Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2020 Jan 2.
Published in final edited form as: J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2017 Jul 20;24(1):11–21. doi: 10.1017/S1355617717000595

Home Environment as a Predictor of Long-Term Executive Functioning following Early Childhood Traumatic Brain Injury

Christianne Laliberté Durish 1, Keith Owen Yeates 2, Terry Stancin 3, H Gerry Taylor 4, Nicolay C Walz 5, Shari L Wade 6
PMCID: PMC6939301  NIHMSID: NIHMS990617  PMID: 28724466

Abstract

Objectives:

This study examined the relationship of the home environment to long-term executive functioning (EF) following early childhood traumatic brain injury (TBI).

Methods:

Participants (N = 134) were drawn from a larger parent study of 3- to 6-year-old children hospitalized for severe TBI (n = 16), complicated mild/moderate TBI (n = 44), or orthopedic injury (OI; n = 74), recruited prospectively at four tertiary care hospitals in the United States and followed for an average of 6.8 years post-injury. Quality of the home environment, caregiver psychological distress, and general family functioning were assessed shortly after injury (i.e., early home) and again at follow-up (i.e., late home). Participants completed several performance-based measures of EF at follow-up. Hierarchical regression analyses examined the early and late home environment measures as predictors of EF, both as main effects and as moderators of group differences.

Results:

The early and late home environment were inconsistent predictors of long-term EF across groups. Group differences in EF were significant for only the TEA-Ch Walk/Don’t Walk subtest, with poorer performance in the severe TBI group. However, several significant interactions suggested that the home environment moderated group differences in EF, particularly after complicated mild/moderate TBI.

Conclusions:

The home environment is not a consistent predictor of long-term EF in children with early TBI and OI, but may moderate the effects of TBI on EF. The findings suggest that interventions designed to improve the quality of stimulation in children’s home environments might reduce the long-term effects of early childhood TBI on EF.

Keywords: Preschool, Neuropsychology, Attention, Problem solving, Parenting, family

INTRODUCTION

Traumatic brain injury (TBI) is prevalent among children, annually affecting over 500,000 children aged 0 to 14 years in the United States (Faul, Xu, Wald, & Coronado, 2010). The highest incidence rates are seen in infants and preschool-aged children 0–4 years old (Faul et al., 2010). Preschool-aged children are especially vulnerable to negative outcomes following a TBI (Anderson, Catroppa, Morse, Haritou, & Rosenfeld, 2005), possibly because damage to the young brain is more likely to have a negative impact on the development of emerging abilities, as compared to older children, whose abilities are more established (Crowe, Catroppa, Babl, & Anderson, 2012).

On average, children with TBI demonstrate a wide array of negative outcomes, including lower cognitive and academic abilities (Ewing-Cobbs et al., 2006), poorer language skills (Crowe, Anderson, Barton, Babl, & Catroppa, 2014), behavioral problems (Schwartz et al., 2003), and social impairments (Yeates et al., 2004). One specific cognitive domain in which children with TBI often demonstrate deficits is executive functions (EF), which include attention (Bigler et al., 2015; Garcia, Hungerford, & Bagner, 2015; Konigs et al., 2015; Papoutsis, Stargatt, & Catroppa, 2014), decision making (Schmidt et al., 2012), goal setting (Beauchamp et al., 2011), and behavior regulation (Potter et al., 2011). EF refers to higher order cognitive processes involved in goal-directed behavior. Deficits in EF can persist for at least 5–10 years after childhood TBI (Beauchamp et al., 2011; Mangeot, Armstrong, Colvin, Yeates, & Taylor, 2010).

Although differences in EF are clearly apparent at a group level following TBI, individual outcomes are heterogeneous. A variety of factors may help to predict lower scores on EF measures after childhood TBI, including lower verbal intellectual ability, greater injury severity, and younger age at injury (Slomine et al., 2002). Surprisingly, however, little research has examined the role of environmental factors as predictors of long-term EF following early childhood TBI.

In healthy children, various aspects of the home environment, such as parental responsiveness, environmental enrichment, and family companionship, predict EF in middle childhood (Blair et al., 2014; Sarsour et al., 2011). Negative parenting practices, such as harsh punishment and inconsistent discipline, are also related to poorer development of EF, specifically inhibition (Roskam et al., 2014). The relationship between the home environment and children’s EF has also been examined in clinical populations of preschool-aged children with low birth weight (LBW), with findings from one study indicating protective effects of sensitive parenting on EF (Camerota et al., 2015).

Family factors also are known to account for significant variability in the outcomes of early childhood TBI. For instance, parenting style, family functioning, and the quality of the home predict behavioral adjustment and social competence in preschool-aged children with TBI (Wade et al., 2011; Yeates et al., 2010). Moreover, the home environment can be an important moderator of the effects of TBI in young children, attenuating negative outcomes for children from better home environments but exacerbating deficits for those from less advantageous home environments (Yeates et al., 2010).

Only two studies, however, have specifically examined the relationship of the home environment to children’s EF after early TBI. One study found that higher levels of family dysfunction and maladaptive parenting styles (i.e., permissive and authoritarian parenting) predicted deficits in behavioral EF (i.e., parent report on questionnaires regarding children’s everyday EF) following early childhood moderate and severe TBI (Kurowski et al., 2011). In a second study, authoritarian and permissive parenting styles moderated the effect of moderate and severe TBI on children’s behavioral EF (Potter et al., 2011). Specifically, higher levels of authoritarian and permissive parenting predicted EF in children with TBI relative to those with orthopaedic injuries (OIs). Both of these studies, however, relied on parent ratings rather than on performance-based tests to measure EF, assessed the home environment at a single point in time, and followed children for a maximum of 5 years. No studies to date have examined both the early and the late home environment as predictors of performance on performance-based tests of EF more than 5 years post-injury.

The current study examined the relationship between the early (i.e., shortly after injury) and late (i.e., at the time of the long-term follow-up) home environment and long-term EF following early childhood TBI. Children with OIs were also included to assess the effects of TBI relative to an other-injury comparison group. We used data drawn from the same parent study as that reported on by Kurowski et al. (2011) and Potter et al. (2011), which involved children who were hospitalized for severe TBI, complicated mild/moderate TBI, or OI between 3 and 6 years of age. In this analysis, we examined the relationship of the early and late home environment (i.e., quality of the home environment, family functioning, caregiver psychological distress) to performance-based tests of EF at an average of 6.8 years post-injury.

Given evidence for the effects of early parenting on the development of EF, we hypothesized that measures of the early home environment would be associated with EF across all groups, such that better home environments would predict better performance on EF measures. We expected that measures of the late home environment would be similarly associated with EF across groups, perhaps acting as a mediator of the effects of the early home environment. Finally, although we hypothesized that better home environments would be associated with better EF across groups, we anticipated that these effects would be amplified in the context of TBI, with previous research suggesting that moderation would be most pronounced for children with complicated mild/moderate TBI (e.g., Yeates et al., 2010).

METHODS

Study Design

The current study drew on data from a larger, prospective cohort study, which aimed to examine environmental factors related to long-term functional outcomes following early childhood TBI. Children and caregivers participated in an initial assessment around the time of injury, as well as a series of follow-up assessments, including a final one as the child entered middle school/early adolescence, an average of 6.8 years post-injury (range: 4.5–10.6 years). During the initial assessment, caregivers completed several measures designed to measure aspects of the family environment (i.e., family functioning, caregiver psychological distress), which was followed up by a visit to the child’s home to assess the quality of the home environment. These measures were re-administered at the time of the long-term follow-up, during which children were administered tests of executive functioning.

Participants

The original parent study enrolled 206 children, ages 3 to 6 years, 11 months, who sustained a severe TBI (n = 23), complicated mild/moderate TBI (n = 64), or OI (n = 119). They were recruited through four tertiary care hospitals in the Midwestern United States (three children’s hospitals, one general hospital). Inclusion criteria for both TBI groups included overnight hospitalization for a brain injury resulting from blunt trauma, absence of pre-injury neurological problems or neurodevelopmental disorders, and English as the primary language in the home.

Participants were excluded if the cause of the injury was documented as child abuse. Severity of TBI was defined as the lowest recorded Glasgow Coma Scale (GCS; Teasdale & Jennett, 1974) score after admission to the emergency department. Severe TBI was defined as a GCS score of <8, moderate TBI was defined as a GCS score of 9–12, and complicated mild TBI was defined as a GCS score of 13–15 with associated brain imaging abnormalities. The latter two groups were combined into a complicated mild/moderate TBI group because of research suggesting the two types of injuries have similar outcomes (e.g., Kashluba, Hanks, Casey, & Mills, 2008), as well as to preserve power due to the small sample size of each group. Eligibility criteria for the OI group included overnight hospitalization for an injury not involving the head, as well as absence of impaired consciousness or any other signs or symptoms suggestive of possible head trauma.

At the time of the long-term follow-up, participants were contacted by both phone and mail, inviting both the child and caregiver to participate in a final assessment. The caregiver was the biological parent in all but two cases, and the caregiver changed from baseline assessment to follow-up in eight cases. Approximately 35% of participants did not participate in the follow-up assessment, leaving 16 severe TBI, 44 complicated mild/moderate TBI, and 74 OI (N = 134; see Table 1 for demographic characteristics of the sample). No differences were observed between those who participated in the long-term follow-up and those who did not in age at injury, sex, race, socioeconomic status (measured by mean sample Z-scores for census tract income and guardian education), or early home environment measures. The institutional review boards of all involved sites approved the parent study, and written informed consent was received from the legal guardians of all participants.

Table 1.

Demographic characteristics of study sample

Severe TBI
Complicated mild/moderate TBI
OI
Significance test
n = 16 n = 44 n = 74 F/ χ2 p-Value
Age at injury
M (SD) 5.04 (0.98) 5.16 (1.23) 5.09 (1.07) 0.09 .913
Range 3.64 to 6.62 3.03 to 6.93 3.15 to 6.88
Age at follow-up
M (SD) 12.17 (1.52) 11.99 (1.05) 11.88 (1.09)
Range 10.66 to 16.68 10.13 to 15.17 10.27 to 16.96 0.48 .618
Baseline time since injury (months)
M (SD) 1.95 (1.59) 1.50 (0.80) 1.15 (0.50)
Range 0.53 to 6.18 0.33 to 3.45 0.26 to 2.30 7.366 .001*
Follow-up time since injury (years)
M (SD) 7.13 (1.35) 6.83 (1.16) 6.78 (1.05)
Range 4.47 to 10.06 4.64 to 9.12 4.79 to 10.58 0.61 .543
Sex
(% female) 38 43 47 0.58 .749
Race
(% not Caucasian) 38 25 23 1.47 .479
SES z-scorea
M (SD) −0.42 (0.68) −0.01 (1.05) 0.11 (0.93) 2.09 .128
Median household income 57,447.36 59,902.34 64,705.28 0.78 .463
M (SD) (20,520.01) (26,050.81) (22,839.21)
Maternal education (years)
M (SD) 14.29 (2.92) 13.00 (2.60) 13.79 (2.41) 2.46 .090
Paternal education (years)
M (SD) 13.43 (2.41) 12.83 (2.10) 13.87 (2.51) 2.68 .072
a

Based on census track income and parent/caregiver education at baseline.

*

p<.05

Measures

Home environment

Three measures were administered to assess different aspects of the home environment: quality of the home environment, caregiver psychological distress, and family functioning. Quality of the home environment was measured using the Home Observation for Measures of the Environment (HOME; Bradley & Caldwell, 1984; Caldwell & Bradley, 1984), administered in the participants’ homes by trained research assistants. The HOME is a comprehensive measure designed to assess aspects of the home environment conducive to child development through a combination of parent interviews, direct observation of parent–child interactions, and assessment of available play and learning materials. The early childhood version of the HOME (EC-HOME) was administered shortly after the time of injury (i.e., early) and the early adolescent version of the HOME (EA-HOME) was administered at follow-up (i.e., late). Two research assistants administered the HOME independently during approximately 5% of the home visits to assess inter-rater reliability (r = .92). Total scores of the HOME were analyzed in this study, with higher scores indicative of a better quality home environment.

Parent/caregiver psychological distress was measured at baseline using the Brief Symptom Inventory (BSI; Derogatis & Melisaratos, 1983) and the Symptom Checklist-90-Revised (SCL-90-R; Derogatis & Lazarus, 1994) at follow-up. The BSI is an abbreviated version of the SCL-90-R. Studies have reported a high correlation of the BSI and the SCL-90-R (r = 0.93 according to Derogatis, 2000). Both scales are self-report measures of severity of psychological symptoms in domains that include somatization, obsessive-compulsive symptoms, interpersonal sensitivity, depression, anxiety, hostility, phobia, paranoia, and psychoticism. The Global Severity Index was included as a measure of psychological distress. Higher scores are indicative of greater caregiver psychological distress.

Family functioning was assessed using the General Functioning subscale of the McMaster Family Assessment Device (FAD-GF; Byles, Byrne, Boyle, & Oxford, 1988; Miller, Bishop, Epstein, & Keitner, 1985). This 12-item subscale measures general communication, relationships, and well-being among family members. At the initial assessment, parents were asked to complete the FAD-GF with reference to the family’s functioning before the child’s injury. Higher scores are indicative of worse family functioning.

Executive functioning

Several measures of EF were administered at the long-term follow-up. The Tower of London-Drexel (ToL-Dx; Culbertson & Zillmer, 1998) assesses planning and problem solving as measured in this study by the standard score for total correct items, with higher scores reflecting better performance. The Attention Network Task (ANT; Fan, McCandliss, Sommer, Raz, & Posner, 2002) is a flanker task that provides a measure of EF related to the executive control of attention, as measured by the Conflict score, with higher scores reflecting worse performance. Scores are not standardized by age.

The Test of Everyday Attention for Children (TEA-Ch; Manly, Robertson, Anderson, & Nimmo-Smith, 1999) is an adaptation of the Test of Everyday Attention (Robertson, Ward, Ridgeway, & Nimmo-Smith, 1996). Three TEA-Ch subtests were administered: Walk/Don’t Walk, to assess inhibitory control; Code Transmission, to assess working memory; and Creature Counting, to assess cognitive flexibility. Scaled scores for each TEA-Ch subtests were included in analyses, with higher scores reflecting better performance.

Finally, the Iowa Gambling Task (IGT; Bechara, Damasio, Damasio, & Anderson, 1994) is a decision-making task that measures risk-taking in relation to rewards and penalties in a computerized card game. The total score for the IGT, measured as the difference in proportions of favorable versus unfavorable decisions, was included in the analysis, with higher scores reflecting better performance. This score also is not standardized by age. See Appendix A for detailed task descriptions.

Statistical Analyses

Statistical analyses were conducted using IBM SPSS, Version 21. Hierarchical linear regression analyses were performed with group and the home environment measures entered as predictors and EF measures examined as dependent variables. Two dummy variables were created to compare the severe TBI and complicated mild/moderate TBI groups to the OI group. For each hierarchical regression analysis, predictors were entered in five steps: (1) two dummy variables for group; (2) three early home variables (i.e., early FAD-GF, early BSI, and early HOME); (3) three late home variables (i.e., late FAD-GF, late SCL-90-R, and late HOME); (4) interactions between group and the early home measures (i.e., interaction terms for each dummy variable and the early FAD-GF, early BSI, and early HOME variables); and (5) interactions between group and the late home measures (i.e., interaction terms for each dummy variable and the late FAD-GF, late SCL-90-R, and late HOME).

Regression analyses were run separately for each of the six outcome variables. Significant interactions, indicative of moderation of group differences by the home environment, were explored using the PROCESS macro (Hayes, 2013), which allows for plotting of group scores on an outcome variable at different levels of a predictor (moderator) variable. The program was used only to explore the nature of significant interactions and was not used for statistical analysis of the data. Multicollinearity among predictors was limited (i.e., VIF <1, >10; tolerance values ranging from .37 to .94). Additionally, separate regressions for early and late home environment measures obtained results that were highly similar to the full regression models.

RESULTS

Group means and standard deviations for each of the home environment variables at both time points (early, late) and for the six outcome variables are presented in Table 2. The groups differed in the quality of the home environment at the long-term follow-up, with the lowest mean quality found in the severe TBI group. The groups also differed in parental psychological distress at both occasions, again with the greatest average distress reported in the severe TBI group. Mean within-group correlations are presented in Table 3. The results of the regression analyses are summarized in Table 4. Detailed results of the regression analyses are presented in Supplementary Tables 510.

Table 2.

Group means and standard deviations on home environment and executive functioning measures

Severe TBI
Complicated mild/moderate TBI
OI
Significance test
n M (SD) Range n M (SD) Range n M (SD) Range F p-Value
Early HOME 16 39.63 (7.46) 28–52 42 40.64 (6.83) 28–54 73 43.07 (7.83) 20–54 2.23 .112
Late HOME 15 45.33 (6.02) 32–55 44 45.18 (8.85) 28–60 74 49.22 (7.26) 24–59 4.42* .014
Early FAD-GF 16 1.73 (0.56) 1–2.75 43 1.56 (0.38) 1–2.25 74 1.49 (0.44) 1–2.75 2.18 .117
Late FAD-GF 16 1.85 (0.46) 1–2.5 42 1.65 (0.37) 1–2.50 72 1.61 (0.38) 1–2.42 2.58 .080
Early BSI 16 56.81 (11.62) 36–71 44 50.86 (11.17) 33–71 73 48.81 (11.00) 33–80 3.44* .035
Late SCL-90-R 16 57.81 (9.67) 44–77 42 52.62 (11.62) 30–74 72 49.31 (12.61) 30–81 3.61* .030
ToL-Dx 15 95.87 (15.13) 74–124 42 93.43 (11.05) 62–116 70 93.83 (10.87) 72–132 0.26 .775
ANT Conflict 14 66.82 (47.70) −3–179 35 56.0 (43.20) −61–180 61 65.07 (43.08) −54–187 0.56 .572
TEA-Ch W/DW 15 4.40 (2.75) 1–12 42 8.07 (3.86) 1–17 71 7.94 (3.40) 1–17 7.01* .001
TEA-Ch CT 14 7.14 (2.69) 3–13 42 9.24 (3.48) 1–14 70 9.16 (3.24) 1–14 2.44 .091
TEA-Ch CC 15 9.27 (3.96) 3–14 42 8.95 (3.22) 2–14 71 8.77 (2.90) 3–14 0.16 .849
IGT 15 −17.47 (15.76) −50–4 38 −16.42 (29.47) −88–52 67 −11.91 (24.82) −72–34 0.53 .588

HOME = Home Observation for Measures of the Environment; FAD-GF = Family Assessment Device - General Functioning subscale; BSI = Brief Symptom Inventory; SCL-90-R = Symptom Checklist-Revised; ToL-Dx = Tower of London-Drexel (total standard score); ANT = Attention Network Test (total score); TEA-Ch = Test of Everyday Attention for Children (W/DW = Walk/Don’t Walk, CT = Code Transmission, CC = Creature Counting; total scaled score); IGT = Iowa Gambling Task (total score).

*

p < .05.

Table 3.

Mean within-group correlations

Early HOME Late HOME Early FAD-GF Late FAD-GF Early BSI Late SCL-90-R ToL-Dx ANT TEA-Ch W/DW TEA-Ch CT TEA-Ch CC IGT
Early HOME .74* −.16 −.19 −.16 −.11 .17 −.05 .06 .02 .17 −.04
Late HOME .77* −.04 −.20* −.11 −.09 .06 .06 .09 −.05 .08 −.12
Early FAD-GF −.20* −.04 .45* .41* .33* −.22* .11 .08 −.09 .04 .04
Late FAD-GF −.10 −.20* .45* .30* .37* −.06 .04 −.07 .00 −.07 −.14
Early BSI −.17 −.11 .41* .30* .52* −.26* .21* .04 −.07 .09 .12
Late SCL-90-R −.04 .01 .38* .26* .48* −.03 .19 .17 −.07 .02 .02
ToL-Dx .17 .06 −.22* −.06 −.26* −.03 −.05 .05 .14 .07 .18
ANT −.05 .06 .11 .04 .21* .19 −.05 −.18 −.26* .01 −.06
TEA-Ch W/DW .06 .09 .08 −.07 .04 .17 .05 −.18 .26* .24* −.01
TEA-Ch CT .02 −.05 −.09 .00 −.07 −.07 .14 −.26* .26* −.02 −.01
TEA-Ch CC .17 .08 .04 −.07 .09 .02 .07 .01 .24* −.02 −.05
IGT −.04 −.12 .04 −.14 .12 .07 .18 −.06 −.01 −.01 −.05

HOME = Home Observation for Measures of the Environment; FAD-GF = Family Assessment Device - General Functioning subscale; BSI = Brief Symptom Inventory; SCL-90-R = Symptom Checklist-Revised; ToL-Dx = Tower of London-Drexel (total standard score); ANT = Attention Network Test – Conflict Score (total score); TEA-Ch = Test of Everyday Attention for Children (W/DW = Walk/Don’t Walk; CT = Code Transmission; CC = Creature Counting; total scaled score); IGT = Iowa Gambling Task (total score).

*

p < .05.

Table 4.

Summary of regression analyses

DV n Step R2Δ p-Value Significant predictors β t p-Value
ToL-Dx Total = 122 1 .01 .743
Severe = 15 2 .08* .026 Early BSI −0.21 −2.09 .039
C.Mild/Moderate = 39 3 .01 .642
OI=68 4 .08 .122
5 .06 .296 C.Mild/Moderate TBI x Late FAD-GF 0.98 2.05 .043
ANT Conflict Total = 104 1 .01 .560
Severe = 13 2 .05 .205
C.Mild/Moderate = 32 3 .03 .418
OI=59 4 .10 .098 C.Mild/Moderate TBI x Early HOME −1.28 −2.06 .043
5 .08 .192 C.Mild/Moderate TBI x Late FAD-GF −1.12 −2.07 .041
TEA-Ch W/DW Total = 123 1 .11* .001 Severe TBI −.32 −3.62 <.001
Severe = 15 2 .01 .605
C.Mild/Moderate = 39 3 .02 .439
OI=69 4 .07 .148
5 .06 .193 C.Mild/Moderate TBI x Late SCL-90-R −1.00 −2.05 .043
TEA-Ch CT Total = 121 1 .05 .064 Severe TBI −0.20 −2.15 .033
Severe = 14 2 .01 .651
C.Mild/Moderate = 39 3 .02 .428
OI=68 4 .03 .716
5 .05 .484
TEA-Ch CC Total = 123 1 .00 .911
Severe = 15 2 .03 .301
C.Mild/Moderate = 39 3 .01 .810
OI=69 4 .03 .756
5 .17* .002 Severe TBI x Late SCL-90-R 2.50 2.57 .012
C.Mild/Moderate TBI x Late HOME 2.72 3.34 .001
IGT Total = 116 1 .02 .419
Severe = 14 2 .01 .654
C.Mild/Moderate = 36 3 .08 .033 Late HOME −0.36 −2.48 .015
Late FAD-GF −0.25 −2.17 .032
OI=66 4 .08 .138 C.Mild/Moderate TBI x Early FAD-GF −0.87 −2.13 .036
5 .07 .168 C.Mild/Moderate TBI x Late SCL-90-R −1.13 −2.29 .024

Step 1: Group (i.e., Severe TBI dummy coded variable, Complicated Mild/Moderate TBI dummy coded variable). Step 2: Group, Early HOME, Early FAD-GF, Early BSI. Step 3: Group, Early HOME, Early FAD-GF, Early BSI, Late HOME, Late FAD-GF, Late SCL-90-R. Step 4: Group, Early HOME, Early FAD-GF, Early BSI, Late HOME, Late FAD-GF, Late SCL-90-R, Severe TBI x Early HOME, Severe TBI x Early FAD-GF, Severe TBI x Early BSI, Complicated Mild/ Moderate TBI x Early HOME, Complicated Mild/Moderate TBI x Early FAD-GF, Complicated Mild/Moderate TBI x Early BSI. Step 5: Group, Early HOME, Early FAD-GF, Early BSI, Late HOME, Late FAD-GF, Late SCL-90-R, Severe TBI x Early HOME, Severe TBI x Early FAD-GF, Severe TBI x Early BSI, Complicated Mild/Moderate TBI x Early HOME, Complicated Mild/Moderate TBI x Early FAD-GF, Complicated Mild/Moderate TBI x Early BSI, Severe TBI x Late HOME, Severe TBI x Late FAD-GF, Severe TBI x Late SCL-90-R, Complicated Mild/Moderate TBI x Late HOME, Complicated Mild/Moderate TBI x Late FAD-GF, Complicated Mild/Moderate TBI x Late SCL-90-R.

*

p < .05.

Main Effects of Group

Across the six measures of EF, when the two dummy variables representing group membership were entered first into the regression analyses, the overall effect for group was significant only for the TEA-Ch Walk/Don’t Walk subtest (R2 = .11; p = .001). Follow-up tests revealed that the severe TBI group performed significantly worse than the OI group, t(120) = −3.62, p< .001, but no other group comparisons were significant.

Main Effects of the Home Environment

When the three measures of the early home environment were added to the regression analyses in the second step, they accounted for significant variance in performance on only the ToL-Dx. The early BSI was a significant unique predictor of ToL-Dx performance, t(116) = −2.09, p = .039, such that more parental distress was associated with poorer performance. The three measures of the late home environment, when added in the third step, accounted for marginally significant variance in performance on only the IGT. The late HOME was a significant unique predictor of IGT performance, t(107) = −2.48, p = .015, but the direction of effect was opposite to expectations, with higher quality of the home associated with poorer performance.

Given that late HOME scores were also negatively associated with IGT when considered in isolation (see Table 4), the latter result did not appear to reflect a suppression effect. In contrast, the late FAD-GF predicted IGT performance in the expected direction, with poorer family functioning associated with poorer performance, t(107) = −2.17, p = .032. Overall, the early home environment measures accounted for 1% to 8% of the variance in EF test performance (R2Δ), with the late home environment measures accounting for an additional 1% to 8% of variance.

Moderating Effects of the Early Home Environment

Group × early home environment interaction terms were added fourth to the regression models to test for moderation of group differences by the early home environment. Across the six EF measures, the early HOME significantly moderated the difference between the complicated mild/moderate TBI and OI groups on theANT Conflict score, t(89) = −2.06, p = .043, and the early FAD-GF significantly moderated the difference between the complicated mild/moderate TBI and OI groups on the IGT, t(101) = −2.13, p = .036. As predicted, follow-up moderation analyses suggested that, at lower values of the early HOME (i.e., 1 SD below the sample mean, indicative of lower quality home environments), the complicated mild/moderate TBI group performed worse than the OI group on the ANT, but their performance was better than the OI group at higher values of the HOME (i.e., 1 SD above the sample mean).

A similar moderating effect was found for the early FAD-GF. At low values of the early FAD-GF (1 SD below the sample mean, indicative of better family functioning), the complicated mild/moderate TBI group performed better than the OI group on the IGT, but they performed worse than the OI group at high values of the early FAD-GF (i.e., 1 SD below the sample mean, indicative of worse family functioning).

Moderating Effects of the Late Home Environment

The last step in each regression involved the addition of group × late home environment interaction terms, to test for moderation of group differences by the late home environment. Significant interactions were found for five of six measures of EF, with most involving moderation of the difference between the complicated mild/moderate TBI and OI groups. The late FAD-GF significantly moderated the difference between the complicated mild/moderate TBI and OI groups on both the ToL-Dx, t(101) = 2.05, p = .043, and the ANT Conflict, t(83) = −2.07, p = .041. Unexpectedly, follow-up moderation analyses for both measures revealed that, at higher levels of the FAD-GF (i.e., worse family functioning), performance of the complicated mild/moderate TBI group was better than that of the OI group. These results did not change when early home environment measures were excluded from the regression model, suggesting the unexpected interactions were not an artifact of multicollinearity.

The late SCL-90-R was a significant moderator of the complicated mild/moderate TBI versus OI comparison for both the TEA-Ch Walk/Don’t Walk subtest, t(102) = −2.05, p = .043, and the IGT, t(95) = −2.29, p = .024. On the TEA-Ch Walk/Don’tWalk subtest, contrary to predictions, the complicated mild/moderate TBI group performed worse than the OI group at low values of the late SCL-90-R (1 SD below the sample mean, indicative of lower caregiver distress) and better than the OI group at high values (i.e., 1 SD above the sample mean, indicative of greater caregiver distress).

In contrast, on the IGT, the complicated mild/moderate TBI group performed better than the OI group at low values of the late SCL-90-R and worse at high values. The late SCL-90-R was also a moderator of the severe TBI versus OI comparison on the TEA-Ch Creature Counting subtest, t(102) = 2.57, p = .012. Again, unexpectedly, the severe TBI group performedworse relative to the OI group at low values of the late SCL-90-R and better at high values. These results were replicated when early home environment measures were excluded fromthe regressionmodel, suggesting the unexpected findings were not the result ofmulticollinearity. Finally, the late HOME was a significant moderator of the difference between the complicated mild/moderate TBI and OI groups on the TEA-Ch Creature Counting subtest, t(102) =3.34, p = .001. Follow-up moderation analyses showed that, as predicted, performance of the complicated mild/moderate TBI group was worse than the OI group at low values of the late HOME and better at high values.

Corrections for the false discovery rate were applied to address potential concerns about multiple comparisons. Many effects did not remain significant after controlling for multiple comparisons, and results should be interpreted in light of this limitation. Only the main effect of group in the prediction of TEA-Ch Walk/Don’t Walk performance, as well as the moderating effect of the late HOME on the difference between the complicated mild/moderate TBI and OI groups on the TEA-Ch Creature Counting subtest, remained significant after controlling for the false discovery rate. Notably, the latter interaction was in the predicted direction.

DISCUSSION

This study examined the association between the early and late home environment and long-term EF following early childhood TBI. In view of previous evidence for long-term effects of TBI on EF (e.g., Beauchamp et al., 2011; Mangeot et al., 2010), we anticipated poorer outcomes for the TBI group than for the OI group. Based on the importance of the home environment in shaping EF in both healthy children and those with chronic health conditions (e.g., see Camerota et al., 2015; Roskam et al., 2014; Sarsour et al., 2011), we also expected that measures of the early and late home environment would be associated with EF across groups, with higher quality home environments predicting better performance on EF measures. An additional prediction was that the home environment would moderate the effects of TBI, with lesser effects of complicated mild/moderate TBI relative to OI for children with higher HOME scores.

Our analyses revealed only one group difference that was not moderated by a measure of the home environment and only two associations of the home environment with EF that did not vary across injury groups. Specifically, scores on the TEA-Ch Walk/Don’t Walk subtest were lower for the severe TBI group than the OI group, and the quality of the home environment was associated with scores on the ToL-Dx and the IGT. In contrast, group differences were evident on all EF measures except for the TEA-Ch Code Transmission test when the early and late home environments were considered as moderators of group differences. The relative absence of main effects for either group membership or home environment in isolation underscores the importance of understanding how the home environment influences the effects of injury on EF. The findings also indicate that moderating effects were generally more pronounced for the complicated mild/moderate TBI than for the severe TBI group. Follow-up moderation analyses revealed several findings consistent with the predicted direction of effect, as well as some that were unexpected.

When examining the early home environment, lower quality home environments (i.e., low scores on the early HOME, high scores on the early FAD-GF) were associated with lower scores on EF tests for the complicated mild/moderate TBI group relative to the OI group, while these group differences diminished for children from higher quality home environments. These results are consistent with our predictions, suggesting that the long-term effects of TBI on EF may be moderated by the family environment, with better family environments increasing children’s resilience to TBI and lower quality family environments increasing the risks of worse outcomes.

However, some of the findings involving the late home environment were not supportive of our hypotheses. We found six instances in which the late home environment was a significant moderator of differences between either the complicated mild/moderate TBI or severe TBI group and the OI group. In four of those instances, the direction of effect was opposite to our expectations. Performance on only two outcome measures was moderated by the late home environment in the expected direction, with both instances involving the difference between the complicated mild/moderate TBI and OI groups (i.e., late SCL-90-R as a moderator of the group difference on the IGT; late HOME as a moderator of the group difference on the TEA-Ch Creature Counting subtest).

However, both the late SCL-90-R and late FAD-GF moderated group performance on several EF measures in the direction opposite to that predicted, with three interactions involving the complicated mild/moderate TBI group and one the severe TBI group. The lone significant instance of moderation in the severe TBI group could be spurious. However, the unexpected findings involving the complicated mild/moderate TBI group are more difficult to dismiss. One potential explanation is that greater parental distress, as reflected in higher BSI scores, may reflect the cumulative effect of parents’ efforts to support their children with TBI. That is, perhaps the longer a caregiver works to support their child’s development after a TBI, the more distress they experience, a possibility consistent with findings from a previous TBI study indicating associations of active parent coping with greater burden (Wade et al., 2001).

This notion, however, is somewhat inconsistent with the negative correlation between the late HOME and late SCL-90-R (r =−.19; p = .027), which suggests that lower parental distress is associated with higher quality home environments. Notably, two of the four interactions involving the home environment that were in the predicted direction, and none of those in the opposite direction involved the HOME, suggesting that the quality of the stimulation provided in the home environment may moderate EF after TBI in a manner more consistent with our predictions, while parental distress and family functioning act in a less consistent manner as moderators.

Notably, although early home environment measures did significantly moderate group differences on two of the EF measures (i.e., ANT Conflict, IGT), more moderating effects were found for late home environment variables (i.e., all but one measure of EF). These findings suggest that, although the early home environment may affect early EF following TBI in preschool-aged children (see Kurowski et al., 2011), the late home environment also has a role to play, particularly for children with complicated mild/moderate TBI. If we could be confident that better quality home environments were consistently associated with better EF in children with mild/ moderate TBI, then interventions to improve the home environment later in childhood might have the potential to mitigate the effects of early childhood TBI on long-term EF. However, given that better home environments were not consistently related to better EF performance, and that our findings largely concern individual variation within the normal range, further research is needed to understand the contributions of the home environment to post-injury performance-based EF.

Finally, the findings were consistent with our expectations that the moderation of group differences by the home environment would be most apparent for children with complicated mild/ moderate TBI. Although research on school-age children has found the strongest moderating effects of the home environment in children with severe TBI (Taylor et al., 2002; Yeates et al., 1997, 2002), we have previously reported that moderating effects in preschool-aged children are more pronounced for children with less severe TBI (Yeates et al., 2010). Children who sustain severe TBI at a young age may be less able to overcome the deleterious effects of those injuries than older children, even in the context of a supportive family environment. In contrast, young children with less severe TBI may have more potential to benefit from a higher-quality home environment. An alternative possibility in this study is that the relative absence of significant interactions involving the severe TBI group reflects the relatively small sample size in that group and attendant low power to detect interactions.

The results should be interpreted in light of several other study limitations. As already noted, the sample size is somewhat small, especially in the severe TBI group, and likely reduced the power to detect group differences in EF or evidence of moderation involving that group. Second, the possibility that some of the significant findings were spurious must be acknowledged, particularly given the large number of predictors entered into each model (i.e., of 19 predictor variables in each model, only 13 significant univariate effects were found across all 6 outcomes).

On the other hand, the models as a whole accounted for between 16% and 30% of the variance in EF test performance, reflecting medium to large effect sizes, suggesting that the relationship of group membership and the home environment to outcomes is non-trivial. Moreover, some findings remained significant after controlling for multiple comparisons. Third, potential confounding variables could play an important role in EF (e.g., other cognitive abilities or other environmental variables), but were not controlled in these analyses. Future studies should consider these possibilities by incorporating larger samples and measures of additional environmental variables (e.g., peer relationships, school environment) that may affect the development of EF in later childhood. Finally, EF measures included in this study were treated as individual measures; however, in future studies, composite scores representing specific EF constructs (e.g., inhibitory control, cognitive flexibility) may be more a powerful approach to understanding these relationships.

In conclusion, the current study suggests that both early and late home environments play a role in moderating long-term EF after a TBI in early childhood, especially among children with a complicated mild/moderate TBI. The late home environment may play a particularly important role as a moderator, although the direction of effect is inconsistent and the mechanisms by which these effects occur remain unclear. The results provide tentative support for the implementation of interventions to improve the quality of the stimulation provided in the home environment after injury to potentially mitigate negative effects of early childhood TBI on long-term EF, but do not suggest that reducing parental distress or improving family functioning will necessarily have similar benefits.

Supplementary Material

Appendix A
Supp Table 5

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

This research was supported by Grant R01 HD42729 from NICHD, in part by USPHS NIH Grant M01 RR 08084, Trauma Research grants from the State of Ohio Emergency Medical Services, as well as the National Center for Research Resources and the National Center for Advancing Translational Sciences, National Institutes of Health, through Grant 8 UL1 TR000077-04. Dr. Yeates received support from career development Grant K02 HD44099 from NICHD. The authors wish to acknowledge the contributions of Christine Abraham, Andrea Beebe, Lori Bernard, Anne Birnbaum, Beth Bishop, Tammy Matecun, Karen Oberjohn, Elizabeth Roth, and Elizabeth Shaver in data collection. The Cincinnati Children’s Medical Center Trauma Registry, Rainbow Pediatric Trauma Center, Rainbow Babies & Children’s Hospital, Nationwide Children’s Hospital Trauma Program, and MetroHealth Center Department of Pediatrics and Trauma Registry provided assistance with recruitment. Finally, Ms. Laliberté Durish received funding from Alberta Innovates-Health Solutions and the Alberta Children’s Hospital Research Institute in the form of a graduate studentship.

Appendix A

Measure Age Rangea Task Description
EC HOME 3–6 years Form containing 55 items that are grouped into 8 subscales: 1) learning materials, 2) language stimulation, 3) physical environment, 4) parental responsivity, 5) learning stimulation, 6) modeling of social maturity, 7) variety in experience, 8) acceptance of child. During a visit to the child’s home, a rater (e.g., clinician, research assistant) places a plus (+) or minus (−) in a box alongside each item if the behaviour is observed during the visit or if a parent reports that the conditions or events are characteristic of the home environment.
EA HOME 10–15 years Form containing 60 items that are grouped into 7 subscales: 1) physical environment, 2) learning materials, 3) modeling, 4) instructional activities, 5) regulatory activities 6) variety of experience, 7) acceptance and responsivity. During a visit to the child’s home, a rater (e.g., clinician, research assistant) places a plus (+) or minus (−) in a box alongside each item if the behaviour is observed during the visit or if a parent reports that the conditions or events are characteristic of the home environment.
BSI 13+ years Self-report questionnaire containing 53 items that ask individuals to rate how much they have been bothered by various symptoms within the past week (0 = not at all, 4 = extremely).
SCL-90-R 13+ years Self-report questionnaire containing 90 items that ask individuals to rate how much they have been bothered by various symptoms within the past week (0 = not at all, 4 = extremely).
FAD-GF 12+ years Self-report questionnaire containing 12 statements about families, on which individuals rate the extent to which they agree or disagree (on a 4 point scale).
ToL-Dx 7–15 years Red, green, and blue beads are arranged on a pegboard to match a diagram. Participants are asked to replicate the configuration on a second pegboard. Scores are calculated for total correct, total moves, total initiation time, total execution time, total time, total time violations, and total rule violations.
ANT 6–85 years Computerized test wherein an arrow appears above or below a fixation point and participants are asked to determine whether an arrow points left or right. Scores reflect how response times are influenced by flankers and are calculated as the median reaction time on congruent trials subtracted from the median reaction time on incongruent trials.
TEA-Ch
W/DW
6–15 years A paper and pencil task wherein the participant is asked to draw one step along a pathway after each tone they hear on a tape. When a tone ends, the child is signalled to stop. The task measures whether the child is able to stop drawing the path when signalled to stop.
TEA-Ch
CT
6–15 years The participant listens to a stream of digits presented at a rate of one every 2 seconds and is asked to identify the digit that occurred immediately prior to a particular sequence (e.g., 5–5).
TEA-Ch
CC
6–15 years The participant is asked to count aliens, repeatedly switching between counting forwards and backwards depending on the direction of an arrow. Accuracy and completion time are recorded.
IGT 8–79 years Computerized task wherein participants are presented with 4 virtual decks of cards and asked to choose a card from one of the decks with the goal of wining as much “money” as possible. “Bad” decks lead to higher initial rewards but higher potential losses, thus lower overall value. “Good” decks lead to lower initial rewards but lower possible losses, thus higher overall value. Participants are expected to learn the nature of the decks through trial and error.

HOME = Home Observation for Measures of the Environment (EC = Early Childhood, EA = Early Adolescent); FAD-GF = Family Assessment Device - General Functioning subscale; BSI = Brief Symptom Inventory; SCL-90-R = Symptom Checklist-Revised; ToL-Dx = Tower of London-Drexel; ANT = Attention Network Test; TEA-Ch = Test of Everyday Attention for Children (W/DW = Walk/Don’t Walk; CT = Code Transmission; CC = Creature Counting); IGT = Iowa Gambling Task

a

Some participants were outside the age range for specific measures (i.e., up to 1 year). However, to limit attrition and maintain consistency, the authors decided to administer the same measures.

Footnotes

The authors do not declare any conflicts of interest.

Supplementary materials

To view supplementary material for this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617717000595

REFERENCES

  1. Anderson VA, Catroppa C, Morse S, Haritou F, & Rosenfeld J (2005). Functional plasticity or vulnerability after early brain injury? Pediatrics, 116, 1374–1382. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Beauchamp M, Catroppa C, Godfrey C, Morse S, Rosenfeld JF, & Anderson V (2011). Selective changes in executive functioning ten years after severe childhood traumatic brain injury. Developmental Neuropsychology, 36(5), 578–595. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Bechara A, Damasio AR, Damasio H, & Anderson SW (1994). Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition, 50(1–3), 7–15. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(94)90018-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bigler ED, Jantz PB, Farrer TJ, Abildskov TJ, Dennis M, Gerhardt CA, … Yeates KO (2015). Day of injury CT and late MRI findings: Cognitive outcome in a pediatric sample with complicated mild traumatic brain injury. Brain Injury, 29(9), 1062–1070. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Blair C, Ravery CC, Berry DJ, & The Family Life Project Investigators. (2014). Two approaches to estimating the effect of parenting on the development of executive function in early childhood. Developmental Psychology, 50(2), 554–565. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bradley RH, & Caldwell BM (1984). The HOME Inventory and family demographics. Developmental Psychology, 20, 315–320. [Google Scholar]
  7. Byles J, Byrne C, Boyle MH, & Oxford OR (1988). Ontario Child Health Study: Reliability and validity of the General Functioning Scale of the McMaster Family Assessment Device. Family Process, 27, 97–104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Caldwell B, & Bradley R (1984). Home observation for measurement of the environment. Little Rock: University of Arkansas at Little Rock. [Google Scholar]
  9. Camerota M, Willoughby MT, Coz M, Greenberg MT, & The Family Life Project Investigators. (2015). Executive function in low birth weight preschoolers: The moderating effect of parenting. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 43(8), 1551–1562. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Crowe LM, Anderson V, Barton S, Babl FE, & Catroppa C (2014). Verbal ability and language outcome following traumatic brain injury in early childhood. The Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 29(3), 217–223. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Crowe LM, Catroppa C, Babl FE, & Anderson V (2012). Intellectual, behavioral, and social outcomes of accidental traumatic brain injury in early childhood. Pediatrics, 129, e262–e268. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Culbertson WC, & Zillmer EA (1998). The Tower of LondonDX: A standardized approach to assessing executive functioning in children. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 13(3), 285–301. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Derogatis LR (2000). The Brief Symptom Inventory-18 (BSI-18): Administration, Scoring, and Procedures Manual. 3rd ed. Minneapolis, MN: National Computer Systems. [Google Scholar]
  14. Derogatis L, & Melisaratos N (1983). The Brief Symptom Inventory: An introductory report. Psychological Medicine, 13, 595–605. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Derogatis L, & Lazarus L (1994). SCL-90–R, Brief Symptom Inventory, and matching clinical rating scales. In Maruish ME (Ed.), The use of psychological testing for treatment planning and outcome assessment (pp. 217–248). Hillsdale, NJ, England: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. [Google Scholar]
  16. Ewing-Cobbs L, Prasad MR, Kramer L, Cox CS Jr., Baumgartner J, Fletcher S, … Swank P (2006). Late intellectual and academic outcomes following traumatic brain injury sustained during early childhood. Journal of Neurosurgery, 105(4 Suppl), 287–296. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Fan J, McCandliss BD, Sommer T, Raz A, & Posner MI (2002). Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14(3), 340–347. doi: 10.1162/089892902317361886 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Faul M, Xu L, Wald MM, & Coronado VG (2010). Traumatic brain injury in the United States: Emergency department visits, hospitalizations and deaths 2002–2006. Atlanta, GA: Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, National Center for Injury Prevention and Control. [Google Scholar]
  19. Garcia D, Hungerford GM, & Bagner DM (2015). Topical review: Negative behavioural and cognitive outcomes following traumatic brain injury in early childhood. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 40(4), 391–397. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Hayes AF (2013). Introduction to Medication, Moderation, and Conditionall Process Analysis: A Regression-Based Approach. New York, NY: The Guilford Press. [Google Scholar]
  21. Kashluba S, Hanks RA, Casey JE, & Mills SR (2008). Neuropsychologic and functional outcome after complicated mild traumatic brain injury. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 89(5), 904–911. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Konigs M, Heij HA, van der Slujis JA, Vermeulen J, Goslings JC, Luitse JSK, … Oosterlaan J (2015). Pediatric traumatic brain injury and attention deficit. Pediatrics, 136(3), 534–541. doi: 10.1542/peds.2015-0437 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Kurowski BG, Taylor GH, Yeates KO, Walz NC, Stancin T, & Wade SL (2011). Caregiver ratings of long-term executive dysfunction and attention problems after early childhood traumatic brain injury: Family functioning is important. PM & R, 3(9), 836–845. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Mangeot S, Armstrong K, Colvin AN, Yeates KO, & Taylor GH (2010). Long-term executive function deficits in children with traumatic brain injuries: Assessment using the Behaviour Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF). Child Neuropsychology, 8(4), 271–284. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Manly T, Robertson IH, Anderson V, & Nimmo-Smith I (1999). TEA-Ch: The Test of Everyday Attention for Children Manual. Bury St. Edmunds, UK: Thames Valley Test Company Limited. [Google Scholar]
  26. Miller IW, Bishop DS, Epstein NB, & Keitner GI (1985). The McMaster Family Assessment Device: Reliability and validity. Journal of Marital and Family Therapy, 11, 345–356. [Google Scholar]
  27. Papoutsis J, Stargatt R, & Catroppa C (2014). Long-term executive functioning outcomes for complicated and uncomplicated mild traumatic brain injury sustained in early childhood. Developmental Neuropsychology, 39(8), 638–645. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Potter JL, Wade SL, Walz NC, Cassedy A, Yeates KO, Stevens HM, & Taylor GH (2011). Parenting style is related to executive dysfunction after brain injury in children. Rehabilitation Psychology, 56(4), 351–358. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Robertson IH, Ward A, Ridgeway V, & Nimmo-Smith I (1996). Test of everyday attention. Journal of the International Neurological Society, 2, 525–534. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Roskam I, Stievenart M, Meunier J-C, & Noel M-P (2014). The development of children’s inhibition: Does parenting matter? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 122, 166–182. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Sarsour K, Sheridan M, Jutte D, Nuru-Jeter A, Hinshaw S, & Boyce WT (2011). Family socioeconomic status and child executive functions: The roles of language, home environment, and single parenthood. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 17, 120–132. doi: 10.1017/S1355617710001335 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Schmidt AT, Hanten GR, Li X, Vasquez AC, Wilde EA, Chapman SB, & Levin HS (2012). Decision making after pediatric traumatic brain injury: Trajectory of recovery and relationship to age and gender. International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience, 30(3), 225–230. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Schwartz L, Taylor HG, Drotar D, Yeates KO, Wade SL, & Stancin T (2003). Long-term behavior problems after pediatric traumatic brain injury: Prevalence, predictors, and correlates. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 28, 251–264. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Slomine BS, Gerring JP, Grados MA, Vasa R, Brady KD, Christensen JR, & Denckla MB (2002). Performance on measures of ‘executive function’ following pediatric traumatic brain injury. Brain Injury, 16(9), 759–772. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Taylor HG, Yeates KO, Wade SL, Drotar D, Stancin T, & Minich N (2002). A prospective study of short- and long-term outcomes after traumatic brain injury in children: Behaviour and achievement. Neuropsychology, 16(1), 15–27. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Teasdale G, & Jennett B (1974). Assessment of coma and impaired consciousness. A practical scale. Lancet, 2(7872), 81–84. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Wade SL, Borawski EA, Taylor HG, Drotar D, Yeates KO, & Stancin T (2001). The relationship of caregiver coping to family outcomes during the initial year following pediatric traumatic brain injury. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 69, 406–415. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Wade SL, Cassedy A, Walz NC, Taylor HC, Stancin T, & Yeates KO (2011). The relationship of parental warm responsiveness and negativity to emerging behavior problems following traumatic brain injury in young children. Developmental Psychology, 47(1), 119–133. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Yeates KO, Swift E, Taylor HG, Wade SL, Drotar D, Stancin T, & Minich N (2004). Short- and long-term social outcomes following pediatric traumatic brain injury. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 10, 412–426. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Yeates KO, Taylor HG, Drotar D, Wade SL, Klein S, Stancin T, & Schatschneider C (1997). Preinjury family environment as a determinant of recovery from traumatic brain injuries in school-age children. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 3(6), 617–630. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Yeates KO, Taylor HG, Walz NC, Stancin T, & Wade SL (2010). The family environment as a moderator of psychosocial outcomes following traumatic brain injury in young children. Neuropsychology, 24(3), 345–356. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Yeates KO, Taylor HG, Woodrome SE, Wade SL, Stancin T, & Drotar D (2002). Race as a moderator of parent and family outcomes following pediatric traumatic brain injury. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 27(4), 393–403. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Appendix A
Supp Table 5

RESOURCES