Abstract
We investigated how initial conflicts in adolescent romantic relationships escalate into serious forms of conflict including intimate partner violence (IPV). We focused on whether adolescents’ micro-level interaction patterns, i.e., coercion and positive engagement, mediated between conflict and future IPV. The sample consisted of 91 heterosexual couples, aged 13–18 years (M = 16.5, SD = 0.99) from a diverse background (42% Hispanic/Latino, 42% White). Participants completed surveys about conflict at Time 1, and participated in videotaped conflict and jealousy discussions. At Time 2, participants completed surveys about conflict and IPV, and an average daily conflict score was calculated from ecological momentary assessments. Multilevel hazard models revealed that we did not find support for dyadic coercion as a risk process leading to escalations in conflict. However, a higher likelihood of ending dyadic positive behaviors mediated between earlier levels of conflict and a latent construct of female conflict and IPV. Classic coercive dynamics may not apply to adolescent romantic relationships. Instead, not being able to reinforce levels of positivity during conflict predicted conflict and IPV as reported by females. The implications of these findings for understanding coercion in the escalation from conflict to IPV in adolescent romantic relationships are discussed.
Keywords: coercion, adolescent romantic relationships, intimate partner violence, observational studies, multilevel hazard models
Tom Dishion was first exposed to family dynamics as a research assistant to Jerry Patterson. One of his first jobs at the Oregon Social Learning Center was to transcribe audio recordings obtained as observers in the family homes whispered codes describing sequences of behavior between mothers and their preschool child. The studies were designed to understand the relationship between macro events in the mother’s environment (e.g., stressors, social support, and maternal depression) and micro-social coercive interactions. Through these transcriptions he learned firsthand the patterned dynamics of coercive family interactions and how small perturbations in the form of small gentle parenting instructions could change the coercive cycle (Patterson, 1982). Tom Dishion strongly believed that his work should benefit the lives of families in need and these micro-level interactions in families would be the basic building blocks for his work. He developed an evidence-based family-centered intervention, the Family Check-Up. Central to this work is the identification of functional family dynamics through careful observation of family interactions and providing families with feedback to identify strengths and areas in need of change. Insight into interpersonal dynamics helps researchers to understand and improve intervention and implementation efforts, helps practitioners to identify areas of change when working with families, and it helps families to recognize their strengths. Moreover, he extended coercion theory to the functional dynamics in adolescent peer relationships as promoters of substance abuse, antisocial behaviors, and violence (Van Ryzin & Dishion, 2013). In keeping with his spirit, we extend this work to investigate coercive dynamics in adolescent romantic relationships.
Being involved in romantic relationships is a normative experience during adolescence. At least 70% of 17-year-old adolescents have had one romantic relationship, and adolescents report spending more time interacting with romantic partners than with parents or friends (Carver, Joyner, & Udry, 2003). Romantic partners are central in adolescents’ lives and become comparable in importance to parents, siblings, and best friends (Furman, Ho, & Low, 2007). Adolescent romantic relationships provide positive experiences, such as companionship, intimacy, and social support from partners (Connolly, Craig, Goldberg, & Pepler, 1999; Furman, Simon, Shaffer, & Bouchey, 2002; Ha, Overbeek, de Greef, Scholte, & Engels, 2010). Unfortunately, negative experiences in adolescent romantic relationships can be potentially disruptive to social and emotional adjustment, impacting depressive symptoms and anxiety (Davila, Steinberg, Kachadourian, Cobb, & Fincham, 2004; Ha & Kim, 2015; Ha, Dishion, Overbeek, Burk, & Engels, 2014; Joyner & Udry, 2000; La Greca & Harrison, 2005; Monroe, Rohde, Seeley, & Lewinsohn, 1999), as well as substance use and intimate partner violence (IPV; Rhule-Louie & McMahon, 2007; Wekerle & Wolfe, 1999).
IPV starts in adolescence and peaks in late adolescence and early adulthood for both females and males (Johnson, Manning, Giordano, & Longmore, 2015). Experiencing IPV during adolescence may increase the vulnerability of young people to experiencing violence in future romantic relationships (Kuijpers, Van der Knaap, & Lodewijks, 2011). However, there is also significant variability in IPV over time (Johnson, Manning, et al., 2015; Shortt et al., 2012; Timmons Fritz & Smith Slep, 2009), indicating that these negative relationship experiences are open to change. The importance of early intervention and prevention of IPV is increasingly recognized (Langhinrichsen-Rohling & Capaldi, 2012) with an emphasis on promoting a healthy relationship and conflict resolution skills of both partners (Pepler, 2012; Shortt et al., 2012). However, it is important to acknowledge that not all conflict is necessarily detrimental. When handled in a constructive way, it provides opportunities for relationship growth. Thus, how romantic partners manage disagreements, rather than conflict itself, may be more related to the development of IPV. More longitudinal and dyadic research is needed to uncover couple dynamics and to explain how initial minor conflicts escalate into more serious forms of conflict including IPV (Capaldi, Knoble, Short, & Kim, 2012). Therefore, we investigated coercive interactions on a micro-social level during conflict and jealousy discussions within adolescent romantic couples. Specifically, we investigated whether these coercive interactions mediated between earlier levels of conflict and more serious forms of future conflict.
From a social learning perspective, coercion theory describes how parents shape their child’s aggressive behavior during critical moments such as parent-child conflicts (Bandura, 1973; Patterson, 1982; Patterson, Reid, & Dishion, 1992). Coercion in parent-child interactions unfolds on a moment-to-moment level often starting with a non-compliant child. If a parent responds with equally aversive behavior, for example yelling or scolding, the child’s behavior may escalate into a tantrum or meltdown. To prevent the conflict from getting worse, the parent may give in or disengage from their child’s aversive behavior. While parents may experience immediate relief, parental capitulation to a child’s aversive behavior socializes children on how to succeed in getting their way (Snyder & Patterson, 1995), thereby increasing the likelihood of future coercive parent-child interactions. Parent-child coercion during early childhood leads to more serious long-term antisocial behaviors in adolescence and adulthood (Reid, Patterson, & Snyder, 2002). A key concept in coercion theory is that these small sequences of micro-social events shape future reliance on coercive behaviors through the principles of operant and classical conditioning. Consequently, it becomes increasingly difficult to engage in behaviors that are positive and constructive as alternatives to the coercive cycle (Granic & Patterson, 2006).
Although marital relationships are very different from parent-child relationships, coercion theory also applies to the understanding of marital functioning (Birchler, Weiss, & Vincent, 1975). A review by Heyman (2001) showed that distressed marital couples, as assessed across different samples and using different coding systems, display high levels of anger and are more likely to reciprocate hostility. A coercion cycle within adult couples starts with aversive behavior displayed by one partner. If the other partner reciprocates with aversive behavior it might successfully terminate the anger and aversive behavior of the first partner. This may bring a temporary escape from conflict, but it will increase the likelihood of subsequent coercive conflicts over time. Coercive interaction patterns are a predictor of marital discord and divorce (Gottman & Levenson, 1984).
Research investigating coercion in adolescent romantic couples and IPV is scarce. A longitudinal study across adolescence and early adulthood found that adolescents who self-reported to be engaged in negative interactions with a romantic partner in the past year were more likely to engage in future IPV (Novak & Furman, 2016). One of the few observational studies among late adolescent and early adult couples found reciprocity between observed psychological and physical aggression during conflict discussions to be related to self-reported IPV (Capaldi & Crosby, 1997). Thus, there is some evidence that coercion, conceptualized as mutual negativity during conflict interactions, is related to IPV in late adolescent/early adult couples. Interestingly, Capaldi and Crosby (1997) also found that a large portion of the observed aggressive behaviors was coded as playful. Playful aggression was directed to the partner with positive affect and the authors hypothesized upon reviewing the videos that the playful aggressive acts seemed to be part of sexual intimacy or to be attention-seeking behaviors, often initiated by the female partner, and reinforced by the male partner. Importantly, adolescents who engaged in negative aggressive behaviors were also more likely to show these playful forms of aggression. Playful bouts of aggression in these young couples may turn into coercion leading to IPV when conflict becomes more intense over time. These results highlight the possibility that coercion might be more nuanced and complex in late adolescent couples (Foshee, Bauman, Linder, Rice, & Wilcher, 2007). Consideration of both mutual negativity and combined negativity and positivity during conflict might be key in improved understanding of how relatively small conflicts can escalate into more serious forms of IPV.
The question remains as to how coercion dynamics play out in adolescent romantic relationships, in couples who are just starting to date during mid-adolescence. Coercive tactics that are readily apparent in parent-child and marital relationships might be less functional for teenage romantic relationships because of the fragile nature of these relationships. Adolescents are highly motivated to be in romantic relationships as is evidenced in the development of sexual attraction and increased relationship involvement over the course of adolescence. A substantial proportion of adolescents’ positive and negative emotions are attributable to romantic relationships (Larson & Asmussen, 1991; Larsson, Clore, & Wood, 1999) and same day experiences of conflict and feelings of jealousy (Rogers, Ha, Updegraff, & Iida, 2018). At the same time, the foundations of these relationships are fragile as adolescent romantic relationships are not likely to last, with nearly 50% of 15–16 year olds experiencing a breakup in the previous six months (Connolly & McIsaac, 2009), and relationships typically last less than 12 months (Ha, Overbeek, Lichtwarck-Aschoff & Engels, 2013). Adolescents may be particularly vulnerable to the prospect of conflict and breakup in these early romantic relationships. They may be willing to go to any length to avoid potential conflicts or disagreements by denying or minimizing differences of opinion to maintain feelings of connection and closeness (Shulman, Tuval-Mashiach, Levran, & Anbar, 2006). Feelings of closeness, trust, and intimacy are highly valued in romantic relationships with an emphasis on mutual enjoyment and fun (Furman & Wehner, 1997). Therefore, in the context of observed conflict discussions, keeping up mutual positivity in the face of conflict and disagreement as a kind of “positive spiral,” permits avoidance of, or escape from, relationship conflicts in the couple’s effort to maintain feelings of closeness.
Given adolescents’ lack of experience of the navigation and repair of high levels of negativity (Ha et al., 2013), it may be more functional to avoid or mask unpleasant relationship experiences than to run the risk of eliciting negativity in the relationship. Coercion processes in adolescent couples may involve a lack of positive reciprocity as a means of avoiding conflict, rather than reinforcement of aversive behaviors as found in marital and in parent-child relationships. To this extent, the unique context of adolescent romantic relationships may suggest a need for an extension of coercion theory.
The current study
In line with coercion theory, dyadic coercion was defined as one partner being negative and the other partner being negative or neutral (the red areas of interest in Figure 1). Based on Capaldi and Crosby’s (1997) findings, we added the negative-positive state as part of coercion, indicating that one partner is negative while the other partner is positive. Dyadic positivity states represented one partner’s positive behaviors, while the other partner is positive or neutral (the green areas of interest in Figure 1). The remaining state is dyadic neutral in which both partners are merely talking about something neutral without showing any negative or positive affect. We investigated whether coercion, dyadic positivity, and neutral interaction patterns during conflict discussions with romantic partners mediated between earlier levels of conflict and more serious forms of future conflict including intimate partner violence.
We used multivariate multilevel survival analyses (MMSA; Stoolmiller & Snyder, 2013) to investigate this research question. Survival analyses originated from the medical field in which death as an event was the outcome of interest. Since then, survival analyses have been widely developed and applied in psychology research (Singer & Willett, 1993). Recently, MMSA was developed as an extension to be applied to observational data coding behavior and emotions in real time (Stoolmiller & Snyder, 2006). This analytic technique allows for the estimation of Hazard Rates (HR) for dyadic states, such as coercion and dyadic positivity, and in turn, estimation of whether the HR of coercion and mutual positivity predicts IPV. MMSA controls for the timing and the censoring of events, which is when the observation ends before some of the events occurred for the participants. Thus, MMSA enables the investigation of the likelihood of a recurring dyadic state, such as coercion and dyadic positivity.
In accordance with coercion theory, we specifically investigated whether a lower likelihood of terminating coercion (a lower HR of dyadic coercion), defined as dyadic negative affect and negative-positive affective states, during adolescent couple conflict interactions would, over time, lead to escalation of conflict into IPV. Additionally, we investigated whether a higher likelihood of terminating positive affect (a higher HR of dyadic positivity) during adolescent couple conflict interactions predicted escalations in conflict.
Method
Participants
The participants were 99 adolescent couples. Because the main analysis required distinguishable dyads based on gender (Kenny, Kashy, & Cook, 2006), we excluded eight couples who identified as homosexual (1 male and 7 female couples). The final analytic sample consisted of 91 heterosexual couples, aged 13–18 years (M = 16.5 years, SD = 0.99; see Ha et al., 2016). The ethnic and racial composition of the final sample was 42% Hispanic/Latino, 42% White, and 16% African-American, Asian American, Native American, or “Other.” Concerning relationship duration, 37% of the participants reported being in a relationship for less than 6 months, 31% between 6 and 12 months, and 32% for longer than a year. Participants came from households with a relatively low socioeconomic status: 44% of participants’ fathers and 36% of their mothers had, at most, a high school diploma. Moreover, 45% of the participants received free or reduced cost lunches at school, and 54% of the participants reported having not enough, or just enough, money to get by.
Adolescents were recruited through (a) announcements in two high schools in a metropolitan area in the Southwestern US (n = 19), (b) targeted Facebook ads (n = 70), and (c) in-person contact at a local shopping mall (n = 2). Chi-square tests revealed differences in ethnicity and SES based on recruitment. Those recruited through schools were more likely to receive free or reduced cost lunches, χ2 (4, N = 166) = 19.773, p < 0.01. Furthermore, the majority of participants recruited through schools were Hispanic/Latino (74%), 8% White, and 18% were African-American, Native American, or other ethnicity, whereas the majority of those recruited through Facebook were White (54%), 34% Hispanic/Latino, 12% were either Black, Asian, Native American, Pacific Islander, or other ethnicity, χ2 (12, N = 179) = 39.749, p < 0.01.
Procedure
After obtaining parental consent and adolescent assent, we contacted and informed romantic partners and their parents about the study goals. Couples were invited to participate in a longitudinal investigation of adolescents’ romantic relationships.
Baseline assessment
Each participant completed online surveys about their relationships with their partner, peers, parents and several behavioral and emotional adjustment indices.
Laboratory assessment
Couples participated in a three-hour laboratory session in which they completed additional surveys and a revised Chatroom Interact Task (Silk et al., 2014), during which electroencephalography (EEG) was recorded (this was not the focus of the current research; see Kuo, Ha, Ebbert, Tucker, & Dishion, 2017). Following this experiment, couples participated in three assigned videotaped discussion tasks, each lasting 5 minutes. The first discussion was a warm-up task in which couples planned a party. Next, participants discussed a conflict topic that was highly rated by either the male or the female participant. Specifically, participants independently rated the extent to which 18 problems caused conflicts in their relationships over the past 30 days on a 10-point scale (e.g., not liking your partner’s attitudes or behaviors, or not liking some of your partner’s friends). The highest rated topic was selected via a coin toss to randomize which partner’s topic (male or female nominated) would be discussed. Finally, they discussed how jealousy affected their relationship.
Ecological momentary assessment
Couples participated in twice-weekly ecological momentary assessments (EMAs; Rogers et al., 2018). Couples received instructions for a texting protocol at the end of the laboratory session. The importance and meaning of confidentiality was discussed and couples were instructed to complete the EMAs individually and they were informed that the answers were private. On the first Sunday following the lab session, participants received a link to a short online survey to complete for the next 12 weeks on Wednesday and Sunday evenings, resulting in 24 surveys. Participants received EMAs every Sunday and Wednesday evening between 7:00 p.m. and 7:30 p.m. for 12 weeks. At 9:00 p.m., a reminder text was sent to adolescents who had not yet completed the EMA. The EMAs took approximately 5 to 10 minutes to complete. Research assistants monitored the progress of the EMAs and phoned adolescents who had missed two consecutive assessments to remind them to complete the survey. Week-to-week completion of EMAs ranged from 60% to 83%; on average, the adolescents completed 17 of 24 (71%) of their assessments.
Follow-up assessment
Approximately six months after the baseline assessment, all couples were contacted and they completed a similar online survey as the baseline survey. Please note that all participants were invited to participate again whether or not they were still together with their partner. A total of 143 adolescents (72.2%) participated in the follow-up assessment.
Participants each received $50 for the baseline survey and the laboratory session. Participants were paid $1.77 for every EMA for a potential total of $42, with a $5 bonus at the halfway point if they had completed at least half of their EMAs. Participants each received $30 for completing the follow-up survey. The Institutional Review Board of Arizona State University approved the project activities.
Measures
Conflict.
The conflict subscale of the Network of Relationships Inventory was used to measure perceptions of conflict in close relationships at T1 and T2 (NRI; Furman & Buhrmester, 1985). The conflict subscale consisted of three questions “How much do you and this person get upset with or mad at each other?”, “How much do you and this person disagree and quarrel?”, and “How much do you and this person argue with each other?” Participants were asked to rate how much each feature occurred with their romantic partner using a 5-point Likert scale. Response options ranged from 1 (none) to 5 (very much). Participants reported conflict at baseline and follow-up. Items were averaged and Cronbach’s α across all items of the scale was .92 for females and .93 for males at T1 and .92 for females and .91 for males at T2.
Intimate Partner Violence.
Intimate partner violence was measured with the Conflict in Adolescent Dating Relationships Inventory (CADRI; Wolfe et al., 2001). We adjusted the original 25-item measure to a 17-item measure by including the full emotional and verbal abuse subscale, and the short versions of the physical abuse subscale, threatening behavior subscale, and sexual abuse subscale (Fernández-González, Wekerle, & Goldstein, 2012). Example items included “I spoke to him/her in a hostile or mean tone of voice,” “I pushed, shoved, or shook him/her,” “I threatened to hit him/her or throw something at him/her,” and, “I kissed her when he/she didn’t want me to.” Participants were asked to rate their own behaviors on a 4-point Likert scale from 0 (never) to 3 (always). Items were averaged to yield a global score of IPV. Cronbach’s α across all items of the scale was .86 for females and .75 for males at T2.
Daily Conflict.
Adolescents reported at each ecological momentary assessment the degree of conflict experienced that day. They responded on a 7-point scale (1 = none, 7 = a lot) to the question, “Today, how much conflict or tension was there between you and your partner?” (Rogers et al., 2018). An average score across all measurements was used in the analyses. Covariates. Age. Adolescents reported their current age in years. Same versus mixed ethnicity status. A dummy code was created for whether a couple was same or mixed ethnicity. Sixty percent of the couples were in a same ethnicity relationship (n = 54). Relationship Length. Participants were asked to indicate the length of their romantic relationship on a scale from 1 (less than a week) to 21 (3 years or more). The average relationship length was 13.05 (SD = 5.14), which translates to around 9 months. Breakup. Breakup was assessed at T2 by asking participants whether they were still with the same partner. Around 25% (n = 23) of couples had broken up at T2.
Observational coding
We coded interactions in real time using Mangold INTERACT software (Mangold, 2017). The Relationship Affect Coding System (Peterson, Winter, Jabson, & Dishion, 2008) was used to capture affect, nonverbal, and verbal behaviors in couples. Each participant was coded separately with three streams of continuous data: affect, verbal, and physical codes. These three streams of data were then collapsed into one continuous stream per individual of behaviors based on a hierarchical system of negative engagement, positive engagement, and lastly, neutral engagement. For example, if a partner shows negative affect (e.g., anger), while simultaneously showing a positive physical behavior (e.g., holding hands), the continuous stream would be classified as negative engagement. This hierarchical system is based on previous work that showed that negative behaviors have more impact on social interactions than positive behaviors (Gottman, 1994). Negative engagement included the following codes, (a) verbal codes included negative talk, resist, stonewalling, and negative directives, (b) physical code was negative physical touch, (c) affective codes, included anger, disgust, and distress. Positive engagement included the following codes, (a) verbal codes included positive talk, active listening, appease, positive structuring, (b) physical code was positive physical touch, (c) affective codes, positive affect such as smiling and laughter. Neutral engagement included the following codes, (a) verbal codes including, no verbal talk, whispering, (b) physical codes included neutral physical contact, (c) affective codes included neutral affect. One master coder trained four coders and weekly coder meetings were held to prevent coder drift. Random samples of 23% of the data were used for inter-rater agreement, which remained on average 75.74% agreement (SD = 9.56), which is acceptable.
State Space grids
State Space Grids were used to visually represent adolescents’ affective behaviors (Granic & Hollenstein, 2003). Adolescents’ affective behavioral responses were plotted in real time against their partners’ affective responses on a quasi-ordinal scale, negative, positive, and neutral, with males’ behaviors depicted on the x-axis and females’ behaviors on the y-axis (Figure 1). Areas of interests are highlighted, dyadic coercion (DC), dyadic positive (DP), and dyadic neutral (DN). An example sequence of dyadic states is plotted within the State Space Grid. A couple started their interaction within the dyadic neutral state, moved into dyadic coercion, and then into dyadic positive.
This hypothetical interaction sequence is also represented in Table 1, which shows the initial data format of the male and female states separately. These data are reformatted into a dyadic data structure for the MMSA analyses (Stoolmiller & Snyder, 2013). Table 2 shows the dyadic data structure based on the combination of individual states in Figure 1. It is now possible to calculate the time (seconds) that it takes for the dyad to end a certain state by subtracting the session time of a certain event from the session time of the next event. For example, it took the dyad 15.00 – 8.00 = 7.00 seconds to end the DP state in the hypothetical example (Table 2). The two conflict discussions were combined to increase observational time to improve the power of the current survival analyses (Stoolmiller & Snyder, 2006).
Table 1.
Episode | Session Time (seconds) | Male’s State | Female’s State |
---|---|---|---|
1 | 0 | NEU | NEU |
2 | 3.20 | NEG | NEU |
3 | 4.60 | NEG | POS |
4 | 8.00 | POS | POS |
5 | 11.32 | NEG | POS |
6 | 15.00 | NEU | NEU |
Note. NEG = negative affect, POS = positive affect, NEU = neutral.
Table 2.
Episode | Session time | Termination time | Event | Dyadic state |
---|---|---|---|---|
1 | 0 | 3.20 | 1 | DN |
2 | 3.20 | 4.80 | 1 | DC |
3 | 8.00 | 7.00 | 1 | DP |
4 | 15.00 | 5.00 | 0 | DN |
Note. DC = Dyadic Coercion, DP = Dyadic Positive, DN = Dyadic Neutral.
Strategy of Analyses
Multivariate Multilevel Survival Analysis (MMSA; Stoolmiller & Snyder, 2013) was employed to answer the current research questions. Survival analysis is a specific type of analysis that investigates the prediction of time-to-event. It considers simultaneously the time variable (when the event happened) and the event occurrence variable (whether the event happened at a specific time or not; Singer & Willett, 2003). The hazard rate is a function of time and event occurrence and indicates the instantaneous rate of an event occurring at a specific time conditioned on the absence of the event up to the specific time point (Allison, 2010). In survival analysis, because of the simplicity of not requiring a specification for the baseline hazard function, the Cox model is a common choice for research questions involving the relationship between a hazard rate and a set of predictors (Cox, 1972). It should be noted that there is also an option to specify the baseline hazard function for the Cox model if that is of particular interest (Singer & Willett, 2003).
The “multilevel” part of the MMSA model takes into account that the event (time to end a dyadic state) is a repeated event and multilevel Cox models deal with the dependency of the repeated events by adding an extra random effect term to the Cox model. The “multivariate” part of the MMSA model is used to study the relationship between the multiple hazard rates of interest (Stoolmiller & Snyder, 2013). In this study, the dyadic states were defined as separate, exclusive states and therefore, the models were separately fitted for the DC, DP, and DN states.
All analyses were conducted in Mplus 8.1 (Muthén & Muthén, 1998–2017). Couple ID was the clustering variable in the multilevel analysis. In order to define the hazard rates at the second level (between-level in Mplus), a between-level latent variable was created with the single survival variable as an indicator and restricting the factor loading to 1.0 and the measurement error variance to 0 (zero) (Stoolmiller & Snyder, 2013). Then, a mediational model was fitted (Figure 2) with male and female conflict at T1 scores predicting the latent hazard rate, and in turn, the latent hazard rate predicted latent variables of male and female IPV at T2. We controlled for several covariates, male and female age, length of relationship, a binary indicator indicating whether couples were of the same ethnicity, and a breakup indicator, which was only controlled for at T2. The latent IPV construct at T2 was measured by three manifest variables, conflict, average daily conflict, and IPV at T2. The model coefficients should be interpreted with care noting that the latent hazard rate variable was in the natural log metric, which is similar to a Cox model.
Results
Descriptive statistics
Table 3 shows the descriptive statistics of the sample. The average duration time in Dyadic Coercion (DC), Dyadic Positive (DP), and Dyadic Neutral (DN) were respectively M = 12.39 (SD = 31.34), M = 60.04 (SD = 48.5), and M = 2.21 (SD = 1.65) seconds. The average time in DP and DC were significantly correlated with female conflict at T1 (respectively, r = −.30 and r = .32), indicating that more time spend in DP related to less conflict at T1 as reported by females, while more time spend in DC related to more conflict at T1 as reported by females. Only the average time in DP was significantly correlated with male conflict at T1 (r = −.29), indicating that more time spent in DP related to less conflict at T1. The average time in DP was significantly correlated with female conflict at T2 (r = −.26) and female IPV at T2 (r = −.25), indicating that more time in DP was related to less female conflict and IPV at T2. The average time in DC was significantly correlated with female IPV at T2 (r = .29), more time spent in DC was related higher levels of female IPV at T2. None of the male conflict-related variables at T2 were significantly correlated with any of the duration time variables. In addition, none of the conflict-related variables at T1 and T2 were related to the duration in DN state.
Table 3.
1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 | 9 | 10 | 11 | 12 | 13 | 14 | 15 | 16 | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. F ageT1 | 1 | .45** | −.06 | .13 | −.55 | .05 | .10 | −.14 | .04 | .00 | −.03 | .10 | .03 | −.03 | .11 | −.05 |
2. M ageT1 | 1 | −.01 | .11 | −.21 | .22* | −.03 | −.18 | −.01 | .05 | −.00 | .18 | −.30** | −.09 | −.06 | −.22* | |
3. Same EthnicityT1 | 1 | .01 | .01 | −.06 | .01 | −.03 | −.12 | −.04 | −.05 | −.08 | −.04 | .08 | .10 | −.10 | ||
4. Relationship lengthT1 | 1 | .03 | 28** | .17 | .01 | .00 | −.18 | .11 | −.01 | −.05 | .12 | −.09 | −.05 | |||
5. BreakupT2 | 1 | −.02 | .19 | .01 | −.21 | −.05 | .02 | −.03 | .37** | 33** | −.17 | −.03 | ||||
6. M conflictT1 | 1 | 53** | 42** | 31** | −.03 | 32** | .22* | .21 | −.01 | −.29** | −.12 | |||||
7. F conflictT1 | 1 | .47** | .51** | −.10 | 55** | 28** | .54** | 32** | −.30** | .06 | ||||||
8. M conflictT2 | 1 | 54** | .27 | 39** | .33* | 39** | .05 | −.15 | −.06 | |||||||
9. F conflictT2 | 1 | .22 | 63** | 35** | 43** | .19 | −.26* | .16 | ||||||||
10. M IPVT2 | 1 | .33* | .28* | .18 | −.00 | −.05 | .09 | |||||||||
11. F IPVT2 | 1 | 46** | 58** | .29 | −.25* | .14 | ||||||||||
12. M daily conflictT2 | 1 | 44** | .00 | −.14 | −.03 | |||||||||||
13. F daily conflictT2 | 1 | .18 | −.18 | .03 | ||||||||||||
14. DC end time | 1 | −.20 | .14 | |||||||||||||
15. DP end time | 1 | .14 | ||||||||||||||
16. DN end time | 1 | |||||||||||||||
Mean | 16.33 | 16.56 | 0.60 | 13.05 | 0.26 | 2.51 | 2.47 | 2.81 | 2.83 | 0.28 | 0.48 | 1.94 | 2.07 | 12.39 | 60.04 | 2.21 |
Standard Deviation | 1.03 | 0.94 | 0.49 | 5.14 | 0.53 | 0.91 | 1.01 | 0.96 | 0.93 | 0.23 | 0.38 | 0.82 | 0.79 | 31.34 | 48.50 | 1.65 |
Note.
p < .05,
p < .01;
DC Dyadic Coercion, DP = Dyadic Positive, DN = Dyadic Neutral; M = Male, F = Female.
Multivariate Multilevel Survival Analysis (MMSA)
Table 4 shows the results of the MMSA. First, in all three models involving DC, DP, and DN ending hazard rates, female conflict at T1 significantly predicted the female latent IPV construct after controlling for the hazard rates and covariates (c4 paths in Figure 2), indicating stability across time. Additionally, the only other c-path that was significant was the female conflict at T1 predicting the male’s latent IPV at T2, after controlling for the DN ending hazard rate and covariates (c3 in path in Figure 2 for the DN model).
Table 4.
Predictor or covariate╲Outcome | Hazard rate | Male IPV T2 | Female IPV T2 |
---|---|---|---|
DC end | |||
Male conflict T1 | a1 = −0.09 | c1 = 0.09 | c2 = 0.01 |
Female conflict T1 | a2 = −0.16 | c3 = 0.23 | c4 = 0.40*** |
Male age T1 | 0.12 | 0.14 | −0.10 |
Female age T1 | −0.01 | −0.03 | 0.04 |
Relationship length | 0.01 | −0.03 | −0.01 |
Breakup | −0.08 | 0.14 | |
Same ethnic group | 0.12 | −0.12 | −0.08 |
DC end hazard rate | b1 = −0.38 | b2 = −0.28 | |
DP end | |||
Male conflict T1 | a1 = 0.19* | c1 = 0.09 | c2 = −0.02 |
Female conflict T1 | a2 = 0.23* | c3 = 0.23 | c4 = 0.37*** |
Male T1 | −0.05 | 0.12 | −0.15 |
Female age T1 | −0.10 | −0.03 | 0.08 |
Relationship length | −0.01 | −0.02 | −0.01 |
Breakup | −0.10 | 0.08 | |
Same ethnic group | −0.22 | −0.03 | 0.01 |
DP end hazard rate | b1 = 0.35 | b2 = 0.40† | |
DN end | |||
Male conflict T1 | a1 = 0.17 | c1 = 0.15 | c2 = 0.07 |
Female conflict T1 | a2 = −0.10 | c3 = 0.34* | c4 = 0.46*** |
Male age at T1 | 0.11 | 0.10 | −0.14 |
Female age at T1 | −0.13 | −0.08 | 0.03 |
Relationship length | 0.00 | −0.02 | −0.01 |
Breakup | −0.09 | 0.13 | |
Same ethnic group | 0.41 | −0.14 | −0.05 |
DN end hazard rate | b1 = 0.07 | b2 = −0.06 |
p < .05;
p < .001;
p < .10.
DC = Dyadic Coercion; DP = Dyadic Positive; DN = Dyadic Neutral
We did not find support for DC mediating between earlier conflict and IPV at T2 for males and females. Results regarding DP showed that male conflict at T1 predicted a significantly higher DP ending hazard rate during conflict and jealousy observations, a1 = 0.19, p < .05. After controlling for covariates, on average a 0.19 increase in the log hazard rate in terminating the DP state was observed as a one-unit increase in male conflict at T1. This can be translated to a exp(0.19) = 1.21 times higher hazard rate or equivalently, a (exp(0.19) – 1)*100 = 21% increase in the hazard rate as a one-unit increase in male conflict at T1. Similarly, female conflict at T1 predicted termination of the DP hazard rate, a2 = 0.23, p < .05. After controlling for other covariates in the model, on average a 0.23 increase in the log hazard rate, or equivalently, a exp(0.23) = 1.26 times higher hazard rate, or a (exp(0.23)-1)*100 = 26% increase in the hazard rate in terminating the DP state was observed as a one-unit increase in the T1 female’s conflict score. In turn, the log DP hazard rate predicted a marginally significant increase in the female’s latent IPV construct at T2, after controlling for conflict at T1 and covariates, b2 = 0.40, p = .07. This means as a one-unit increase in the log DP ending hazard rate, a 0.4 unit increase in the T2 female’s latent IPV was observed. All other predictions in the MMSA model were not significant.
Discussion
We investigated how initial conflicts in adolescent romantic couples could escalate into more serious forms of conflict including IPV. We focused on adolescents’ micro-level interaction patterns during conflict as possible underlying processes. Unexpectedly, we did not find support for dyadic coercion as a risk process leading to escalations in conflict. A lower likelihood to end mutually-reinforcing aversive behaviors did not predict higher levels of IPV. However, a higher likelihood to end dyadic positive behaviors mediated between earlier levels of conflict and a latent construct of conflict and IPV six months later. Specifically, males and females reported that conflict predicted higher hazard rates of dyadic positive behaviors, indicating that couples who experienced higher levels of conflict in their relationships were more likely to end mutually-positive behaviors during conflict discussions. In turn, a higher hazard rate in dyadic positive behaviors predicted higher levels of a latent construct of conflict and IPV, although this was marginally significant and was only found for female IPV. Lastly, no results for dyadic neutral were found. These results were found while controlling for age, relationship duration, ethnicity, and breakup.
Conflict is a key socialization context because it comprises a social exchange in which two people are forced to come together and work things out (Dishion, Nelson, Winter, & Bullock, 2004; Forgatch & Patterson, 2010; Heyman, 2001). When not handled well, as in the case of coercive exchanges, frustration accumulates. In turn, this can evoke negative interaction patterns that potentially can lead to violence in relationships. In contrast to previous research on coercion in parent-child and in marital relationships, dyadic coercion was not a risk process for the escalation of conflict in adolescent romantic relationships. Instead, a lack of reinforcement of positive behaviors during conflict was more predictive of escalation in conflict. Classic coercive dynamics may not apply to adolescent romantic relationships for the development of IPV. Being unable to reinforce levels of positivity during conflict predicted conflict and IPV as reported by females. These results underscore the unique context of adolescent romantic relationships, the high level of motivation involved in being in these relationships, and the need to maintain closeness and intimacy.
We included the possibility that negative behaviors were observed in the context of playful aggression (Capaldi & Crosby, 1997), by including the dyadic state of dyadic positive-negative behaviors in coercion. Therefore, our dyadic positive state is expected to reflect only positively-engaging behaviors during conflict. Within marital relationships, positive affect during conflict, such as humor and affection, are constructive conflict resolution styles, which predict relationship satisfaction and relate to less conflict (Gottman, Coan, Swanson, and Carrere, 1998). From an emotion regulation perspective, positive emotions during conflict indicate effective regulation to resolve relationship problems as experiencing negative emotions during conflictual interaction increases the likelihood of IPV to resolve relationship problems (Crane & Testa, 2014; Finkel, 2007; McNulty & Hellmuth, 2008; Watkins, DiLillo, Hoffman, & Templin, 2015). In line with results found among committed couples, adolescents who constructively engaged and regulated their emotions during conflict were less likely to develop IPV in their romantic relationships. Adolescents who were less able to mutually reinforce positivity during an observed conflict interaction may experience difficulties when conflicts play out in real life. Difficulties regulating emotions may lead to conflicts getting out of control resulting in possible instances of IPV.
However, another possible explanation is that adolescents were not necessarily constructively engaging in conflict but were downplaying or avoiding conflict in their interactions. Upon inspection of the video recordings, first statements of a conflict discussion often involved one of the partners directly denying or downplaying the conflict topic. For example, “I do not think we have issues at all, but I had to choose something” or “we already resolved this a long time ago.” Partners often positively reinforced this initial avoidant response, which initiated a cycle of positive engagement during the conflict discussion. Alternatively, these couples may have less conflict and therefore less intense conflict interactions. Thus, adolescents’ positive behaviors might actually reflect not having relationship issues and not constitute conflict avoidance. With the current real time coding system, we were not able to distinguish between constructively engaging in conflict, avoiding conflict, or just not having much conflict. The current coding system taps into a process level rather than a content level of conflict discussions. Future research could develop coding systems that would include both content (what partners say and do in addition to the affective tone of their behaviors) and process level codes to identify specific mechanisms that are either protective or potentially leading to future IPV. Based on the current findings it would be important to code for potential conflict engagement or conflict denial, which then could be positively or negatively reinforced by the partner. This is similar to Dishion’s deviancy training in adolescent friendships, in which deviant talk is reinforced by positivity of the friend creating deviant values and norms in these friendships. Deviancy training predicted long-term antisocial behaviors and substance abuse, long after these original friendships had dissolved and new ones had formed (Dishion, Capaldi, Spracklen, & Li, 1995; Dishion, Eddy, Haas, Li, & Spracklen, 1997; Dishion, Spracklen, Andrews, & Patterson, 1996).
We found that variation in the likelihood of ending positivity instead of negativity predicted IPV. This highlights the fact that influence and change within these early romantic relationships are a function of positive experiences during conflict. Interestingly, the positive interaction patterns, operationalized as a lower likelihood to end positivity during conflict, were found to increase depressive symptoms two years later in a sample of Dutch dating adolescents (Ha & Kim, 2016; Ha et al., 2014). Upregulation of positive affect was hypothesized to prohibit effective conflict resolution resulting in more emotional problems over time, but the current study implies that this pattern is protective of the development of IPV. Investigating important relationship and environmental moderators to better understand when and how these positive dynamics are protective or harmful is important for future research. Moreover, it will be crucial to investigate how the lack of reinforcing positivity during conflict that was observed during 10 minutes of videotaped interactions in this study relates to dealing with conflict and stress in romantic relationships over a longer period. A previous ecological momentary study including the same sample as the current study showed that conflict and feelings of jealousy and doubt increased negative affect on the same day, indicating that conflict has important day-to-day effects on adolescents’ mood (Rogers et al., 2018). Linking the current observational findings to conflict engagement in daily life may reveal when the lower levels of reinforcing positivity during observed conflict turn into more classic coercive processes in daily life because of the contextual changes of romantic relationships, such as increased tensions and strains in adolescents’ lives.
We investigated adolescent couples’ interactions, which is in line with Capaldi and Kim’s (2007) dynamic developmental systems theory. This theory focuses uniquely on the dyadic nature of IPV to better understand the emergence and course of IPV. When considering both partners’ behaviors during conflict interactions, we found that a higher likelihood of ending dyadic positive states marginally predicted levels of female IPV. More research is needed to investigate which interaction patterns predict male IPV. The current results are in line with recent studies indicating higher engagement of female IPV during adolescence (Capaldi et al., 2012; Johnson, Giordano, Manning, & Longmore, 2015). It is of critical importance to extend this research to the study of multiple relationships over time to better understand how the long-term, cross-relationship stability and change in coercion and positive reinforcement of positivity predict later IPV. Recent studies showed low stability of IPV across different relationship partners, which suggests that each new romantic relationship is a learning opportunity and a new experience (Johnson, Manning, et al., 2015; Shortt et al., 2012). This provides unique opportunities for early prevention programs to promote healthy relationships and partner choice among adolescents. It is important to keep in mind that IPV emerges in the context of positive relationship experiences, such as intimacy and a high motivation to be in romantic relationships. Problematic interaction patterns might be particularly nuanced when they encompass the likelihood of ending positivity during conflict. Combined with adolescents’ limited relationship experiences, it might be particularly difficult for adolescents to understand and acknowledge when relationships dynamics are unhealthy. Therefore, the support of parents and important adult figures will be key in the promotion of healthy adolescent romantic relationships.
Limitations
Although this study used micro-level observations to gain insight into how disagreement escalates into more serious forms of conflict including IPV in a sample of ethnically diverse adolescent romantic relationships, some limitations should be addressed in future research. First, the current results should be replicated in an independent sample of adolescent romantic relationships. In addition, the sample size is relatively small, especially for the analysis of both coercion and dyadic positivity in determining their relative importance in predicting IPV. Moreover, even though breakup was controlled for in the current analyses, it was not possible to test whether results differ for couples who break up once, or have unstable relationships in which they break up and get back together multiple times. Future studies could investigate whether the lack of reinforcing dyadic positivity might contribute to a series of breakups followed by getting back together, a relationship pattern that might be vulnerable to the development of IPV. We only assessed adolescents’ IPV perpetration and not their victimization. Although it is increasingly recognized that IPV perpetration and victimization often co-occur within relationships (Capaldi & Kim, 2007; Capaldi, Shortt, Tiberio, & Low, 2019), it will be important to investigate if results change depending on levels of perpetration and victimization. Due to the small number of same sex couples, we were not able to investigate if similar patterns would be relevant for IPV in same sex couples (Halpern, Young, Waller, Martin, & Kupper, 2004). Finally, the current study does not consider the broader context of adolescent relationships, such as friendships, relationships with parents, and previous experiences in romantic relationships. From a prevention perspective there is a need to better understand the etiology of IPV in adolescent romantic relationships.
We investigated how initial conflicts in adolescent romantic couples escalate into more serious forms of conflict, including IPV. We showed that classic coercive dynamics in adolescent romantic relationships were not predictive of IPV. Instead, a higher likelihood to end dyadic positive behaviors mediated between earlier levels of conflict and a latent construct of conflict and IPV six months later. These results underscore the unique context of adolescent romantic relationships. Linking these microsocial partner interactions to experiences in peer and family relationships as well as macro level developmental outcomes will be essential in understanding when and how adolescent romantic relationships present a risk for IPV. Each new romantic relationship is a potential turning point in adolescent development creating potentially powerful opportunities for early prevention programs to promote healthy relationships and partner choice among adolescents.
Acknowledgements:
Support for this research was provided to Ha by the T. Denny Sanford School of Social and Family Dynamics as part of the Lives of Teens Enterprise, and from the REACH Institute at Arizona State University. Ha was supported during preparation of this manuscript by a grant from the National Institute on Drug Abuse (DA07031) and a grant from the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse & Alcoholism (AA022071). The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily reflect the official views of the National Institute on Drug Abuse or the National Institute on Alcoholism and Alcohol Abuse. We thank Jenene Peterson for assisting with the coding of the observations, Jasmine Sutton for her editorial work, and Dr. Marion Forgatch for her helpful feedback. We greatly appreciate the efforts of the principals and staff in facilitating data collection.
References
- Allison PD. (2010). Survival analysis using SAS: a practical guide. Cary, NC, USA: SAS Institute. [Google Scholar]
- Bandura A. (1973). Aggression: A social learning analysis. Oxford, England: Prentice-Hall. [Google Scholar]
- Birchler GR, Weiss RL, & Vincent JP. (1975). Multimethod analysis of social reinforcement exchange between maritally distressed and nondistressed spouse and stranger dyads. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 31, 349–360. 10.1037/h0076280 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Capaldi DM, & Crosby L. (1997). Observed and reported psychological and physical aggression in young, at‐risk couples. Social Development, 6, 184–206. 10.1111/j.1467-9507.1997.tb00101.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Capaldi DM, & Kim HK. (2007). Typological approaches to violence in couples: A critique and alternative conceptual approach. Clinical Psychology Review, 27, 253–265. 10.1016/j.cpr.2006.09.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Capaldi DM, Knoble NB, Shortt JW, & Kim HK. (2012). A systematic review of risk factors for intimate partner violence. Partner Abuse, 3, 231–280. doi: 10.1891/1946–6560.3.2.231 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Capaldi DM, Shortt JW, Tiberio SS, & Low S. (2019). Violence begets violence: Addressing the dual nature of partner violence in adolescent and young adult relationships In Wolfe DA & Temple JR. (Eds.), Adolescent Dating Violence (pp. 341–364). 10.1016/B978-0-12-811797-2.00014-1 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Carver K, Joyner K, & Udry JR. (2003). National estimates of adolescent romantic relationships In Florsheim P. (Ed.), Adolescent romantic relations and sexual behavior: Theory, research, and practical implications (pp. 23–56). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates [Google Scholar]
- Connolly J, Craig W, Goldberg A, & Pepler D. (1999). Conceptions of cross-sex friendships and romantic relationships in early adolescence. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 28, 481–494. 10.1023/A:1021669024820 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Connolly J, & McIsaac C. (2009). Adolescents’ explanations for romantic dissolutions: A developmental perspective. Journal of Adolescence, 32, 1209–1223. 10.1016/j.adolescence.2009.01.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cox DR. (1972). Regression models and life tables (with discussion). Journal of the Royal Statistical Society, Series B, 34, 187–220. 10.1111/j.2517-6161.1972.tb00899.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Crane CA, & Testa M. (2014). Daily associations among anger experience and intimate partner aggression within aggressive and nonaggressive community couples. Emotion, 14, 985–994. 10.1037/a0036884 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davila J, Steinberg SJ, Kachadourian L, Cobb R, & Fincham F. (2004). Romantic involvement and depressive symptoms in early and late adolescence: The role of a preoccupied relational style. Personal Relationships, 11, 161–178. 10.1111/j.1475-6811.2004.00076.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Dishion TJ, Capaldi D, Spracklen KM, & Li F. (1995). Peer ecology of male adolescent drug use. Development and Psychopathology, 7, 803–824. 10.1017/S0954579400006854 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Dishion TJ, Eddy JM, Haas E, Li F, & Spracklen K. (1997). Friendships and violent behavior during adolescence. Social Development, 6, 207–223. 10.1111/j.1467-9507.1997.tb00102.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Dishion TJ, Nelson SE, Winter CE, & Bullock BM. (2004). Adolescent friendship as a dynamic system: Entropy and deviance in the etiology and course of male antisocial behavior. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 32, 651–663. 10.1023/B:JACP.0000047213.31812.21 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dishion TJ, Spracklen KM, Andrews DW, & Patterson GR. (1996). Deviancy training in male adolescent friendships. Behavior Therapy, 27, 373–390. 10.1016/S0005-7894(96)80023-2 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Finkel EJ. (2007). Impelling and Inhibiting Forces in the Perpetration of Intimate Partner Violence. Review of General Psychology, 11, 193–207. 10.1037/1089-2680.11.2.193 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Fernández-González L, Wekerle C, & Goldstein AL. (2012). Measuring adolescent dating violence: Development of conflict in adolescent dating relationships inventory short form. Advances in Mental Health, 11, 35–54. 10.5172/jamh.2012.11.1.35 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Forgatch MS, & Patterson GR. (2010). Parent Management Training—Oregon Model: An intervention for antisocial behavior in children and adolescents In Weisz JR & Kazdin AE. (Eds.), Evidence-based psychotherapies for children and adolescents (pp. 159–177). New York, NY, US: The Guilford Press. [Google Scholar]
- Foshee VA, Bauman KE, Linder F, Rice J, & Wilcher R. (2007). Typologies of adolescent dating violence: Identifying typologies of adolescent dating violence perpetration. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 22, 498–519. 10.1177/0886260506298829 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Furman W, & Buhrmester D. (1985). Children’s perceptions of the personal relationships in their social networks. Developmental Psychology, 21, 1016–1024. 10.1037/0012-1649.21.6.1016 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Furman W, Ho MJ, & Low SM. (2007). The rocky road of adolescent romantic experience: Dating and adjustment In Engels R, Kerr M, & Stattin H. (Eds.), Friends, lovers and groups: Key relationships in adolescence (pp. 61–80). New York: Wiley. [Google Scholar]
- Furman W, Simon VA, Shaffer L, & Bouchey HA. (2002). Adolescents’ working models and styles for relationships with parents, friends, and romantic partners. Child Development, 73, 241–255. 10.1111/1467-8624.00403 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Furman W, & Wehner EA. (1997). Adolescent romantic relationships: A developmental perspective In Shulman S & Collins WA. (Eds.), New directions for child development: Adolescent romantic relationships (pp. 21–36). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gottman JM. (1994). What predicts divorce? Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum [Google Scholar]
- Gottman JM, Coan J, Carrere S, & Swanson C. (1998). Predicting marital happiness and stability from newlywed interactions. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 5–22. DOI: 10.2307/353438 [DOI]
- Gottman JM, & Levenson RW. (1984). Why marriages fail: Affective and physiological patterns in marital interaction In Masters JC & Yarkin-Levin K. (Eds.), Boundary areas in social and developmental psychology (pp. 67–106). New York: Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
- Granic I, & Patterson GR. (2006). Toward a comprehensive model of antisocial development: A dynamic systems approach. Psychological Review, 113, 101–131. 10.1037/0033-295X.113.1.101 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ha T, Dishion TJ, Overbeek G, Burk WJ, & Engels RC. (2014). The blues of adolescent romance: Observed affective interactions in adolescent romantic relationships associated with depressive symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 42, 551–562. 10.1007/s10802-013-9808-y [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ha T, & Kim H. (2016). Upregulation of positive affect as a coercive dynamical process in adolescent romantic relationships in predicting depressive symptoms In Dishion TJ & Snyder J. (Eds.) Oxford Handbook of Coercive dynamics in Close Relationships: Implications for Development, Psychopathology and Intervention Science. New York: Oxford Press. [Google Scholar]
- Ha T, Overbeek G, De Greef M, Scholte RH, & Engels RC. (2010). The importance of relationships with parents and best friends for adolescents’ romantic relationship quality: Differences between indigenous and ethnic Dutch adolescents. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 34, 121–127. 10.1177/0165025409360293 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Ha T, Overbeek G, Lichtwarck-Aschoff A, & Engels RC. (2013). Do conflict resolution and recovery predict the survival of adolescents’ romantic relationships? PloS one, 8(4), e61871 10.1371/journal.pone.0061871 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ha T, Yeung EW, Rogers AA, Poulsen FO, Kornienko O, & Granger DA. (2016). Supportive behaviors in adolescent romantic relationships moderate adrenocortical attunement. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 74, 189–196. 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2016.09.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Halpern CT, Young ML, Waller MW, Martin SL, & Kupper LL. (2004). Prevalence of partner violence in same-sex romantic and sexual relationships in a national sample of adolescents. Journal of Adolescent Health, 35, 124–131. 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2003.09.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heyman RE. (2001). Observation of couple conflicts: Clinical assessment applications, stubborn truths, and shaky foundations. Psychological Assessment, 13, 5–35. 10.1037/1040-3590.13.1.5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson WL, Giordano PC, Manning WD, & Longmore MA. (2015). The age–IPV curve: Changes in the perpetration of intimate partner violence during adolescence and young adulthood. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 44, 708–726. 10.1007/s10964-014-0158-z [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson WL, Manning WD, Giordano PC, & Longmore MA. (2015). Relationship context and intimate partner violence from adolescence to young adulthood. Journal of Adolescent Health, 57, 631–636. 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2015.08.014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Joyner K, & Udry JR. (2000). You don’t bring me anything but down: Adolescent romance and depression. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 41, 369. doi: 10.2307/2676292 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kenny DA, Kashy DA, & Cook WL. (2006). Dyadic data analysis. New York, Guilford Press. [Google Scholar]
- Kuijpers KF, van der Knaap LM, & Lodewijks IA. (2011). Victims’ influence on intimate partner violence revictimization: A systematic review of prospective evidence. Trauma, Violence, & Abuse, 12, 198–219. 10.1177/1524838011416378 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuo CC, Ha T, Ebbert AM, Tucker DM, & Dishion TJ. (2017). Dynamic responses in brain networks to social feedback: a dual EEG acquisition study in adolescent couples. Frontiers in Computational Neuroscience, 11, 46 10.3389/fncom.2017.00046 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- La Greca AM, & Harrison HM. (2005). Adolescent peer relations, friendships, and romantic relationships: Do they predict social anxiety and depression? Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 34, 49–61. 10.1207/s15374424jccp3401_5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Langhinrichsen-Rohling J, & Capaldi DM. (2012). Clearly we’ve only just begun: Developing effective prevention programs for intimate partner violence. Prevention Science, 13, 410–414. 10.1007/s11121-012-0310-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Larson R, & Asmussen L. (1991). Anger, worry, and hurt in early adolescence: An enlarging world of negative emotions In Colten ME & Gore S. (Eds.), Adolescent stress: Causes and consequences (pp. 21–41). New York: Aldine De Gruyter. [Google Scholar]
- Larson RW, Clore GL, & Wood GA. (1999). The emotions of romantic relationships: Do they wreak havoc on adolescents In Furman W, Brown BB, & Feiring C. (Eds.), The development of romantic relationships in adolescence (pp. 19–49). New York: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Mangold (2017): INTERACT User Guide. Mangold International GmbH (Ed.) www.mangold-international.com
- McNulty JK, & Hellmuth JC. (2008). Emotion regulation and intimate partner violence in newlyweds. Journal of Family Psychology, 22, 794–797. 10.1037/a0013516 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Monroe SM, Rohde P, Seeley JR, & Lewinsohn PM. (1999). Life events and depression in adolescence: Relationship loss as a prospective risk factor for first onset of major depressive disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 108, 606 10.1037/0021-843X.108.4.606 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Muthén LK, & Muthén BO. (1998–2017). Mplus User’s Guide. Seventh Edition. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén. [Google Scholar]
- Novak J, & Furman W. (2016). Partner violence during adolescence and young adulthood: Individual and relationship level risk factors. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 45, 1849–1861. 10.1007/s10964-016-0484-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Patterson GR. (1982). Coercive family process (Vol. 3). Eugene, OR: Castalia Publishing Company. [Google Scholar]
- Patterson GR, Reid JB, & Dishion TJ. (1992). Antisocial boys (Vol. 4). Eugene, OR: Castalia Publishing Company. [Google Scholar]
- Pepler D. (2012). The development of dating violence: What doesn’t develop, what does develop, how does it develop, and what can we do about it? Prevention Science, 13, 402–409. 10.1007/s11121-012-0308-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peterson J, Winter C, Jabson J, & Dishion TJ. (2008). Relationship affect coding system. Unpublished coding manual, University of Oregon, Child and Family Center, Eugene, OR. [Google Scholar]
- Reid JB, Patterson GR, & Snyder J. (Eds.). (2002). Antisocial behavior in children and adolescents: A developmental analysis and model for intervention. Washington, DC, US: American Psychological Association. [Google Scholar]
- Rhule-Louie DM, & McMahon RJ. (2007). Problem behavior and romantic relationships: Assortative mating, behavior contagion, and desistance. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 10, 53–100. 10.1007/s10567-006-0016-y [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rogers AA, Ha T, Updegraff KA, & Iida M. (2018). Adolescents’ daily romantic experiences and negative mood: a dyadic, intensive longitudinal study. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 47, 1517–1530. 10.1007/s10964-017-0797-y [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shortt JW, Capaldi DM, Kim HK, Kerr DC, Owen LD, & Feingold A. (2012). Stability of intimate partner violence by men across 12 years in young adulthood: Effects of relationship transitions. Prevention Science, 13, 360–369. 10.1007/s11121-011-0202-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shulman S, Tuval-Mashiach R, Levran E, & Anbar S. (2006). Conflict resolution patterns and longevity of adolescent romantic couples: A 2-year follow-up study. Journal of Adolescence, 29, 575–588. 10.1016/j.adolescence.2005.08.018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Silk JS, Siegle GJ, Lee KH, Nelson EE, Stroud LR, & Dahl RE. (2014). Increased neural response to peer rejection associated with adolescent depression and pubertal development. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9, 1798–1807. 10.1093/scan/nst175 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Singer JD, & Willett JB. (1993). It’s about time: Using discrete-time survival analysis to study duration and the timing of events. Journal of educational statistics, 18, 155–195. 10.3102/10769986018002155 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Singer JD, & Willett JB. (2003). Applied longitudinal data analysis: Modeling change and event occurrence. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Snyder JJ, & Patterson GR. (1995). Individual differences in social aggression: A test of a reinforcement model of socialization in the natural environment. Behavior Therapy, 26, 371–391. 10.1016/S0005-7894(05)80111-X [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Stoolmiller M, & Snyder J. (2006). Modeling heterogeneity in social interaction processes using multilevel survival analysis. Psychological Methods, 11, 164–177. 10.1037/1082-989X.11.2.164 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stoolmiller M, & Snyder J. (2013). Embedding multilevel survival models of dyadic social interaction in structural equation models: Hazard rates as both outcomes and predictors. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 39, 222–232. 10.1093/jpepsy/jst076 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Timmons Fritz PA, & Smith Slep AM. (2009). Stability of physical and psychological adolescent dating aggression across time and partners. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 38, 303–314. 10.1080/15374410902851671 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Ryzin MJ, & Dishion TJ. (2013). From antisocial behavior to violence: A model for the amplifying role of coercive joining in adolescent friendships. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54, 661–669. 10.1111/jcpp.12017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Watkins LE, DiLillo D, Hoffman L, & Templin J. (2015). Do self-control depletion and negative emotion contribute to intimate partner aggression? A lab-based study. Psychology of Violence, 5, 35–45. 10.1037/a0033955 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Wekerle C, & Wolfe DA. (1999). Dating violence in mid-adolescence: Theory, significance, and emerging prevention initiatives. Clinical Psychology Review, 19, 435–456. 10.1016/S0272-7358(98)00091-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wolfe DA, Scott K, Reitzel-Jaffe D, Wekerle C, Grasley C, & Straatman AL. (2001). Development and validation of the conflict in adolescent dating relationships inventory. Psychological Assessment, 13, 277–293. 10.1037/1040-3590.13.2.277 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]