Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2022 Mar 1.
Published in final edited form as: Appl Neuropsychol Adult. 2019 Jun 23;28(2):230–244. doi: 10.1080/23279095.2019.1627357

Normative data stratified by age and education for a Spanish neuropsychological test battery: Results from the Colombian Alzheimer’s Prevention Initiative Registry

Valeria L Torres a,+, Clara Vila-Castelar b,+, Yamile Bocanegra c, Ana Baena c, Edmarie Guzmán-Vélez b, Daniel C Aguirre-Acevedo c, Victoria Tirado c, Claudia Munoz c, Eliana Henao c, Sonia Moreno c, Margarita Giraldo c, Natalia Acosta c, Silvia Rios Romenets c, Jessica B Langbaum d, William Cho e, Eric M Reiman d, Pierre N Tariot d, Monica Rosselli a, Yakeel T Quiroz b,c,f,*, Francisco Lopera c,*
PMCID: PMC6926163  NIHMSID: NIHMS1534035  PMID: 31230490

Abstract

Objective:

Neuropsychologists continue to face challenges when assessing Spanish-speaking individuals due to limited availability of normative data. We developed comprehensive normative data stratified by age and education for a Spanish neuropsychological test battery used by the Grupo de Neurociencias de Antioquia (Colombia) and the Colombian Alzheimer’s Prevention Initiative Registry, which have followed large families at risk for autosomal-dominant Alzheimer’s disease (ADAD) since the 1990s. Approximately 75% of these individuals are cognitively-unimpaired and are not genetically predisposed to develop ADAD.

Methods:

We conducted a retrospective study on neuropsychological evaluations from 2,673 cognitively unimpaired individuals (56% female), with ages ranging from 18 to 86 years and education from 1 to 25 years. Neuropsychological measures included the Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease-Colombia, and other multi-domain Spanish tests. We examined associations between age, education, and sex with cognitive performance.

Results:

Norms stratified by age and education are presented. Cognitive performance showed small associations with age and education and was unrelated to sex.

Conclusions:

We provided population-based norms for Spanish tests targeting multiple cognitive domains using a large Colombian sample. These normative data may be helpful for the neuropsychological characterization of Spanish speakers from Latin America in clinical and research settings.

Keywords: Normative data, Spanish, Assessment, Neuropsychological battery, Cognition

Introduction

Previous work, such as that conducted by Ardila, Rosselli, and Puente (1994), represent some of the pioneering efforts to highlight the need to develop normative data for Spanish-speaking individuals (including older adults and illiterate individuals), as well as improving the translation and validation of available measures. The field of neuropsychology, in recent years, has broadened our understanding of how cultural factors impact neuropsychological performance (Ardila, 2005; Brickman, Cabo, & Manly, 2006). As such, neuropsychologists have emphasized the need to develop sensitive and specific normative data to better assess individuals from diverse cultural and linguistic backgrounds (Rivera-Mindt, Byrd, Saez, & Manly, 2010), which has helped advance the field of cross-cultural neuropsychology.

As a result, several normative studies with Spanish-speaking individuals from different regions have been developed, including combined Latin American countries (Guàrdia-Olmos, Peró-Cebollero, Rivera, & Arango-Lasprilla, 2015; Arango-Lasprilla, 2015; Alegret et al., 2012), Mexico (Ostrosky-Solís, Ardila, & Rosselli, 1999; Ostrosky-Solís et al., 2007; Ostrosky-Solís, Gutierrez, Flores, & Ardila, 2007), the United States (including Puerto Rico) (Stricks, Pittman, Jacobs, Sano, & Stern, 1998; Pontón et al., 1996; Hall et al., 2018) and Spain (Peña-Casanova et al., 2012; Muntal et al., 2017). Despite these tremendous advances, there is a continued need for culturally and linguistically-appropriate norms in order to serve the needs of heterogeneous Spanish-speaking populations.

As part of ongoing efforts from the Neuroscience Group of Antioquia (Grupo de Neurociencias de Antioquia, GNA) in Medellín, Colombia and in collaboration with the Banner Alzheimer’s Institute of Arizona, US, the Colombian Alzheimer’s Prevention Initiative (API) Registry has evaluated over 5,000 individuals that belong to families at risk for autosomal dominant Alzheimer’s disease (ADAD). These individuals have undergone clinical and cognitive assessments since the 1990s. Approximately 75% of these individuals are cognitively unimpaired and are not genetically predisposed to develop ADAD (Cornejo, Lopera, Uribe, & Salinas, 1987).

The CERAD is a neuropsychological screening battery originally developed in English (Morris et al., 1988) that includes 7 subtests: Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE), Semantic Fluency (Animals), Word List Learning, Word List Recall, Word List Recognition, modified Boston Naming (15 items), and Constructional Praxis. This battery has been validated to distinguish between AD and normal aging (Morris et al., 1989; Welsh et al., 1994). The Spanish CERAD has been standardized in the United States (O’Bryant et al., 2018), Costa Rica (Guerrero-Berroa et al. 2016), Puerto Rico (Carrión-Baralt et al., 2009), and Colombia (Aguirre-Acevedo et al., 2007).

Previous work by this group validated the Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease (CERAD) in Spanish (CERAD-Col), using data from 150 cognitively unimpaired and 151 impaired individuals (50-95 years of age) who belong to the extended families followed by the GNA (Aguirre-Acevedo et al., 2007). Henao-Arboleda and colleagues (2010) developed norms stratified by age and education for the CERAD-Col using 848 cognitively unimpaired older adults with a mean age of 64, 36 individuals with mild cognitive impairment with a mean age of 69, and 151 participants with AD with a mean age of 72. These efforts resulted in a set of normative data for this extensive battery that can be used to evaluate cognitive functioning in healthy and cognitively impaired Spanish-speaking individuals.

Since the original normative works in Colombia, we have been able to obtain cross-sectional cognitive data from 2,673 individuals between the ages of 18 to 86. Our aims for the present paper were twofold: 1) expand existing standardized norms for the CERAD-Col and additional cognitive measures routinely used at the GNA by examining a larger sample with wider range of ages and with more heterogeneous educational backgrounds; and 2) develop norms for Spanish speaking populations of tests frequently used in clinical assessment (Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure [RCFT], Raven’s Progressive Matrices, Part A, Trail Making Test-A [TMT-A] WAIS-III Digit Symbol, Phonemic Fluency [FAS] and the Wisconsin Card Sorting Task [WCST]) and of other tests that currently lack normative data (i.e., Memory Impairment Screening and Memory Capacity Test). In sum, this is a large normative study that provides comprehensive norms for a broad set of Spanish-speaking neuropsychological measures targeting multiple cognitive domains.

Method

Participants

Participants were recruited from the Colombian API Registry, an effort led by the Grupo de Neurociencias (GNA) in Antioquia, Colombia, to recruit and conduct clinical and cognitive evaluations on individuals at risk for autosomal dominant AD (ADAD) due to a Presenilin-1 (PSEN1) E280A mutation who were at least 8 years of age. Detailed description of the inclusion and exclusion criteria for the registry can be found in Rios-Romenets et al. (2017) and Tariot et al. (2018). Briefly, for this retrospective study, we included clinically normal men and women PSEN1 E280A noncarriers who were at least 18 years old with a MMSE score ≥ 26. Individuals were excluded if they met criteria for Mild Cognitive Impairment due to AD as defined by cutoff scores greater than 2 on the Global Deterioration Scale (GDS; Reisberg, Ferris, de Leon, & Crook, 1988). Individuals with a significant medical, psychiatric or neurological condition or disorder; history of stroke, seizures, substance abuse, or other disorders that affect motor, visuospatial or cognitive abilities; clinically significant screening blood laboratory abnormalities; use of any other medications with the potential to significantly affect cognition were excluded. Illiterate individuals or with zero years of formal education were also excluded.

A total of 2,673 cognitively unimpaired individuals were included in the development of these normative data (Figure 1). Participants’ ages ranged from 18 to 86 years (M = 32.94, SD = 12.10), and their years of education ranged from 1 to 25 years (M = 8.89, SD = 4.34). Approximately half of the sample consisted of women (56.2 %).

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Recruitment Process

Recruitment process for normative sample; GDS = Global Deterioration Scale

Procedure

Participants provided written informed consent before enrollment into the registry. Participants were studied under guidelines approved by local institutional review boards. Ethics approval was obtained from the University of Antioquia Ethics Committee. Consenting and administration of the neuropsychological examination was conducted in Spanish, the participants’ native language, by psychometricians and neuropsychologists. All tests were administered during one session at the GNA in Medellín, Antioquia, Colombia.

Measures

CERAD Total:

We calculated a CERAD Total score as previously reported by Aguirre-Acevedo et al. (2016) following the Chandler method (Chandler et al., 2005), which includes six subtests from the original CERAD (Morris, 1988). This included Semantic Fluency (Animals), Boston Naming Test (15 items), Word List Learning, World List Learning Recall, Word List Learning Recognition, and Constructional Praxis Copy.

Mini-mental State Examination (MMSE).

The MMSE is a cognitive screening test (Folstein, Folstein, & McHugh, 1975) consisting of 30 items that measure orientation, attention, calculation, memory, visuospatial functioning, and language. This adapted version used the recall words “mesa”, “silla” and “lapiz”, and excluded the backward spelling of the word “world” (Aguirre-Acevedo et al., 2007).

Semantic Fluency (Animals).

On this language test, participants are asked to name as many animals as possible in sixty seconds. The score is the total number of correctly named animals (Newcombe, 1969).

Boston Naming Test (BNT-15).

The BNT is a measure of naming. On this version of the BNT (Kaplan, Goodglass, & Weintraub, 1983), participants are asked to name 15 drawings (i.e., 5 high frequency, 5 medium frequency, and 5 low frequency). Each drawing is shown for a maximum of 10 seconds, and semantic and phonemic cues are not given. A point is awarded for each word that is named correctly for a maximum score of 15.

CERAD Word List Memory Task.

The Word List Memory task is divided into learning, free recall, and recognition. First, participants are shown 10 cards with a word each over three trials and are instructed to read them aloud and memorize them. Each card was presented for two seconds. The score is calculated as the sum of words recalled correctly over three trials, for a total of 30 points (Word List Learning). Intrusions are recorded and scored separately.

After a 7-minute delay, participants are asked to recall the 10 words that were previously shown (Word List Delayed Recall). The total score is calculated as the total number of correct words for a maximum score of 10. Finally, participants are shown a list of 20 words (10 target words and 10 distractors) and are asked to indicate whether they recognized the target words. The total score is calculated as the sum of correct “yes” or ‘no” answers minus 10 (Word List Recognition). Negative totals are given a score of zero (Aguirre-Acevedo et al., 2007).

CERAD Constructional Praxis (CP).

This visuospatial construction and memory task is divided into a learning and a recall phase. During the learning phase, participants are asked to copy four simple drawings (i.e., circle, rhombus, cube, and superimposed rectangles) on paper (CP Copy). After a two-minute delay, participants are asked to recall and draw the previously learned figures, which are scored following previously established criteria for a total of 11 points (CP Delayed Recall: Aguirre-Acevedo et al., 2007).

Memory Impairment Screening (MIS).

The MIS (Buschke et al., 1999) is a screen for memory impairment. This test was adapted for Spanish-speaking individuals by Romero-Vanegas, Valencia-Marin, Aguirre-Acevedo, Buschke, and Lopera (2010). Participants are asked to read four words paired to a semantic category. A delayed free recall and delayed cued recall are administered after an interference task. The total score is the sum of the total correct words recalled in the free and cued conditions, for a maximum score of 8 (MIS Free Recall and MIS Cued Recall, respectively).

Memory Capacity Test (MCT).

The MCT is a measure of associative memory (Buschke, 1984) that was adapted for Spanish-speaking individuals (Romero-Vanegas et al., 2010). This test consists of two list of 16 items each. Words in each list belong to a different semantic category; the semantic categories are the same on both lists. First, participants read the words and the examiner pairs the word with the category. Participants are given the semantic cue and asked to recall the words (MCT List 1 Cued Recall). This procedure is repeated for the second list (MCT List 2 Cued Recall). Then, a cued recall phase for both lists is administered (MCT List 1 Cued Recall 2 & MCT List 2 Cued Recall 2). Finally, participants are asked to freely recall all words (List 1 & 2: MCT Total Free Recall).

Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure (RCFT).

The RCFT (Osterrieth, 1944) is a visual test that measures planning, attention, and memory. During the learning phase of the task (RCFT Copy), participants are asked to copy a complex figure. The figures are scored based on previously established criteria for a total of 36 points for each. After a 3-minute delay, they are asked to reproduce the figure from memory (RCFT Delayed Recall).

Raven’s Progressive Matrices, Part A.

On this task of non-verbal reasoning, participants are asked to identify the missing component from a set of five figures that increase in difficulty, for a total score of 12 (Raven’s Progressive Matrices: Raven, 1996).

Trail Making Test-A (TMT-A).

The TMT-A is task of psychomotor speed in which participants are instructed to draw lines connecting circles with numbers from one to 25 on numerical order, as quickly as possible (Reitan & Wolfson, 1985). The score is calculated as the total time, in seconds, taken to complete the task.

WAIS-III Digit Symbol.

On this task of psychomotor speed, participants are shown a series of symbols that correspond to a distinct number from one to nine. They are then asked to draw the symbol that corresponds to the number, as quickly as possible. Participants are allowed 120 seconds. One point is given for each correct response (Wechsler, 1997).

“A” Cancellation Test.

This is a test of sustained attention (Ardila, Rosselli, & Puente, 1994) wherein participants are presented with a series of scattered letters on a paper and asked to cross out the ‘A’s as quickly as possible. Participants are timed, and the score is the number of correctly identified A’s.

Phonemic Fluency (FAS).

Participants are instructed to name as many words as possible beginning with the letters F, A, and S that are not proper names, or words sharing similar roots, in 60 seconds each (Benton, 1976).. The total score is the number of correct words given for the three letters.

Wisconsin Card Sorting Task (WCST).

The WCST (Nelson, 1976) is a test of cognitive flexibility in which participants are presented with four fixed cards, each with a different shape, color, and number of shapes. Participants are instructed to correctly match 48 cards to one of the four fixed cards, one at a time. Participants are asked to determine the correct way to match the cards by using the feedback that is provided (i.e., right or wrong). The rule changes after the participants correctly match 6 consecutive cards, which forces participants to shift categories.

Normative Procedure

A retrospective study was conducted on neuropsychological data from the GNA and the Colombian API registry, acquired between 1993 and 2017. To obtain normative data, we divided the age ranges into eleven subgroups: 18-20, 21-25, 26-30, 31-35, 36-40, 41-45, 46-50, 51-55, 56-60, 61-65, and 65 and over; and divided years of education based on educational attainment: elementary, high school, or college, which correspond to 1-5, 6-11, or 12 or more years of education, respectively. Given the smaller sample of older individuals with higher levels of education, we used median years of education to stratify the groups in high and low education levels for all groups over the age of 41. We also provided additional normative data grouping all individuals above age 60 for clinical use when indicated (Please see Supplemental Table 1). Means and standard deviations for all subgroups were calculated for each cognitive measure, as described above.

Statistical Analyses

All analyses were conducted with IBM SPSS 25 (Armonk, NY). Normative data for each neuropsychological test can be found on tables 111 and includes the mean and standard deviation by age group and educational level. Preliminary analyses revealed that data were not normally distributed (See Supplemental Table 2 for average, standard deviation, median, interquartile range, expected range, observed range, floor and ceiling effects, skewness and kurtosis coefficients). We examined the associations between age and education with cognitive measures assumed to be normally distributed (kurtosis coefficient < 7 and skewness coefficient < 2) using Pearson correlation coefficients (as described in Curran, West, & Finch, 1996). For the remaining variables, non-parametric Spearman’s rho (rs) was calculated. Cognitive performance among males and females was examined for clinical relevance using t-test analyses. Effect sizes for each test were calculated and are presented on Table 13 (Fritz, Morris, & Richler, 2012). The Bonferroni method was used to correct for multiple comparisons (p ≤ .002, α = .05, 24 comparisons).

Table 1.

Means and standard deviation for ages 18-20

Education (years)
1-5 (n = 48) 6-11 (n = 210) ≥ 12 (n = 106)
Test n M (SD) M (SD) M (SD)
Cognitive Screen
CERAD Total 364 69.81 (13.2) 71.07 (11.82) 71.96 (11.75)
MMSE /30 364 28.37 (2.46) 28.74 (1.8) 28.71 (1.8)
Language
Semantic Fluency (Animals) 364 18.17 (4.9) 17.36 (4.81) 18.11 (4.96)
BNT-15 /15 364 12.06 (2.41) 12.3 (2.03) 12.73 (1.85)
Memory
Word List Learning /30 364 16.47 (4.18) 17 (3.97) 16.83 (4.13)
Word List Delayed Recall /10 364 6.15 (1.81) 6.23 (1.91) 6.27 (1.79)
Word List Recognition /10 364 9.19 (1.55) 9.39 (1.35) 9.49 (1.16)
MIS Free Recall /8 364 6.1 (2.34) 6.38 (1.8) 6.22 (1.91)
MIS Cued Recall /8 364 1.1 (0.77) 0.91 (0.73) 1.1 (0.73)
MCT List 1 Cued Recall 364 14.32 (2.13) 14.64 (2.01) 14.84 (1.62)
MCT List 2 Cued Recall 364 11.05 (3.28) 11.63 (3.6) 12.1 (3.28)
MCT List 1 Cued Recall 2 364 14.03 (2.51) 14.14 (2.52) 14.28 (1.95)
MCT List 2 Cued Recall 2 364 11.89 (3.42) 11.96 (3.56) 12.55 (3.08)
MCT Total Free Recall 364 19.16 (6.48) 18.94 (6.19) 19.91 (5.66)
CP Delayed Recall /11 364 8 (3.2) 8.43 (2.6) 8.42 (2.73)
RCFT Delayed Recall /16 364 14.56 (6.91) 15.71 (6.87) 15.83 (6.81)
Visuospatial
CP Copy /11 364 9.4 (1.95) 9.74 (1.54) 9.6 (1.65)
RCFT Copy /36 364 26.01 (7.84) 27.62 (5.9) 27.62 (6.27)
Raven’s Progressive Matrices /12 364 8.64 (2.21) 8.72 (2.13) 8.81 (1.73)
Processing Speed
TMT-A (seconds) 364 69.67 (44.46) 68.32 (40.47) 69.52 (44.12)
WAIS-III Digit Symbol 364 42.63 (20.99) 45.96 (19.96) 47.1 (21.14)
A Cancellation Correct /16 364 14.86 (0.38) 14.41 (1.18) 14.47 (0.99)
Executive Functioning
Phonemic Fluency (FAS) 364 25.75 (13.2) 26.12 (11.54) 25.73 (11.83)
WAIS-III Arithmetic 364 8.17 (2.79) 7.75 (1.92) 6.87 (3.11)
WCST Total Correct 364 19.58 (8.57) 20.51 (8.53) 22.16 (8.51)
WCST Total Errors 364 28.38 (8.61) 27.25 (8.73) 25.84 (8.51)
WCST Total Categories /6 364 2.42 (1.31) 2.52 (1.43) 2.9 (1.49)
WCST Perseverations 364 21.02 (8.84) 19.14 (8.5) 18.24 (7.76)
WCST Failure Maintain Set 364 0.53 (0.68) 0.62 (1.04) 0.45 (0.88)

Note. Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE); Boston Naming Test (BNT); Memory Impairment Screening (MIS); Memory Capacity Test (MCT); CERAD Constructional Praxis (CP); Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure (RCFT); Trail Making Test-A (TMT-A); Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III (WAIS-III); Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST); Mean (M); Standard Deviation (SD).

Table 11.

Means and standard deviation for ages 65 and above

Education (years)
0-4 (n = 17) ≥ 5 (n = 24)
Test n M (SD) M (SD)
Cognitive Screen
CERAD Total 41 69.12 (11.22) 68.74 (11.87)
MMSE /30 41 28.41 (3.16) 28.33 (2.24)
Language
Semantic Fluency (Animals) 41 17 (4.85) 16.79 (4.17)
BNT-15 /15 41 11.76 (2.31) 12 (1.89)
Memory
Word List Learning /30 41 16.12 (3.6) 15.58 (3.62)
Word List Delayed Recall /10 41 5.29 (1.79) 5.46 (2.13)
Word List Recognition /10 41 9.35 (1.17) 9.37 (1.3)
MIS Free Recall /8 41 6.75 (1.44) 6.11 (2)
MIS Cued Recall /8 41 0.89 (0.33) 1.18 (0.87)
MCT List 1 Cued Recall 41 13.71 (2.92) 14.22 (2.29)
MCT List 2 Cued Recall 41 11.64 (3.08) 10.89 (4.19)
MCT List 1 Cued Recall 2 41 13.43 (2.77) 13.61 (3.11)
MCT List 2 Cued Recall 2 41 11.71 (3.29) 11.11 (4.01)
MCT Total Free Recall 41 18.29 (6.06) 16.67 (8.03)
CP Delayed Recall /11 41 7.24 (2.41) 6.92 (3.36)
RCFT Delayed Recall /16 41 13.65 (6.74) 11.54 (7.12)
Visuospatial
CP Copy /11 41 9.59 (1.42) 9.29 (1.78)
RCFT Copy /36 41 26.76 (4.18) 24.73 (6.67)
Raven’s Progressive Matrices /12 41 8.06 (2.11) 7.71 (2.26)
Processing Speed
TMT-A (seconds) 41 83.31 (44.68) 85.46 (54.81)
WAIS-III Digit Symbol 41 41.56 (19.9) 41.76 (21.76)
A Cancellation Correct /16 41 . (.) 13.83 (1.94)
Executive Functioning
Phonemic Fluency (FAS) 41 26.13 (12.99) 20.39 (12)
WAIS-III Arithmetic 41 . (.) 6.17 (2.32)
WCST Total Correct 41 18.76 (9.84) 21.78 (7.89)
WCST Total Errors 41 29.24 (9.84) 26.17 (7.84)
WCST Total Categories /6 41 2.47 (1.46) 2.57 (1.27)
WCST Perseverations 41 22.18 (10.55) 19.22 (7.17)
WCST Failure Maintain Set 41 0.41 (0.71) 0.85 (1.04)

Note. Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE); Boston Naming Test (BNT); Memory Impairment Screening (MIS); Memory Capacity Test (MCT); CERAD Constructional Praxis (CP); Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure (RCFT); Trail Making Test-A (TMT-A); Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III (WAIS-III); Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST); Mean (M); Standard Deviation (SD).

Table 13.

Test performance by sex

Males Females

Test M (SD) t p d
Cognitive Screen
CERAD Total 73.38 (10.76) 72.19 (11.17) −2.63 .009 −.15
MMSE 28.80 (1.84) 28.72 (1.78) −1.10 .269 −.13
Language
Semantic Fluency (Animals) 18.03 (4.78) 17.68 (4.82) −1.89 .059 −.11
BNT-15 12.35 (1.95) 12.33 (2.02) −.26 .796 −.06
Memory
Word List Learning 17.24 (3.83) 16.74 (4.04) −3.27 .001** −.04
Word List Delayed Recall 6.37 (1.80) 6.16 (1.89) −2.87 .004* −.09
Word List Recognition 9.52 (1.07) 9.46 (1.10) −1.37 .172 −.04
MIS Free Recall 6.51 (1.80) 6.44 (1.83) −.97 .330 −.08
MIS Cued Recall 0.94 (0.67) 0.95 (0.70) .22 .823 .01
MCT List 1 Cued Recall 14.82 (1.81) 14.65 (1.98) −2.18 .029* −.09
MCT List 2 Cued Recall 11.94 (3.23) 11.79 (3.50) −1.07 .280 −.04
MCT List 1 Cued Recall 2 14.27 (2.22) 14.08 (2.47) −1.95 .051 −.08
MCT List 2 Cued Recall 2 12.39 (3.10) 12.12 (3.46) −1.92 .055 −.08
MCT Total Free Recall 19.52 (5.65) 19.03 (6.06) −1.99 .047* −.08
CP Delayed Recall 8.36 (2.66) 8.16 (2.71) −1.91 .056 −.07
RCFT Delayed Recall 15.23 (6.92) 14.93 (7.09) −1.09 .274 −.04
Visuospatial
CP Copy 9.70 (1.53) 9.67 (1.56) −.48 .633 −.02
RCFT Copy 27.42 (6.13) 27.29 (6.35) −.52 .600 −.02
Raven’s Progressive Matrices 8.74 (1.99) 8.66 (1.95) −1.08 .280 −.04
Processing Speed
TMT-A (seconds) 69.09 (50.30) 71.72 (49.16) 1.35 .176 .05
WAIS-III Digit Symbol 45.42 (18.58) 43.71 (18.80) −2.16 .031* −.09
A Cancellation Correct 14.41 (1.06) 14.39 (1.13) −.20 .841 −.03
Executive Functioning
Phonemic Fluency (FAS) 26.07 (11.41) 24.72 (11.97) −2.95 .003* −.12
WCST Total Correct 20.94 (8.55) 20.96 (8.34) .05 .961 .00
WCST Total Errors 27.03 (8.57) 26.98 (8.41) −.15 .879 −.01
WCST Total Categories 2.71 (1.45) 2.70 (1.42) −.08 .937 −.01
WCST Perseverations 19.40 (8.64) 19.15 (8.22) −.74 .458 −.03
WCST Failure Maintain Set 0.49 (0.82) 0.49 (0.93) −.06 .953 .00
**.

t-test is significant after Bonferroni correction (p ≤ .002)

*.

t-test is significant at the 0.05 level (2-tailed).

Note. Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE); Boston Naming Test (BNT); Memory Impairment Screening (MIS); Memory Capacity Test (MCT); CERAD Constructional Praxis (CP); Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure (RCFT); Trail Making Test-A (TMT-A); Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III (WAIS-III); Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST); Mean (M); Standard Deviation (SD).

Results

Norms

Tables 1 through 11 report the mean and standard deviation of the neuropsychological tests for each age group divided by level of education (Age ranges: 18-20, 21-25, 26-30, 31-35, 36-40, 41-45, 46-50, 51-55, 56-60, 61-65, and > 65; Education (years): 1-5, 6-11, 12).

Effect of Age, Education and Sex

After the Bonferroni correction was applied, age was negatively correlated with Constructional Praxis Delayed Recall (r = −.080, p < .001), RCFT Delayed Recall (r = −.081, p < .001), RCFT Copy (r = −.052, p < .001), WAIS-III Digit Symbol (r = −0.67, p < .001), and Phonemic Fluency (FAS) (r = −0.53, p < .001), while it was positively correlated to TMT- A (rs = .063, p < .001). None of the other cognitive measures showed an association with age (see Table 12).

Table 12.

Correlations between tests with age and years of education


Test Age Education
Cognitive Screen
CERAD Total −0.028 0.097**
MMSE a −0.012 0.049*
Language
Semantic Fluency (Animals) −0.023 0.063**
BNT-15 −0.011 0.103**
Memory
Word List Learning −0.035 0.086**
Word List Delayed Recall −0.034 0.055**
Word List Recognition a −0.008 0.006
MIS Free Recall −0.028 0.021
MIS Cued Recall 0.016 0.048
MCT List 1 Cued Recall a −0.032 0.078**
MCT List 2 Cued Recall −0.042* 0.065**
MCT List 1 Cued Recall 2 −0.012 0.089**
MCT List 2 Cued Recall 2 −0.021 0.052*
MCT Total Free Recall −0.043* 0.075**
CP Delayed Recall −0.080** 0.106**
RCFT Delayed Recall −0.081** 0.070**
Visuospatial
CP Copy −0.023 0.065**
RCFT Copy −0.052** 0.087**
Raven’s Progressive Matrices −0.052* 0.074**
Processing Speed
TMT-Aa 0.063** −0.126**
WAIS-III Digit Symbol −0.067** 0.081**
A Cancellation Correct a −0.096 0.044
Executive Functioning
Phonemic Fluency (FAS) −0.053** 0.044*
WAIS-III Arithmetic −0.037 0.043
WCST Total Correct −0.026 0.059**
WCST Total Errors 0.029 −0.060**
WCST Total Categories −0.022 0.059**
WCST Perseverations 0.031 −0.061**
WCST Failure Maintain Set a −0.006 0.017
a

= Spearman’s rho was used in these tests due to lack of normality.

**.

Correlation is significant after Bonferroni correction (p ≤ .002, α = .05, 24 models)

*.

Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (2-tailed).

Note. Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE); Boston Naming Test (BNT); Memory Impairment Screening (MIS); Memory Capacity Test (MCT); CERAD Constructional Praxis (CP); Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure (RCFT); Trail Making Test-A (TMT-A); Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III (WAIS-III); Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST); Mean (M); Standard Deviation (SD).

Most cognitive measures, including Semantic Fluency, BNT-15, Word List Learning, Raven’s Progressive Matrices, and WAIS-III Digit Symbol had significant positive correlations with education (e.g., Semantic Fluency [Animals], r = .063, p < .001; BNT-15, r = .103, p < .001; Word List Learning, r = .086, p < .001; Raven’s Progressive Matrices, r = .074, p < .001; and WAIS-III Digit Symbol, r = .081, p < .001). Processing speed, as measured by TMT-A completion time was negatively correlated with education (rs = −.126, p < .001), as well as WCST Total Errors (r = −.06, p < .001) and WCST Perseverations (r = −.06, p < .001). T-test analyses revealed sex differences in performance on a few cognitive measures. Nevertheless, only the sex difference on the subtest of Word List Learning survived correction for multiple comparisons (p ≤ .002), and all effect size were small (see Table 13).

Discussion

Norms for neuropsychological tests have been traditionally obtained from English-speaking individuals posing a challenge when evaluating individuals from diverse cultural and linguistic backgrounds, such as Spanish-speaking individuals. Further, Hispanic populations are culturally heterogeneous, which poses a greater challenge since cultural backgrounds are known to impact neuropsychological performance (Ardila, 2005; Brickman et al., 2006). Recognizing these limitations, the neuropsychology field in Latin America and other Spanish-speaking regions has made great strides validating appropriate measures and developing norms, but there is still a need to develop more comprehensive normative data that accounts for different sociodemographic characteristics such as age, gender, level of education, and linguistic background to improve diagnostic accuracy and assessment of cognitive function (Ardila, Rosselli, & Puente, 1994).

We provided norms from a well-characterized Colombian population, which has been followed by the GNA since the 1990s. We extended previous normative studies for the CERAD-Col using an extraordinarily large sample that included 2,673 cognitively normal Spanish-speaking individuals. Our sample also broadened the age and educational attainment range (18 to 86 years old; 1-25 years of education). Large sample sizes are uncommon in normative studies with Spanish-speaking individuals, highlighting the contribution of this project to the existing literature. Moreover, past efforts to establish normative information for Spanish speakers using the CERAD have been conducted primarily with older or oldest old adults (Carrión-Baralt et al., 2009; Guerrero-Berroa et al., 2016; Fillenbaum, Kuchibhatla, Henderson, Clark, & Taussig, 2007). Lastly, to our knowledge, we provided the first comprehensive normative data for commonly used measures including the Memory Impairment Screening and Memory Capacity Test (Pérez-Martinez, Baztán, González-Becerra, & Socorro, 2005; Kuslansky, Buschke, Katz, Sliwinski, & Lipton, 2002; O’Connell & Tuokko, 2002).

Effect of Age, Education, and Sex

The results from the correlation analyses did not reveal strong associations between age and education and performance on the neuropsychological tests. With this caveat in mind, age was negatively associated with several cognitive measures as previously shown by Tombaugh (2004), wherein as age increased, performance on Constructional Praxis Delayed Recall, RCFT Delayed Recall, RCFT Copy, WAIS-III Digit Symbol, and Phonemic Fluency (FAS) worsened. Similarly, as age increased, performance on a measure of processing speed slowed down (i.e., TMT-A: Salthouse, 1985). No significant correlations were obtained for age and the WCST, Semantic Fluency (Animals), Word List Learning and the MMSE, perhaps because changes in these tests are typically observed in older populations (Butler, Ashford, & Snowdon, 1996; Mungas, Reed, Farias, & DeCarli, 2009). Regarding the associations between education level and neuropsychological test performance, while several correlations remained significant after correcting for multiple comparisons, their strength was very small. Nevertheless, as previously shown, higher levels of education related to higher scores (Ardila, Rosselli, & Rosas, 1989; Manly et al., 1999) in both verbal and nonverbal tests (Rosselli & Ardila, 2003). Finally, while males performed significantly better on Word List Learning than females, the effect size was very small. Thus, we decided not to stratify the norms per sex. In sum, the effects of age, education, and sex were small, suggesting the robustness of these norms.

Limitations

There are important limitations to this work. While the sample size was appropriate for this normative procedure, the age distribution was skewed, with approximately half of the sample falling under the age of 30. This may result from the recruitment efforts of the API registry targeting non-carriers from families at risk for early-onset AD. This age distribution may also explain the weak correlations of some test scores with age. Furthermore, given that individuals over the age of 40 were less likely to have 12 or more years of education, we stratified the norms in high and low education for those age groups using the median years of education. This arrangement resulted in larger sample sizes for those age groups and better representation of the sample. Notably, differences in access to education and its quality vary greatly in rural as opposed to urban zones in developing countries, such as Colombia (Pulido, Heredia, & Angel, 2010). Thus, differences in quality of education may restrict the validity of utilizing years of education as a measure. Future research may investigate how factors related to education such as quality of education can impact cognitive test performance (Sisco et al., 2015).

Furthermore, the sample was restricted geographically as all participants were recruited from the same area of Colombia (Antioquia region), and thus generalization to other Spanish-speaking individuals may not be appropriate. These limitations are not uncommon in retrospective studies with Latin American older adults, and do not hamper the importance and utility of this study. In addition, these normative data are an important resource for clinicians and researchers working with Colombian populations, or those with similar characteristics. Future research should strive to address these limitations by recruiting individuals from larger representative sample from diverse regions and backgrounds.

Additionally, the sample size is not equal among all tests because not all participants received the same battery of tests across the years, an inherent limitation of a retrospective study that nonetheless does not reduce the clinical utility of the normative data. Notably, the “A” Cancellation test was removed from the protocol due to psychometric limitations, resulting in smaller number of participants over the age of 33 with scores for this test.

Lastly, some of the norms obtained from this sample may differ from previous norms published with Spanish-speaking individuals from other regions. For instance, our norms varied from those published by Ostrosky-Solís et al. (2007) across ages and education levels. Several factors may account for these differences, including sociodemographic, cultural, or educational characteristics of our sample. Future research should explore differences among available norms directly and identify contributing factors to such differences so that we expand our understanding on how social and cultural factors impact neuropsychological performance. These limitations are not uncommon in retrospective studies with Latin American older adults, and do not hamper the importance and utility of this study. Future research should strive to address these limitations by recruiting individuals from larger representative sample from diverse regions and backgrounds.

Conclusion & Future Directions

In sum, we provide normative data stratified by age and level of education for frequently used neuropsychological measures. Our work expands previous efforts by increasing the sample size, widening the age range and educational backgrounds, and providing normative data for tests that currently lack norms for Spanish-speaking individuals. These norms may be useful to clinicians and researchers to improve the diagnostic utility of neuropsychological evaluations and reduce misdiagnosis across the lifespan. Future research will benefit from including individuals from diverse regions, across the lifespan, including older adults with higher education. These data may also inform research studies, including ongoing biomarker studies and clinical trials that are being conducted with individuals with autosomal-dominant AD (Tariot et al., 2018: API Clinical Trial [ NCT01998841]).

Supplementary Material

Supp 1

Table 2.

Means and standard deviation for ages 21-25

Education (years)
1-5 (n = 89) 6-11 (n = 251) ≥ 12 (n = 191)
Test n M (SD) M (SD) M (SD)
Cognitive Screen
CERAD Total 531 69.6 (11.83) 72.24 (11.11) 72.31 (10.6)
MMSE /30 531 28.47 (2.48) 28.9 (1.5) 28.86 (1.72)
Language
Semantic Fluency (Animals) 531 16.87 (4.71) 17.95 (4.65) 17.92 (4.48)
BNT-15 /15 531 11.91 (2.26) 12.22 (1.92) 12.57 (1.74)
Memory
Word List Learning /30 531 16.78 (3.9) 17.06 (3.97) 17.25 (3.89)
Word List Delayed Recall /10 531 6.28 (1.62) 6.31 (1.8) 6.38 (1.83)
Word List Recognition /10 531 9.7 (0.78) 9.54 (0.96) 9.54 (0.95)
MIS Free Recall /8 531 6.57 (1.86) 6.63 (1.77) 6.59 (1.85)
MIS Cued Recall /8 531 0.8 (0.68) 0.93 (0.73) 1.03 (0.64)
MCT List 1 Cued Recall 531 14.78 (1.62) 14.85 (1.89) 14.94 (1.73)
MCT List 2 Cued Recall 531 12.01 (3.51) 12.37 (3.27) 12.12 (3.07)
MCT List 1 Cued Recall 2 531 13.83 (2.43) 14.42 (2.17) 14.28 (2.28)
MCT List 2 Cued Recall 2 531 12.18 (3.28) 12.78 (3.23) 12.52 (2.98)
MCT Total Free Recall 531 18.58 (5.58) 20.06 (5.61) 19.43 (5.51)
CP Delayed Recall /11 531 7.81 (3.02) 8.63 (2.5) 8.49 (2.46)
RCFT Delayed Recall /16 531 14.29 (6.64) 15.74 (7.22) 15.17 (6.65)
Visuospatial
CP Copy /11 531 9.48 (1.79) 9.73 (1.53) 9.81 (1.42)
RCFT Copy /36 531 26.81 (5.99) 27.52 (6) 28.41 (5.17)
Raven’s Progressive Matrices /12 531 8.93 (1.99) 8.65 (2.08) 8.55 (1.84)
Processing Speed
TMT-A (seconds) 531 73.74 (42.58) 65.63 (50.75) 63.93 (38.85)
WAIS-III Digit Symbol 531 42.82 (17.4) 46.19 (18.29) 43.56 (17.55)
A Cancellation Correct /16 531 14.53 (0.83) 14.48 (1.08) 14.5 (0.92)
Executive Functioning
Phonemic Fluency (FAS) 531 24.92 (12.17) 27.19 (11.18) 26.43 (11.81)
WAIS-III Arithmetic 531 7.36 (2.62) 7.52 (2.2) 7.86 (1.98)
WCST Total Correct 531 20.88 (8.91) 21.56 (7.96) 20.94 (7.83)
WCST Total Errors 531 27.12 (8.91) 26.41 (8.01) 27 (7.84)
WCST Total Categories /6 531 2.62 (1.56) 2.8 (1.35) 2.73 (1.31)
WCST Perseverations 531 19.67 (9.23) 18.46 (7.59) 19.13 (7.82)
WCST Failure Maintain Set 531 0.49 (0.82) 0.46 (0.78) 0.45 (0.85)

Note. Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE); Boston Naming Test (BNT); Memory Impairment Screening (MIS); Memory Capacity Test (MCT); CERAD Constructional Praxis (CP); Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure (RCFT); Trail Making Test-A (TMT-A); Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III (WAIS-III); Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST); Mean (M); Standard Deviation (SD).

Table 3.

Means and standard deviation for ages 26-30

Education (years)
1-5 (n = 114) 6-11 (n = 214) ≥ 12 (n = 140)
Test n M (SD) M (SD) M (SD)
Cognitive Screen
CERAD Total 468 69.46 (12.49) 72.62 (11) 73.92 (11.59)
MMSE /30 468 28.57 (1.67) 28.76 (1.63) 28.97 (1.67)
Language
Semantic Fluency (Animals) 468 17.43 (4.98) 18.17 (4.76) 18.66 (4.89)
BNT-15 /15 468 11.96 (1.93) 12.56 (1.81) 12.83 (1.84)
Memory
Word List Learning /30 468 16.19 (4.01) 17.45 (3.82) 17.54 (4.21)
Word List Delayed Recall /10 468 5.95 (1.99) 6.35 (1.85) 6.5 (1.85)
Word List Recognition /10 468 9.34 (1.38) 9.41 (1.19) 9.39 (1.32)
MIS Free Recall /8 468 6.19 (1.94) 6.63 (1.77) 6.74 (1.61)
MIS Cued Recall /8 468 0.96 (0.64) 0.9 (0.61) 0.89 (0.69)
MCT List 1 Cued Recall 468 14.14 (2.76) 14.86 (1.52) 15.06 (1.65)
MCT List 2 Cued Recall 468 11.35 (3.92) 11.91 (2.98) 12.64 (3.05)
MCT List 1 Cued Recall 2 468 13.31 (3.14) 14.21 (2.11) 14.49 (2.32)
MCT List 2 Cued Recall 2 468 11.52 (3.86) 12.11 (3.06) 12.9 (3.14)
MCT Total Free Recall 468 18.08 (6.15) 19.17 (5.56) 20.95 (5.59)
CP Delayed Recall /11 468 8.03 (2.77) 8.41 (2.63) 8.73 (2.5)
RCFT Delayed Recall /16 468 14.75 (7.14) 15.34 (7.46) 16.72 (7.14)
Visuospatial
CP Copy /11 468 9.65 (1.71) 9.69 (1.51) 9.88 (1.38)
RCFT Copy /36 468 26.03 (6.48) 27.68 (6.49) 28.07 (5.82)
Raven’s Progressive Matrices /12 468 8.43 (2.1) 8.8 (1.95) 9.2 (2.09)
Processing Speed
TMT-A (seconds) 468 77.64 (47.36) 68.78 (40.94) 71.44 (75.5)
WAIS-III Digit Symbol 468 41.29 (18.57) 45.48 (18.15) 48.58 (18.24)
A Cancellation Correct /16 468 14.71 (0.77) 14.31 (1.12) 14.11 (1.28)
Executive Functioning
Phonemic Fluency (FAS) 468 24.89 (11.71) 25.32 (10.97) 26.1 (11.77)
WAIS-III Arithmetic 468 7.38 (2.25) 7.73 (1.93) 8.17 (5.66)
WCST Total Correct 468 18.83 (8.48) 20.83 (8.75) 21.55 (8.76)
WCST Total Errors 468 29.17 (8.48) 26.99 (8.96) 26.41 (8.82)
WCST Total Categories /6 468 2.41 (1.33) 2.71 (1.53) 2.71 (1.5)
WCST Perseverations 468 20.92 (9.08) 19.33 (8.47) 19.12 (9.09)
WCST Failure Maintain Set 468 0.34 (0.76) 0.5 (0.8) 0.66 (1.3)

Note. Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE); Boston Naming Test (BNT); Memory Impairment Screening (MIS); Memory Capacity Test (MCT); CERAD Constructional Praxis (CP); Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure (RCFT); Trail Making Test-A (TMT-A); Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III (WAIS-III); Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST); Mean (M); Standard Deviation (SD).

Table 4.

Means and standard deviation for ages 31-35

Education (years)
1-5 (n = 134) 6-11 (n = 182) ≥ 12 (n = 87)
Test n M (SD) M (SD) M (SD)
Cognitive Screen
CERAD Total 403 70.56 (10.27) 71.62 (11.67) 74.2 (9.63)
MMSE /30 403 28.84 (1.72) 28.99 (1.63) 28.89 (1.42)
Language
Semantic Fluency (Animals) 403 17.66 (5.15) 17.83 (5.22) 18.56 (4.4)
BNT-15 /15 403 11.97 (2.19) 12.36 (2.18) 12.66 (1.86)
Memory
Word List Learning /30 403 16.67 (3.32) 16.9 (3.85) 17.77 (3.66)
Word List Delayed Recall /10 403 6.37 (1.61) 6.21 (1.9) 6.35 (1.73)
Word List Recognition /10 403 9.56 (0.94) 9.74 (0.69) 9.64 (0.71)
MIS Free Recall /8 403 6.52 (1.57) 6.73 (1.78) 6.56 (1.66)
MIS Cued Recall /8 403 0.82 (0.59) 0.9 (0.8) 1.05 (0.63)
MCT List 1 Cued Recall 403 14.66 (1.9) 14.66 (1.96) 14.92 (2)
MCT List 2 Cued Recall 403 11.59 (3.24) 11.79 (3.57) 12.19 (3.24)
MCT List 1 Cued Recall 2 403 14.06 (2.36) 14.18 (2.43) 14.67 (2.01)
MCT List 2 Cued Recall 2 403 11.97 (3.21) 12.03 (3.65) 12.59 (3.03)
MCT Total Free Recall 403 19.03 (5.94) 19.56 (6.16) 19.67 (5.12)
CP Delayed Recall /11 403 7.99 (2.81) 8.43 (2.79) 8.67 (2.45)
RCFT Delayed Recall /16 403 13.91 (6.42) 15.62 (7.02) 15.95 (7.34)
Visuospatial
CP Copy /11 403 9.6 (1.67) 9.81 (1.45) 9.74 (1.47)
RCFT Copy /36 403 26.84 (5.81) 27.99 (6.4) 28.63 (5.87)
Raven’s Progressive Matrices /12 403 8.59 (1.93) 8.87 (1.94) 9.12 (1.64)
Processing Speed
TMT-A (seconds) 403 77.89 (48.04) 67.86 (45.16) 60.52 (38.46)
WAIS-III Digit Symbol 403 40.52 (18.19) 47.63 (20.09) 48.09 (18.11)
A Cancellation Correct /16 403 14.74 (0.54) 14.41 (0.93) 15 (0)
Executive Functioning
Phonemic Fluency (FAS) 403 23.92 (11.95) 24.43 (11.21) 27.17 (10.53)
WAIS-III Arithmetic 403 8.33 (1.32) 7.96 (2.16) 8.37 (1.6)
WCST Total Correct 403 21.11 (9.14) 22.85 (8.44) 19 (8.33)
WCST Total Errors 403 26.86 (9.17) 25.14 (8.45) 29 (8.33)
WCST Total Categories /6 403 2.83 (1.55) 3.12 (1.53) 2.6 (1.48)
WCST Perseverations 403 19.66 (8.84) 17.65 (8.3) 20.33 (8.72)
WCST Failure Maintain Set 403 0.42 (0.89) 0.38 (0.65) 0.35 (0.77)

Note. Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE); Boston Naming Test (BNT); Memory Impairment Screening (MIS); Memory Capacity Test (MCT); CERAD Constructional Praxis (CP); Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure (RCFT); Trail Making Test-A (TMT-A); Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III (WAIS-III); Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST); Mean (M); Standard Deviation (SD).

Table 5.

Means and standard deviation for ages 36-40

Education (years)
1-5 (n = 111) 6-11 (n = 99) ≥ 12 (n = 67)
Test n M (SD) M (SD) M (SD)
Cognitive Screen
CERAD Total 277 68.89 (12.95) 71.92 (11.25) 71.25 (10.24)
MMSE /30 277 28.24 (2.45) 28.72 (1.89) 28.75 (1.65)
Language
Semantic Fluency (Animals) 277 17.77 (4.77) 17.76 (4.64) 18.09 (4.67)
BNT-15 /15 277 11.79 (2.22) 12.46 (1.9) 12.42 (1.96)
Memory
Word List Learning /30 277 16.48 (4.19) 17.28 (4.27) 17.06 (3.32)
Word List Delayed Recall /10 277 5.9 (2.04) 6.51 (2.09) 6.22 (1.81)
Word List Recognition /10 277 9.41 (1.37) 9.34 (1.22) 9.47 (1.03)
MIS Free Recall /8 277 6.14 (1.94) 6.45 (1.71) 6.15 (2.24)
MIS Cued Recall /8 277 0.83 (0.7) 1.04 (0.74) 1.1 (0.72)
MCT List 1 Cued Recall 277 14.41 (2.22) 14.86 (1.99) 14.62 (1.97)
MCT List 2 Cued Recall 277 11.2 (3.58) 12.13 (3.38) 11.38 (3.71)
MCT List 1 Cued Recall 2 277 13.55 (2.79) 14.08 (2.6) 14.21 (2.18)
MCT List 2 Cued Recall 2 277 11.56 (3.47) 12.35 (3.26) 11.73 (3.64)
MCT Total Free Recall 277 18.45 (5.97) 18.89 (5.97) 18.56 (6.07)
CP Delayed Recall /11 277 7.54 (2.7) 8.3 (2.58) 8.58 (2.37)
RCFT Delayed Recall /16 277 14.49 (6.78) 15.08 (6.39) 15.64 (6.8)
Visuospatial
CP Copy /11 277 9.38 (1.57) 9.7 (1.51) 9.97 (1.28)
RCFT Copy /36 277 25.77 (7.49) 26.85 (6.47) 27.7 (6.34)
Raven’s Progressive Matrices /12 277 8.45 (1.94) 8.75 (1.99) 8.98 (1.93)
Processing Speed
TMT-A (seconds) 277 84.21 (53.93) 65.93 (33.71) 66.57 (43.82)
WAIS-III Digit Symbol 277 42.74 (19.73) 43.94 (18.02) 44.63 (20.9)
A Cancellation Correct /16 277 14.4 (1.08) 14.67 (0.62) 14.53 (0.74)
Executive Functioning
Phonemic Fluency (FAS) 277 23.95 (14.62) 24.97 (11.69) 25.55 (13.09)
WAIS-III Arithmetic 277 7.12 (2.47) 8.43 (1.6) 7.8 (1.9)
WCST Total Correct 277 20.1 (7.91) 22.22 (8.2) 22.65 (7.68)
WCST Total Errors 277 27.85 (7.92) 25.74 (8.24) 25.35 (7.68)
WCST Total Categories /6 277 2.55 (1.32) 2.84 (1.48) 2.97 (1.45)
WCST Perseverations 277 20.38 (8.88) 18.24 (7.88) 18.23 (7.55)
WCST Failure Maintain Set 277 0.44 (0.67) 0.6 (1.16) 0.57 (0.72)

Note. Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE); Boston Naming Test (BNT); Memory Impairment Screening (MIS); Memory Capacity Test (MCT); CERAD Constructional Praxis (CP); Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure (RCFT); Trail Making Test-A (TMT-A); Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III (WAIS-III); Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST); Mean (M); Standard Deviation (SD).

Table 6.

Means and standard deviation for ages 41-45

Education (years)
0-4 (n = 38) ≥5 (n = 145)
Test n M (SD) M (SD)
Cognitive Screen
CERAD Total 183 72.26 (12.75) 73.23 (11.34)
MMSE /30 183 28.32 (2.29) 28.88 (1.51)
Language
Semantic Fluency (Animals) 183 17.95 (4.76) 18.53 (4.98)
BNT-15 /15 183 11.76 (2.14) 12.17 (1.95)
Memory
Word List Learning /30 183 16.58 (5.1) 16.81 (3.9)
Word List Delayed Recall /10 183 6.18 (2.15) 6.27 (1.8)
Word List Recognition /10 183 9.59 (0.89) 9.5 (0.91)
MIS Free Recall /8 183 6.71 (1.51) 6.36 (1.84)
MIS Cued Recall /8 183 0.67 (0.49) 0.95 (0.69)
MCT List 1 Cued Recall 183 14.55 (2.43) 14.84 (1.68)
MCT List 2 Cued Recall 183 11.68 (3.28) 11.61 (3.41)
MCT List 1 Cued Recall 2 183 13.58 (2.73) 14.22 (2.29)
MCT List 2 Cued Recall 2 183 12.29 (3.2) 12.21 (3.14)
MCT Total Free Recall 183 18.32 (5.88) 18.99 (6.16)
CP Delayed Recall /11 183 7.53 (3.45) 8.1 (2.62)
RCFT Delayed Recall /16 183 14.61 (7.69) 14.07 (7.35)
Visuospatial
CP Copy /11 183 9.63 (1.55) 9.66 (1.47)
RCFT Copy /36 183 27.37 (6.8) 27.2 (5.87)
Raven’s Progressive Matrices /12 183 8.59 (2.2) 8.54 (1.97)
Processing Speed
TMT-A (seconds) 183 76.03 (50.38) 69.41 (35.89)
WAIS-III Digit Symbol 183 42.32 (20.41) 43.5 (17.81)
A Cancellation Correct /16 183 13.5 (1.52) 14.44 (1.34)
Executive Functioning
Phonemic Fluency (FAS) 183 22.62 (11.78) 24.09 (11.38)
WAIS-III Arithmetic 183 8 (2.35) 7.7 (1.92)
WCST Total Correct 183 21.61 (7.2) 21.48 (8.95)
WCST Total Errors 183 26.39 (7.2) 26.52 (8.95)
WCST Total Categories /6 183 2.69 (1.31) 2.75 (1.45)
WCST Perseverations 183 19.47 (7.04) 18.45 (8.62)
WCST Failure Maintain Set 183 0.69 (1.31) 0.61 (0.91)

Note. Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE); Boston Naming Test (BNT); Memory Impairment Screening (MIS); Memory Capacity Test (MCT); CERAD Constructional Praxis (CP); Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure (RCFT); Trail Making Test-A (TMT-A); Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III (WAIS-III); Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST); Mean (M); Standard Deviation (SD).

Table 7.

Means and standard deviation for ages 46-50

Education (years)
0-4 (n = 50) ≥ 5 (n = 120)
Test n M(SD) M (SD)
Cognitive Screen
CERAD Total 170 73.27 (10.92) 73 (11.6)
MMSE /30 170 28.88 (2.04) 28.85 (1.87)
Language
Semantic Fluency (Animals) 170 17.6 (4.46) 17.74 (5.32)
BNT-15 /15 170 12.66 (1.81) 12.41 (1.89)
Memory
Word List Learning /30 170 16.46 (4.98) 17.37 (4.11)
Word List Delayed Recall /10 170 6.1 (2.35) 6.25 (1.76)
Word List Recognition /10 170 9.39 (1.07) 9.49 (0.98)
MIS Free Recall /8 170 6.83 (1.38) 6.2 (1.81)
MIS Cued Recall /8 170 0.76 (0.44) 0.95 (0.63)
MCT List 1 Cued Recall 170 14.81 (1.65) 14.81 (1.75)
MCT List 2 Cued Recall 170 11.75 (3.88) 11.74 (3.45)
MCT List 1 Cued Recall 2 170 14.31 (2.21) 14.44 (1.93)
MCT List 2 Cued Recall 2 170 12.53 (3.64) 12.34 (3.38)
MCT Total Free Recall 170 18.81 (6.35) 19.56 (5.66)
CP Delayed Recall /11 170 7.88 (3.03) 8.13 (2.72)
RCFT Delayed Recall /16 170 14.74 (8.19) 15.12 (7.28)
Visuospatial
CP Copy /11 170 9.62 (1.52) 9.77 (1.63)
RCFT Copy /36 170 26.82 (6.91) 27.9 (6.77)
Raven’s Progressive Matrices /12 170 8.19 (2.11) 8.79 (1.94)
Processing Speed
TMT-A (seconds) 170 68.15 (38.51) 69.43 (34.76)
WAIS-III Digit Symbol 170 45.03 (19.26) 42.19 (16.3)
A Cancellation Correct /16 170 14 (1.24) 14.38 (0.81)
Executive Functioning
Phonemic Fluency (FAS) 170 23.8 (13.57) 24.38 (10.81)
WAIS-III Arithmetic 170 7.91 (2.39) 8.07 (1.54)
WCST Total Correct 170 19.53 (7.9) 20.5 (8.95)
WCST Total Errors 170 28.47 (7.9) 27.47 (9)
WCST Total Categories /6 170 2.38 (1.36) 2.72 (1.46)
WCST Perseverations 170 20.13 (7.65) 20.58 (9.78)
WCST Failure Maintain Set 170 0.45 (0.65) 0.44 (0.76)

Note. Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE); Boston Naming Test (BNT); Memory Impairment Screening (MIS); Memory Capacity Test (MCT); CERAD Constructional Praxis (CP); Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure (RCFT); Trail Making Test-A (TMT-A); Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III (WAIS-III); Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST); Mean (M); Standard Deviation (SD).

Table 8.

Means and standard deviation for ages 51-55

Education (years)
0-4 (n = 40) ≥ 5 (n = 80)
Test n M (SD) M (SD)
Cognitive Screen
CERAD Total 120 70.41 (10.71) 72.76 (11.69)
MMSE /30 120 28.6 (1.88) 28.83 (1.44)
Language
Semantic Fluency (Animals) 120 16.38 (3.71) 17.69 (5.38)
BNT-15 /15 120 12.18 (2.07) 12.57 (1.97)
Memory
Word List Learning /30 120 16 (4.19) 17.34 (3.98)
Word List Delayed Recall /10 120 6.08 (1.65) 6.32 (1.84)
Word List Recognition /10 120 9.59 (0.82) 9.45 (1.14)
MIS Free Recall /8 120 6.24 (1.95) 6.3 (1.82)
MIS Cued Recall /8 120 1.05 (0.78) 0.92 (0.66)
MCT List 1 Cued Recall 120 14.44 (1.93) 14.79 (1.56)
MCT List 2 Cued Recall 120 11.35 (3.89) 12.12 (3.15)
MCT List 1 Cued Recall 2 120 13.41 (2.79) 14.33 (2.04)
MCT List 2 Cued Recall 2 120 11.85 (3.47) 12.77 (2.74)
MCT Total Free Recall 120 17.74 (6.32) 20.2 (5.48)
CP Delayed Recall /11 120 7.58 (2.74) 8.12 (2.59)
RCFT Delayed Recall /16 120 13.91 (6.49) 13.96 (6.4)
Visuospatial
CP Copy /11 120 9.83 (1.48) 9.6 (1.63)
RCFT Copy /36 120 26.87 (6.33) 28.01 (5.04)
Raven’s Progressive Matrices /12 120 8.41 (1.91) 8.66 (1.57)
Processing Speed
TMT-A (seconds) 120 69.89 (39.12) 76.49 (110.08)
WAIS-III Digit Symbol 120 41.18 (19.28) 42.76 (18.12)
A Cancellation Correct /16 120 13.6 (1.52) 14.69 (0.63)
Executive Functioning
Phonemic Fluency (FAS) 120 24.17 (10.9) 24.5 (11.92)
WAIS-III Arithmetic 120 6.75 (1.26) 7.92 (1.88)
WCST Total Correct 120 18.37 (7.63) 19.96 (8.24)
WCST Total Errors 120 29.63 (7.63) 28.04 (8.24)
WCST Total Categories /6 120 2.45 (1.31) 2.56 (1.28)
WCST Perseverations 120 22.24 (7.73) 19.38 (7.97)
WCST Failure Maintain Set 120 0.35 (0.65) 0.33 (0.75)

Note. Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE); Boston Naming Test (BNT); Memory Impairment Screening (MIS); Memory Capacity Test (MCT); CERAD Constructional Praxis (CP); Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure (RCFT); Trail Making Test-A (TMT-A); Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III (WAIS-III); Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST); Mean (M); Standard Deviation (SD).

Table 9.

Means and standard deviation for ages 56-60

Education (years)
0-4 (n = 29) ≥ 5 (n = 38)
Test n M (SD) M (SD)
Cognitive Screen
CERAD Total 67 74.22 (12.57) 72.19 (9.49)
MMSE /30 67 28.52 (2.43) 28.32 (1.92)
Language
Semantic Fluency (Animals) 67 17.71 (4.66) 17.13 (4.06)
BNT-15 /15 67 12.68 (2.2) 12.61 (1.84)
Memory
Word List Learning /30 67 17.54 (3.95) 16 (3.68)
Word List Delayed Recall /10 67 6.46 (1.9) 5.78 (2.11)
Word List Recognition /10 67 9.52 (1.12) 9.55 (0.68)
MIS Free Recall /8 67 6.52 (2.11) 6.39 (2.28)
MIS Cued Recall /8 67 1.3 (0.48) 1.31 (0.85)
MCT List 1 Cued Recall 67 14.78 (2.26) 14.52 (1.84)
MCT List 2 Cued Recall 67 11.57 (4.09) 11.26 (3.45)
MCT List 1 Cued Recall 2 67 14.61 (2.33) 14.23 (2.26)
MCT List 2 Cued Recall 2 67 12.43 (3.68) 11.94 (3.18)
MCT Total Free Recall 67 19.65 (6.29) 18.23 (6.06)
CP Delayed Recall /11 67 8.46 (2.47) 7.58 (2.93)
RCFT Delayed Recall /16 67 16.07 (6.44) 13.32 (6.8)
Visuospatial
CP Copy /11 67 9.82 (1.25) 9.61 (1.46)
RCFT Copy /36 67 27.46 (5.16) 25.11 (8.01)
Raven’s Progressive Matrices /12 67 8.55 (2.11) 8.42 (1.79)
Processing Speed
TMT-A (seconds) 67 72.86 (54.75) 86.53 (70.13)
WAIS-III Digit Symbol 67 46 (18.11) 41.57 (18.56)
A Cancellation Correct /16 67 14.83 (0.41) 12.83 (2.64)
Executive Functioning
Phonemic Fluency (FAS) 67 24.21 (12.43) 26.08 (12.31)
WAIS-III Arithmetic 67 8.2 (1.92) 7.6 (2.61)
WCST Total Correct 67 20.82 (8.05) 20.97 (7.89)
WCST Total Errors 67 27.18 (8.05) 27.03 (7.89)
WCST Total Categories /6 67 2.57 (1.17) 2.47 (1.4)
WCST Perseverations 67 19.14 (7.99) 18.06 (6.66)
WCST Failure Maintain Set 67 0.39 (0.58) 0.68 (1.3)

Note. Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE); Boston Naming Test (BNT); Memory Impairment Screening (MIS); Memory Capacity Test (MCT); CERAD Constructional Praxis (CP); Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure (RCFT); Trail Making Test-A (TMT-A); Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III (WAIS-III); Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST); Mean (M); Standard Deviation (SD).

Table 10.

Means and standard deviation for ages 61-65

Education (years)
0-4 (n = 22) ≥ 5 (n = 32)
Test n M (SD) M (SD)
Cognitive Screen
CERAD Total 54 68.62 (7.1) 72.28 (10.76)
MMSE /30 54 28.91 (1.06) 28.25 (2.24)
Language
Semantic Fluency (Animals) 54 16.09 (3.16) 17.91 (4.69)
BNT-15 /15 54 11.77 (2.22) 12.47 (1.8)
Memory
Word List Learning /30 54 15.45 (2.52) 16.38 (3.54)
Word List Delayed Recall /10 54 6.14 (1.64) 6.34 (1.54)
Word List Recognition /10 54 9.76 (0.54) 9.45 (1.09)
MIS Free Recall /8 54 6.6 (1.31) 5.86 (2)
MIS Cued Recall /8 54 0.67 (0.65) 1.16 (0.9)
MCT List 1 Cued Recall 54 14.3 (1.72) 14.29 (1.98)
MCT List 2 Cued Recall 54 11.05 (3.41) 11.71 (2.73)
MCT List 1 Cued Recall 2 54 13.95 (1.88) 14.21 (2.5)
MCT List 2 Cued Recall 2 54 11.4 (3.44) 12.21 (2.7)
MCT Total Free Recall 54 17.35 (6.71) 18.18 (6.3)
CP Delayed Recall /11 54 7.55 (2.96) 7.34 (2.68)
RCFT Delayed Recall /16 54 13.39 (7.11) 11.75 (6.1)
Visuospatial
CP Copy /11 54 9.32 (1.39) 8.97 (2.06)
RCFT Copy /36 54 26.14 (5.98) 24.39 (7.55)
Raven’s Progressive Matrices /12 54 8.43 (1.94) 8.16 (2)
Processing Speed
TMT-A (seconds) 54 87.27 (54.51) 76.31 (48.02)
WAIS-III Digit Symbol 54 33.5 (10.58) 41.04 (15.53)
A Cancellation Correct /16 54 12 (2.83) 13.67 (2.31)
Executive Functioning
Phonemic Fluency (FAS) 54 24.5 (10.92) 27.47 (10)
WAIS-III Arithmetic 54 4.5 (0.71) 5.5 (0.71)
WCST Total Correct 54 19.73 (6.13) 20.06 (8.72)
WCST Total Errors 54 28.27 (6.13) 27.87 (8.73)
WCST Total Categories /6 54 2.55 (1.18) 2.48 (1.36)
WCST Perseverations 54 19.27 (6.21) 20.26 (9.04)
WCST Failure Maintain Set 54 0.48 (0.68) 0.63 (0.81)

Note. Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE); Boston Naming Test (BNT); Memory Impairment Screening (MIS); Memory Capacity Test (MCT); CERAD Constructional Praxis (CP); Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure (RCFT); Trail Making Test-A (TMT-A); Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III (WAIS-III); Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST); Mean (M); Standard Deviation (SD).

Acknowledgments

Funding

This work was supported by the Programa de Sostenibilidad 2018-2019 del CODI awarded to the Grupo de Neurociencias de Antioquia (FL), and grants from the NIH Office of the Director (DP5OD019833 [YTQ]), the National Institute on Aging (R01 AG054671 [YTQ], 1RF1AG041705, R01 AG055444 [EMR, PT, FL]; the Massachusetts General Hospital ECOR (1200-228010 [YTQ] and 1200-228767 [YTQ]), Banner Alzheimer’s Foundation, Genentech, and an anonymous Foundation.

Footnotes

Disclosure

The authors declare that they do not have any financial interest or benefit arising from direct applications of this research.

References

  1. Aguirre-Acevedo DC, Gómez RD, Moreno S, Henao-Arboleda E, Motta M, Muñoz C, & Lopera F (2007). Validez y fiabilidad de la batería neuropsicológica CERAD-Col. Revista de Neurología, 45(11), 655–660. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Aguirre-Acevedo DC, Jaimes-Barragán F, Henao E, Tirado V, Muñoz C, Reiman EM, Lopera F (2016). Diagnostic accuracy of CERAD total score in a Colombian cohort with mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease affected by E280A mutation on presenilin-1 gene. International Psychogeriatrics, 28(3), 503–510. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Alegret M, Espinosa A, Vinyes-Junqué G, Valero S, Hernández I, Tárraga L, Becker J, & Boada M (2012). Normative data of a brief neuropsychological battery for Spanish individuals older than 49. Journal of Clinical Experimental Neuropsychology, 34(2), 209–219. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Arango-Lasprilla JC (2015). Commonly used Neuropsychological Tests for Spanish Speakers: Normative Data from Latin America. NeuroRehabilitation, 37(4), 489–491. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Ardila A, Rosselli M, & Rosas P. (1989). Neuropsychological assessment in illiterates: Visuospatial and memory abilities. Brain and Cognition, 11(2), 147–166. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Ardila A, Rosselli M, & Puente AE (1994). Critical issues in neuropsychology Neuropsychological evaluation of the Spanish speaker. New York, NY, US: Plenum Press. [Google Scholar]
  7. Ardila A (2005). Cultural values underlying psychometric cognitive testing. Neuropsychology Review, 15(4), 185–195. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Benton AL & Hamsher K (1976) Multilingual Aphasia Examination Aphasia Examination. Iowa City, IA: University of Iowa. [Google Scholar]
  9. Brickman AM, Cabo R, & Manly JJ (2006). Ethical issues in cross-cultural neuropsychology. Applied Neuropsychology, 13(2), 91–100. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Buschke H (1984). Cued recall in amnesia. Journal of Clinical Neuropsychology, 6, 433–440. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Buschke H, Kuslansky G, Katz M, Stewart WF, Sliwinski MJ, Eckholdt HM, & Lipton RB (1999). Screening for dementia with the memory impairment screen. Neurology, 52(2), 231–238. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Butler SM, Ashford JW, & Snowdon DA (1996). Age, education, and changes in the Mini-Mental State Exam scores of older women: findings from the Nun Study. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 44(6), 675–681. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Carrión-Baralt JR, Meléndez-Cabrero J, Beeri MS, Sano M, & Silverman JM (2009). The neuropsychological performance of nondemented Puerto Rican nonagenarians. Dementia and geriatric cognitive disorders, 27(4), 353–360. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Chandler MJ, Lacritz LH, Hynan LS, Barnard HD, Allen G, Deschner M, Cullum CM (2005). A total score for the CERAD neuropsychological battery. Neurology, 65(1), 102–106. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Cornejo W, Lopera F, Uribe CS, & Salinas M (1987). Descripción de una familia con demencia presenil tipo Alzheimer. Acta Medica Colombiana, 12, 55–61. [Google Scholar]
  16. Curran PJ, West SG, & Finch JF (1996). The robustness of test statistics to nonnormality and specification error in confirmatory factor analysis. Psychological methods, 1(1), 16. [Google Scholar]
  17. Fillenbaum GG, Kuchibhatla M, Henderson VW, Clark CM, & Taussig IM (2007). Comparison of performance on the CERAD neuropsychological battery of Hispanic patients and cognitively normal controls at two sites. Clinical Gerontologist, 30(3), 1–22. [Google Scholar]
  18. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, & McHugh PR (1975). “Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12(3), 189–198. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Fritz CO, Morris PE, & Richler JJ (2012). Effect size estimates: current use, calculations, and interpretation. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 141(1), 2–18. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Guàrdia-Olmos J, Peró-Cebollero M, Rivera D, & Arango-Lasprilla JC (2015). Methodology for the development of normative data for ten Spanish-language neuropsychological tests in eleven Latin American countries. NeuroRehabilitation, 37(4), 493–499. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Guerrero-Berroa E, Schmeidler J, Raventos H, Valerio D, Beeri MS, Carrión-Baralt JR, ... & Silverman JM. (2016). Neuropsychological test performance in cognitively normal Spanish-speaking nonagenarians with little education. Journal of cross-cultural gerontology, 31(2), 129–141. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Hall JR, Balldin VH, Gamboa A, Edwards ML, Johnson LA, & O’Bryant SE (2018). Texas Mexican American adult normative studies: Normative data for the Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status (RBANS). Developmental Neuropsychology, 43:1, 27–35. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Henao-Arboleda E, Muñoz C, Aguirre-Acevedo DC, Lara E, Pineda DA, & Lopera F (2010). Datos normativos de pruebas neuropsicológicas en adultos mayores en una población Colombiana. Revista Chilena de Neuropsicología, 5(3), 214–226. [Google Scholar]
  24. IBM Corp. Released 2017. IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 25.0. Armonk, NY: IBM Corp. [Google Scholar]
  25. Judd T, Capetillo D, Carrión-Baralt J, Mármol LM, Miguel-Montes LS, Navarrete MG, NAN Policy and Planning Committee. (2009). Professional considerations for improving the neuropsychological evaluation of Hispanics: a National Academy of Neuropsychology education paper. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology: The Official Journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists, 24(2), 127–135. 10.1093/arclin/acp016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Kaplan EF, Goodglass H, Weintraub S (1983). The Boston Naming Test. 2nd Philadelphia: Lea & Febiger. [Google Scholar]
  27. Kuslansky G, Buschke H, Katz M, Sliwinski M, & Lipton RB (2002). Screening for Alzheimer’s Disease: The Memory Impairment Screen Versus the Conventional Three-Word Memory Test. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 50(6), 1086–1091. 10.1046/j.1532-5415.2002.50265.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Manly JJ, Jacobs DM, Sano M, Bell K, Merchant CA, Small SA, & Stern Y (1999). Effect of literacy on neuropsychological test performance in nondemented, education-matched elders. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 5(3), 191–202. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Morris JC, Mohs RC, Rogers H, Fillenbaum G, & Heyman A (1988). Consortium to establish a registry for Alzheimer’s disease (CERAD) clinical and neuropsychological assessment of Alzheimer’s disease. Psychopharmacology Bulletin, 24(4), 641–652. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Morris JC, Heyman A, Mohs RC, Hughes JP, Van Belle G, Fillenbaum GDME, ... & Clark C. (1989). The consortium to establish a registry for Alzheimer’s disease (CERAD): I. Clinical and neuropsychological assessment of Alzheimer’s disease . Neurology. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Mungas D, Reed BR, Farias ST, & DeCarli C (2009). Age and education effects on relationships of cognitive test scores with brain structure in demographically diverse older persons. Psychology and Aging, 24(1), 116–128. 10.1037/a0013421 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Muntal S, Doval E, Badenes D, Casas-Hernanz L, Cerulla N, Calzado N, & Aguilar M (2017). Nuevos datos normativos de la versión española de la Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status (RBANS) forma A. Neurología. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Nelson HE (1976). A modified card sorting test sensitive to frontal lobe defects. Cortex, 12, 313–324. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Newcombe F (1969). Missile wounds of the brain: A study of psychological deficits. London: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  35. O’Bryant SE, Edwards M, Johnson L, Hall J, Gamboa A, & O’jile J (2018). Texas Mexican American adult normative studies: Normative data for commonly used clinical neuropsychological measures for English-and Spanish-speakers. Developmental neuropsychology, 43(1), 1–26. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. O’Connell ME, & Tuokko H (2002). The 12-item Buschke memory test: appropriate for use across levels of impairment. Applied Neuropsychology, 9(4), 226–233. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Osterrieth PA (1944). Le test de copie d’une figure complexe; contribution à l’étude de la perception et de la mémoire. Archives de psychologie. [Google Scholar]
  38. Ostrosky-Solís F, Ardila A, & Rosselli M (1999). NEUROPSI: A brief neuropsychological test battery in Spanish with norms by age and educational level. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 5(05), 413–433. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Ostrosky-Solís F, Esther Gomez-Perez M, Matute E, Rosselli M, Ardila A, & Pineda D (2007). NEUROPSI ATTENTION AND MEMORY: a neuropsychological test battery in Spanish with norms by age and educational level. Applied Neuropsychology, 14(3), 156–170. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Ostrosky-Solís F, Gutierrez AL, Flores MR, & Ardila A (2007). Same or different? Semantic verbal fluency across Spanish-speakers from different countries. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 22(3), 367–377. 10.1016/j.acn.2007.01.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Peña-Casanova J, Casals-Coll M, Quintana M, Sánchez-Benavides G, Rognoni T, Calvo L, … Manero RM (2012). Spanish normative studies in a young adult population (NEURONORMA young adults Project): methods and characteristics of the sample. Neurologia (Barcelona, Spain), 27(5), 253–260. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Pérez-Martinez DA, Baztán JJ, González-Becerra M, & Socorro A (2005). Evaluation of the diagnostic value of a Spanish adaptation of the Buschke Memory Impairment Screen in the detection of dementia and cognitive impairment. Revista De Neurologia, 40(11), [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Pontón MO, Satz P, Herrera L, Ortiz F, Urrutia CP, Young R, & Namerow N (1996). Normative data stratified by age and education for the Neuropsychological Screening Battery for Hispanics (NeSBHIS): Initial report. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 2(2), 96–104.644–648. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Pulido O, Hereida M, & Angel C (2010). Las Desigualdades Educativas en Colombia. Ensayos e Investigaciones No 1. Retrieved from: https://www.academia.edu/31803582/LAS_DESIGUALDADES_EDUCATIVAS_EN_COLOMBIA.
  45. Rivera Mindt M, Byrd D, Saez P, & Manly J (2010). Increasing culturally competent neuropsychological services for ethnic minority populations: a call to action. Clinical Neuropsychology, 24(3), 429–453. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Reisberg B, Ferris SH, de Leon MJ, & Crook T (1988). Global Deterioration Scale (GDS). Psychopharmacology Bulletin, 24(4), 661–663. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Reitan RM, & Wolfson D (1985). The Halstead–Reitan Neuropsychological Test Battery: Therapy and Clinical Interpretation. Tucson, AZ: Neuropsychological Press. [Google Scholar]
  48. Rios-Romenets S, Lopez H, Lopez L, Hincapie L, Saldarriaga A, Madrigal L, & Giraldo M (2017). The Colombian Alzheimer’s Prevention Initiative (API) Registry. [Google Scholar]
  49. Romero-Vanegas SJ, Valencia-Marín CM, Aguirre-Acevedo DC, Buschke H, & Lopera F (2010). Alteraciones de la memoria episódica verbal en fases preclínica y temprana de la enfermedad de Alzheimer familiar precoz por mutación E280A en PS1. Acta Neurologica Colombiana, 26(4). [Google Scholar]
  50. Rosselli M & Ardila A (2003) The impact of culture and education on nonverbal neuropsychological measurements: A critical review. Brain and Cognition, 52, 326–333. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Salthouse TA (1985). Speed of behavior and its implications for cognition In Birren JE.; Schaie KW, (Ed.), Handbook of the Psychology of Aging (2nd ed.). New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold. [Google Scholar]
  52. Sisco S, Gross AL, Shih RA, Sachs BC, Glymour MM, Bangen KJ, Manly JJ (2015). The role of early-life educational quality and literacy in explaining racial disparities in cognition in late life. The Journals of Gerontology. Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 70(4), 557–567. 10.1093/geronb/gbt133 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Stricks L, Pittman J, Jacobs DM, Sano M, & Stern Y (1998). Normative data for a brief neuropsychological battery administered to English- and Spanish-speaking community-dwelling elders. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 4(4), 311–318 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Tariot PN, Lopera F, Langbaum JB, Thomas RG, Hendrix S, Schneider LS, & Ramos C (2018). The Alzheimer’s Prevention Initiative Autosomal-Dominant Alzheimer’s Disease Trial: A study of crenezumab versus placebo in preclinical PSEN1 E280A mutation carriers to evaluate efficacy and safety in the treatment of autosomal-dominant Alzheimer’s disease, including a placebo-treated noncarrier cohort. Alzheimer’s & Dementia: Translational Research & Clinical Interventions, 4, 150–160. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Wechsler D (1997). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (3rd edn.). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation. [Google Scholar]
  56. Welsh KA, Butters N, Mohs RC, Beekly D, Edland S, Fillenbaum G, & Heyman A (1994). The Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease (CERAD). Part V. A normative study of the neuropsychological battery. Neurology, 44(4), 609–609. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supp 1

RESOURCES