Abstract
Behavioral Inhibition (BI) is a temperament type that predicts social withdrawal in childhood and anxiety disorders later in life. However, not all BI children develop anxiety. Attention bias (AB) may enhance the vulnerability for anxiety in BI children, and interfere with their development of effective emotion regulation. In order to fully probe attention patterns, we used traditional measures of reaction time (RT), stationary eye-tracking, and recently emerging mobile eye-tracking measures of attention in a sample of 5- to 7-year-olds characterized as BI (N = 23) or non-BI (N = 58) using parent reports. There were no BI-related differences in RT or stationary eye-tracking indices of AB in a dot-probe task. However, findings in a subsample from whom eye-tracking data were collected during a live social interaction indicated that BI children (N = 12) directed fewer gaze shifts to the stranger than non-BI children (N = 25). Moreover, the frequency of gazes toward the stranger was positively associated with stationary AB only in BI, but not in non-BI, children. Hence, BI was characterized by a consistent pattern of attention across stationary and ambulatory measures. We demonstrate the utility of mobile eye-tracking as an effective tool to extend the assessment of attention and regulation to social interactive contexts.
Keywords: attention bias, behavioral inhibition, dot-probe task, eye-tracking, mobile eye-tracking
Visual attention shapes learning, self-regulation, and behavior (Morales, Fu, & Pérez-Edgar, 2016; Posner & Rothbart, 2007). Affect-biased attention emerges relatively early in development (Leppänen & Nelson, 2012) and is characterized by automatic attentional prioritization based on a stimulus’ relative affective and motivational salience to the individual (Ehlers & Todd, 2017; Todd, Cunningham, Anderson, & Thompson, 2012). It also plays a role in emotion regulation (Todd et al., 2012; White, Helfinstein, Reeb-Sutherland, Degnan, & Fox, 2009), impacting voluntary and involuntary processes that modulate emotional experiences in order to serve one’s goals (Thompson, 1994). Affect-biased attention is shaped by the individual’s past experience and current emotional and motivational state and can, over time, influence individuals’ experience of the social world by creating a habitual filter that canalizes perception, emotional response, and behavior (Morales, Fu, et al., 2016; Todd et al., 2012). Attention bias (AB) toward perceived threatening stimuli, subserved by both automatic (bottom-up) and regulatory (top-down) processes, is a tractable form of emotion dysregulation that may increase risk for psychopathology (Jazaieri, Morrison, Goldin, & Gross, 2015; Tone, Garn, & Pine, 2016). The present study implemented reaction time (RT), and stationary and ambulatory eye-tracking measures of threat-related attention in 5- to 7-year-old children. We aimed to illustrate that multimodal eye-tracking assessments can enhance our understanding of how AB is a form of emotion dysregulation that contributes to increased vulnerability for socioemotional maladjustment.
Attention Plays a Role in Emotion Regulation During Development
Posner’s neurobehavioral model of attention is composed of three interdependent components: alerting, orienting, and executive attention (Rothbart, Sheese, Rueda, & Posner, 2011; Petersen & Posner, 2012). During infancy, attentional control is largely involuntary and dominated by the orienting network (Rothbart et al., 2011). The executive network begins to play a larger role as the prefrontal cortex matures across childhood and adolescence (Eisenberg, Spinrad, & Eggum, 2010; Posner, Rothbart, Sheese, & Voelker, 2014). Attention and other emotion regulation processes share overlapping frontolimbic circuitries involving bidirectional influences from both bottom-up subcortical structures like the amygdala and ventral striatum and top-down prefrontal regions encompassing the orbitofrontal cortex, anterior cingulate, and ventrolateral, dorsolateral, and ventromedial prefrontal cortices (Beauchaine & Zisner, 2017; Corbetta & Shulman, 2002). Aberrant activation and connectivity in the frontolimbic circuitries are linked to AB to threat (Bishop, 2008), emotion dysregulation (Hilt, Hanson, & Pollak, 2011), and psychopathology (Sylvester et al., 2012).
Threat-related AB Is Associated With Anxiety Dysregulation
Affect-biased attention serves as a means of engaging with emotionally provocative stimuli while also regulating emotional experiences (Gross, 1998; Todd et al., 2012). However, attention deployment can also lead to patterns of emotion dysregulation that propagate and amplify maladaptive behavior (Tone et al., 2016). Emotion dysregulation, marked by avoidance of fear eliciting stimuli, prevents the development of more adaptive emotion regulation strategies, such as reappraisal or extinction (Jazaieri et al., 2015). Avoidance is a hallmark symptom of anxiety disorders (Aldao, Nolen-Hoeksema, & Schweizer, 2010), and AB to threat has been linked to anxiety in both children and adults (Bar-Haim, Lamy, Pergamin, Bakermans-Kranenburg, & van IJzendoorn, 2007; Dudeney, Sharpe, & Hunt, 2015; Roy, Dennis, & Warner, 2015).
The dot-probe task is commonly used to index AB in anxiety. This task presents pairs of faces. In each trial, one face is affectively salient (e.g., threat) and one is neutral. The face pair is followed by a probe (e.g., an asterisk) that appears in the same location as the affective face (congruent trials) or in the same location as the neutral face (incongruent trials). AB is quantified by subtracting RTs in the congruent trials from RTs in the incongruent trials. A positive score indicates more rapid attention deployment toward the affective face, whereas a negative score indicates bias away from the threat (Abend, Pine, & Bar-Haim, 2014). At the neural level, dot-probe studies have found that high trait anxiety or anxiety disorders are associated with perturbations in both subcortical regions associated with orienting and in prefrontal networks subserving emotion regulation. That is, anxious youth display overactive amygdala, and altered activation in the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, and anterior cingulate (for reviews, see Blackford & Pine, 2012; Sylvester & Pine, 2018). While correlational evidence suggests that AB to threat may be related to deficient anxiety regulation (Tone et al., 2016), it is not clear how AB contributes to the onset and development of anxiety dysregulation.
Threat-Related AB Increases the Risk for Anxiety and Broad Socioemotional Problems
Studying threat-related AB in children at risk for anxiety is important for understanding the role of AB in the emergence of anxiety dysregulation (Shechner et al., 2012). Behavioral inhibition (BI) is a biologically based temperament characterized by heightened vigilance and overreactivity to novelty in infancy (Kagan, Reznick, Clarke, Snidman, & Garcia-Coll, 1984). BI is conceptualized as a qualitatively distinct profile relative to non-BI children (Fox, Snidman, Haas, Degnan, & Kagan, 2015). In childhood, stable BI across time often manifests as social withdrawal (SW; Fox, Henderson, Rubin, Calkins, & Schmidt, 2001), which in turn is associated with further elevation in risk for developing internalizing symptoms by adolescence and young adulthood (Prior, Smart, Sanson, & Oberklaid, 2000; Rubin, Coplan, & Bowker, 2009). Despite being the most robust individual difference predictor of anxiety (reviewed in Pérez-Edgar & Guyer, 2014), the majority of BI children do not develop clinical anxiety (Degnan et al., 2014; Degnan & Fox, 2007). Differential patterns of emotion regulation and dysregulation, including patterns of AB, may be an important cofactor that accounts for this multifinality. When BI co-occurs with threat-related AB, longitudinal studies have revealed a strong relation with the development of childhood SW and anxiety (Morales, Pérez-Edgar, & Buss, 2015; Nozadi et al., 2016; Pérez-Edgar et al., 2010, 2011; White et al., 2017).
In addition to the RT studies suggesting threat-related AB underlies the link between BI and anxiety, a number of neuroimaging studies (for reviews, see Blackford, Clauss, & Benningfield, 2018; Sylvester & Pine, 2018) suggest that BI is characterized by hyperreactivity in the amygdala-based system underlying threat detection (Kagan, 2012). Individual differences in threat responsiveness may relate to enhanced orienting driven by bottom-up processes (e.g., Schwartz, Wright, Shin, Kagan, & Rauch, 2003). Hyperactive automatic attention processes may impede development of neural networks that support adaptive strategies, including executive attention (Fu, Taber-Thomas, & Pérez-Edgar, 2017; Hardee et al., 2013), fear regulation, and appraisal (Clauss, Benningfield, Rao, & Blackford, 2016; Shechner et al., 2018). Over time, these regulatory systems may become less efficient and flexible (Henderson, Pine, & Fox, 2015; Henderson & Wilson, 2017), resulting in an entrenched and habitual pattern of maladaptive emotion regulation (Morales, Fu, et al., 2016; Pérez-Edgar, 2018).
Multimodal Approaches Facilitate the Delineation of Attention Patterns
Although some studies have found that BI children showed AB toward threat using RT measures (Pérez-Edgar et al., 2010; Szpunar & Young, 2012), and these are consistent with the neuroimaging literature indicating threat hyperreactivity in subcortical structures, the literature is far from consistent. For example, Morales et al. (2015) found that children characterized with dysregulated fear, another fearful temperament profile, displayed AB away from threat. The majority of the studies have found no zero-order correlation between BI and RT measures of AB (Broeren, Muris, Bouwmeester, Field, & Voerman, 2011; Cole, Zapp, Fettig, & Pérez-Edgar, 2016; Pérez-Edgar et al., 2011; Vervoort et al., 2011; White et al., 2017), even when group differences in neural activations underlying attention processes toward the same stimuli are evident (e.g., Auday, Taber-Thomas, & Pérez-Edgar, 2018; Fu et al., 2017; Hardee et al., 2013). Moreover, studies also failed to find zero-order correlations between AB measures and SW levels (Cole et al., 2016; Morales et al., 2015; Pérez-Edgar et al., 2011).
The overreliance on dot-probe based RTs as the primary measure of attention deployment may contribute to the inconsistent findings. Dot-probe RT measures have poor test–retest reliability, particularly in youth (Britton et al., 2013; Brown et al., 2014). In part, this may be because response-based measures do not provide a direct measure of attention processes, which reduces measurement reliability (Rodebaugh et al., 2016). Attention processes are continuous and dynamic; multiple attention shifts may occur during and after stimulus presentation before the manual response is made (Yiend, 2010). RT measures are particularly “noisy” indicators of attention as the final button press incorporates multiple mechanisms from initial sensory processing to initial orienting and potentially disengagement from other stimuli, through to response selection and motor engagement (Shechner et al., 2012).
Due to the problems inherent in the dot-probe task and other RT measures of AB, there have been recent calls to implement multiple measures of AB across levels of analysis to characterize affect-biased attention (Fu & Pérez-Edgar, 2019; Price et al., 2015; Rodebaugh et al., 2016). Eye-tracking measures of attention are promising as they provide a more proximal, continuous, and temporally sensitive measure of visual attention (Armstrong & Olatunji, 2012). Eye-tracking indices can be calculated from computerized tasks to capture components of AB conceptually associated with anxiety (Eysenck, Derakshan, Santos, & Calvo, 2007; Mogg & Bradley, 1998).
Only a handful of studies have utilized eye-tracking measures with dot-probe paradigms to examine AB in youth (Burris, Barry-Anwar, & Rivera, 2017; Burris, Barry-Anwar, Sims, et al., 2017; Hilt, Leitzke, & Pollak, 2017; Price et al., 2013, 2016; Tsypes, Owens, & Gibb, 2017). These studies commonly measured (a) initial attention vigilance to emotional faces (e.g., angry face; e.g., Price et al., 2016; Tsypes et al., 2017); and (b) sustained attention preference toward the emotional faces (e.g., Hilt et al., 2017). These indices showed improved internal consistency and reliability compared to RT measures in 9- to 13-year-olds (Price et al., 2015). Findings from three studies that compared dot-probe eye-tracking and RT indices showed that while eye-tracking measures were associated with levels of rumination (Hilt et al., 2017), suicidal ideation (Tsypes et al., 2017), and transition from anxiety to later depression (Price et al., 2016). Eye-tracking findings were evident even when there were no symptom-related differences in RT scores. Hence, eye-tracking measures might be more sensitive in capturing AB patterns associated with internalizing symptoms in children than the RT measures (Price et al., 2015).
Extending Threat-Related Attention to Social Interactive Contexts
Thus far, existing assessments of AB (including RT, eye-tracking, and neuroimaging methods) rely on screen-based, computer-controlled paradigms that examine attention toward preselected, static, and relatively artificial stimuli, often black-and-white photos. However, there is a real-time, dynamic relation between attention, emotion regulation (i.e., changes in initial emotion responses), and socioemotional behavior (Cole, Hall, & Hajal, 2017; Morales, Fu, et al., 2016; Todd et al., 2012). While eye-tracking measures of attention offer improvements over RT measures, using this technology in the static dot-probe task still limits the ability to delineate attention patterns in the context of real-life social interactions. A core aspect of emotion dysregulation in BI children is the tendency to monitor and avoid rather than engage and explore with the social environment (Pérez-Edgar, 2018). Hence, we need to understand how AB is deployed in more dynamic than screen-based task contexts. To this end, we need to incorporate more ecologically valid and interactive paradigms to capture attention patterns in vivo to fully capture risk for developing socioemotional maladjustments (Redcay & Warnell, 2018).
Mobile eye-tracking offers a spatially and temporally sensitive assessment tool for capturing person-centered attention processes in naturalistic contexts. By continuously recording an individual’s field of view and attention focus, we can chart real-time within-person changes in attention as the individual interacts with the social world (Franchak, 2017; Hayhoe & Rothkopf, 2011). Emerging mobile eye-tracking studies indicate that screen-based, stationary attention patterns are different from patterns observed during naturalistic exploration (Bambach, Crandall, Smith, & Yu, 2018; Franchak, in press). Specifically, the opportunity for real-life social interaction and the resulting awareness of the self as an active social agent, rather than a passive observer, leads to different looking behavior in adults (Foulsham, Walker, & Kingstone, 2011; Freeth, Foulsham, & Kingstone, 2013; Kretch & Adolph, 2015; Laidlaw, Foulsham, Kuhn, & Kingstone, 2011). In addition, behavioral observations in the laboratory indicate that attention patterns toward affectively salient stimuli and events predict emotional behavior (e.g., Crockenberg, Leerkes, & Bárrig Jó, 2008; Kiel & Buss, 2011). Thus, incorporating mobile eye-tracking in traditional observational paradigms could reveal more fine-grained attention patterns that play a role in emotion regulation and dysregulation. Finally, evidence suggests that laboratory-based mobile eye-tracking paradigms can capture threat-related AB associated with psychopathology. For example, Woody et al. (2019) found that adolescent girls showed more frequent and longer eye gazes toward a critical judge, relative to a positive judge, when giving a speech. This pattern, in turn, was associated with levels of depressive symptomatology.
The Current Study
To reconcile the consistent findings of neural differences in response to threat with inconsistent findings from RT measures of threat bias in BI, the current study was designed to assess the possibility that eye-tracking might be a more reliable measure of AB in BI children. In addition, in order to begin to explore the utility of incorporating multiple paradigms and multiple contexts to study patterns of AB (Price et al., 2015; Rodebaugh et al., 2016), we developed an eye-tracking paradigm that could be used in a real-world setting (Fu & Pérez-Edgar, 2019). The present study used a screen-based stationary eye-tracking and a mobile eye-tracking paradigm to assess attention patterns among a group of 5- to 7-year-old BI children. This age group precedes the typical onset of clinically significant anxiety (Beesdo, Knappe, & Pine, 2009), but is marked by the transition to formal schooling, when SW is commonly expressed in BI children (Coplan & Arbeau, 2008). As such, the current study helps delineate more fine-grained, trait-level attention patterns that may underlie behavior dysregulation and potentiate risks for socioemotional maladjustment.
In this three-part investigation, we first aimed to examine whether there are BI-linked differences in AB indices computed from a traditional dot-probe task using RTs and traditional stationary eye-tracking, and whether stationary AB modulates parent report of SW levels. In Part 2, we aimed to investigate whether there are BI-linked differences in looking behavior toward a putative social threat (i.e., an unfamiliar adult), and whether ambulatory threat-related attention is associated with SW. In Part 3, we explored (a) the association between stationary and ambulatory attention indices, (b) whether the association varied by BI status, and (c) whether stationary and ambulatory attention measures jointly predict SW levels. This three-part systematic analysis is designed to illustrate the strength and promise of both stationary and mobile eye-tracking as additional tools for capturing AB, while also generating initial indicators of power and effect size.
Method
The present analysis drew from an ongoing multivisit study examining temperament-related individual differences in affect-biased attention using both stationary and mobile eye-tracking measures. Exclusion criteria for participating in the larger study included being a non-English speaker, having gross developmental delays, or having severe neurological and medical illnesses. All study procedures were approved by the institutional review board at Pennsylvania State University, and written informed consent/assent was obtained prior to participation.
All participants completed the dot-probe task while undergoing traditional stationary eye-tracking. A subsample also provided data from a mobile eye-tracking paradigm that measured ambulatory attention toward a putative social threat, in a live social context. Given the novel mobile eye-tracking methodology used in the current study, we were mindful of good practices for reporting eye-tracking data (Oakes, 2010) and aimed to demonstrate the utility of the eye-tracking technology in developmental research. Thus, we have carefully noted the specifications of the equipment used and the protocols needed to capture visual attention patterns during social interactions.
Participants
Part 1: Dot-probe task
Participants were 81 5- to 7-year-olds (Mage = 6.03, SD = 0.61; 40 boys), recruited through a university database of families interested in participating in research studies, community outreach, and word of mouth. Potential participants (158 children; Mage = 5.86, SD = 0.72; 86 boys) were screened based on parent report (92.4% maternal report) using the Behavioral Inhibition Questionnaire (BIQ; Bishop, Spence, & McDonald, 2003). Of the 158 children characterized for the current study, 37 (23.4%) met the BI criterion (see below), and 23 participated in the study. Fifty-eight children who scored below the BI cutoffs also participated in the current analysis. The sample was predominantly Caucasian (86.7%), which reflects the surrounding rural community. The remaining families self-identified as Asian (N= 5), African American (N = 4), Hispanic (N = 1), and biracial (N = 1). The sample included two sibling pairs. One child from each pair was excluded from data analyses (2 boys, both non-BIs).
All 81 participants attempted the dot-probe task. Seventy-one children (Mage = 6.06, SD = 0.6; 35 boys; Table 1) were included in the final analyses of RT data. Eye-tracking data analyses included 73 participants (Mage = 6.06, SD = 0.61; 34 boys; Table 1). Sixty-six participants provided both valid RT and stationary eye-tracking data (Mage = 6.08, SD = 0.59; 31 boys). RT and eye-tracking data were excluded if the participants were from a sibling pair (N = 2) or completed too few trials of the task (N = 1). Specifically, for the RT data, participants were excluded if they provided no RT data (N = 4), or had poor performance (i.e., <60% valid trials; N= 3). For the eye-tracking data, participants were excluded if they had no fixation data (N = 1), or provided too few trials with at least one valid face fixation (N = 4). The included participants did not differ from the excluded participants in sex, RT data: χ2 = 0.002, p = .97, φ = .005, eye-tracking data: χ2 = 2.33, p = .13, φ = .17; BI status, RT data: χ2 = .01, p = .90, φ = .01, eye-tracking data: χ2 = 0.05, p = .82, φ = .03; age, RT data: t (78) = 1.19, p = .24, d = 0.27, eye-tracking data: t (78) = 1.52, p = .13, d = 0.34; and SW levels, RT data: t(78) = −0.49, p = .63, d = −0.11, eye-tracking data: t (78) = −0.69, p = .49, d = 0.16.
Table 1.
Variable | All sample Mean (SD) | BI Mean (SD) | Non-BI Mean (SD) |
---|---|---|---|
Dot-probe RTs participant information | |||
Included N | 71 | 20 | 51 |
Sex (M/F) | 35/36 | 12/8 | 23/28 |
Age | 6.06 (0.6) | 6.07 (0.59) | 6.05 (0.61) |
Social withdrawal | 0.47 (0.32) | 0.75 (0.3) | 0.36 (0.25)** |
Dot-probe RT indices | |||
Angry bias (ms) | 9.18 (56.82) | 4.72 (60.82) | 10.94 (55.71) |
Happy bias (ms) | 11.29 (72.1) | 0.48 (90.74) | 15.53 (63.9) |
Dot-probe eye-tracking participant information | |||
Included N | 73 | 21 | 52 |
Sex (M/F) | 34/39 | 11/10 | 23/29 |
Age | 6.06 (0.61) | 6.11 (0.61) | 6.04 (0.61) |
Social withdrawal | 0.47 (0.33) | 0.77 (0.31) | 0.35 (0.26)** |
Dot-probe eye-tracking indices | |||
Angry bias (latency in ms) | 1.43 (19.78) | 1.59 (17.62) | 1.36 (20.76) |
Happy bias (latency in ms) | 2.71 (16.81) | 2.39 (19.55) | 2.83 (15.78) |
Angry bias (dwell time in ms) | −0.62 (21.69) | −2.92 (19.73) | 0.31 (22.55) |
Happy bias (dwell time in ms) | 0.01 (18.09) | −3.77 (19.84) | 1.54 (17.3) |
Angry bias (frequency) | 1.99 (4.97) | 1.71 (5.17) | 2.1 (4.94) |
Happy bias (frequency) | 2.74 (4.77) | 3.9 (5.47) | 2.27 (4.42) |
Notes: Mean and standard deviation for RT and stationary eye-tracking bias scores were computed from Winsorized values. BI, behavioral inhibition. RT, reaction time.
p < .001.
Parts 2 and 3: Mobile eye-tracking
Thirty-seven (Mage = 6.13, SD = 0.63; 18 boys; 12 BI; 91.9% Caucasian) of the 81 children who participated in the larger study were included in analyses using mobile eye-tracking data. Data from the remaining participants were not included in analyses for a variety of reasons: the first 19 children were used for technical development of the protocol, 1 family declined to participate, 4 data sets were lost due to technical problems, and we were unable to obtain satisfactory calibration on 13 participants. The final subsample of 37 children did not contain any members of a sibling pair. Independent samples t tests indicate that the subsample did not differ from the larger sample on age, t (78) = 1.32, p = .19, d = 0.3, and SW levels, t (78) = −0.61, p = .55, d = −0.14. In addition, they did not differ on sex, χ2 (1) = 0.02, p = .90, φ = .01, and BI status, χ2 (1) = 0.55, p = .46, φ= .08.
Apparatus and procedures
Stationary eye-tracking
Data were acquired using a RED-m Eye Tracking System (SensoMotoric Instruments). Children were seated 60 cm from a 22-inch (1600 × 900 pixels) presentation monitor. The eye-tracker has embedded cameras that detect and record the reflection of an infrared light source on the cornea relative to the pupil from both eyes. The eye-tracking system has a 60-Hz sampling rate and an average accuracy of 0.5 to 1 degree, equivalent to 0.5- to 1-cm area on the screen with the 60-cm viewing distance. Once the experimenter made sure the child’s eye gaze was on the center of the screen, testing began with a 5-point calibration and 4-point validation procedure during which an audiovisual animation was presented at the center and four corners of the screen. The calibration and validation procedure were administered before each of the four task blocks. Children could choose to take a short break after each block. Testing continued until all 100 trials had been presented, or the child declined to continue.
Mobile eye-tracking
As shown in Figure 1a, participants wore a Pupil head-mounted eye-tracker (Pupil Labs; Kassner, Patera, & Bulling, 2014). The system consists of two eye cameras with infrared illumination for dark pupil tracking and a world camera with a fisheye lens. System specifications (resolution, sampling rate, etc.) are provided in the online-only Supplement 1. The system enables eye fixation information to be integrated with visual information from the participants’ perspective. Data were recorded with Pupil Capture v.0.9.12 (Pupil Labs) installed on an MSI VR One Backpack PC (Windows 10; Figure 1a).1 In order to allow for the real-time monitoring of data collection during the experiment, a monitor located in a separate room was remotely connected to the Backpack PC. The headset plus the Backpack PC were light enough to enable children to move freely throughout the recording session.
Upon starting the mobile eye-tracking session of the study, the child was led to a testing room for eye-tracker placement and calibration. Calibration and validation procedures are crucial for ensuring accurate and reliable mobile data (Franchak, 2017). The procedures are noted in detail in the online-only Supplement 1. After calibration and validation, the child was left alone in the room to complete the Stranger Approach episode (Goldsmith, Reilly, Lemery, Longley, & Prescott, 1994; see online-only Supplement 1). The experimenter jointed the child again after the episode to continue the procedures for the larger study. The order of stationary and mobile eye-tracking procedures was randomized across participants.
Measures
BI
Parents completed the BIQ (Bishop et al., 2003), a 30-item instrument that measures the frequency of BI-linked behavior in the domains of social and situational novelty (plus a summed total score) on a 7-point scale ranging from 1 (hardly ever) to 7 (almost always). The questionnaire has adequate internal consistency, construct validity, and validity in differentiating behaviorally inhibited from noninhibited children (Bishop et al., 2003; Mernick, Pine, Gendler, & Shechner, 2018). Parent reports on the BIQ correlate with laboratory observations of BI in social scenarios (Dyson, Klein, Olino, Dougherty, & Durbin, 2011). The BIQ had good internal consistency in the present study (Cronbach’s α = 0.95).
Across the literature, BI has typically been characterized as a distinct group although this group can be characterized using composites of fearful behaviors that are on a continuum (reviewed in Buss & Kiel, 2013). The categorical approach employed here reflects, in part, the original characterization of BI as a qualitatively distinct temperamental profile (Kagan et al., 2003), as well as recruitment procedures that relied on an extreme-groups approach (Fox et al., 2001). We used group-based analyses, as our primary interest was to identify distinct attention patterns from stationary and ambulatory attention that may differentiate BI and non-BI profiles.2
Children were designated as BI if they scored high on either the social novel subscale (≥60), the grand total score (≥119), or both. Cutoff scores were based on previous studies of extreme temperament in children aged 4–15 years (Broeren & Muris, 2010). Our group previously used the same parameters to screen 706 9- to 12-year-olds and established a BI distribution consistent with published findings (Broeren & Muris, 2010), identifying 25% of the sample as BI (e.g., Auday et al., 2018; Liu, Taber-Thomas, Fu, & Pérez-Edgar, 2018). These studies found that the BI group displayed altered patterns of threat-related attention at both behavioral (Morales, Taber-Thomas, & Pérez-Edgar, 2017) and neural (Auday et al., 2018; Fu et al., 2017; Thai, Taber-Thomas, & Pérez-Edgar, 2016) levels.
SW
The MacArthur Health Behavior Questionnaire (Armstrong, Goldstein, & MacArthur Working Group on Outcome Assessment, 2003; Essex et al., 2002) consists of 172 items that mothers rated on a dichotomous (yes or no) or a 3-point scale (0 = never or not true, 1 = sometimes or somewhat true, or 2 = often or very true) regarding their children’s mental and physical health and functioning during the past 6 months. The Health Behavior Questionnaire is particularly sensitive to internalizing problems (Lemery-Chalfant et al., 2007). The SW composite is computed as the mean of the scores on items from the social inhibition (3 items; e.g., “is afraid of strangers”) and asocial with peers (6 items; e.g., “avoids peers”) scales. The composite has adequate internal consistency in the present study (Cronbach’s α = 0.76).
Stationary AB
The dot-probe task (Figure 2) has been previously administered behaviorally with 5- to 6-year-old children (Cole et al., 2016; Kujawa et al., 2010; Morales, Pérez-Edgar, & Buss, 2015, 2016). The dot-probe task consisted of 8 practice trials and 100 experimental trials randomly presented in 4 blocks of 25 trials. Each trial began with the presentation of a central fixation cross for 500 ms followed by a pair of faces (NimStim; Tottenham et al., 2009) presented side by side for 500 ms. Faces were then removed and replaced by an asterisk (i.e., the probe) in the location of one of the preceding faces (2500 ms). Participants indicated the location of the asterisk by pressing a button on a keyboard as fast as they could (response recorded for 2500 ms; intertrial interval of 1800 ms). Task presentation was controlled by Experimenter Center (SensoMotoric Instruments).
In congruent trials, the probe replaced the emotional face (angry or happy). Incongruent trials had the probe replace the neutral face. Three combinations of faces were presented: angry-neutral (40 trials; 20 congruent trials), happy-neutral (40 trials; 20 congruent trials), and neutral-neutral (20 trials). The face stimulus set consisted of 10 different actors (half male). Each face was presented 10 times. The probe appeared in the right and left positions equally. The face pictures were each 14×19cm (visual angle 13.31×17.99 degrees), with a distance of 26.5 cm between their centers.
Ambulatory attention
The Stranger Approach episode (Goldsmith et al., 1994) is a standardized observational paradigm designed to study temperament-related individual differences in interacting with a novel, putative social threat in a context that is optimized for experimental control. In the episode (Figures 1 and 3), a stranger (i.e., a research assistant who the child has never met) knocked on the door, entered the room, and stood by the door. The stranger then engaged in a prescribed set of actions; the script is presented in detail in online-only Supplement 1. Seven research assistants acted as the stranger for children in the current subsample; all strangers, but one, were male. All strangers were clean-shaven and wore identical clothing and a hat, with hair tucked in, in order to minimize individual differences.
Data processing
Dot-probe manual RTs
Data cleaning was based on published methods (Morales et al., 2015, 2016). Trials with missing responses, incorrect responses, and RTs outside a 150- to 2000-ms window post-probe presentation were excluded. Next, RTs of included trials were averaged for each participant and trials with RTs +/− 2SD of the individual child’s mean were excluded. Children who had poor task performance (<60% valid trials) were excluded from RT data analyses. Similar inclusion/exclusion cutoffs have been applied in previous dot-probe studies in children with the age range of the current sample (Cole et al., 2016; Pérez-Edgar et al., 2011; White et al., 2017).
The cleaned RTs from the angry-neutral and happy-neutral trials were used to compute AB scores to the emotional faces by subtracting the mean RTs to probes on the congruent trials from the mean RTs to the probes on the incongruent trials.
Positive scores denote a bias to emotional faces (angry or happy) whereas negative scores suggest a bias away from emotional faces (angry or happy). One outlier for the angry bias score (<mean – 3SD) was Winsorized by reassigning the outlier to the threshold for the minimum value (i.e., mean – 3SD).
Stationary eye-tracking
The raw x-y position coordinates of fixations, defined as gaze maintained for at least 80 ms within a 100-pixel maximum dispersion, were exported with BeGaze (SensoMotoric Instruments). An area of interest (AOI) encircling and including the entire face and probe display areas was created using BeGaze. Current analyses used fixation location, latency, and dwell time for each face AOI fixation in each trial computed using in-house Python scripts (Python Software Foundation, http://www.python.org/). After examining calibration (see online-only Supplement 1), we calculated the number of trials that contained at least one valid face fixation for each participant. Participants who provided too few trials (<mean trial number – 2SD; i.e., <26 trials) were excluded.
Indices of AB
For each participant, we computed mean fixation latency for each face type (only the initial face fixation latency in each trial were included), mean dwell time on each face type, and the total number of trials in which each face type was fixated upon. The cleaned eye-tracking fixation data from the angry-neutral and happy-neutral trials were used to compute AB scores to the emotional faces. Six eye-tracking AB scores were computed (Table 1). Two AB latency scores were calculated by subtracting the mean fixation latency on the emotional face (angry or happy) from the mean fixation latency on the competing neutral face. Two AB dwell time scores were calculated by subtracting the mean dwell time on the neutral face from the mean dwell time on the emotional face (angry or happy). Finally, two AB frequency scores were computed by subtracting the total number of trials with a neutral face fixation from the total number of trials in which participants fixated on the emotional face (angry or happy). Across all six indices, positive values indicate AB toward the emotional faces, whereas negative values indicate bias away from the emotional faces. One outlier happy bias latency score was Winsorized.
Mobile eye-tracking recording
Data from the eye cameras and the world camera were recorded to separate files. The recordings were combined and further processed using Pupil Player v.0.9.12 (Pupil Labs). Detailed procedures are noted in online-only Supplement 1. The room recording from video cameras and the exported eye-tracking recording were synchronized into a single composite video using Final Cut Pro (Figure 1). The composite recordings were inspected to make sure that the recordings from two sources were not out of synchronization for more than three frames. The composite videos were exported for coding with a resolution of 1920×1080 pixels at 30 fps.
Coding ambulatory gaze behavior
We used Datavyu software (Datavyu Team, 2014) for coding based on published methods (Franchak & Adolph, 2010; Franchak, Kretch, & Adolph, 2017; Franchak, Kretch, Soska, & Adolph, 2011; Kretch & Adolph, 2015; Kretch, Franchak, & Adolph, 2014). oders inspected the composite video frame by frame to denote the onset and offset time of each valid AOI fixation. For recordings with good calibration, the red circle was used to infer gaze location on AOIs: stranger’s face (defined as any part of the head), stranger’s body, child him/herself (when the child was looking at his or her own body), and the background room. For recordings with less optimal calibration, the yellow circle was used to determine gaze locations. The AOIs for these recordings were stranger’s face only, stranger’s body only, stranger’s face and body (when the yellow circle enclosed both parts), child him/herself, and the background room.
A valid AOI fixation is defined as ≥3 consecutive frames (≈99.9 ms) of stable gaze on the same AOI. When the child looked down at him/herself, the pupils were often out of the range of the eye cameras (i.e., no points of gaze were visible). In such cases, coders scored the onset and offset times of self-looking using the room recording synced with the eye-tracking recording. Coders also denoted the onset and offset times for gazes on an AOI for shorter than 3 consecutive frames and unusable frames, defined as loss of tracking due to eye blinks, or when pupils were not properly detected due to reasons other than self-looking.
As the world camera can capture the stranger’s behavior during the face-to-face interaction, coders marked the onset and offset times of the stranger’s continuous movement: entering the room, standing by the door, approaching the child, standing by the chair, sitting on the chair, and walking away from the child. The time period when the stranger was standing by the chair was not scored for one participant because the stranger was out of the child’s field of view. To ensure interrater reliability, a master coder scored 100% of all recordings, with 20% double coding for each participant. We had an average agreement of 94.2% (κ = 0.84) for eye gaze coding and 99.6% for stranger behavior coding (κ = 0.77).
Mobile eye-tracking indices of attention toward the putative social threat
Our primary interest was to characterize attention patterns toward the stranger. Continuous gaze behavior coding produced a time series of gaze locations for each participant. From this time series, we were able to make a number of computations. We calculated total number of gaze visits to the stranger. Each unique visit must be preceded by a fixation away from the stranger. In addition, we computed mean latency of gaze reengagement to the stranger, defined as the average time elapsed from the end of a visit to the stranger to the next visit to the stranger. We excluded the latency of first look to the stranger, as the stranger knocked on the door before entering, which may have primed children’s looking behavior. We found that 6 children had zero latency to engage to the stranger. To capture sustained attention toward the stranger, we computed mean visit duration by dividing the total dwell time on the stranger by the number of visits made to the stranger, and proportion of dwell time on the stranger relative to the total duration of valid AOI fixations. The mean latency of reengagement and mean visit duration were subsequently log-transformed to correct for the skewed distributions.
We also aligned the timing of gaze coding and stranger behavior coding in Datayvu (Datavyu Team, 2014). This allowed us to explore children’s gaze patterns as the stranger’s behavior changed over the course of the episode (Figure 3).
Statistical analyses
Part 1: BI, stationary attention measures, and SW
Exploratory analyses suggested that participant age was not correlated with RT and stationary eye-tracking AB scores (ps > .19; Table 2). Boys showed greater happy bias than girls, indicated by mean latency, t (71) = 2.02, p = .047, d = 0.48. Independent samples t tests indicated that there were no sex differences in other indices (ps > .24, ds < 0.28). Hence, sex was added as a covariate only in models with latency indices of AB as the dependent variables (DVs). Because participants differed in total numbers of valid angry-neutral and happy-neutral trials (24–79 trials; M = 56.95, SD = 14.09), the effect of valid trial number was controlled for in analyses with latency and dwell time bias scores as DVs.
Table 2.
Variable | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 | 9 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. Age | |||||||||
2. Social withdrawal | −0.14 | ||||||||
3. Angry bias (RT in ms) | 0.14 | 0.01 | |||||||
4. Happy bias (RT in ms) | −0.11 | −0.11 | −0.07 | ||||||
5. Angry bias (latency in ms) | 0.02 | −0.08 | 0.07 | −0.2 | |||||
6. Happy bias (latency in ms) | 0.03 | −0.22+ | −0.13 | 0.03 | 0.03 | ||||
7. Angry bias (dwell time in ms) | 0.03 | −0.08 | −0.02 | −0.09 | 0.71** | 0.12 | |||
8. Happy bias (dwell time in ms) | −0.03 | −0.22+ | −0.24+ | 0.13 | 0.09 | 0.64** | 0.006 | ||
9. Angry bias (frequency) | 0.07 | 0.07 | 0.004 | 0.11 | −0.23+ | −0.001 | −0.26* | 0.09 | |
10. Happy bias (frequency) | 0.16 | 0.14 | −0.01 | 0.05 | −0.13 | −0.02 | −0.09 | −0.08 | 0.28* |
Note: The correlations among attention bias RT and stationary eye-tracking indices were based on 66 participants who provided both valid RT and stationary eye-tracking data. RT, reaction time.
p < .07.
p < .05.
p < .001.
For RT AB scores, a 2×2 mixed measures analysis of variance was used to test the effect of emotion (angry vs. happy) and BI status (BI vs. non-BI). If there was a significant correlation between any RT bias score and SW, a linear regression was used to test whether the relation was significant controlling for BI status.
To examine the pattern of stationary eye-tracking AB as a function of emotion and BI status, we first fitted a mixed-measures analysis of covariance with emotion (angry vs. happy) as the within-subjects factor and BI status (BI vs. non-BI) as the between-subjects factor on latency AB scores. The model was then repeated with dwell time scores as the DV. Finally, a mixed-measures analysis of variance was used to test the effects of emotion and BI status on AB frequency scores.
For significant correlations between any stationary eye-tracking AB score and SW levels, we then examined the effect of the AB score on SW, controlling for BI using a linear regression model.
Part 2: BI, ambulatory attention measures, and SW
We explored children’s ambulatory gaze patterns using state space grids (Hollenstein, 2007; Lamey, Hollenstein, Lewis, & Granic, 2004; Lewis, Lamey, & Douglas, 1999). Mobile eye-tracking paradigms generate rich, high-intensity data. Thus, data visualization is a vital step that facilitates hypothesis generation and testing (Yu, Yurovsky, & Xu, 2012). State space grids provide a useful tool for visualizing how children’s gaze patterns evolve as the stranger′s behavior unfolds in real time (Figure 3). The unequal sample sizes in the BI and non-BI groups prevented us from making between-group comparisons. Instead, we examined within-person changes in children’s gaze patterns across the six states of stranger’s behavior in the BI and non-BI groups separately. We used the transitional entropy index produced by GridWare (Lewis et al., 1999) as a measure of the organization of children’s AOI looking behavior in each of the stranger’s behavior states (Hollenstein, 2007; Lewis et al., 1999). A high entropy score indicates high level of gaze transitions across AOIs (stranger child him/herself and the background room).
Preliminary analyses indicated that age was not correlated with mobile eye-tracking indices of attention patterns toward the stranger (ps > .26; Table 3), and independent-samples t tests suggest that there were no sex differences in these measures (ps > .10, ds < 0.60). Hence, the effects of age and sex were not controlled for in analyses for parsimony. However, the total coded episode duration (equal to the duration of stranger presence) did vary across subjects and was correlated with mean latency of reengagement to the stranger, r = .47, p = .003, proportion of time looking at the stranger, r = −.37, p = .02, and SW levels, r = −.34, p = .04 (Table 3). Therefore, all analyses accounted for the total duration of the stranger episode.
Table 3.
Variable | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. Age | ||||||
2. Social withdrawal | −0.03 | |||||
3. Total coded durations | −0.23 | −0.34* | ||||
4. Total number of gaze visits | −0.19 | −0.43** | 0.20 | |||
5. Mean latency of gaze visits (log-transformed) | −0.06 | 0.07 | 0.47** | −0.48** | ||
6. Mean visit duration (log-transformed) | 0.13 | 0.28 | −0.18 | −0.29 | −0.48** | |
7. Proportion of dwell time | 0.06 | 0.09 | −0.37* | 0.13 | −0.84*** | 0.82*** |
p < .05.
p < .01.
p < .001.
The descriptive statistics for ambulatory attention coding are presented in Table 4. While the stranger’s behavior and speech were standardized across participants, children’s social behavior, such as the duration of their responses to the stranger’s prompts, can drive the duration of stranger presence, which in turn, may influence children’s gaze behavior. Hence, the coded episode duration was entered as a covariate in models with mobile eye-tracking indices and SW as DVs. Moreover, the mean number of visits to the stranger was higher in recordings when we allowed a margin of error for determining AOI looking (M = 30.69, SD = 11.35) versus recordings that did not use the error margin (M = 22.71, SD = 10.26), t (35) = 2.18, p = .04, d = 0.74. To control for this effect, a dichotomous variable (use of error margin) was entered as a covariate in models with mobile eye-tracking measures as the DV.
Table 4.
Variable | All sample Mean (SD) | BI Mean (SD) | Non-BI Mean (SD) |
---|---|---|---|
Mobile eye-tracking participant information | |||
Included N | 37 | 12 | 25 |
Sex (M/F) | 18/19 | 7/5 | 11/14 |
Age | 6.13 (0.63) | 6.13 (0.66) | 6.13 (0.64) |
Social withdrawal | 0.45 (0.3) | 0.71 (0.15) | 0.33 (0.27)** |
Coding information | |||
Total coded durations (seconds) | 102.60 (25.56) | 91.13 (13.3) | 108.10 (28.31)* |
Number of recordings with error margin coding | 13 | 4 | 9 |
Indices of attention patterns toward the stranger | |||
Total number of gaze visits | 25.51 (11.18) | 19.67 (8.54) | 28.32 (11.35)* |
Mean latency of gaze visits (seconds) | 2.91 (3.24) | 3.18 (3.94) | 2.78 (2.93) |
Mean visit duration (seconds) | 1.80 (1.37) | 2.38 (1.97) | 1.52 (0.88) |
Proportion of dwell time (%) | 46.52 (23.33) | 48.83 (26.89) | 45.41 (21.94) |
Note: Total coded duration equals the duration of the time period when the stranger was present in the room. We allowed a margin of error in determining eye gaze locations for participants with less optimal mobile eye-tracking calibration. Mean latency of gaze visit and mean visit duration were log-transformed for analyses. Raw values are presented here for ease of interpretation.
p < .05.
p < .001.
Our analytic approach to mobile eye-tracking data involved two steps. We examined whether BI status influenced each mobile eye-tracking index of attention toward the putative social threat. We ran four linear regressions models with stranger presence duration and error margin coding as covariates, BI status as the predictor, and (a) total number of visits to the stranger, (b) mean latency of reengagement, (c) mean visit duration, and (d) proportion of dwell time on the stranger as the DV, respectively. To further explore whether BI is associated with a distinct pattern of ambulatory attention, we used a multivariate analysis of covariance to test the effect of BI on the linear combination of the four mobile eye-tracking indices, controlling for stranger presence duration and error margin coding.
When we found a significant correlation between any of the four mobile eye-tracking measures and SW levels, we tested whether the mobile eye-tracking index predicted SW after controlling for stranger presence duration and BI status.
Part 3: BI, comparison of stationary and ambulatory attention measures, and SW
Our focus here was to integrate multiple indices. We examined whether mobile eye-tracking indices of attention patterns toward the stranger correlated with stationary eye-tracking indices of AB across the overlapping subsample of 37 children. Rather than examining the correlations between all possible stationary-ambulatory pairs, we assessed the correlations between ambulatory gaze frequency (i.e., total number of gaze visits to the stranger) and stationary fixation frequency measures of AB, between ambulatory gaze latency (i.e., mean latency of reengagement) and stationary latency AB scores, and between ambulatory gaze dwell time (i.e., mean visit duration and proportion of dwell time on the stranger) and stationary dwell time AB scores. These measures were chosen as they were conceptually similar across the tasks.
Next, we investigated whether the relations between ambulatory attention toward the stranger and stationary indices of AB differed as a function of BI. In order to minimize the number of models tested, we used the findings from Part 1 and 2 to select measures that differentiated between the BI groups. We employed a multiple regression model to test if the measure of ambulatory looking behavior, BI status, and their interaction predicted the stationary eye-tracking measure of angry bias. The model was repeated with the stationary eye-tracking index of happy bias as the DV.
Next, using the same stationary and mobile eye-tracking indices as the previous analysis, we tested whether the mobile eye-tracking measure could better explain the variance in SW levels over and above the stationary eye-tracking measure of AB, and whether the two types of eye-tracking indices jointly predicted SW levels. We employed a hierarchical regression model predicting SW levels by entering the stationary eye-tracking index of angry bias, the mobile eye-tracking measure, and their interaction. We ran an additional regression model to examine whether the stationary eye-tracking index of happy bias, the mobile eye-tracking measure, and their interaction predicted SW levels. The coded episode duration (i.e., the stranger presence duration) was entered in both models as a covariate (see Part 2 analyses).
Results
Part 1: BI, stationary attention measures, and SW
Descriptive statistics and intercorrelations of study variables
Descriptive statics for study variables are presented in Table 1. Preliminary analyses found no significant RT-based AB either across the whole sample: angry bias, t (70) = 1.36, p = .18, d = 0.33; happy bias, t (70) = 1.32, p = .19, d = 0.32; or separately within the BI group: angry bias, t (19) = 0.35, p = .73, d = 0.20; happy bias, t (19) = 0.02, p = .98, d = 0.01; and non-BI group: angry bias, t (50) = 1.4, p = .17, d = 0.40; happy bias, t (50) = 1.74, p = .09, d = 0.49. Correlation analyses (Table 2) indicated that neither RT angry bias nor happy bias was correlated with SW levels, ps > .36.
BI group differences in stationary AB scores
Across the RT and stationary eye-tracking AB indices, models revealed no significant main effect of BI (BI vs. non-BI), ps > .18, emotion (angry vs. happy), ps > .66, or Emotion × BI interaction effects, ps > .17, on these stationary AB measures (Table 5).
Table 5.
Outcome variable | Predictor | B | t | p | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Attention bias (reaction time) | Emotion (angry vs. happy) | −4.59 | −0.36 | .72 | .002 |
BI status (BI vs. non-BI) | −15.04 | −0.88 | .38 | .010 | |
Emotion × BI | 8.83 | 0.36 | .72 | .002 | |
Attention bias (latency) | Sex | −7.96 | −2.67 | .01 | .097 |
N trials with valid face fixations | −0.29 | −2.70 | .01 | .100 | |
Emotion (angry vs. happy) | −1.47 | −0.42 | .67 | .003 | |
BI status (BI vs. non-BI) | −1.78 | −0.39 | .70 | .002 | |
Emotion × BI | 0.67 | 0.10 | .92 | <.001 | |
Attention bias (dwell time) | N trials with valid face fixations | −0.27 | −2.28 | .03 | .071 |
Emotion (angry vs. happy) | −1.23 | −0.32 | .75 | .001 | |
BI status (BI vs. non-BI) | −5.95 | −1.16 | .25 | .020 | |
Emotion × BI | 2.08 | 0.29 | .77 | .001 | |
Attention bias (frequency) | Emotion (angry vs. happy) | −0.17 | −0.21 | .83 | <.001 |
BI status (BI vs. non-BI) | 1.64 | 1.30 | .19 | .024 | |
Emotion × BI | −2.02 | −1.34 | .18 | .025 |
Part 2: BI, ambulatory attention measures, and SW
Visualization of gaze patterns in BI and non-BI children
Figure 3 displays the visualization of AOI gazes in BI and non-BI children using the state space grids. We found that for both BI and non-BI children, the transitional entropy score was highest for the “stranger approach” state (BI: 7.88; non-BI: 16.46) compared to the other states. The score was lowest for the “stranger sitting” state (BI: 2.04; non-BI: 2.53). This suggests that children’s gaze patterns were the least stable during “stranger approach,” possibly due to the high saliency of the stranger (approach movement and unfamiliarity). The gaze patterns became the most stable and organized during “stranger sitting,” possibly because this is the longest segment of the episode. In addition, the stranger was sitting still and most likely engaged in conversation with the child.
BI group differences in attention patterns toward the stranger
The linear regression model revealed that after controlling for total coded episode duration, B = −0.02, ß = −0.04, t = −0.22, p = .83, and the coding method, B = 8.07, ß = 0.35, t = 2.16, p = .04, the BI group showed fewer gaze visits to the stranger than the non-BI group, B = −8.72, ß = −0.37, t = −2.32, p = .03, R2 = .25. BI status did not significantly predict mean latency of gaze reengagement toward the stranger, B = 0.18, ß = 0.26, t = 1.76, p = .09, R2 = .34, mean visit duration, B = 0.08, ß = 0.13, t = 0.7, p = .49, R2 = .05, or proportion of time looking at the stranger, B = −2.97, ß = −0.06, t = −0.36, p = .73, R2 = .15. Likewise, The multivariate analysis of covariance model indicated that there was no BI difference in an overall attention pattern indexed by the four mobile eye-tracking measures, F (4, 30) = 1.80, p = .15, .
The effect of number of visits to the stranger on SW
As shown in Table 3, the number of visits to the stranger was negatively correlated with SW levels, r = −.43, p = .01. However, the number of visits did not significantly predict SW levels, B = −0.01, ß= −0.22, t = −1.58, p = .12, R2 = .44, over and above the effect of BI.
Part 3: BI, comparison of stationary and ambulatory attention measures, and SW
Relation between ambulatory attention and stationary attention patterns by BI status
Across all children, happy bias dwell time score was negatively related to average visit duration to the stranger, r = −.5, p = .002, and proportion of dwell time on the stranger, r = −.32 p = .05. No additional correlations were found between mobile and stationary eye-tracking indices, ps > .15.
Given the findings from Part 2, we examined whether BI status moderated the association between the number of visits to the stranger and angry bias indexed by the dot-probe face-fixation frequency. The linear regression model revealed a significant Visit Number × BI interaction effect, B = 0.41, ß = 0.51, t = 2.46, p = .02, R2 = .16 (Figure 4). That is, number of visits to the stranger was positively related to the stationary eye-tracking angry bias index for BI children B = 0.35, ß = 0.9, t = 2.39, p = .02, whereas the association was not significant for non-BI children, B = −0.05, ß = −0.14, t = −0.72, p = .47. A second regression model indicated that BI status did not moderate the relation between number of visits to the stranger and the happy bias index, B = 0.21, ß = 0.27, t = 1.21, p = .23, R2 = .06.
The effect of ambulatory and stationary attention patterns on SW levels
The hierarchical regression model suggested that after controlling for the effect of stranger presence duration, B = −0.004, ß = −0.34, t = −2.11, p = .04, the effect of the stationary angry bias did not significantly explain SW levels, B = 0.01, ß = 0.09, t = 0.57, p = .58, R2 = .13. Including the ambulatory number of visits to the stranger explained significantly more variance in SW levels, B = −0.01, ß = −0.38, t = −2.51, p = .02, ΔR2 = .14. The Visit Number × Angry Bias interaction effect on SW levels was not significant, B = 0.002, ß = 0.29, t = 1.88, p = .07, R2 = .34. Similarly, the second regression model revealed that the number of visits to the stranger accounted for significantly more variance in SW levels, relative to only including stranger presence duration and stationary happy bias score as predictors, B = −0.01, ß = −0.36, t = −2.34, p = .03, ΔR2 = 0.12. The Visit Number × Happy Bias interaction effect on SW levels was also not significant, B = 0.002, ß = 0.28, t = 1.93, p = .06, R2 = .36.
Discussion
The present study is the first to examine attention patterns toward affectively salient stimuli in children assessed for BI by directly comparing manual RTs with stationary and mobile eye-tracking measures of attention. Affect-biased attention plays a central role in anxiety regulation (Todd et al., 2012; White et al., 2009) and the development of socioemotional functioning (Morales, Fu, et al., 2016). Existing literature strives to understand how age-related changes in affect-biased attention interact with early vulnerability factors, such as BI, to influence the emergence of psychopathology (Field & Lester, 2010; Morales, Fu, et al., 2016; Shechner et al., 2012). However, the extant literature is inconsistent in this association, which may relate to how attention is measured. The current study advances attention assessment beyond screen-based paradigms and into social interactive contexts.
This study examined whether BI-related differences emerged in stationary and ambulatory measures of attention: manual RT and eye-tracking indices in the dot-probe paradigm, and mobile eye-tracking measures of ambulatory attention during active social interaction. We also examined whether the stationary and ambulatory attention measures independently predicted parent reports of SW levels. Finally, we examined whether the association between the stationary and ambulatory attention indices varied as a function of BI status, and whether the stationary and ambulatory indices jointly predicted SW levels.
The current study revealed several important findings. First, while there were no BI group differences in RT and eye-tracking indices of AB, the proof-of-concept investigation among a subsample of the participants found that BI children made fewer gaze visits to the stranger than the non-BI group during active interaction with the stranger. Second, the association between gaze frequency to the stranger and stationary AB (indexed by the difference in angry vs. neutral face fixation frequency) differed as a function of BI status. That is, the BI group was characterized by a consistent pattern of stationary and ambulatory threat-related attention. Third, while stationary AB alone did not predict SW, including the ambulatory gaze frequency measure as an additional predictor added some explanatory power. We did not find strong evidence suggesting that stationary and ambulatory attention patterns jointly predict SW levels. Together, the current study provided preliminary evidence suggesting that BI may be associated with trait-like attention patterns toward putative social threats evident across contexts. We will consider each of these findings in turn.
RT scores of AB to angry or happy faces did not differ between BI groups, nor was a relation found when BI scores were considered as a continuous measure (see online-only Supplement 2). This is consistent with studies that compared dot-probe RT performance in 9- to 12-year-olds characterized using the same BI cutoff (Auday et al., 2018; Fu et al., 2017; Morales et al., 2017) and in 5- to 7-year-olds characterized as BI in toddlerhood (Pérez-Edgar et al., 2011; White et al., 2017). Furthermore, the bias scores did not correlate with SW levels, in line with prior studies (Cole et al., 2016; Morales et al., 2015; Pérez-Edgar et al., 2011). While some studies have found a relation between dot-probe responses and anxiety in adults, this relationship is not consistent, particularly in children (Dudeney et al., 2015; Roy et al., 2015; Shechner et al., 2012). The RT difference score has poor reliability in youth (Britton et al., 2013; Brown et al., 2014), which might have further undermined our power to detect BI difference with a modest sample size (Hedge, Powell, & Sumner, 2018). A major problem with RT measures is that attention is captured as snapshots in time with the probe presentation (Rodebaugh et al. 2016; Yiend, 2010). As such, these measures might be too “noisy” to reliably capture the core attention patterns associated with vulnerability for socioemotional problems, such as anxiety (Shechner et al., 2012). These evident limitations of dot-probe RT measures generated current calls for implementing multiple levels of attention measurements (Price et al., 2015; Rodebaugh et al., 2016).
Because of the improvements in attention assessments embedded in eye-tracking measures, we predicted BI children would display AB indexed by dot-probe eye-tracking measures. However, contrary to our hypothesis, there were no BI-related differences in the stationary eye-tracking measures. In addition, there was no significant relation between stationary AB scores and SW levels. This is an important relation because SW is conceptualized as an intermediate development sequela in the link between BI and the emergence of anxiety (Rubin et al., 2009). Although there are still relatively few eye-tracking studies using established paradigms in children, eye-tracking indices of AB has generated some improvements in reliability over RT measures (Price et al., 2015). However, inconsistencies are still evident in pediatric studies. For example, pediatric anxiety was associated with initial vigilance toward angry faces in one study (Shechner et al., 2013), with threat avoidance in others (Gamble & Rapee, 2009; In-Albon, Kossowsky, & Schneider, 2009; Shechner et al., 2017), and in some, with difficulty in disengaging from threat (Seefeldt, Krämer, Tuschen-Caffier, & Heinrichs, 2014). Some of this variability may relate to variance in paradigm and stimulus presentation duration. These studies employed variations of free-viewing with stimulus presentation ranging from 500 ms (Gamble & Rapee, 2009) to 4000 ms (In-Albon et al., 2009). It is also possible that anxiety-related AB takes place at both early and late stages of visual information processing (Cisler & Koster, 2010). The 500-ms face presentation implemented in the current study might be too short to capture temperament-related individual differences in later components of AB. By using a long face presentation of 2000 ms in a dot-probe task, Price et al. (2015) showed that the eye-tracking index of difficulty in disengaging from fearful faces had good test–retest reliability in 9- to 13-year-olds, but the reliability score was lower than the eye-tracking indices for initial attention vigilance and sustained attention toward threat. Hence, to increase the reliability and sensitivity of stationary eye-tracking AB measures, it would be fruitful to use multiple task paradigms that are optimized to capture different components of AB.
Even with implemented variations, computerized attention paradigms still limit our ability to understand the interactive relation between affect-biased attention, emotion dysregulation, and socioemotional behavior in the real-world settings (Fu & Pérez-Edgar, 2019; Morales et al., 2017; Todd et al., 2012). Affect-biased attention is one method used to regulate negative emotional responses (Cole et al., 2017). Attention engagement and disengagement in a dynamic environment impact the development from early BI to subsequent socioemotional adjustment, as suggested by theoretical models (Morales, Fu, et al., 2016; Henderson et al., 2015) and longitudinal evidence (Morales et al., 2015; Nozadi et al., 2016; Pérez-Edgar et al., 2010, 2011; White et al., 2017). Thus, we expected to see that BI children display altered attention patterns during an active interaction with a putative social threat.
Here, we found that BI children directed fewer eye gazes toward the stranger compared to their non-BI peers. However, there were no BI group differences in average latency of reengagement and sustained attention toward the stranger. Thus, the BI group was associated with a gaze pattern that is characterized specifically by reduced frequency of reengagement to the social partner, rather than a global avoidance pattern. A similar pattern of attention avoidance from threat was also found in 6-year-old children who were characterized by a dysregulated fear temperament profile in toddlerhood (Morales et al., 2015). Thus, the current findings suggest that fearful temperament is associated with attention avoidance from affectively salient social stimuli in a naturalistic context.
BI children may use attention avoidance as a reactive, involuntary emotion regulation strategy to reduce initial fear reactivity (Gross, 2014; Rueda, 2012). Attention avoidance may temporarily ameliorate negative affect, although in the long term it is an ineffective emotion regulation strategy that predicts heightened anxiety (Aldao et al., 2010; Jazaieri et al., 2015). Adult studies showed that AB away from imminent threat predicts concurrent posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms (Bar-Haim et al., 2010), as well as posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms a year later (Wald et al., 2011). Recent research (Troller-Renfree, Buzzell, Pine, Henderson, & Fox, 2019) examining the chronometry of regulatory control revealed that toddlerhood BI predicted increased engagement of reactive control even after the eliciting challenge is removed. Moreover, adolescents characterized with early BI who also adopt reactive control display increased anxiety symptoms.
It should be noted that BI levels, measured using continuous scores, did not predict the frequency of gaze visit to the stranger (online-only Supplement 2). The BI category represents 23% of the 158 children screened. Hence, it is possible that the attention avoidance pattern is not a linear effect and only characterizes children with extreme levels of BI. Replication in a larger sample with BI scores computed from both parent-report and observational measures (Fox et al., 2001) would help assess the generalizability and reliability of the current preliminary data.
We provide preliminary evidence suggesting that children’s looking behavior in vivo can facilitate the identification of attention patterns that underlie the vulnerability of developing socioemotional maladjustment. Specifically, the BI group was characterized by a consistent threat-related attention pattern across the computerized and social interactive task contexts. The positive association between stationary and ambulatory attention measures was also significant at high levels of BI measured using continuous scores (online-only Supplement 2), indicating a potentially robust linear effect. Largely in parallel with the present findings, Morales et al. (2017) showed that BI children (9- to 12-year-olds) also showed a cross-task correlation for RT threat bias scores. In addition, a consistent pattern of threat bias across both tasks was linked to greater social anxiety (Morales et al., 2017). Corroborating previous findings using screen-based paradigms, it is likely that BI is characterized by traitlike, and possibly inflexible AB patterns in both static and dynamic (real-life) social contexts.
BI is characterized by a hyperreactive neural response pattern toward both threat and reward (Kagan, 2012; Guyer et al., 2006). This hyperreactive response style may frequently engage regulatory attention control functions, such as avoidance, as an attempt to dampen the experienced intensity of affective stimuli. Over time, the engagement of regulatory processes becomes more reactive, rigid, and less efficient in regulating bottom-up attention orienting toward the affectively salient stimuli. An entrenched and context-independent attention pattern may gradually form through this cyclic process, which in turn increases the risk for socioemotional maladjustment and anxiety in BI children (Henderson et al., 2015; Henderson & Wilson, 2017; Pérez-Edgar, 2018).
We believe that incorporating mobile eye-tracking in a real-life social interactive context can complement computer-based assessments to uncover fine-grained attention patterns underlying anxiety vulnerability. There were, however, several limitations in the mobile eye-tracking portion of this study. First, we had a small sample of children who contributed mobile eye-tracking data (N = 37). Although the sample size is comparable to published mobile eye-tracking studies in infants (e.g., Franchak et al., 2017; Kretch et al., 2014), adolescents (Woody et al., 2019), and adults (e.g., Freeth et al., 2013; Laidlaw et al., 2011), it is not adequate for high-level modeling. The small sample size may have especially affected the power of our multiple regression models examining interaction effects (Maxwell, 2000). While the moderating effect of BI in the relation between ambulatory and stationary attention measures has adequate effect size (Cohen, 1988), replications using larger samples are needed to uncover robust interaction effects. The mobile eye-tracking method is valuable but labor-intensive, which limits the extent to which large sizes can be easily acquired. The efficiency of data processing can be improved with the development of more automated fixation AOI identification functions.
Second, our BI characterization was based on parents’ reports. While the BIQ is a reliable and valid instrument that has been extensively used for BI identification (e.g., Liu et al., 2018; Mernick et al., 2018), the investigation of BI-related differences in ambulatory attention patterns will benefit from relying on laboratory observations to identify the BI group (Kagan, 2003). One future direction is to compute a BI composite from coded parameters (e.g., latency, frequency, and duration) of fearful behavior across Lab-TAB fear-eliciting episodes (Fox et al., 2015; Goldsmith et al., 1994; Kagan et al., 1984). Researchers can then examine how the trait-level BI status and the state-level fear responses during the episode may influence children’s online gaze patterns, and how these patterns may interact with BI to predict SW and anxiety levels. Furthermore, at a microlevel, a dynamic systems approach can model the ebb and flow of fear responses and the engagement of fear regulation during the exposure to putative social threats (Cole et al., 2017; Morales et al., 2018). The current study did not directly examine the moment-by-moment relation between attention and fear responses. By providing spatially and temporally sensitive assessments gazes in real time, mobile eye-tracking opens the opportunity to study how ambulatory attention influences the unfolding of fear responses and regulation during social interactions.
Third, the present study assessed attention processes in a narrow developmental window that is marked by increased SW problems as children transition to formal schooling. Another future direction may be to chart age-related changes in stationary and ambulatory attention. Cross-sectional (Pérez-Edgar et al., 2017) and longitudinal (Leppänen, Cataldo, Enlow, & Nelson, 2018; Peltola, Yrttiaho, & Leppänen, 2018) stationary eye-tracking studies in infants have shown significant age effects on threat-related attention patterns. While it is expected that children might be better able to exert some regulatory control on bottom-up attention orienting with the development of executive attention in childhood (Rothbart et al., 2011), we lack the data needed to depict possible linear or nonlinear changes in affect-biased attention patterns from infancy through childhood (Morales, Fu, et al., 2016; Shechner et al., 2012). With respect to ambulatory threat-related attention specifically, researchers could investigate whether there is a shift from attention preference toward threat in toddlerhood (e.g., Kiel & Buss, 2011) to threat avoidance in childhood (e.g., Morales et al., 2015).
Fourth, although we adopted a social interactive paradigm, the data from the current study was still acquired in a laboratory setting. Thus, we lack full ecological validity in our assessment. The experimental paradigm was implemented to maximize control of stimulus presentation for this preliminary study. However, there are factors such as familiarity and context that are likely to play an important role in real-world settings that are not incorporated into our paradigm. With technological optimization of offline calibration, and the use of small wearable devices (e.g., cell phones) for data collection and online data quality monitoring (Pupil Labs, https://pupil-labs.com/pupil/), future studies will be able to venture into the “real” world to sample children’s ambulatory attention as they navigate their specific social environments (Jung, Zimmerman, & Pérez-Edgar, 2018). This would vastly improve our understanding of the real-world clinical utility of incorporating visual attention measures into clinical and therapeutic interventions.
In conclusion, the present proof-of-concept study demonstrates the utility and benefits of using mobile eye-tracking technology to interrogate affect-biased attention patterns that may contribute to emotion regulation and evident vulnerability for socioemotional problems. Specifically, we highlighted that, relative to RT and computerized eye-tracking paradigms, assessing ambulatory attention patterns in the context of an active social interaction may enhance our sensitivity in detecting BI-related individual differences in threat-related attention patterns. Furthermore, the findings indicated that BI might be characterized by a potentially inflexible, context-independent pattern of attention deployment, which limits emotional regulation capacity and increases risk for socioemotional difficulties.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments.
We thank families for their participation in the study.
Financial support. The study was supported by the National Institute of Mental Health Grant R21 MH111980 (to K.P.E.). Preparation of the manuscript was supported by internal funding from the Research Institute at Nationwide Children’s Hospital.
Footnotes
Supplementary material. The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579419000427.
In the earlier phase of the larger study, we recorded mobile eye-tracking data using an earlier version of Pupil Capture (v.0.9.6) installed on a Microsoft Surface Pro 3 tablet with Windows 10. Data from 9 participants in the current subsample were collected using this configuration. To examine the impact of the software and hardware update, we created a binary variable to index the two types of equipment. Independent samples t tests suggested that our key mobile eye-tracking DVs did not differ between the two sets of participants (ps > .11). Thus, this variable was not included in models for parsimony.
Additional analyses using continuous scores of BI are presented in the online-only Supplement 2.
References
- Abend R, Pine DS, & Bar-Haim Y (2014). The TAU-NIMH Attention Bias Measurement Toolbox. Retrieved from http://people.socsci.tau.ac.il/mu/anxietytrauma/research/
- Aldao A, Nolen-Hoeksema S, & Schweizer S (2010). Emotion-regulation strategies across psychopathology: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 30, 217–237. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2009.11.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Armstrong JM, Goldstein LH, & MacArthur Working Group on Outcome Assessment. (2003). Manual for the MacArthur Health and Behavior Questionnaire (HBQ 1.0). Pittsburgh, PA: University of Pittsburgh, MacArthur Foundation Research Network on Psychopathology and Development. [Google Scholar]
- Armstrong T, & Olatunji BO (2012). Eye tracking of attention in the affective disorders: A meta-analytic review and synthesis. Clinical Psychology Review, 32, 704–723. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2012.09.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Auday ES, Taber-Thomas BC, & Pérez-Edgar KE (2018). Neural correlates of attention bias to masked facial threat cues: Examining children at-risk for social anxiety disorder. NeuroImage: Clinical, 19, 202–212. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2018.04.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bambach S, Crandall DJ, Smith LB, & Yu C (2018). Toddler-inspired visual object learning. Paper presented at the 32nd Conference on Neural Information Processing Systems (NIPS 2018), Montréal, Canada. [Google Scholar]
- Bar-Haim Y, Holoshitz Y, Eldar S, Frenkel TI, Muller D, Charney DS, … Wald I (2010). Life-threatening danger and suppression of attention bias to threat. American Journal of Psychiatry, 167, 694–698. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.09070956 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bar-Haim Y, Lamy D, Pergamin L, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, & van IJzendoorn MH (2007). Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious individuals: A meta-analytic study. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 1–24. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.133.1.1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beauchaine TP, & Zisner A (2017). Motivation, emotion regulation, and the latent structure of psychopathology: An integrative and convergent historical perspective. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 119, 108–118. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2016.12.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beesdo K, Knappe S, & Pine DS (2009). Anxiety and anxiety disorders in children and adolescents: Developmental issues and implications for DSM-V. Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 32, 483–524. doi: 10.1016/j.psc.2009.06.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bishop G, Spence SH, & McDonald C (2003). Can parents and teachers provide a reliable and valid report of behavioral inhibition? Child Development, 74, 1899–1917. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bishop SJ (2008). Neural mechanisms underlying selective attention to threat. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1129, 141–152. doi: 10.1196/annals.1417.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blackford JU, Clauss JA, & Benningfield MM (2018). The neurobiology of behavioral inhibition as a developmental mechanism In Pérez-Edgar K & Fox NA (Eds.), Behavioral inhibition: Integrating theory, research, and clinical perspectives (pp. 113–134). Cham: Springer International. [Google Scholar]
- Blackford JU, & Pine DS (2012). Neural substrates of childhood anxiety disorders: A review of neuroimaging findings. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 21, 501–525. doi: 10.1016/j.chc.2012.05.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Britton JC, Bar-Haim Y, Clementi MA, Sankin LS, Chen G, Shechner T, … Pine DS (2013). Training-associated changes and stability of attention bias in youth: Implications for Attention Bias Modification Treatment for pediatric anxiety. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 4, 52–64. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2012.11.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Broeren S, & Muris P (2010). A psychometric evaluation of the behavioral inhibition questionnaire in a non-clinical sample of Dutch children and adolescents. Child Psychiatry & Human Development, 41, 214–229. doi: 10.1007/s10578-009-0162-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Broeren S, Muris P, Bouwmeester S, Field AP, & Voerman JS (2011). Processing biases for emotional faces in 4- to 12-year-old non-clinical children: An exploratory study of developmental patterns and relationships with social anxiety and behavioral inhibition. Journal of Experimental Psychopathology, 2, 454–474. [Google Scholar]
- Brown HM, Eley TC, Broeren S, MacLeod C, Rinck M, Hadwin JA, & Lester KJ (2014). Psychometric properties of reaction time based experimental paradigms measuring anxiety-related information-processing biases in children. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 28, 97–107. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2013.11.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burris JL, Barry-Anwar RA, & Rivera SM (2017). An eye tracking investigation of attentional biases towards affect in young children. Developmental Psychology, 53, 1418–1427. doi: 10.1037/dev0000345 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burris JL, Barry-Anwar RA, Sims RN, Hagerman RJ, Tassone F, & Rivera SM (2017). Children with fragile X syndrome display threat-specific biases toward emotion. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, 2, 487–492. doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2017.06.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buss KA, & Kiel EJ (2013). Temperamental risk factors for pediatric anxiety disorders In Vasa RA & Roy AK (Eds.), Pediatric anxiety disorders (pp. 47–68). New York: Springer. [Google Scholar]
- Cisler JM, & Koster EHW (2010). Mechanisms of attentional biases towards threat in anxiety disorders: An integrative review. Clinical Psychology Review, 30, 203–216. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2009.11.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clauss JA, Benningfield MM, Rao U, & Blackford JU (2016). Altered prefrontal cortex function marks heightened anxiety risk in children. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 55, 809–816. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2016.05.024 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen J (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. [Google Scholar]
- Cole CE, Zapp DJ, Fettig NB, & Pérez-Edgar K (2016). Impact of attention biases to threat and effortful control on individual variations in negative affect and social withdrawal in very young children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 141, 210–221. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2015.09.012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cole PM, Hall SE, & Hajal NJ (2017). Emotion dysregulation as a risk factor for psychopathology In Beauchaine TP & Hinshaw SP (Eds.), Child and adolescent psychopathology (3rd ed., pp. 346–386). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. [Google Scholar]
- Coplan RJ, & Arbeau KA (2008). The stresses of a “brave new world”: Shyness and school adjustment in kindergarten. Journal of Research in Childhood Education, 22, 377–389. doi: 10.1080/02568540809594634 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Corbetta M, & Shulman GL (2002). Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3, 201–215. doi: 10.1038/nrn755 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crockenberg SC, Leerkes EM, & Bárrig Jó PS (2008). Predicting aggressive behavior in the third year from infant reactivity and regulation as moderated by maternal behavior. Development and Psychopathology, 20, 37–54. doi: 10.1017/S0954579408000023 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Team Datavyu. (2014). Datavyu: A Video Coding Tool Databrary Project, New York University; http://datavyu.org [Google Scholar]
- Degnan KA, Almas AN, Henderson HA, Hane AA, Walker OL, & Fox NA (2014). Longitudinal trajectories of social reticence with unfamiliar peers across early childhood. Developmental Psychology, 50, 2311–2323. doi: 10.1037/a0037751 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Degnan KA, & Fox NA (2007). Behavioral inhibition and anxiety disorders: Multiple levels of a resilience process. Development and Psychopathology, 19, 729–746. doi: 10.1017/S0954579407000363 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dudeney J, Sharpe L, & Hunt C (2015). Attentional bias towards threatening stimuli in children with anxiety: A meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 40, 66–75. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2015.05.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dyson MW, Klein DN, Olino TM, Dougherty LR, & Durbin CE (2011). Social and non-social behavioral inhibition in preschool-age children: Differential associations with parent-reports of temperament and anxiety. Child Psychiatry & Human Development, 42, 390–405. doi: 10.1007/s10578-011-0225-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ehlers MR, & Todd RM (2017). Genesis and maintenance of attentional biases: The role of the locus coeruleus-noradrenaline system, 2017, 6817349. doi: 10.1155/2017/6817349 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eisenberg N, Spinrad TL, & Eggum ND (2010). Emotion-related self-regulation and its relation to children’s maladjustment. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 6, 495–525. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.121208.131208 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Essex MJ, Boyce WT, Goldstein LH, Armstrong JM, Kraemer HC, & Kupfer DJ (2002). The confluence of mental, physical, social, and academic difficulties in middle childhood: II. Developing the MacArthur Health and Behavior Questionnaire. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 41, 588–603. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200205000-00017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eysenck MW, Derakshan N, Santos R, & Calvo MG (2007). Anxiety and cognitive performance: Attentional control theory. Emotion, 7, 336–353. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.7.2.336 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Field AP, & Lester KJ (2010). Is there room for “development” in developmental models of information processing biases to threat in children and adolescents? Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 13, 315–332. doi: 10.1007/s10567-010-0078-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Foulsham T, Walker E, & Kingstone A (2011). The where, what and when of gaze allocation in the lab and the natural environment. Vision Research, 51, 1920–1931. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2011.07.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fox NA, Henderson HA, Rubin KH, Calkins SD, & Schmidt LA (2001). Continuity and discontinuity of behavioral inhibition and exuberance: Psychophysiological and behavioral influences across the first four years of life. Child Development, 72, 1–21. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fox NA, Snidman N, Haas SA, Degnan KA, & Kagan J (2015). The relations between reactivity at 4 months and behavioral inhibition in the second year: Replication across three independent samples. Infancy, 20, 98–114. doi: 10.1111/infa.12063 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Franchak JM (2017). Using head-mounted eye tracking to study development In Hopkins B, Geangu E, & Linkenauger S (Eds.), The Cambridge encyclopedia of child development (2nd ed.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Franchak JM (in press). Looking with the eyes and head In Wagman J & Blau J (Eds.), Perception as information detection: Reflections on Gibson’s ecological approach to visual perception. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. [Google Scholar]
- Franchak JM, & Adolph KE (2010). Visually guided navigation: Head-mounted eye-tracking of natural locomotion in children and adults. Vision Research, 50, 2766–2774. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2010.09.024 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Franchak JM, Kretch KS, & Adolph KE (2017). See and be seen: Infant–caregiver social looking during locomotor free play. Developmental Science, 21, e12626. doi: 10.1111/desc.12626 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Franchak JM, Kretch KS, Soska KC, & Adolph KE (2011). Head-mounted eye-tracking: A new method to describe infant looking. Child Development, 82(6), 1738–1750. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01670.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Freeth M, Foulsham T, & Kingstone A (2013). What affects social attention? Social presence, eye contact and autistic traits. PLOS ONE, 8, e53286. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0053286 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fu X, & Pérez-Edgar K (2019). Threat-related attention bias in socioemotional development: A critical review and methodological considerations.Developmental Review, 51, 31–57. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2018.11.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fu X, Taber-Thomas BC, & Pérez-Edgar K (2017). Frontolimbic functioning during threat-related attention: Relations to early behavioral inhibition and anxiety in children. Biological Psychology, 122, 98–109. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2015.08.010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gamble AL, & Rapee RM (2009). The time-course of attentional bias in anxious children and adolescents. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 23, 841–847. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2009.04.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goldsmith HH, Reilly HH, Lemery KS, Longley S, & Prescott A (1994). Manual for the Preschool Laboratory Temperament Assessment Battery (Lab-TAB). Unpublished manuscript, University of Wisconsin, Madison. [Google Scholar]
- Gross JJ (1998). Antecedent- and response-focused emotion regulation: Divergent consequences for experience, expression, and physiology. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 224–237. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gross JJ (2014). Handbook of emotion regulation (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford Press. [Google Scholar]
- Guyer AE, Nelson EE, Perez-Edgar K, Hardin MG, Roberson-Nay R, Monk CS, … Ernst M (2006). Striatal Functional Alteration in Adolescents Characterized by Early Childhood Behavioral Inhibition. Journal of Neuroscience, 26(24), 6399–6405. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0666-06.2006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hardee JE, Benson BE, Bar-Haim Y, Mogg K, Bradley BP, Chen G, … Pérez-Edgar K (2013). Patterns of neural connectivity during an attention bias task moderate associations between early childhood temperament and internalizing symptoms in young adulthood. Biological Psychiatry, 74, 273–279. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.01.036 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hayhoe MM, & Rothkopf CA (2011). Vision in the natural world. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 2, 158–166. doi: 10.1002/wcs.113 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hedge C, Powell G, & Sumner P (2018). The reliability paradox: Why robust cognitive tasks do not produce reliable individual differences. Behavior Research Methods, 50, 1166–1186. doi: 10.3758/s13428-017-0935-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Henderson HA, Pine DS, & Fox NA (2015). Behavioral inhibition and developmental risk: A dual-processing perspective. Neuropsychopharmacology, 40,207–224. doi: 10.1038/npp.2014.189 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Henderson HA, & Wilson MJG (2017). Attention processes underlying risk and resilience in behaviorally inhibited children. Current Behavioral Neuroscience Reports, 4, 99–106. doi: 10.1007/s40473-017-0111-z [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Hilt LM, Hanson JL, & Pollak SD (2011). Emotion dysregulation In Brown BB & Prinstein MJ (Eds.), Encyclopedia of adolescence (Vol. 3, pp. 160–169). New York: Elsevier. [Google Scholar]
- Hilt LM, Leitzke BT, & Pollak SD (2017). Can’t take my eyes off of you: Eye tracking reveals how ruminating young adolescents get stuck. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 46, 858–867. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2015.1121824 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hollenstein T (2007). State space grids: Analyzing dynamics across development. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 31, 384–396. doi: 10.1177/0165025407077765 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- In-Albon T, Kossowsky J, & Schneider S (2009). Vigilance and avoidance of threat in the eye movements of children with separation anxiety disorder. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 38, 225–235. doi: 10.1007/s10802-009-9359-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jazaieri H, Morrison AS, Goldin PR, & Gross JJ (2015). The role of emotion and emotion regulation in social anxiety disorder. Current Psychiatry Reports, 17, 531. doi: 10.1007/s11920-014-0531-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jung YJ, Zimmerman HT, & Pérez-Edgar K (2018). A methodological case study with mobile eye-tracking of child interaction in a science museum. TechTrends, 62, 509–517. doi: 10.1007/s11528-018-0310-9 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Kagan J (2003). Behavioral inhibition as a temperamental category In Davidson RJ, Sherer KR, & Goldsmith H. Hill (Eds.), Handbook of affective sciences (pp. 320–331). New York: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Kagan J (2012). The biography of behavioral inhibition In Zentner M & Shiner RL (Eds.), The handbook of temperament (pp. 69–82). New York: Guilford Press. [Google Scholar]
- Kagan J, Reznick JS, Clarke C, Snidman N, & Garcia-Coll C (1984). Behavioral inhibition to the unfamiliar. Child Development, 55, 2212–2225. doi: 10.2307/1129793 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Kassner M, Patera W, & Bulling A (2014). Pupil: An open source platform for pervasive eye tracking and mobile gaze-based interaction. ArXiv E-Prints, 1405. arXiv:1405.0006
- Kiel EJ, & Buss KA (2011). Toddlers’ duration of attention towards putative threat. Infancy, 16, 198–210. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-7078.2010.00036.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kretch KS, & Adolph KE (2015). Active vision in passive locomotion: Real-world free viewing in infants and adults. Developmental Science, 18, 736–750. doi: 10.1111/desc.12251 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kretch KS, Franchak JM, & Adolph KE (2014). Crawling and walking infants see the world differently. Child Development, 85, 1503–1518. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12206 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kujawa AJ, Torpey D, Kim J, Hajcak G, Rose S, Gotlib IH, & Klein DN (2010). Attentional biases for emotional faces in young children of mothers with chronic or recurrent depression. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 39, 125–135. doi: 10.1007/s10802-010-9438-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Laidlaw KEW, Foulsham T, Kuhn G, & Kingstone A (2011). Potential social interactions are important to social attention. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108, 5548–5553. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1017022108 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lemery-Chalfant K, Schreiber JE, Schmidt NL, Hulle CAV, Essex MJ, & Goldsmith HH (2007). Assessing internalizing, externalizing, and attention problems in young children: Validation of the MacArthur HBQ. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 46, 1315–1323. doi: 10.1097/chi.0b013e3180f616c6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lamey A, Hollenstein T, Lewis MD, & Granic I (2004). GridWare (Version 1.1). [Computer software]. Retrieved from http://www.statespace-grids.org
- Leppänen JM, Cataldo JK, Enlow MB, & Nelson CA (2018). Early development of attention to threat-related facial expressions. PLOS ONE, 13, e0197424. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0197424 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leppänen JM, & Nelson CA (2012). Early development of fear processing. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21, 200–204. doi: 10.1177/0963721411435841 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Lewis MD, Lamey AV, & Douglas L (1999). A new dynamic systems method for the analysis of early socioemotional development. Developmental Science, 2, 457–475. doi: 10.1111/1467-7687.00090 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Liu P, Taber-Thomas BC, Fu X, & Pérez-Edgar KE (2018). Biobehavioral markers of attention bias modification in temperamental risk for anxiety: A randomized control trial. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 57, 103–110. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2017.11.016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maxwell SE (2000). Sample size and multiple regression analysis. Psychological Methods, 5, 434–458. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.5.4.434 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mernick B, Pine A, Gendler T, & Shechner T (2018). A psychometric evaluation of the behavioral inhibition questionnaire in a non-clinical sample of Israeli children and adolescents. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 27, 1794–1804. doi: 10.1007/s10826-018-1027-2 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Mogg K, & Bradley BP (1998). A cognitive-motivational analysis of anxiety. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 36, 809–848. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7967(98)00063-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morales S, Fu X, & Pérez-Edgar KE (2016). A developmental neuroscience perspective on affect-biased attention. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 21, 26–41. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2016.08.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morales S, Pérez-Edgar KE, & Buss KA (2015). Attention biases towards and away from threat mark the relation between early dysregulated fear and the later emergence of social withdrawal. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 43, 1067–1078. doi: 10.1007/s10802-014-9963-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morales S, Pérez-Edgar K, & Buss KA (2016). Longitudinal relations among exuberance, externalizing behaviors, and attentional bias to reward: The mediating role of effortful control. Developmental Science, 19, 853–862. doi: 10.1111/desc.12320 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morales S, Ram N, Buss KA, Cole PM, Helm JL, & Chow S-M (2018). Age-related changes in the dynamics of fear-related regulation in early childhood. Developmental Science, 21, e12633. doi: 10.1111/desc.12633 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morales S, Taber-Thomas BC, & Pérez-Edgar KE (2017). Patterns of attention to threat across tasks in behaviorally inhibited children at risk for anxiety. Developmental Science, 20. doi: 10.1111/desc.12391 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nozadi S, Troller-Renfree S, White LK, Frenkel T, Degnan KA, Bar-Haim Y, … Fox NA (2016). The moderating role of attention biases to threat on the link between behavioral inhibition and anxiety in children. Journal of Experimental Psychopathology, 7, 451–465. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oakes LM (2010). Infancy guidelines for publishing eye-tracking data. Infancy, 15, 1–5. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-7078.2010.00030.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peltola MJ, Yrttiaho S, & Leppänen JM (2018). Infants’ attention bias to faces as an early marker of social development. Developmental Science, 21, e12687. doi: 10.1111/desc.12687 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pérez-Edgar K (2018). Attention mechanisms in behavioral inhibition: Exploring and exploiting the environment In Pérez-Edgar K & Fox NA (Eds.), Behavioral inhibition: Integrating theory, research, and clinical perspectives (pp. 237–261). Cham: Springer International. [Google Scholar]
- Pérez-Edgar K, Bar-Haim Y, McDermott JM, Chronis-Tuscano A, Pine DS, & Fox NA (2010). Attention biases to threat and behavioral inhibition in early childhood shape adolescent social withdrawal Emotion (Washington, DC: ), 10, 349–357. doi: 10.1037/a0018486 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pérez-Edgar KE, & Guyer AE (2014). Behavioral inhibition: Temperament or prodrome? Current Behavioral Neuroscience Reports, 1, 182–190. doi: 10.1007/s40473-014-0019-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pérez-Edgar K, Morales S, LoBue V, Taber-Thomas BC, Allen EK, Brown KM, & Buss KA (2017). The impact of negative affect on attention patterns to threat across the first 2 years of life. Developmental Psychology, 53, 2219–2232. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pérez-Edgar K, Reeb-Sutherland BC, McDermott JM, White LK, Henderson HA, Degnan KA, … Fox NA (2011). Attention biases to threat link behavioral inhibition to social withdrawal over time in very young children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 39, 885–895. doi: 10.1007/s10802-011-9495-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petersen SE, & Posner MI (2012). The attention system of the human brain: 20 years after. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 35, 73–89. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-062111-150525 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Posner MI, & Rothbart MK (2007). Research on attention networks as a model for the integration of psychological science. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 1–23. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085516 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Posner MI, Rothbart MK, Sheese BE, & Voelker P (2014). Developing attention: Behavioral and brain mechanisms. Advances in Neuroscience, 2014, e405094. doi: 10.1155/2014/405094 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Price RB, Kuckertz JM, Siegle GJ, Ladouceur CD, Silk JS, Ryan ND, … Amir N (2015). Empirical recommendations for improving the stability of the dot-probe task in clinical research. Psychological Assessment, 27, 365–376. doi: 10.1037/pas0000036 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Price RB, Rosen D, Siegle GJ, Ladouceur CD, Tang K, Allen KB, … Silk JS (2016). From anxious youth to depressed adolescents: Prospective prediction of 2-year depression symptoms via attentional bias measures. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 125, 267–278. doi: 10.1037/abn0000127 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Price RB, Siegle GJ, Silk JS, Ladouceur C, McFarland A, Dahl RE, & Ryan ND (2013). Sustained neural alterations in anxious youth performing an attentional bias task: A pupilometry study. Depression and Anxiety, 30, 22–30. doi: 10.1002/da.21966 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Prior M, Smart D, Sanson A, & Oberklaid F (2000). Does shy-inhibited temperament in childhood lead to anxiety problems in adolescence? Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 39, 461–468. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200004000-00015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Redcay E, & Warnell KR (2018). A social-interactive neuroscience approach to understanding the developing brain. Advances in Child Development and Behavior, 54, 1–44. doi: 10.1016/bs.acdb.2017.10.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rodebaugh TL, Scullin RB, Langer JK, Dixon DJ, Huppert JD, Bernstein A, … Lenze EJ (2016). Unreliability as a threat to understanding psychopathology: The cautionary tale of attentional bias. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 125, 840–851. doi: 10.1037/abn0000184 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rothbart MK, Sheese BE, Rueda MR, & Posner MI (2011). Developing mechanisms of self-regulation in early life. Emotion Review, 3, 207–213. doi: 10.1177/1754073910387943 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roy AK, Dennis TA, & Warner CM (2015). A critical review of attentional threat bias and its role in the treatment of pediatric anxiety disorders. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy, 29, 171–184. doi: 10.1891/0889-8391.29.3.171 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rubin KH, Coplan RJ, & Bowker JC (2009). Social withdrawal in childhood. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 141–171. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163642 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rueda MR (2012). Effortful control In Zentner M & Shiner RL (Eds.), The handbook of temperament (pp. 145–167). New York: Guilford Press. [Google Scholar]
- Schwartz CE, Wright CI, Shin LM, Kagan J, & Rauch SL (2003). Inhibited and uninhibited infants “Grown Up”: Adult amygdalar response to novelty. Science, 300, 1952–1953. doi: 10.1126/science.1083703 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seefeldt WL, Krämer M, Tuschen-Caffier B, & Heinrichs N (2014). Hypervigilance and avoidance in visual attention in children with social phobia. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 45, 105–112. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2013.09.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shechner T, Britton JC, Pérez-Edgar K, Bar-Haim Y, Ernst M, Fox NA, … Pine DS (2012). Attention biases, anxiety, and development: Toward or away from threats or rewards? Depression & Anxiety (1091–4269), 29, 282–294. doi: 10.1002/da.20914 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shechner T, Fox NA, Mash JA, Jarcho JM, Chen G, Leibenluft E, … Britton JC (2018). Differences in neural response to extinction recall in young adults with or without history of behavioral inhibition. Development and Psychopathology, 30, 179–189. doi: 10.1017/S0954579417000554 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shechner T, Jarcho JM, Britton JC, Leibenluft E, Pine DS, & Nelson EE (2013). Attention bias of anxious youth during extended exposure of emotional face pairs: An eye-tracking study. Depression and Anxiety, 30, 14–21. doi: 10.1002/da.21986 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shechner T, Jarcho JM, Wong S, Leibenluft E, Pine DS, & Nelson EE (2017). Threats, rewards, and attention deployment in anxious youth and adults: An eye tracking study. Biological Psychology, 122(Suppl. C), 121–129. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2015.10.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sylvester CM, Corbetta M, Raichle ME, Rodebaugh TL, Schlaggar BL, Sheline YI, … Lenze EJ (2012). Functional network dysfunction in anxiety and anxiety disorders. Trends in Neurosciences, 35, 527–535. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2012.04.012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sylvester CM, & Pine DS (2018). The biological bridge between behavioral inhibition and psychopathology In Pérez-Edgar K & Fox NA (Eds.), Behavioral inhibition: Integrating theory, research, and clinical perspectives (pp. 309–335). Cham: Springer International. [Google Scholar]
- Szpunar M, & Young AR (2012). Information processing biases in behaviorally inhibited children: Response to threat and novelty. Child Health and Education, 4, 47–63. [Google Scholar]
- Thai N, Taber-Thomas BC, & Pérez-Edgar KE (2016). Neural correlates of attention biases, behavioral inhibition, and social anxiety in children: An ERP study. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 200–210. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2016.03.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thompson RA (1994). Emotion regulation: A theme in search of definition. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59(2–3), 25–52. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-5834.1994.tb01276.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Todd RM, Cunningham WA, Anderson AK, & Thompson E (2012). Affect-biased attention as emotion regulation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16, 365–372. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.06.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tone EB, Garn CL, & Pine DS (2016). Anxiety regulation: A developmental psychopathology perspective In Cicchetti D (Ed.), Developmental psychopathology (3rd ed., pp. 1–34). New York: Wiley. [Google Scholar]
- Tottenham N, Tanaka JW, Leon AC, McCarry T, Nurse M, Hare TA, … Nelson C (2009). The NimStim set of facial expressions: Judgments from untrained research participants. Psychiatry Research, 168, 242–249. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2008.05.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Troller-Renfree SV, Buzzell GA, Pine DS, Henderson HA, & Fox NA (2019). Consequences of not planning ahead: Reduced proactive control moderates longitudinal relations between behavioral inhibition and anxiety. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. Advance online publication. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2018.06.040 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tsypes A, Owens M, & Gibb BE (2017). Suicidal ideation and attentional biases in children: An eye-tracking study. Journal of Affective Disorders, 222, 133–137. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2017.07.012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vervoort L, Wolters LH, Hogendoorn SM, Prins PJ, de Haan E, Boer F, & Hartman CA (2011). Temperament, attentional processes, and anxiety: Diverging links between adolescents with and without anxiety disorders? Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 40, 144–155. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2011.533412 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wald I, Shechner T, Bitton S, Holoshitz Y, Charney DS, Muller D, … Bar‐Haim Y (2011). Attention bias away from threat during life threatening danger predicts PTSD symptoms at one-year follow-up. Depression and Anxiety, 28, 406–411. doi: 10.1002/da.20808 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- White LK, Degnan KA, Henderson HA, Pérez-Edgar K, Walker OL, Shechner T, … Fox NA (2017). Developmental relations among behavioral inhibition, anxiety, and attention biases to threat and positive information. Child Development, 88, 141–155. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12696 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- White LK, Helfinstein SM, Reeb-Sutherland BC, Degnan KA, & Fox NA (2009). Role of attention in the regulation of fear and anxiety. Developmental Neuroscience, 31, 309–317. doi: 10.1159/000216542 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Woody ML, Rosen D, Allen KB, Price RB, Hutchinson E, Amole MC, & Silk JS (2019). Looking for the negative: Depressive symptoms in adolescent girls are associated with sustained attention to a potentially critical judge during in vivo social evaluation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 179, 90–102. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2018.10.011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yiend J (2010). The effects of emotion on attention: A review of attentional processing of emotional information. Cognition & Emotion, 24, 3–47. doi: 10.1080/02699930903205698 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Yu C, Yurovsky D, & Xu T (2012). Visual data mining: An exploratory approach to analyzing temporal patterns of eye movements. Infancy, 17,33–60. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-7078.2011.00095.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.