Abstract
Background
Although male and female rats differ in their patterns of alcohol use, little is known regarding the neural circuit activity that underlies these differences in behavior. The current study used a machine learning approach to characterize sex differences in local field potential (LFP) oscillations that may relate to sex differences in alcohol-drinking behavior.
Methods
LFP oscillations were recorded from the nucleus accumbens shell and the rodent medial prefrontal cortex of adult male and female Sprague-Dawley rats. Recordings occurred before rats were exposed to alcohol (n = 10/sex × 2 recordings/rat) and during sessions of limited access to alcohol (n = 5/sex × 5 recordings/rat). Oscillations were also recorded from each female rat in each phase of estrous prior to alcohol exposure. Using machine learning, we built predictive models with oscillation data to classify rats based on: (1) biological sex, (2) phase of estrous, and (3) alcohol intake levels. We evaluated model performance from real data by comparing it to the performance of models built and tested on permutations of the data.
Results
Our data demonstrate that corticostriatal oscillations were able to predict alcohol intake levels in males (p < 0.01), but not in females (p = 0.45). The accuracies of models predicting biological sex and phase of estrous were related to fluctuations observed in alcohol drinking levels; females in diestrus drank more alcohol than males (p = 0.052), and the male vs. diestrus female model had the highest accuracy (71.01%) compared to chance estimates. Conversely, females in estrus drank very similar amounts of alcohol to males (p = 0.702), and the male vs. estrus female model had the lowest accuracy (56.14%) compared to chance estimates.
Conclusions
The current data demonstrate that oscillations recorded from corticostriatal circuits contain significant information regarding alcohol drinking in males, but not alcohol drinking in females. Future work will focus on identifying where to record LFP oscillations in order to predict alcohol drinking in females, which may help elucidate sex-specific neural targets for future therapeutic development.
Keywords: Alcohol, Local field potentials, Sex differences, Corticostriatal circuits, Machine learning
Background
Alcohol use contributes to 5.1% of the global disease burden, accounting for 5% of all deaths in men and 1% of all deaths in women in the USA alone [1–3]. While historically men drink more alcohol than women, this gender gap is closing [4], and women tend to escalate to alcohol dependence more rapidly than men [2, 5]. Though these sex differences partly arise from sociocultural factors, there are known sex differences in the activity of brain regions that underlie substance use behavior [5, 6]. However, the specific neurobiological underpinnings contributing to sex differences in alcohol drinking are poorly understood, limiting the development of more efficacious, targeted therapies for problematic alcohol use.
One barrier to the development of better therapies for excessive alcohol use is the fact that the majority of preclinical neuroscience studies have used only male animals [7, 8]. However, the available behavioral data in rodent models of alcohol drinking demonstrate that female rats, in a non-dependent state, drink more alcohol and show greater alcohol preference than male rats [9], as well as display heightened sensitivity to the rewarding effects of alcohol compared to males [10]. The behavioral differences between females and males are biological in nature as neonatal masculinization of females reduces alcohol intake compared with intact female rats, resulting in patterns of drinking similar to those displayed by males [11]. In a similar study, intact female rats showed a heightened reward response to alcohol than either males or ovariectomized females, suggesting that ovarian hormones help facilitate the reinforcing properties of alcohol [10]. Ovarian hormone status has also been associated with small fluctuations in alcohol consumption in intact females [12, 13]. However, it is currently unknown whether the neural circuits that regulate alcohol consumption show sexually dimorphic activity patterns (and whether these patterns are influenced by ovarian hormone status) that may explain the sex differences in alcohol drinking behavior.
The mechanistic role of corticostriatal circuits in regulating the rewarding properties of alcohol is well characterized in male rodents [14]. In rats (and humans), the nucleus accumbens (NAc) integrates cortical inputs and indirectly sends feedback to frontal brain regions (medial prefrontal cortex in humans (mPFC); prelimbic (PL), and infralimbic (IL) cortices in rats) [15] and is particularly important in the motivating properties of abused drugs [16]. The mPFC is also activated in response to reward-related cues, and it has been suggested that deficits in the ability to inhibit responses to drugs arises from dysregulated communication between the mPFC and striatal regions [17]. Thus, we hypothesize that male and female rats might display inherent (i.e., trait-level) differences in corticostriatal circuit activity, which may be associated with sex differences in alcohol-drinking behaviors.
Activity in the corticostriatal circuit can be examined longitudinally by measuring local field potential (LFP) oscillations in awake, freely behaving rats. LFP oscillations provide a readout of electrical potential from a group of neurons that relates to individual neuronal activity, as demonstrated by neuronal phase locking and ensemble classification [18–20]. LFP oscillations recorded from reward-related regions have been shown to change during behavior [21] and reflect pharmacologic manipulation [22–24]. For instance, in male rats, low-frequency oscillations decrease while high-frequency oscillations increase following an injection of alcohol [25]. Furthermore, low-frequency oscillations in the cortex and NAc appear to be hypoconnected in alcohol-preferring rats (sex not reported) compared to outbred rats, which was reversed by alcohol exposure [26]. LFP oscillations can therefore be a valuable readout of circuit dynamics related to alcohol-drinking behaviors (i.e., amount of alcohol consumed) in rodents.
In the current experiment, we measured corticostriatal LFP oscillations in adult male and female rats prior to and during alcohol-drinking behavior. Using an unbiased machine learning approach, we aimed to determine whether LFPs recorded from corticostriatal circuits contained information regarding: (1) biological sex, (2) ovarian hormone status, and (3) the amount of alcohol consumed during an alcohol drinking session. We hypothesized that sex differences in inherent corticostriatal circuit activity might be related to sex differences in alcohol-drinking behavior.
Methods
Subjects and housing
Male and female Sprague-Dawley rats (n = 10/sex) were purchased from Charles River (Wilmington, MA, USA) and arrived on postnatal day 60. All animals were housed individually on a reverse 12-h light cycle with ad libitum access to food and water. All experiments were carried out in accordance with the National Institute of Health Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals (NIH Publications No. 80-23) and were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of Dartmouth College.
Electrode construction and implantation
Electrodes were designed and constructed in-house and were similar to those used in our previous publication [27]. Animals were anesthetized with isoflurane gas (4% induction, 2% maintenance) and secured into a stereotaxic frame. Custom electrodes were implanted bilaterally targeting the NAc shell (NAcSh; from bregma: DV − 8 mm; AP + 1.2 mm; ML ± 1.0 mm) and PL/IL junction of the mPFC (from bregma: DV − 5 mm; AP + 3.7 mm; ML ± 0.75 mm). The NAcSh was targeted based on previous work demonstrating that deep brain stimulation of the NAcSh could reduce alcohol intake in male P rats and high-alcohol drinking outbred rats [28, 29]. Thus, we hypothesized that LFPs recorded from the NAcSh would contain information regarding alcohol intake in rats. Four stainless steel skull screws were placed around the electrode site and dental cement (Dentsply, York, PA, USA) was applied to secure the electrodes in place.
Recording and processing local field potential oscillations
LFP oscillations were recorded in sound-attenuated chambers distinct from the rats’ home cages. Rats engaged in free behavior while tethered through a commutator to a Plexon data acquisition system and time-synchronized videos were recorded for each session (Plexon, Plano, TX). Noise-free data from the entire recording session were analyzed using established frequency ranges from the rodent literature (delta (Δ) = 1–4 Hz, theta (θ) = 5–10 Hz, alpha (α) = 11–14 Hz, beta (β) = 15–30 Hz, low gamma (lγ) = 45–65 Hz, and high gamma (hγ) 70–90 Hz [30, 31]) and standard LFP signal processing was used to characterize the power spectral densities (PSDs) within, and coherence between brain regions for each rat using custom code written for Matlab R2017b. A fourth order Chebychev type I notch filter centered at 60 Hz was applied to all of the data to account for 60-Hz line noise. The data was then down-sampled by a factor of five from 2 kHz to 400 Hz. A threshold of ±2 mV was used to identify noise artifacts and remove data using intervals 12.5 milliseconds before and 40 s after the artifacts. To capture the power and coherence dynamics of the signal, we used only epochs that were at least 3 s long. For epochs that were longer than 3 s, we segmented them into 3-s sections removing the remainder to keep all of the data continuous over the same amount of time. An example trace LFP oscillation is shown in Fig. 1a.
PSDs were computed using MATLAB’s pwelch function using a 1.6-s Hamming window with 50% overlap. The PSDs for each 3-s segment were then averaged together to get a single representative PSD for the 30-min recording session. Total power (dB) was calculated for each frequency range. To account for the 60-Hz notch filter, power values of frequencies from 59 to 61 Hz were not included in the analysis. The power per frequency band was then normalized as a percent of the average total power of the signal from 1 to 90 Hz (beginning of Δ to end of hγ).
Coherence was computed using the function mscohere with a 1.3 s sliding Hamming window with 50% overlap. The average coherence between each pair of frequency bands from 1 to 90 Hz (excluding 59 to 61 Hz) was used to normalize the average coherence of each frequency band within that neural site pair.
Determination of estrous phase
After each baseline recording session, estrous cycle was determined via vaginal lavage as described previously [13]. Slides were stained using thionin and the stage of estrus was assessed using an AmScope light microscope (Irvine, CA). Proestrus was characterized as > 75% of the cells in the sample being nucleated epithelial cells. Estrus was characterized as dense sheets of cornified epithelial cells, and diestrus was characterized as scattered nucleated and cornified epithelial cells, along with leukocytes (diestrus-1), or the relative lack of any cells (diestrus-2).
Verification of electrode placement
At the end of the experiment, rats were euthanized using CO2 gas, brains were extracted and subsequently snap frozen in 2-methylbutane on dry ice. Tissue was stored at − 20 °C prior to being sectioned at 40 μm using a Leica CM1850 cryostat and stained with thionin. Electrode placement was verified using an AmScope light microscope (Irvine, CA). Figure 1b shows the electrode placements. Three animals’ (two males and one female) brains were not preserved properly so we were unable to verify electrode placements in those rats. However, based on our previous experience targeting the NAcSh and mPFC with < 1% misplacement, we elected to include the data from that those animals’ in this study.
Experimental overview
Following 1 week of habituation to the animal facility, rats were implanted with bilateral recording electrodes targeting corticostriatal regions. After at least 1 week of recovery, baseline LFPs were recorded in two, 30-min sessions for each male rat, and in each phase of estrous (proestrus, estrus, and diestrus) for each female rat. After baseline LFP recordings were collected, rats were allowed to drink 10% alcohol (100% ethanol diluted with tap water) in a limited access paradigm for 9 sessions (90 min a day, MWF, in a neutral chamber) in order to introduce each rat to alcohol. Animal weights and the volume of alcohol consumed were measured following each session in order to calculate g/kg of alcohol consumed. Next, LFP oscillations were recorded without access to alcohol for 15 min, and then with access to alcohol for 30 min, across five distinct sessions. It is important to note that the male rats in this study were also used for a separate study investigating the impact of deep brain stimulation on alcohol-drinking behaviors. See Fig. 1c for an experimental timeline.
Statistical analysis
Linking corticostriatal LFPs to biological sex and phase of estrous
In order to link corticostriatal activity to biological sex or phase of estrous we used an unbiased machine learning approach similar to what we have published previously [32, 33]. We built predictive models using corticostriatal LFPs to classify rats by biological sex and female rats by phase of estrous. Each recording session produced 60 LFP features: 24 measures of power (6 frequency bands × 4 channels) and 36 measures of coherence (6 frequency bands × 6 channel combinations). We used a penalized regression method (lasso) in order to capture potential combinations of LFP features that correlated with biological sex or phase of estrous. The Matlab package Glmnet [34] was used to implement the lasso using a fourfold cross-validation with 100 repetitions for each of the following models: (1) male vs. female (diestrus), (2) male vs. female (estrus), (3) male vs. female (proestrus), (4) diestrus vs. estrus, (5) diestrus vs. proestrus. and (6) estrus vs. proestrus. The accuracy of the model is reported as the average cross-validated accuracy.
Permutation testing
In order to assess the relative accuracy of the prediction models, we compared the real model performance to models built and tested on 100 different random permutations of the data. As the outcomes of these models are binary, the random permutation models should estimate chance predictions. Thus, if the real models performed better than chance, we determined that there is some information in the circuit related to our binary outcome. Because we used multiple recording sessions from the same rat as separate samples in the real model, we also evaluated models built on permutations of binary rat groupings (group permutations). This was done by keeping the LFP oscillation data together with the rat it was recorded from and shuffling the group assignment of each rat’s set of recordings. Biological sex was equally represented in each group (see Fig. 2). The group permutation test thus evaluated the information contained within LFPs about all possible rat groupings. We calculated the mean accuracy and 95% confidence intervals of cross-validated accuracy from the real, random permutation, and group permutation distributions, as well as z-scores comparing the real and random permutation distributions.
Linking corticostriatal LFPs to alcohol intake levels
In order to analyze the impact of hormone status on alcohol intake during the recording sessions, we used a linear mixed model because two females were lacking at least one drinking day in either estrus or proestrus. Hormone status (diestrus, proestrus, estrus, or male) was used as the fixed effect, controlling for rat identification as the random effect, to predict alcohol intake during each session.
We used a similar machine learning approach (as described above) to link corticostriatal activity to alcohol intake levels, except the outcomes were continuous (g/kg of alcohol consumed by each rat across each day) rather than binary. P values were calculated to determine the magnitude of the difference between the random permutation distributions and the real distributions. Additionally, if the lasso indicated that information existed in the LFP signal, we implemented exhaustive single feature regressions using each LFP predictor to determine the relative information content of each feature, as we have previously described in detail [33].
Results
The ability of corticostriatal LFPs to predict biological sex depends on female estrous phase
Models built from corticostriatal LFP features were able to outperform randomly permuted data in predicting biological sex, and the accuracy of the model performance depended on the hormone status of the females. Models predicting males vs. females in diestrus performed with the highest average accuracy; Fig. 3 shows the predictive models for males vs. females in diestrus (random permutation μ = 54.96 ± 0.6%, real μ = 71.01 ± 1%, z = 1.71; 3A), males vs. females in proestrus (random permutation μ = 43.85 ± 0.8%, real μ = 57.7 ± 1.5%, z = 1.09; 3B), and males vs. females in estrus (random permutation μ = 48.15 ± 0.6%, real μ = 56.1 ± 1.3%, z = 0.81; 3C). It is important to note, however, that models built on group permutations of male vs. females in diestrus performed just as well as the real models (group permutation μ = 73.28 ± 0.0002), indicating that the magnitude of sex-based differences corticostriatal circuit activity was no greater than random groupings of rats (balanced for sex) in this sample.
For the female rats, the accuracy of models built from corticostriatal LFP features to predict phase of estrous fluctuated based on hormone status. Models predicting estrus vs. diestrus performed with the highest accuracy; Fig. 4 shows the predictive models for estrus vs. diestrus (random permutation μ = 50.72 ± 0.6%, real μ = 64.92 ± 1.2%, z = 1.57; 4A), estrus vs. proestrus (random permutation μ = 40.97 ± 0.6%, real μ = 53.94 ± 1.5%, z = 1.38; 4B), and diestrus vs. proestrus (random permutation μ = 57.49 ± 0.6%, real μ = 51.74 ± 1.1%, z = − 0.65; 4C).
Corticostriatal LFPs predict alcohol intake levels in males, but not females
Due to headcap failures, only 5 rats from each sex were able to be recorded following being trained to drink alcohol. A linear mixed effect model indicated that hormone status significantly impacted alcohol intake levels [F (3,17.32) = 4.11, p < 0.05], with males drinking significantly less alcohol than females in diestrus (p = 0.052; 5A). During proestrus and estrus, female drinking amounts were not significantly different than male drinking amounts (p = 0.073 for proestrus; p = 0.702 for estrus).
We also evaluated whether we could predict biological sex in the context of alcohol drinking by using LFP oscillations collected during alcohol consumption. Figure 5b shows the predictive models for males vs. females in diestrus (random permutation μ = 44.99 ± 0.2%, real μ = 86.81 ± 0.01%, z = 3.76; group permutation μ = 86.55 ± 0.0008) while alcohol was available. Again, corticostriatal oscillations do not contain more information regarding biological sex (in the context of alcohol drinking) than information about all possible groupings of rats balanced for sex.
Notably, models built from corticostriatal LFPs to predict alcohol intake levels were able to outperform randomly permuted data in males (random permutation error = 0.11 ± 0.005, real error = 0.03 ± 0.001, p < 0.01; 4C), but not in females (random permutation error = 0.37 ± 0.03, real error = 0.24 ± 0.01, p = 0.45; 4D). Table 1 lists the top five neural features important in predicting alcohol naïve males vs. females in diestrus, as well as the amount of alcohol males consumed.
Table 1.
Male vs. female (diestrus) | Predicting alcohol intake levels: males | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Feature | Mean AUC | Direction | Feature | R2 | Slope |
Left NAcSh θ | 0.818 | Male > Female | Right NAcSh α | 0.505 | − 243.08 |
Right NAcSh θ | 0.788 | Male < Female | Left NAcSh α | 0.483 | −283.80 |
Left mPFC–right NAcSh lγ | 0.782 | Male < Female | Left NAcSh θ | 0.422 | −186.40 |
Left mPFC–right NAcSh hγ | 0.767 | Male < Female | Right mPFC – Right NAcSh lγ | 0.409 | −4.37 |
Left mPFC–left NAcSh Δ | 0.766 | Male > Female | Right NAcSh β | 0.393 | −82.98 |
The top 5 LFP features used in models predicting males vs. diestrus females and alcohol intake levels in males. Frequency bands [delta (Δ), theta (θ), alpha (α), beta (β), low gamma (lγ), and high gamma (hγ)] are described for power features within and coherence features between neural sites
Discussion
Here, we demonstrate that LFP oscillations recorded within corticostriatal circuits contain significant information regarding alcohol intake levels in males, but not in females. We also show that while corticostriatal LFPs may contain some trait-level information (i.e., biological sex), the amount of information is similar to that observed in group permutations of animals balanced for sex. In the females, we observed small fluctuations in model accuracies as a function of ovarian hormone status, which correlated with observed differences in alcohol intake across phases of estrous and between sexes. Overall, the current experiment suggests that inherent electrical activity within corticostriatal circuits is not substantially different between sexes, but that the neural circuits that contain information regarding alcohol intake are sexually dimorphic.
The most compelling data from this study is that corticostriatal oscillations predicted alcohol intake levels in males, but not in females. When single feature logistic regression models were applied to each neural feature, we determined that low-frequency power in the NAcSh (particularly in the θ, α, and β ranges) was negatively associated with alcohol intake levels in males. Interestingly, NAcSh θ power, while negatively correlated with alcohol intake in males, also tended to be higher in males compared to females in diestrus (when males were drinking significantly lower amounts of alcohol compared to females). While these data are correlative, they do suggest that NAcSh θ power might represent a trait-level neural feature that relates to the sex differences observed in alcohol consumption. Previous studies have demonstrated that θ oscillations in the striatum, which are coherent with hippocampal rhythms, are implicated in working memory and attention tasks, and are inhibited by NAc dopamine receptor blockade [35–37]. Along with the present study, these findings collectively suggest that NAc θ oscillations may be important in reward learning, and that low-frequency NAcSh oscillations may perhaps serve as a potential therapeutic target in future research.
This work is further supported by previous studies using corticostriatal oscillations to characterize the neurobiological underpinnings of alcohol-drinking behaviors in male rats. For instance, in male rats chronically exposed to alcohol, β power in the NAcSh is reduced during alcohol-consumption periods compared to alcohol-deprivation periods [21]. This change in NAcSh β power coincides with an increase in NAcSh dopamine content, suggesting that changes in NAcSh β oscillations are influenced by dopamine signaling in the striatum (or vice-versa). Additionally, alcohol-preferring P rats (sex unspecified) show reduced PFC-NAc θ coherence, which is enhanced during alcohol drinking, compared to Wistar rats, suggesting that reduced connectivity in corticostriatal circuits may be related to the increased alcohol consumption in P rats [26]. A significant amount of future work is required to understand the neural circuit dynamics of reward behavior across rat strains and across spatial resolutions (e.g., from single-cell to multi-cell to LFP recordings), but the current data supports the notion that electrical signals recorded in the NAcSh can serve as a valuable readout of substance use behaviors in male rodents.
There are several potential circuits that may contain more information regarding alcohol intake levels in females. In clinical samples, women tend to use alcohol for negative reinforcement reasons, while men tend to use alcohol for positive reinforcement reasons [38]. Women are also more sensitive to stress-induced relapse [5, 39], and similar results have been seen in rodent models of alcohol drinking, where female rats are more sensitive to stress-induced reinstatement of alcohol seeking [40]. Therefore, regions involved in emotional regulation may contain more information about female drinking behavior. One particular region of interest is the insula, which is activated by natural and drug rewards, is involved in craving, and integrates emotional stimuli contributing to mood regulation [14]. Clinical studies report that reduced insular gray matter volume is correlated with increased alcohol expectancy in female problem drinkers, but not in male problem drinkers [41]. Interestingly, insular activation is further enhanced by alcohol cues in alcohol-dependent women compared to non-dependent women, while men show greater alcohol cue reactivity in the striatum compared to women [42, 43]. In light of these previous reports, the current experiment supports the notion that different neural circuits regulate alcohol-drinking behaviors in males and females. Ongoing work from our group aims to elucidate what brain regions contain information about female alcohol intake, focusing on the anterior insula and amygdala.
The current findings align well with previous work describing sex differences in alcohol-drinking behavior. Here, we replicate findings that female rats (in diestrus) drink more alcohol than male rats when accounting for body weight, with female alcohol intake levels fluctuating slightly across the different phases of estrous [12, 13, 44]. Interestingly, when predicting phase of estrous in females from corticostriatal LFPs, the accuracies of the prediction models line up with differences in drinking levels across estrous phases. Specifically, the model predicting estrus from diestrus performed the best, which aligns with the phases in which female drinking behavior is most different. These data are particularly interesting considering that ovarian hormone status has been shown to influence addictive behavior in female rats and in women (though less so with alcohol and more so with other addictive substances like cocaine [5, 45, 46]). Our future work will continue to investigate the role of ovarian hormones in altering substance use behaviors (and the underlying neural circuits) with the aim of developing a more comprehensive picture of the neurobiology of addiction in female rodents.
It is important to consider a couple of caveats to the current study. When attempting to predict males vs. females in diestrus, the real model outperformed models built on random permutations (chance); however, the group permutation models had a similar accuracy to the real model. This adds a layer of complexity to the interpretation of the data, as the accuracy of the group permutations suggests that the information in the circuit regarding biological sex is no greater than the information describing natural variability in circuit activity between similar sized groups of animals (balanced for sex). There are likely many psychological domains in which corticostriatal circuit activity contains information; thus, some of the group permutations may be finding real differences between rats that are not related to biological sex. However, if biological sex was associated with substantially different corticostriatal oscillations, we would expect the real models to perform better than both the random and group permutations. It is unclear whether adding more rats to the experiment would have altered the relative accuracies of the real models and group permutations, so our future work will systematically analyze how many animals/samples are necessary to build a group permutation model with accuracies that approach chance. Nevertheless, this limitation does not reduce the importance of the present data. The models predicting alcohol intake levels in males and females were within-animal, meaning that the neural features identified in the continuous prediction models are directly related to the variability in alcohol intake in males.
Secondly, Sprague-Dawley rats tend to drink less alcohol than other rodent strains [44], and the male rats in this study were indeed drinking very small amounts of alcohol with low variability compared to the females. This variability in alcohol drinking is an important factor in interpreting model performance (error in the prediction). We would expect that models operating at chance (permuted models) predicting data with higher mean values and variance (female data) would produce greater prediction errors compared to permuted models trying to predict data with lower means and variance (male data). Because the error of the permuted models accounts for these differences in the distributions of the male and female drinking data, we can interpret the real model error by the relationship to the distribution of permuted error. Therefore, even though the male drinking data had a lower mean and variance, the fact that the real model could predict individual alcohol intake values with a significantly lower error than the permuted models, indicates that these models were performing better than would be expected by chance—unlike the female models. Thus, we were able to conclude that there was significant information contained within corticostriatal LFPs regarding alcohol intake in males (but not females).
Our future work aims to extend these findings to alcohol-dependent rodents (which will lead to substantial increases in alcohol intake) and other rodent strains. The goal of future work is to provide a causal link by specifically manipulating the neural features associated with alcohol intake in the hopes of changing alcohol-drinking behavior.
Perspectives and significance
The current dataset contributes to our long-term goal of characterizing the neural circuits that underlie alcohol-drinking behavior in males and females, and our data suggest that these circuits are sexually dimorphic in nature. Moreover, the present data set reinforces the need to develop more personalized therapies for alcohol-related problems, and to help achieve this aim, current work in our laboratory attempts to identify the neural circuits that underlie female alcohol-drinking behavior. Additionally, we aim to characterize how circuit oscillations change across states of alcohol dependence in males and females in order to isolate (perhaps sex-specific) neural targets for reducing problematic alcohol use.
Acknowledgements
The authors would like to thank Elise Bragg for her assistance in collecting portions of the behavioral data.
Authors’ contributions
AMH and EDKS contributed to planning the experimental design, collected the, data and wrote the manuscript. LLD analyzed the local field potential data, performed the computational modeling, and contributed to editing the manuscript. KMK and EDA assisted with data collection. WTD and AIG contributed to planning the experimental design and editing the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.
Funding
This work was supported by funds from the Department of Psychiatry at the Geisel School of Medicine at Dartmouth (AG), the Hitchcock Foundation (AH), NIDA T32 training grant (DA037202; AH), an LRP grant from the NIH NCATS (WD), an NIAAA training grant (F31AA027441; LD), and the Dartmouth Clinical and Translational Science Institute from the NIH NCATS (KL2TR001088; WD).
Availability of data and materials
The datasets used and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request. This data is available as a preprint on bioRxiv (doi: 10.1101/750711).
Ethics approval and consent to participate
All experiments were carried out in accordance with the National Institute of Health Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals (NIH Publications No. 80-23) and were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of Dartmouth College.
Consent for publication
Not applicable
Competing interests
Over the past 3 years, AG has received research grants from Alkermes, Novartis, and Janssen. He has served as an (uncompensated) consultant to Otsuka and Alkermes and as a member of a Data Monitoring Board for Lilly. The other authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher’s Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Angela M. Henricks and Emily D. K. Sullivan contributed equally to this work.
References
- 1.World Health Organization . Global status report on alcohol and health 2018. 2018. p. 450. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Erol A, Karpyak VM. Sex and gender-related differences in alcohol use and its consequences: contemporary knowledge and future research considerations. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2015;156:1–13. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2015.08.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Rehm J, Dawson D, Frick U, Gmel G, Roerecke M, Shield KD, et al. Burden of disease associated with alcohol use disorders in the United States. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2014;38(4):1068–1077. doi: 10.1111/acer.12331. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Grant BF, Chou SP, Saha TD, Pickering RP, Kerridge BT, Ruan WJ, et al. Prevalence of 12-month alcohol use, high-risk drinking, and DSM-IV alcohol use disorder in the United States, 2001-2002 to 2012-2013. JAMA Psychiatry. 2017;20852:2001–2002. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2017.2161. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Becker JB, McClellan ML, Reed BG. Sex differences, gender and addiction. J Neurosci Res. 2017;95(1–2):136–147. doi: 10.1002/jnr.23963. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Becker Jill B, Perry Adam N, Westenbroek Christel. Sex differences in the neural mechanisms mediating addiction: a new synthesis and hypothesis. Biology of Sex Differences. 2012;3(1):14. doi: 10.1186/2042-6410-3-14. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Beery AK, Zucker I. Sex bias in neuroscience and biomedical research. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2011;35(3):565–572. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.07.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Zucker Irving, Beery Annaliese K. Males still dominate animal studies. Nature. 2010;465(7299):690–690. doi: 10.1038/465690a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Li T-K, Lumeng L. Alcohol preference and voluntary alcohol intakes of inbred rat strains and the National Institutes of Health heterogeneous stock of rats. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 1984;8(5):485–486. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1984.tb05708.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Torres OV, Walker EM, Beas BA, O’Dell LE. Female rats display enhanced rewarding effects of ethanol that are hormone dependent. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2014;38(31):108–115. doi: 10.1111/acer.12213. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Almeida OFX, Shoaib M, Deicke J, Fischer D, Darwish MH, Patchev VK. Gender differences in ethanol preference and ingestion in rats. J Clin Invest. 1998;101(12):2677–2685. doi: 10.1172/JCI1198. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Roberts AJ, Smith AD, Weiss F, Rivier C, Koob GF. Estrous cycle effects on operant responding for ethanol in female rats. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 1998;22(7):1564–1569. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1998.tb03950.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Henricks AM, Berger AL, Lugo JM, Baxter-Potter LN, Bieniasz KV, Craft RM, et al. Sex differences in alcohol consumption and alterations in nucleus accumbens endocannabinoid mRNA in alcohol-dependent rats. Neuroscience. 2016;335:195–206. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2016.08.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Koob GF, Volkow ND. Neurobiology of addiction: a neurocircuitry analysis. Lancet Psychiatry. 2016;3(8):760–773. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(16)00104-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Goto Y, Grace AA. Limbic and cortical information processing in the nucleus accumbens. Trends Neurosci. 2008;31(11):552–558. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2008.08.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Koob GF, Volkow ND. Neurocircuitry of addiction. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010;35(1):217–238. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Goldstein RZ, Volkow ND. Drug addiction and its underlying neurobiological basis: neuroimaging evidence for the involvement of the frontal cortex. Am J Psychiatry. 2002;159(10):1642–1652. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.159.10.1642. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Howe MW, Atallah HE, McCool A, Gibson DJ, Graybiel AM. Habit learning is associated with major shifts in frequencies of oscillatory activity and synchronized spike firing in striatum. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2011;108(40):16801–16806. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1113158108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Thorn CA, Graybiel AM. Differential entrainment and learning-related dynamics of spike and local field potential activity in the sensorimotor and associative striatum. J Neurosci. 2014;34(8):2845–2859. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1782-13.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.van der Meer MAA, Kalenscher T, Lansink CS, Pennartz CMA, Berke JD, Redish AD. Integrating early results on ventral striatal gamma oscillations in the rat. Front Neurosci. 2010;4:300. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2010.00300. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Hadar R, Voget M, Vengeliene V, Haumesser JK, van Riesen C, Avchalumov Y, et al. Altered neural oscillations and elevated dopamine levels in the reward pathway during alcohol relapse. Behav Brain Res. 2017;316:131–135. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2016.08.045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Berke JD. Fast oscillations in cortical-striatal networks switch frequency following rewarding events and stimulant drugs. Eur J Neurosci. 2009;30(5):848–859. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.06843.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Morra JT, Glick SD, Cheer JF. Cannabinoid receptors mediate methamphetamine induction of high frequency gamma oscillations in the nucleus accumbens. Neuropharmacology. 2012;63(4):565–574. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2012.04.036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Dejean C, Boraud T, Le Moine C. Opiate dependence induces network state shifts in the limbic system. Neurobiol Dis. 2013;59:220–229. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2013.07.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Tsurugizawa T, Abe Y, Le Bihan D. Water apparent diffusion coefficient correlates with gamma oscillation of local field potentials in the rat brain nucleus accumbens following alcohol injection. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab. 2017;37(9):3193–3202. doi: 10.1177/0271678X16685104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.McCane AM, Ahn S, Rubchinsky LL, Janetsian-Fritz SS, Linsenbardt DN, Czachowski CL, et al. COMT inhibition alters cued-evoked oscillatory dynamics during alcohol drinking in the rat. Eneuro. 2018;5(October):ENEURO.0326-18.2018. doi: 10.1523/ENEURO.0326-18.2018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Doucette WT, Khokhar JY, Green AI. Nucleus accumbens deep brain stimulation in a rat model of binge eating. Transl Psychiatry. 2015;5(12):e695. doi: 10.1038/tp.2015.197. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Henderson MB, Green AI, Bradford PS, Chau DT, Roberts DW, Leiter JC. Deep brain stimulation of the nucleus accumbens reduces alcohol intake in alcohol-preferring rats. Neurosurg Focus. 2010;29(2):E12. doi: 10.3171/2010.4.FOCUS10105. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Henricks AM, Dwiel LL, Deveau NH, Simon AA, Ruiz-Jaquez MJ, Green AI, et al. Corticostriatal oscillations predict high vs. low drinkers in a rat model of limited access alcohol consumption. Front Syst Neurosci. 2019;13(August):1–8. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2019.00035. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Mccracken CB, Grace AA. Nucleus accumbens deep brain stimulation produces region-specific alterations in local field potential oscillations and evoked responses in vivo. J Neurosci. 2009;29(16):5354–5363. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0131-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Catanese J, Carmichael JE, van der Meer MAA. Low- and high-gamma oscillations deviate in opposite directions from zero-phase synchrony in the limbic corticostriatal loop. J Neurophysiol. 2016;116(1):5–17. doi: 10.1152/jn.00914.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Doucette WT, Dwiel L, Boyce JE, Simon AA, Khokhar JY, Green AI. Machine learning based classification of deep brain stimulation outcomes in a rat model of binge eating using ventral striatal oscillations. Front Psychiatry. 2018;9:336. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00336. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Dwiel LL, Khokhar JY, Connerney MA, Green AI, Doucette WT. Finding the balance between model complexity and performance: using ventral striatal oscillations to classify feeding behavior in rats. PLoS Comput Biol. 2019;15(4):e1006838. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1006838. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Qian J, Hastie T, Friedman J, Tibshirani R, Simon N. Glmnet in Matlab. 2013. [Google Scholar]
- 35.DeCoteau WE, Thorn C, Gibson DJ, Courtemanche R, Mitra P, Kubota Y, et al. Learning-related coordination of striatal and hippocampal theta rhythms during acquisition of a procedural maze task. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2007;104(13):5644–5649. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0700818104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.DeCoteau WE, Thorn C, Gibson DJ, Courtemanche R, Mitra P, Kubota Y, et al. Oscillations of local field potentials in the rat dorsal striatum during spontaneous and instructed behaviors. J Neurophysiol. 2007;97(5):3800–3805. doi: 10.1152/jn.00108.2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Matulewicz P, Kasicki S, Hunt MJ. The effect of dopamine receptor blockade in the rodent nucleus accumbens on local field potential oscillations and motor activity in response to ketamine. Brain Res. 2010;1366:226–232. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.09.088. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Peltier MR, Verplaetse TL, Mineur YS, Petrakis IL, Cosgrove KP, Picciotto MR, et al. Sex differences in stress-related alcohol use. Neurobiol Stress. 2019;10:100149. doi: 10.1016/j.ynstr.2019.100149. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Hudson A, Stamp JA. Neuroscience and biobehavioral reviews ovarian hormones and propensity to drug relapse: a review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2011;35(3):427–436. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.05.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Bertholomey ML, Nagarajan V, Torregrossa MM. Sex differences in reinstatement of alcohol seeking in response to cues and yohimbine in rats with and without a history of adolescent corticosterone exposure. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2016;233(12):2277–2287. doi: 10.1007/s00213-016-4278-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Ide JS, Zhornitsky S, Hu S, Zhang S, Krystal JH, Li CR. Sex differences in the interacting roles of impulsivity and positive alcohol expectancy in problem drinking: a structural brain imaging study. NeuroImage Clin. 2017;14:750–759. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2017.03.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Tapert SF, Brown GG, Baratta MV, Brown SA. fMRI BOLD response to alcohol stimuli in alcohol dependent young women. Addict Behav. 2004;29(1):33–50. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2003.07.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Kaag AM, Wiers RW, de Vries TJ, Pattij T, Goudriaan AE. Striatal alcohol cue-reactivity is stronger in male than female problem drinkers. Eur J Neurosci. 2018; Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/29888821. Cited 17 July 2019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 44.Priddy BM, Carmack SA, Thomas LC, Vendruscolo JCM, Koob GF, Vendruscolo LF. Sex, strain, and estrous cycle influences on alcohol drinking in rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2017;152:61–67. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2016.08.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Jackson LR, Robinson TE, Becker JB. Sex differences and hormonal influences on acquisition of cocaine self-administration in rats. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2006;31:129–138. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300778. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Allen AM, Lunos S, Heishman SJ, al’Absi M, Hatsukami D, Allen SS. Subjective response to nicotine by menstrual phase. Addict Behav. 2015;43:50–53. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2014.12.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
The datasets used and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request. This data is available as a preprint on bioRxiv (doi: 10.1101/750711).