeavors. Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev. 2006;12(3):208–15. 10.1002/mrdd.20116 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Achenbach TM, Ruffle TM. The Child Behavior Checklist and related forms for assessing behavioral/emotional problems and competencies. Pediatr Rev. 2000;21(8):265–71. 10.1542/pir.21-8-265 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Gioia G, Espy KA., Isquith PK. Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function®—Preschool Version (BRIEF®-P). 16204 North Florida Avenue, Lutz, Florida: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc. (PAR); 1996. [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Ssenkusu JM, Hodges JS, Opoka RO, Idro R, Shapiro E, John CC, et al. Long-term Behavioral Problems in Children With Severe Malaria. Pediatrics. 2016;138(5):e20161965 10.1542/peds.2016-1965 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Helbok R, Kendjo E, Issifou S, Lackner P, Newton Charles R, Kombila M, et al. The Lambaréné Organ Dysfunction Score (LODS) Is a Simple Clinical Predictor of Fatal Malaria in African Children. The Journal of Infectious Diseases. 2009;200(12):1834–41. 10.1086/648409 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Conroy AL, Hawkes M, Hayford K, Namasopo S, Opoka RO, John CC, et al. Prospective validation of pediatric disease severity scores to predict mortality in Ugandan children presenting with malaria and non-malaria febrile illness. Crit Care. 2015;19:47 Epub 2015/04/17. 10.1186/s13054-015-0773-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Conroy AL, Opoka RO, Bangirana P, Idro R, Ssenkusu JM, Datta D, et al. Acute kidney injury is associated with impaired cognition and chronic kidney disease in a prospective cohort of children with severe malaria. BMC Med. 2019;17(1):98 10.1186/s12916-019-1332-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Gerstein ED, Pedersen YAA, Crnic KA, Ryu E, Baker BL, Blacher J. Developmental risk and young children's regulatory strategies: predicting behavior problems at age five. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2011;39(3):351–64. 10.1007/s10802-010-9471-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Baker BL, McIntyre LL, Blacher J, Crnic K, Edelbrock C, Low C. Pre-school children with and without developmental delay: behaviour problems and parenting stress over time. J Intellect Disabil Res. 2003;47(4–5):217–30. 10.1046/j.1365-2788.2003.00484.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Greenough WT, Black JE, Wallace CS. Experience and brain development. Child Dev. 1987;58(3):539–59. . [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Bronfenbrenner U. Toward an experimental ecology of human development. Am Psychol. 1977;32(7):513–31. [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Maulik PK, Darmstadt GL. Childhood disability in low- and middle-income countries: overview of screening, prevention, services, legislation, and epidemiology. Pediatrics. 2007;120 Suppl 1(Supplement 1):S1–55. 10.1542/peds.2007-0043B . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Kristensen K, Omagor-Loican M, Onen N, Okot D. Opportunities for inclusion? The education of learners with special educational needs and disabilities in special schools in Uganda. British Journal of Special Education. 2006;33(3):139–47. 10.1111/j.1467-8578.2006.00429.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Nguansangiam S, Day NP, Hien TT, Mai NT, Chaisri U, Riganti M, et al. A quantitative ultrastructural study of renal pathology in fatal Plasmodium falciparum malaria. Trop Med Int Health. 2007;12(9):1037–50. Epub 2007/09/19. 10.1111/j.1365-3156.2007.01881.x . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Milner DA Jr., Whitten RO, Kamiza S, Carr R, Liomba G, Dzamalala C, et al. The systemic pathology of cerebral malaria in African children. Front Cell Infect Microbiol. 2014;4:104 10.3389/fcimb.2014.00104 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Hanson J, Lee SJ, Hossain MA, Anstey NM, Charunwatthana P, Maude RJ, et al. Microvascular obstruction and endothelial activation are independently associated with the clinical manifestations of severe falciparum malaria in adults: an observational study. BMC Med. 2015;13:122 10.1186/s12916-015-0365-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Barber BE, Grigg MJ, Piera KA, William T, Cooper DJ, Plewes K, et al. Intravascular haemolysis in severe Plasmodium knowlesi malaria: association with endothelial activation, microvascular dysfunction, and acute kidney injury. Emerg Microbes Infect. 2018;7(1):106 Epub 2018/06/07. 10.1038/s41426-018-0105-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Nongnuch A, Panorchan K, Davenport A. Brain-kidney crosstalk. Crit Care. 2014;18(3):225 10.1186/cc13907 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Kinsey GR, Li L, Okusa MD. Inflammation in acute kidney injury. Nephron Exp Nephrol. 2008;109(4):e102–7. Epub 09/18. 10.1159/000142934 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Andres-Hernando A, Dursun B, Altmann C, Ahuja N, He Z, Bhargava R, et al. Cytokine production increases and cytokine clearance decreases in mice with bilateral nephrectomy. Nephrol Dial Transplant. 2012;27(12):4339–47. Epub 07/09. 10.1093/ndt/gfs256 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Fitzgerald JC, Basu RK, Akcan-Arikan A, Izquierdo LM, Pineres Olave BE, Hassinger AB, et al. Acute Kidney Injury in Pediatric Severe Sepsis: An Independent Risk Factor for Death and New Disability. Crit Care Med. 2016;44(12):2241–50. 10.1097/CCM.0000000000002007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Mammen C, Al Abbas A, Skippen P, Nadel H, Levine D, Collet JP, et al. Long-term risk of CKD in children surviving episodes of acute kidney injury in the intensive care unit: a prospective cohort study. Am J Kidney Dis. 2012;59(4):523–30. 10.1053/j.ajkd.2011.10.048 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Calderon-Margalit R, Golan E, Twig G, Leiba A, Tzur D, Afek A, et al. History of Childhood Kidney Disease and Risk of Adult End-Stage Renal Disease. N Engl J Med. 2018;378(5):428–38. 10.1056/NEJMoa1700993 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Familiar I, Ruisenor-Escudero H, Giordani B, Bangirana P, Nakasujja N, Opoka R, et al. Use of the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function and Child Behavior Checklist in Ugandan Children with HIV or a History of Severe Malaria. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2015;36(4):277–84. 10.1097/DBP.0000000000000149 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Alister G Craig

30 Oct 2019

PONE-D-19-26285

Acute kidney injury in Ugandan children with severe malaria is associated with long-term behavioral problems

PLOS ONE

Dear Dr Conroy,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

==============================

Both reviewers are highly supportive of publishing this study and point out the current interest in the role of kidney injury in paediatric malaria.  They both have some very minor comments that you might wish to address, but I do not envisage having to send the manuscript out for re-review once you have done this.  

==============================

We would appreciate receiving your revised manuscript by Dec 14 2019 11:59PM. When you are ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter.

To enhance the reproducibility of your results, we recommend that if applicable you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io, where a protocol can be assigned its own identifier (DOI) such that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). This letter should be uploaded as separate file and labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. This file should be uploaded as separate file and labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. This file should be uploaded as separate file and labeled 'Manuscript'.

Please note while forming your response, if your article is accepted, you may have the opportunity to make the peer review history publicly available. The record will include editor decision letters (with reviews) and your responses to reviewer comments. If eligible, we will contact you to opt in or out.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Alister G. Craig

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

1. When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at

http://www.journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and http://www.journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2. Please include captions for your Supporting Information files at the end of your manuscript, and update any in-text citations to match accordingly. Please see our Supporting Information guidelines for more information: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/supporting-information.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: This is a report assessing the correlation between malaria associated Acute Kidney Injury (AKI) and long-term behavioral problems. The group is a leader in the malaria field and has recently clarified and highlighted the previously underrecognized role of kidney injury in pediatric malaria. The report uses a cohort of malaria patients in Uganda that have been used for multiple publications, including a recent report in BMC Medicine showing the effect of AKI on short- and long-term neuro cognitive outcomes. Using the same cohort, the investigators use the CBCL and BRIEF behavioral assessment tools for two years post discharge. They have found significant deficits in the areas of externalizing behavior, global executive function, and behavior regulation in those children with malaria-related AKI. These findings remain significant in a mixed model that includes twelve covariates as fixed effects.

The study is well performed, the data analyzed appropriately and the results clear. The problem is a large one – with malaria related morbidity having an immense impact across sub-Saharan Africa. I would recommend publication with only the following small comments.

1) I am interested in the role of AKI itself (as opposed to AKI specifically in the setting of malaria) in these long-term sequelae. Are there any data on the role of AKI of other etiologies on long-term neurocognitive or behavioral changes?

2) The conclusion sentence may be confusing for those without a knowledge of the definition of ‘school aged children’. Perhaps the sentence should explicitly state the cut-off of 6 years of age.

3) Very minor. There appear to be some typos in the sentences starting on lines 115 and 277.

Reviewer #2: This is a well conducted study in a fairly large number of African children which has expanded on the authors previous studies which evaluated the association between AKI and neurological disability and cognitive impairment

The authors have now detailed the impact of CM/SM and AKI on behavioral issues with longitudinal assessment. This finding done in a robust and standardized method is novel data.

There are only a few minor comments

1) Was creatinine only assessed at enrollment? How did the authors assess if impaired renal function at the time of assessment at months 12 and 24 months contribute to their results

2) It will be good to get more details on the who did the behavioral assessments and how were they trained to ensure standardization

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: Yes: Tsin Wen Yeo

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files to be viewed.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email us at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2019 Dec 17;14(12):e0226405. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0226405.r002

Author response to Decision Letter 0


5 Nov 2019

We have responded to all revision requests to the best of our abilities, and have uploaded a response to reviewers that details all the edits to the manuscript.

Attachment

Submitted filename: PONE Response to Reviewers 2019.11.01.docx

Decision Letter 1

Alister G Craig

27 Nov 2019

Acute kidney injury in Ugandan children with severe malaria is associated with long-term behavioral problems

PONE-D-19-26285R1

Dear Dr. Conroy,

We are pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it complies with all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you will receive an e-mail containing information on the amendments required prior to publication. When all required modifications have been addressed, you will receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will proceed to our production department and be scheduled for publication.

Shortly after the formal acceptance letter is sent, an invoice for payment will follow. To ensure an efficient production and billing process, please log into Editorial Manager at https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the "Update My Information" link at the top of the page, and update your user information. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to enable them to help maximize its impact. If they will be preparing press materials for this manuscript, you must inform our press team as soon as possible and no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

With kind regards,

Alister G. Craig

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

Reviewers' comments: Thank you for responding fully to the reviewers' comments.

Acceptance letter

Alister G Craig

10 Dec 2019

PONE-D-19-26285R1

Acute kidney injury in Ugandan children with severe malaria is associated with long-term behavioral problems

Dear Dr. Conroy:

I am pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper at this point, to enable them to help maximize its impact. If they will be preparing press materials for this manuscript, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org.

For any other questions or concerns, please email plosone@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE.

With kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Prof. Alister G. Craig

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 Data. AKI Behavior Data Export.

    File containing primary data used in the analysis in wide and long format, and an accompanying codebook.

    (XLS)

    S1 Table. Primary behavioral outcomes for community children based on the presence of malaria by PCR on enrollment.

    (DOCX)

    S2 Table. Secondary socio-emotional outcomes for children <6 years with severe malaria by acute kidney injury (AKI) status.

    (DOCX)

    S3 Table. Behavioral outcomes for children ≥6 years with severe malaria by acute kidney injury (AKI) status stratified by age at enrollment.

    (DOCX)

    S4 Table. Behavioral outcomes for children ≥6 years with severe malaria by acute kidney injury (AKI) status stratified by study group at enrollment.

    (DOCX)

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: PONE Response to Reviewers 2019.11.01.docx

    Data Availability Statement

    All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting information files.


    Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES