Skip to main content
Alzheimer's Research & Therapy logoLink to Alzheimer's Research & Therapy
. 2019 Dec 21;11:108. doi: 10.1186/s13195-019-0568-y

Pay attention to the basal ganglia: a volumetric study in early dementia with Lewy bodies

Anne Botzung 1,, Nathalie Philippi 1,2, Vincent Noblet 2, Paulo Loureiro de Sousa 2, Frédéric Blanc 1,2
PMCID: PMC6925479  PMID: 31864422

Abstract

Background

Cortical and subcortical cognitive impairments are usually found in dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB). Roughly, they comprise visuo-constructive/executive function and attention/processing speed impairments, whereas memory would remain relatively spared. In this study, we focused on the neuro-anatomical substrates of attention and processing speed, which is still poorly understood. For the purpose of the study, we examined the correlations between behavioral scores measuring the speed of processing and the degree of cerebral atrophy in patients with prodromal to moderate DLB.

Methods

Ninety-three prodromal to moderate DLB patients (mean MMSE = 25.5) were selected to participate in the study as well as 28 healthy elderly subjects (mean MMSE = 28.9), matched in terms of age and educational level. The Trail Making Test A (TMTA) and the Digit Symbol Substitution Test (DSST) were used to assess attention and processing speed. Behavioral performances were compared between patients and healthy control subjects. Three-dimensional MRI images were acquired for all participants, and correlational analyses were performed in the patient group using voxel-based morphometry (VBM).

Results

The behavioral results on both the TMTA (p = .026) and the DSST (p < .001) showed significantly impaired performances in patients in comparison with control subjects. In addition, correlational analyses using VBM revealed for the TMTA negative correlations in the caudate nucleus (left cluster peak significant at .05 FWE corrected), the putamen, the left thalamus, and the subthalamic nuclei (p < .05 FDR corrected). Some positive correlations associated with the DSST were found in the right inferior frontal gyrus, the left thalamus, and the left cerebellum (p < .001 uncorrected).

Conclusions

The behavioral results are in line with the literature on the DLB cognitive profile and confirm the existence of attention and processing speed impairment. Interestingly, VBM analysis revealed the involvement of the basal ganglia, in particular, the left caudate nucleus, which is part of the attention cerebral network, suggesting an important role of this structure for attentional processing speed. This also suggests the clinical implication of damage in this region relatively early in the course of the disease.

Keywords: Dementia with Lewy bodies, Prodromal, Mild cognitive impairment, Attention, Processing speed, Neuroimaging, MRI, VBM, Basal ganglia

Background

Dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB) is the second most common form of neurodegenerative disease after Alzheimer’s disease (AD), with prevalence rates up to 5% in the elderly population and up to around 20% of all cases of dementia [1, 2]. According to the most recent consensus criteria [3], a diagnosis of probable DLB can be made if two or more core clinical features are present, among fluctuating cognition, recurrent visual hallucinations, rapid eye movement sleep behavior disorder (RBD), and spontaneous features of parkinsonism. But essential to the diagnosis of DLB remains the progressive cognitive decline, with deficits on tests of attention, executive functions, and visuo-perception in the foreground, which may occur early [4, 5]. Among these deficits, attentional dysfunction, which is typically assessed in clinical routine by measures of speed of processing, is a prominent and distinguishing neuropsychological feature of DLB as compared to AD at the prodromal stages [4, 6]. For instance, based upon a large prospective study comparing DLB with AD, the slowing of cognitive processing (i.e., deficits of cognitive reaction time) appears to be specific to DLB in the early stages of the disease [7]. Tasks of attention and processing speed requiring a graphomotor response seem to be particularly useful in discriminating DLB from normal status or versus AD [6]. Previous studies have also indicated a disruption of attention in the visual modality in DLB as compared to AD and Parkinson’s disease (PD) patients [811].

Attention is a broad concept and, accordingly, many supporting brain regions are involved. Current neuroanatomical descriptions mostly come from the literature on fMRI in normal subjects and are drawn in a network perspective: in the context of attention-consuming tasks, the dorsal attention network (DAN) and the salience network (SN) both exhibit increased activity whereas the default network (DN), including regions of the cortical midline, is disengaged [12, 13]. The DAN encompasses the frontal eye field and inferior parietal sulcus and is involved in directed attention and working memory (Corbetta and Shulman [14]). The SN involves the anterior insula (AI), which plays a prominent role in the detection of salient stimuli, and the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC), more involved in task control [15]. The subcortical areas, including the thalamus, striatum, superior colliculus, and connecting white matter tracts, also participate in these networks [16].

Interestingly, structural neuroimaging studies using voxel-based morphometry (VBM) in patients with DLB, relative to AD patients, PD patients, and/or normal controls, have revealed gray matter (GM) atrophy in some of the abovementioned attentional regions. If relative to AD patients, DLB patients show a relative preservation of medial temporal lobe volumes [1720]; compiling VBM studies of DLB patients versus control subjects revealed consistent decreased GM volumes in the right lateral temporal/insular cortex and left lenticular nucleus (putamen and globus pallidus)/insular cortex [21]. These data are at least partially corroborated by studies conducted in our team with DLB patients versus controls, showing reduced GM volumes of the bilateral insula and anterior cingulate cortex in prodromal DLB (pro-DLB) using VBM [22], reduced GM volumes of the bilateral insula in pro-DLB using DARTEL-VBM, and diminished right insula and right orbito-frontal cortex volumes in pro-DLB using measures of cortical thinning [23]. Additionally, reduction of GM density has been shown in the occipito-parietal areas and bilateral lenticular nucleus in DLB versus PD [24], in the left caudate nucleus in DLB versus controls [25], in the bilateral thalamus in DLB versus controls [26], and in the bilateral putamen in DLB versus controls and AD [27].

So far, the question of the cerebral regions involved in attention deficits in DLB patients has been investigated in a few papers only. In an fMRI study, Firbank at al [28]. have shown in Lewy body disease (LBD) patients (DLB and PD) greater activation of the DAN for incongruent and more difficult trials as well as heightened deactivation of the default network, interpreted as an attempt to allocate resources to impaired attentional networks. Using functional connectivity analysis in the same cohort of patients, Koboleva et al. [29] found a decreased connectivity between the DAN and the ventral regions. Based on the same cohort again, Cromarty et al. [30], in a VBM study, did not show any significant correlations between attentional performance, despite deficits in the tasks, and GM volumes, suggesting, according to the authors, that these effects were unlikely due to region-specific structural deficits. On the other hand, Watson et al. [26] found in a pure group of DLB patients that impaired attentional function (measured by simple and choice reaction times) was correlated to the left thalamic regions (pulvinar and ventral lateral nucleus). Overall, these studies have not so far provided a clear picture of the regions involved in the attentional deficits of DLB patients.

The aim of the present study was to better understand the underlying structural mechanisms of the attentional deficit in DLB patients. Based on the fact that both the insular cortex and basal ganglia (caudate and thalamus) are important to attention function, that both areas are highly interconnected [31, 32], and given that these regions are atrophied at an early stage in DLB patients, we posit that attention disturbances in DLB might be related to diminished volume in these regions. To test this hypothesis, we took advantage of a unique cohort of 93 patients diagnosed with prodromal to moderate DLB, who underwent a full neuropsychological assessment, including attentional measures, as well as structural MRI.

Methods

Participants

Ninety-three prodromal to moderate DLB patients were selected to participate in the study as well as 28 healthy elderly control subjects (HCs), matched in terms of age and educational level (see Table 1). Patients were recruited from the tertiary memory clinic of Strasbourg University Hospital, France, including the geriatrics and neurology departments. HCs were recruited among friends and relatives of the patients or via the listing of controls of the local clinical investigation center. In all patients, the diagnosis was made by clinicians with expertise in dementia, who performed a complete anamnesis and medical and neurologic examination, encompassing the 4 core features of McKeith et al.’s criteria [3]. Features of parkinsonism were evaluated using the Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale (UPDRS, part 3): akinesia, rigidity, and tremor at rest (rated from 0 for no symptoms to 4 for serious symptoms). Fluctuations were assessed with the Mayo Clinic Fluctuations scale [33] and the Newcastle upon Tyne Clinician Assessment of Fluctuation scale [34]. The Parkinson’s disease-associated psychotic symptoms questionnaire [35] was used to evaluate the presence of hallucinations. RBD was evaluated using a sleep questionnaire on RBD from the publication by Gjerstad et al. [36], simplified into 4 questions for the patient and the caregiver: one concerning movements during sleep, the second concerning vivid dreams and nightmares. Patients with prodromal DLB were defined as patients with MCI and preservation of independence (assessed by the Instrumental Activities of Daily Living [37, 38], DSM-5 [39], and McKeith’s criteria [1, 3] meeting probable DLB criteria except for the presence of dementia).

Table 1.

Demographic and clinical characteristics of dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB) patients and healthy control subjects

Characteristics DLB patients (N = 93) Controls (N = 28) p/χ2 value
Age, years 70.3 (9.7) 67.6 (7.8) p = .189
Education, years 11.3 (4.1) 12.9 (2.7) p = .063
Sex, M/F 52/41 16/12 χ2 = .908
Handedness, R/L 83/10 26/2 χ2 = .575
Fluctuations (%) 49/93 (52.7) 1/28 χ2< .001
Hallucinationsa (%) 56/91 (61.5) 3/26 χ2< .001
Parkinsonism
 Akinesia (%)b 58/90 (64.4) 0/28 χ2< .001
 Rigidity (%)b 60/90 (66.7) 0/28 χ2< .001
 Tremor at rest (%)c 17/85 (20) 2/28 χ2 = .115

Standard deviations for age and education are shown in parentheses. Significant p and χ2 values are in italics

aData missing for two patients

bData missing for three patients and two controls

cData missing for eight patients

Exclusion criteria for all participants included a history of alcohol/substance abuse, evidence suggesting alternative neurological or psychiatric explanations for symptoms/cognitive impairment (for patients), or the presence of other severe or unstable medical illnesses. Patients or controls with contraindications to MRI were excluded. All participants also underwent a large battery of neuropsychological tests (see Kemp et al. [4] for a complete description of assessments) in addition to the MMSE [40] and to the 2 attention tasks (see below). On the basis of the MMSE scores, 70 patients were at the prodromal stage, 17 patients were at a mild stage of dementia, and 6 patients had moderate dementia.

This study was part of the larger cohort study AlphaLewyMA (http://clinicaltrials.gouv/ct2/show/NCT01876459) and was approved by the local ethics committee of East France (IV); all participants provided written informed consent for the study according to the Declaration of Helsinki.

Assessment of attention

The Trail Making Test A (TMTA) [41] measures attention, visual screening, and processing speed. It consists of 25 circles, distributed over a sheet of paper. Circles are numbered 1–25, and the participant is asked to draw lines to connect the numbers in ascending order as quickly as possible. The completion time and the number of errors are recorded.

The Digit Symbol Substitution Test (DSST) [42] measures working memory, visuospatial processing, attention, and processing speed. It involves a key in which numbers 1–9 are each paired with a unique symbol. Below the key, numbers 1–9 are shown in random order. The participant is allowed 120 s to fill in the corresponding symbol for each number. Each correct pairing is scored 1 (maximum total raw score = 133). Raw scores can be converted to standard scores (total = 19).

Neuroimaging study

We used VBM to investigate the neuroanatomical correlates of attention/speed processing performances in the DLB patients. To map the regions of atrophy related to the attentional deficit, we tested the correlation between the GM volume at a voxel level and the scores on both attentional tasks in the patient group. Each patient underwent a high-resolution anatomical MRI scan at inclusion. T1-weighted three-dimensional anatomical images were obtained using a 3T MRI scanner (Verio 32-channel Tim Siemens scanner; Siemens, Erlangen, Germany) using a volumetric magnetization-prepared rapid acquisition with gradient-echo (MPRAGE) sequence (FOV = 256 × 256 mm2, image matrix = 256 × 256, slice thickness = 1 mm, repetition time = 1900 ms, echo time = 2.52 ms, flip angle = 9°). VBM analyses included image preprocessing and statistical analyses. These steps were carried out using the SPM12 software package (Wellcome Department of Imaging Neuroscience, London; http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/) running on Matlab R2017b (MathWorks, Natick, MA, USA). Anatomical MRI images were spatially preprocessed using standard procedures [43]. All T1-weighted structural images were first segmented, bias-corrected, and spatially normalized to the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) space using an extension of the unified segmentation procedure that includes six classes of tissue [44]. The DARTEL registration toolbox was then used to build a study-specific template and to bring into alignment all of the segmentation images. The VBM analysis was done on modulated GM images; that is, the GM value in each voxel was multiplied by the Jacobian determinant derived from the spatial normalization. This procedure preserves the total amount of GM from the original images. These modulated GM images were smoothed with a Gaussian kernel (FWHM, 8 mm).

Statistical analysis

Behavioral analyses

Between-group differences for demographic data, MMSE scores, and attention scores on both tasks of interest (time in seconds and raw scores on the TMTA and DSST, respectively) were analyzed in STATISTICA using t tests for quantitative measures. For categorical measures (sex, handedness, fluctuations, hallucinations, and parkinsonism), χ2 tests were applied.

VBM analyses

Statistical correlations between local GM volume and scores on both attention tests were then investigated using the general linear model (GLM). Time in seconds on the TMTA and raw scores on the DSST were tested successively in the patient group. The correlations were tested using t-contrasts (one-tailed test), assuming that increased time in the TMTA and decreased raw scores on the DSST would be associated with decreased GM volumes. Subjects’ age, educational level, and the total GM volume were considered as covariates in the model. The results were analyzed using different statistical significance, either corrected for multiple comparisons using FWE or FDR at p < .05 or without correction at p < .001, while considering a spatial extent of 50 voxels. The XjView software package (http://www.alivelearn.net/xjview/) was used to visualize the results and to report the brain regions involved in the detected clusters. To ensure that the main results were independent of the severity of the disease, we also performed an additional analysis in considering MMSE scores as a nuisance covariate in an additional model of analysis.

Results

Behavioral results

Table 1 shows the demographic and group characteristics of healthy controls and patients with DLB. The groups were well matched in terms of age, educational level, sex, and handedness. Patients presented with significantly more fluctuations, hallucinations, and two out of the three features of parkinsonism (akinesia and rigidity) than controls (Table 1). As expected, significant differences were found between patients and controls on neuropsychological measures (Table 2). Behavioral analyses revealed in patients, compared to controls, significantly lower scores on the MMSE (p < .001) and higher scores on the DSST (p < .001). DLB patients also took significantly more time to achieve completion of the TMTA (p = .003), indicating an impairment in the speed of processing.

Table 2.

MMSE, TMTA, and DSST scores of dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB) patients and healthy control subjects

DLB patients Controls p value
MMSE 25.5 (3.8) 28.9 (0.9) p < .001
TMTA, time in seconds, N = 87 76.2 (59.9) 41.0 (11.7) p = .0026
DSST, raw scores, N = 81 39.3 (15.9) 62.4 (15.6) p < .001

Standard deviations are shown in parentheses. Significant p values are in italics

MMSE Mini-Mental Status Examination, TMTA Trail Making Test A, DSST Digit Symbol Substitution Test

Neuroimaging results

TMTA

Six DLB patients were excluded from the analysis because TMTA scores were missing. VBM analyses revealed that atrophy in bilateral basal ganglia regions was associated with impaired performances on the TMTA (Fig. 1 and Table 3). More precisely, there were significant negative correlations between mean RT and GM volume in two large clusters involving bilaterally the caudate nucleus, the putamen, the thalamus, and the right globus pallidus (p < .05, FDR corrected); notably, the left caudate was significant at a voxel-level threshold of p < .05 FWE corrected (− 15 10.5 9, t = 5.07). Negative correlations were also found in the subthalamic nuclei (p < .05, FDR corrected).

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

The brain regions correlated with the Trail Making Test A (TMTA) in dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB) patients. Negative correlations were found between TMTA scores and two large bilateral clusters including primarily the caudate nucleus (a) (p < .05 FDR and FWE corrected), and a third cluster involving the subthalamic nuclei (b) (p < .05 FDR corrected)

Table 3.

Brain regions negatively correlated with the time on the Trail Making Test A (TMTA; significant at p < .05, FDR corrected) and positively correlated with raw scores on the Digit Symbol Substitution Test (DSST; significant at p < .001 uncorrected) for the DLB group

Brain region R/L BA k x y z T
TMTA (N = 87)
 Cluster 1
  Caudate nucleus (head and body) L na 1033/1646 − 15 10.5 9 5.07*
  Putamen L na 121/1646 − 18 15 3 3.71
  Globus pallidus L na 18/1646 − 13.5 4.5 3 3.6
  Thalamus (ventral anterior nucleus) L na 21/1646 − 10 − 1.5 6 3.2
 Cluster 2
  Caudate nucleus (head and body) R na 1000/1484 12 0 13.5 4.44
  Putamen R na 114/1484 22.5 13.5 10.5 3.82
 Cluster 3
  Subthalamic nucleus L/R na 230 − 4.5 − 13.5 − 3 4.1
DSST (N = 81)
 Cerebellum L na 268 − 43.5 − 70.5 − 43.5 4.1
 Thalamus (ventral lateral nucleus) L na 201 − 15 − 16.5 10.5 3.77
 Inferior frontal gyrus R 44/45 105 55.5 18 19.5 3.59

x, y, z MNI coordinates of the cluster peak; BA Brodmann area; k cluster extent; T t value for the cluster peak

*Cluster peak significant at p < .05 FWE corrected

DSST

Twelve DLB patients were excluded from the analysis because DSST scores were missing. Our results revealed positive correlations between DSST raw scores and three distinct clusters located in the left cerebellum, left thalamus, and right inferior frontal gyrus (Brodmann areas [BAs] 44 and 45) at p < .001 uncorrected (Fig. 2). No clusters were found, either at p < .05 FDR corrected or at p < .05 FWE corrected.

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

The brain regions correlated with the Digit Symbol Substitution Test (DSST) in dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB) patients (p < .001 uncorrected). Positive correlations were found between DSST scores and the left thalamus (a), the right inferior frontal gyrus (BA 45) (b), and the left cerebellum (c)

Additional analysis

When MMSE scores as a nuisance covariate were included, there were negative correlations between mean RT on the TMTA in two large clusters involving bilaterally the caudate nucleus and the putamen, as well as the left thalamus (p < .0005, uncorrected). Positive correlations were found between DSST scores and a cluster located in the left cerebellum (p < .001, uncorrected).

Discussion

The aim of the present study was to investigate which brain regions are involved in the attentional difficulties of DLB patients. For this purpose, we used VBM correlational analysis between behavioral performances on two typical clinical measures of attention and the degree of GM cerebral atrophy. As expected, we found an attentional deficit by means of tasks such as the TMTA and the DSST that are used in clinical routine. In accordance with our hypothesis, volumetric analyses highlighted the correlations between altered attentional scores and decreased volumes in the basal ganglia: in the striatum (mainly the caudate nucleus) and subthalamic nucleus for the TMTA, in the left thalamus for both tasks, and in the right inferior frontal gyrus (BAs 44 and 45) and the left cerebellum for the DSST. The results of our additional analysis, including MMSE scores as a nuisance covariate, confirmed that correlations between TMTA mean RT and caudate nucleus and between DSST scores and left cerebellum were not explained by the severity of the disease, reinforcing the role of these regions independently of the progression of the disease.

By comparing our group of DLB patients to healthy controls, we were able to confirm the existence of attentional deficits, even at the early stage of the disease, which is consistent with the literature [4, 6, 45]. Indeed, the majority of the patients included in the present study were at the prodromal or mild stages of the disease (only 6 out of the 93 patients were at the moderate stage at the time of the evaluations). The 2 tasks we used have in common that they measure the speed of processing as they require a series of operations to be performed under time pressure, and they both involve visual analysis, focused attention, response selection, and motor execution, yet each task has its own specificity. The DSST is a widely used, standardized psychometric test that also targets the maintenance of stimulus-response associations, and has high re-test reliability [42, 46]. Hence, this measure involves more operations than the TMTA, which might explain, based on the variability of patterns of atrophy in DLB patients, why correlations between DSST scores and the degree of atrophy are less specific of one particular brain region. Conversely, the TMTA requires more basic processes involving the correct sequencing of simple elements. The present results are in accordance with the literature suggesting that this measure constitutes a key discriminator between DLB and AD [11].

Negative correlations were found between mean reaction times in the TMTA and clusters that were very delimited to the regions of the basal ganglia, including primarily the caudate nucleus, the putamen, the globus pallidus bilaterally, the left thalamus, and the subthalamic nuclei, meaning that the slower the processing speed, the more these regions were atrophied. The basal ganglia consist of an array of the subcortical nuclei, including the caudate nucleus and the putamen (collectively referred to as the striatum in humans), the globus pallidus, the subthalamic nuclei, and the substantia nigra [47].

The striatum is involved in loops interacting with the prefrontal cortex (PFC). There are at least five of these circuits anatomically described: motor, oculomotor, dorsolateral PFC, orbital PFC, and anterior cingulate circuits [48], supported by both neuroanatomical and neuroimaging studies [47]. They all originate in the PFC and form a loop passing through the striatum (caudate or putamen), globus pallidus, and substantia nigra and finally through the thalamus. In the present study, part of the striatum, the caudate nucleus, was more involved in the dorsal part of its head, and our results also indicate that the most significant correlation was for the left caudate nucleus, for which the cluster peak reached significance at FWE-corrected level. Previous physiological, disease, and lesion studies have demonstrated that the caudate nucleus is associated with processing speed. Patients with traumatic brain injury and initial severe concussion presented a correlation between the volumes of caudate and the speed of processing, assessed using the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-IV (WAIS IV) processing speed index [49]. In rhesus monkeys, a spatial visual working memory task activated bilaterally the head of the caudate nuclei [50] (see also Derauf et al. for patients with reduced caudate volumes due to prenatal methamphetamine exposure correlated to reaction times in an attention task [51] and Spies et al. for HIV patients with previous childhood psychological trauma presenting with diminished volume of the left caudate nucleus, together with slower processing speed [52]). However, in prodromal Huntington’s disease, where the caudate nucleus is atrophied, no correlation was found between the caudate nucleus and speed of processing, using DSST or TMTA [50, 53]. The strong correlation we found between the TMTA and the caudate nucleus could reflect the different processes involved in this task, with a more particular involvement of the dorsal caudate/dorsolateral PFC for the cognitive aspect of the task. Such a correlation has never previously been reported in DLB. However, our results confirm the robust link between processing speed and caudate lesion recently demonstrated in traumatic brain injury by Tate et al. [49]. In line with the idea that the caudate atrophy is involved in non-motor impairment in DLB patients, the volumetric study by Barber et al. [25] highlighted a reduction of the volume of the left caudate nucleus that was not itself correlated with parkinsonism symptoms. These data are consistent with the dorsal caudate nucleus being involved in cognitive rather than motor aspects of the TMTA [47, 54]. Based on all these data, we would suggest that in this pathological condition (DLB), the basal ganglia are the point of disruption of the cognitive loops, even if VBM-GM methods cannot inform us whether afferent pathways, efferent pathways, or both are disturbed.

We also found correlations between scores on the TMTA and atrophy in the subthalamic nuclei. This region was initially described as sustaining motor functions, because of its involvement in some motor symptoms, for example, in PD; interestingly, however, this initial view of its role has been revised in the past decades to incorporate wider cognitive functions [55].

Conversely, since the putamen is connected with the PFC supplementary motor area (SMA), and hence is implicated in motor circuits, this region might rather sustain the motor aspects of the TMTA. Accordingly, it has been proposed that the putamen contributes to movement preparation during self-initiated behavior [47].

The last subcortical region that we found to be correlated with attentional scores and common to both tasks was the left thalamus in its ventral portion. More precisely, the degree of atrophy of the ventral anterior nucleus was correlated to scores on the TMTA and those of the ventral lateral nucleus to scores on the DSST. It is interesting to note that distinct regions of the thalamus are also integrated into the cortico-subcortical circuits described before, receiving afferent projections from the striatum [48]. It has recently been suggested that two of the DLB core features, hallucinations and fluctuations, might be related to the pathological mechanisms at the level of the thalamus, by disturbing the thalamocortical circuits [56]. In DLB patients, attentional impairment is associated with cognitive fluctuations, for which thalamic dysfunction has been shown (Chabran E, Noblet V, Loureiro de Sousa P et al: Changes in gray matter volume and functional connectivity in dementia with Lewy Bodies compared to Alzheimer’s disease: implications for fluctuations, submitted). More importantly, our result corroborates the recent finding by Watson et al. [26], one of the two studies that, to date and to our knowledge, have looked for correlations between attention scores and GM volumes: using linear regression analysis centered on the thalamus, and attentional tasks of simple reaction time, choice reaction time, and vigilance, they found that atrophy in the ventral lateral nucleus was associated with impaired attentional function in DLB. Taken together, these data point towards a dysfunction of the thalamic ventral lateral nucleus underlying attentional impairment in DLB patients.

Overall, the implication of the subcortical regions is consistent with the subcortical syndrome clinical picture that is characterized by a general slowing of cognitive and motor functions, occurring as a consequence of strokes, vascular lesions, and metabolic diseases [57].

Turning now to non-subcortical regions, neither the insula nor other regions of the attentional networks described in the introduction were found to be correlated to attentional scores. Given the fact that the insula, which is part of the SN, has been shown to be involved in the detection of salient stimuli and might not be involved in the tasks we studied. As for the absence of involvement of the DAN [12, 13], the tasks we used might not have required either a redirection of attention or the recruitment of strong working memory processes.

On the contrary, we found positive correlations between DSST scores and atrophy in the right inferior frontal gyrus (BA 45) and in the left cerebellum. Regarding the former, if its left counterpart corresponds to the motor speech area of Broca, the right inferior frontal region has been associated with attentional control [14] and also with reasoning about indeterminate relations [58]. This appears consistent with our result since the specificity of the DSST is to associate unknown and insignificant symbols with numbers. We could also speculate that BA 45 in the right hemisphere serves as a premotor area for non-language codes.

Similar to other brain regions we have already discussed in the present paper, there is cumulative evidence, based on neuroimaging findings and neuropsychological research, suggesting that the cerebellum is involved in cognition in a way that is independent of motor functions [59]. Relevant to the combined atrophy we displayed in the cerebellum, thalamus, and PFC correlated to impaired performance on the DSST is the concept of “cognitive dysmetria” proposed by Andreasen et al. [60], observed in schizophrenic patients, which implicates a disruption of connectivity between these three nodes—cerebellum, thalamus, and prefrontal cortex—and produces, behaviorally, “difficulties in prioritizing, processing, coordinating, and responding to information” [60, 61]. Disturbed connectivity between these three brain regions, as reflected by our correlational results, might explain the impaired performances on the DSST in our group of DLB patients. As these results were reported at an uncorrected level of p < .001, these interpretations need to be treated with caution.

Overall, the positive correlations we found between raw scores on the DSST and GM volumes were less clearly delimited to one brain structure and less strong than with the TMTA, which might indicate a lower sensitivity of this test to gray matter damage of the basal ganglia. Conversely, DSST performance seems to be sensitive to white matter damage due to injury or disease [62, 63] and aging [64]. As a perspective, we will now examine the correlations between white matter morphometry as well as diffusion tensor imaging and attention scores on the same set of data. To disentangle the aspects of motor and/or cognitive speed impairment, we will explore motor speed independently, for instance, by focusing on walking speed in DLB patients.

Conclusions

This is the first study to have examined the neural correlates of attentional dysfunction in DLB patients using voxel-based morphometry and attention tasks typically used in clinical routine. While the existing literature on the neurobiological correlates of attentional dysfunction in DLB is still unclear, we were able to demonstrate, using correlation analysis between attentional scores and GM volumes, the involvement of the striatum—particularly the left caudate nucleus—and the subthalamic nucleus, associated with increasing time on the TMTA, the involvement of the right inferior PFC and the left cerebellum, associated with diminished performances on the DSST, and the involvement of the left thalamus in both tasks. Partially confirming our hypotheses, these results reveal the involvement of the basal ganglia in processing speed and attention in DLB.

Acknowledgements

We are grateful to the patients and their relatives and the healthy control subjects and their relatives who took part in the study. The authors thank the medical doctors of the Memory Center of Strasbourg (CM2R), Pierre Anthony, Benjamin Cretin, Catherine Demuynck, and Catherine Martin-Hunyadi; the neuropsychologists, Timothée Albasser, Emmanuelle Ehrhardt, Jennifer Kemp, Catherine Kleitz, Laetitia Monjoin, and Clélie Phillipps; the research clinical assistants, Lucie Rauch and Melanie Stackfleth; and the secretary, Gabrielle Huck, for the essential collection of the clinical data. They also thank Jean-Paul Armspach, Julien Lamy, and Corrine Marrer from the ICube Laboratory (IMIS team and IRIS platform).

Authors’ contributions

AB participated in the design and coordination of the study, performed the behavioral statistical analysis, interpreted the data, and drafted the manuscript. NP participated in the design of the study and in the interpretation of the data and revised the manuscript. VN performed the MRI preprocessing and statistical analysis. PLDS optimized the MRI sequences. FB conceived the study, participated in its design and coordination, examined the patients, diagnosed the patients with DLB, participated in the interpretation of the data, and revised the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Funding

This study was funded by Projet Hospitalier de Recherche Clinique (PHRC) inter-régional (IDRCB 2012-A00992-41).

Availability of data and materials

The datasets analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Ethics approval and consent to participate

This research was approved by the local ethics committee (Comité de Protection des Personnes Strasbourg Est IV). All participants provided written informed consent.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Footnotes

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

References

  • 1.McKeith IG, Dickson DW, Lowe J, Emre M, O’Brien JT, Feldman H, Cummings J, Duda JE, Lippa C, Perry EK, et al. Diagnosis and management of dementia with Lewy bodies: third report of the DLB Consortium. Neurology. 2005;65(12):1863–1872. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000187889.17253.b1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Zaccai J, McCracken C, Brayne C. A systematic review of prevalence and incidence studies of dementia with Lewy bodies. Age Ageing. 2005;34(6):561–566. doi: 10.1093/ageing/afi190. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.McKeith IG, Boeve BF, Dickson DW, Halliday G, Taylor JP, Weintraub D, Aarsland D, Galvin J, Attems J, Ballard CG, et al. Diagnosis and management of dementia with Lewy bodies: fourth consensus report of the DLB Consortium. Neurology. 2017;89(1):88–100. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000004058. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Kemp J, Philippi N, Phillipps C, Demuynck C, Albasser T, Martin-Hunyadi C, Schmidt-Mutter C, Cretin B, Blanc F. Cognitive profile in prodromal disease (dementia) with Lewy bodies. Alzheimers Res Ther. 2017;16(9):9–19. doi: 10.1186/s13195-017-0242-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Petrova M, Pavlova R, Zhelev Y, Mehrabian S, Raycheva M, Traykov L. Investigation of neuropsychological characteristics of very mild and mild dementia with Lewy bodies. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2015;38(3):354–360. doi: 10.1080/13803395.2015.1117058. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Ferman TJ, Smith GE, Boeve B, Graff-Radford NR, Lucas JA, Knopman DS, Petersen RC, Ivnik RJ, Wszolek Z, Uitti R, et al. Neuropsychological differentiation of dementia with Lewy bodies from normal aging and Alzheimer’s disease. Clin Neuropsychol. 2006;20(4):623–636. doi: 10.1080/13854040500376831. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Ballard C, O’Brien J, Gray A, Cormack F, Ayre G, Rowan E, Thompson P, Bucks R, McKeith I, Walker M, et al. Attention and fluctuating attention in patients with dementia with Lewy bodies and Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol. 2001;58(6):977–982. doi: 10.1001/archneur.58.6.977. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Bradshaw JM, Saling M, Anderson V, Hopwood M, Brodtmann A. Higher cortical deficits influence attentional processing in dementia with Lewy bodies, relative to patients with dementia of the Alzheimer’s type and controls. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2006;77(10):1129–1135. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2006.090183. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Calderon J, Perry RJ, Erzinclioglu SW, Berrios GE, Dening TR, Hodges JR. Perception, attention, and working memory are disproportionately impaired in dementia with Lewy bodies compared with Alzheimer’s disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2001;70(2):157–164. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.70.2.157. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Collerton D, Burn D, McKeith I, O’Brien J. Systematic review and meta-analysis show that dementia with Lewy bodies is a visual-perceptual and attentional-executive dementia. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2003;16:229–237. doi: 10.1159/000072807. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Metzler-Baddeley C. A review of cognitive impairments in dementia with Lewy bodies relative to Alzheimer’s disease and Parkinson’s disease with dementia. Cortex. 2007;43(5):583–600. doi: 10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70489-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Raichle ME, MacLeod AM, Snyder AZ, Powers WJ, Gusnard DA, Shulman GL. A default mode of brain function. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2001;98(2):676–682. doi: 10.1073/pnas.98.2.676. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Raichle ME. The brain’s default mode network. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2015;38:433–447. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-071013-014030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Corbetta M, Shulman GL. Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2002;3(3):201–215. doi: 10.1038/nrn755. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Menon V, Uddin LQ. Saliency, switching, attention and control: a network model of insula function. Brain Struct Funct. 2010;214(5–6):655–667. doi: 10.1007/s00429-010-0262-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Gitelman DR. Attention and its disorders. Br Med Bull. 2003;65:21–34. doi: 10.1093/bmb/65.1.21. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Mak E, Su L, Williams GB, O’Brien JT. Neuroimaging characteristics of dementia with Lewy bodies. Alzheimers Res Ther. 2014;6(2):18. doi: 10.1186/alzrt248. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Nedelska Z, Schwarz CG, Boeve BF, Lowe VJ, Reid RI, Przybelski SA, Lesnick TG, Gunter JL, Senjem ML, Ferman TJ, et al. White matter integrity in dementia with Lewy bodies: a voxel-based analysis of diffusion tensor imaging. Neurobiol Aging. 2015;36(6):2010–2017. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2015.03.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Oppedal K, Ferreira D, Cavallin L, Lemstra AW, Ten Kate M, Padovani A, Rektorova I, Bonanni L, Wahlund LO, Engedal K, et al. A signature pattern of cortical atrophy in dementia with Lewy bodies: a study on 333 patients from the European DLB Consortium. Alzheimers Dement. 2019;15(3):400–409. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2018.09.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Yousaf T, Dervenoulas G, Valkimadi PE, Politis M. Neuroimaging in Lewy body dementia. J Neurol. 2019;266(1):1–26. doi: 10.1007/s00415-018-8892-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Zhong J, Pan P, Dai Z, Shi H. Voxelwise meta-analysis of gray matter abnormalities in dementia with Lewy bodies. Eur J Radiol. 2014;83(10):1870–1874. doi: 10.1016/j.ejrad.2014.06.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Blanc F, Colloby SJ, Cretin B, de Sousa PL, Demuynck C, O’Brien JT, Martin-Hunyadi C, McKeith I, Philippi N, Taylor JP. Grey matter atrophy in prodromal stage of dementia with Lewy bodies and Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Res Ther. 2016;8:31. doi: 10.1186/s13195-016-0198-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Blanc F, Colloby SJ, Philippi N, de Petigny X, Jung B, Demuynck C, Phillipps C, Anthony P, Thomas A, Bing F, et al. Cortical thickness in dementia with Lewy bodies and Alzheimer’s disease: a comparison of prodromal and dementia stages. PLoS One. 2015;10(6):e0127396. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0127396. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Lee JE, Park B, Song SK, Sohn YH, Park HJ, Lee PH. A comparison of gray and white matter density in patients with Parkinson’s disease dementia and dementia with Lewy bodies using voxel-based morphometry. Mov Disord. 2010;25(1):28–34. doi: 10.1002/mds.22858. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Barber R, McKeith I, Ballard C, O’Brien J. Volumetric MRI study of the caudate nucleus in patients with dementia with Lewy bodies, Alzheimer’s disease, and vascular dementia. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2002;72(3):406–407. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.72.3.406. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Watson R, Colloby SJ, Blamire AM, Wesnes KA, Wood J, O’Brien JT. Does attentional dysfunction and thalamic atrophy predict decline in dementia with Lewy bodies? Parkinsonism Relat Disord. 2017;45:69–74. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2017.10.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Cousins DA, Burton EJ, Burn D, Gholkar A, McKeith IG, O’Brien JT. Atrophy of the putamen in dementia with Lewy bodies but not Alzheimer’s disease: an MRI study. Neurology. 2003;61(9):1191–1195. doi: 10.1212/01.WNL.0000091889.20347.30. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Firbank M, Kobeleva X, Cherry G, Killen A, Gallagher P, Burn DJ, Thomas AJ, O’Brien JT, Taylor JP. Neural correlates of attention-executive dysfunction in lewy body dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Hum Brain Mapp. 2016;37(3):1254–1270. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Kobeleva X, Firbank M, Peraza L, Gallagher P, Thomas A, Burn DJ, O’Brien J, Taylor JP. Divergent functional connectivity during attentional processing in Lewy body dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Cortex. 2017;92:8–18. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2017.02.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Cromarty RA, Schumacher J, Graziadio S, Gallagher P, Killen A, Firbank MJ, Blamire A, Kaiser M, Thomas AJ, O’Brien JT, et al. Structural brain correlates of attention dysfunction in Lewy body dementias and Alzheimer’s disease. Front Aging Neurosci. 2018;10:347. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2018.00347. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Chikama M, McFarland NR, Amaral DG, Haber SN. Insular cortical projections to functional regions of the striatum correlate with cortical cytoarchitectonic organization in the primate. J Neurosci. 1997;17(24):9686–9705. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-24-09686.1997. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Fudge JL, Breitbart MA, Danish M, Pannoni V. Insular and gustatory inputs to the caudal ventral striatum in primates. J Comp Neurol. 2005;490(2):101–118. doi: 10.1002/cne.20660. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Ferman TJ, Smith GE, Boeve BF, Ivnik RJ, Petersen RC, Knopman D, Graff-Radford N, Parisi J, Dickson DW. DLB fluctuations: specific features that reliably differentiate DLB from AD and normal aging. Neurology. 2004;62(2):181–187. doi: 10.1212/WNL.62.2.181. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Walker MP, Ayre GA, Cummings JL, Wesnes K, McKeith IG, O’Brien JT, Ballard CG. The clinician assessment of fluctuation and the One Day Fluctuation Assessment Scale. Two methods to assess fluctuating confusion in dementia. Br J Psychiatry. 2000;177:252–256. doi: 10.1192/bjp.177.3.252. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Fenelon G, Soulas T, Zenasni F, Cleret de Langavant L. The changing face of Parkinson’s disease-associated psychosis: a cross-sectional study based on the new NINDS-NIMH criteria. Mov Disord. 2010;25(6):763–766. doi: 10.1002/mds.22839. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Gjerstad MD, Boeve B, Wentzel-Larsen T, Aarsland D, Larsen JP. Occurrence and clinical correlates of REM sleep behaviour disorder in patients with Parkinson’s disease over time. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2008;79(4):387–391. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2007.116830. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Lawton MP, Brody EM. Assessment of older people: self-maintaining and instrumental activities of daily living. Gerontologist. 1969;9(3):179–186. doi: 10.1093/geront/9.3_Part_1.179. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Israel L, Waintraub L. Autonomie ou capacité fonctionnelle? Revue critique de quelques échelles actuellement utilisées en gériatrie pour l’évaluation des activités de la vie quotidienne. Psychol Med (Paris) 1986;18(14):2225–2231. [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Association AP . Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5. 5. Arlington: VA Publisher American Psychiatric Publishing; 2013. [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. “Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res. 1975;12(3):189–198. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Tombaugh TN. Trail Making Test A and B: normative data stratified by age and education. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2004;19(2):203–214. doi: 10.1016/S0887-6177(03)00039-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Wechsler D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-3rd Edition (WAIS-3) Personal Clinical Assessment: San Antonio; 1997. [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Good CD, Johnsrude IS, Ashburner J, Henson RN, Friston KJ, Frackowiak RS. A voxel-based morphometric study of ageing in 465 normal adult human brains. Neuroimage. 2001;14(1 Pt 1):21–36. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0786. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Ashburner J, Friston KJ. Unified segmentation. Neuroimage. 2005;26(3):839–851. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.02.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Ferman TJ, Smith GE, Kantarci K, Boeve BF, Pankratz VS, Dickson DW, Graff-Radford NR, Wszolek Z, Van Gerpen J, Uitti R, et al. Nonamnestic mild cognitive impairment progresses to dementia with Lewy bodies. Neurology. 2013;81(23):2032–2038. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000436942.55281.47. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Salthouse TA, Berish DE. Correlates of within-person (across-occasion) variability in reaction time. Neuropsychology. 2005;19(1):77–87. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.19.1.77. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Provost JS, Hanganu A, Monchi O. Neuroimaging studies of the striatum in cognition part I: healthy individuals. Front Syst Neurosci. 2015;9:140. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2015.00140. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Alexander GE, DeLong MR, Strick PL. Parallel organization of functionally segregated circuits linking basal ganglia and cortex. Annu Rev Neurosci. 1986;9:357–381. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.09.030186.002041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Tate DF, Wade BSC, Velez CS, Drennon AM, Bolzenius JD, Cooper DB, Kennedy JE, Reid MW, Bowles AO, Thompson PM, et al. Subcortical shape and neuropsychological function among U.S. service members with mild traumatic brain injury. Brain Imaging Behav. 2019;13(2):377–388. doi: 10.1007/s11682-018-9854-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Jurgens CK, van de Wiel L, van Es AC, Grimbergen YM, Witjes-Ane MN, van der Grond J, Middelkoop HA, Roos RA. Basal ganglia volume and clinical correlates in ‘preclinical’ Huntington’s disease. J Neurol. 2008;255(11):1785–1791. doi: 10.1007/s00415-008-0050-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Derauf C, Lester BM, Neyzi N, Kekatpure M, Gracia L, Davis J, Kallianpur K, Efird JT, Kosofsky B. Subcortical and cortical structural central nervous system changes and attention processing deficits in preschool-aged children with prenatal methamphetamine and tobacco exposure. Dev Neurosci. 2012;34(4):327–341. doi: 10.1159/000341119. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Spies G, Ahmed-Leitao F, Fennema-Notestine C, Cherner M, Seedat S. Effects of HIV and childhood trauma on brain morphometry and neurocognitive function. J Neuro-Oncol. 2016;22(2):149–158. doi: 10.1007/s13365-015-0379-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Harrington DL, Liu D, Smith MM, Mills JA, Long JD, Aylward EH, Paulsen JS. Neuroanatomical correlates of cognitive functioning in prodromal Huntington disease. Brain Behav. 2014;4(1):29–40. doi: 10.1002/brb3.185. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Huang H, Nguyen PT, Schwab NA, Tanner JJ, Price CC, Ding M. Mapping dorsal and ventral caudate in older adults: method and validation. Front Aging Neurosci. 2017;9:91. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2017.00091. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Weintraub DB, Zaghloul KA. The role of the subthalamic nucleus in cognition. Rev Neurosci. 2013;24(2):125–138. doi: 10.1515/revneuro-2012-0075. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Esmaeeli S, Murphy K, Swords GM, Ibrahim BA, Brown JW, Llano DA. Visual hallucinations, thalamocortical physiology and Lewy body disease: a review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2019;103:337–351. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.06.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Starosta RT, Vidor MV, Roriz-Cruz M. The frontal-subcortical syndrome. J Alzheimers Dis Parkinsonism. 2016;6(4):1–4. [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Goel V, Stollstorff M, Nakic M, Knutson K, Grafman J. A role for right ventrolateral prefrontal cortex in reasoning about indeterminate relations. Neuropsychologia. 2009;47(13):2790–2797. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.06.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Sokolov AA, Miall RC, Ivry RB. The cerebellum: adaptive prediction for movement and cognition. Trends Cogn Sci. 2017;21(5):313–332. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2017.02.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Andreasen NC, Paradisio S, O’Leary DS. “Cognitive dysmetria” as an integrative theory of schizophrenia: a dysfunction in cortical-subcortical-cerebellar circuitry? Schizophr Bull. 1998;24(2):203–218. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a033321. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Nopoulos PC, Ceilley JW, Gailis EA, Andreasen NC. An MRI study of cerebellar vermis morphology in patients with schizophrenia: evidence in support of the cognitive dysmetria concept. Biol Psychiatry. 1999;46(5):703–711. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(99)00093-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Charlton RA, Barrick TR, McIntyre DJ, Shen Y, O’Sullivan M, Howe FA, Clark CA, Morris RG, Markus HS. White matter damage on diffusion tensor imaging correlates with age-related cognitive decline. Neurology. 2006;66(2):217–222. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000194256.15247.83. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.O’Brien JT, Wiseman R, Burton EJ, Barber B, Wesnes K, Saxby B, Ford GA. Cognitive associations of subcortical white matter lesions in older people. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2002;977:436–444. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2002.tb04849.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Salthouse TA. The processing-speed theory of adult age differences in cognition. Psychol Rev. 1996;103(3):403–428. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.103.3.403. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.


Articles from Alzheimer's Research & Therapy are provided here courtesy of BMC

RESOURCES