Abstract
We sought to determine if upstream amyloid accumulation and downstream cognitive impairment have independent relationships with microglial activation and tau pathology. Fifty-eight older adults were stratified by amyloid and cognitive status based on 18F-florbetaben PET, history, and neuropsychological testing. Of these, 57 had 11C-PBR28 PET to measure microglial activation and 43 had 18F-MK-6240 PET to measure tau pathology. Amyloid and cognitive status were associated with increased overall binding for both 11C-PBR28 and 18F-MK-6240 (p’s < 0.01). While there was no interaction between amyloid and cognitive status in their association with 11C-PBR28 binding (p = 0.6722), there was an interaction in their association with 18F-MK-6240 binding (p = 0.0115). Binding of both radioligands was greater in amyloid-positive controls than in amyloid-negative controls; however, this difference was seen in neocortical regions for 11C-PBR28 and only in medial temporal cortex for 18F-MK-6240. We conclude that, in the absence of cognitive symptoms, amyloid deposition has a greater association with microglial activation than with tau pathology.
Keywords: Alzheimer’s disease, amyloid, tau, microglia, neuroinflammation
1. INTRODUCTION
The emergence of β-amyloid pathophysiology—defined by reduction of Aβ42 in CSF or by cortical retention of amyloid radioligands on PET—is the earliest detectable in vivo change in Alzheimer’s disease pathogenesis (Jack et al., 2013). However, imaging studies have identified both cognitive controls with high amyloid signal and patients with Alzheimer’s-like impairment with no apparent amyloid binding (Landau et al., 2016; Petersen et al., 2013; Pike et al., 2011), demonstrating that amyloidosis is neither necessary nor sufficient for clinical expression of an Alzheimer’s phenotype. Therefore, other pathogenic factors appear to be required for cognitive decline, in both Alzheimer’s disease and in disorders that mimic Alzheimer’s disease.
Neuroinflammation is related to cognitive decline in Alzheimer’s disease. Binding of PET radioligands to the 18 kDa translocator protein (TSPO)—a marker of microglial activation—is increased in Alzheimer’s disease and correlates with disease severity (Edison et al., 2008; Kreisl et al., 2016; Kreisl et al., 2013). However, neuroinflammation may be a non-specific response to neurodegeneration, as TSPO binding is increased in several non-Alzheimer neurodegenerative disorders, including frontotemporal dementia, Parkinson’s disease, and amyotrophic lateral sclerosis (Cagnin et al., 2004; Corcia et al., 2012; Gerhard et al., 2006), and in vitro studies demonstrate microglial localization to axonal debris (Tanaka et al., 2009). On the other hand, recent genome-wide association studies identified mutations in microglia-expressed genes (e.g., TREM2 and CD33) that confer increased risk of Alzheimer’s disease (Colonna and Wang, 2016). Moreover, results from tissue culture studies showed that physiological concentrations of Aβ42 stimulate microglia to release neurotoxic cytokines (Maezawa et al., 2011; Marlatt et al., 2014), and inflammatory markers such as YKL-40 are elevated in CSF in the preclinical and prodromal stages of Alzheimer’s disease (Janelidze et al., 2018). These latter results suggest that microglial activation appears early in Alzheimer’s disease pathogenesis, perhaps in response to early amyloid deposition, and may contribute to clinical progression.
PET imaging studies show that neurofibrillary aggregation of tau is more frequent in amyloid-positive than amyloid-negative controls (Jack et al., 2018; Schultz et al., 2018), and tau pathology is generally detected after substantial amyloid plaque deposition (Jack et al., 2013). To date, only two PET studies have measured these three biomarkers in the same subjects. One study found correlations between TSPO and tau binding in both mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease patients (Dani et al., 2018); however, correlations were also found in amyloid-negative subjects and the measured amount of TSPO binding was similar among patients and controls. The other study found no correlation between TSPO and tau binding (Parbo et al., 2018). This latter study may have lacked sensitivity though, due to the low specific-to-non-specific binding of the TSPO radioligand used, 11C-(R)-PK11195.
We sought to determine how microglial activation and tau pathology relate to the upstream amyloidosis and downstream cognitive decline seen in Alzheimer’s disease. To achieve this goal, we stratified subjects by amyloid and cognitive status to test if amyloid-positivity and cognitive impairment are each independently associated with the amount of microglial activation and tau pathology measured with PET. We also sought to infer a temporal order of these Alzheimer’s-associated markers from the cross-sectional imaging data. Microglial activation was measured using the TSPO radioligand 11C-PBR28 (Briard et al., 2008; Lyoo et al., 2015). Tau pathology was measured with 18F-MK-6240, a recently developed radioligand that binds to paired helical filament tangles (Betthauser et al., 2019; Hostetler et al., 2016; Lohith et al., 2019; Pascoal et al., 2018).
2. MATERIALS AND METHODS
2.1. Subject selection
Seventy-three adults aged 50 years and older were recruited from the Columbia University Irving Medical Center (CUIMC) Aging and Dementia clinic, self-referrals, the Columbia Alzheimer’s Disease Research Center, and other research cohorts at CUIMC. All subjects underwent routine history, physical and neurological examination; routine laboratory studies; and a T1 MPRAGE (160 slice 1 mm resolution, 256 x 200 voxel count) on a 3T Phillips Achieva MRI machine in the CUIMC Hatch MRI center.
These were done to rule out significant medical, neurological, or psychiatric illness; presence of cortical infarcts on imaging; and use of any immunosuppressive medication. All subjects (or their representative) provided informed consent according to the Declaration of Helsinki and this study was approved by the CUIMC Institutional Review Board.
2.2. Cognitive Testing
Subjects who passed screening underwent neuropsychological testing, which included the Clinical Dementia Rating scale (CDR (Hughes et al., 1982)), Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) (Folstein et al., 1983), Selective Reminding Test Delayed Recall (SRT-DR) (Ruff et al., 1989), Trail Making Test Parts A and B, and Category and Phonemic Fluency. All cognitive test scores were transformed into z-scores using age-, sex-, and education-adjusted normative data. To be considered cognitively impaired, subjects had to have a primary memory complaint and meet clinical criteria for either amnestic mild cognitive impairment (MCI) (Albert et al., 2011) or Alzheimer’s disease (McKhann et al., 2011). None of the impaired patients met clinical criteria for any specific disorder other than Alzheimer’s disease (e.g., dementia with Lewy bodies, vascular dementia, Parkinson’s disease dementia, corticobasal degeneration, progressive supranuclear palsy, or frontotemporal dementia). Control subjects were free of any cognitive complaint, and history and neuropsychological test scores were used to confirm absence of clinically meaningful cognitive impairment.
2.3. TSPO genotyping
TSPO binding affinity was determined at screening. Genomic DNA from each subject was used to genotype the rs6971 polymorphism using a TaqMan assay (Owen et al., 2012). Subjects homozygous for the low affinity allele were excluded from any further testing detailed below, and from the final analysis (n = 8).
2.4. PET imaging
All PET imaging was performed in the CUIMC Kreitchman PET Center on the same Siemens Biograph mCT with low-dose CT scan used for attenuation correction. PET scans were performed on separate days. Amyloid status was determined with 18F-florbetaben (FBB) PET. FBB images were acquired from 50–70 min post-injection (injected activity = 300 MBq) in 4 x 5 min frames. 11C-PBR28 PET images were acquired 60–90 min post-injection (injected activity = 599 ± 140 MBq) in 6 x 5 min frames. 18F-MK-6240 images were acquired 80–100 min post injection (injected activity = 172 ± 10 MBq) in 4 x 5 min frames. All 11C-PBR28 scans were performed within 12 months of MRI and FBB scan. Mean interval between 11C-PBR28 and 18F-MK-6240 scans was 6 ± 4 months for patients and 11 ± 7 months for controls. Subjects who had greater than 12 months delay between 11C-PBR28 and 18F-MK-6240 scans had neuropsychological testing and brain MRI repeated.
2.4.1. PET image processing
The FBB, 11C-PBR28, and 18F-MK-6240 PET images were processed using the same pipeline. Reconstructed PET images were first realigned and corrected for subject motion using SPM12 (Wellcome Centre for Human Neuroimaging.). Images were then preprocessed with PMOD 3.8 (PMOD Technologies) using the PNEURO tool. T1-weighted MRI scans were segmented and normalized to standard space and the Hammers-N30R83–1MM atlas was used to define regions of interest (ROIs). The ROI volumes were then reverse-warped to the subject’s native space. ROIs were inspected and manually corrected as necessary. PET images were coregistered to the native space MRI. For 11C-PBR28 and 18F-MK-6240, the coregistered PET images were then corrected for partial volume effects with the region-based voxelwise method (Thomas et al., 2011). PET images from each acquisition (50–70 min for FBB, 60–90 min for 11C-PBR28, 80–100 min for 18F-MK-6240) were then averaged, and the native space ROIs were applied. Concentration of radioactivity (kBq/cc) was then extracted from each ROI.
Volume-weighted gray matter ROIs were created from the Hammers atlas ROIs, consisting of prefrontal cortex (middle frontal gyrus, inferior frontal gyrus, superior frontal gyrus, straight gyrus, anterior orbital gyrus, medial orbital gyrus, lateral orbital gyrus, posterior orbital gyrus); middle and inferior temporal gyri (medial part of anterior temporal lobe, lateral parts of anterior temporal lobe and middle and inferior temporal gyri); superior temporal gyrus (anterior part of superior temporal gyrus, posterior part of superior temporal gyrus); medial temporal cortex (amygdala, parahippocampal gyrus, and entorhinal cortex); posterior cingulate cortex; superior parietal lobule; inferior parietal lobule; striatum (caudate nucleus and putamen); and cerebellum. For FBB and 11C-PBR28, the concentration of radioactivity of each ROI was then divided by that of the cerebellum to create standardized uptake value ratios (SUVRs), a method that has been validated for both tracers in prior studies (Bullich et al., 2017; Lyoo et al., 2015). Because uptake of 18F-MK-6240 is sometimes seen in the anterior lobe of the cerebellum (Betthauser et al., 2019), likely due to spill-over from ventral temporal cortex and occipital cortex or off-target binding in tentorium cerebelli, we used only inferior cerebellar gray matter as reference region for this radioligand. For 11C-PBR28 and 18F-MK-6240, SUVRs were calculated for both partial volume corrected and uncorrected images.
2.4.2. Amyloid status determination
Reconstructed FBB PET images were averaged to create a single image for each subject and a visual read by an experienced neurologist (WCK) blinded to the subject’s diagnosis was used to determine the presence or absence of fibrillar amyloid plaque (Bullich et al., 2017). ROI data from the 50–70 min FBB images were used to calculate a composite SUVR (weighted average of SUVRs from prefrontal cortex, middle and inferior temporal gyri, superior temporal gyrus, medial temporal cortex, posterior cingulate cortex, superior parietal lobule, and inferior parietal lobule) using cerebellar gray matter as reference region.
2.4.3. T1 MRI analysis
Volumes for target ROIs (prefrontal cortex, middle and inferior temporal gyrus, superior temporal gyrus, medial temporal cortex, posterior cingulate cortex, superior parietal lobule, inferior parietal lobule, striatum, and cerebellum) were derived using PMOD as detailed above. To determine if amyloid-negative patients had an Alzheimer’s-like pattern of neurodegeneration, ROIs for the right and left hippocampi for all subjects were manually drawn by investigators blind to the diagnosis. The volume of each ROI was corrected for differences in brain size by dividing by the total intracranial volume.
2.5. Statistical analysis
Statistical analyses were carried out using SAS version 9.4 and R version 3.5.1. Subjects were classified based on their amyloid status and clinical profile (as described above) into one of four groups: (1) amyloid-positive patients, (2) amyloid-positive controls, (3) amyloid-negative patients, and (4) amyloid-negative controls. Mean and standard deviation was derived for key descriptive characteristics for each subgroup, and an analysis of variance was performed to assess between group differences in these descriptive statistics. To validate the visual read of amyloid status as described above, the composite FBB SUVR was compared between amyloid positive and amyloid negative subjects.
To determine if amyloid and cognitive status are related to microglial activation and pathological tau burden, we performed separate multivariate analyses of variance (MANOVA) for 11C-PBR28 and 18F-MK-6240 binding, using the eight PMOD-derived target ROIs: prefrontal cortex; middle and inferior temporal gyri; superior temporal gyrus; medial temporal cortex; posterior cingulate; superior parietal lobule; inferior parietal lobule; and striatum. Amyloid and cognitive status were the independent variables. Age and TSPO genotype (for 11C -PBR28 binding) were included in the model as covariates. We also tested for interactions between amyloid positivity and cognitive impairment. For all MANOVA analyses, the residuals were examined to check normality and sphericity.
To more closely determine if amyloid and cognitive status are related to microglial activation and tau burden on a regional level, we ran a univariate analysis of variance for each region of interest listed above for both 11C-PBR28 and 18F-MK-6240 binding. Amyloid and cognitive status were the independent variables. Age and TSPO genotype (for 11C-PBR28 binding) were included as covariates. We determined effect size using partial Eta-square and tested for interactions between amyloid and cognitive status. For this primary analysis, we applied Bonferroni correction to compensate for the 16 multiple comparisons (two independent variables x eight regions).
In a secondary analysis, to determine when and where 11C-PBR28 and 18F-MK-6240 binding first increase, we stratified subjects along the AD clinical spectrum from normal aging to Alzheimer’s disease dementia (i.e., amyloid-negative controls, amyloid-positive controls, amyloid-positive patients with mild cognitive impairment, and amyloid-positive patients with dementia), and then performed a four-way ANOVA to test for between-group differences within each of the eight aforementioned ROIs. Partial correlations, adjusting for age and TSPO genotype (for 11C-PBR28), tested the relationships of MMSE score with 11C-PBR28 and 18F-MK-6240 binding in these ROIs as well. Partial correlations were also performed to test the relationship between 11C-PBR28 and 18F-MK-6240 binding in each region.
3. RESULTS
3.1. Characteristics of included participants
Fifty-seven subjects completed screening procedures, MRI, and the 11C-PBR28 scan: 23 amyloid-positive patients, seven amyloid-positive controls, 10 amyloid-negative patients and 17 amyloid-negative controls. Forty-two of these (14 amyloid-positive patients, seven amyloid-positive controls, nine amyloid-negative patients, and 12 amyloid-negative controls), plus one amyloid-negative patient who did not undergo 11C-PBR28 imaging, had 18F-MK6240 PET. One amyloid-positive patient was unable to complete the SRT due to poor memory performance. Impaired status for this participant was assigned based on memory complaint corroborated by caregiver, 0/3 delayed recall on MMSE testing, and performance on non-memory tests. Both patient groups had lower SRT-DR z-scores than the control groups (p′s < 0.01); however, amyloid-positive patients had lower MMSE score than the other three groups (p′s < 0.01). Both patient groups had smaller hippocampal volume (corrected for total intracranial volume) than the amyloid-negative controls (p’s < 0.01). Amyloid-positive patients additionally had smaller corrected volume for inferior parietal lobule than the other three groups (p′s < 0.01), while amyloid-negative patients had smaller corrected volume for striatum than the other three groups (p′s < 0.01). No group differences in corrected volume were seen for any other ROI. In agreement with our visual read of amyloid status, both amyloid-positive groups had greater FBB SUVRs in the composite ROI than the amyloid-negative groups (p′s < 0.01). Table 1 shows demographic and clinical information for the 58 participants included in the analysis. The number of high affinity (“HAB”) and mixed affinity (“MAB”) binders in each group are also reported.
Table 1.
Descriptive data for subject participants based on amyloid and cognitive status.a
Aβ (+) patients (n = 23) | Aβ (+) controls (n = 7) | Aβ (−) patients (n = 10) | Aβ (−) controls (n = 17) | F statistics of 4-group ANOVA | p-value of F statistics | Partial Eta-Square | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Age (years)bcg | 65.8 ± 8.4 | 73.1 ± 2.7 | 75.8 ± 9.8 | 67.6 ± 3.8 | 5.87 | 0.0015 | 0.2459 |
Male/Female | 20/3 | 4/3 | 7/4 | 5/12 | N/A | N/A | N/A |
Education (years) | 16.3 ± 2.7 | 14.9 ± 2.5 | 16.5 ± 3.5 | 15.6 ± 2.7 | 0.66 | 0.5804 | 0.0354 |
MMSE scorebcd | 23.0 ± 4.7 | 28.9 ± 2.0 | 26.9 ± 2.8 | 29.4 ± 0.8 | 14.19 | <0.0001 | 0.4407 |
SRT-DR (z-score)bcdeg | −3.32 ± 0.45 | 0.23 ± 1.19 | −2.53 ± 0.63 | 0.72 ± 1.02 | 100.95 | <0.0001 | 0.8511 |
TSPO genotype (HAB/MAB) | 13/10 | 3/4 | 6/5 | 12/5 | N/A | N/A | N/A |
FBB Composite SUVRbcdef | 1.65 ± 0.18 | 1.49 ± 0.14 | 1.17 ± 0.14 | 1.12 ± 0.06 | 54.04 | <0.0001 | 0.7501 |
% Hippocampal Volumebdg | 0.86 ± 0.15 | 1.00 ± 0.16 | 0.85 ± 0.19 | 1.04 ± 0.14 | 5.69 | 0.0018 | 0.2402 |
% Inferior Parietal Lobulebcd | 5.19 ± 0.41 | 5.66 ± 0.34 | 5.73 ± 0.24 | 5.61 ± 0.40 | 7.46 | 0.0003 | 0.2929 |
Global WMH score (mm3)g | 2.33 ± 2.03 | 4.85 ± 5.75 | 4.58 ± 4.76 | 1.78 ± 2.80 | 2.38 | 0.0795 | 0.1189 |
MMSE = Mini Mental Status Exam, SRT-DR = Selective Reminding Test – Delayed Recall, TSPO = 18 kDa translocator protein, HAB = high affinity binder, MAB = mixed affinity binder, FBB = 18F-Florbetaben, SUVR = standardized uptake value ratio, % volume = % total intracranial volume, WMH = White matter hyperintensity
Fifteen participants did not undergo 18F-MK-6240 PET and one participant did not undergo 11C-PBR28 PET
Significant difference (p < 0.05) between Aβ (+) patients and Aβ (+) controls
Significant difference (p < 0.05) between Aβ (+) patients and Aβ (−) patients
Significant difference (p < 0.05) between Aβ (+) patients and Aβ (−) controls
Significant difference (p < 0.05) between Aβ (+) controls and Aβ (−) patients
Significant difference (p < 0.05) between Aβ (+) controls and Aβ (−) controls
Significant difference (p < 0.05) between Aβ (−) patients and Aβ (−) controls
3.2. Effect of amyloid status and cognitive impairment on 11C-PBR28 binding
We first tested how microglial activation relates to amyloid and cognitive status. We found that amyloid positivity, cognitive impairment, greater age, and high affinity TSPO genotype all had positive associations with overall 11C-PBR28 binding (p′s < 0.01). However, we found no interaction between amyloid status and cognitive status (p = 0.6722).
We then looked at regional effects of amyloid and cognitive status on microglial activation. After correcting for age and TSPO genotype, we found a positive association of both amyloid positivity and cognitive impairment on 11C-PBR28 binding in most target ROIs (Table 2). Amyloid positivity was associated with greater 11C-PBR28 binding in all regions except for the striatum. Cognitive impairment was associated with greater 11C-PBR28 binding in all regions except superior temporal gyrus and striatum. There were no interactions between amyloid status and cognitive status observed in any region. The independence of amyloid-positivity and cognitive impairment resulted in additive effects on 11C-PBR28 binding, such that amyloid-positive patients had greater binding than amyloid-negative controls, with amyloid-positive controls and amyloid-negative patients having intermediate binding (Fig 1A).
Table 2.
Partial volume corrected 11C-PBR28 SUVR values for each subject group, stratified by amyloid and cognitive status.
Region | Aβ (+) patients (n = 23) | Aβ (+) controls (n = 7) | Aβ (−) patients (n = 10) | Aβ (−) controls (n = 17) | Aβ status | Cognitive status | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
F Statistic F(1,52) | P-value | Partial eta squared | F Statistic F(1,52) | P-value | Partial eta squared | |||||
Prefrontal | 1.27 ± 0.08 | 1.21 ± 0.07 | 1.22 ± 0.15 | 1.10 ± 0.17 | 6.32 | 0.0151 | 0.1083 | 7.80 | 0.0073 | 0.1305 |
Mid/Inf temporal | 1.24 ± 0.13 | 1.13 ± 0.06 | 1.17 ± 0.13 | 1.04 ± 0.10 | 7.51 | 0.0084 | 0.1262 | 14.86 | 0.0003* | 0.2223 |
Superior temporal | 1.23 ± 0.08 | 1.19 ± 0.07 | 1.18 ±0.15 | 1.09 ± 0.13 | 7.09 | 0.0103 | 0.1200 | 3.93 | 0.0528 | 0.0702 |
Medial temporal | 1.25 ± 0.18 | 1.15 ± 0.16 | 1.16 ± 0.16 | 1.02 ± 0.13 | 7.13 | 0.0101 | 0.1206 | 9.24 | 0.0037 | 0.1508 |
Posterior cingulate | 1.27 ± 0.11 | 1.13 ± 0.08 | 1.16 ± 0.17 | 1.09 ± 0.12 | 7.08 | 0.0103 | 0.1199 | 10.24 | 0.0023* | 0.1645 |
Superior parietal | 1.29 ± 0.12 | 1.16 ± 0.11 | 1.14 ± 0.14 | 1.04 ± 0.16 | 14.52 | 0.0004* | 0.2183 | 9.78 | 0.0029* | 0.1582 |
Inferior parietal | 1.25 ± 0.11 | 1.11 ± 0.08 | 1.11 ± 0.13 | 1.00 ± 0.15 | 12.50 | 0.0009* | 0.1938 | 11.49 | 0.0013* | 0.1809 |
Striatum | 0.85 ± 0.08 | 0.81 ± 0.07 | 0.86 ± 0.06 | 0.82 ± 0.13 | 0.00 | 0.9621 | 0.0000 | 2.72 | 0.1052 | 0.0497 |
Survives Bonferroni correction of 16 tests (corrected P-value < 0.05)
Fig 1. Jitter plots showing 11C-PBR28 (A) and 18F-MK-6240 binding (B) in middle and inferior temporal gyrus and medial temporal cortex in participants stratified by amyloid status (gray = negative, white = positive) and cognitive status.
For 11C-PBR28, amyloid-positivity and cognitive impairment independently conferred additive effects on binding, such that amyloid-positive patients have greater binding than amyloid-negative controls, with amyloid-negative patients and amyloid-positive patients having intermediate amounts. However, for 18F-MK-6240, greater binding is only seen in amyloid-positive patients.
Increased age was associated with greater 11C-PBR28 binding in middle and inferior temporal gyri, superior temporal gyrus, and medial temporal cortex (p′s < 0.05). TSPO genotype had an effect on 11C-PBR28 binding in middle and inferior temporal gyri, medial temporal cortex, posterior cingulate cortex, superior parietal lobule, and striatum (p′s < 0.05).
The results of the analyses using 11C-PBR28 data uncorrected for partial volume effects generally agreed with those using partial volume-corrected data. Uncorrected SUVRs for each group are reported in Supplementary Table 1.
3.3. Effect of amyloid status and cognitive impairment on 18F-MK-6240 binding
When we tested how tau pathology relates to amyloid and cognitive status, we found that amyloid positivity (p = 0.0007) and cognitive impairment (p = 0.0025) were associated with greater 18F-MK-6240 binding. However, unlike with 11C-PBR28 binding, we found an overall interaction between amyloid positivity and cognitive impairment (p = 0.0115). The interaction between amyloid-positivity and cognitive impairment resulted in a multiplicative effect on 18F-MK6240 binding, such that binding was increased in amyloid-positive patients and low in the other three groups (Fig 1B). There was no effect of age on 18F-MK-6240 binding (p = 0.2987).
When we looked at regional effects of amyloid and cognitive status on tau pathology we found that both amyloid positivity and cognitive impairment were associated with greater 18F-MK-6240 binding in all measured ROIs (p′s < 0.05, Table 3). Positive interactions between amyloid positivity and cognitive impairment were observed in all ROIs (p′s < 0.05) except prefrontal cortex (although an interaction was seen here at trend level with p = 0.0757) and striatum (p = 0.5546). We found no effect of age in any ROI.
Table 3.
Partial volume corrected 18F-MK-6240 SUVR values for each subject group, stratified by amyloid and cognitive status.
Region | Aβ (+) patients (n = 14) | Aβ (+) controls (n = 7) | Aβ (−) patients (n = 10) | Aβ (−) controls (n = 12) | Aβ status | Cognitive status | Aβ status x Cognitive status | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
F Statistic FFFF(1,52) | P-value | Partial eta squared | F Statistic FFFF(1,52) | P-value | Partial eta squared | F Statistic FFFF(1,52) | P-value | Partial eta squared | |||||
Prefrontal | 3.83 ± 2.42 | 1.29 ± 0.24 | 1.26 ± 0.17 | 1.13 ± 0.14 | 7.74 | 0.0084 | 0.1692 | 9.06 | 0.0046 | 0.1925 | 3.34 | 0.0757 | 0.0807 |
Mid/Inf temporal | 4.33 ± 2.03 | 1.76 ± 0.71 | 1.49 ± 0.29 | 1.25 ± 0.14 | 17.76 | 0.0001* | 0.3185 | 13.15 | 0.0008* | 0.2571 | 6.74 | 0.0133 | 0.1507 |
Superior temporal | 3.42 ± 1.65 | 1.35 ± 0.27 | 1.24 ± 0.19 | 1.15 ± 0.15 | 14.03 | 0.0006* | 0.2696 | 12.30 | 0.0012* | 0.2446 | 7.54 | 0.0092 | 0.1656 |
Medial temporal | 3.88 ± 1.11 | 2.04 ± 1.17 | 1.76 ± 0.64 | 1.11 ± 0.31 | 36.04 | <0.0001* | 0.4868 | 22.17 | <0.0001* | 0.3684 | 7.84 | 0.0080 | 0.1711 |
Posterior cingulate | 4.76 ± 2.62 | 1.14 ± 0.12 | 1.18 ± 0.22 | 1.00 ± 0.12 | 12.08 | 0.0013* | 0.2412 | 16.28 | 0.0003* | 0.2999 | 5.40 | 0.0256 | 0.1243 |
Superior parietal | 5.91 ± 3.65 | 1.43 ± 0.25 | 1.38 ± 0.23 | 1.19 ± 0.13 | 10.21 | 0.0028 | 0.2118 | 12.44 | 0.0011* | 0.2466 | 4.29 | 0.0452 | 0.1014 |
Inferior parietal | 5.67 ± 3.10 | 1.45 ± 0.45 | 1.28 ± 0.22 | 1.16 ± 0.16 | 14.02 | 0.0006* | 0.2695 | 14.76 | 0.0005* | 0.2797 | 6.05 | 0.0186 | 0.1374 |
Striatum | 1.11 ± 0.34 | 0.88 ± 0.12 | 0.84 ± 0.18 | 0.71 ± 0.15 | 8.14 | 0.0070 | 0.1764 | 6.30 | 0.0165 | 0.1422 | 0.36 | 0.5546 | 0.0093 |
Survives Bonferroni correction of 16 tests (corrected P-value < 0.05)
As with 11C-PBR28, the results of the analyses using data uncorrected for partial volume effects generally agreed with those using partial volume-corrected data. Uncorrected SUVRs for each group are reported in Supplementary Table 2.
We corrected for MANOVAs performed (testing microglial activation and tau pathology respectively). After multiple comparisons corrections, all significant comparisons survived.
3.4. Microglial activation and tau pathology along the Alzheimer’s clinical spectrum
We next stratified subjects who had 11C-PBR28 PET along the Alzheimer’s clinical spectrum, including amyloid-negative controls (normal aging, n = 17), amyloid-positive controls (early Alzheimer’s pathophysiological change, n = 7), amyloid-positive MCI patients (the clinical prodrome of Alzheimer’s disease, n = 7), and amyloid-positive patients with Alzheimer’s disease dementia (full clinical expression of the disease, n = 16). We found that 11C-PBR28 binding increased in a step-wise manner, particularly in neocortical ROIs (Fig 2A, Table 4). Thus, each disease stage showed greater 11C-PBR28 binding than an earlier disease stage, with temporal and parietal ROIs showing the largest differences.
Fig 2. 11C-PBR28 and 18F-MK-6240 binding stratified across the Alzheimer’s disease continuum.
(A) For 11C-PBR28, binding increases in a step-wise pattern across the continuum in superior parietal lobule, such that increased binding is seen even in amyloid-positive controls. However, in medial temporal cortex, increased binding is seen only in amyloid-positive MCI and AD patients. (B) The opposite pattern is seen for 18F-MK-6240, where increased binding is only seen in amyloid-positive MCI and AD patients in superior parietal lobule. However, in medial temporal cortex, a step-wise pattern is seen, with increased binding in amyloid-positive controls.
Table 4.
11C-PBR28 SUVR values (partial volume corrected) across the clinical Alzheimer’s spectrum.
11C-PBR28 binding (SUVR) | Group-wise comparison* | |||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Region | Aβ (−) controls (n = 17) | Aβ (+) controls (n = 7) | Aβ (+) MCI (n = 7) | Aβ (+) ADD (n = 16) | Aβ (−) controls vs. Aβ (+) controls | Aβ (−) Controls vs. Aβ (+) MCI | Aβ (−) controls vs. Aβ (+) ADD | Aβ (+) controls vs. Aβ (+) MCI | Aβ (+) controls vs. Aβ (+) ADD | Aβ (+) MCI vs. Aβ (+) ADD |
Prefrontal | 1.10 ± 0.17 | 1.21 ± 0.07 | 1.23 ± 0.08 | 1.29 ± 0.08 | −1.9688 (0.0554) | −2.4681 (0.0176) | −4.5680 (<0.0001) | −0.4196 (0.6769) | −1.5601 (0.1261) | −1.0652 (0.2927) |
Mid/inf temporal | 1.04 ± 0.10 | 1.13 ± 0.06 | 1.15 ± 0.07 | 1.28 ± 0.13 | −2.0137 (0.0503) | −2.4234 (0.0197) | −6.6251 (<0.0001) | −0.3442 (0.7324) | −3.0967/ (0.0034) | −2.6907 (0.0101) |
Superior temporal | 1.09 ± 0.13 | 1.19 ± 0.07 | 1.19 ± 0.07 | 1.25 ± 0.08 | −2.2802 (0.0276) | −2.1303 (0.0389) | −4.3732 (<0.0001) | −0.1260 (0.9003) | −1.1017 (0.2767) | −1.2503 (0.2180) |
Medial temporal | 1.02 ± 0.13 | 1.15 ± 0.16 | 1.20 ± 0.17 | 1.27 ± 0.19 | −1.7648 (0.0847) | −2.4803 (0.0171) | −4.4196 (<0.0001) | −0.6011 (0.5509) | −1.6481 (0.1066) | −0.9391 (0.3529) |
Posterior cingulate | 1.09 ± 0.12 | 1.13 ± 0.08 | 1.21 ± 0.13 | 1.30 ± 0.10 | −0.6557 (0.5155) | −2.3127 (0.0256) | −5.4455 (<0.0001) | −1.3922 (0.1710) | −3.5358 (0.0010) | −1.8937 (0.0650) |
Superior parietal | 1.04 ± 0.16 | 1.16 ± 0.11 | 1.25 ± 0.14 | 1.30 ± 0.11 | −2.0893 (0.0426) | −3.6251 (0.0008) | −5.7561 (<0.0001) | −1.2904 (0.2038) | −2.3538 (0.0232) | −0.8318 (0.4101) |
Inferior parietal | 1.00 ± 0.15 | 1.11 ± 0.08 | 1.19 ± 0.11 | 1.27 ± 0.11 | −1.9123 (0.0625) | −3.4927 (0.0011) | −6.4202 (<0.0001) | −1.3278 (0.1913) | −3.0397 (0.0040) | −1.4735 (0.1479) |
Striatum | 0.82 ± 0.13 | 0.81 ± 0.07 | 0.83 ± 0.08 | 0.86 ± 0.08 | −0.1426 (0.8873) | −0.2398 (0.8117) | −1.2434 (0.2205) | −0.3212 (0.7496) | −1.0970 (0.2787) | −0.7181 (0.4766) |
Results given as t statistics, with P values in parentheses.
We then stratified subjects who had 18F-MK-6240 PET along the Alzheimer’s clinical spectrum, including amyloid-negative controls (n = 12), amyloid-positive controls (n = 7), amyloid-positive MCI patients (n = 4), and amyloid-positive patients with Alzheimer’s disease dementia (n = 10). We found that 18F-MK-6240 binding increased along the Alzheimer’s disease spectrum; however, the pattern was different from that seen with 11C-PBR28 (Fig 2B, Table 5). The earliest Alzheimer’s pathophysiological change was associated with increase in 18F-MK-6240 binding in medial temporal cortex only, with increase in neocortical binding seen only in the MCI and AD groups.
Table 5.
18F-MK-6240 SUVR values (partial volume corrected) across the clinical Alzheimer’s spectrum.
18F-MK-6240 binding (SUVR) | Group-wise comparison* | |||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Region | Aβ (−) controls (n = 12) | Aβ (+) controls (n = 7) | Aβ (+) MCI (n = 4) | Aβ (+) ADD (n = 10) | Aβ (−) controls vs. Aβ (+) controls | Aβ (−) Controls vs. Aβ (+) MCI | Aβ (−) controls vs. Aβ (+) ADD | Aβ (+) controls vs. Aβ (+) MCI | Aβ (+) controls vs. Aβ (+) ADD | Aβ (+) MCI vs. Aβ (+) ADD |
Prefrontal | 1.13 ± 0.14 | 1.29 ± 0.24 | 2.22 ± 0.95 | 4.43 ± 2.54 | −0.2401 (0.8120) | −1.2986 (0.2043) | −5.2919 (<0.0001) | −1.0140) (0.3190) | −4.3662 (0.0001) | −2.5627 (0.0158) |
Mid/inf temporal | 1.25 ± 0.14 | 1.76 ± 0.71 | 2.91 ± 1.07 | 4.88 ± 2.06 | −0.8609 (0.3963) | −2.3011 (0.0288) | −6.7992 (<0.0001) | −1.4663 (0.1533) | −5.0766 (<0.0001) | −2.6753 (0.0121) |
Superior temporal | 1.15 ± 0.16 | 1.35 ± 0.27 | 2.59 ± 1.05 | 3.72 ± 1.77 | −0.3977 (0.6938) | −2.3791 (0.0242) | −5.7029 (<0.0001) | −1.8897 (0.0688) | −4.5712 (<0.0001) | −1.8057 (0.0814) |
Medial temporal | 1.11 ± 0.31 | 2.04 ± 1.17 | 3.58 ± 0.17 | 3.98 ± 1.30 | −2.1327 (0.0415) | −4.6544 (<0.0001) | −7.3027 (<0.0001) | −2.6691 (0.0123) | −4.2868 (0.0002) | −0.7431 (0.4634) |
Posterior cingulate | 1.00 ± 0.12 | 1.14 ± 0.12 | 2.99 ± 1.96 | 5.44 ± 2.56 | −0.1855 (0.85) | −2.2030 (0.0357) | −6.6353 (<0.0001) | −1.8885 (0.0690) | −5.5861 (<0.0001) | −2.6523 (0.0128) |
Superior parietal | 1.19 ± 0.13 | 1.43 ± 0.25 | 4.94 ± 4.14 | 6.21 ± 3.54 | −0.2139 (0.8322) | −2.7228 (0.0108) | −4.9176 (<0.0001) | −2.3458 (0.0260) | −4.0663 (0.0003) | −0.9019 (0.3745) |
Inferior parietal | 1.16 ± 0.15 | 1.45 ± 0.45 | 4.36 ± 3.20 | 6.15 ± 3.03 | −0.3085 (0.7599) | −2.7770 (0.0095) | −5.8532 (<0.0001) | −2.3239 (0.0273) | −4.7878 (<0.0001) | −1.5262 (0.1378) |
Striatum | 0.71 ± 0.15 | 0.88 ± 0.12 | 0.90 ± 0.23 | 1.20 ± 0.35 | −1.4756 (0.1508) | −1.3644 (0.1829) | −4.8862 (<0.0001) | −0.1371 (0.8919) | −2.8213 (0.0085) | −2.2049 (0.0355) |
Results given as t statistics, with P values in parentheses.
Correlations between 11C-PBR28 binding and MMSE score showed that 11C-PBR28 binding was negatively associated with MMSE score in all ROIs except striatum (p’s < 0.01) when all participants were combined. When we stratified participants based on amyloid status, this negative correlation remained for amyloid-positive subjects in each ROI except medial temporal cortex (p′s < 0.05) (Fig 3A, Supplementary Fig 1). 11C-PBR28 binding did not correlate with MMSE score when only amyloid-negative participants were included.
Fig 3. Both 11C-PBR28 and 18F-MK-62420 binding negatively correlate with Mini Mental State Exam score.
Scatter plots for inferior parietal lobule with best-fit lines are shown for 11C-PBR28 (A) and 18F-MK-6240 (B). Significant correlations are seen in amyloid-positive (red) but not amyloid-negative subjects (blue) for both radioligands. Closed circles = impaired patients, open circles = cognitively normal controls. Partial Pearson correlation coefficients (corrected for age and in the case of 11C-PBR28 TSPO genotype) and P-values are shown. Scatter plots for other ROIs are included in Supplementary Fig 1.
Similar to 11C-PBR28, when all subjects who had 18F-MK-6240 PET were included (n = 43), binding negatively correlated with MMSE score in all ROIs, including striatum (p’s < 0.01). Negative correlations were seen among amyloid-positive subjects in all ROIs (p’s < 0.05) and no correlations were seen in amyloid-negative subjects when considered separately (Fig 3B, Supplementary Fig 2). Correlation analyses using 11C-PBR28 and 18F-MK-6240 data uncorrected for partial volume effects followed a similar pattern (Supplementary Fig 3 and 4).
3.5. Correlation between microglial activation and tau deposition
For subjects with both 18F-MK-6240 PET and 11C-PBR28 PET performed (n = 42), we performed a correlation between these radioligands to look for regional relationships between TSPO and tau. Using the partial volume corrected SUVR data, we found a significant positive correlation between 18F-MK-6240 and 11C-PBR28 binding in every region except the striatum (p’s < 0.005). Results from correlation analysis using imaging data uncorrected for partial volume effects followed a similar pattern (Supplementary Table 3).
4. DISCUSSION
We demonstrated that amyloid positivity and cognitive impairment are independently associated with 11C-PBR28 binding. These results suggest that increased microglial activation co-occurs with amyloid plaque deposition in the absence of cognitive impairment and with cognitive impairment in the absence of amyloidosis. Since cognitive impairment is the clinical result of neurodegeneration, our results argue that microglial activation could be both a specific response to amyloid plaque deposition in early stages of Alzheimer’s disease and a non-specific response to neurodegeneration in both Alzheimer’s and non-Alzheimer’s dementias. In contrast, amyloid and impairment interact in their association with 18F-MK-6240 binding, such that binding is synergistically increased in amyloid-positive patients but not increased in amyloid-negative patients. These results suggest that tau pathology is most often seen when both amyloid and cognitive impairment are both already present. That 11C-PBR28 and 18F-MK-6240 binding negatively correlate with MMSE score among amyloid-positive subjects suggests a unidirectional increase in both microglial activation and tau pathology throughout the progression of Alzheimer’s disease.
We also found that 11C-PBR28 and 18F-MK-6240 showed different patterns of binding along the Alzheimer’s disease clinical spectrum. 11C-PBR28 binding increased in a step-wise pattern in temporo-parietal neocortex, starting in amyloid-positive controls. In contrast, 18F-MK-6240 binding was increased in amyloid-positive controls only in medial temporal cortex, with neocortical increases only seen in MCI and Alzheimer’s disease patients. These results seem to agree with earlier observations with 18F-AV-1451 that showed amyloid-positive controls were more likely than amyloid-negative controls to have high tau PET signal, particularly in medial temporal regions (Lowe et al., 2017). Notably, in our study we found an interaction between amyloid positivity and cognitive impairment on 18F-MK-6240 binding in the medial temporal cortex (p < 0.01), suggesting that even in this region increased tau pathology is related to antecedent amyloid deposition. These results suggest a possible temporal order of Alzheimer’s pathophysiology beginning with deposition of amyloid plaque, subsequent microglial activation in the neocortex, and increased tau pathology in the medial temporal cortex. These events are then followed by the appearance of increased tau pathology in neocortex and concurrent onset of memory impairment. Alternatively, it is possible that neuroinflammation and tau deposition occur at the same time, as 18F-MK6240 may be less sensitive than 11C-PBR28 in detecting small changes in pathology. In either case, these cross-sectional findings require confirmation in a longitudinal study as inferences about temporal order cannot be made in a cross-sectional study.
That amyloid positivity and memory impairment are independently associated with increased 11C-PBR28 binding is consistent with distinct roles of microglial activation at different time points: an early response to amyloidosis and a later response to neurodegeneration, the latter not necessarily specific to Alzheimer’s pathogenesis (Sierra et al., 2013). Increased TSPO binding in amyloid-positive controls has been previously reported (Hamelin et al., 2016). Downstream response of microglia to neurodegeneration has been demonstrated by their activation in the presence of axonal injury (Tanaka et al., 2009), and would explain increased TSPO binding in a variety of neurodegenerative diseases (Schain and Kreisl, 2017). However, imaging studies alone cannot determine the functional role of microglia in Alzheimer’s disease. Whether microglial activation plays a protective or pathogenic role in the preclinical stage remains unclear, as both phagocytic behavior and release of neurotoxic cytokines have been observed by microglia in response to β-amyloid species in vitro (D’Andrea et al., 2004; Maezawa et al., 2011). While some investigators have posited a bimodal relationship between TSPO and Alzheimer’s disease progression (Calsolaro and Edison, 2016; Fan et al., 2017; Hamelin et al., 2016), the independent associations of 11C-PBR28 binding with amyloid positivity and cognitive impairment, and apparent linear increase in 11C-PBR28 binding along the disease spectrum, suggest a unimodal increase in overall microglial activation throughout the Alzheimer’s process.
The underlying pathologies of the included amyloid-negative subjects with cognitive impairment are not known, but no participant had cardinal symptoms of dementia with Lewy bodes, frontotemporal dementia, progressive supranuclear palsy, corticobasal syndrome, or vascular dementia. Our amyloid-negative patients, on average, had smaller hippocampal volumes than our amyloid-negative controls, suggesting these patients in general had an Alzheimer’s-like pattern of neurodegeneration. Hippocampal sclerosis and argyrophilic grain disease are two Alzheimer’s disease mimics potentially represented among patients lacking Alzheimer’s pathophysiology (Jicha et al., 2006). Because cognitive impairment was associated with increased 11C-PBR28 binding in the absence of amyloid pathophysiology, TSPO PET could potentially be used as a disease marker in non-Alzheimer amnestic disorders for which no in vivo molecular biomarkers yet exist.
To our knowledge, this is the first study to use 11C-PBR28 to measure microglial activation and 18F-MK-6240 to measure tau pathology in the same participants. Prior studies used 11C-(R)-PK 11195 to measure TSPO binding (Parbo et al., 2018) and/or 18F-AV-1451 to measure tau binding (Dani et al., 2018). 11C-PBR28 has greater specific-to-nonspecific binding than 11C-(R)-PK 11195 (Fujita et al., 2017; Kreisl et al., 2010); has been validated in AD subjects using a 30 min imaging window (Lyoo et al., 2015); has demonstrated longitudinal increases in Alzheimer’s disease (Kreisl et al., 2016); and co-localizes to neurodegeneration in vivo (Kreisl et al., 2017b) and microglial activation ex vivo (Kreisl et al., 2017a). Based on our prior study (Lyoo et al., 2015), SUVR measurement of 11C-PBR28 binding results in greater statistical power than kinetic modeling using the arterial input function in detecting differences between AD patients and controls. Therefore, the SUVR approach with 11C-PBR28 may be preferred over full quantification for detecting small differences in 11C-PBR28 binding, such as that seen between amyloid-negative and amyloid-positive control groups. We recently demonstrated that using supervised clustering analysis also resulted in improved ability to detect increased 11C-PBR28 binding in AD patients compared to kinetic modeling with the arterial input function (Fregonara et al., 2019). Although using SUVR has not been directly compared to using SVCA, either approach appears to be a suitable alternative to arterial sampling, at least in cross sectional studies. The SUVR approach has the additional advantage of shorter imaging times (and therefore reduced participant burden).
18F-MK-6240 appears to have less off-target binding in choroid plexus and basal ganglia than 18F-AV-1451 (Betthauser et al., 2019). Moreover, the two studies using 18F-AV-1451 showed somewhat discordant results, with one showing a correlation between these two radioligands (Dani et al., 2018) and the other finding no correlation (Parbo et al., 2018). Interestingly, our results are more in line with that of Dani et al., which similarly used 11C-PBR28 for TSPO imaging. Using 11C-PBR28 and 18F-MK-6240 together may have increased the sensitivity of detecting increased TSPO and tau pathology in our study.
Correction for partial volume effects is an important consideration in AD studies - as atrophy of the cerebral cortex exacerbates the partial volume effects from CSF inherent in PET imaging - and may be a more accurate representation of true radioligand activity (Su et al., 2015). While corrected data resulted in larger effect sizes, the direction of the relationships between radioligand binding and the comparative outcome variable remained the same regardless of whether partial volume correction (PVC) was applied (see Supplementary Information). In addition, that we still found correlations between 11C-PBR28 and 18F-MK-6240 and MMSE score in amyloid-positive subjects in partial volume uncorrected image data argues that our results were not simply due to over-estimation caused by PVC.
Our conclusions are somewhat limited by our sample size. While we obtained PET imaging in 58 participants overall, only 10 amyloid-negative patients and seven amyloid-positive controls had 11C-PBR28 PET imaging. Also, the smaller number of participants in these two groups reflects the lower likelihood of amyloid-negativity in amnestic patients with AD patterns of neurodegeneration and of amyloid-positivity in controls, particularly given the relatively young ages included (69.2 ± 4.3 years for controls overall). While enrolling older participants is expected to increase the prevalence of amyloid-positivity in controls (Chetelat et al., 2013), this approach would also increase the prevalence of non-Alzheimer co-pathology in the amyloid-positive patient group. The young age of amyloid-positive patient group (65.8 ± 8.4 years) reduces the likelihood that concomitant neurodegenerative disorders influenced our results. That 11C-PBR28 binding is influenced by TSPO genotype introduces an element of variability in the analysis, and is a shortcoming of this radioligand. However, excluding low affinity binders and including TSPO genotype in the statistical analysis, as performed in this study, has been shown to overcome the confounding effects of this nuisance variable (Kreisl et al, 2013). Another limitation of our study is that only a subset of participants underwent 18F-MK-6240 PET. However, the large dynamic range of this radioligand and large effects sizes seen in ours and prior studies (Betthauser et al., 2019; Lohith et al., 2019; Pascoal et al., 2018) argue that even modestly-sized studies have sufficient power to see group differences in 18F-MK-6240 binding.
In conclusion, amyloid positivity and memory impairment are independently related to 11C-PBR28 binding. In contrast, amyloid and impairment interact in their relationship with 18F-MK-6240 binding.
Supplementary Material
HIGHLIGHTS.
Microglial activation is independently related to amyloid-positivity and memory impairment.
Amyloid and impairment interact in their relationship with tau pathology.
Neuroinflammation increases throughout the Alzheimer’s disease clinical spectrum.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
18F-Florbetaben was supplied by Life Molecular Imaging. 18F-MK-6240 was supplied by Cerveau Technologies. TSPO genotyping was performed by Regina Santella, PhD and the Columbia University Biomarkers Shared Resource. The authors wish to acknowledge the faculty and staff at the Irving Institute Clinical Research Resource, the MRI Center, and the David A. Gardner PET Imaging Research Center at the Columbia University Irving Medical Center for their contributions to this work.
FUNDING
This study was supported by National Institutes of Health grants K23AG052633 (WCK), R01AG026158 (YS) and R56AG034189 (AMB). This study was supported by Columbia University’s CTSA grant (UL1 TR000040), and the Columbia University Alzheimer’s Disease Research Center (P50AG008702). Data collection and sharing for this project was supported by the Washington Heights-Inwood Columbia Aging Project (WHICAP, P01AG07232, R01AG037212, RF1AG054023). This manuscript has been reviewed by WHICAP investigators for scientific content and consistency of data interpretation with previous WHICAP Study publications. We acknowledge the WHICAP study participants and the WHICAP research and support staff for their contributions to this study.
Abbreviations:
- MCI
mild cognitive impairment
- TSPO
18 kDa translocator protein
- SRT-DR
Selective Reminding Test – Delayed Recall
- MMSE
Mini Mental State Exam
- ROI
region of interest
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
COMPETING INTERESTS
Dr. Kreisl is a consultant for Cerveau Technologies. However, Cereveau was not involved in the design or execution of this study or in the interpretation of the results.
VERIFICATION
We verify that no other authors have any personal or financial conflicts of interest that would inappropriately influence the contents of this research. We additionally verify that our institution has no contracts or financial interests related to the outcomes of this research, and no other organization stands to gain financially now or in the future from the results of this research.
All subjects (or their representative) provided informed consent according to the Declaration of Helsinki and this study was approved by the CUIMC Institutional Review Board.
All authors have reviewed this manuscript, validate the accuracy of the data, and approve of its submission. The contents of this manuscript have not been published elsewhere, and have not and will not be submitted elsewhere while under consideration by Neurobiology of Aging.
REFERENCES
- Albert MS, et al. , 2011. The diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment due to Alzheimer’s disease: recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement 7(3), 270–279. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Betthauser TJ, et al. , 2019. In Vivo Characterization and Quantification of Neurofibrillary Tau PET Radioligand (18)F-MK-6240 in Humans from Alzheimer Disease Dementia to Young Controls. J Nucl Med 60(1), 93–99. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Briard E, et al. , 2008. Synthesis and evaluation in monkey of two sensitive 11C-labeled aryloxyanilide ligands for imaging brain peripheral benzodiazepine receptors in vivo. J Med Chem 51(1), 17–30. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bullich S, et al. , 2017. Optimized classification of (18)F-Florbetaben PET scans as positive and negative using an SUVR quantitative approach and comparison to visual assessment. Neuroimage Clin 15, 325–332. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cagnin A, et al. , 2004. In vivo detection of microglial activation in frontotemporal dementia. Ann Neurol 56(6), 894–897. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Calsolaro V, Edison P, 2016. Neuroinflammation in Alzheimer’s disease: Current evidence and future directions. Alzheimers Dement 12(6), 719–732. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chetelat G, et al. , 2013. Amyloid imaging in cognitively normal individuals, at-risk populations and preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. Neuroimage Clin 2, 356–365. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Colonna M, Wang Y, 2016. TREM2 variants: new keys to decipher Alzheimer disease pathogenesis. Nat Rev Neurosci 17(4), 201–207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Corcia P, et al. , 2012. Molecular imaging of microglial activation in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. PLoS One 7(12), e52941. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- D’Andrea MR, et al. , 2004. The microglial phagocytic role with specific plaque types in the Alzheimer disease brain. Neurobiol Aging 25(5), 675–683. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dani M, et al. , 2018. Microglial activation correlates in vivo with both tau and amyloid in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain 141(9), 2740–2754. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Edison P, et al. , 2008. Microglia, amyloid, and cognition in Alzheimer’s disease: An [11C](R)PK11195-PET and [11C]PIB-PET study. Neurobiol Dis 32(3), 412–419. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fan Z, et al. , 2017. An early and late peak in microglial activation in Alzheimer’s disease trajectory. Brain 140(3), 792–803. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Folstein MF, et al. , 1983. The mini-mental state examination. Archives of general psychiatry 40(7), 812–812. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fregonara PZ, et al. , 2019. Automatic extraction of a reference region for the noninvasive quantification of translocator protein in brain using 11C-PBR28. Journal of Nuclear Medicine, jnumed-118. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Fujita M, et al. , 2017. Comparison of four (11)C-labeled PET ligands to quantify translocator protein 18 kDa (TSPO) in human brain: (R)-PK11195, PBR28, DPA-713, and ER176-based on recent publications that measured specific-to-non-displaceable ratios. EJNMMI Res 7(1), 84. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gerhard A, et al. , 2006. In vivo imaging of microglial activation with [11C](R)-PK11195 PET in idiopathic Parkinson’s disease. Neurobiol Dis 21(2), 404–412. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hamelin L, et al. , 2016. Early and protective microglial activation in Alzheimer’s disease: a prospective study using 18F-DPA-714 PET imaging. Brain 139(Pt 4), 1252–1264. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hostetler ED, et al. , 2016. Preclinical Characterization of 18F-MK-6240, a Promising PET Tracer for In Vivo Quantification of Human Neurofibrillary Tangles. J Nucl Med 57(10), 1599–1606. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hughes CP, et al. , 1982. A new clinical scale for the staging of dementia. The British journal of psychiatry 140(6), 566–572. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jack CR Jr., et al. , 2013. Tracking pathophysiological processes in Alzheimer’s disease: an updated hypothetical model of dynamic biomarkers. Lancet Neurol 12(2), 207–216. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jack CR Jr., et al. , 2018. Longitudinal tau PET in ageing and Alzheimer’s disease. Brain 141(5), 1517–1528. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Janelidze S, et al. , 2018. CSF biomarkers of neuroinflammation and cerebrovascular dysfunction in early Alzheimer disease. Neurology 91(9), e867–e877. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jicha GA, et al. , 2006. Neuropathologic outcome of mild cognitive impairment following progression to clinical dementia. Arch Neurol 63(5), 674–681. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kreisl WC, et al. , 2010. Comparison of [(11)C]-(R)-PK 11195 and [(11)C]PBR28, two radioligands for translocator protein (18 kDa) in human and monkey: Implications for positron emission tomographic imaging of this inflammation biomarker. Neuroimage 49(4), 2924–2932. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kreisl WC, et al. , 2013. A genetic polymorphism for translocator protein 18 kDa affects both in vitro and in vivo radioligand binding in human brain to this putative biomarker of neuroinflammation. Journal of Cerebral Blood Flow & Metabolism 33(1), 53–58. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kreisl WC, et al. , 2017a. (11)C-PBR28 PET detects translocator protein in a patient with astrocytoma and Alzheimer disease. Neurology 88(10), 1001–1004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kreisl WC, et al. , 2017b. Distinct patterns of increased translocator protein in posterior cortical atrophy and amnestic Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol Aging 51, 132–140. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kreisl WC, et al. , 2016. (11)C-PBR28 binding to translocator protein increases with progression of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol Aging 44, 53–61. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kreisl WC, et al. , 2013. In vivo radioligand binding to translocator protein correlates with severity of Alzheimer’s disease. Brain 136(Pt 7), 2228–2238. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Landau SM, et al. , 2016. Amyloid negativity in patients with clinically diagnosed Alzheimer disease and MCI. Neurology 86(15), 1377–1385. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lohith TG, et al. , 2019. Brain Imaging of Alzheimer Dementia Patients and Elderly Controls with (18)F-MK-6240, a PET Tracer Targeting Neurofibrillary Tangles. J Nucl Med 60(1), 107–114. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lowe VJ, et al. , 2017. Widespread brain tau and its association with ageing, Braak stage and Alzheimer’s dementia. Brain 141(1), 271–287. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lyoo CH, et al. , 2015. Cerebellum Can Serve As a Pseudo-Reference Region in Alzheimer Disease to Detect Neuroinflammation Measured with PET Radioligand Binding to Translocator Protein. J Nucl Med 56(5), 701–706. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maezawa I, et al. , 2011. Amyloid-beta protein oligomer at low nanomolar concentrations activates microglia and induces microglial neurotoxicity. J Biol Chem 286(5), 3693–3706. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marlatt MW, et al. , 2014. Proliferation in the Alzheimer hippocampus is due to microglia, not astroglia, and occurs at sites of amyloid deposition. Neural Plast 2014, 693851. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McKhann GM, et al. , 2011. The diagnosis of dementia due to Alzheimer’s disease: recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement 7(3), 263–269. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Owen DR, et al. , 2012. An 18-kDa translocator protein (TSPO) polymorphism explains differences in binding affinity of the PET radioligand PBR28. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 32(1), 1–5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Parbo P, et al. , 2018. Does inflammation precede tau aggregation in early Alzheimer’s disease? A PET study. Neurobiol Dis 117, 211–216. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pascoal TA, et al. , 2018. In vivo quantification of neurofibrillary tangles with [(18)F]MK-6240. Alzheimers Res Ther 10(1), 74. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petersen RC, et al. , 2013. Mild cognitive impairment due to Alzheimer disease in the community. Ann Neurol 74(2), 199–208. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pike KE, et al. , 2011. Cognition and beta-amyloid in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease: data from the AIBL study. Neuropsychologia 49(9), 2384–2390. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ruff RM, et al. , 1989. Selective reminding tests: A normative study of verbal learning in adults. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology 11(4), 539–550. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schain M, Kreisl WC, 2017. Neuroinflammation in Neurodegenerative Disorders-a Review. Curr Neurol Neurosci Rep 17(3), 25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schultz SA, et al. , 2018. Widespread distribution of tauopathy in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol Aging 72, 177–185. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sierra A, et al. , 2013. Janus-faced microglia: beneficial and detrimental consequences of microglial phagocytosis. Front Cell Neurosci 7, 6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Su Y, et al. , 2015. Partial volume correction in quantitative amyloid imaging. Neuroimage 107, 55–64. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tanaka T, et al. , 2009. Engulfment of axon debris by microglia requires p38 MAPK activity. J Biol Chem 284(32), 21626–21636. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thomas BA, et al. , 2011. The importance of appropriate partial volume correction for PET quantification in Alzheimer’s disease. Eur J Nucl Med Mol Imaging 38(6), 1104–1119. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.